ArticlePDF Available

Reproductive Biology of the Coyote (Canis latrans): Integration of Mating Behavior, Reproductive Hormones, and Vaginal Cytology

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

The reproductive biology of wild Canis species is often described as unique among mammals because an unusual combination of behavioral and physiological characteristics including a seasonally monestrous cycle, copulatory lock or tie, obligatory pseudopregnancy, social monogamy, and biparental care of the young. We investigated social behavior, endocrine profiles, and vaginal cytology of female coyotes (Canis latrans) during 4 breeding seasons, 2000-2003. Blood levels of estradiol, progesterone, prolactin, and relaxin were measured, and mating behavior and changes in vaginal epithelium were documented. After aligning the data from each individual to her estimated day of ovulation, we compared pregnant coyotes with nonpregnant females and evaluated temporal relationships among hormone levels, behavior, and vaginal cytology. We found that patterns of proceptive and receptive behaviors correlated with the secretion of steroid hormones, as did vaginal epithelial cytomorphosis. In addition, although progesterone levels of pregnant and pseudopregnant coyotes were indistinguishable, prolactin demonstrated a discernible intergroup difference and relaxin was only detectable in pregnant females. Although this study included characteristics not previously published for this species, it also showed how key aspects of reproduction were correlated temporally, and emphasized the importance of an integrated perspective when addressing the reproductive biology of coyotes, or other wild species of canids.
Content may be subject to copyright.
REPRODUCTIVE BIOLOGY OF THE COYOTE (CANIS LATRANS):
INTEGRATION OF MATING BEHAVIOR, REPRODUCTIVE
HORMONES, AND VAGINAL CYTOLOGY
DEBRA A. CARLSON*AND ERIC M. GESE
Department of Wildland Resources, Utah State University, Logan, UT 84322-5230, USA (DAC)
United States Department of Agriculture, Animal and Plant Health Inspection Service,
Wildlife Services, National Wildlife Research Center, Department of Wildland
Resources, Utah State University, Logan, UT 84322-5230, USA (EMG)
The reproductive biology of wild Canis species is often described as unique among mammals because an unusual
combination of behavioral and physiological characteristics including a seasonally monestrous cycle, copulatory
lock or tie, obligatory pseudopregnancy, social monogamy, and biparental care of the young. We investigated
social behavior, endocrine profiles, and vaginal cytology of female coyotes (Canis latrans) during 4 breeding
seasons, 2000–2003. Blood levels of estradiol, progesterone, prolactin, and relaxin were measured, and mating
behavior and changes in vaginal epithelium were documented. After aligning the data from each individual to her
estimated day of ovulation, we compared pregnant coyotes with nonpregnant females and evaluated temporal
relationships among hormone levels, behavior, and vaginal cytology. We found that patterns of proceptive and
receptive behaviors correlated with the secretion of steroid hormones, as did vaginal epithelial cytomorphosis. In
addition, although progesterone levels of pregnant and pseudopregnant coyotes were indistinguishable, prolactin
demonstrated a discernible intergroup difference and relaxin was only detectable in pregnant females. Although
this study included characteristics not previously published for this species, it also showed how key aspects of
reproduction were correlated temporally, and emphasized the importance of an integrated perspective when
addressing the reproductive biology of coyotes, or other wild species of canids.
Key words: Canis latrans, coyote, mating behavior, ovarian cycle, pseudopregnancy, reproductive hormones, vaginal
cytology, wild canid
Coyotes (Canis latrans) are medium-sized wild canids
indigenous to North America. They are seasonally monestrus
(Gier 1968; Hamlett 1938; Kennelly and Johns 1976; Stellflug
et al. 1981), socially monogamous, and territorial (Andelt
1985; Bekoff and Wells 1986; Bromley and Gese 2001;
Camenzind 1978; Gese 2001). Once bonded, a coyote pair
remains together for an indefinite number of years, sharing
responsibility for territory maintenance. Litters averaging 3–7
pups are typically born March–May in most North American
latitudes after a gestation of 60–63 days (Gier 1968; Hamlett
1938; Kennelly et al. 1977; Knowlton 1972) and both parents
participate in the care and rearing of young (Andelt 1985;
Camenzind 1978; Gier 1968; Hatier 1995; Mengel 1971; Silver
and Silver 1969).
Mature offspring may disperse or remain within their natal
territories, assisting in the defense of resources and infant pups,
but typically only the dominant male and female breed (Andelt
1985; Bekoff and Wells 1986; Gese 2001; Gese et al. 1989,
1996). Juvenile coyotes ,12 months of age can be reproduc-
tively active in their 1st winter, but available evidence suggests
that juvenile and yearling (12–24 months) females are less
fecund than adult females 2 years of age (Gier 1968; Green
et al. 2002; Hamlett 1938; Kennelly and Johns 1976; Sacks
2005; Windberg 1995). Older females 10 years of age
gradually pass into reproductive senescence (Green et al. 2002;
Sacks 2005), whereas a male coyote was reported to have sired
pups when 12 years of age (Gese 1990). Older coyotes may
continue to maintain territory residency or revert to a transient
lifestyle (Gese 1990; Windberg 1995).
The reproductive tracts of adult coyotes experience extensive
remodeling during the breeding season, and histological evi-
dence suggests a female coyote is incapable of serial ovula-
tions, even if she is not impregnated during her 1st estrus (Gier
1968; Hamlett 1938; Kennelly and Johns 1976). Ovulation is
* Correspondent: debra.carlson@usu.edu
Ó2008 American Society of Mammalogists
www.mammalogy.org
Journal of Mammalogy, 89(3):654–664, 2008
654
spontaneous, synchronous, and bilateral. The subsequent
corpora lutea crowd other ovarian tissue to such a degree that
the existence of additional tertiary follicles appears improbable.
Furthermore, ovarian retrogression is protracted, the corpora
lutea taking .9 months to degenerate, thereby inhibiting a new
wave of follicular recruitment (Hamlett 1938).
Hypertrophy of the uterus and vagina are also remarkable
with gross morphological differences between sexually mature
and immature females (Gier 1968; Kennelly and Johns 1976).
Juvenile females may experience up-regulation of reproductive
hormones (specifically estradiol) and concomitant physical
signs such as vulvar edema and a serosanguineous vaginal
discharge. However, they may not ovulate (Hodges 1990;
Stellflug et al. 1981), with follicular development arresting
before, or at, the tertiary stage (Kennelly and Johns 1976).
Alternatively, subordinate females may ovulate, but proestrus
and estrus in these individuals appears delayed relative to the
estrous phases of dominant female pack-mates, and typically
subordinate females fail to breed (Hodges 1990).
Among wild species, not dependent on human intervention,
successful reproduction relies on a progression of key elements
(Asa and Valdespino 1998; Kleiman and Eisenberg 1973).
Mate acquisition, conception, gestation, and parental care rely
upon effective synchronization of physiological processes,
anatomical modifications, and social behaviors. During 4
consecutive breeding seasons (2000–2003) we measured the
levels of estradiol, progesterone, prolactin, and relaxin in
coyote sera and plasma. Concurrently, samples of exfoliated
vaginal epithelium were collected in proestrus, estrus, and
diestrus, and examined microscopically; observations of
mating behavior also were documented. The data were then
aligned to each individual female’s estimated day of ovulation.
Females housed with their mates (and that became pregnant)
were compared to a nonpregnant cohort (sequestered females).
Herein, we describe our observations and findings, comparing
pregnant coyotes with pseudopregnant females, but also
examine associations among behavior, endocrine patterns,
and vaginal cytology. Examination of our data suggests that
important relationships exist between these factors, and
integrated examinations of complex systems can increase our
understanding to an extent that might not be accrued from
simple experimental constructions.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Animals.—Coyotes were captive born or wild caught as
pups, and reared at the National Wildlife Research Center
facility in Millville, Utah (418689N, 1118829W). All animals
were housed in outdoor enclosures with natural lighting. Adult
(2 years of age) coyotes were randomly assigned an unrelated
mate before winter, and whenever possible, an established pair
remained together for several years. Mated pairs resided in
individual 0.1-ha pens with access to sheltered den boxes.
Three pens formed a clover-shaped cluster separated by double
fencing and concrete barriers. Although physically separated,
all pairs were within visual and audible range of other coyotes.
As required, females were sequestered from their mates during
the breeding season and served as nonpregnant controls. In
these cases, the coyotes were housed individually in sheltered
outdoor kennels with attached den boxes for privacy, and pair-
mates were kept near each other in adjacent kennels during
their separation.
Mean (6SD) ages for female and male coyotes in this study
were 4.7 62.0 and 5.6 63.2 years, respectively; and females
weighed an average 11.0 61.3 kg (weights of males were not
collected). The coyotes were fed a commercially prepared
carnivore diet (Fur Breeders Agricultural Cooperative, Sandy,
Utah) once daily, and fasted 1 day per week. Water was
provided ad libitum. Vaccinations were given annually against
canine distemper, hepatitis, leptospirosis, parvovirus, para-
influenza, type 2 coronavirus, adenovirus, and rabies. Routine
parasite control was administered as indicated. Research proto-
cols were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use
Committees at Utah State University and National Wildlife
Research Center, and were in compliance with guidelines of the
American Society of Mammalogists (Gannon et al. 2007).
Study design.—During 2000–2003 breeding seasons (January–
March), mating behavior of 32 pairs of coyotes was observed
and recorded. In 2000–2002, serial blood and vaginal cytology
samples were collected from a subset of 18 females, in 2 cohort
groups—8 sequestered during estrus and 10 that remained with
their mates.
Females were considered ‘‘pregnant’’ in years when they
resided with their mates, and were observed with live pups
after parturition, or tested positive for relaxin. Alternatively,
sequestered females were considered ‘‘nonpregnant.’’ Hormone
profiles of 2 coyotes were excluded when they experienced
idiopathic spontaneous abortions in mid- and late-term
gestation. In the 1st case, 2 expelled fetuses were recovered;
in the 2nd case, 1 fetus was seen but later was consumed by the
female. So although their pregnancies were confirmed, cause of
the abortions was unknown, and the hormone data were con-
sidered potentially misrepresentative of a normal pregnancy.
