ArticlePDF Available

What is 'manipulation'? A reappraisal

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Due primarily to its colloquial function, 'manipulation' is a poor term for distinguishing one healthcare intervention from another. With reports continuing to associate serious adverse events with manipulation, particularly relating to its use in the cervical spine, it is essential that the term be used appropriately and in accordance with a valid definition. The purpose of this paper is to identify empirically-derived features that we propose to be necessary and collectively sufficient for the formation of a valid definition for manipulation. A final definition is not offered. However, arguments for and against the inclusion of features are presented. Importantly, these features are explicitly divided into two categories: the 'action' (that which the practitioner does to the recipient) and the 'mechanical response' (that which occurs within the recipient). The proposed features are: 1) A force is applied to the recipient; 2) The line of action of this force is perpendicular to the articular surface of the affected joint; 3) The applied force creates motion at a joint; 4) This joint motion includes articular surface separation; 5) Cavitation occurs within the affected joint.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Professional issue
What is ‘manipulation’? A reappraisal
David W. Evans
a
,
*
, Nicholas Lucas
b
a
Research Centre, British School of Osteopathy, London SE1 1JE, UK
b
Sydney School of Public Health, University of Sydney, Australia
article info
Article history:
Received 27 November 2008
Received in revised form
3 November 2009
Accepted 21 December 2009
Keywords:
Joint manipulation
Spinal manipulation
Taxonomy
Definition
abstract
Due primarily to its colloquial function, ‘manipulation’ is a poor term for distinguishing one healthcare
intervention from another. With reports continuing to associate serious adverse events with manipulation,
particularly relating to its use in the cervical spine, it is essential that the term be used appropriately and
in accordance with a valid definition. The purpose of this paper is to identify empirically-derived features
that we propose to be necessary and collectively sufficient for the formation of a valid definition for
manipulation. A final definition is not offered. However, arguments for and against the inclusion of
features are presented. Importantly, these features are explicitly divided into two categories: the ‘action’
(that which the practitioner does to the recipient) and the ‘mechanical response’ (that which occurs
within the recipient). The proposed features are: 1) A force is applied to the recipient; 2) The line of
action of this force is perpendicular to the articular surface of the affected joint; 3) The applied force
creates motion at a joint; 4) This joint motion includes articular surface separation; 5) Cavitation occurs
within the affected joint.
Crown Copyright Ó2009 Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
1. Introduction
Scientific enquiry often requires researchers to consider the
foundations upon which important clinical and academic
assumptions have been built. For the professions that use manual
therapy, few foundations lie as deep as definitions of the very
interventions that distinguish manual therapy from other areas of
healthcare. It is difficult for practitioners to make rational decisions
about the use of an intervention when that intervention is poorly
defined or not mechanistically understood. Indeed, the ramifica-
tions of this uncertainty may be more far reaching than judgements
made by individual clinicians.
For example, definitions of healthcare interventions may be
used by purchasers to make inferences about the potential efficacy,
safety and appropriateness of that intervention, when applied to
populations (e.g. Shekelle et al., 1991; Coulter et al., 1996; Gatter-
man et al., 2001). Given that clinical trials have so far provided few
clear answers to inform the choice of one physical treatment over
another, particularly in relation to musculoskeletal problems (Kel-
ler et al., 2007; van der Velde et al., 2008), the perceived charac-
teristics of an intervention are likely to be used to provide clinical
guidance. In addition, with reports continuing to associate serious
adverse events with manipulation (e.g. Ernst, 2007), particularly
relating to its use in the cervical spine, the term should be used
appropriately and in accordance with a valid definition.
Manipulation is one intervention for which a satisfactory defi-
nition is lacking. Due primarily to its colloquial function, ‘manipu-
lation’ is a poor term for distinguishing one physical treatment from
another. Indeed, so vague is the term that when used in scientific
journals, supplementary details are often required to differentiate
‘real’ manipulation from its manual therapy counterparts (e.g.
Keller et al., 2002; Harvey et al., 2003; Skyba et al., 2003; Colloca
et al., 2004, 2006; Song et al., 2006). Oversights of this kind may be
avoided if what is currently termed ‘manipulation’ were accurately
defined.
The purpose of this paper is to present features proposed to be
necessary components of a valid definition of manipulation. A final
definition of manipulation is not offered, but arguments for and
against the inclusion of these empirically-derived features are
presented as a first step in this direction.
2. Defining manipulation
Prior to contemplating a definition of manipulation, it is
necessary to consider how a definition should be formed. Estab-
lished criteria for a definition are presented in Table 1 and are
compared to those criteria that meet the requirements for
adescription. A useful definition of manipulation should encom-
pass all characteristics that empirical research has shown to be
universally valid in all parts of the body, yet exclude any
*Corresponding author. Tel.: þ44 7853914487.
E-mail address: dwe@spinalmanipulation.org.uk (D.W. Evans).
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Manual Therapy
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/math
1356-689X/$ – see front matter Crown Copyright Ó2009 Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.math.2009.12.009
Manual Therapy 15 (2010) 286–291
characteristic shown to be surplus or redundant in any part of the
body.
Previous attempts at a definition of manipulation have appeared
in diverse sources of literature (representative examples are given
in Table 2), and reveal several notable features. Firstly, when
compared to the criteria in Table 1, it is clear that most of these
previous ‘definitions’ are actually descriptions. Furthermore, none
of these can qualify as definitive as there is variation, and discor-
dance, between them. Lastly, none is empirically-derived using the
existing basic science literature on manipulation; a process that has
the potential to identify characteristics that may distinguish
manipulation from other physical treatments.
One consistent attribute of previous ‘definitions’ is that they
relate to a physical intervention (oraction) that one person (usually
a practitioner) performs upon another (the recipient, who may be
a healthy subject or patient). General (colloquial) definitions of the
term manipulation focus entirely upon the action of the practi-
tioner, without conveying the potential importance of the events
that occur within the recipient. In comparison, many definitions in
a therapeutic context describe a proposed mechanical effect (or
response) within the recipient, which is caused by the action. This
mechanical response may be associated with distinct physiological,
neurological or psychological responses (Evans, 2002; Cramer
et al., 2006; Bolton and Budgell, 2006; Williams et al., 2007).
However, rather than including these secondary responses, which
have yet to be clearly delineated, we shall follow the convention of
prior definitions and limit our discussion to the action of the
practitioner and the passive mechanical response within the
recipient.
3. Features of manipulation
Several empirically-derived features are likely to be necessary to
define ‘manipulation’. A necessary feature should be applicable
irrespective of the body region in which manipulation is achieved.
We consider that, although each feature may not be unique to
manipulation, their combination will be. It is this combination that
will represent a framework to sufficiently define manipulation. We
have divided these features into two categories: (1) the ’action’
(that which the practitioner does to the recipient), and (2) the
accompanying ‘mechanical response’ (that which occurs within the
recipient). The merits of each identified feature are discussed
below.
3.1. Action (that which the practitioner ‘does’ to the patient)
3.1.1. A force is applied to the recipient
Manipulation involves a force being applied to the recipient.
Most commonly, this force is externally generated and is usually
applied to the recipient by physical contact at the skin surface
(Kawchuk et al., 1992; Herzog et al., 1993a, 2001; van Zoest and
Gosselin, 2003). The force may include reaction forces from furni-
ture, such as a plinth or chair (Kirstukas and Backman, 1999) and, in
some circumstances, gravitational force may be utilised. The
application of force is proposed to be a necessary feature for
a definition of manipulation.
