Content uploaded by Marius Dilling
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Marius Dilling on May 03, 2025
Content may be subject to copyright.
Received: November Revised: February Accepted: April
DOI: ./asap.
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
The authoritarian syndrome as an attempt to
restore control and its mediating role in
anti-Semitism and xenophobia in Germany
Marius Dilling1Alina Sorrentino2Elmar Brähler3
Jörg M. Fegert4Oliver Decker1,5
Else-Frenkel-Brunswik-Institute,
University of Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany
InstituteofPsychology,Universityof
Bonn, Bonn, Germany
University of Leipzig, University of
Mainz, Leipzig, Germany
University Hospital Ulm, Ulm, Germany
Else-Frenkel-Brunswik-Institute,
Department of Medical Psychology and
Medical Sociology, University of Leipzig,
Sigmund Freud University, Germany
Correspondence
Marius Dilling, University of Leipzig,
Else-Frenkel-Brunswik-Institute
Dittrichring -, Leipzig,
Germany. Email:
marius.dilling@uni-leipzig.de
Marius Dilling and Alina Sorrentino
should be considered joint first author.
Abstract
For many people, the coronavirus pandemic meant an
enormous and existential loss of control. At the same
time, an increase in right-wing extremist attitudes like
xenophobia could be observed in Germany. In this study,
we hypothesize that the loss of control caused by the
pandemic has contributed to the rise in xenophobic and
anti-Semitic attitudes in Germany. We propose that this
occurs through an attempt to restore control via ele-
ments of a revised authoritarian syndrome understood
as both the classic authoritarian dynamic of aggression,
submission and conventionalism on the one hand, and
a general belief in conspiracy theories on the other. In
a representative, probability-based study, N= par-
ticipants were surveyed on locus of control, right-wing
authoritarianism, conspiracy mentality, xenophobia, and
anti-Semitism. It was found that right-wing authoritarian-
ism and conspiracy mentality mediated the relationship
between external locus of control and xenophobia (par-
tial mediation) and anti-Semitism (full mediation). Sur-
prisingly, internal control beliefs had a direct effect on
This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and
reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
© The Author(s). Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy published by Wiley Periodicals LLC on behalf of Society for the
Psychological Study of Social Issues.
Anal Soc Issues Public Policy ;:e. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/asap 1of25
https://doi.org/./asap.
2of25 DILLING .
right-wing authoritarianism—an effect that also leads to
increased resentment. We conclude that social crises make
people particularly vulnerable to regaining control via
conspiracy theories and authoritarianism, which harbors
dangers such as right-wing extremism as a consequence.
Limitations are discussed.
KEYWORDS
authoritarianism, conspiracy mentality, locus of control, anti-
semitism, xenophobia, structural equation modelling
Public significance statement
This study examines how a perceived loss of control dur-
ing the COVID- pandemic may have contributed to rising
xenophobic and anti-Semitic attitudes in Germany. Find-
ings suggest that individuals may attempt to regain control
through authoritarian attitudes and a general belief in
conspiracy theories, reinforcing prejudice. We conclude
that social crises make people particularly vulnerable to
these dynamics, which harbors dangers such as right-wing
extremism as a consequence.
INTRODUCTION
Societies worldwide are confronted with multiple, overlapping crises—a phenomenon increas-
ingly referred to as a polycrisis (Tooze, ). Among these challenges, the COVID- pandemic
not only triggered a public health emergency but also precipitated a profound collective loss of
control. In Germany, for example, the temporary expansion of executive powers to combat the
crisis was accompanied by significant restrictions on individual freedoms, leaving citizens feeling
powerless in the face of an invisible, potentially lethal threat. One’s own dependence on the state,
science, and on chance as well as the anomie that accompanies it, became very salient during the
pandemic. This loss of control is not merely situational; it strikes at a fundamental human need
(Brehm, ). Extensive research has demonstrated that a sense of control is vital for well-being,
whereas its absence is associated with increased anxiety, depression, and other negative emotions
(Fritsche et al., ). When an entire society experiences a diminished internal belief in personal
control, the political consequences can be profound. Theories linking a deprived need for control
with political radicalization suggest that feelings of powerlessness may foster right-wing extrem-
ist attitudes, including xenophobia and anti-Semitism (Decker, Kiess et al., ; Zick & Mokros,
).