However, behavioral and vaginal cytology data for these indi-
viduals were included with those of the pregnant cohort.
Ovarian cycle.—The reproductive cycle of a female is
partitioned into phases based upon changes in physiology or
behavior. For coyotes in this study, estrus was the phase during
which the female was receptive and cooperative with her
mate’s attempts to copulate, and the beginning of estrus was
demarcated by the 1st observed copulatory tie (or sperm in the
vaginal smear). Proestrus precedes estrus and is characterized
by the presence of red blood cells in vaginal exudate. Diestrus
immediately follows estrus and includes pregnancy and the
protracted luteal phase of nonpregnant females. Luteal activity
is typically prolonged in nonpregnant canids; and because the
expression of progesterone is indistinguishable between
pregnant and nonpregnant females, the latter are commonly
described as pseudopregnant (Concannon et al. 1989; Feldman
and Nelson 2004). Accordingly, a coyote was described as
pseudopregnant if there was a marked and sustained elevation
of serum progesterone after ovulation. Pseudopregnancy was
assumed to be covert and subclinical (i.e., no mammary
development, lactation, or denning behavior) unless otherwise
June 2008 655CARLSON AND GESE—REPRODUCTIVE BIOLOGY OF THE COYOTE
noted. After parturition, or regression of the corpora lutea,
hormone synthesis diminishes and anestrus follows diestrus.
Specimen collection and handling.— Peripheral blood sam-
ples were collected weekly by venipuncture of the cephalic or
saphenous veins, or daily from an indwelling catheter in the
external jugular vein. Samples were collected between 0700
and 0900 h, before the animals were fed and without sedation
or anesthesia. In some cases sampling began as early as 4
weeks before a female was receptive to her mate’s attempts to
copulate, or 6 weeks before a sequestered female ovulated.
Blood sampling continued throughout estrus and diestrus with
the latest samples collected 3 weeks after the birth of pups. To
minimize investigator disturbance of mating activity, blood
collection from paired females was restricted to weekly
sampling until 3 weeks after the 1st copulatory tie.
For quantitative analyses of progesterone, estradiol, and
prolactin, whole blood was collected in an evacuated tube and
allowed to clot at room temperature (20–248C) for 30–120 min.
The serum was separated from the blood cells, divided
into aliquots, and stored at 208C until testing. Progesterone
levels were determined from 727 specimens, and sample
subsets also were used to measure estradiol (n¼405) and
prolactin (n¼205).
Specimens for qualitative assay of relaxin were collected
as whole blood in sodium or lithium heparin. Plasma was
separated as soon as possible and stored at 208C until
testing. Samples for relaxin were collected from the 32 coyote
females included in this study but also from additional animals
enrolled in other studies at the research facility. Collectively,
these data were used to validate relaxin as a diagnostic marker
of pregnancy in coyotes and the results have been discussed
elsewhere (Carlson and Gese 2007).
Exfoliated vaginal epithelial cells were collected weekly
(typically the same day as a blood sample) using a sterile swab
premoistened with normal saline. The swab was gently passed
into the vaginal vault, carefully avoiding the clitoral fossa, and
rotated against the lining of the vaginal lumen (Feldman and
Nelson 2004). Once withdrawn the swab was immediately
rolled along a clean glass slide in 2 or 3 rows. The sample was
allowed to air dry at room temperature then fixed and stained as
soon as possible.
Mating behavior.—Continuous scanning observations were
conducted daily throughout available daylight hours, January–
March, 2000–2003. The animals were habituated to low-level
human activity before data collection although all enclosures
could be viewed through binoculars or a spotting scope from
observation sites 100–500 m away. The observer would view
a pen, document any interactive behavior occurring between
pair-mates, then scan the next pen. Because this process rarely
took more than 30 s per pen, all pens were viewed at least once
every 5–10 min.
Characterization of mating behavior was standardized
between observers and documented (Bekoff and Diamond
1976; Golani and Mendelssohn 1971; Schenkel 1967).
Behaviors recorded included courtship (nonantagonistic play-
wrestling or play-chases; allogrooming face, ears, or back;
body-bumps; hip-pushes; or sleeping curled against each
other); olfactory sampling (sniff or lick of the female’s
anogenital region by the male, solicitation by females with
diverted tail, and sniff or lick of the male’s inguinal area by the
female); overt sexual activity (attempted mount usually
preceded by the male standing perpendicular to the female
with his head or bent foreleg on her shoulders or back, male
mounting the female, and copulatory tie or lock); and mate
guarding (male shadowing the female around the pen, or when
in view of neighbors the male would stand on the female with
stiff forelegs on her back, or stand over her as she lay on the
ground).
Observers avoided redundant documentation by recording
a mating behavior only once even if a pair of coyotes continued
the behavior for an extended period of time (e.g., playing might
last 15 min and through several scanning passes). Exceptions
were any behavior that was terminated then reinitiated (e.g.,
precopulatory mounts).
Reproductive hormone assays.— Quantitative measurement
of progesterone in coyote sera was performed by competitive
binding enzyme immunoassay according to the manufacturer’s
instructions (Progesterone EIA, DSL-10-3900; Diagnostic
Systems Laboratories, Inc., Webster, Texas). By this method,
horseradish peroxidase–labeled progesterone competed with
free progesterone in coyote sera for a fixed quantity of rabbit
anti-progesterone. Microtiter wells coated with goat anti-rabbit
immunoglobulin G captured the antibody-bound progesterone.
Extraneous material was rinsed from the well, and addition of
tetramethylbenzidine chromogenic solution permitted photo-
metric measurement (Benchmark microplate reader; Bio-Rad
Laboratories, Hercules, California) of reagent standards,
controls, and unknown samples. Unknown coyote samples
were compared to a standard curve generated for each run
using Microplate Manager/PC software (version 4.0; Bio-Rad
Laboratories), with the quantity of progesterone being in-
versely proportional to the intensity of color development.
Stated level of sensitivity for progesterone was 0.13 ng/ml.
Quantitative measurement of estradiol was performed by
competitive binding enzyme immunoassay (3rd Generation
Estradiol EIA, DSL-10-39100; Diagnostic Systems Laborato-
ries). In this assay, estradiol–biotin conjugate competed with
free estradiol in coyote sera for available rabbit anti-estradiol
sites fixed to microtiter wells. Streptavidin–horseradish perox-
idase was added, binding to the biotinylated estradiol, and
tetramethylbenzidine precipitated color development in the
reagent standards, controls, and unknown coyote samples.
Color development measured with a photometer (Benchmark
microplate reader) was inversely proportional to the quantity of
estradiol captured in each well, and unknown coyote samples
were compared to a standard curve generated for each run
using Microplate Manager/PC software (version 4.0). Level of
sensitivity for this assay was 1.5 pg/ml.
Coyote samples were not pretreated or extracted before
testing for progesterone or estradiol. Validation procedures
including linearity and recovery assessments were performed
and the assays were found acceptable for use in this species
(Carlson 2008). When possible, samples from the same cohort
were tested together to reduce reagent lot-to-lot variability.
656 JOURNAL OF MAMMALOGY Vol. 89, No. 3
Unknown samples were tested in duplicate and intra-assay
coefficient of variation (CV) was 10% for all results included
in the data set. For progesterone reagent standards and controls,
within-lot interassay mean CV was 9.6%, and interlot CV was
20.2%. For estradiol (single lot only) interassay mean CV was
11.2%.
Prolactin was quantitatively measured by double-antibody
prolactin radioimmunoassay at the Colorado State University
Endocrine Laboratory (Colorado State University, Fort Collins,
Colorado). In this assay, coyote prolactin competed with
125
I
canine prolactin for a fixed amount of rabbit anti-canine
prolactin antibodies. Anti-rabbit immunoglobulin G was added
and radioactivity of the precipitated pellet was measured.
Unknown coyote samples were compared to a standard curve,
with the amount of iodinated antibody–antigen complexes
detected being inversely proportional to the quantity of pro-
lactin in the coyote sera. Lowest detectable limit for prolactin
was 2.33 ng/ml.
Qualitative measurement of relaxin in heparinized coyote
plasma was performed by enzyme-linked immunoassay for
canine relaxin (ReproCHEK; Synbiotics Corporation, San
Diego, California). Free relaxin in an unknown sample was
captured between polyclonal anti-relaxin antibodies in solid
phase (microtiter wells) and monoclonal anti-relaxin antibodies
conjugated to horseradish peroxidase. Subsequent color de-
velopment was directly associated with presence or absence of
relaxin in the sample. Absorbance was measured photometri-
cally, and an optical density .0.050 (minimum threshold for
a positive result) was adopted to distinguish between preg-
nancy and pseudopregnancy in the coyote. It was also im-
portant to note that relaxin persisted in peripheral blood after
parturition and could not reliably predict abortion in coyotes
(Carlson and Gese 2007).
Vaginal exfoliative cytology.— Air-dried samples of vaginal
epithelial cells and uterine exudate on glass slides were fixed
with methanol and stained with a modified Wright–Giemsa
stain (Diff-Quik; Jorgensen Laboratories, Loveland, Colorado).
Slides were then examined under high dry magnification
(400) with at least 5 fields per row (10 fields per slide) of
stained material viewed. The observed epithelial cells were
characterized as parabasal, small intermediate, large interme-
diate, superficial, and keratinized (anuclear) superficial (Shutte
classification—Christie et al. 1972; Feldman and Nelson 2004),
and their relative representations in the sample were graded on
a semiquantitative scale of 1–5 (Bradley and Benson 1974). In
addition, inclusions such as white blood cells, red blood cells,
mucus, amorphous debris (degenerating blood and epithelial
cells), and spermatozoa also were noted.
