ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

The aim of this study was to obtain a greater insight into the association between vacations and happiness. We examined whether vacationers differ in happiness, compared to those not going on holiday, and if a holiday trip boosts post-trip happiness. These questions were addressed in a pre-test/post-test design study among 1,530 Dutch individuals. 974 vacationers answered questions about their happiness before and after a holiday trip. Vacationers reported a higher degree of pre-trip happiness, compared to non-vacationers, possibly because they are anticipating their holiday. Only a very relaxed holiday trip boosts vacationers' happiness further after return. Generally, there is no difference between vacationers' and non-vacationers' post-trip happiness. The findings are explained in the light of set-point theory, need theory and comparison theory.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Vacationers Happier, but Most not Happier
After a Holiday
Jeroen Nawijn &Miquelle A. Marchand &
Ruut Veenhoven &Ad J. Vingerhoets
Received: 22 July 2009 / Accepted: 17 December 2009 /Published online: 10 February 2010
#Springer Science+Business Media B.V./
The International Society for Quality-of-Life Studies (ISQOLS) 2010
Abstract The aim of this study was to obtain a greater insight into the association
between vacations and happiness. We examined whether vacationers differ in
happiness, compared to those not going on holiday, and if a holiday trip boosts post-
trip happiness. These questions were addressed in a pre-test/post-test design study
among 1,530 Dutch individuals. 974 vacationers answered questions about their
happiness before and after a holiday trip. Vacationers reported a higher degree of
pre-trip happiness, compared to non-vacationers, possibly because they are
anticipating their holiday. Only a very relaxed holiday trip boosts vacationers
happiness further after return. Generally, there is no difference between vacationers
and non-vacationerspost-trip happiness. The findings are explained in the light of
set-point theory, need theory and comparison theory.
Keywords Happiness .Holiday stress .Quality of life .Subjective well-being .
Tourism
Applied Research Quality Life (2010) 5:3547
DOI 10.1007/s11482-009-9091-9
J. Nawijn :R. Veenhoven
Faculty of Social Sciences, Erasmus University Rotterdam, Rotterdam, the Netherlands
J. Nawijn
Academy for Tourism, Breda University of Applied Sciences, Breda, the Netherlands
J. Nawijn (*)
Mgr. Hopmansstraat 1, P.O. Box 3917, 4800 DX Breda, the Netherlands
e-mail: nawijn.j@nhtv.nl
M. A. Marchand
CentERdata, Tilburg University, Tilburg, the Netherlands
A. J. Vingerhoets
Faculty of Social and Behavioural Sciences, Tilburg University, Tilburg, the Netherlands
Introduction
Tourism
A characteristic of post-industrial societies is that the majority of people in them practice
tourism (Bell 1973). Tourism is evolving into one of the most important industries
worldwide. The percentage of vacationers is ever increasing; the World Tourism
Organisation (UNWTO) predicts 1.6 billion international arrivals by the year 2020
(UNWTO 2008). In addition, trip frequency has increased considerably over peoples
lifespan (Opperman 1995) and there is a trend to take more than one holiday per year.
Vacationerswho have a history of past tourism experiences in a certain destination, or
who take similar types of holidays regularly, are better capable of matching their wants
to their needs (Ryan 1998). This implies that tourists may become increasingly able to
derive benefit from their holidays in terms of fulfillment, enjoyment and happiness.
Tourism is generally regarded as an activity which allows for an escape from the routine
of daily life (Prentice 2004). Gross (1961) argued that touristsescape is a time-out,
much needed for healthy functioning in society. Thus, Gross regards holiday trips as an
experience needed to cope with daily life after a holiday trip. Boorstin (1964)and
MacCannell (1976) argued that people generally live alienated inauthentic lives, and that
during their holiday trip they strive to escape to a more satisfying life. These views seem
outdated nowadays and have been replaced by more postmodern views of tourism
(Uriely 2005). The distinction between tourism and everyday life is not as clear anymore
as it perhaps once was. Leisure, tourism and everyday life have become a much more
integral part of life (Larsen 2008;McCabe2002) because experiences that were once
confined to tourism, are now accessible in everyday life (Lash and Urry 1994).
Happiness
The subject of happiness has been a playground for speculative philosophyfor
hundreds of years (Veenhoven 2009, p. 45), but is currently also being studied
empirically by the social sciences. This type of research took off in the 1970s and
has been thriving recently, partly due to the popularity of positive psychology. These
studies conclude that life is getting better (Veenhoven and Hagerty 2006) and that
most people are happy in modern society (Diener and Diener 1996). However, not
all changes in society imply progress in terms of happiness. For instance, increased
consumption does not automatically lead to greater happiness and may even reduce
happiness through demoralization of society (Myers 2000), stress and income
differences (Easterbrook 2003), over-consumption (Easterbrook 2003; Schor 1998)
and over-worked or burnt-out employees (Easterbrook 2003; Schor 1991).
Several quality of life theories offer insight into happiness. Comparison theory
states that we judge the quality of our lives mainly by estimating the gap between the
realities of our lives and common standards of the good. In this view, happiness
largely involves keeping up with the Joneses. Set-point theory argues that happiness
is a rather stable traitand that whatever we do, we cannot change our happiness
much. In this view, particular experiences can at best provide a temporary uplift,
after which we return to our set-point (Cummins 2005; Veenhoven 2006). Finally,
need theory emphasizes that happiness is more a reflection of how we generally feel
36 J. Nawijn et al.
and an indicator of whether certain needs are gratified (Veenhoven 2006). In this
view, experiences add to happiness only if they involve need gratification.
The findings of the current study may contribute to an explanation of how
vacationing adds to greater happiness of individuals, in the light of the above-
mentioned quality of life theories.
Tourism and Happiness
Previous studies have provided data about the effect of holiday trips on happiness,
including the role of length of stay.