Empirical studies provide further support for this idea. For instance, research indicates that
individuals with an external locus of control—those who believe that outcomes are determined
by external forces—are more likely to endorse xenophobic and anti-Semitic views (Holtmann &
15302415, 2025, 2, Downloaded from https://spssi.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/asap.70009 by Marius Dilling - Universitaetsbibliothek Leipzi , Wiley Online Library on [03/05/2025]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
AUTHORITARIAN CONTROL 3of25
Görl, ; see also Duckitt, , for similar results regarding racist attitudes). This phenomenon
is observable in various groups, such as voters of the right-wing extremist German party Alterna-
tive für Deutschland (Alternative for Germany, AfD; Mays et al., ) and right-wing extremist
adolescents (Sturzbecher et al., ).
Building on these findings, we propose that this devaluation of outgroups—a central element of
German right-wing extremist ideology (Kreis, )—can be understood partly as a dysfunctional
attempt to restore a lost sense of control via an authoritarian detour. We thereby refer to a recent
conceptualization of authoritarianism, called the authoritarian syndrome, which was developed
by a group of German researchers (Decker et al., ). The authoritarian syndrome is understood
as the dynamic of authoritarian aggression, submission and conventionalism (nowadays mainly
referred to as Right-Wing Authoritarianism; RWA; see Altemeyer, ) on the one hand, comple-
mented by a general belief in conspiracy theories on the other.Most importantly, we hypothesize
that the attempt to re-establish control through the authoritarian syndrome is dysfunctional:
although these mechanisms may temporarily alleviate feelings of powerlessness, they simulta-
neously contribute to the marginalization of outgroups, thereby posing a threat to democracy.
In this paper, we investigate these interrelationships in greater detail using structural equation
modeling. We hypothesize that internal and external control beliefs are positively related to two
outgroup derogatory attitudes that hold particular significance in right-wing extremist ideology:
that is xenophobia and anti-Semitism. Furthermore, we assume that these effects are mediated
by the aforementioned elements of the authoritarian syndrome.
Loss of control and control restitution
Rotter’s (, ,) concept of the locus of control suggests that individuals attribute out-
comes either to their own actions (internal control) or to external forces (external control)—which
are not traceable to one’s own behavior—such as luck, fate or other external influences. Empirical
findings from the COVID- pandemic illustrate that social crises can shift these control beliefs:
during the pandemic, individuals’ external control scores increased while internal control scores
decreased, a change that was associated with heightened psychological distress, lower mental
well-being and an increased perception of threat (e.g., Krampe et al., ;Taginietal.,;
Würtzen et al., ).
Compensatory control theory (Kay et al., , ;Landauetal.,) states that such a
perceived loss of control motivates individuals to seek alternative sources of control—ranging
from increased reliance on religious or governmental structures or by identifying patterns in noise,
for example, by holding on to superstitions and conspiracy theories.
Rothbaum et al. () introduced a two-process model of control in which people seek control
not only through direct manipulation of the environment (primary control) but also, if this is not
possible—in particular, when there is perceived uncontrollability—by adapting the self to the
environment (secondary control). According to the authors, secondary control can be perceived as
safer, more continuous and associated with fewer disappointments and can also be preferred for
these reasons. In most cases, however, forms of primary and secondary control are intertwined.
Superstition, that is, the belief in supernatural, magical powers, is also included as part of the dimension projectivity by
Decker et al. (). However, in this paper, we focus on sadomasochist authoritarianism/RWA and conspiracy mentality
and their relevance as potential mediators between Locus of Control and xenophobia and anti-Semitism respectively.
15302415, 2025, 2, Downloaded from https://spssi.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/asap.70009 by Marius Dilling - Universitaetsbibliothek Leipzi , Wiley Online Library on [03/05/2025]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
4of25 DILLING .
Secondary control is regarded by the authors as being close to the concept of external Locus
of Control and can be expressed in four different forms: (a) predictive control, a limitation
of one’s own expectations by attributing an event to one’s own limited abilities and passivity,
(b) illusory control via magical thinking such as belief in luck or fate and the active attempt
to bring luck to one’s side, (c) vicarious control through identification with powerful leaders,
and (d) interpretive control by attributing a deeper meaning to uncontrollable events. How-
ever, Rothbaum et al.’s thesis in their influential paper is not uncontroversial (Skinner, ).
The model of group based control restoration (Fritsche et al., ; Stollberg et al., ), points
out that processes of control restoration through group identification are anchored in the self
and can therefore not be clearly labeled as secondary, arguing for extended primary control
mechanisms.
