ArticlePDF Available

Kin discrimination in cannibalistic tadpoles of the Green Poison Frog, Dendrobates auratus (Anura, Dendrobatidae)

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Cannibalizing a related individual can reduce the inclusive fitness of the cannibal. Hence, mechanisms that allow a tadpole to recognize and modify its behavior toward kin may reduce the inclusive fitness costs of cannibalism. Alternatively, ecological factors may cause preferential treatment of kin to be too costly to be favored by selection. We tested these two predictions in the Green Poison Frog, Dendrobates auratus. The effect of kinship on larval cannibalism was examined through a series of kin-discrimination trials. The behavior of large tadpoles was observed when presented with two small, tethered tadpoles, one a clutchmate and one an unrelated tadpole. In these simultaneous presentation tests, tadpoles displayed a significant preference for attacking kin. In a series of timed trials, pairs of unequally sized tadpoles were placed together in containers. The majority (70%) of large tadpoles took less than24 hr to consume the small tadpole. Kinship did not affect the survival time of the small tadpole. Our results are consistent with observations that D. auratus is an indiscriminate predator. As conspecifics may be serious competitors, their swift elimination would be an advantage, particularly in the small, nutrient-poor poolsused by this species.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Phyllomedusa
- 8(1), July 2009
41
Kin discrimination in cannibalistic tadpoles of the
Green Poison Frog, Dendrobates auratus (Anura,
Dendrobatidae)
Heather M. Gray1, Kyle Summers2,3 and Roberto Ibáñez D.3
1Redpath Museum, McGill University, 859 Sherbrooke St. W., Montreal, Quebec, Canada H3A 2K6. E-mail:
grayhm@hotmail.com.
2Department of Biology, East Carolina University, Greenville, NC 27834, USA. E-mail: summersk@mail.ecu.edu.
3Smithsonian Tropical Research Institute, Attn: Roberto Ibáñez D., Apartado 2072, Balboa, Ancón, Republica de Panamá.
E-mail: ibanezr@tivoli.si.edu.
Received 5 December 2008.
Accepted 23 April 2009.
Distributed July 2009.
Phyllomedusa 8(1):41-50, 2009
© 2009 Departamento de Ciências Biológicas - ESALQ - USP
ISSN 1519-1397
Abstract
Kin discrimination in cannibalistic tadpoles of the Green Poison Frog,
Dendrobates auratus (Anura, Dendrobatidae). Cannibalizing a related individual
can reduce the inclusive fitness of the cannibal. Hence, mechanisms that allow a
tadpole to recognize and modify its behavior toward kin may reduce the inclusive
fitness costs of cannibalism. Alternatively, ecological factors may cause preferential
treatment of kin to be too costly to be favored by selection. We tested these two
predictions in the Green Poison Frog, Dendrobates auratus. The effect of kinship
on larval cannibalism was examined through a series of kin-discrimination trials.
The behavior of large tadpoles was observed when presented with two small,
tethered tadpoles, one a clutchmate and one an unrelated tadpole. In these
simultaneous presentation tests, tadpoles displayed a significant preference for
attacking kin. In a series of timed trials, pairs of unequally sized tadpoles were
placed together in containers. The majority (70%) of large tadpoles took less than
24 hr to consume the small tadpole. Kinship did not affect the survival time of the
small tadpole. Our results are consistent with observations that D. auratus is an
indiscriminate predator. As conspecifics may be serious competitors, their swift
elimination would be an advantage, particularly in the small, nutrient-poor pools
used by this species.
Keywords: Anura, Dendrobatidae, cannibalism, kinship, larvae.
Phyllomedusa
- 8(1), July 2009
42
Introduction
Hamilton’s inclusive fitness theory provides
a framework to understand the evolution of
altruistic and aggressive behavior (Hamilton
1964). Hamilton’s Rule predicts that altruism
(or reduced aggression) will be favored when
rbc > 0, where c is the fitness cost to the
altruist, b is the fitness benefit to the beneficiary
and r is their genetic relatedness (Hamilton
1964). The rationale behind preferential
treatment of related individuals is that it will aid
the propagation of genes shared with kin.
To behave differently toward kin, related
individuals must be recognized as such
(Hamilton 1964). Kin recognition has been
identified in a wide variety of organisms,
including many amphibians (reviewed in
Fletcher and Michener 1987, Sherman et al.
1997). Studies of kin recognition in amphibians
have often examined it in the context of
cannibalism (Walls and Roudebush 1991,
Pfennig and Collins 1993, Pfennig et al. 1993,
Pfennig 1997). Cannibalism, or the killing and
ingestion of conspecifics, is widespread in
nature (Polis 1981, Elgar and Crespi 1992) and
is found in many groups of amphibians, with
the most frequent type being that of larvae
Resumo
Discriminação de relativos em girinos canibais de Dendrobates auratus (Anura, Dendrobatidae).
O consumo de indivíduos aparentados pode reduzir a aptidão inclusiva do canibal. Assim, mecanismos
que permitam que um girino reconheça seus relativos e modifique seu comportamento pode reduzir
os custos do canibalismo. Alternativamente, fatores ecológicos podem tornar o tratamento preferencial
dos relativos custoso demais para que seja favorecido pela seleção natural. Testamos essas duas
previsões no dendrobatídeo Dendrobates auratus. O efeito do parentesco sobre o canibalismo larval
foi examinado por meio de uma série de tentativas de discriminação de parentes. Observamos o
comportamento de girinos de grande porte diante de dois girinos menores imobilizados, um irmão e
outro não-aparentado. Nesses testes de apresentação simultânea, os girinos mostraram uma preferência
significativa por atacar irmãos. Em outra série de tentativas, pares de girinos de tamanhos diferentes
foram colocados juntos em aquários. A maioria dos girinos de grande porte (70%) consumiu o girino
menor em menos de 24 horas. O parentesco não afetou o tempo de sobrevivência do girino pequeno.
Nossos resultados são consistentes com as observações de que D. auratus é um predador
indiscriminado. Como os co-específicos podem ser fortes competidores, sua eliminação rápida poderia
ser vantajosa, particularmente nas pequenas poças pobres em nutrientes utilizadas por essa espécie.
Palavras-chave: Anura, Dendrobatidae, canibalismo, parentesco, larvas.
consuming larvae (Crump 1992). The benefits
derived from consuming conspecifics are
primarily nutritional, allowing the cannibal
enhanced growth or survival success (Crump
1992). There may be additional benefits of
consuming conspecifics as they represent the
proper proportion of nutrients required for the
cannibal’s growth (Nagai et al. 1971, Meffe and
Crump 1987, Crump 1990, Wildy et al. 1998).
Another benefit of cannibalism may be that it
eliminates sources of competition (Polis 1981).
Despite the nutritional benefits of
cannibalism, there are costs. These include the
risk of injury to the attacking animal, greater
exposure to species-specific disease and the risk
of eating a relative (Polis 1981, Crump 1992).
Cannibals tend to attack animals smaller than
themselves or animals that are unable to defend
themselves (Elgar and Crespi 1992), thereby
reducing the risk of injury. More efficient
spread of pathogens to conspecifics has been
demonstrated in a variety of organisms (Polis
1981, Pfennig et al. 1991) and has been
suggested as the main reason certain animals
avoid cannibalism (Pfennig et al. 1998). The
loss of inclusive fitness by eating a relative can
have a direct selective impact (Hamilton 1964).