Data analysis.—Data were aligned by the estimated day of
ovulation for an individual before it was compiled by study
group. This estimate was either back-calculated from the day of
parturition, or based upon changes in serum progesterone
levels. Examination of data presented by Kennelly and Johns
(1976) suggested that coyotes ovulate immature (primary)
oocytes, similar to domestic dogs (Canis familiaris). If true,
then fertilization probably does not occur until 2–3 days after
ovulation (domestic dog—Tsutsui 1989). In this study, there-
fore, gestation was standardized and assumed to be 62 days
from fertilization (Gier 1968; Hamlett 1938; Kennelly et al.
1977) or 64 days after ovulation. Alternatively, the day of
ovulation for nonpregnant females was inferred from the
change in daily progesterone levels; specifically, the day on
which progesterone concentrations approximately doubled
from previous samples.
Hormone and vaginal cytology data are presented herein as
weekly mean values of all females within a cohort, and results
obtained on the estimated day of ovulation are included in
‘‘Week 1.’’ In circumstances in which multiple hormone assay
results were available for an individual within a given week,
a weekly mean value for that individual was calculated in order
to normalize the influence of each individual’s contribution to
the cohort mean. Behavioral observations were categorized,
aligned by the day of ovulation for each individual, then
reported as cumulative daily or weekly data for the cohort.
Multivariate analysis of variance and repeated-measures
statistical procedures were used to analyze endocrine hormone
profiles and detect differences between study groups, and
between successive weeks (Statistical Analysis System, SAS,
version 8.2; SAS Institute Inc., Cary, North Carolina). Pearson
correlation coefficient and multiple regression procedures
were used to analyze relationships between hormones and
behavior, and between hormones and vaginal cytology. Unless
otherwise noted, we assumed a level of statistical significance
to be ,0.05.
RESULTS
Behavior.—Females in this captive population were natu-
rally synchronized; each individual began behavioral estrus
within a 4-week period, mid-January to mid-February. Before
estrus, intrapair activity consisted of courtship behavior
including allogrooming, play-wrestling, play-chases, body-
bumps, and hip-pushes (Fig. 1). Olfactory sampling (i.e.,
anogenital and inguinal sniff or lick) by both the male and
female, as well as mate guarding, also increased during pro-
estrus. Males began mounting attempts; however, the overtures
were usually rejected by the female using gentle admonition,
aggressive rebuff, or passive avoidance tactics (such as sitting,
lying down, or running away).
Mounting attempts became more frequent, and the 1st cop-
ulatory tie marked the start of estrus. In contrast to the response
observed in proestrus, females were tolerant and receptive to
the males’ mounting attempts in estrus, and often the female
would solicit attention by positioning herself in front of the
male and diverting her tail. Also, an increased frequency of
mounting was expected during estrus because this was the
antecedent posture to copulation, and it was common for a male
to mount and dismount several times before achieving
intromission and coital lock.
Copulatory ties generally lasted 5–45 min, with ties occurring
early in estrus lasting longer than those observed later toward
the end. The earliest a copulatory tie was observed during this
study was on day 9, and the latest on day 15; however, 98.4%
(179/182) of all ties occurred between day 8 and day 10. At
June 2008 657CARLSON AND GESE—REPRODUCTIVE BIOLOGY OF THE COYOTE
the individual level, behavioral estrus lasted (mean 6SD) 7.6 6
6.0 days, with 59.4% (19/32) of pairs of coyotes beginning
estrus before ovulation (day 2.2 63.9 days).
During preovulatory estrus, physical contact such as body-
bumps and hip-pushes continued to rise. Mate guarding
postures such as a stand-over within view of neighbors and
shadowing became more frequent. Olfactory sampling (male
and female) increased almost 3-fold; specifically, vaginal sniff
or lick by males doubled on day 6 from the previous day,
continued to increase, then peaked on the estimated day of
ovulation.
Immediately after the periovulatory pulse of activity there
was a brief quiescence before the sexual activities of the cohort
pairs peaked again on day 5, particularly copulations (Fig. 1).
Sixty percent (203/341) of mounts and 65% (119/182) of ties
occurred during postovulation estrus. When pairs of coyotes
were observed in .1 tie per day, the multiple ties most fre-
quently occurred on days 4–6. Mate guarding also showed
a postovulatory surge on days 5–6. Mounting attempts peaked
on day 7 although this was associated with a decline in the
number of successful copulations.
As estrus waned, females began to reject some (but not yet
all) of the males’ attempts to copulate. Physical bodily contact
remained high in late estrus, but play behavior was only
sporadically observed until the transition into diestrus. On day
11 postovulation, sexual activities abruptly declined and obser-
vation of copulatory ties (2/182), mounting attempts (4/341),
olfactory sampling (14/667), or mate guarding (2/163) were
relatively rare (1.1%, 1.2%, 2.1%, and 1.2%, respectively)
during the next 10 days (Fig. 1). In 2001 a pair was observed in
a single tie 17 days after the previously recorded copulation;
similarly in 2002, another pair tied 15 days after their previous
mating. Back-calculation from parturition suggested an earlier
date of fertilization in both cases, and because neither copula-
tion was associated with other sexual behaviors (the tie lasted
,2 min), these events were considered to have had some other
unexplained intrapair social function rather than a sign of
extended or split estrus.
Although there was a dearth of sexual activity, the coyotes
in diestrus continued to engage in physical contact such as
allogrooming, body-rubs, play-wrestling, and chasing. In addi-
tion, a previously unseen behavior emerged, begging (Fig. 1).
Beginning on day 10 and continuing through the end of the
observations, females were periodically observed in a juvenile-
like submissive behavior that sometimes provoked regurgita-
tion of food by the male. Specifically, the female would
approach her mate with her tail held low and wagging, then she
would lick his mouth or gently bite his lower jaw, matching his
movement if he tried to turn away. Sometimes the male would
admonish the female and escape. At other times, however, the
female would cease the behavior and appear to be eating off the
ground. In 7 of 55 cases line of sight allowed the observer to
confirm that the food being consumed by the female had just
been regurgitated by the male.
Reproductive hormones.—During proestrus and estrus,
spontaneous up-regulation of ovarian steroid synthesis was
evident in changes in peripheral blood levels of estradiol and
progesterone, regardless of a coyote’s social status (Fig. 2).
Sequestered females (Fig. 2A) experienced a pattern of steroid
expression (estradiol followed by progesterone) and periodicity
similar to that of mated females (Fig. 2B), and distinction
between groups was not discernible until pregnancy was
established.
In both study groups of coyotes, fluctuating serum estradiol
levels generally increased during proestrus (Fig. 3), and an
interweek comparison, week 3 to week 2, suggested a
significant incremental rise in mean values (F¼20.93, d.f. ¼
1, 8, P¼0.002). Also during this time, the rate of change
among females residing with their mates appeared different in
contrast to the sequestered females (F¼7.84, d.f. ¼1, 8, P¼
0.023), although the between-group weekly means remained
statistically indistinct (week 3, P¼0.917; week 2, P¼
0.245). In estrus, weekly preovulation (week 1) estradiol
levels peaked at (mean 6SD) 57.1 616.3 pg/ml (n¼5)
among mated females (pregnant cohort) and 44.2 621.1 pg/ml
(n¼5) in the nonmated (nonpregnant cohort), whereas
postovulation (week 1) levels subsequently declined in both
groups.
Estradiol levels continued to fall from late estrus to early
diestrus (week 1 to week 2). Although the decremental change
was similar between groups (F¼0.07, d.f. ¼1, 8, P¼0.801),
comparison of the between-group mean difference was border-
line (week 2, Pjtj
0.05(2),8
2.16 ¼0.063, F¼4.45, d.f. ¼4, 4).
From week 2 to week 3, however, the study groups demon-
strated a notable divergence (F¼6.41, d.f. ¼1, 8, P¼0.035)
in estradiol levels. Specifically, the pregnant cohort experi-
enced a transient spike in week 3 (38.9 615.0 pg/ml, n¼5)
that was different (Pjtj
0.05(2),8
2.70 ¼0.027, F¼1.80, d.f. ¼
4, 4) from the nonpregnant group (16.2 611.2 pg/ml, n¼5;
Fig. 3). Nevertheless, serum estradiol levels continued to fall in
both cohort groups, and fluctuations appeared to dampen as
pregnant females approached parturition and nonpregnant
females entered anestrus (Fig. 3).
FIG.1.—Social and mating behaviors of coyotes (Canis latrans)
shown as daily cumulative data (n¼1,757) from 32 mated pairs during
breeding seasons in 2000–2003. Observations represent 21 days before
and after the estimated day of ovulation (day 0 on the chart). Behavioral
estrus ranged from day 8 to day 10 (as shown by the solid bar).
658 JOURNAL OF MAMMALOGY Vol. 89, No. 3
Although estradiol was the predominant ovarian hormone in
proestrus, progesterone synthesis was detectable in female
coyotes during this period (Fig. 4). Immediately before estrus,
the incremental change in mean progesterone levels, from week
2 to week 1, was significant (F¼6.24, d.f. ¼1, 14, P¼
0.026). Furthermore, while the most notable change observed
was periovulation (week 1 to week 1: F¼27.94, d.f. ¼1, 14,
P,0.001), successive weekly levels rose or fell significantly
(P,0.05) from week 2 through week 7; exceptions were
week 1 to week 2 (F¼3.22, d.f. ¼1, 14, P¼0.094), and week
3 to week 4 (F¼0.93, d.f. ¼1, 14, P¼0.352).