However, our focus is not on the debate whether the control restoration is secondary or
extended primary. We propose that elements of the revised authoritarian syndrome (Decker et al.,
) may represent an attempt at restoring a diminished sense of control. This can manifest in
two ways: through identification with powerful leaders, as inherent in the “classic” understand-
ing of authoritarianism as already described by the seminal works of Erich Fromm ( [])
and in the form of magical thinking and the tendency to attribute deeper meaning and agency to
uncontrollable events—which is particularly present in a general belief in conspiracies.
The revised authoritarian syndrome
Right-wing extremist attitudes like xenophobia and anti-Semitism (as defined by Heller et al.,
;Kreis,) are multifaceted and deeply rooted social phenomena, extensively investigated
across diverse theoretical traditions and disciplines at various levels (Eicker, ). Initial attempts
on theorizing authoritarianism go back almost a century (e.g., Fromm, ; Stagner, ). Ever
since the Authoritarian Personality of the so-called Berkeley group surrounding Adorno et al.
(), authoritarianism has been considered one of the most influential predictors of right-
wing extremist attitudes.Seeking to explain the rise of fascism and anti-Semitism in Europe,
the authors proposed an individual susceptibility to prejudice and fascist tendencies, rooted in
early childhood family dynamics and hindering the development of a stable moral value system
within the personality. This ego weakness, they argued, fosters identification with and submission
to authority while redirecting repressed aggression toward out-groups in the form of discrimi-
nation and prejudice. The Berkeley group developed their California F-Scale, an influential but
highly criticized (Christie, ; for an overview, see Funke, and Iser, ) measure that
originally captured authoritarianism in nine psychoanalytically based facets. In keeping with
the empiricist trend, Altemeyer () discarded the developmental-psychodynamic and social-
theoretical model and instead adopted a pragmatic approach. He focused on the facets with the
highest discriminatory power and reduced the scale to three subfacets: conventionalism, author-
itarian aggression, and authoritarian submission. His right-wing authoritarianism (RWA) scale
has been shown in previous research to be an important and reliable predictor of anti-democratic
and far-right–in particular anti-Semitism and Xenophobia–and sexist attitudes, as well as support
for military violence and war (for an overview, see Duckitt, ; for Germany, see also Decker,
Kiess et al., ).
Other explanatory approaches include the modernization loser thesis (Spier, ), the impact of various facets of
deprivation (Rippl & Baier, ), and the cultural backlash thesis (Inglehart & Norris, ).
15302415, 2025, 2, Downloaded from https://spssi.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/asap.70009 by Marius Dilling - Universitaetsbibliothek Leipzi , Wiley Online Library on [03/05/2025]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
AUTHORITARIAN CONTROL 5of25
While Altemeyer’s three-dimensional scale is still a suitable and widely used operational-
ization of what Fromm (, []) understood as a “sadomasochist”psychodynamic, his
rather pragmatic conceptualization of authoritarianism may, however, overlook aspects crucial
for understanding contemporary authoritarianism (e.g., Aho, ; Funke, ). This is high-
lighted by the variety of theoretical approaches that exist today. Examples of recognized theories
include the authoritarian reaction (Oesterreich, ), which emphasizes authoritarianism as a
reaction to perceived threats; Feldman’s () exploration of authoritarianism in the context of
social conformity and the Dual-Process Motivational Model (DPM; Duckitt, , ), which
situates authoritarianism within a broader framework of social and ideological attitudes.
Recently, the COVID- pandemic and the accompanying protests that were particularly
prominent in Germany and Austria, have reignited the academic debate in Germany about the
dimensionality of authoritarianism. These protests revealed a mix of conspiracy beliefs, well-
known authoritarian attitudes, and even openness to overt neo-Nazism–an overlap of phenomena
already considered by the Berkeley group.
Conspiracy theories usually propose intentional malevolent forces operating in the background
as an explanation for an event, and, although legally protected by freedom of expression, are
regarded as a threat to democracy: they are associated with political extremism (van Prooijen et al.,
)—especially with right-wing extremist attitudes and group-based enmity (Dilling et al., ;
Imhoff & Decker, ; Rees & Lamberty, ). Individuals who believe in one conspiracy theory
usually believe in multiple conspiracy theories, even if they contradict each other (Goertzel, ;
Wood et al., ). Imhoff and Bruder () therefore postulate a general conspiracy mentality
(Moscovici, ) that should be distinguished from specific conspiracy theories due to its greater
stability and less ideological coloration. Although the relationship between authoritarianism and
conspiracy beliefs is ambiguous (Abalakina-Paap et al., ; Baier & Manzoni, ; Douglas
et al., ; Grzesiak-Feldman, ), most studies indicate a positive correlation (Dilling et al.,
; Đorđević et al., ; Decker et al., ; Grzesiak-Feldman & Irzycka, ; Richey, ).