A number of amphibian species reduce this
Gray et al.
Phyllomedusa
- 8(1), July 2009
43
impact by avoiding cannibalizing kin (examples
of anurans: Bufo americanus [Waldman 1985],
Spea bombifrons and S. multiplicata [Pfennig
and Frankino 1997], and salamanders: Hynobius
retardatus [Wakahara 1997], Ambystoma
tigrinium nebulosum [Pfennig and Collins 1993,
Pfennig et al. 1999]). In contrast, some species
do not seem to avoid cannibalizing kin (e.g.,
Walls and Blaustein 1995). The evolution of
mechanisms of kin discrimination should
depend on how larval ecology affects the costs
and benefits of cannibalism.
The Neotropical anuran genus Dendrobates
comprises many species with larval ecologies
that make cannibalism advantageous. Most
Dendrobates tadpoles have massive beaks with
serrated teeth that would allow carnivory
(Silverstone 1975, Caldwell and Myers 1990).
Combined with this morphogical modification,
the aggressive behavior of Dendrobates
tadpoles renders them successful predators
(Summers 1990, Caldwell and Araújo 1998,
Summers 1999). The exceptions to this are the
obligate egg-eating tadpoles of the Oophaga
histrionicus Group that survive on unfertilized
trophic eggs provided by the female parent
(Weygoldt 1987), and therefore these tadpoles
are excluded from further discussion.
Dendrobates deposit terrestrial eggs that
develop into tadpoles, and then are transported
by a parent to a pool of water to complete
development. Females produce many small
clutches of eggs year round so there is a steady
supply of tadpoles to be deposited (Weygoldt
1987). All Dendrobates tadpoles are deposited
in small pools of water (i.e., phytotelmata),
which include water that collects in fruit husks,
such as capsules of Brazil nuts (Caldwell 1993),
calabash (Summers 1990) and cacao pods
(H. M. Gray, pers. observ.), tree holes (Dunn
1941, Eaton 1941) and leaf axils of plants
(Summers 1992, Brust 1993). Not only are
these pools small and at risk of drying out, but
often they are freshly formed the night before
tadpole deposition, and thus are low in accumu-
lated nutrients (H. M. Gray, pers. observ.).
For Dendrobates auratus, the benefits
derived from consuming conspecifics are
primarily nutritional, allowing the cannibal
enhanced growth and survival (Crump 1992).
D. auratus tadpoles are generally voracious
predators and will consume any insect larvae,
and heterospecific or conspecific tadpoles
deposited in the same pool (Summers 1990,
Caldwell and Araújo 1998). Conspecific
tadpoles provide a relatively large prey item in
a usually nutrient-poor environment. Increasing
the food available to D. auratus tadpoles
increases larval growth rate (Summers 1990).
Although the consumption of conspecifics has
not been shown to confer any additional
advantage relative to other sources of calories
and nutrients, cannibalism does eliminate the
tadpoles’ closest competitors, as well as their
potential predators (i.e., other cannibals).
The costs of cannibalism in Dendrobates
auratus include the risk of injury to the
attacking animal, pathogen transmission, and
the loss of inclusive fitness (Crump 1992, Polis
1981). The risk of injury to tadpoles attempting
to cannibalize conspecifics is apparently high;
in pools in which there are more than two D.
auratus, individuals have damaged tails
(Summers 1990). Tadpoles usually are able to
complete metamorphosis despite tail damage
(H. M. Gray, unpubl. data), although these
injuries may impose a cost in terms of reduced
growth and developmental rates as was found
in Rana catesbeiana (Wilbur and Semlitsch
1990). Pathogen transmission via cannibalism
in this species has not been explored. Also, it
is unknown whether any mechanism exists to
recognize kin and avoid a loss of inclusive
fitness in D. auratus.
Field observations indicate that individual
male Dendrobates auratus return to pools and
deposit tadpoles from different clutches into the
same pool, although they typically deposit
tadpoles from a single clutch into different
pools (Summers 1990). In most pools where
tadpoles were deposited, more than one male
was observed bringing tadpoles to the pool
Kin discrimination in cannibalistic tadpoles of the Green Poison Frog
Phyllomedusa
- 8(1), July 2009
44
(Summers 1990). Hence, pools typically contain
a mixture of related and unrelated tadpoles.
Because tadpoles are rarely deposited
simultaneously, they typically vary in size.
Even under experimental conditions when
tadpoles are deposited at similar sizes, they
rapidly diverge in size, owing to intraspecific
competition (Summers 1990). Typically, the
total number of tadpoles deposited is small;
thus, cannibals do not have the option of
choosing among a large number of relatives and
non-relatives.
We examined tadpole choice by simulta-
neously presenting a large tadpole with a
small related and a small unrelated
conspecific. We also examined, in a series of
paired timed trials, the time for a large
tadpole to cannibalize a small tadpole, and
whether kinship influences the outcome. The
simplest prediction with regard to kin
discrimination in Dendrobates auratus is that
tadpoles will avoid eating relatives, thereby
increasing their inclusive fitness. However,
owing to the larval ecology of D. auratus, this
prediction may be too simplistic. Selection
may not favor kin discrimination if the small
size and nutrient-poor status of the pools, and
the associated high levels of competition and
cannibalism, make kin discrimination
prohibitively costly.
Materials and Methods
Tadpole collection and maintenance
Experiments were conducted during May–
July 1998 and May–August 1999 in the
Republic of Panama. Sibling Dendrobates
auratus tadpoles were obtained from two
sources. Tadpoles were collected from the
backs of males transporting more than one
tadpole. Males usually transport a single
tadpole, but occasionally transport more.
During a 2-mo period, the number of tadpoles
being transported by males was counted at the
two field sites (Cerro Ancon, 8°56’N,
79°34’W and Isla Taboga, 8°47’N, 79°34’W).
Of 46 males observed transporting tadpoles,
42 (91.3%) were transporting a single
tadpole, 3 (6.52%) were carrying two, and 1
individual (2.17%) was carrying three
tadpoles. Males were assumed to be the father
of the tadpoles that they carried. Cuckoldry
of one male by another has not been observed
in field studies of the mating system of D.
auratus, and the aggressive territoriality of
males in this species makes cuckoldry
unlikely (Summers 1989). Because multiple
tadpoles on a given male’s back were always
of the same size, they were assumed to be of
the same clutch.
The second source of tadpoles was a group
of captive Dendrobates auratus. Breeding pairs
of adults were captured in the field and were
maintained in 10-gal (37.85-L) terraria at the
Tupper Center of the Smithsonian Tropical
Institute in Panama City. Terraria were kept at
ambient temperature and were furnished with
leaf litter, a hollowed branch, a bowl of
dechlorinated water, and a dish containing
pieces of ripe fruit. Animals were fed lab-
reared, wingless Drosophila dusted with
vitamin powder. They were also able to eat the
insects that were attracted to the rotting fruit in
the terraria. Tadpoles were collected from the
water bowls placed in the terraria with a single
pair of frogs. The date tadpoles were collected
was noted in order to keep track of which
tadpoles were from the same clutch. All related
tadpoles used were full siblings. In total, 29
sibships were used in the experiments. Nineteen
different sibships were examined for the kin
discrimination trials, and 10 different sibships
were used in the timed trials. Experiments were
conducted with tadpoles that were 7–14 days
old.