3.1.2. The line of action of this force is perpendicular to the articular
surface of the affected joint
The earliest biomechanical studies to investigate what is now
termed ‘manipulation’ (Roston and Wheeler Haines, 1947; Uns-
worth et al., 1971) examined the phenomenon of ‘joint cracking’ in
metacarpophalangeal (MCP) joints. These studies investigated the
relationship between joint surface separation and the production of
a ‘crack’ sound (discussed in more detail later). Both studies used
a similar design to induce the cracking sound in that the surfaces of
MCP joints were separated using ‘traction force’, which was applied
along the length of the finger, perpendicular to the articular
surfaces. The results were equally simple: joint surface separation
beyond a certain magnitude created an obvious cracking sound and
an immediate increase in articular surface separation. For a short,
unspecified period this cracking noise could not be repeated; an
observation explained by reduced cohesion within synovial fluid
due to the presence of tiny bubbles (Unsworth et al., 1971;Mierau
et al., 1988;Evans, 2002). Similar findings were found in later
studies of MCP ‘joint cracking’ (Me
´al and Scott, 1986; Watson et al.,
1989).
Importantly, in every study in which the cracking sound in MCP
joints has been examined, the force has always been applied along
a line of action perpendicular to the articular surfaces of the
affected joint. Moreover, the motion produced by this force was
joint surface separation, without any obvious ‘gliding’ motion. As
synovial joint surfaces are designed to glide smoothly over one
another, the motion produced during this type of MCP joint
manipulation is hence distinguished from that produced during
typical ‘physiological’ motion.
A complexity of this feature is that most synovial joints are
curved rather than planar, and are not always congruent. Whereas
Table 1
A comparison of the criteria required for definitions and descriptions.
Definition Description
A statement expressing the essential
nature of something
Discourse intended to give a mental
image of something experimental
May be stipulated, or assigned
meaning
Must derive from observation
or experience
When applied to a class of
phenomena, must apply fully
to all members of the class
When applied to a class of phenomena,
may yield an aggregate set of features,
all of which need to apply to each
particular member of the class
From O’Connor et al., 1997.
Table 2
Previous definitions and descriptions of manipulation and spinal manipulation.
Definition/description (quotes) Source
General (colloquial)
To handle something, or move or work it with
the hands, especially in a skilful way
Chambers 21st Century
Dictionary, 2009
Therapeutic (general)
To apply therapeutic treatment with the hands
to (a part of the body)
Chambers 21st Century
Dictionary, 2009
The therapeutic application of manual force American Association of
Colleges of Osteopathic
Medicine, 2006
High velocity, low amplitude passive
movements that are applied directly
to the joint or through leverage
Chartered Society of
Physiotherapy, 2006
A manual procedure that involves a directed
thrust to move a joint past the physiological
range of motion, without exceeding
the anatomical limit
Gatterman and Hansen, 1994
Therapeutic (spinal)
Spinal manipulation is .the sudden
application of a force, whether by
manual or mechanical means, to any
part of a person’s body that affects a joint
or segment of the vertebral column’’
New South Wales
Department of Health, 2001
Spinal manipulation entails high velocity, low
amplitude manual thrusts to spinal
joints that extend slightly beyond
their physiological range of motion
Ernst, 2001
D.W. Evans, N. Lucas / Manual Therapy 15 (2010) 286–291 287
the line of action of the applied force may be perpendicular to one
point along the articular surface, this will not be the case with the
entire articular surface. Hence, the applied force may be more
accurately described as acting perpendicular to a plane that is
tangential to a point of contact between the articular surfaces of the
joint.
Although the relationship between forces applied to spinal
segments during manipulation procedures and the motion that
ensues is often assumed to be self-evident, the existence of
coupling patterns in spinal segments can preclude such certainty.
Limited kinematic data exist for spinal segmental and joint motions
during spinal manipulation procedures. However, the little data
that are available do appear to validate this proposed feature in the
spine (Evans, 2009).
Bereznick (2005) measured substantial force applied perpen-
dicular to the skin surface during side-posture lumbar manipula-
tion. Due to the negligible friction between the skin and the
underlying tissues (Bereznick et al., 2002), the line of action of the
majority of this force can be assumed to have been parallel to
the transverse plane of the recipient. Additionally, Cramer et al.
(2002) confirmed that the same side-posture lumbar spine
manipulation technique produces transverse rotation of lumbar
spinal segments. Since transverse rotation in the lumbar spine is
not obviously coupled with any other motion (Legaspi and Edmond,
2007), the applied force is again likely to act along the transverse
plane of the recipient. In turn, the approximately planar articular
surfaces of all lumbosacral posterior joints are perpendicular to the
transverse plane; typical lumbar zygapophysial joints (L1–L5) are
aligned close to the sagittal plane, whereas those of the lumbosa-
cral (L5–S1) joints are more frontally orientated (Giles, 1997; van
Schaik et al., 1997; Singer et al., 2004).
Evidence for a similar relationship exists in the thoracic spine.
Several studies (Herzog et al., 1993a, 2001; Ga
´l et al., 1995)have
shown that manipulation forces are applied in a posterior-anterior
direction, parallel with both sagittal and transverse planes, and
therefore perpendicular to the frontal plane. In contrast, the artic-
ular surfaces of typical thoracic zygapophysial joints (T4–10) are
known to be frontally orientated (Singer et al., 2004). Unfortu-
nately, there are limited kinematic data available for cervical spine
manipulation, but the small amount that does exist also provides
support for this proposed feature (Evans, 2009).
3.1.3. The magnitude of this force increases to a peak over a finite
period of time
The available data demonstrate that the magnitude of the
applied force varies considerably between individuals, but consis-
tently increases from zero over a finite period of time until a peak
force is reached, after which the magnitude decreases once again to
zero, in a single, non-repeating cycle. The increase and decrease of
the force is not necessarily linear, sometimes taking the form of
several distinct phases of unequal duration that vary with the
location of the manipulated joint (Roston and Wheeler Haines,
1947; Unsworth et al.,1971; Watson et al.,1989; Hessell et al., 1990;
Kawchuk et al., 1992; Kawchuk and Herzog, 1993; Herzog et al.,
1993a; Herzog, 2000).
These observations suggest temporal limits for manipulation
forces, in contrast to other manual therapeutic interventions (e.g.
mobilisation), which may consist of periodical, repeating phases
(Lee et al., 2000). However, it is difficult to justify that such force-
time constraints are necessary for manipulation. It is feasible that
manipulation could still be achieved if the force-time characteris-
tics varied from that typically observed. Furthermore, other inter-
ventions may be modified to share such characteristics. Hence,
specifying the time frame over which force is applied is not
currently proposed as a necessary criterion to define manipulation.
3.2. Mechanical response (that which occurs within the recipient)
3.2.1. The applied force produces motion at a joint
The force applied to the recipient induces motion between the
articular surfaces of a joint. This is a fundamental feature of
manipulation and other manual therapy interventions (Lee et al.,
2000), and is frequently indicated in previous descriptions and
definitions (e.g. Table 2). We consider this criterion to be necessary.
While manipulation is often applied with the intent of
producing an effect at a specific joint (or joints), research has
demonstrated that some manipulation techniques are not suffi-
ciently accurate to always affect the chosen, ‘target’ joint (Ross
et al., 2004). It is therefore more precise to refer to the ‘affected’
joint rather than the ‘target’ joint.
3.2.2. This joint motion always includes articular surface separation
The applied force induces motion between the articular surfaces
of the affected joint, and when measured, articular surface sepa-
ration (gapping) has always been observed (Roston and Wheeler
Haines, 1947; Unsworth et al., 1971;Mierau et al., 1988;Watson
et al., 1989; Watson and Mollan, 1990; Cramer et al., 2002). We
propose that this is a necessary criterion for a definition of
manipulation as few, if any, other manual therapeutic interventions
appear to produce this type of joint motion.
3.2.3. The velocity of joint motion is variable
One of manipulation’s most common pseudonyms is the ‘high
velocity–low amplitude thrust’ – a composition of biomechanical
terms frequently appearing in prior ‘definitions’ (e.g. Table 2).