Holiday Trips
A few cross-sectional studies find weak positive associations between holiday trips and
happiness (Kemp et al. 2008;Milman1998;Neal2000). Three pre-test/post-test design
studies (Hoopes and Lounsbury 1989; Lounsbury and Hoopes 1986; Strauss-Blasche et
al. 2000) analyzed the impact of time off from work, which may, or may not have,
included a holiday trip. Just one pre-test/post-test design study focused exclusively on
holiday trips. It measured the impact of a holiday trip on touristspost-trip happiness
(Gilbert and Abdullah 2004). Generally, the impact of a holiday on post-trip life
satisfaction is small (De Bloom et al. 2009).
Positive effects of a holiday do not last very long. Westman and Eden (1997)and
Westman and Etzion (2001) found that people who suffer from burnout returned to pre-
vacation levels by the time of their second measurement, three (Westman and Eden
1997) to four weeks (Westman and Etzion 2001) after the respite from work had
ended. Another study (Strauss-Blasche et al. 2000) reported that improvement in
mood and quality of sleep lasted only up to five weeks. Gilbert and Abdullahs
(2004) post-trip assessment period spanned a period of two to six months. They
found that vacationers, compared to non-vacationers, experienced a higher
amount of pleasant feelings after their holidays, but found no differences in
post-trip happiness between the three periods they distinguished.
Holiday Stress
Holiday trips are not merely pleasant experiences. Quite a few studies report a series of
health problems, such as homesickness, associated with vacationing (Kop et al. 2003;
Pearce 1981; Van Heck and Vingerhoets 2007; Vingerhoets et al. 1997 In addition to
health problems, vacationers report many worries during their holiday (Larsen et al.
2009) and some experience relational problems (Ryan 1991)oracultureshock(Pearce
1981). However, the association between holiday stress and post-trip happiness has not
been investigated yet. Intuitively, it is plausible that when one experiences a holiday
with a lot of stress, this would lead to lower feelings of happiness.
Length of Stay
The role of length of stay is not yet clear at this time. Neal (2000) concludes that length of
stay moderates several of the relationships in her model. A follow-up study by Neal and
Vacationers Happier, but Most not Happier After a Holiday 37
Sirgy (2004) finds a moderating effect of length of stay between holiday satisfaction and
happiness. However, Gilbert and Abdullah (2004), Lounsbury and Hoopes (1986)and
Kemp et al. (2008) found no association between length of stay and post-trip happiness.
Questions
In conclusion, a literature review reveals many positive feelings to be associated with
holiday trips, but does not inform us whether vacationers are happier in general.
Research findings until now suggest that a time off from work does not boost happiness
much afterwards. We currently do not know whether this is the case for holiday trips; the
single study on post-trip happiness, until now, used rather crude time intervals (Gilbert
and Abdullah 2004). Finally, studies to date have yielded inconsistent empirical
evidence concerning the importance of the factor length of stay.
This study adds to this literature by attempting to answer the following questions:
(1) Are vacationers happier compared to non-vacationers? (2) Does a holiday trip
boost happiness? (3) If a holiday trip boosts happiness, then for how long does this
effect last? and (4) What are the roles of length of stay and holiday stress?
Method
Respondents
The sample consists of 1,530 respondents. 52% are men. Mean age is 50 (SD=16)
and mean monthly net income is 2,420 (SD = 1,810). Most respondents hold a
graduate or undergraduate degree (35%), have a paid job (51%) or are retired (21%).
74% is either married or cohabiting.
All respondents are members of the CentERdata Databank, who made the data for
this study available to us. CentERdata is a research institute, which is part of Tilburg
University in the Netherlands.
The CentERpanel is composed of well over 2,000 households, completing online
questionnaires at home every week, and this panel is considered a representative
sample of the Dutch-speaking population. Households without Internet access are
given a device called a Net.Box, which allows them to enter data on the Internet, using
their televisions as screens. We make use of 2006 data from the health monitor, which
is a questionnaire that is administered to CentERpanel members every eight weeks.
The questionnaire of week 35 of that year contained questions about respondents
holiday trip, if they had made any. The day of departure and day of return differed.
The holiday trip began at some point in time between week 27 and week 35. The
questionnaires of week 11, 19 and 43 were also available to us.
Variables
Happiness
Our focus was on hedonic level of affect, which Veenhoven (1984) considers the
affective dimensionof happiness. Hedonic level of affect can be measured using an
38 J. Nawijn et al.
Affect Balance Score (ABS), such as PANAS (Watson et al. 1988), which has also been
applied previously in several tourism studies (Gilbert and Abdullah 2002,2004;Milman
1998). The ABS used in this study was derived from responses to three questions, one
positively formulated item and two negative ones. The mean score of the two negative
items was deducted from the positive item. The positive item measured whether
respondents had recently enjoyed their daily tasks. The two negative statements
evaluated whether respondents had recently felt unhappy,andgloomy and dejected.
Possible answers were never,almost never,sometimes,very oftenand always.
Type of Holiday
Two groups were distinguished of, respectively, non-vacationers and vacationers.
The first group (n=556) did not go on a holiday, they continued their everyday life.
The second group (n=974) consisted of vacationers who started their holiday trip
between week 27 and week 35. A holiday trip is defined as a trip where people are
traveling to and staying in places outside their usual environment for no longer than
one year and specifically for leisure purposes (UNWTO 2002).
Length of Stay
To determine how long a holiday trip had lasted, respondents had to answer the
following question: How long did your holiday last?The response alternatives were
less than 5 days,58days,914 days,1521 daysand more than 21 days.
Holiday Stress
A question on the respondentslevel of experienced stress during their holiday trip was
included in the questionnaire. The exact question was: How did you experience your
holiday elsewhere?The response alternatives were very stressful,stressful,
neutral,relaxedand very relaxed.
Days Passed After Having Returned
The questionnaire also included a question on how many days had passed since the
respondent had returned from his/her holiday trip. The response alternatives were I
am still on holiday,less than one week,less than two weeks,less than three
weeks,less than four weeks,less than six weeksand less than eight weeks.
Socio-demographic Variables
Educational level,net household income,employment status,age,andsex were all
assessed.
Health Status
Respondentshealth status was measured by the number of sick days in the past
month. Possible responses were 0 days,1 or 2 days,35 days,510 days
and more than 10 days.