Tadpoles to be used as the unrelated
individuals were collected from the field in
areas known to have large breeding populations
of Dendrobates auratus. Small plastic cups,
filled with dechlorinated water, were placed in
areas not overlapping with the source areas for
Gray et al.
Phyllomedusa
- 8(1), July 2009
45
the related tadpoles. The next day, all tadpoles
deposited in these cups were collected.
Tadpoles from all sources were maintained
individually in plastic cups containing 75 ml of
dechlorinated water and were fed commercial
goldfish food every few days.
Experiment 1: Kin discrimination trials
Each kin discrimination trial involved three
tadpoles—i.e., a pair of siblings and one
unrelated individual. One of the siblings was
designated the test tadpole. This animal was fed
fish food and mosquito larvae ad libitum for the
week prior to trials. The other tadpoles were fed
limited fish food only. The trials were
conducted with unequal-sized individuals
because this allowed the experiment to progress
more rapidly. In the field, when individuals of
the same size share a small pool, one will
outcompete the others and will increase in size
much more rapidly than the others (Summers
1990).
The day before the trial, tadpoles were
weighed (Salter AND electronic balance FX-
200) and their body length from the snout to the
junction of the posterior body wall with the tail,
or snout-vent length (SVL), measured with dial
calipers. In 1998, only the small tadpoles were
measured, whereas in 1999, the test tadpoles
also were measured. Each small kin tadpole was
carefully matched for size with an unrelated
tadpole. All three tadpoles were placed into
separate containers with clean water and left
without food for 24 hr to ensure that the animals
were all at the same hunger level during trials.
The experimental arena (Figure 1) was a
clear plastic rectangular tank (18.5 × 13.25 cm).
A black line was drawn on the bottom of the
tank dividing it into halves. The tank was filled
with water to a depth of 2.5 cm. We placed an
open-ended net cylinder in the middle of the
tank, straddling the centerline. The cylinder was
a tube of fine open-meshed material (tulle) with
wire rings (5-cm diameter) at either end to give
the tube rigidity. The test tadpole was placed in
this cylinder during acclimation to the arena. A
short piece of black thread was tied around the
base of the tail of each of the small tadpoles.
The end of the thread of each tethered tadpole
was then taped to opposite sides of the test
arena. In this way, the small tadpoles were able
to swim freely around in their half of the test
arena but were unable to cross the center line.
The side of the tank occupied by the related or
unrelated tadpole was switched for each trial.
After a 20-min acclimation period, the cylinder
was removed and the test tadpole was able to
swim freely about the entire arena. Interactions
between the test tadpole and its smaller sibling
and the smaller unrelated tadpole were filmed
for 1 hr with a video camera (Sony Hi-8 video
CCD-TR100 NTSC). At the end of the hour, the
small tadpoles were untied and their condition
noted. The videotapes were then watched by an
observer who noted the number of attacks and
bites the test tadpole inflicted on each of the
smaller animals. To control for any a priori
assumptions about outcome, the observer did
not know the relatedness of the tadpoles when
scoring the videotapes.
Figure 1 - Experimental arena during acclimation period.
After 20-min of acclimation, the net cylinder
was removed and the interactions between the
test tadpole and its clutch mate and an
unrelated tadpole (shaded grey on the right
side of the tank) were filmed. Tethering
ensured that the small tadpoles remained on
opposite sides of the tank.
Kin discrimination in cannibalistic tadpoles of the Green Poison Frog
Phyllomedusa
- 8(1), July 2009
46
Experiment 2: Timed cannibalism trials
To determine if relatedness of tadpoles
affected the amount of time for a large tadpole
to consume a small tadpole, we conducted
paired trials with animals in small containers.
These experiments also controlled for the effect
of tethering in the first experiment, ensuring
that the attacks on the small tadpoles in the first
experiment did not occur simply because the
tadpoles were tethered. Two tadpoles, again one
larger than the other, were placed together in
small plastic containers containing 75 mL of
dechlorinated water. Each paired trial consisted
of a container holding a large and small, related
tadpole and a container holding a large and
small, unrelated tadpole. In each set of trials,
large and small animals were carefully matched
for size, both in wet weight and snout-vent
length. After measuring, all tadpoles were
placed individually into clean containers with
water and left for the 24 hr prior to the
experiment. After the 24 hr, the large and
small, related animals were put together and
the large and small, unrelated animals were
put together in a separate container. A pellet
of goldfish food was added to each container
with the pair of tadpoles. This was done to
give the big tadpole a choice of foods and
eliminate the possibility that a starving
tadpole would indiscriminately eat any
tadpole in the same pool. Containers were
checked every 12 hr and the time taken for
the large tadpole to kill the small tadpole was
noted.
Statistical analysis
Statistical analyses were carried out using
Systat Version 8.0 (SPSS Inc., 1998). Data were
tested for normality of distribution
(Kolmogorov-Smirnov normality test) and for
homogeneity of variances (Bartlett’s test).
Proportional data were arcsine–square root
transformed to meet parametric assumptions of
normality (Zar 1999).
In experiment 1, body length and mass
differences were examined with two-sample t-
tests. Kruskal-Wallis one-way analysis of
variance was used to determine if the test
tadpoles were significantly larger than the
tethered tadpoles. To determine if the number
of attacks and bites were based on kinship, a
Mann-Whitney U-test was used. To take into
consideration the range in attack behavior of the
different test tadpoles, we analyzed, with t-tests,
the proportion of attacks and bites inflicted on
related and unrelated small tadpoles.
In experiment 2, body length and mass
differences were examined with two-sample t-
tests. To determine if kinship influenced the
time to cannibalism, a Mann-Whitney U-test
was used (Zar 1999).
Results
Experiment 1: Kin discrimination trials
There were no significant differences in the
sizes of the small tadpoles used in each kin
discrimination trial (mean ± standard deviation;
small tadpoles related to the test tadpole
0.065 ± 0.0184 g wet weight, 6.5 ± 0.704 mm
body length, N = 19; small tadpoles unrelated
to the test tadpole 0.064 ± 0.0165 g,
6.49 ± 0.603 mm, N = 19; two-sample t-test for
mass: t36 = 0.315, P = 0.754; two-sample t-test
for body length: t36 = 0.025, P = 0.98). The test
tadpoles were significantly larger than the small
tadpoles (0.12 ± 0.0435 g wet weight,
7.97 ± 0.986 mm body length, N = 13; Kruskal-
Wallis one-way analysis of variance for mass:
H = 16.11, k = 3, P << 0.05; analysis of
variance for SVL: F = 15.94, df(factor) = 2,
df(residual) = 36, P << 0.05). The test tadpoles
were almost twice the weight and on average
1.5 mm longer than the small tadpoles.
There was no significant difference in the
number of attacks on the small tadpoles based
on kinship (mean number of attacks on kin
8.89 ± 9.73 and on nonkin 4.63 ± 6.73, Mann-
Whitney U-test: U = 235, N1 = N2 = 19, P
Gray et al.