Velocity is the rate of change of displacement with respect to time.
High velocity joint motion may occur during everyday activities
(e.g. throwing, running or kicking), as well as during passive
manual or instrument-assisted procedures (e.g. manipulation and
mobilisation). Hence, the velocity of joint motion alone cannot
define manipulation. Moreover, several studies have shown that
manipulation may be achieved at relatively low velocity joint
motions (Unsworth et al., 1971; Me
´al and Scott, 1986; Watson et al.,
1989;Suter et al., 1994). Thus, given the current available data,
velocity is not considered a necessary criterion.
3.2.4. The sum displacement of the articulating bones is usually
zero
Importance has been attached to the amplitude of joint motion
achieved during physical interventions, and a ‘grading’ system has
been proposed (Maitland, 1966). However, the sum (resultant)
displacement or deformation of tissue does not appear to be
a necessary feature for the achievement of manipulation. Assuming
that tissues have not undergone damage through being deformed
beyond their elastic limit, are no longer under the action of any
external force, and are under constant environmental temperature
(Watson et al., 1989; Kernohan et al., 1990) and pressure (Semlak
and Ferguson, 1970), all studies that have measured bone
displacement before and after manipulation show no lasting
change, once elastic tissue deformation has been allowed to recover
(Unsworth et al., 1971;Mierau et al., 1988;Watson et al., 1989; Ga
´l
et al., 1994, 1995, 1997; Tullberg et al., 1998; Cramer et al., 20 02). As
such, the final resultant displacement of the articulating bones
following a manipulation is usually zero. This raises some concern
with use of the term ‘adjustment’, which conveys a notion of lasting
tissue displacement.
This feature was considered useful as it distinguishes manipu-
lation from procedures to reduce a dislocation or realign fractured
bone. However, it is conceivable that a manipulation delivered with
excessive force may damage some of the joints restraining tissues,
and result in lasting tissue displacement or deformation. Moreover,
D.W. Evans, N. Lucas / Manual Therapy 15 (2010) 286–291288
a manipulation that induces tissue damage is still manipulation,
irrespective of an adverse outcome. Hence, the criterion for zero
tissue displacement seems unnecessary for the definition of
manipulation.
3.2.5. Cavitation occurs within the affected joint
Associated with joint surface separation is the elicitation of
a high frequency vibration that manifests as an audible ‘click’ or
‘crack’ sound (Roston and Wheeler Haines, 1947; Unsworth et al.,
1971; Watson et al., 1989). These vibrations are readily measured
using microphones or accelerometers, and have been investigated
in various joints across several studies (Me
´al and Scott, 1986;
Watson et al., 1989; Herzog et al., 1993b; Ga
´l et al., 1995; Reggars
and Pollard, 1995; Reggars, 1996a,b, 1999; Beffa and Mathews,
2004; Bolton et al., 2007).
The most likely and widely accepted explanation for this audible
sound during joint manipulation is a process known as cavitation,
occurring within the synovial fluid of the affected joint (Evans and
Breen, 2006). Cavitation is an engineering term used to describe the
formation and activity of bubbles (or cavities) within fluid, which
are formed when tension is applied to the fluid as a result of a local
reduction in pressure (Unsworth et al., 1971; Trevena, 1987; Young,
1999). Evidence for this explanation of the sound has come in
several forms.
There is face validity for cavitation as the explanatory mecha-
nism of ‘joint cracking’. The earliest scientific study of the
phenomenon identified articular surface separation as a key
component (Roston and Wheeler Haines, 1947). The characteristic
triphasic force–displacement graphs obtained during increasing
joint surface separation (Roston and Wheeler Haines, 1947; Uns-
worth et al., 1971; Watson et al., 1989), combined with the diver-
gent return pathway, are strongly suggestive of a rapid and
temporarily irreversible change in the cohesive properties of
synovial fluid, which was brought about by increased intra-artic-
ular volume and consequent decreased intra-articular pressure. In
synovial joints, the reduction in intra-articular pressure is likely
only achieved with a corresponding deformation of the joint
capsule (Brodeur, 1995), although this suggestion remains
speculative.
Radiographs have consistently demonstrated a radiolucent
region between the articular surfaces of the affected joint, imme-
diately following the elicitation of the sound, whilst these surfaces
remain separated (Fick, 1911; Dittmar, 1933; Nordheim, 1938; Fuiks
and Grayson, 1950; Unsworth et al., 1971; Watson and Mollan,
1990). No study has measured how long this state may persist by
continuously maintaining joint surface separation, although theo-
retically this could be indefinitely (Roston and Wheeler Haines,
1947 ).
Finally, several studies have shown that the sound cannot be
elicited more than once within a relatively short period of time
after the articular surfaces of the affected joint are allowed to return
to their resting configuration (Roston and Wheeler Haines, 1947;
Unsworth et al., 1971); a period that has been shown to extend as
long as 90 min following lumbar spine manipulation (Bereznick
et al., 2008). Furthermore, the location and quantity of these high
frequency vibrations recorded during manipulation procedures in
the spine is consistent with them originating from the synovial
zygapophysial joints (Ross et al., 2004; Bereznick et al., 2008).
One may ask whether cavitation is a necessary feature of
manipulation? Physiological changes may take place during
‘manipulation’ in the absence of cavitation (e.g. electromyographic
signals). However, cavitation is associated with distinct osteoki-
nematics (Unsworth et al., 1971; Watson et al., 1989; Watson and
Mollan, 1990; Ga
´l et al., 1995; Cramer et al., 2002). In addition,
clinicians frequently regard cavitation as an indicator of success in
the technical delivery of a manipulation (Evans and Breen, 2006).
Conversely, some commentators consider cavitation to be an
unnecessary outcome of manipulation because research has yet to
demonstrate an association with clinical outcomes (Flynn et al.,
2003, 2006). Nevertheless, for the purpose of defining manipula-
tion, the clinical success, or otherwise, of the intervention is irrel-
evant. By corollary, the occurrence of surgery, acupuncture or any
other physical intervention would not be defined by a successful or
failed clinical outcome. Cavitation may also, on occasion, occur
spontaneously during everyday movements, or during extreme
joint motions that may damage a joint. Hence, the occurrence of
cavitation in isolation cannot constitute a definition of
manipulation.
We propose that cavitation is a necessary feature of manipula-
tion. However, we are aware that the inclusion of this criterion will
be controversial for the reasons given above. It is also reasonable to
argue that cavitation is not the intended outcome of other types of
manual therapeutic interventions. For example, traction of
peripheral joints has been shown to result in joint surface separa-
tion (Hsu et al., 2008). If such a procedure resulted in cavitation,
then this would, by definition, be a manipulation. By contrast,
traction of the lumbar spine does not result in zygapophysial joint
surface separation (Humke et al., 1996); a likely consequence of the
complex kinematics of spinal segments (Evans, 2009). Alterna-
tively, if all other proposed criteria were present, yet cavitation was
not achieved, this would not fulfil all necessary criteria of
a ‘manipulation’ so should not be referred to as such.
4. Summary
Of the features discussed above, those we propose to be
necessary for the achievement of manipulation are summarised in
Table 3. We have attempted to retain the minimum number of
features. Collectively, these features should sufficiently constitute
the required components of a valid definition. Used in isolation,
each of these features is insufficient to define manipulation; their
sufficiency is dependent upon their collective occurrence. This is
consistent with defining causal mechanisms as a set of factors that
are jointly sufficient to induce an outcome event (Rothman, 1976);
under the omission of just one factor, the outcome would be
different.
Fig. 1 demonstrates the relationship of the proposed necessary
features of manipulation compared to other manual therapy
interventions, illustrating their potential importance within
a wider empirically-derived taxonomy of manual therapy.