Vacationers Happier, but Most not Happier After a Holiday 39
Extraversion
A particular personality trait (McCrae and John 1992) which is moderately
positively associated with happiness is extraversion (Diener et al. 1992; Pavot et al.
1990). Extraverts have an active leisure life (Brandstätter 1994), which may be
associated with greater happiness (Brown et al. 1991). We had information on the
respondentslevel of extraversion, which was measured in 2007 on a five-point
scale. Even though this personality assessment took place a year later than the
measured variables in this study, it was appropriate to use since personality traits
are deemed to be relatively stable (Costa and McCrae 1994;Gustavssonetal.
1997; Hampson and Goldberg 2006;McCraeandCosta2003; Terraciano et al.
2006). Our assumption is that extraverts are likely to take different types of
holidays, compared to other individuals. Since we had no information on type of
holiday, we controlled for extraversion.
Results
Are Vacationers Happier?
Figure 1shows the happiness scores of vacationers (n=974) and non-vacationers
over time (n=556). Vacationers display a higher degree of pre-trip as well as post-
trip happiness. To test whether this difference in happiness is significant, we
performed two one-way between groups multivariate analyses of covariance.
Pre-trip Happiness
To determine the possible pre-trip happiness difference, three dependent variables
were used: happiness levels of week 11, 19 and 27. As covariates we used socio-
demographics, health status and extraversion. Preliminary assumption testing was
conducted to check for normality, linearity, univariate and multivariate outliers,
homogeneity of variance-covariance matrices, and multicollinearity, with no serious
violations noted. There was a statistically significant difference between vacationers
1.8
1.9
2
2.1
2.2
2.3
2.4
11 19 27 35 43
Vacationers
Non-Vacationers
Fig. 1 Mean happiness over
time
40 J. Nawijn et al.
and those staying at home, F(3, 531)=3.39, p=.018; WilksLambda=.98; η
2
=.02.
Vacationers have a significantly higher pre-trip happiness score (M= 2.25, SD = .08)
compared to those not going on a holiday trip (M= 2.07, SD = .09).
Post-trip Happiness
Post-trip happiness comprised of two dependent variables, namely the happiness
scores of week 35 and week 43. Again, preliminary assumption testing was
conducted, with no violations noted. Vacationers still on holiday in week 35 (n=142)
were excluded from the analysis. This time we found no statistically significant
difference in happiness between both groups, F(2, 783)=2.00, ns.
Does a Holiday Trip Boost Happiness?
Post-trip Happiness Boost
A three-way between-groups multivariate analysis of covariance was performed to
investigate differences in happiness. Happiness levels of week 35 and week 43 were
the dependent variables. The independent variables were holiday stress, length of
stay and days passed after having returned. None of the vacationers considered their
holiday to be very stressful. Only 14 considered theirs stressful and 67 neutral. We
grouped these two response categories and labeled this combined category stressful
or neutral. Socio-demographics, health status and extraversion were used as
covariates and vacationers still on holiday in week 35 were again excluded from
the analysis. Preliminary assumption testing was conducted to check for normality,
linearity, univariate and multivariate outliers, homogeneity of variance-covariance
matrices, and multicollinearity. Since Boxs test of equality of variances was
significant, we used Pillais Trace Criterion instead of WilksLambda. No other
violations were observed. We found a statistically significant main effect of holiday
stress on the combined post-trip happiness variable, F(4, 718)=9.24, p.001;
Pillais Trace=.10; η
2
=.05. An inspection of the mean happiness scores indicated
that vacationers who rated their holiday very relaxed (M=2.64, SD=.10) or relaxed
(M=2.16, SD=.09) are happier than vacationers who rated their holiday as stressful
or neutral (M=1.07, SD=.22). We found no significant main effect for length of stay,
F(8, 718)=1.59, ns. There was also no significant main effect for days passed after
having returned,F(10, 718)= .90, ns. The interaction effect between holiday stress
and length of stay, F(16, 718)= 1.08, ns, and holiday stress and days passed after
having returned, F (20, 718)=1.19, ns, were both not statistically significant. Thus,
holiday stress is negatively associated with post-trip happiness, whereas length of
stay and the number of days passed after having returned fail to have a significant
impact on it.
Fade-out
Happiness for the relaxedgroup (n=521)inweek27is2.27(SD=1.08). Their post-
holiday happiness scores were all very similar to their happiness level in week 27, and
did not significantly fluctuate. The very relaxedgroup had a pre-trip mean happiness
Vacationers Happier, but Most not Happier After a Holiday 41
score of 2.48 (SD=1.26). This happiness level of this group slowly, but gradually,
decreased, until it reached their pre-trip level at eight weeks after return, with significant
higher post-trip happiness levels in the first two weeks of return. The subgroups for the
stressful or neutral category were too small to draw any meaningful conclusions.
Discussion
Vacationers are Happier
Our study demonstrated that vacationers generally displayed greater pre-trip
happiness than non-vacationers, although the differences were small. In contrast,
post-trip happiness did not differ between vacationers and non-vacationers. Possibly,
anticipation played an important role in explaining the observed differences in pre-
trip happiness between vacationers and non-vacationers. Holiday trips are
experiences which people look forward to (Miller et al. 2007). For most, the
enjoyment starts weeks, even months before the holiday actually begins.
Remarkably, post-trip happiness is generally not different for vacationers and
non-vacationers. However, holiday stress is associated with post-trip happiness.
Holidays Briefly Boost Post-trip Happiness
The benefits of a very relaxedholiday trip last maximally for two weeks. A relaxed
holiday without stress is not only good for ones health (Gump and Matthews 2000;
Vingerhoets et al. 1997; Westman and Etzion 2001), but is also important in terms of
post-trip happiness. However, the relaxed group of vacationers does not draw any
post-trip happiness benefits, which suggests a holiday has to be very relaxing in
order to draw post-trip benefits, in terms of post-trip happiness.
Our study finds a similar fade-out gradient for happiness as De Bloom et al.