Phyllomedusa
- 8(1), July 2009
47
= 0.104). Similarly, there was no significant
difference in the number of bites inflicted by the
test tadpole on the small related or unrelated
tadpoles (mean number of bites to kin
49.63 ± 159.831 and to nonkin 11 ± 16.9345;
Mann-Whitney U-test: U = 224, N1 = N2 = 19,
P = 0.195). The large variance in numbers of
bites was attributable, in particular, to one test
tadpole that attacked the related small tadpole
23 times, biting the small tadpole 707 times.
This value is an order of magnitude greater than
the next highest number of bites (65). The test
tadpoles displayed a significant preference for
attacking kin (proportion of attacks on kin,
0.74 ± 0.302; one sample t-test: t15 = 2.821,
P = 0.013). There was no significant preference,
relative to a baseline of 50%, found when the
proportion of bites inflicted was examined
(proportion of bites to kin 0.676 ± 0.358; one
sample t-test: t15 = 1.670, P = 0.116). There
were however significantly greater proportions
of both attacks and bites made to kin tadpoles
when compared to the proportion of attacks and
bites to nonkin (two-sample t-test for attacks:
t30 = 3.99, P = 0.000; two-sample t-test for
bites: t30 = 2.361, P = 0.025).
The use of the observed condition of the
tadpoles as they were removed from the setup
proved problematic as the test tadpole often
continued to attack tadpoles during the
dismantling of the arena. The end result was
that during the 19 trials, three kin and three
unrelated small tadpoles were fatally wounded
by the time they were removed from the test
arena. From the videotapes, only the three
related and one of the unrelated small tadpoles
were attacked during filming meaning that
enough injuries were sustained during the
dismantling period to kill an additional two
unrelated tadpoles.
There was no significant tendency for the
test tadpole to preferentially attack or bite the
tadpole on a particular side of the test arena
(one-sample t-test for attacks: t15 = –1.833,
P = 0.087; one-sample t-test for bites: t15=
1.796, P = 0.093).
Experiment 2: Timed cannibalism trials
The large tadpoles in the timed kin trials
(0.14 ± 0.038 g; 8.29 ± 0.887 mm, N = 10) and
the large tadpoles in the timed unrelated trials
(0.14 ± 0.0370 g; 8.49 ± 1.01 mm, N = 10) were
not significantly different in size (two-sample
t-test for mass: t18 = 0.254, P = 0.802; two-
sample t-test for body length: t18 = –0.470,
P = 0.644). There was also no significant
difference in size of the small tadpoles (small
tadpoles in kin trials 0.061 ± 0.0147 g,
6.41 ± 0.499 mm body length, N = 10; small
tadpoles in unrelated trials 0.061 ± 0.0141 g,
6.22 ± 0.657 mm, N = 10; two-sample t-test for
mass: t18 = –0.015, P = 0.988; two-sample t-test
for body length: t18 = 0.727, P = 0.476). The
large tadpoles were significantly larger than the
small tadpoles (two-sample t-test for mass:
t38 = 8.71, P = <<0.05; two-sample t-test for
body length: t38 = 8.47, P = <<0.05).
It took large tadpoles less than 12 hours to
up to two weeks to fully consume the smaller
tadpole in the container. The majority of large
tadpoles took less than 24 hours to consume the
smaller tadpole (70%, N = 20). In four of 10
paired trials, the time that it took for the large
tadpole to kill the small tadpole was the same.
In three trials, the small tadpole in the related
container died first and in three trials the
unrelated small animal died first. There was no
significant difference between the mean time
until cannibalism between trials with kin and
trials with non-kin (Mann-Whitney U test,
N = 10, Xkin = 42.8 hr, SE = 14.6 hr, Xnonkin
= 22.8 hr, SE = 3.3 hr, U = 41, P = 0.496).
Discussion
In some species, tadpoles recognize their kin
and modify their cannibalistic behavior in order
to avoid a reduction of inclusive fitness (Crump
1990). We found no evidence that Dendrobates
auratus tadpoles avoid cannibalizing their kin.
Our results demonstrated that the test tadpoles
directed a higher proportion of both attacks and
Kin discrimination in cannibalistic tadpoles of the Green Poison Frog
Phyllomedusa
- 8(1), July 2009
48
bites toward related, rather than unrelated
tadpoles, although tadpoles in both groups
ultimately suffered the same fate. The numbers
of attacks and bites directed at unrelated
tadpoles were not significantly different than
those directed at related tadpoles, and an equal
number of unrelated and related small tadpoles
ultimately died in the preference trials. In
experiment 2, the similar speed with which
large tadpoles killed and consumed unrelated or
related smaller tadpoles confirms that even if
tadpoles are able to discriminate kin from
unrelated tadpoles this does not modify their
cannibalistic behavior.
Our results raise several questions. First,
why did large tadpoles not discriminate in favor
of their kin, as has been observed in other
species? Most anuran species that possess kin
discrimination live in temporary or permanent
ponds (e.g., Spea bombifrons, Pfennig et al.
1993). Because these ponds contain large
clutches of related and unrelated larvae, there
are typically many individuals from which to
choose and a cannibal can be selective and
avoid eating a relative. This is not the case for
Dendrobates auratus tadpoles which are
deposited into small, nutrient-poor pools.
Indiscriminate killing of related and unrelated
conspecifics occurs in other species that utilize
small pools (e.g., Adelphobates castaneoticus
[Caldwell and Araújo 1998] and mosquito
larvae [Sheratt et al. 1999]). In such pools,
competition for severely limited resources may
make the costs of altruism toward kin (e.g.,
refraining from cannibalism) too high (Pfennig
1997). Given the small size of the pools utilized
by D. auratus, there may simply not be enough
food available for a tadpole to allow even a
small full sibling to survive and compete for
resources. Hence, the best strategy may be to
eat all small tadpoles introduced into the pools.
It is possible that cannibalism in
Dendrobates auratus occurs as a result of
completely indiscriminate predation, in which
all competitors and potential predators are
attacked, and no distinction is made between
different species (Crump 1992, Caldwell and
Araújo 1998). D. auratus tadpoles are efficient
predators of the tadpoles of sympatric species
of frogs (Summers 1990, Caldwell and Araújo
1998). D. auratus tadpoles also attack
invertebrate larvae (Finke 1994). Other than
some of these larvae which become predators
when large, the greatest threat to D. auratus
tadpoles are conspecifics, which may explain
why the large tadpoles in experiment 2 chose
to kill and consume the smaller tadpole in
preference to eating the fish food. The drive to
attack was not simply a consequence of hunger,
implying that the calories and nutrients gained
from cannibalism are not the immediate
motivator of the behavior. Instead, the
elimination of a potentially long-term
competitor (or predator) may be the most
important benefit.
A second reason that large tadpoles did not
discriminate in favor of kin may be that the risk
of attacking close relatives is comparatively
low. The tadpoles of Dendrobates auratus are
transported individually by their male parent to
a pool. The male chooses where to deposit each
of his offspring. Male D. ventrimaculatus have
been shown to avoid placing young into pools
which already contain a large tadpole (Summers
1999), and the same may be true for D. auratus.
Male D. auratus spend substantial amounts of
time evaluating and searching for suitable pools
to deposit tadpoles (Summers 1990). Part of this
evaluation may involve the number and the size
of other tadpoles, as well as the presence of
previous offspring in the pool. Males transport
tadpoles of the same clutch to different pools
(Summers 1990). These deposition strategies of
male D. auratus may reduce the probability that
tadpoles will encounter and consume close
relatives, and hence decrease the indirect fitness
costs of indiscriminate predation of
conspecifics.