During estrus, progesterone levels in the mated females
(pregnant cohort) rose from (weekly mean 6SD) 58.9 667.5
ng/ml (n¼7) to 89.3 694.4 ng/ml (n¼8), whereas levels in
the nonmated females rose from 18.5 623.2 ng/ml (n¼8) to
74.1 645.2 ng/ml (n¼8). However, the mean levels were not
different between groups (week 1, P¼0.179; week 1, P¼
0.208; and week 2, P¼0.687). In fact, no overall effect of
status was detected throughout the study period (week 2to
week 7: F¼2.34, d.f. ¼9, 6, P¼0.157), possibly because
of the degree of individual variability observed among the
coyotes. For example; among females in estrus and residing
with their mates (pregnant cohort), the progesterone minimum,
maximum, and CV, respectively, were: week 1¼2.8 ng/ml,
181.4 ng/ml, 1.1; week 1 ¼6.7 ng/ml, 266.5 ng/ml, 1.0; and
week 2 ¼10.3 ng/ml, 305.5 ng/ml, 1.1. Among sequestered
females in estrus (nonpregnant cohort) the same hormone
parameters were: week 1: 5.4 ng/ml, 75.1 ng/ml, 1.3; week 1:
13.3 ng/ml, 108.9 ng/ml, 0.7; and week 2: 15.8 ng/ml, 147.5
ng/ml, 0.6.
Regardless of the variation, the secretion pattern of pro-
gesterone was generally consistent between study groups.
Progesterone levels (mean 6SD) peaked between week 3
(pregnant, 104.6 697.8 ng/ml, n¼7) and week 4
(nonpregnant, 85.0 657.8 ng/ml, n¼8) in pregnancy and
diestrus. Subsequently, levels declined in both groups. The
pregnant cohort appeared to experience a transient surge in
week 7, but the groups remained statistically indistinct until
parturition and the end of sample collection (Fig. 4).
In contrast to progesterone, there was a distinctive overall
effect of status (F¼6.03, d.f. ¼6, 6, P¼0.023) on coyote
prolactin blood levels. During early pregnancy and diestrus,
week 2 through week 4, mean prolactin levels did not markedly
change within either cohort group; however, subsequent weeks
showed a pronounced elevation among pregnant females
(Fig. 5). Specifically, a significant change occurred between
weeks 4 and 5, both in mean prolactin levels (F¼41.26, d.f. ¼
1, 11, P,0.001) and intercohort rate of change (F¼13.34,
d.f. ¼1, 11, P¼0.004). Levels in pregnant coyotes rose from
24.8 64.5 ng/ml (week 4, n¼5) to 33.0 67.8 ng/ml (week 5,
n¼5), whereas among nonpregnant coyotes prolactin
FIG.2.—Temporal relationship of weekly mean blood levels of
estradiol (pg/ml), progesterone (ng/ml), prolactin (ng/ml), and relaxin
(exp(optical density) 10; for graphical representation only), aligned
to the estimated day of ovulation (day 0). A) Pseudopregnant females;
B) pregnant coyotes. Solid arrow indicates ovulation. Columns
indicate weekly number of copulatory ties observed during the
breeding seasons in 2000–2003; dashed arrow indicates day of
parturition (day 64 postovulation). Solid bars indicate phases of
ovarian cycle studied.
FIG.3.—Weekly mean (6SD) serum levels of estradiol (pg/ml)
from 5 pregnant (n¼117) and 5 nonpregnant (n¼288) coyotes
during the breeding seasons in 2000–2002, aligned to the estimated
day of ovulation (day 0). An asterisk (*) indicates statistical difference
detected between study groups. Missing columns are weeks for which
there were insufficient data available.
June 2008 659CARLSON AND GESE—REPRODUCTIVE BIOLOGY OF THE COYOTE
increased from 19.6 65.5 ng/ml (week 4, n¼8) to 21.8 66.2
ng/ml (week 5, n¼8). Thereafter, prolactin levels remained
elevated throughout pregnancy, parturition, and the 1st week of
lactation in those coyotes observed with live pups. Non-
pregnant females, meanwhile, continued to synthesize prolactin
but at lower levels (P0.006).
Relaxin was detectable in plasma of pregnant coyotes within
4 weeks after ovulation (Fig. 5); specifically, 10 of 11 pregnant
coyotes tested positive (optical density .0.050) on day 27
postovulation, and 20 of 21 females with pups were positive on
days 28–30. By comparison, relaxin was not detected (optical
density ,0.033) in samples collected from 2 females residing
with castrated mates, 7 nonmated females, or 8 male coyotes.
In addition, from week 5 through parturition, relaxin remained
detectable in all samples collected from pregnant females; and
although absorbance intensity weakened, postpartum females
did not revert to negative until several weeks after whelping.
Vaginal exfoliative cytology.— A serosanguineous discharge
from the vagina was not always apparent upon gross exami-
nation of a female coyote in proestrus, but red blood cells were
typically observed microscopically when the vaginal smear
was examined (Fig. 6A). Epithelial cells on these smears
were primarily of parabasal and intermediate cell types but
gradually, as the female progressed through proestrus, the ex-
foliated epithelial cells appeared larger and samples presented
as admixtures of parabasal, small and large intermediate, and
superficial epithelial cells (Fig. 7). Red blood cells, amorphous
debris, and mucus remained grossly obvious throughout
proestrus (Fig. 8). However, leukocytes were only occasionally
seen on smears from this stage, and their occurrence was likely
due to secondary passage with the high number of red blood
cells rather than by diapedesis.
A coyote’s vulva appeared turgid but relaxed in estrus, and
passing a swab into the vagina for collection of exudate
was easier than in proestrus. On vaginal smears, superficial
epithelial cells were the predominant cell type from approxi-
mately day 4 through ovulation to day 7; these cells were
either keratinized (anuclear) or retained pyknotic nuclei
(Fig. 7). Concurrently, appearance of red blood cells, mucus,
and amorphous debris were diminished, and white blood cells
were rarely seen (Fig. 8). Thus, the appearance of superficial
cells (nuclear and anuclear) against a clear background repre-
sented the characteristic vaginal smear during estrus in the
coyotes (Fig. 6B), particularly after ovulation.
Spermatozoa were sometimes viewed in the vaginal smears
(Fig. 6B), confirming that mating had occurred, but they were
an unpredictable and erratic element during estrus. In several
circumstances spermatozoa were not recovered although the
females were known to be actively breeding. Sperm deposition
in the coyote was assumed to be transcervical, as in the
domestic dog, and if true, would thus explain the inconsistent
findings. Nevertheless, among those samples that did contain
spermatozoa, most were collected during the period of frequent
copulations, days 3–6 postovulation.
Toward the end of estrus (days 7–10), vaginal epithelial cells
regressed to intermediate forms; eventually returning to a pre-
ponderance of parabasal cells as the coyotes entered diestrus
(Fig. 7). Also during diestrus, red blood cells, amorphous
debris, and mucus once again became abundant (Fig. 8). Most
notable in this phase, however, was the reappearance and dis-
proportionate number of leukocytes (Fig. 6C), particularly in
relationship to the number of red blood cells (Fig. 8). Thereafter,
a fluctuating mix of blood cells and cellular debris persisted into
anestrus. We observed no discernible difference between the
vaginal smears from pregnant and pseudopregnant coyotes.
Interrelationships among behavior, endocrine patterns, and
vaginal cytology.—During proestrus, significant relationships
between olfactory sampling and other behaviors were detected.
FIG.4.—Weekly mean (6SD) serum progesterone (ng/ml) levels
from 8 pregnant (n¼245) and 8 nonpregnant (n¼482) coyotes
during the breeding seasons in 2000–2002, aligned to the estimated
day of ovulation (day 0). No intergroup statistical difference was
detected. Missing columns are weeks for which there were insufficient
data available.
FIG.5.—Weekly mean (6SD) serum levels of prolactin (ng/ml)
from 5 pregnant (n¼85) and 8 nonpregnant (n¼120) female coyotes
during the breeding seasons in 2000–2002. Data are aligned to the
estimated day of ovulation (day 0). An asterisk (*) indicates statistical
difference detected between study groups. Also shown are plasma
relaxin (OD ¼optical density) readings from 82 pregnant coyotes
(n¼209); seasons 2000–2003. Relaxin was ,0.100 OD for
nonpregnant coyotes (data not shown).
660 JOURNAL OF MAMMALOGY Vol. 89, No. 3
Specifically, there was a strong positive correlation between
olfactory sampling and mate-guarding (r¼0.739, P¼0.0039).
Also, relationships between olfactory sampling and mounting
attempts (r¼0.581, P¼0.0374), as well as with courtship
(r¼0.567, P¼0.0432), suggested that olfactory sampling
may stimulate other behavior, or inform coyotes (both male and
female) of their mate’s physiological status.
Eighty-four percent (558/667) of olfactory investigations
occurred during estrus; with the apparent peak in activity on
day 3 (56 events) reflecting an increase in vulva sniff or
lick and female solicitations. Meanwhile, 88% (143/163) of
mate-guarding events also occurred during estrus, and as in
proestrus, mate-guarding and olfactory sampling maintained
a positive relationship (r¼0.550, P¼0.0146).
Twenty-three percent (42/182) of all copulatory ties occurred
between day 8 and day 1; a time when progesterone syn-
thesis was increasing and estradiol was reaching its preovula-
tion peak on day 4 (Fig. 9). Throughout estrus, however,
copulatory ties appeared to have a significant relationship with
progesterone (r¼0.554, P¼0.0139) yet a poor correlation
with estradiol (r¼0.084, P¼0.7324). Furthermore, an
initial spike in the daily number of ties (18 events) was
observed on the estimated day of ovulation, a day when mean
progesterone levels experienced the greatest incremental
FIG.6.—Representative examples of exfoliated epithelium and
other inclusions commonly seen on a coyote vaginal smear (480).
A) Proestrus, dotted arrow indicates a parabasal cell, dashed arrow
FIG.7.—Relative proportion of exfoliated vaginal epithelial cells
viewed on vaginal smears (n¼133) collected weekly from 18 coyotes
during the breeding seasons in 2000–2002. Data are aligned to the
estimated day of ovulation (day 0) including 5 weeks preovulation to
6 weeks postovulation. Kerat Superf ¼keratinized (anuclear) super-
ficial, Superf ¼nucleated superficial, Lg Intrmd ¼large intermediate,
Sm Intrmd ¼small intermediate, Parabasal ¼parabasal cells.
indicates an intermediate cell, and solid arrow indicates a red blood
cell. Amorphous debris and mucus is conspicuous in the background.