(2009) reported in their meta-analysis on post-trip health. It is not surprising that a
holiday trip does not have a prolonged effect on happiness, since most vacationers
have to return to work or other daily tasks and consequently fall back into their
normal routine fairly quickly.
Additionally, this study demonstrates that length of stay is not associated with
post-trip happiness. This contradicts Neal (2000) and Neal and Sirgys(2004)
findings, but supports the findings of Gilbert and Abdullah (2004), Lounsbury and
Hoopes (1986) and Kemp et al. (2008). Our data confirm that returning home
involves a swift return to pre-trip happiness levels, independent of the duration of the
trip. As long as the holiday trip itself provides sufficient opportunity to relax and
reduce stress, there should be no reason for any differences in post-trip happiness,
regardless of how many days are spent on holiday.
Quality of Life Theories
How do these findings fit the three theories of quality of life, as mentioned in the
introduction section? Set-point theory cannot explain the pre-trip high very well, but
adequately predicts that the holiday uplift is short-lived or even absent. Need theory
42 J. Nawijn et al.
can explain the pre-trip high if one assumes that we have an innate need for
wandering, possibly a leftover of our hunter-gatherer past and that this need can
already be partially gratified by anticipation. In this theory, the absence of a post-trip
boost could be due to saturation. Another possible explanation is that the non-
vacationers, or their travel partner(s), feel temporarily less happy and therefore do
not have the need for a holiday. However, this does not explain the absence of
differences in post-trip happiness between vacationers and non-vacationers very
well. The need for status could possibly explain our findings also. Comparison
theory would suggest that social comparison is involved and that people who
anticipate a holiday feel to be better off than those who intend to stay at home. The
absence of a post-trip happiness difference between vacationers and non-vacationers
can be explained from a comparison theory perspective as well. The holiday is over
and vacationers are no longer different from non-vacationers, which would explain
the similar happiness levels.
Implications
The findings of our study suggest several managerial, policy and individual implications.
From an individual point of view, vacationing is something which is looked forward
to. However, the length of such a vacation does not matter in terms of post-trip happiness.
This suggests that people derive more happiness from two or more short breaks spread
throughout the year, than from having just a single longer holiday once a year.
This brings us to the policy implications. In order for families to stagger their
holiday time throughout the year, the school system would have to become more
flexible. Some countries have rather lengthy summer holidays, leaving little time for
short additional vacations in the rest of the year. This poses a constraint to families
with school-going children in particular. However, as Butler (2001) suggests,
changing the long summer school holiday may not have the desired effect; tourists
for example attach much value to the weather condition and in addition, host
communities may not be so keen to have tourists all year round.
From a managerial perspective we would advise tourism managers to provide
holiday products, with a minimum amount of stress-inducing aspects. Obviously,
companies do not purposely create stressful holiday services. However, certain
experiences may enhance stress and should be avoided as much as possible. An
example of such a stressful experience is waiting in line at a theme park or cuing at
the entrance to a museum. These clearly are examples of rather mildly stressful
experiences. Nonetheless, when waiting in line for attractions at a theme park, in the
heat, accompanied by impatient young children, such mildly stressful experiences
could easily evolve into much more stressful events. Several methods to reduce the
negative effects of waiting lines exist (Kostecki 1996); the tourism industry should
use these approaches to greater extent. Another example of a stress-inducing aspect
of certain holidays is long haul air travel. Several aspects of air travel contribute to
stress (Stokes and Kite 1994). Jet lag is an all too familiar phenomenon among air
travelers. Long haul air travel may also cause health problems, such as a cold, which
could turn into a painful and stressful experience at high altitudes (Vingerhoets et al.
1997). However, information on how to reduce jet lag or prevent other health issues
is not always clearly communicated by airlines and tour operators.
Vacationers Happier, but Most not Happier After a Holiday 43
Specific stress-factors aside, managers should generally go out of their way to
create holiday packages which are very relaxing, as very relaxed holidays boost
post-trip happiness. The increased attention for spa, health and wellness tourism
(Smith and Puckó 2009) may be a sign that the tourism industry is already attending
to this increasing need.
Future Research
We had no information about the type of holidays, or the type of activities these
vacationers engaged in during their trip. Even though we know that very relaxed
holidays are the most favorable in terms of post-trip happiness, we do not know
whether certain types of holidays or certain types of holiday activities are considered
very relaxedmore so than others. If stressful holidays do not increase post-trip
happiness, then why are there still so many active holiday packages being
purchased? The answer probably lies in finding the right person-environment fit.
Sensation-seekers and extraverts could benefit from vacationing in different ways
than other vacationers. These matters should be explored in future research.
From the point of view of comparison theory, vacationing may possibly lower post-
trip happiness when vacationers encounter richer tourists at exclusive, expensive resorts
and luxurious hotels. Vacationers may also be confronted with extreme poverty in more
exotic, but poor, destinations. Future research is needed to assess how frequently such
confrontations take place and if they affect vacationerspost-trip happiness.
Post-trip, the possible effect of holiday satisfaction (Lounsbury and Hoopes 1986;
Westman and Eden 1997) and its relation to holiday stress as a moderator of post-trip
happiness requires further investigation.
Specific attention must be paid to the recollection phase of a holiday trip
(Clawson and Knetsch 1966), which involves savoring the holiday experience
(Bryant and Veroff 2007) through reminiscing of past holiday events (Morgan and
Xu 2009). Selective retention, selective recall (Hickson III and Beck 2008) and a
rosy view (Mitchell et al. 1997) can potentially enhance the positive effects of a
holiday trip. These processes could add to greater happiness of vacationers. More
research is needed to address these phenomena.
Finally, additional research is also needed to further assess the impact of anticipation
of a holiday trip (Parrinello 1993) on vacationerspre-trip happiness. Moreover, the
duration of such a possible uplift in pre-trip happiness needs to be assessed. Holiday
expectation (Gilbert and Abdullah 2002;Gnoth1997) and homesickness (Van Tilburg
et al. 1996) could be assessed for possible moderating effects.