Another question is why did kin receive a
greater proportion of attacks? One possibility is
that tadpoles were able to recognize kin as
potential competitors more quickly and
Gray et al.
Phyllomedusa
- 8(1), July 2009
49
efficiently than they were able to recognize
non-kin. It is not known what cue tadpoles use
to determine either relatedness or conspecific
status. Other species of amphibians have been
shown to use chemical cues (olfactory) to
recognize kin (Pfennig and Collins 1993). Adult
Dendrobates pumilio have been shown to use
olfactory cues in territory identification
(Forester and Wisnieski 1991), and tadpoles of
Dendrobates species may also have well-
developed chemosensory reception. The D.
auratus tadpoles may be using olfactory cues to
identify another tadpole in the pool. As the
experimental tadpoles were raised in isolation,
the test tadpole may recognize a tadpole that
smells like itself as a conspecific competitor
more quickly and this may explain the initial
preference for kin. This use of self to develop
a standard for comparison is described by
Grafen (1990), who posited that kin recognition
(and discrimination) might be an artifact of
species recognition. Specifically, he proposed
that, if the cues used for species recognition are
perceived by matching those cues to a template
derived from the animals’ own phenotype, then
individuals may tend to recognize closely
related individuals more effectively, simply as
a byproduct of the recognition system. This
could produce non-adaptive biases in
recognition and discrimination that are
associated with kinship.
Acknowledgements
We thank the scientific and administrative
staff of the Smithsonian Tropical Research
Institute for logistic support and advice, and the
Autoridad Nacional Del Ambiente (ANAM) of
the Republic of Panama for collection and
research permits. Permission to carry out the
research was obtained from ANAM and from
the animal care and use committee of East
Carolina University. We thank A.S. Rand, S.
MacKenzie, D. Pfennig, and J. Stamps for
comments and discussion. The line drawing was
kindly done by A.V. Savage.
References
Brust, D. G. 1993. Maternal brood care by Dendrobates
pumilio: a frog that feeds its young. Journal of
Herpetology 27: 96–98.
Caldwell, J. P. 1993. Brazil nut fruit capsules as
phytotelmata: interactions among anuran and insect
larvae. Canadian Journal of Zoology 71: 1193–1201.
Caldwell, J. P. and M. C. Araújo. 1998. Cannibalistic
interactions resulting from indiscriminate predatory
behavior in tadpoles of the poison frogs (Anura:
Dendrobatidae). Biotropica 30: 92–103.
Caldwell, J. P. and C. W. Myers. 1990. A new poison frog
from Amazonian Brazil, with further revision of the
quinquevittatus group of Dendrobates. American
Museum Novitates 2988: 1–21.
Crump, M. L. 1990. Possible enhancement of growth in
tadpoles through cannibalism. Copeia 1990: 560–564.
Crump, M. L. 1992. Cannibalism in amphibians. Pp. 256–
276 in M. A. Elgar and B. J. Crespi (eds.),
Cannibalism: Ecology and Evolution Among Diverse
Taxa. Oxford. Oxford University Press.
Dunn, E. R. 1941. Notes on Dendrobates auratus. Copeia
1941: 88–93.
Eaton, T. H. Jr. 1941. Notes on the life history of
Dendrobates auratus. Copeia 1941: 93–95.
Elgar, M. A. and B. J. Crespi (eds.). 1992. Cannibalism:
Ecology and Evolution Among Diverse Taxa. Oxford.
Oxford University Press. 376 pp.
Finke, O. M. 1994. Population regulation of a tropical
damselfly in the larval stage by food limitation,
cannibalism, intraguild predation and habitat drying.
Oecologia 100: 118–127.
Fletcher, D. J. C and C. D. Michener. 1987. Kin
Recognition in Animals. New York. Wiley. 476 pp.
Forester, D.C. and A. Wisnieski. 1991. The significance
of airborne olfactory cues to the recognition of home
area by the Dart-poison frog Dendrobates pumilio.
Journal of Herpetology 25: 502–504.
Grafen, A. 1990. Do animals really recognize kin? Animal
Behaviour 39: 42–54.
Hamilton, W. D. 1964. The genetical evolution of social
behavior I and II. Journal of Theoretical Biology 7: 1–
52.
Meffe, G. K. and M. L. Crump. 1987. Possible growth and
reproductive benefits of cannibalism in the
mosquitofish. The American Naturalist 129: 203–212.
Nagai, Y., S. Nagai and T. Nishikawa. 1971. The nutritional
efficiency of cannibalism and an artificial feed for the
growth of tadpoles of Japanese toad (Bufo vulgaris
sp.). Agricultural Biology and Chemistry 35: 697–703.
Kin discrimination in cannibalistic tadpoles of the Green Poison Frog
Phyllomedusa
- 8(1), July 2009
50
Pfennig, D. W. 1997. Kinship and cannibalism. Bioscience
47: 667–675.
Pfennig, D. W. and J. P. Collins. 1993. Kinship affects
morphogenesis in cannibalistic salamanders. Nature
362: 836–838.
Pfennig, D. W. and W. A. Frankino. 1997. Kin-mediated
morphogenesis in facultatively cannibalistic tadpoles.
Evolution 51: 1993–1999.
Pfennig, D. W., M. L. G. Loeb and J. P. Collins. 1991.
Pathogens as a factor limiting the spread of canniba-
lism in tiger salamanders. Oecologia 88: 161–166.
Pfennig, D. W., H. K. Reeve and P. W. Sherman. 1993.
Kin recognition and cannibalism in spadefoot toad
tadpoles. Animal Behaviour 46: 97–94.
Pfennig, D. W., S. G. Ho and E. A. Hoffman. 1998.
Pathogen transmission as a selective force against
cannibalism. Animal Behaviour 55: 1255–1261.
Pfennig, D. W., J. P. Collins and R. E. Ziemba. 1999. A
test of alternative hypotheses for kin recognition in
cannibalistic tiger salamanders. Behavioral Ecology
10: 436–443.
Polis, G. 1981. The evolution and dynamic of intraspecific
predation. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics
12: 125–151.
Sherman, P. W., H. K. Reeve and D. W. Pfennig. 1997.
Recognition Systems. Pp. 69–96 in J. R. Krebs and N.
B. Davies (eds.), Behavioural Ecology: an Evolutio-
nary Approach. 4th edition. London. Blackwell.
Sherratt, T. N., S. E. Ruff and S. C. Church. 1999. No
evidence for kin discrimination in cannibalistic tree-
hole mosquitoes (Diptera: Culicidae). Journal of Insect
Behavior 12: 23–128.
Silverstone, P. A. 1975. A revision of the poison-arrow
frogs of the genus Dendrobates Wagler. Bulletin of Los
Angeles County Museum of Natural History 21: 1–55.
Summers, K. 1989. Sexual selection and intra-female
competition in the green poison-dart frog, Dendrobates
auratus. Animal Behaviour 37: 797–805.
Summers, K. 1990. Paternal care and the cost of polygyny
in the green dart-poison frog. Behavioral Ecology and
Sociobiology 27: 307–313.