B) Estrus, superficial cells with pyknotic nuclei and spermatozoa are
shown; note the relatively clear background as compared to A and C.
C) Diestrus, dashed arrows indicate intact white blood cells
(neutrophilic leukocytes). Also, parabasal and intermediate epithelial
cells have reemerged, and red blood cells, mucus, and amorphous
debris are abundant.
June 2008 661CARLSON AND GESE—REPRODUCTIVE BIOLOGY OF THE COYOTE
change (CV
(day 1 to day 0)
¼0.275); and another major peak in
copulations (23 events, day 5) immediately followed a second-
ary surge in progesterone (CV
(day 3 to day 4)
¼0.247; Fig. 9).
We noted peaks in mate-guarding (11–13 events per day) on
day 5, day 2 through day 1, and day 5; days immediately
adjacent to peaks in steroid hormone activity and sexual
behavior. However, although mate-guarding and copulatory
ties appeared to be positively correlated (r¼0.521, P¼
0.0221), mate-guarding did not appear to be statistically related
to estradiol (r¼0.248, P¼0.3054) or progesterone (r¼
0.032, P¼0.8964) levels, either singly or as covariables
(
Adj
R
2
¼0.047, P¼0.5629).
As estradiol levels diminished, superficial cells disappeared
(Fig. 10), and a strong association was detected between
estradiol blood levels and the appearance of superficial cells
(r¼0.897, P¼0.0004). Although this evidence suggests
that vaginal smears might be used as a surrogate measure of
breeding condition, defining the specific day of ovulation was
not possible and changes in cytology did not appear to be
correlated with progesterone levels (r¼0.340, P¼0.3358).
DISCUSSION
This study describes for the coyote the secretion pattern and
temporal relationship between estradiol, progesterone, pro-
lactin, and relaxin, from late proestrus through estrus and
diestrus; mating behaviors observed during those same estrous
periods; and changes in vaginal cytology. Furthermore, it
compares reproductive endocrine patterns of females residing
with their mates with those of females sequestered during
estrus, thereby providing contrasting profiles of pregnancy and
pseudopregnancy in the coyote. Graphic and statistical
comparisons also provided insight into relationships between
various elements of coyote reproduction, emphasizing the
importance of viewing reproductive biology as a spectrum and
integration of physiology and behavior.
Proestrus.—Proestrus appears to be a crucial period of
preparation and staging as levels of estradiol and progesterone
rise coincidentally with alterations in coyote behavior and
reproductive tissue. Appearance of red cells in vaginal exudate,
and transformation of epithelial cells, suggested progressive
remodeling of the reproductive tract for the ensuing rigors
of mating and pregnancy. Meanwhile, physical and social
interactions between pair-mates increased, as well as defensive
displays. Positive relationships between olfactory sampling and
other behaviors suggested a possible mechanism by which
either sex might assess the physiological status of their mate,
FIG. 10.—Weekly mean blood levels of estradiol (pg/ml) and
progesterone (ng/ml) and the relative proportion of epithelial cells
observed on vaginal smears from coyotes during the breeding seasons
in 2000–2002, 4 weeks preovulation to 6 weeks postovulation. Data
are aligned to the estimated day of ovulation.
FIG.8.—Inclusions other than epithelial cells viewed on weekly
vaginal smears (n¼133) from 18 coyotes during the breeding seasons
in 2000–2002. RBC ¼red blood cells, WBC ¼white blood cells,
Amorph ¼amorphous debris, Mucus ¼mucus, and Sperm ¼
spermatozoa. Data are aligned to the estimated day of ovulation
(day 0) including 5 weeks preovulation to 6 weeks postovulation.
FIG.9.—Daily mean blood levels of estradiol (pg/ml) and
progesterone (ng/ml) overlaying sexually specific behaviors in coyote
pairs (olfactory sampling, mounts, and copulatory ties) observed
during the breeding seasons in 2000–2003. Data are aligned to the
estimated day of ovulation (day 0).
662 JOURNAL OF MAMMALOGY Vol. 89, No. 3
and ritualized agonistic behaviors may reinforce the pair-bond
or inform a coyote about its mate’s readiness to breed, defend
territory, or provide parental care.
Estrus.—As previously noted, the majority of copulations
occurred after ovulation, suggesting that the coyotes may have
concentrated their reproductive effort to coincide with the
optimal time for viable sperm to encounter mature ova. Yet
interestingly, most 1st ties were observed before ovulation.
Because coyotes ovulate spontaneously, there is no apparent
physiological function that can be ascribed to exceptionally
early copulations (although spermatozoa can survive for several
days within the female reproductive tract). We also observed
exceptionally late copulations (albeit with very short ties).
It is unclear if such sexual behavior exists among free-
roaming coyotes; if so, it would presumably be costly because
a pair would be especially vulnerable to predation while in
a coital lock. But possible social and future benefits gained
through employment of multiple copulations include mate
fidelity, guaranteed paternity, advertisement of a confirmed
pair-bond (thus discouraging competitors), or assured access to
resources (Gier 1975; Hunter et al. 1993; Sillero-Zubiri et al.
1996). The influence of steroid hormones on preovulatory
copulations also cannot be ruled out. Progesterone levels in
coyotes began rising before ovulation and were positively
correlated with copulations. Furthermore, estradiol followed by
progesterone has been linked to expression of sexual behavior
in the domestic dog (Concannon et al. 1979). Therefore, the
preovulation ascendancy of progesterone described in this
study may suggest a possible physiological stimulus for
seemingly premature copulations among coyotes.
Ironically, mate-guarding appeared correlated to copulatory
ties, but not to either steroid. However, these results are not
surprising when one considers the male’s role in mating
behavior (and the influence testosterone certainly exerts during
the breeding season). Regardless, the association between
rising progesterone levels and overt sexual behavior reflects the
female coyote’s sexual determination. A male may shadow
a female, investigate her anogenital scent, play, groom her, et
cetera, but copulations will not occur without her permission
and explicit cooperation.
Cytomorphosis of vaginal epithelium to keratinized or
nucleated superficial cells was transient, but correlated with
estradiol levels. Predominance of superficial cells was most
pronounced after the preovulatory peak in estradiol, and per-
sisted until estradiol was withdrawn in late estrus. Meanwhile,
other cellular inclusions were conspicuously rare or absent.
In cases when mating behavior is unobservable or protracted,
vaginal smears may serve as a qualitative assessment of estra-
diol synthesis, thereby predicting a period of receptivity, or
optimal fertility.
Diestrus.—Estradiol secretion generally dampened during
diestrus although pregnant coyotes experienced an estradiol
pulse during week 3—the period in which implantation occurs
(Gier 1968). Meanwhile, progesterone levels were indistin-
guishable between pregnant coyotes and nonpregnant females.
However, prolactin and relaxin emerged as salient hormones
in diestrus, differentiating pregnancy from pseudopregnancy.
Relaxin was pregnancy specific and became detectable in
week 4, followed shortly thereafter by an increase in prolactin.
Prolactin levels in pregnant coyotes then diverged from
pseudopregnant females in week 5. Both hormones remained
elevated thereafter until parturition, persisting for several weeks
after the pups were born. It has been hypothesized (but not yet
established) that relaxin may serve as a signal between embryo
and mother, possibly stimulating prolactin synthesis, which in
turn facilitates the persistence of progesterone (required for
the maintenance of pregnancy in canines—Concannon et al.
2001). But although the temporal associations between relaxin,
prolactin, and progesterone are suggestive in this data set,
under the conditions of this study we were unable establish a
definitive link in the coyote.
As sexual behavior waned, food-begging immediately
appeared in diestrus. Episodes of begging, and sometimes
regurgitation, were seen throughout pregnancy with greater
intensity occurring during weeks 4 and 5. Interestingly,
begging was not restricted to pregnant females. In a tangent
study, pseudopregnant females who remained housed with their
mates throughout the breeding season also were observed
begging food (and receiving regurgitate) from the males
(Carlson 2008).
In conclusion, this study showed that key elements of
reproductive behavior, endocrine profiles, and vaginal cytol-
ogy are discernible in coyotes. Biologists studying wild or
captive populations might find it helpful to focus on selected
criteria when assessing a breeding population. The value of
seeing a pair in a copulatory tie, viewing spermatozoa on
a vaginal smear, or detecting pups in a den are obvious.
However, in the absence of such indicators, investigators might
depend instead upon the observation of affinitive and
appetitive behaviors in coyotes. When blood samples can be
obtained, measurement of blood hormone levels can provide
evidence of ovulation, and distinguish pregnancy from
pseudopregnancy. Also, the relative prevalence of exfoliated
epithelial cells, red cells, leukocytes, and other inclusions on
a vaginal smear can help predict the breeding status or
reproductive potential of an animal. However, the use of
several of these elements together will increase the overall
confidence in determining the reproductive status of an
individual, or population of coyotes.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We thank T. D. Bunch, F. F. Knowlton, R. T. Skirpstunas, M. L.
Wolfe, and 2 anonymous reviewers for their insightful comments on
an earlier version of this manuscript and their valuable suggestions
for improvement. Thanks also to D. A. Wannemacher. We appreciate
the following National Wildlife Research Center staff as well as
undergraduate and graduate students at Utah State University for
assistance in the handling and care of the study animals, behavior
observations, and specimen collections: K. Anderson, R. Bartel, S.
Brummer, K. Casper, P. Darrow, R. Harrison, J. Hedelius, D. Jones, R.
Kikkert, S. Kirshner, L. Minter, H. Phillips, J. Robinson, A. Seglund,
H. Smith, J. Tegt, K. Wenning, M. Wollbrink, and D. Zemlicka. We
are grateful to T. J. DeLiberto for arranging the funding and logistical
support provided by United States Department of Agriculture, Animal
June 2008 663CARLSON AND GESE—REPRODUCTIVE BIOLOGY OF THE COYOTE
and Plant Health Inspection Service, Wildlife Services, National
Wildlife Research Center, Logan Field Station, Utah State University,
Logan, Utah.