Conclusions
This study demonstrates that vacationers are happier, compared to non-vacationers,
but a holiday trip does not add much to their happiness. Generally, there were no
differences between vacationersand non-vacationerspost-trip happiness. Only
vacationers on a very relaxedholiday trip benefit in terms of post-trip happiness.
The pre-trip happiness difference between vacationers and non-vacationers could be
an indication of vacationers looking forward to their holiday.
44 J. Nawijn et al.
We tried to explain these findings in the light of the three quality of life theories,
as mentioned in the introduction section. Comparison theory and need theory explain
our findings best. Comparison theory explains the pre-trip difference well, assuming
that social comparison is involved and that people who anticipate a vacation, feel
better off than non-vacationers. Generally, once the holiday is over, vacationers are
no happier than non-vacationers because the holiday is over and vacationers are, in
that sense, equal to non-vacationers. If one assumes that we have an innate need for
wandering, and that this need is fulfilled by taking a holiday trip, then need theory
explains the pre-trip difference rather well. Furthermore, assuming that saturation
takes place, need theory would also explain the absence of a post-trip happiness
boost for most tourists.
References
Bell, D. (1973). The coming of post-industrial society: A venture in social forecasting. New York: Basic
Books.
Boorstin, D. (1964). The image: A guide to pseudo-events in america. New York: Harper.
Brandstätter, H. (1994). Pleasure of leisure-pleasure of work: personality makes the difference. Personality
and Individual Differences, 16(6), 931946.
Brown, B. A., Frankel, G. B., & Fennell, M. (1991). Happiness through leisure: the impact of type of
leisure activity, age, gender and leisure satisfaction on psychological well-being. Journal of Applied
Recreation Research, 16(4), 368392.
Bryant, F. B., & Veroff, J. (2007). Savoring: A new model of positive experience. Mahwah: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Butler, R. W. (2001). Seasonality in tourism: Issues and implications. In T. Baum & S. Lundtorp (Eds.),
Seasonality in tourism. Kindlington: Pergamon.
Clawson, M., & Knetsch, J. L. (1966). Economics of outdoor recreation. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins.
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1994). Set like plaster? Evidence for the stability of adult personality. In T.
F. Heatherton & J. L. Weinberger (Eds.), Can personality change? (pp. 2140). Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association Books.
Cummins, R. A. (2005). Moving from the quality of life concept to a theory. Journal of Intellectual
Disability Research, 49(10), 699705.
De Bloom, J., Kompier, M., Geurts, S., De Weerth, C., Taris, T., & Sonnentag, S. (2009). Do we recover
from vacation? Meta-analysis of vacation effects on health and well-being. Journal of Occupational
Health, 51(1), 1325.
Diener, E., & Diener, C. (1996). Most people are happy. Psychological Science, 7(3), 181185.
Diener, E., Sandvik, E., Pavot, W., & Fujita, F. (1992). Extraversion and subjective well-being in U.S.
National probability sample. Journal of Research in Personality, 26(3), 205215.
Easterbrook, G. (2003). The progress paradox: How life gets better while people feel worse (1st ed.). New
York: Random House.
Gilbert, D., & Abdullah, J. (2002). A study on the impact of the expectation of a holiday on an
individuals sense of well-being. Journal of Vacation Marketing, 8(4), 352361.
Gilbert, D., & Abdullah, J. (2004). Holidaytaking and the sense of well-being. Annals of Tourism
Research, 31(1), 103121.
Gnoth, J. (1997). Tourism motivation and expectation formation. Annals of Tourism Research, 24(2), 283304.
Gross, E. (1961). A functional approach to leisure analysis. Social Problems, 9(1), 28.
Gump, B. B., & Matthews, K. A. (2000). Are vacations good for your health? The 9-year mortality
experience after the multiple risk factor intervention trial. Psychosomatic Medicine, 62(5), 608612.
Gustavsson, J. P., We inryb, R. M., Göransson, S., Pedersen, N. L., & Åsberg, M. (1997). Stability and predictive
ability of personality traits across 9 years. Personality and Individual Differences, 22(6), 783792.
Hampson, S. E., & Goldberg, L. R. (2006). A first large cohort study of personality trait stability over the
40 years between elementary school and midlife. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91
(4), 763779.
Vacationers Happier, but Most not Happier After a Holiday 45
Hickson, M., III, & Beck, C. M. (2008). Genetic, neurological, and social bases of empathy. Human
Communication, 11(3), 359382.
Hoopes, L. L., & Lounsbury, J. W. (1989). An investigation of life satisfaction following a vacation: a
domain-specific approach. Journal of Community Psychology, 17(3), 129140.
Kemp, S., Burt, C. D. B., & Furneaux, L. (2008). A test of the peak-end rule with extended
autobiographical events. Memory & Cognition, 36(1), 132138.
Kop, W. J., Vingerhoets, A., Kruithof, G.-J., & Gottdiener, J. S. (2003). Risk factors for myocardial
infarction during vacation travel. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65(3), 396401.
Kostecki, M. (1996). Waiting lines as a marketing issue. European Management Journal, 14(3), 295303.
Larsen, S. (2008). De-exoticizing tourist travel: Everyday life and sociality on the move. Leisure Studies,
27(1), 2134.
Larsen, S., Brun, W., & Ogaard, T. (2009). What tourists worry aboutconstruction of a scale measuring
tourist worries. Tourism Management, 30(2), 260265.
Lash, S., & Urry, J. (1994). Economies of signs and space. London: Sage.
Lounsbury, J. W., & Hoopes, L. L. (1986). A vacation from work: Changes in work and nonwork
outcomes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 71(3), 392401.
MacCannell, D. (1976). The tourist: A new theory of the leisure class. New York: Schocken Books.
McCabe, S. (2002). The tourist experience and everyday life. In G. M. S. Dann (Ed.), The tourist as a
metaphor of the social world (pp. 6177). Wallingford: CABI.
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (2003). Personality in adulthood: A five-factor theory perspective. New
York: Guilford.