Summers, K. 1992. Mating strategies in two species of
dart-poison frogs: a comparative study. Animal
Behaviour 43: 907–919.
Summers, K. 1999. The effects of cannibalism on
Amazonian poison frog egg and tadpole deposition and
survivorship in Heliconia axil pools. Oecologia 119:
557–564.
Wakahara, M. 1997. Kin recognition among intact and
blinded, mixed-sibling larvae of a cannibalistic
salamander Hynobius retardatus. Zoological Science
14: 893–899.
Waldman, B. 1985. Olfactory basis of kin recognition in
toad tadpoles. Journal of Comparative Physiology A
156: 565–577.
Walls S. C. and A. R. Blaustein. 1995. Larval marbled
salamanders, Ambystoma opacum, eat their kin. Animal
Behaviour 50: 537–545.
Walls, S. C. and R. E. Roudebush. 1991. Reduced
aggression toward siblings as evidence of kin
recognition in cannibalistic salamanders. The American
Naturalist 138: 1027–1038.
Weygoldt, P. 1987. Evolution of parental care in dart
poison frogs (Amphibia: Anura: Dendrobatidae).
Zeitschrift für zoologische Systematik und
Evolutionsforschung 25: 51–67.
Wilbur, H. M. and R. D. Semlitsch. 1990. Ecological
consequences of tail injury in Rana tadpoles. Copeia
1990: 18–24.
Wildy, E. L., D. P. Chivers, J. M. Kiesecker, and A. R.
Blaustein. 1998. Cannibalism enhances growth in
larval long-toed salamanders (Ambystoma
macrodactylum). Journal of Herpetology 32: 286–289.
Zar, J. H. 1999. Biostatistical Analysis. 4th edition. New
Jersey. Prentice Hall. 929 pp.
Gray et al.
... Cannibalism is a relatively common behaviour in tadpoles (Crump 1983(Crump , 1992Polis et al. 1989) and, in poison frogs, it has been widely studied in Bufonidae (e.g., Pfennig et al. 1993;Heinen and Abdella 2005; Crossland and Shine 2011) and Dendrobatidae (e.g., Caldwell and de Araujo 1998;Summers 1999;Brown et al. 2009;Gray et al. 2009;Rojas 2014;Carvajal-Castro et al. 2021;Fouilloux et al. 2022). In dendrobatids, significant efforts have been made to understand the biotic and abiotic factors that have promoted the evolution of cannibalism and how, in turn, this behaviour is related to parental decision-making (e.g., Schulte et al. 2011;Schulte and Lötters 2013;Rojas 2014;Summers and Tumulty 2014). ...
... A high asymmetry in body size and developmental stage among conspecifics (Rojas 2014;Fouilloux et al. 2022), coupled with low resource availability or other factors indicating a high risk of mortality (e.g., due to pool desiccation), may lead some individuals to readily feed on conspecifics. By contrast, a high degree of relatedness between individuals can delay cannibalistic attempts (Fouilloux et al. 2022), although, ultimately, conspecifics can be indiscriminately eaten regardless of whether they are kin or not (Summers and Symula 2001;Gray et al. 2009). Cannibalism has been documented predominantly in species exhibiting the most elaborate parental care behaviours, which include the transport of tadpoles to phytotelmata and, in some cases, provisioning of food until metamorphosis (Carvajal-Castro et al. 2021). ...
Article
Full-text available
Poison frogs of the superfamily Dendrobatoidea have fascinated researchers since the 19th century, which is reflected in multiple studies on their natural history, taxonomy, toxicity, colour pattern diversity, and elaborate territorial, reproductive and parental care behaviours. Broadly speaking, however, the term “poison frogs” may apply as well to other taxa which also possess skin toxins. Hereon, we refer to poison frogs as this extended group involving, besides Dendrobatoidea (Dendrobatidae + Aromobatidae), a few genera in families such as Bufonidae and Mantellidae (among others). Most studies on poison frogs have focused on species considered charismatic due to their bright and flashy colours, limiting possible generalisations of patterns and mechanisms explaining the high morphological, ecological, and behavioural differentiation, but also convergence, among these groups. Furthering our understanding of the evolutionary ecology of poison frogs requires increasing not only the scope and depth of our own questions, but also the number and diversity of study systems. Inspired by the special issue on dendrobatid and aromobatid poison frogs published ten years ago in this same journal, we have put together the present special issue aiming to broaden both the topics and the coverage of poison frog clades. This first part consists of 15 manuscripts covering topics ranging from larval behavioural ecology and parental care to space use and its environmental determinants, along with conservation implications.
... Kinship between individuals can also explain aggression. This has been shown to be an important factor in several cannibalistic species that demonstrate kin discrimination and avoid eating kin (Pfennig et al. 1994;Pfennig and Frankino 1997;van den Beuken et al. 2019), although there are also examples of cannibals consuming their kin without avoidance (Boots 2000;Gray et al. 2009). Although differences in opponent size and relatedness have individually been identified as variables that shape cannibalistic decisions, the interaction between these two variables has yielded diverse results across taxa where, for example, studies have reported a strong interactive effect in earwigs (Dobler and Kölliker 2011), the absence of size effect in spiders (Bilde and Lubin 2001;Roberts et al. 2003), and both a stage and phenotype dependent adversity where spadefoot toads are less likely to cannibalize other cannibals (Pfennig 1999) as well as more developed siblings (Dugas et al. 2016b). ...
... In these environments, cannibalism is common (Rojas 2014(Rojas , 2015, but not necessary for the successful development and metamorphosis of an individual tadpole. In closely related poison frogs, cannibalism is usually an outcome of sequentially intensified attacks (Summers and Symula 2001;Gray et al. 2009), although exceptions where tadpole aggression does not include cannibalism exist (i.e., obligate egg-feeders with parental care, Dugas et al. 2016a). Here, we aim to better understand what drives cannibals to express aggression towards conspecifics and disentangle the apparent variation that exists in this behavior. ...
Article
Full-text available
In juveniles extreme intraspecies aggression can seem counter-intuitive, as it might endanger their developmental goal of surviving until reproductive stage. Ultimately, aggression can be vital for survival, although the factors (e.g., genetic or environmental) leading to the expression and intensity of this behavior vary across taxa. Attacking (and sometimes killing) related individuals may reduce inclusive fitness; as a solution to this problem, some species exhibit kin discrimination and preferentially attack unrelated individuals. Here, we used both experimental and modeling approaches to consider how physical traits (e.g., size in relation to opponent) and genetic relatedness mediate aggression in dyads of cannibalistic Dendrobates tinctorius tadpoles. We paired full-sibling, half-sibling, and non-sibling tadpoles of different sizes together in an arena and recorded their aggression and activity. We found that the interaction between relative size and relatedness predicts aggressive behavior: large individuals in non-sibling dyads are significantly more aggressive than large individuals in sibling dyads. Unexpectedly, although siblings tended to attack less overall, in size-mismatched pairs they attacked faster than in non-sibling treatments. Using a theoretical model to complement these empirical findings, we propose that larval aggression reflects a balance between relatedness and size where individuals trade-off their own fitness with that of their relatives. Lay Summary Before you eat someone, you have to attack them first. Here, we investigated the factors that shape aggression in the cannibalistic tadpoles of the dyeing poison frog. We find that aggression depends on both size and relatedness: when set in pairs, large tadpoles are half as aggressive towards their smaller siblings than to nonsibs. It looks like belonging to the same family provides some protection against aggression, though no one is ever truly safe.