An important attribute of our proposed features is that they
are explicitly divided into two categories: the ‘action’ (that which
the practitioner does to the recipient) and the ’mechanical
response’ (that which occurs within the recipient). Interestingly,
Table 3
Proposed necessity of manipulation features.
Necessary
Action (that which the practitioner does to the recipient)
A force is applied to the recipient Yes
The line of action of this force is perpendicular to the
articular surface of the affected joint
Yes
The magnitude of this force increases to a peak
over a finite period of time
No
Mechanical response (that which occurs within the recipient)
The applied force creates motion at a joint Yes
This joint motion includes articular surface separation Yes
The velocity of joint motion is variable No
The sum displacement of the articulating bones is usually zero No
Cavitation occurs within the affected joint Yes
D.W. Evans, N. Lucas / Manual Therapy 15 (2010) 286–291 289
whilst all of the ‘action’ features are included at the discretion of
the practitioner (and if any are excluded, the minimally sufficient
criteria for ‘manipulation’ would not be met), there is a causative
chain in operation with the ‘response’ features; once all of the
‘action’ components have been achieved, the induction of some
joint motion is necessary for the occurrence of joint surface
separation, and in turn this is necessary for the occurrence of
cavitation (Fig. 1).
5. Conclusion
We have identified empirically-derived features of manipula-
tion that we propose to be necessary for a valid definition, and have
provided arguments for and against their inclusion in such a defi-
nition. In addition, we have specified that each feature must occur
in order that the required defining criteria for manipulation are met
and that it be clearly distinguished from other manual therapeutic
interventions within a wider empirically-derived taxonomy of
manual therapy.
References
American Association of Colleges of Osteopathic Medicine. Glossary of osteopathic
terminology. The Educational Council on Osteopathic Principles and the
American Association of Colleges of Osteopathic Medicine. Available at: http://
www.osteopathic.org/pdf/sir_collegegloss.pdf; 2006.
Beffa R, Mathews R. Does the adjustment cavitate the targeted joint? An investi-
gation into the location of cavitation sounds. J Manipulative Physiol Ther
2004;27(2):e2.
Bereznick DE. Lumbar manipulation: quantification and modification of the
external kinetics affecting the presence and site of cavitation (PhD thesis).
Ontario: University of Waterloo; 2005.
Bereznick DE, Ross JK, McGill SM. The frictional properties at the thoracic skin–
fascia interface: implications in spine manipulation. Clin Biomech (Bristol Avon)
2002;17(4):297–303.
Bereznick DE, Pecora CG, Ross JK, McGill SM. The refractory period of the audible
‘‘crack’’ after lumbar manipulation: a preliminary study. J Manipulative Physiol
Ther 2008;31:199–203.
Bolton PS, Budgell BS. Spinal manipulation and spinal mobilization influence
different axial sensory beds. Med Hypotheses 2006;66(2):258–62.
BoltonA,MoranRW,StandenC.Aninvestigationintothesideofjointcavitation
associated with cervical spine manipulation. Int J Ost Med 2007;10(4):
88–96.
Brodeur R. The audible release associated with joint manipulation. J Manipulative
Physiol Ther 1995;18(3):155–64.
Chartered Society of Physiotherapy. Clinical guidelines for the physiotherapy
management of persistent low back pain. Part 2: manual therapy. London:
Chartered Society of Physiotherapy; 2006. p. 32.
Chambers 21st century dictionary. Available at. Glasgow: HarperCollins, http://
www.chambersharrap.co.uk/chambers/features/chref/chref.py/
main?query¼retribution&title¼21st; 2009 [accessed January 2009].
Colloca CJ, Keller TS, Gunzburg R. Biomechanical and neurophysiological responses
to spinal manipulation in patients with lumbar radiculopathy. J Manipulative
Physiol Ther 2004;27(1):1–15.
Colloca CJ, Keller TS, Harrison DE, Moore RJ, Gunzburg R, Harrison DD. Spinal
manipulation force and duration affect vertebral movement and neuromuscular
responses. Clin Biomech (Bristol Avon) 2006;21(3):254–62.
Coulter ID, Hurwitz EL, Adams AH, Meeker WC, Hansen DT, Mootz RD, et al. The
appropriateness of spinal manipulation and mobilization of the cervical spine.
Santa Monica: RAND. Available at: http://www.rand.org/pubs/monograph_
reports/2007/MR781.pdf; 1996.
Cramer GD, Gregerson DM, Knudsen JT, Hubbard BB, Ustas LM, Cantu JA. The effects
of side-posture positioning and spinal adjusting on the lumbar Z joints:
a randomized controlled trial with sixty-four subjects. Spine 2002;27(22):
2459–66.
Cramer G, Budgell B, Henderson C, Khalsa P, Pickar J. Basic science research related
to chiropractic spinal adjusting: the state of the art and recommendations
revisited. J Manipulative Physiol Ther 2006;29(9):726–61.
Dittmar O. Zu
¨rRo
¨ntgenologie des Kniegelenks. Verhandlaingen Deutschen Ortho-
pedischen Gesellschaft (27 Congr., Mannheim, 1932); 1933.
Ernst E. Life-threatening complications of spinal manipulation. Stroke 2001;32(3):
809–10.
Fig. 1. The relationship of the proposed necessary features of manipulation, compared with other manual therapy interventions.
D.W. Evans, N. Lucas / Manual Therapy 15 (2010) 286–291290
Ernst E. Adverse effects of spinal manipulation: a systematic review. J R Soc Med
2007;100(7):330–8.
Evans DW. Mechanisms and effects of spinal high-velocity, low-amplitude thrust
manipulation: previous theories. J Manipulative Physiol Ther 2002;25(4):
251–62.
Evans DW, Breen AC. A biomechanical model for mechanically efficient cavitation
production during spinal manipulation: prethrust position and the neutral
zone. J Manipulative Physiol Ther 2006;29(1):72–82.
Evans DW. Why do spinal manipulation techniques take the form they do? Towards
a general model of spinal manipulation. Man Ther 2009;. doi:10.1016/
j.math.2009.03.006.
Fick R. Zum Streit um den Gelenkdruck. Anat Hefte (Abt. 1) 1911;43:397.
Flynn TW, Fritz JM, Wainner RS, Whitman JM. The audible pop is not necessary for
successful spinal high-velocity thrust manipulation in individuals with low
back pain. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2003;84(7):1057–60.
Flynn TW, Childs JD, Fritz JM. The audible pop from high-velocity thrust manipu-
lation and outcome in individuals with low back pain. J Manipulative Physiol
Ther 2006;29(1):40–5.
Fuiks DM, Grayson CE. Vacuum pneumarthrography and the spontaneous occur-
rence of gas in the joint spaces. J Bone Jt Surg Am 1950;32A(4):933–8.
Ga
´l J, Herzog W, Kawchuk G, Conway P, Zhang Y-T. Biomechanical studies of spinal
manipulative therapy (SMT): quantifying the movements of vertebral bodies
during SMT. J Can Chiropractic Assoc 1994;38(1):11–24.
Ga
´l JM, Herzog W, Kawchuk GN, Conway PJ, Zhang Y-T. Forces and relative vertebral
movements during SMT to unembalmed post-rigor human cadavers: pecu-
liarities associated with joint cavitation. J Manipulative Physiol Ther 1995;18:
4–9.
Ga
´l J, Herzog W, Kawchuk G, Conway PJ, Zhang YT. Movements of vertebrae during
manipulative thrusts to unembalmed human cadavers. J Manipulative Physiol
Ther 1997;20(1):30–40.
Gatterman M, Hansen D. The development of chiropractic nomenclature through
consensus. J Manipulative Physiol Ther 1994;17(5):302–9.
Gatterman MI, Cooperstein R, Lantz C, Perle SM, Schneider MJ. Rating specific
chiropractic technique procedures for common low back conditions. J Manip-
ulative Physiol Ther 2001;24(7):449–56.