McCrae, R. R., & John, O. P. (1992). An introduction to the five-factor model and its applications. Journal
of Personality, 60(2), 175215.
Miller, G., Rathouse, K., Scarles, C., Holmes, K., & Tribe, J. (2007). Public understanding of sustainable
leisure and tourism: A report to the department for environment, food and rural affairs. London:
Delfra, University of Surrey.
Milman, A. (1998). The impact of tourism and travel experience on senior travelerspsychological well-
being. Journal Of Travel Research, 37(2), 166170.
Mitchell, T. R., Thompson, L., Peterson, E., & Cronk, R. (1997). Temporal adjustments in the evaluation
of events: the Rosy view.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33(4), 421448.
Morgan, M., & Xu, F. (2009). Student travel experiences: memories and dreams. Journal of Hospitality
Marketing & Management, 18(2), 216236.
Myers, D. G. (2000). The American paradox: Spiritual hunger in an age of plenty. New Haven: Yale
University Press.
Neal, J. D. (2000). The effects of different aspects of tourism services on travelersquality of life: Model
validation, refinement, and extension. Blacksburg: Virginia Polytechnic Institute And State University.
Neal, J. D., & Sirgy, M. J. (2004). Measuring the effect of tourism services on travelersquality of life:
further validation. Social Indicators Research, 69(3), 243277.
Opperman, M. (1995). Travel life cycle. Annals of Tourism Research, 22(3), 535552.
Parrinello, G. L. (1993). Motivation and anticipation in post-industrial tourism. Annals of Tourism
Research, 20(2), 233249.
Pavot, W., Diener, E., & Fujita, F. (1990). Extraversion and happiness. Personality and Individual
Differences, 11(12), 12991306.
Pearce, P. L. (1981). Environment shock: a study of touristsreactions to two tropical islands. Journal of
Applied Social Psychology, 11(3), 268280.
Prentice, R. C. (2004). Tourist motivation and typologies. In A. A. Lew, C. M. Hall, & A. M. Williams
(Eds.), Companion to tourism (pp. 261279). Oxford: Pergamon.
Ryan, C. (1991). Recreational tourism: A social science approach. London: Routledge.
Ryan, C. (1998). The travel career ladder: an appraisal. Annals of Tourism Research, 25(4), 936957.
Schor, J. (1991). The overworked American. New York: Basic Books.
Schor, J. (1998). The overspent American. New York: Harper Collins.
Smith, M., & Puckó, L. (Eds.). (2009). Health and wellness tourism. Amsterdam: Butterworth-Heinemann.
Stokes, A. F., & Kite, K. (1994). Flight stress: Stress, fatigue, and performance in aviation. Aldershot:
Avebury Aviation.
Strauss-Blasche, G., Ekmekcioglu, C., & Marktl, W. (2000). Does vacation enable recuperation? Changes
in well-being associated with time away from work. Occupational Medicine, 50(3), 167172.
Terraciano, A., Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (2006). Personality plasticity after age 30. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(8), 9991009.
46 J. Nawijn et al.
UNWTO. (2002). Definition of tourism. Retrieved 01 September 2008, from http://www.world-tourism.
org/statistics/tsa_project/TSA_in_depth/chapters/ch3-1.htm
UNWTO. (2008). Tourism market trends 2006. World overview and tourism topics (No. 2006 ed). Madrid:
UNWTO.
Uriely, N. (2005). The tourist experience: conceptual developments. Annals of Tourism Research, 32(1),
199216.
Van Heck, G. L., & Vingerhoets, A. J. J. M. (2007). Leisure sickness: a biopsychosocial perspective.
Psychological Topics, 16(2), 187200.
Van Tilburg, M. A., Vingerhoets, A. J., Van Heck, G. L., & Kirschbaum, C. (1996). Mood changesin homesick
persons during a holiday trip. A multiple case study. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 65(2), 9196.
Veenhoven, R. (1984). Conditions of happiness. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.
Veenhoven, R. (2006). How do we assess how happy we are? Tenets, implications and tenability of three
theories. USA: University of Notre Dame.
Veenhoven, R. (2009). The international scale interval study: Improving the comparability of responses to
survey questions about happiness. In V. Moller & D. Huschka (Eds.), Quality of life in the new millennium:
Advances in quality-of-life studies, theory and research, vol. 35 (pp. 4558). Dordrecht: Springer.
Veenhoven, R., & Hagerty, M. R. (2006). Rising happiness in nations 19462004: a reply to Easterlin.
Social Indicators Research, 79(3), 421436.
Vingerhoets, A. J. J. M., Sanders, N., & Kuper, W. (1997). Health issues in international tourism: the role
of health behavior, stress and adaptation. In M. V. Tilburg & A. J. J. M. Vingerhoets (Eds.),
Psychological aspects of geographical moves: Homesickness and acculturation stress (pp. 197211).
Amsterdam: Amsterdam Academic Archive.
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive
and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 10631070.
Westman, M., & Eden, D. (1997). Effects of a respite from work on burnout: vacation relief and fade-out.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 82(4), 516527.
Westman, M., & Etzion, D. (2001). The impact of vacation and job stress on burnout and absenteeism.
Psychology and Health, 16(5), 595606.
Vacationers Happier, but Most not Happier After a Holiday 47
... Due to the increasing work demands, many individuals find shorter respites insufficient and often seek longer nonwork periods for comprehensive recovery (Kühnel et al., 2017). According to Nawijn et al. (2010), a vacation presents an opportunity for a longer break, enabling employees to momentarily disengage from their duties at work. ...
Article
In our fast-paced world, work-related stress takes a toll on health and well-being. To address this, individuals seek respite through vacations. Employing a mixed-methods approach, data were collected from 422 participants through on-site and online surveys, analyzing engagement in activities and their influence on psychological detachment, relaxation, control, and mastery. Factor analysis revealed seven distinct vacation dimensions – outdoor activities, cultural activities, entertainment, social activity, nature recreation, shopping and light recreation. Findings revealed that engagement in entertainment, light recreation, social, and cultural activities positively affect psychological detachment. Outdoor activities, nature recreation, and entertainment enhance relaxation. Light recreation and shopping boost visitors’ sense of control, while social activities promote mastery. This research highlights the importance of tailored vacation activities for enhancing recovery and overall well-being. It offers insights for researchers and practitioners alike, emphasizing the need to meet tourists’ recovery needs for enriched vacation experiences and enhanced well-being.