... While some studies have revealed, for example, the fascinating behavioural and ecological adaptations of larval life in self-contained, ephemeral bodies of water in plant structures ( Fig. 1F) (i.e. phytotelmata; Caldwell 1993; Caldwell and de Araujo 1998; Summers and Symula 2001; Lehtinen et al. 2004;Brown et al. 2009b;Gray et al. 2009;von May et al. 2009;Ryan and Barry 2011;Dugas et al. 2016b;Schulte and Mayer 2017;Fouilloux et al. 2021Fouilloux et al. , 2022aFouilloux et al. , b, 2023Szabo et al. 2021;Surber-Cunningham et al. 2024), and how these can be modulated via parental decision-making (Maple 2000;Poelman and Dicke 2007;Schulte et al. 2011;McKeon and Summers 2013;Lötters 2013, 2014;Rojas 2014Rojas , 2015Erich et al. 2015;Ringler et al. 2017;Fischer et al. 2019a, Fischer et al. 2019bFouilloux et al. 2021), we still know very little about poison frog tadpole biology in comparison to what we know about adults. Therefore, it would be interesting to investigate in more depth, for instance, how the rearing environment shapes life history, particularly in the face of global change, and whether the carry-over effects of developing in a particular environment differ between cryptic and aposematic species, and between species that sequester and synthesise their own toxins. ...
... Interference competition has been documented in the tadpoles of amphibians (Steinwascher 1978). Due to similar resource utilization, resource competition between relatives in different natural environments may be more intense, which may lead to the emergence of cannibalism among tadpoles (Gray et al. 2009). Conversely, the decreased aggression level among siblings may provide more time for tadpoles to feed due to the maximization of fitness, so that tadpoles can live in good growth conditions (Brown & Brown 1993). ...
... Earlier studies in anuran larvae have shown that tadpoles reared in kin groups showed more even growth rates (Pakkasmaa and Aikio 2003), lower aggressiveness (Fouilloux et al. 2022) and higher activity (Hokit and Blaustein 1995), and that tadpoles prefer to shoal with kin over non-kin (e.g., Blaustein and O'Hara 1986; Blaustein and Waldman 1992; Hase and Kutsukake 2019). However, when competition is high, related individuals in some species show lower mass at metamorphosis (Hokit and Blaustein 1994) and increased amounts of kin-directed cannibalism (Pfennig et al. 1993;Gray et al. 2009). In such scenarios, individuals might prefer to group with unrelated individuals or with smaller groups (cf. ...
Article
Full-text available
Group living results in various benefits and costs, which often depend on group size and ontogenetic state of the individual. Therefore, certain group sizes are favourable over others, often depending on individual age or intrinsic state. Under natural conditions many amphibians can be found in aggregations, especially during the larval stage. Yet, whether these aggregations are the result of active social preferences or are driven by environmental factors has only been tested in a limited number of taxa. This study explores social preferences and group size discrimination in golden mantella (Mantella aurantiaca) tadpoles. We gave tadpoles the choice between different numbers of conspecifics using a two-choice design. To test for general social tendency, tadpoles could either join a group of three or remain solitary (3 vs. 0). To test for group size preferences, we tested tadpoles in two different ratios: 2 vs. 1 and 4 vs. 2 conspecifics. We repeated the trials weekly until metamorphosis to determine potential shifts in preference through ontogeny. Tadpoles preferred being with a group over being alone, and the strength of this preference declined with increasing age. Furthermore, tadpoles preferred to be close to the larger of two groups. This preference was stronger in the 2 vs. 1 treatment. Mantella aurantiaca tadpoles, therefore, show clear social tendencies and possess the ability to spontaneously discriminate between group sizes. The lower preference at higher group sizes might indicate cognitive limitations or lower benefits of choosing the larger of two groups when overall group size is high. These findings are one of the few showing social preferences depending on group size in tadpoles and contribute to our understanding of social behaviour in amphibians in general.
... ephemeral pools) in which they develop (Polis and Myers 1985;Crump 1990Crump , 1992. Although tadpole cannibalism has been studied in a variety of contexts, such as kin recognition (Pfennig et al. 1993;Pfennig 1997;Gray et al. 2009), foraging behaviour (Caldwell and de Araujo 1998), phenotypic plasticity (Pfennig 1990(Pfennig , 1992, neuroethology (Fischer et al. 2020), and parental care (Summers 1999;Downie et al. 2001;Brown et al. 2009;Rojas 2014), its prevalence across the amphibian phylogeny is not well known. ...
Preprint
Cannibalistic behaviour between tadpoles of dendrobatoid poison frogs has been observed in several species with complex parental care dynamics, leading to the idea that it may have played a role in the evolution of parental care. However, the existence or characteristics of this behaviour beyond a handful of well-studied species remains largely unknown. I report direct and indirect observations of larval cannibalism in two species of the genus Phyllobates, which seem to occur less frequently than in other, better studied species, and possibly associated with body size differences. Beyond expanding the phylogenetic breadth of tadpole cannibalism among poison frogs, my observations point to interesting aspects of this behaviour, such as its apparently plastic and continuous nature, and highlight the importance of studying other lineages to understand the its drivers and effects on poison frog evolution.
... This inference implies that most species using phytotelmata as rearing sites should exhibit tadpole cannibalism, which appears not to be the case. Larval cannibalism has been studied in only a few species 37,95,[105][106][107][108][109] and, in some instances, it appears to be facultative or opportunistic. The fact that few instances of cannibalistic behavior have been documented might be the result of limited observations, and we thus propose that this behavior might be more widespread than currently thought. ...
Article
Full-text available
Many organisms have evolved adaptations to increase the odds of survival of their offspring. Parental care has evolved several times in animals including ectotherms. In amphibians, ~ 10% of species exhibit parental care. Among these, poison frogs (Dendrobatidae) are well-known for their extensive care, which includes egg guarding, larval transport, and specialized tadpole provisioning with trophic eggs. At least one third of dendrobatids displaying aposematism by exhibiting warning coloration that informs potential predators about the presence of defensive skin toxins. Aposematism has a central role in poison frog diversification, including diet specialization, and visual and acoustic communication; and it is thought to have impacted their reproductive biology as well. We tested the latter association using multivariate phylogenetic methods at the family level. Our results show complex relationships between aposematism and certain aspects of the reproductive biology in dendrobatids. In particular, aposematic species tend to use more specialized tadpole-deposition sites, such as phytotelmata, and ferry fewer tadpoles than non-aposematic species. We propose that aposematism may have facilitated the diversification of microhabitat use in dendrobatids in the context of reproduction. Furthermore, the use of resource-limited tadpole-deposition environments may have evolved in tandem with an optimal reproductive strategy characterized by few offspring, biparental care, and female provisioning of food in the form of unfertilized eggs. We also found that in phytotelm-breeders, the rate of transition from cryptic to aposematic phenotype is 17 to 19 times higher than vice versa. Therefore, we infer that the aposematism in dendrobatids might serve as an umbrella trait for the evolution and maintenance of their complex offspring-caring activities.