Giles LGF. Zygapophysial (facet) joints. In: Giles LGF, Singer KP, editors. Clinical
anatomy and management of low back pain. Oxford: Butterworth Heinemann;
1997. p. 72–96 [chapter 5].
Harvey E, Burton AK, Moffett JK, Breen A, UK BEAM trial team. Spinal manipulation
for low-back pain: a treatment package agreed to by the UK chiropractic,
osteopathy and physiotherapy professional associations. Man Ther 2003;8(1):
46–51.
Herzog W. The mechanical, neuromuscular, and physiologic effects produced by
spinal manipulation. In: Herzog W, editor. Clinical biomechanics of spinal
manipulation. New York: Churchill Livingstone; 2000. p. 191–207 [chapter 5].
Herzog W, Conway PJ, Kawchuk GN, Zhang Y, Hasler EM. Forces exerted during
spinal manipulative therapy. Spine 1993a;18(9):1206–12.
Herzog W, Zhang YT, Conway PJ, Kawchuk GN. Cavitation sounds during spinal
manipulative treatments. J Manipulative Physiol Ther 1993b;16(8):523–6.
Herzog W, Kats M, Symons B. The effective forces transmitted by high-speed, low-
amplitude thoracic manipulation. Spine 2001;26(19):2105–10.
Hessell BW, Herzog W, Conway PJ, McEwen MC. Experimental measurement of the
force exerted during spinal manipulation using the Thompson technique.
J Manipulative Physiol Ther 1990;13(8):448–53.
Hsu AT, Chiu JF, Chang JH. Biomechanical analysis of axial distraction mobilization
of the glenohumeral joint – a cadaver study. Man Ther 2009;14(4):381–6.
Humke T, Grob D, Grauer W, Sandler A, Dvorak J. Foraminal changes with distrac-
tion and compression of the L4/5 and L5/S1 segments. Eur Spine J 1996;5(3):
183–6.
Kawchuk GN, Herzog W, Hasler EM. Forces generated during spinal manipulative
therapy of the cervical spine: a pilot study. J Manipulative Physiol Ther
1992;15(5):275–8.
Kawchuk GN, Herzog W. Biomechanical characterization (fingerprinting) of five
novel methods of cervical spine manipulation. J Manipulative Physiol Ther
1993;16(9):573–7.
Keller A, Hayden J, Bombardier C, van Tulder M. Effect sizes of non-surgical treat-
ments of non-specific low-back pain. Eur Spine J 2007;16(11):1776–88.
Keller TS, Colloca CJ, Beliveau JG. Force-deformation response of the lumbar spine:
a sagittal plane model of posteroanterior manipulation and mobilization. Clin
Biomech (Bristol Avon) 2002;17(3):185–96.
Kernohan WG, Beverland DE, McCoy GF, Hamilton A, Watson P, Mollan R. Vibration
arthrometry. A preview. Acta Orthop Scand 1990;61(1):70–9.
Kirstukas SJ, Backman JA. Physician-applied contact pressure and table force
response during unilateral thoracic manipulation. J Manipulative Physiol Ther
1999;22(5):269–79.
Lee M, Ga
´l JM, Herzog W. Biomechanics of manual therapy. In: Dvir Z, editor.
Clinical biomechanics. Philadelphia: Churchill Livingstone; 2000. p. 209–38
[chapter 9].
Legaspi O, Edmond SL. Does the evidence support the existence of lumbar spine
coupled motion? A critical review of the literature. J Orthop Sports Phys Ther
2007;37(4):169–78.
Maitland GD. Manipulation–mobilisation. Physiotherapy 1966;52(11):382–5.
Me
´al GM, Scott RA. Analysis of the joint crack by simultaneous recording of sound
and tension. J Manipulative Physiol Ther 1986;9(3):189–95.
Mierau D, Cassidy JD, Bowen V, Dupuis P, Noftall F. Manipulation and mobilization
of the third metacarpophalangeal joint. Man Med 1988;3:135–40.
New South Wales Department of Health. Definition quoted in Chiropractors Bill and
Osteopaths Bill. Availableat: http://www.parliament.nsw.gov.au/prod/parlment/
HansArt.nsf/448b6decbe0283adca2571e1001663bb/
ca256d110 00bd3aaca256a2a0027baee!OpenDocument;2001.
Nordheim Y. Eine neue Methode den Gelenkk norpel besonders die Kniege-
lenkmenisken ro
¨ntgenologisch darzu stellen. Fortschr Ro
¨ntgenstr 1938;57:
479.
O’Connor BB, Calabrese C, Carden
˜a E, Eisenberg D, Fincher J, Hufford DJ, et al. (Panel
on definition and description, CAM research methodology conference, April
1995). Defining and describing complementary and alternative medicine.
Altern Therap 1997;3(2):49–57.
Reggars JW. The manipulative crack: frequency analysis. Aust Chiro Osteopath
1996a;5(2):39–44.
Reggars JW. Recording techniques and analysis of the articular crack: a critical
review of the literature. Aust Chiro Osteopath 1996b;5(3):86–92.
Reggars JW. Multiple channel recording of the articular crack associated with
manipulation of the metacarpophalangeal joint: an observational study. Aust
Chiro Osteopath 1999;8(1):16–20.
Reggars JW, Pollard HP. Analysis of zygapophyseal joint cracking during chiropractic
manipulation. J Manipulative Physiol Ther 1995;18(2):65–71.
Ross JK, Bereznick DE, McGill SM. Determining cavitation location during lumbar
and thoracic spinal manipulation: is spinal manipulation accurate and specific?
Spine 2004;29(13):1452–7.
Roston JB, Wheeler Haines R. Cracking in the metacarpo-phalangeal joint. J Anat
1947;81(Pt 2):165–73.
Rothman KJ. Causes. Am J Epidemiol 1976;104:587–92.
Semlak K, Ferguson AB. Joint stability maintained by atmospheric pressure. An
experimental study. Clin Orthop Relat Res 1970;68:294–300.
Shekelle PG, Adams AH, Chassin MR, Hurwitz EL, Park RE, Phillips RB, et al. The
appropriate use of spinal manipulation for back pain: indications and ratings by
a multi-disciplinary expert panel. Santa Monica: RAND. Available at: http://
www.rand.org/pubs/reports/2007/R4025.2.pdf;1991.
Singer KP, Boyle JJW, Fazey P. Comparative anatomy of the zygapophysi al joints.
In: Boyling JD, Jull G, editors. Grieve ’s modern manu al therapy: the
vertebral column. 3rd ed. Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone; 2004. p. 17–29
[chapter 3].
Skyba DA, Radhakrishnan R, Rohlwing JJ, Wright A, Sluka KA. Joint manipulation
reduces hyperalgesia by activation of monoamine receptors but not opioid or
GABA receptors in the spinal cord. Pain 2003;106(1–2):159–68.
Song XJ, Gan Q, Cao JL, Wang ZB, Rupert RL. Spinal manipulation reduces pain and
hyperalgesia after lumbar intervertebral foramen inflammation in the rat.
J Manipulative Physiol Ther 2006;29(1):5–13.
Suter E, Herzog W, Conway PJ, Zhang YT. Reflex response associated with manip-
ulative treatment of the thoracic spine. J Neuromusculoskelet Syst 1994;2:
124–30.
Trevena DH. Cavitation and tension in liquids. Bristol: Adam Hilger; 1987.
Tullberg T, Blomberg S, Branth B, Johnsson R. Manipulation does not alter the
position of the sacroiliac joint. A roentgen stereophotogrammetric analysis.
Spine 1998;23(10):1124–8.
Unsworth A, Dowson D, Wright V. ‘Cracking joints’. A bioengineering study of
cavitation in the metacarpophalangeal joint. Ann Rheum Dis 1971;30(4):
348–58.
van der Velde G, Hogg-Johnson S, Bayoumi AM, Cassidy JD, Co
ˆte
´P, Boyle E, et al.