LITERATURE CITED
ANDELT, W. F. 1985. Behavioral ecology of coyotes in south Texas.
Wildlife Monographs 94:1–45.
ASA, C. S., AND C. VALDESPINO. 1998. Canid reproductive biology: an
integration of proximate mechanisms and ultimate causes. Amer-
ican Zoologist 38:251–259.
BEKOFF, M., AND J. DIAMOND. 1976. Precopulatory and copulatory
behavior in coyotes. Journal of Mammalogy 57:372–375.
BEKOFF, M., AND M. C. WELLS. 1986. Social ecology and behavior of
coyotes. Advances in the Study of Behavior 16:251–338.
BRADLEY, G. M., AND E. S. BENSON. 1974. Examination of the urine.
15th ed. Pp. 15–83 in Clinical diagnosis by laboratory methods (I.
Davidsohn and J. B. Henry, eds.). W. B. Saunders Company,
Philadelphia, Pennsylvania.
BROMLEY, C., AND E. M. GESE. 2001. Effects of sterilization on
territory fidelity and maintenance, pair bonds, and survival rates of
free-ranging coyotes. Canadian Journal of Zoology 79:386–392.
CAMENZIND, F. J. 1978. Behavioral ecology of coyotes on the National
Elk Refuge, Jackson, Wyoming. Pp. 267–294 in Coyotes: biology,
behavior, and management (M. Bekoff, ed.). Academic Press, Inc.,
New York.
CARLSON, D. A. 2008. Reproductive biology of the coyote (Canis
latrans): integration of behavior and physiology. Ph.D. dissertation,
Utah State University, Logan.
CARLSON, D. A., AND E. M. GESE. 2007. Relaxin as a diagnostic tool
for pregnancy in the coyote. Animal Reproduction Science
101:304–312.
CHRISTIE, D. W., J. B. BAILEY,AND E. T. BELL. 1972. Classification of
cell types in vaginal smears during the canine oestrous cycle. British
Veterinary Journal 128:301–310.
CONCANNON, P. W., N. WEIGAND,S.WILSON,AND W. HANSEL. 1979.
Sexual behavior in ovariectomized bitches in response to estrogen
and progesterone treatments. Biology of Reproduction 20:799–809.
CONCANNON, P. W., J. P. MCCANN,AND M. TEMPLE. 1989. Biology and
endocrinology of ovulation, pregnancy and parturition in the dog.
Journal of Reproduction and Fertility, Supplement 39:3–25.
CONCANNON, P., T. TSUTSUI,AND V. SHILLE. 2001. Embryo de-
velopment, hormonal requirements and maternal responses during
canine pregnancy. Journal of Reproduction and Fertility, Supple-
ment 57:169–179.
FELDMAN,E.C.,AND R. W. NELSON. 2004. Canine and feline
endocrinology and reproduction. 3rd ed. Saunders, St. Louis, Missouri.
GANNON,W.L.,R.S.SIKES,AND THE ANIMAL CARE AND USE
COMMITTEE OF THE AMERICAN SOCIETY OF MAMMALOGISTS. 2007.
Guidelines of the American Society of Mammalogists for the use of
wild mammals in research. Journal of Mammalogy 88:809–823.
GESE, E. M. 1990. Reproductive activity in an old-age coyote in
southeastern Colorado. Southwestern Naturalist 35:101–102.
GESE, E. M. 2001. Territorial defense by coyotes (Canis latrans)in
Yellowstone National Park, Wyoming: who, how, where, when,
and why. Canadian Journal of Zoology 79:980–987.
GESE, E. M., O. J. RONGSTAD,AND W. R. MYTTON. 1989. Population
dynamics of coyotes in southeastern Colorado. Journal of Wildlife
Management 53:174–181.
GESE, E. M., R. L. RUFF,AND R. L. CRABTREE. 1996. Foraging ecology
of coyotes (Canis latrans): the influence of extrinsic factors and
a dominance hierarchy. Canadian Journal of Zoology 74:769–783.
GIER, H. T. 1968. Coyotes in Kansas. Agricultural Experiment Station,
Kansas State University, Agriculture and Applied Science,
Manhattan.
GIER, H. T. 1975. Ecology and behavior of the coyote (Canis latrans).
Pp. 247–262 in The wild canids (M. W. Fox, ed.). Van Nostrand
Reinhold Company, New York.
GOLANI, I., AND H. MENDELSSOHN. 1971. Sequences of precopulatory
behavior of the jackal (Canis aureus L.). Behaviour 38:169–192.
GREEN, J. S., F. F. KNOWLTON,AND W. C. PITT. 2002. Reproduction in
captive wild-caught coyotes (Canis latrans). Journal of Mammal-
ogy 83:501–506.
HAMLETT, G. W. D. 1938. The reproductive cycle of the coyote.
United States Department of Agriculture, Technical Bulletin
616:1–11.
HATIER, K. G. 1995. Effects of helping behaviors on coyote packs in
Yellowstone National Park, Wyoming. M.S. thesis, Montana State
University, Bozeman.
HODGES, C. M. 1990. The reproductive biology of the coyote (Canis
latrans). Ph.D. dissertation, Texas A&M University, College
Station.
HUNTER, F. M., M. PETRIE,M.OTRONEN ,T.BIRKHEAD,AND A. P.
MØLLER. 1993. Why do females copulate repeatedly with one male?
Trends in Ecology and Evolution 8:21–26.
KENNELLY, J. J., AND B. E. JOHNS. 1976. The estrous cycle of coyotes.
Journal of Wildlife Management 40:272–277.
KENNELLY, J. J., B. E. JOHNS,C.P.BREIDENSTEIN,AND J. D. ROBERTS.
1977. Predicting female coyote breeding dates from fetal measure-
ments. Journal of Wildlife Management 41:746–750.
KLEIMAN, D. G., AND J. F. EISENBERG. 1973. Comparisons of canid
and felid social systems from an evolutionary perspective. Animal
Behaviour 21:637–659.
KNOWLTON, F. F. 1972. Preliminary interpretations of coyote
population mechanics with some management implications. Journal
of Wildlife Management 36:369–382.
MENGEL, R. M. 1971. A study of dog–coyote hybrids and implications
concerning hybridization in Canis. Journal of Mammalogy 52:
316–336.
SACKS, B. N. 2005. Reproduction and body condition of California
coyotes (Canis latrans). Journal of Mammalogy 86:1036–1041.
SCHENKEL, R. 1967. Submission: its features and functions in the wolf
and dog. American Zoologist 7:305–381.
SILLERO-ZUBIRI, C., D. GOTTELLI,AND D. W. MACDONALD. 1996. Male
philopatry, extra-pack copulations and inbreeding avoidance in
Ethiopian wolves (Canis simensis). Behavioral Ecology and
Sociobiology 38:331–340.
SILVER, H., AND W. T. SILVER. 1969. Growth and behavior of the
coyote-like canid of northern New England with observations on
canid hybrids. Wildlife Monographs 17:1–41.
STELLFLUG, J. N., P. D. MUSE,D.O.EVERSON,AND T. M. LOUIS. 1981.
Changes in serum progesterone and estrogen of the nonpregnant
coyote during the breeding season. Proceedings of the Society for
Experimental Biology and Medicine 167:220–223.
TSUTSUI, T. 1989. Gamete physiology and timing of ovulation and
fertilization in dogs. Journal of Reproduction and Fertility,
Supplement 39:269–275.
WINDBERG, L. A. 1995. Demography of a high-density coyote
population. Canadian Journal of Zoology 73:942–954.
Submitted 16 December 2006. Accepted 23 October 2007.
Associate Editor was Roger A. Powell.
664 JOURNAL OF MAMMALOGY Vol. 89, No. 3
... Coyotes are monoestrous, reproducing once per year. The coyote breeding season is from December through February (Carlson & Gese, 2008;Lord et al., 2013), but coyotes maintain pair bonds throughout the year Bekoff & Wells, 1986;Hennessy et al., 2012). During the breeding season, female coyotes exhibit increases in serum oestradiol and progesterone levels during the oestrus and dioestrus periods, respectively (Carlson & Gese, 2008), and serum testosterone levels peak in male coyotes during this same window of time (Minter & DeLiberto, 2008). ...
... The coyote breeding season is from December through February (Carlson & Gese, 2008;Lord et al., 2013), but coyotes maintain pair bonds throughout the year Bekoff & Wells, 1986;Hennessy et al., 2012). During the breeding season, female coyotes exhibit increases in serum oestradiol and progesterone levels during the oestrus and dioestrus periods, respectively (Carlson & Gese, 2008), and serum testosterone levels peak in male coyotes during this same window of time (Minter & DeLiberto, 2008). In addition to gonadal hormones, faecal cortisol metabolite levels in both male and female captive coyotes are also significantly greater in early gestation relative to late gestation (Gese et al., 2023;Schell et al., 2016). ...
... Given these trends, we predicted that pair mate proximity would be greater in the breeding season relative to the nonbreeding season, given the increase in necessarily proximal mating behaviours naturally occurring during this time. We also expected to observe increases in female and male steroid hormones in the breeding season relative to the nonbreeding season in synchrony with increases in reproductive behaviour (Carlson & Gese, 2008;Carter & Perkeybile, 2018). We anticipated that pair bond tenure would influence pair mate proximity, although how proximity varied would depend on additional external factors, as it does for coppery titi monkeys (Rothwell et al., 2020). ...
... Beyond the family unit and/or mated pair, however, prosocial behavior among coyotes is much more scarce. In fact, most of the literature regarding non-kin coyote interactions pertains entirely to territory defense, whereby male and female coyotes exhibit indirect (e. g., scent marking) and/or direct (e.g., fighting) means of warding off unfamiliar conspecifics (Gese, 2001;Gese and Ruff, 1997;Seidler and Gese, 2012), and mate guarding, whereby adult coyotes guard their mate against same-sex conspecifics (Carlson and Gese, 2008;Macdonald et al., 2019). Even more, in other monogamous species (e.g., the prairie vole; Microtus ochrogaster), adult pair-mates will even aggress against opposite-sex conspecifics as a means of actively maintaining the pair bond (Lee and Beery, 2022;Young et al., 2011). ...