Article
Full-text available
Adelphobates contains three species, and the inaccurate identification of A. quinquevittatus and the scarcity of records of A. castaneoticus complicate inference of their distributions; the latter species occurs in sympatry with A. galactonotus. Our objective was to revise the distributions of Adelphobates by compiling data and modeling habitat suitability, as range limits may be shaped by landscape features and biotic interactions. We initially analyzed the existence of operational taxonomic units within the nominal species and subsequently inferred the observed and potential distributions, taking into account the possible independent lineages for the three species, and we also generated a molecular time tree to understand the chronology of interspecific diversification events. Adelphobates quinquevittatus was found to have a more easterly distribution than previously described, and specimens with phenotypic variation were found to occur in areas inconsistent with the modeling, and A. castaneoticus was concentrated in the Tapajós-Xingu interfluve, surrounded by A. galactonotus. Models indicated that the right bank of the Xingu River is suitable for both species, indeed, both were found there. Despite Adelphobates species having their distributions delimited by major Amazonian rivers, estimated divergence times predate the formation of the modern river network, suggesting that other mechanisms were involved in their diversification.
Article
Full-text available
Predator-prey interactions are vital for organismal survival. They shape anti-predator mechanisms and often depend on sensory abilities. Tadpoles use chemical cues, such as injury cues (alarm cues), to assess predation risks and modify their life-history, morphology, and behaviours accordingly. However, the prevalence of chemically mediated anti-predator responses in species with distinct ecological niches (e.g. within phytotelmata) remains unknown, hindering our understanding of the ecological significance and evolution of alarm substances. Therefore, our study aimed to investigate chemically mediated anti-predator responses in tadpoles of two Neotropical poison dart frogs, Ranitomeya sirensis and Epipedobates anthonyi (and compare their responses to two Palearctic model organisms, Rana temporaria and Bufo bufo, which are known to utilise alarm substances). Through behavioural bioassays, we exposed predator-naïve tadpoles to extracts of each species (i.e. con- and heterospecific cues), including water as a control (i.e. five treatments per species). We assessed changes in their activity before and after stimulus introduction. Our results show that E. anthonyi did not respond to any of the stimuli, whereas R. sirensis displayed increased activity levels exclusively in response to conspecific cues, but not to heterospecific cues. With this, our findings suggest a specialized recognition system in R. sirensis, potentially directed at conspecific competitors but likely unrelated to anti-predator mechanisms. In contrast, E. anthonyi may be insensitive to injury cues or utilize alternative sensory modalities to respond to acute predation events. This study sheds light on the chemical alarm response system of Neotropical poison dart frog tadpoles, providing foundational understanding of how dendrobatids react to injury cues. It prompts questions about the ecological significance and evolutionary implications of chemical communication in species facing extreme resource limitation during development and underscores the importance of comparative research for understanding chemical communication in diverse aquatic ecosystems.
Article
Cannibalistic behaviour between tadpoles of dendrobatoid poison frogs has been observed in several species with complex parental care dynamics, leading to the idea that it may have played a role in the evolution of parental care. However, the existence or characteristics of this behaviour remain largely unknown beyond a handful of well-studied species. I report direct and indirect observations of larval cannibalism in two species of the genus Phyllobates, which seem to occur less frequently than in better studied species, and are possibly associated with body size differences. Beyond expanding the phylogenetic breadth of tadpole cannibalism among poison frogs, my observations point to interesting aspects of this behaviour, such as its apparently plastic and continuous nature, and highlight the importance of studying other lineages to understand its drivers and effects on poison frog evolution.
Article
Full-text available
Inclusive fitness theory predicts that organisms can increase their fitness by helping or not harming relatives, and many animals modify their behavior toward kin in a manner consistent with this prediction. Morphogenesis also may be sensitive to kinship environment, particularly in species where certain individuals facultatively develop structures that can be used against conspecifics as weaponry. We tested this hypothesis by examining whether and how consanguinity affected the probability that a structurally distinctive carnivore phenotype, which is opportunistically cannibalistic, would be produced in plains spadefoot toad tadpoles (Spea bombifrons) and southern spadefoot toad tadpoles (S. multiplicata). For tadpoles of S. multiplicata, individuals were significantly more likely to express the carnivore phenotype in mixed sibship groups than in pure sibship groups. For tadpoles of S. bombifrons, individuals were significantly more likely to express the carnivore phenotype when reared alone than in pure sibship groups. Both outcomes were independent of food availability or sibship specific differences in size or growth rate, and waterborne chemical signals from nonkin were sufficient to trigger expression of the carnivore phenotype. Our results suggest that morphogenesis may be responsive to kinship environment in any species or population that occurs as multiple, environmentally induced forms (polyphenism) that differ in their ability to help or to harm others.
Article
Inclusive fitness theory predicts that organisms can increase their fitness by helping or not harming relatives, and many animals modify their behavior toward kin in a manner consistent with this prediction. Morphogenesis also may be sensitive to kinship environment, particularly in species where certain individuals facultatively develop structures that can be used against conspecifics as weaponry. We tested this hypothesis by examining whether and how consanguinity affected the probability that a structurally distinctive carnivore phenotype, which is opportunistically cannibalistic, would be produced in plains spadefoot toad tadpoles (Spea bombifrons) and southern spadefoot toad tadpoles (S. multiplicata). For tadpoles of S. multiplicata, individuals were significantly more likely to express the carnivore phenotype in mixed sibship groups than in pure sibship groups. For tadpoles of S. bombifrons, individuals were significantly more likely to express the carnivore phenotype when reared alone than in pure sibship groups. Both outcomes were independent of food availability or sibship specific differences in size or growth rate, and waterborne chemical signals from nonkin were sufficient to trigger expression of the carnivore phenotype. Our results suggest that morphogenesis may be responsive to kinship environment in any species or population that occurs as multiple, environmentally induced forms (polyphenism) that differ in their ability to help or to harm others.
Article
Tadpoles of Japanese toad (Bufo vulgaris) were cultured by an artificial feed and by forced cannibalism from the egg until metamorphosis finished. The best growth was found in the artificial feed group, however, the highest feed efficiency was observed in the cannibalism group. The tadpoles were metamorphosed by the culture with amino acid mixture feed but no body weight increase was seen in this case. The nutritional disturbance hindered the metamorphosis. The relative amino acid composition was constant throughout all the stages of the tadpole growth. The relative amino acid composition of the toad was diversed according to the species. Several unidentified ninhydrine-positive substances were detected on the column chromatograms of amino acid analysis of tadpoles and their adult forms.
Article
Three experiments on the ecological consequences of tail injury yielded mixed results. In the first experiment, experimentally incurred tail damage reduced growth and developmental rates in Rana catesbeiana tadpoles reared for 36 d in screen enclosures in a pond. In a second field experiment three levels of tail damage were combined with four different ages when the damage was inflicted. There were no differences in growth or development in the 12 combinations of these treatments among tadpoles reared in plastic containers for 60 d. In a third experiment, R. utricularia tadpoles with four levels of damaged tails were exposed to predation by newts (Notophthalmus viridescens). The numbers killed during 3 h intervals over 24 h did not differ among groups in a simple laboratory arena. These data suggest that tail loss by anuran larvae incurs little cost and therefore may be an important mechanism to reduce the effect of predation.