Identifying the best treatment among common nonsurgical neck pain treat-
ments: a decision analysis. Spine 2008;33(4 Suppl.):S184–91.
van Schaik JP, van Pinxteren B, Verbiest H, Crowe A, Zuiderveld KJ. The facet
orientation circle. A new parameter for facet joint angulation in the lower
lumbar spine. Spine 1997;22(5):531–6.
van Zoest GGJM, Gosselin G. Three-dimensionality of direct contact forces in
chiropractic spinal manipulative therapy. J Manipulative Physiol Ther 2003;26:
549–56.
Watson P, Kernohan WG, Mollan RA. A study of the cracking sounds from the
metacarpophalangeal joint. Proc Inst Mech Eng [H] 1989;203(2):109–18.
Watson P, Mollan RA. Cineradiography of a cracking joint. Br J Radiol
1990;63(746):145–7.
Williams NH, Hendry M, Lewis R, Russell I, Westmoreland A, Wilkinson C.
Psychological response in spinal manipulation (PRISM): a systematic review of
psychological outcomes in randomised controlled trials. Complement Ther Med
2007;15(4):271–83.
Young FR. Cavitation. London: Imperial College Press; 1999.
D.W. Evans, N. Lucas / Manual Therapy 15 (2010) 286–291 291
... Consequently, less commonly used techniques are not represented in the current article. In 2010, Evans and Lucas [36] differentiated between definition and description while investigating a robust definition for manipulation. Their criteria [37] for new definitions overlapped with the framework our FGs struggled through to describe techniques. ...
... For example, a common term-like manipulation should be easily understood by all practitioners. However, Evans and Lucas [36,37] reported major differences between professions regarding the use and understanding of manipulation, as is identified in the interprofessional glossary. The work of Evans and Lucas [36,37] and the work of the ICMT illustrate the necessity of sustaining an interprofessional glossary. ...
... However, Evans and Lucas [36,37] reported major differences between professions regarding the use and understanding of manipulation, as is identified in the interprofessional glossary. The work of Evans and Lucas [36,37] and the work of the ICMT illustrate the necessity of sustaining an interprofessional glossary. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective The International Consortium on Manual Therapies (ICMT) is a grassroots interprofessional association open to any formally trained practitioner of manual therapy (MT) and basic scientists promoting research related to the practice of MT. Currently, MT research is impeded by professions’ lack of communication with other MT professions, biases, and vernacular. Current ICMT goals are to minimize these barriers, compare MT techniques, and establish an interprofessional MT glossary. Methods Practitioners from all professions with training in manual therapies were encouraged by e-mail and website to participate (www.ICMTConferene.org). Video conferences were conducted at least bimonthly for 2.5 years by profession-specific and interprofessional focus groups (FGs). Members summarized scopes of practice, technique descriptions, associated mechanisms of action (MOA), and glossary terms. Each profession presented their work to the interprofessional FG to promote dialogue, understanding and consensus. Outcomes were reported and refined at numerous public events. Results Focus groups with representatives from 5 MT professions, chiropractic, massage therapy, osteopathic, physical therapy and structural integration identified 17 targeting osseous structures and 49 targeting nonosseous structures. Thirty-two techniques appeared distinct to a specific profession, and 13 were used by more than 1. Comparing descriptions identified additional commonalities. All professions agreed on 4 MOA categories for MT. A glossary of 280 terms and definitions was consolidated, representing key concepts in MT. Twenty-one terms were used by all MT professions and basic scientists. Five terms were used by MT professions exclusive of basic scientists. Conclusion Outcomes suggested a third to a half of techniques used in MT are similar across professions. Additional research is needed to better define the extent of similarity and how to consistently identify those approaches. Ongoing expansion and refinement of the glossary is necessary to promote descriptive clarity and facilitate communication between practitioners and basic scientists.
... In 2010 [10], we not only argued that the features listed in Table 1 were necessary; we also argued that these features were minimally sufficient, being the fewest number of features that collectively were sufficient in describing the characteristics of manipulation as it may occur in any synovial joint in the body. In this regard, it is reassuring that these necessary features fit neatly into a causal pathway without any obvious gaps (Fig. 1). ...
... Existing definitions of manipulation (e.g., those in Table 2) clearly broaden the scope of components beyond those we suggested in 2010 [10]. Hence, before focusing on the individual components that should constitute a new definition of manipulation, consideration needs to be given to the types of components that should be included and excluded, which need to be selected in a systematic manner. ...
... For pharmaceutical interventions, a biological target is typically a tissue receptor (at which effects are usually either agonistic or antagonistic). For manipulation, there is ample evidence that the biological target is a synovial joint [10,25,[35][36][37][38][39][40][41][42]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Definitions are important in healthcare. Unfortunately, problems can be found with all existing definitions of manipulation. Methods This paper derives a set of eligibility criteria from prior definitions of manipulation to inform what should (and should not) be incorporated within a valid definition. These criteria were then used to select components from currently available empirical data to create a new definition. Results The resulting definition of manipulation is: “Separation (gapping) of opposing articular surfaces of a synovial joint, caused by a force applied perpendicularly to those articular surfaces, that results in cavitation within the synovial fluid of that joint.” The corresponding definition for the mechanical response of a manipulation is: “Separation (gapping) of opposing articular surfaces of a synovial joint that results in cavitation within the synovial fluid of that joint.” In turn, the action of a manipulation can be defined as: “A force applied perpendicularly to the articular surfaces.” Conclusions We believe these definitions to be valid (derived from and consistent with all available empirical data), complete (containing all necessary components), minimally sufficient (minimal redundancy, and sufficient to distinguish manipulation from other physical interventions), and robust (able to withstand important limitations embodied within sensible eligibility criteria). It is hoped that the simplicity and clarity of these definitions, and the transparency of their formation, will encourage their wide adoption in clinical, research, educational and professional settings.
... If one attempts to read through the extensive literature that relates to manipulation (in the manual therapy context), it becomes very noticeable that literally dozens of definitions and descriptions have been proposed [1]. Often, these definitions conflict with one another and on occasion can be found in unexpected places [2], such as within primary legislation [3,4]. ...
... The composition of definitions listed in Table 1 is worthy of attention. Firstly, most of them commit to specifying a joint as the unit of manipulation, which deserves credit [1]. Beyond this, however, one can easily find flaws. ...
... Indeed, the wording of most current definitions, [10], was "Typical load-separation curve for a cracking joint" Fig. 4 Cavitation occurring between the articular surfaces of synovial joints. Based on Chen et al. [12] including those listed in Table 1, is derived from Sandoz's two-dimensional arc-shaped model, his terminology and conclusions [1,2]. There is, however, one problem with this: Sandoz got the most important element of his model totally and utterly wrong! ...
Article
Full-text available
For manipulation, this paper addresses arguably the most fundamental question that can be asked about any therapeutic intervention: what is it ? In answering this question, this paper presents the prevailing model of joint manipulation (of Sandoz) and explains why this influential model is fundamentally flawed. The early research on ‘joint cracking’ that led to the development of this model is described in chronological order, alongside how this research was misinterpreted, which gave rise to the model’s flaw. Of concern, the flaw in this model makes worrying predictions that could lead to dangerous clinical decisions. Understandably, these predictions have attracted criticism over the use of manipulation as a therapeutic intervention. A corrected model, first published by Evans and Breen more than 15 years ago, is then presented and explained. Unlike the flawed model, this corrected model makes predictions in line with all available empirical data and additionally provides reassuring answers to critics. Many current definitions of manipulation have inherited the flaw from Sandoz’s model. Hence, a better, empirically derived definition, consistent with the corrected model, is now required.