... The third aspect of service-learning that contributed to academic development of alumni was socialization which loaded co-curricular activities, tours and travels, and social parties. These activities depicted the social life of alumni through adventures and leisure (Ikendi et al., 2022c;Jarvis & Peel, 2008;Nawijn et al., 2010). Activities of this nature managed the fatigue of students after fieldwork which also provided opportunities to learn about peers' unique talents and cultures after field activities and learn about and from nature and the environment in tours. ...
Article
Full-text available
In recent decades, the concept of well-being has often been the subject of psychological studies. The current study examines how tourism relates to well-being from the perspective of tourists/visitors, through the study of the older and more recent literature. Especially when it comes to the complex and multifaceted issue of well-being, in relation to the also complex and sensitive sectors of tourism, the specificity of this relationship is reflected in the evidence and the growing number of articles published in mainstream and reliable journals. By using the PRISMA framework, this article compares and evaluates published papers on the study of well-being and tourist behavior to guide and direct future research. It has been found that the relationship between well-being and tourism, especially after the emergence of the COVID-19 pandemic, has been significantly strengthened. This study highlights the need to adopt a holistic approach that considers all the connections between well-being and tourism.
Article
This study explored the influence of digital celebrities on gastronomic tourism through the lens of the broaden‐and‐build theory. Utilizing partial least squares structural equation modeling (PLS‐SEM) and a case study focused on TikTok, the research examined how consumers' subjective well‐being (SWB) affects para‐social interaction, novelty seeking, and audience participation, and how these factors, in turn, influence behavioral intention in the domains of food and travel. The results validated the proposed hypotheses, underscoring the importance of SWB in shaping behavioral intention within gastronomic tourism driven by digital celebrities. This paper enhances the current literature by elucidating the role of digital celebrities in gastronomic tourism and offers practical insights for marketers and tourism professionals on optimizing the influence of digital celebrities to improve the gastronomic tourism experience.
Thesis
Full-text available
Rekreasyon ve turizm, çeşitli yollarla birçok alana fayda sağlarken, fiziki, ekonomik, kültürel ve sosyal çevre üzerinde bazı olumsuz etkiler yaratabilir. Bu olumsuz etkileri en aza indirip maksimum fayda sağlamak amacıyla çeşitli çalışmalar yürütülmektedir ve bu çalışmalar sürdürülebilirlik çerçevesinde değerlendirilmektedir. Önde gelen uluslararası turizm organizasyonlarından olan Birleşmiş Milletler Dünya Turizm Örgütü ve Küresel Sürdürülebilir Turizm Konseyi, 2015 yılında Birleşmiş Milletler Gündem 2030 kapsamında belirlenen 17 sürdürülebilir kalkınma hedefi ve 169 alt hedef doğrultusunda turizmin sürdürülebilirliği ile ilgili önemli adımlar atmışlardır. Birleşmiş Milletler Dünya Turizm Örgütü, kırsal kesimlerin turizmden yarar sağlaması amacıyla benimsenen sürdürülebilir kalkınma hedeflerin kapsamında, turistik çekiciliklere sahip düşük nüfuslu köyleri çeşitli kriterlerle değerlendirerek "En İyi Turizm Köyü" unvanı vermektedir. Bu unvan, köylerin gelişmesine, tanınırlığının artmasına ve kırsal bölgelerde yaşayan halkın desteklenmesine katkı sağlamaktadır. Ancak En İyi Turizm Köyü unvanına sahip destinasyonlarda belirlenen sürdürülebilirlik kriterleri arasında, köydeki rekreasyon etkinliklerinin sürdürülebilirliğinin yer almadığı görülmektedir. Bu durum, sürdürülebilirlik unvanına sahip bir köyde, sürdürülebilirliğe aykırı etkinliklerin yapılmasına imkan tanıyabilir ve bu da bir tutarsızlığa yol açabilir. Bu doğrultuda çalışmanın amacı En İyi Turizm Köyü unvanına sahip destinasyonlarda sürdürülebilir rekreasyon etkinliklerinin mevcut durumunu değerlendirmek ve bu etkinliklerin ekonomik, sosyal ve çevresel sürdürülebilirlik açısından yeterliliğini araştırmaktır. Aynı zamanda, En İyi Turizm Köyü unvanı kriterlerinde sürdürülebilir rekreasyon etkinliklerine dair bir eksiklik olup olmadığını belirleyerek, sürdürülebilirlik hedeflerine uygun olmayan etkinliklerin önüne geçilmesine yönelik öneriler geliştirmektir. Bu bağlamda, çalışma, sürdürülebilir turizmin paydaşlarının farkındalığını artırmayı ve uygulamaların iyileştirilmesine katkı sağlamayı hedeflemektedir. Çalışmanın amacı doğrultusunda nicel ve nitel yöntemlerin birlikte kullanılması uygun görülmüştür. Bir başka deyişle, araştırma karma yöntemle gerçekleştirilmiştir. Araştırmanın evrenini, 130 adet En İyi Turizm Köyü unvanına sahip destinasyondaki sürdürülebilir turizm paydaşları oluşturmaktadır. Evreni temsil etmesi amacıyla Mustafapaşa Köyü örneklem olarak seçilmiştir. Yapılan analizler sonucunda, yerel halkın sürdürülebilir rekreasyona yönelik algılarının düşük düzeyde olduğu tespit edilmiştir. Yerli ve yabancı ziyaretçilerin rekreasyonda sorumlu tüketici davranışları da oldukça düşüktür. İşletmeler ve köy muhtarlığı, sürdürülebilirlik uygulamaları konusunda yeterince desteklenmemekte ve bu nedenle istenilen performans seviyesine ulaşamamaktadır. Bulgular bir arada değerlendirildiğinde, En İyi Turizm Köyü kriterlerinin sürdürülebilir rekreasyon etkinliklerini teşvik etmediği, bölgede yapılan etkinliklerin ekonomik, sosyal ve çevresel anlamda sürdürülebilir olmadığı gözlemlenmiştir.