... Even more, in other monogamous species (e.g., the prairie vole; Microtus ochrogaster), adult pair-mates will even aggress against opposite-sex conspecifics as a means of actively maintaining the pair bond (Lee and Beery, 2022;Young et al., 2011). Thus, given the existing evidence for the coyote's adherence to social monogamy (Carlson and Gese, 2008;Lord et al., 2013;Macdonald et al., 2019), we predicted this population of captive coyotes would also exhibit a selective social preference for a pair-mate versus a stranger, when tested in a partner preference test. ...
... In fact, coyotes are known to be largely neophobic (Mettler and Shivik, 2007;Parsons et al., 2022). Therefore, at this point in the PPT, focal coyotes were likely actively working to maintain an awareness of the location of their pair-mate versus an unwelcome intruder (i.e., stranger) in order to retain their territory (Carlson and Gese, 2008;Kleiman, 1977), and perhaps also to guard their mate against that stranger (Bales et al., 2021;Macdonald et al., 2019). Accordingly, during this same window of time (observations 2 and 3), the percentage of partner-directed affiliation was greater than chance. ...
Article
Social monogamy is a unique social system exhibited by only 3-5 percent of mammalian taxa; however, all wild canid species exhibit this social system. Despite the high prevalence of social monogamy among canids, little is known about how they form selective social attachment relationships among non-kin. Thus, we aimed to quantify monogamous behavior in a highly ubiquitous canid, the coyote (Canis latrans). We adapted the three-chambered partner preference test, which was originally developed for prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster), to assess social preference in mated pairs of captive coyotes at the USDA Predator Research Facility. We quantified monogamy-related behaviors, such as time spent in spatial proximity to a pair-mate versus a stranger. Our behavioral ethogram also included visual seeking, olfactory investigations, ears down, scent marking, and affiliative behavior. Test subjects showed significantly greater affiliative behavior toward their partner than toward a stranger. However, there was extremely high variability both within and between coyote pairs across behavioral measures. These data suggest the need for larger sample sizes when working with species with high individual variability, as well as the need for species- and facility-specific modifications to this testing paradigm and/or ethogram to better adapt it from its laboratory and rodent-based origins.
... Over the course of 11 months, the amputee exhibited space use of a resident animal except between 27 Nov 2021-3 Feb 2022 in which he resided along the western and northern edges of his territory while making several brief excursions to the north into resident01's and resident02's home ranges ( Figure 1). Given his advanced age, we assumed that he may have lost his breeder position in the pack and was temporarily displaced during the breeding season (January-February) likely returning when the female breeder was pregnant and no longer in estrus (Carlson and Gese 2008 property's game preserve indicated that he did not attempt to enter the preserve. This avoidance may be due to more complex behavioral factors related to learning and exploratory behavior vis-á-vis his experience of being trapped and handled by humans (Young et al. 2022, Barrett et al. 2019. ...
Article
Full-text available
Wildlife rehabilitation is a widespread practice, but it is rarely provided for research animals in wild settings when injuries such as bone fractures occur during field work. Integrating rehabilitation and post-release monitoring with field research involving radio telemetry could improve our ability to rehabilitate wild animals by assessing the efficacy of clinical and rehabilitation techniques. While conducting a study in coastal southwestern Louisiana during 2021–2023 designed to assess coyote (Canis latrans) populations for red wolf (Canis rufus) ancestry, we severely injured a coyote in a foothold trap. Instead of humanely euthanizing the animal, we opted to provide clinical treatment which involved amputating the coyote’s injured forelimb. The three-legged coyote was released with a Global Positioning System (GPS) collar and monitored until his death. Using time local convex hulls and resource selection functions, we observed the three-legged coyote exhibiting similar movement speed and space use as did his three uninjured neighboring GPS-collared coyotes (control animals). However, the amputee coyote exhibited stronger selection for roads and open wetlands than did the control animals, indicating that the amputation may have altered his ability to traverse some land cover types such as areas with increasing vegetation cover. Although the control animals were killed by humans while attempting to enter a fenced-in game preserve, the three-legged coyote avoided entering the same preserve and was presumably killed by an American Alligator (Alligator mississippiensis) within his territory indicating that he avoided areas with high potential for human-coyote conflict. Despite the small sample size of one clinically treated animal, we overcame other common limitations to post-release monitoring studies such as a lack of detailed space use data or use of control animals by using GPS technology on a treated coyote and its neighboring coyotes. Wildlife rehabilitation can provide second chances to animals severely injured by research activities, and we suggest that clinical treatment and rehabilitation should be considered in study designs as rehabilitated animals can maintain good general health and welfare following releases and contribute to local population persistence.
... Interestingly, these concentrations were the same between pregnant and nonpregnant females. Nonpregnant female coyotes do undergo a pseudopregnancy with no differences in mating behaviors and progesterone levels between pregnant and nonpregnant females, with similarly high individual variability in reproductive hormone levels before and during breeding [54]. Whether pheromones play a role in nonpregnant coyotes exhibiting similar changes in fGCM levels as the pregnant coyotes deserve further investigation. ...
Article
Full-text available
Reproduction is considered an energetically and physiologically demanding time in the life of an animal. Changes in physiological stress are partly reflected in changes in glucocorticoid metabolites and can be measured from fecal samples. We examined levels of fecal glucocorticoid metabolites (fGCMs) in 24 captive coyotes (Canis latrans) to investigate responses to the demands of reproduction. Using 12 pairs of coyotes (five pairs produced pups, seven pairs did not), we analyzed 633 fecal samples covering 11 biological periods (e.g., breeding, gestation, and lactation). Levels of fGCMs showed high individual variability, with females having higher fGCM levels than males. The production of pups showed no statistical effect on fGCM levels among females or males. Among females, fGCM levels were highest during 4–6 weeks of gestation compared to other periods but were not significantly different between pregnant and nonpregnant females. Among males, the highest fGCM levels were during 1–3 weeks of gestation compared to other periods, but were not significantly different between males with a pregnant mate versus nonpregnant mate. Of females producing pups, litter size did not influence fGCM levels. Given that they were fed ample food throughout the year, we found that the demands of producing pups did not appear to statistically influence measures of fGCM concentrations in captive coyotes.
... However, because of the solitary nature of the maned wolf, it has evolved an important adaptation to ensure reproductive success. Most canid species studied to date are spontaneous ovulators, meaning that the female does not require a signal prompting ovulation: domestic dog (Canis familiaris) [11][12][13], gray wolf (Canis lupus) [14,15], red wolf (Canis rufus) [16], coyote (Canis latrans) [17], African wild dog (Lycaon pictus) [18,19], bush dog (Speothos venaticus) [20], red fox (Vulpes vulpes) [21,22], and arctic fox (Alopex lagopus) [23]. Intriguingly, female maned wolves ovulate only in the presence of a male [7,[24][25][26]. ...
Article
Full-text available
The maned wolf (Chrysocyon brachyurus) is an induced ovulator. Though the mechanism of ovulation induction remains unknown, it is suspected to be urinary chemical signals excreted by males. This study assessed volatile organic compounds (VOCs) in weekly urine samples across 5 months from 13 maned wolves (6 intact males, 1 neutered male, 6 females) with the goal of identifying VOCs that are differentially expressed across sex, reproductive status, and pairing status. Solid-phase microextraction (SPME) and gas chromatography-mass spectrometry (GC-MS) were used to extract and separate VOCs that were identified via spectral matching with authentic standards, with spectral libraries, or with new software that further matches molecular fragment structures with mass spectral peaks. Two VOCs were present across all 317 urine samples: 2,5-dimethyl pyrazine and 2-methyl-6-(1-propenyl)-pyrazine. Fifteen VOCs differed significantly (Adj. P < 0.001 and |log2 fold change| >2.0) between intact males and females. Using partial least squares-discriminant analysis, the compounds with the highest importance to the sex classification were delta-decalactone, delta-dodecalactone, and bis(prenyl) sulfide. Sixty-two VOCs differed between intact males and the neutered male. Important classifier compounds were 3-ethyl 2,5-dimethyl pyrazine, 2-methyl-6-(1-propenyl)-pyrazine, and tetrahydro-2-isopentyl-5-propyl furan. Several VOCs established as important here have been implicated in reproductive communication in other mammals. This study is the most robust examination of differential expression in the maned wolf thus far and provides the most comprehensive analysis of maned wolf urinary VOCs to date, increasing the sample size substantially over previous chemical communication studies in this species. New data analysis software allowed for the identification of compounds in the hormone-producing mevalonate pathway which were previously unreported in maned wolf urine. Several putative semiochemicals were identified as good candidates for behavioral bioassays to determine their role in maned wolf reproduction, and specifically in ovulation induction.
... While gonadal hormones are believed to play a role in modulating coyote behavior particularly during the breeding season as hormone levels become elevated, evidence is conflicting throughout the literature. Mating behaviors between pair-bonds were positively associated with increases in estradiol and progesterone levels in captive females during proestrus (Carlson and Gese, 2008). However, alterations in hormones of male captive coyotes using reproductive inhibitors were found to not alter behavior (Young et al., 2018), indicating potential extraneous factors influencing coyote behavior warranting further research. ...