... Manual therapy is a form of physical treatment used by several healthcare professionals to treat MSK pain and disability through the use of mobilization and manipulation of the body's neuro-musculoskeletal structures to improve mobility and function [34][35][36]. It has been pointed out as a highly beneficial therapeutic approach to MSK pain treatment, being more ecological and cost-effective, and with a lower risk of adverse events compared to pharmacotherapy [37][38][39]. ...
Article
Full-text available
(1) Background: The aim of this review was to evaluate the effects of manual therapy for musculoskeletal pain in menopausal women. (2) Methods: A comprehensive search of manuscripts published from inception until 29 February 2024 was conducted on PubMed, EBSCO Information Services (EBSCOhost), Physiotherapy Evidence Database (Pedro), Web of Science, Scientific Electronic Library Online (SciELO), Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Clinical Trials, and Scopus using Medical Subject Headings (MeSH) and free terms. Randomized controlled trials (RCT) investigating the effects of manual therapy for musculoskeletal pain in postmenopausal women were included. Articles published in non-English or non-Portuguese languages, case reports, expert opinions, dissertations, conference papers, and gray literature were excluded. Studies were screened for population, musculoskeletal pain, intervention, and pain outcome by two independent reviewers using an ad hoc data extraction form. (3) Results: A total of 5 RCTs (4 with high risk of bias and 1 with low risk of bias) were included (total sample = 245), addressing thumb carpometacarpal osteoarthritis, thoracic kyphosis, chronic neck and backache, knee osteoarthritis, and sternocostal joint pain. The combined results of these studies showed improved musculoskeletal pain in menopausal women; duration of the follow-up was between 4 weeks to 6 months. Conclusions: The majority of studies included in this systematic review were effective in reducing musculoskeletal pain in menopausal women. These results come mainly from studies with a high risk of bias with small sample sizes, and the most representative follow-up period was short-term. Therefore, the results of this systematic review should be interpreted with caution.
... The popping sound is still a controversial topic for both the effectiveness (i.e., by influencing clinical outcomes) and the definition of SM. Notably, IFOMPT included the popping sound in its definition of SM [3]; furthermore, Evans and Lucas [69] concluded that the popping sound is one of the five necessary criterion for a valid definition of SM. Nevertheless, in the current survey, most Italian physiotherapists did not consider the popping sound as an important indicator for the successful technical delivery of SM. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background and Objective High-velocity low-amplitude thrust spinal manipulation (SM) is a recommended and commonly used manual therapy intervention in physiotherapy. Beliefs surrounding the safety and effectiveness of SM have challenged its use, and even advocated for its abandonment. Our study aimed to investigate the knowledge and beliefs surrounding SM by Italian physiotherapists compared with similar practitioners in other countries. Methods An online survey with 41 questions was adapted from previous surveys and was distributed via a mailing list of the Italian Physiotherapists Association (March 22–26, 2020). The questionnaire was divided into 4 sections to capture information on participant demographics, utilization, potential barriers, and knowledge about SM. Questions were differentiated between spinal regions. Attitudes towards different spinal regions, attributes associated with beliefs, and the influence of previous educational background were each evaluated. Results Of the 7398 registered physiotherapists, 575 (7.8%) completed the survey and were included for analysis. The majority of respondents perceived SM as safe and effective when applied to the thoracic (74.1%) and lumbar (72.2%) spines; whereas, a smaller proportion viewed SM to the upper cervical spine (56.8%) as safe and effective. Respondents reported they were less likely to provide and feel comfortable with upper cervical SM (respectively, 27.5% and 48.5%) compared to the thoracic (respectively, 52.2% and 74.8%) and lumbar spines (respectively, 46.3% and 74.3%). Most physiotherapists (70.4%) agreed they would perform additional screening prior to upper cervical SM compared to other spinal regions. Respondents who were aware of clinical prediction rules were more likely to report being comfortable with SM (OR 2.38–3.69) and to perceive it as safe (OR 1.75–3.12). Finally, physiotherapists without musculoskeletal specialization, especially those with a traditional manual therapy background, were more likely to perform additional screening prior to SM, use SM less frequently, report being less comfortable performing SM, and report upper cervical SM as less safe ( p < 0.001). Discussion The beliefs and attitudes of physiotherapists surrounding the use of SM are significantly different when comparing the upper cervical spine to other spinal regions. An educational background in traditional manual therapy significantly influences beliefs and attitudes. We propose an updated framework on evidence-based SM.
... 3,16,17 The audible popping sound is hypothesized to be the main characteristic of an effective TM. 2,[18][19][20][21][22][23][24] It is also hypothesized that the absence of a popping sound during TM potentially increases the patient's perception of an ineffective intervention. [25][26][27] The few systematic reviews that have analyzed the topic found no differences between TM and sham TM treatments (ie, a hands-on technique without thrust and popping sound). ...
Article
Objective: The purpose of this study was to assess whether beliefs about the origin of the popping sound and the effects of thrust manipulation (TM) were in agreement with current scientific evidence and whether a practitioner's explanation could influence patient beliefs of theoretical mechanisms. Methods: A cross-sectional online survey was conducted in Italy from January 7, 2019 to April 20, 2019. The questionnaire was sent to 900 Italian adults through online recruitment, including people with and without a history of manipulation, such as given by physiotherapists, chiropractors, osteopaths, and manual medicine physicians to manage musculoskeletal disorders. The questionnaire consisted of 11 multiple-choice questions and could be completed within 15 weeks. The Likert scale was used to investigate participants' attitudes. Sex and previous experience of TM variables were evaluated using a Student's t-test; a 1-way F analysis of variance test was performed to evaluate age, educational qualification, and the professional who performed the TM. Results: We retrieved 478 questionnaires, including 175 participants with no TM history and 303 with TM history. There were 31% of participants (n = 94) with a history of TM who reported they did not receive explanations regarding manipulation. The participants' beliefs mostly disagreed with the current hypotheses provided by the scientific literature on the theoretical mechanisms of popping sound (tribonucleation and cavitation). There were 9.9% (n = 30) of participants who answered "realignment of bone positional fault" to explain the mechanism behind TM. There was a high degree of agreement with the belief that the popping sound should be present for a successful TM (respectively, 2.8 standard deviation [SD; 1.2] and 2.6 SD [1.2] for TM+ and TM- participants). No statistically significant differences were found between participants with and without a history of TM. Conclusion: The participants in this study reported a belief that popping was related to effectiveness of TM. A high percentage of this sample had beliefs about TM mechanisms for the audible popping sound that were inconsistent with current literature. Beliefs were similar between groups, suggesting that instructions given by TM practitioners did not seem to be an influence on these patients' beliefs.
... My first argument is that the PPS is an illusion and therefore unable to hold credibility in discussions of the clinical act of manipulation and the specific techniques including the chiropractic thrust that are gathered by the catch-all term 'manipulation'. 26 Indeed, I classify 'manipulation' as a vapid term impractical to define with specificity and disagree with its use as representative of the chiropractic-specific clinical act of spinal adjustment by finely controlled manual thrust. ...
Research
Full-text available
Objective: The objective of this paper is to demonstrate that the paraphysiological space described by Sandoz is an illusion and to show that the proposition of Australian authorities that spinal manipulation takes a spinal joint beyond its range of physiological motion is not credible.