Article
This study challenges the top-down theory’s view that travel does not lead to sustained increases in life satisfaction. The impact of travel experience factors on life satisfaction before, during, and after travel was longitudinally evaluated among 252 young travelers (aged 20–30 years). The survey results showed that expectations before the trip, serendipity during the trip, and savoring after the trip affected life satisfaction directly or indirectly. In particular, the influence of savoring after travel on life satisfaction was much higher than that associated with travel satisfaction. In addition, the effect of travel satisfaction on savoring was nonsignificant, while that of serendipity was significant. Thus, beyond travel satisfaction, serendipity and savoring offer alternative paths to amplify life satisfaction derived from travel experiences.
Article
With the development of technology, the decrease in weekly working hours and the increasing awareness of human beings, the leisure time we can use freely is increasing rapidly. However, as leisure time increases, the problems of managing it increase. Pioneering studies have preferred to classify the factors that limit the effective and efficient use of leisure time into certain patterns. However, the constraints that individuals face in making the best use of their leisure time are too broad and personal to fit into any mould. Leisure constraints, which may manifest in different ways for different individuals, may cause problems in the management of leisure time and may also have a negative impact on the phenomenon of leisure time. From this point, the concept of "unmanageable leisure time" emerges. In this context, in-depth evaluations were made in order to better define the concept of unmanageable leisure time, which is one of the important individual and social management problems of our modern age. The study aims to make a descriptive contribution to the literature and researchers by addressing it conceptually.
Book
Full-text available
This book is about the degree to which people take pleasure in life: in short 'happiness'. It tries to identify conditions that favor a positive appreciation of life. Thus it hopes to shed more light on a longstanding and intriguing ques­ tion and, possibly, to guide attempts to improve the human lot. During the preceding decades a growing number of investigations have dealt with this issue. As a result there is now a sizable body of data. Yet it is quite difficult to make sense of it. There is a muddle of theories, concepts and indicators, and many of the findings seem to be contradictory. This book attempts to bring some order into the field. The study draws on an inventory of empirical investigations which involved valid indicators of happiness; 245 studies are involved, which together yield some 4000 observations: for the main part correlational ones. These results are presented in full detail in the simultaneously published 'Databook of Happiness' (Veenhoven 1984). The present volume distils conclusions from that wealth of data. It tries to assess the reality value of the findings and the degree to which correlations reflect the conditions of happiness rather than the consequences of it. It then attempts to place the scattered findings in context. As such, this work is not a typical study of literature on happiness.
Book
Full-text available
Health and Wellness Tourism takes an innovative look at this rapidly growing sector of today's thriving tourism industry. This book examines the range of motivations that drive this diverse sector of tourists, the products that are being developed to meet their needs and the management implications of these developments. A wide range of international case studies illustrate the multiple aspects of the industry and new and emerging trends including spas, medical wellness, life-coaching, meditation, festivals, pilgrimage and yoga retreats. The authors also evaluate marketing and promotional strategies and assess operational and management issues in the context of health and wellness tourism. This text includes a number of features to reinforce theory for advanced students of hospitality, leisure and tourism and related disciplines.
Article
This book includes 15 papers selected from the presentations that were delivered at the 15th World Congress of the International Sociological Association. These papers were prepared for that organization's research committee on international tourism, which took as its overall theme 'the tourist as a metaphor of the social world'. Following the introductory chapter, two contributions (chapters 2 and 3) raise serious fundamental questions about how academics should research (and hence theorize) about tourists. The next two contributions (chapters 4 and 5) provide a critique of conventional tourism wisdom. Chapter 6 suggests that tourism is an intricate and evolving social network with varying degrees of trust that are based on the correlative processes of self-presentation and interpretation. Chapter 7 conceptualizes the tourist and the social world within a paradigm of social facts so that the former becomes a sign of the latter. Chapter 8 sees the tourist as never entirely alone on an uncharted journey, because there are always those who have undertaken it before. Chapter 9 asks the question as to whether or not tourists experience reality. Chapter 10 provides an analogy between tourism and love in all its many phases. Chapter 11 emphasizes that it is necessary to go beyond traditional analyses that focus solely on the 'tourist gaze'. The next two essays (chapters 12 and 13) highlight the fact that not all tourists are identical and hence, by implication, that the tourist as a metaphor of the social world must be understood as a multiple persona figurative of complex reality. The final three contributions (chapters 14-16) present postmodern and futuristic perspectives on tourists. The book has a subject index.
Article
The relationship of a vacation to global and domain?specific life satisfaction was examined. Comparison of pre?and postvacation measures indicated an increase in global life satisfaction and a slight increase in satisfaction within the domain of money, but no significant change in the domains of marriagel/family, work, leisure, community, or outdoors/nature. Retrospective measures of domain and global vacation satisfactions increased the predictability of postvacation satisfaction significantly for four of the six specific domains and for overall satisfaction. The domain specificity of satisfactions was supported only for the domains of marriage/family and work. The results are discussed in terms of their support for a domain?specific approach to life satisfaction.
Article
Focusing on post-industrial economies, the study examines social inequality and changing experiences of time, space, culture, travel, the environment and globalization. Through a comparative analysis of the UK and US, Germany and Japan, the authors show how restructuration after organized capitalism has its basis in increasingly reflexive social actors and organizations. The consequence is not only the much-vaunted "postmodern condition' but a growth in reflexivity. In exploring this new reflexive world, the authors argue that today's economies are increasingly economies of signs - information, symbols, images, desire - and of space, where both signs and social subjects - refugees, financiers, tourists, flaneurs - are mobile over ever greater distances. They show how an understanding of such flows contributes to the analysis of changes in social relations, from the organization of work to the "culture industries', from the formation of an underclass to new forms of citizenship. -Publisher