Article
Full-text available
Coyotes (Canis latrans) involved in depredation of livestock, an act frequently resulting in human-wildlife conflict, often do so out of necessity for provisioning pups. Surgical sterilization methods such as vasectomy that preserve gonadal hormones have been successful in reducing depredation by free-ranging coyotes while allowing individuals to maintain territoriality and mate fidelity. However, use of these methods remain costly and ineffective for wide-scale use. Given the alternative proposal of using chemical sterilization techniques, we investigated whether the use of hormone-altering sterilization methods impacted behavior of captive coyote pairs (i.e., male-female pair bonds). Our objective was to evaluate behavior and reproductive hormones of mated coyote pairs that had received different surgical sterilization treatments. We assigned mated pairs of captive coyotes to different sterilization treatment groups (vasectomy, spay, neuter, ovary-sparing spay, and intact) and coded their behavior as the time spent in resting versus active (i.e., walking, running, scent communication, and aggressive interactions) behaviors. Additionally, hormone concentrations were analyzed to determine effectiveness of hormone-altering treatment, given the potential role of gonadal hormones in regulating behavior. The study was repeated across three breeding seasons. The top model comparing time spent active versus resting was the null model, although the model that included whether sterilization type altered hormones and year also had a ΔAIC of < 2.0. Testosterone concentrations between neutered and vasectomized or intact males was significantly different, indicating sterilization treatment was successful and the different sterilization techniques impact hormones differently; there were no statistical difference for estradiol or progesterone levels among female treatment groups. No sterilized pairs produced pups, but the intact pairs did. Although there are potentially some differences in behavior across sterilization treatment types, our results suggest sterilization of coyotes holds potential as a future management strategy as behavior did not differ among different treatments. Potential difference across years suggest further research is necessary to determine potential extraneous factors influencing behavior and the effect of treatment on territoriality on free-ranging coyotes.
Article
Full-text available
Large predators are known to shape the behavior and ecology of sympatric predators via conflict and competition, with mesopredators thought to avoid large predators, while dogs suppress predator activity and act as guardians of human property. However, interspecific communication between predators has not been well‐explored and this assumption of avoidance may oversimplify the responses of the species involved. We explored the acoustic activity of three closely related sympatric canids: wolves Canis lupus, coyotes Canis latrans, and dogs Canis familiaris. These species have an unbalanced triangle of risk: coyotes, as mesopredators, are at risk from both apex‐predator wolves and human‐associated dogs, while wolves fear dogs, and dogs may fear wolves as apex predators or challenge them as intruders into human‐allied spaces. We predicted that risk perception would dictate vocal response with wolves and dogs silencing coyotes as well as dogs silencing wolves. Dogs, in their protective role of guarding human property, would respond to both. Eleven passive acoustic monitoring devices were deployed across 13 nights in central Wisconsin, and we measured the responses of each species to naturally occurring heterospecific vocalizations. Against our expectation, silencing did not occur. Instead, coyotes were not silenced by either species: when hearing wolves, coyotes responded at greater than chance rates and when hearing dogs, coyotes did not produce fewer calls than chance rates. Similarly, wolves responded at above chance rates to coyotes and at chance rates when hearing dogs. Only the dogs followed our prediction and responded at above chance rates in response to both coyotes and wolves. Thus, instead of silencing their competitors, canid vocalizations elicit responses from them suggesting the existence of a complex heterospecific communication network.
Article
This datasheet on Canis latrans covers Identity, Overview, Distribution, Dispersal, Diagnosis, Biology & Ecology, Environmental Requirements, Natural Enemies, Impacts, Uses, Prevention/Control, Further Information.
Article
Objective Lactatio sine graviditate of the bitch can become clinically relevant in particularly severe manifestations. The aim of the study was to relate the hormone pattern consisting of progesterone (P4), estradiol 17β (E2) and prolactin to the time of occurrence of lactatio sine graviditate in the course of metoestrus and anoestrus as well as to its symptomatology. Material and methods Sixty-eight bitches with apparent lactatio sine graviditate were divided into 3 groups according to their cycle status. All bitches were examined for gynaecological findings. Furhtermore, their blood progesterone, oestrogen, and prolactin concentrations were determined and compared with the 133-day hormone profile of 7 control animals. Results Lactatio sine graviditate occurring in early metoestrus was characterised more by a shifted P4:E2 ratio than by hyperprolactinaemia. Overall, the prolactin concentration in the peripheral blood was significantly increased. Analysis of the individual cases revealed that hyperprolactinaemia was present to varying degrees. It could be detected in almost all bitches that showed full symptomatology at the end of metoestrus or at the beginning of anoestrus. Only then clinical signs correlated with an increased prolactin concentration in the peripheral blood. In most cases, the estradiol-17β concentration was within the reference range. Conclusion and clinical relevance The study indicates that the administration of prolactin inhibitors alone is not indicated in all cases of lactatio sine graviditate and that the timepoint of onset of the clinically relevant symptoms and the current prolactin level should be taken into account in the treatment of affected bitches.
Article
Full-text available
Resumen Los lineamientos para el uso de especies de mamíferos de vida silvestre en la investigación con base en Sikes et al. (2011) se actualizaron. Dichos lineamientos cubren técnicas y regulaciones profesionales actuales que involucran el uso de mamíferos en la investigación y enseñanza; también incorporan recursos nuevos, resúmenes de procedimientos y requisitos para reportes. Se incluyen detalles acerca de captura, marcaje, manutención en cautiverio y eutanasia de mamíferos de vida silvestre. Se recomienda que los comités institucionales de uso y cuidado animal (cifras en inglés: IACUCs), las agencias reguladoras y los investigadores se adhieran a dichos lineamientos como fuente base de protocolos que involucren mamíferos de vida silvestre, ya sea investigaciones de campo o en cautiverio. Dichos lineamientos fueron preparados y aprobados por la ASM, en consulta con profesionales veterinarios experimentados en investigaciones de vida silvestre y IACUCS, de quienes cuya experiencia colectiva provee un entendimiento amplio y exhaustivo de la biología de mamíferos no-domesticados. La presente versión de los lineamientos y modificaciones posteriores están disponibles en línea en la página web de la ASM, bajo Cuidado Animal y Comité de Uso: (http://mammalogy.org/uploads/committee_files/CurrentGuidelines.pdf). Recursos adicionales relacionados con el uso de animales de vida silvestre para la investigación se encuentran disponibles en (http://www.mammalsociety.org/committees/animal-care-and-use#tab3).
Article
Full-text available
We examined the influence of intrinsic (age, sex, and social status) and extrinsic (snow depth, snowpack hardness, temperature, available ungulate carcass biomass) factors in relation to time-activity budgets of coyotes (Canis latrans) in Yellowstone National Park, Wyoming. We observed 54 coyotes (49 residents from 5 packs, plus 5 transients) for 2507 h from January 1991 to June 1993. Snow depth, ungulate carcass biomass, and habitai type influenced the amount of time coyotes rested, travelled, hunted small mammals, and fed on carcasses. Coyotes decreased travelling and hunting and increased resting and feeding on carcasses as snow depth and available carcass biomass increased. Age and social status of the coyote influenced activity budgets. During times of deep snow and high carcass biomass, pups fed less on carcasses and hunted small mammals more than alpha and beta coyotes. Pups apparently were restricted by older pack members from feeding on a carcass. Thus, pups adopted a different foraging strategy by spending more time hunting small mammals. Coyotes spent most of their time hunting small mammals in mesic meadows and shrub-meadows, where prey densities were highest. Prey-detection rates and prey-capture rates explained 78 and 84%, respectively, of the variation in the amount of lime coyotes spent hunting small mammals in each habitat in each winter. Our findings strongly suggested that resource partitioning, as mediated by defense by older coyotes, occurred among coyote pack members in Yellowstone National Park.
Chapter
Behavioral patterns are subject to natural selection and behavior like any other attributes of an animal, which contributes to individual survival. The chapter summarizes a long-term study of coyotes that was conducted in the Grand Teton National Park, in the northwest comer of Wyoming. There is remarkable agreement in the results stemming from a limited number of field projects concerned with the social behavior and behavioral ecology of coyotes, and some general principles concerning social ecology, scent marking, predatory behavior, time budgeting, and reproductive and care-giving patterns can be developed that are applicable not only to coyotes but to many other carnivores.
Article
The estrous cycle in coyotes (Canis latrans) is characterized by a long proestrus (2-3 months). Vaginal smear patterns associated with proestrus, estrus, and metestrus are described. Data from 41 adults indicated estrus begins about 13 March and persists for 10 days. Neither onset date nor duration of estrus differed with body weight or age; however, the number of corpora lutea increased with age. Of 22 juveniles, 19 showed sexual activity but no more than 11 ovulated. In adults, ovulation apparently occurred as early as the 1st and as late as the 9th day of estrus. Left and right ovaries were similar in weight and number of corpora lutea.
Article
We captured 96 coyotes (Canis latrans) on the Piñon Canyon Maneuver Site (PCMS) in southeastern Colorado from March 1983 to December 1986. Of these, 88 (23, 18, and 59% pups, yearlings, and ad, respectively; 146 M: 100 F) were radiocollared and tracked. Home ranges of residents did not overlap, whereas transients overlapped resident and other transient home ranges. Annual survival rates for adults, yearlings, and pups were 0.87, 0.52, and 0.51, respectively. Residents, transients, and dispersers had annual survival rates of 0.87, 0.61, and 0.39, respectively. Of the 88 radio-collared coyotes, 31 died during the study; man was responsible for 81% of the mortalities. Thirteen coyotes dispersed from the study area a mean distance of 59 km. Mean litter size for 16 litters captured on the study area was 3.2 pups (138 M: 100 F). A prewhelping density of 0.29 coyote/km2 was determined for the study area. Results of our study supported the hypothesis that coyote populations may be regulated by social intolerances, as mediated by the abundance and availability of food.
Article
Crown-rump length, hind-foot length and weight of known-age coyote (Canis latrans) fetuses dated from estrus and metestrus onset were measured both fresh and after formalin fixation. Regression equations were established for predicting fetal age. All equations provide reliable estimates of age; the lowest correlation coefficient among the 12 sets of data was 0.97. Crown-rump measurements provided the best estimate of fetal age. To facilitate estimating breeding dates from field-collected specimens, a ruler-type scale was derived from the crown-rump measurements of fresh, estrus dated fetuses.