Article
Full-text available
Introduction High velocity thrust manipulation is commonly used when managing joint dysfunctions. Often, these thrust maneuvers will elicit an audible pop. It has been unclear what conclusively causes this audible sound and its clinical meaningfulness. This study sought to identify the effect of the audible pop on brainwave activity directly following a prone T7 thrust manipulation in asymptomatic/healthy subjects. Methods This was a quasi-experimental repeated measure study design in which 57 subjects completed the study protocol. Brain wave activity was measured with the Emotiv EPOC+, which collects data with a frequency of 128 HZ and has 14 electrodes. Testing was performed in a controlled environment with minimal electrical interference (as measured with a Gauss meter), temperature variance, lighting variance, sound pollution, and other variable changes that could have influenced or interfered with pure EEG data acquisition. After accommodation each subject underwent a prone T7 posterior-anterior thrust manipulation. Immediately after the thrust manipulation the brainwave activity was measured for 10 seconds. Results The non-audible group ( N = 20) consisted of 55% males, and the audible group ( N = 37) consisted of 43% males. The non-audible group EEG data revealed a significant change in brain wave activity under some of the electrodes in the frontal, parietal, and the occipital lobes. In the audible group, there was a significant change in brain wave activity under all electrodes in the frontal lobes, the parietal lobe, and the occipital lobes but not the temporal lobes. Conclusion The audible sounds caused by a thoracic high velocity thrust manipulation did not affect the activity in the audible centers in the temporal brain region. The results support the hypothesis that thrust manipulation with or without audible sound results in a generalized relaxation immediately following the manipulation. The absence of a significant difference in brainwave activity in the frontal lobe in this study might indicate that the audible pop does not produce a “placebo” mechanism.
Article
Background: Manual therapy (MT) is commonly used to manage low back pain (LBP) and involves a complex interaction between the practitioner and patient. Attitudes and beliefs about MT may play a role in the outcomes seen in patients experiencing LBP. However, knowledge of patients' attitudes and beliefs regarding MT is currently limited. Objective: To map the existing published literature on the attitudes and beliefs about MT in patients experiencing LBP. Design: Scoping Review. Method: A systematic search was conducted across the PubMed, CINAHL, PsycINFO and Scopus databases. Study selection involved screening 1) title and abstracts and 2) full text articles. Data was analyzed to provide a descriptive summary of the studies and to develop themes of patients' attitudes and beliefs about MT. Results: A total of 767 records were identified from the initial search strategy. Following study selection, 14 articles were included for data analysis. Five themes related to patients' attitudes and beliefs about MT were developed from the existing literature. Attitudes and beliefs about MT are explored and measured inconsistently with only one validated tool available. Conclusion: MT is believed to be a preferential and effective treatment with accepted levels of post-treatment soreness. This review indicated that patients believe that MT has a biomedical mechanism of action and is suitable for biomedical causes of LBP. Several gaps in the literature are present that require further investigation.
Article
Article
Study design: A roentgen stereophotogrammetric analysis study of patients with sacroiliac joint dysfunction. Objectives: To investigate whether manipulation can influence the position between the ilium and the sacrum, and whether positional tests for the sacroiliac joint are valid. Summary of background data: Sacroiliac joint dysfunction is a subject of controversy. The validity of different sacroiliac joint tests is unknown. Long-standing therapeutic tradition is to manipulate supposed dysfunctions of the sacroiliac joint. Many manual therapists claim that their good clinical results are a consequence of a reduction of subluxation. Methods: Ten patients with symptoms and sacroiliac joint tests results indicating unilateral sacroiliac joint dysfunction were recruited. Twelve sacroiliac joint tests were chosen. The results of most of these tests were required to be positive before manipulation and normalized after manipulation. Roentgen stereophotogrammetric analysis was performed with the patient in the standing position, before and after treatment. Results: In none of the 10 patients did manipulation alter the position of the sacrum in relation to the ilium, defined by roentgen stereophotogrammetric analysis. Positional test results changed from positive before manipulation to normal after. Conclusions: Manipulation of the sacroiliac joint normalized different types of clinical test results but was not accompanied by altered position of the sacroiliac joint, according to roentgen stereophotogrammetric analysis. Therefore, the positional test results were not valid. However, the current results neither disprove nor prove possible beneficial clinical effects achieved by manipulation of the sacroiliac joint. Because the supposed positive effects are not a result of a reduction of subluxation, further studies of the effects of manipulation should focus on the soft tissue response.
Article
Joint mobilization and manipulation are commonly used by physiotherapists and chiropractors for the treatment of spinal and peripheral joint pain. There are, however, few quantitative studies about the effects of these manoeuvres on joint function. We studied and compared the effects of manipulation and mobilization on metacarpophalangeal coaptation and mobility. Sixty-two third metacarpophalangeal (MCP) joints were studied radiographically before and after applying a long-axis distraction force across the joint space. An audible crack and radiographically visible gas arthrogram were highly associated with a significant decrease in joint coaptation under tension for the manipulated joints. There was no evidence of a gas arthrogram, audible crack, or change in coaptation for the mobilized joints. In a further study, 62 MCP joints were randomly allocated to either manipulation or mobilization. The treatment was followed by blind assessment of passive flexion of the treated joint. The manipulated group demonstrated a significant increase in passive MCP joint flexion over the mobilized group. These results show that manipulation and mobilization are distinct therapies with different effects on joint function and that these effects in clinical trials of manual therapy should not be considered equivalent, as they have been in the past.
Article
Study Design. A roentgen stereophotogrammetric analysis study of patients with sacroiliac joint dysfunction. Objectives. To investigate whether manipulation can influence the position between the ilium and the sacrum, and whether positional tests for the sacroiliac joint are valid. Summary of Background Data. Sacroiliac joint dysfunction is a subject of controversy. The validity of different sacroiliac joint tests is unknown. Long‐standing therapeutic tradition is to manipulate supposed dysfunctions of the sacroiliac joint. Many manual therapists claim that their good clinical results are a consequence of a reduction of subluxation. Methods. Ten patients with symptoms and sacroiliac joint tests results indicating unilateral sacroiliac joint dysfunction were recruited. Twelve sacroiliac joint tests were chosen. The results of most of these tests were required to be positive before manipulation and normalized after manipulation. Roentgen stereophotogrammetric analysis was performed with the patient in the standing position, before and after treatment. Results. In none of the 10 patients did manipulation alter the position of the sacrum in relation to the ilium, defined by roentgen stereophotogrammetric analysis. Positional test results changed from positive before manipulation to normal after. Conclusions. Manipulation of the sacroiliac joint normalized different types of clinical test results but was not accompanied by altered position of the sacroiliac joint, according to roentgen stereophotogrammetric analysis. Therefore, the positional test results were not valid. However, the current results neither disprove nor prove possible beneficial clinical effects achieved by manipulation of the sacroiliac joint. Because the supposed positive effects are not a result of a reduction of subluxation, further studies of the effects of manipulation should focus on the soft tissue response.
Article
Study Design. A blinded, randomized controlled trial was conducted. Objective. To test the hypothesis that chiropractic side-posture manipulation (adjusting) of the lumbar spine separates (gaps) the zygapophysial (Z) joints. Summary of Background Data. Spinal adjusting is thought to gap the Z joints, yet no studies have conclusively validated this hypothesis, and some investigators have reported that the lumbar Z joints do not gap during rotation. Methods. For this study, 64 healthy student volunteers (32 men and 32 women) ages 22 to 30 years with no history of significant low back pain were randomized into four groups of 8 men and 8 women each. Interventions included lumbar side-posture spinal adjusting (manipulation) and side-posture positioning. Anterior to posterior measurements of the Z joints from MRI scans taken before and after side-posture spinal adjusting and before and after side-posture positioning were compared. Results. Observers performing the measurements were blinded as to group and first and second scans. Reliability of the measurements was established. Differences were found between the groups (F = 24.15;P < 0.000, analysis of variance). Side-posture positioning showed greater gapping than the control condition (mean difference, 1.18;P < 0.000); side-posture adjusting showed greater gapping than the control condition (mean difference, 1.89;P < 0.000), and side-posture adjusting showed greater gapping than side-posture positioning (mean difference, 0.71;P = 0.047). Conclusions. Spinal adjusting produced increased separation (gapping) of the Z joints. Side-posture positioning also produced gapping, but less than that seen with lumbar side-posture adjusting. This study helps to increase understanding about the mechanism of action for spinal manipulation.