The overlap between conspiracy beliefs and right-wing authoritarianism is not confined to the
German context; it can also be observed in phenomena such as post-factual politics and Trump-
ism, highlighting the need to adjust the RWA concept to contemporary times. Drawing on classic
studies of authoritarianism, some scholars argue for renewed theoretical focus on conspiracy
beliefs, related phenomena, and the broader social context (see Amlinger & Nachtwey, ;Hei-
demeyer et al., in preparation; Henkelmann et al., ; Jäger, ;King,). This scientific
reorientation and reconceptualization are reflected in the concept of the authoritarian syndrome
(Decker et al., ), which we highlight in this study. Decker et al. () suggest that the gen-
eral belief in conspiracy theories or in the omnipresent existence of sinister and evil machinations
should be (re-)integrated into this concept of contemporary authoritarianism. They present a two-
factor model, with the ‘authoritarian syndrome’ as higher order factor encompassing both the
classic “sadomasochistic” authoritarianism (aggression, submission and conventionalism) and
conspiracist alongside superstitious beliefs as lower order factors, thus proposing two distinct yet
authoritarian social dynamics. With reference to the Berkeley group, they call this second dimen-
sion “projectivity”, which, psychoanalytically speaking, represents a denial of reality. It is based
Within the Studies on Authority and the Family, Erich Fromm ( []) used the term sadomasochism to describe
both the “masochist” aspect of the renouncing and voluntary authoritarian submission on one hand and the “sadist”
aspect which is expressed in the authoritarian aggression against the weak on the other. Oesterreich later pointed out that
the term “sadomasochism” evokes a clinical association for everyday, non-pathological processes—a problem that Fromm
himself later addressed by replacing the term “sadomasochism” with authoritarianism (cf. Oesterreich , p. ).
15302415, 2025, 2, Downloaded from https://spssi.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/asap.70009 by Marius Dilling - Universitaetsbibliothek Leipzi , Wiley Online Library on [03/05/2025]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
6of25 DILLING .
on the defense mechanism of splitting into “good” and “bad” parts, followed by the identifica-
tion with the “good” parts of one’s own self or group and the externalization of the “bad” parts
onto “others”. In the debate on the dimensionality of authoritarianism, we follow the analysis of
Decker et al. () and understand conspiracy beliefs in the following as a sub-dimension of the
authoritarian syndrome.
The authoritarian syndrome as an attempt to restore control
We argue that the revised authoritarian syndrome may represent an attempt to restore control,
especially in the case of feeling dependent on external forces or losing control. In Studies on
Authority and Family (Horkheimer, []), Fromm already spoke of the reassuring func-
tion and “prosthetic” security (p. ) of authoritarianism, through which the world would lose its
chaotic character for the individual. Rothbaum et al. () state that “by aligning themselves with
more powerful others, individuals can share in their victories and in their accomplishments—in
short, in their control.” (p. ) and compare their concept of vicarious control to Fromm’s ()
process of deindividuation, in which the individual flees from their own individual freedom due to
an “unbearable state of his powerlessness and loneliness” (p. ) in order to gain power through
identification with authorities. According to Fromm, however, this mechanism is only a tem-
porary relief (p. ), which cannot establish permanent control, but leads to a vicious circle of
dependency,insecurity and powerlessness. The previously mentioned approaches by Oesterreich
(), Feldman (), and Duckitt (, ) also support this idea as they all link authoritar-
ianism to uncertainty, threat, and the desire for stability and security, all of which are ultimately
related to control in some way.
As for the empirical relationship between LoC and RWA, the literature is sparse and incon-
clusive. Some older studies primarily show no relationship between the constructs (Baron, ;
McCollaum & Lester, ; Nicol, ), while others indicate positive relationships with internal,
rather than external, locus of control (Diakonova & Gilgen, ), particularly in relation to the
dimension of conventionalism (Chertkova et al., ). However, the idea that authoritarianism
in particular could also be used as a control restitution mechanism in uncertain times of crisis is
supported by a large body of literature that shows that RWA reacts strongly to and interacts with
societal threats (e.g., Duckitt, ; Duckitt & Sibley, ; Haase et al., ; Russo et al.,
provides an overview). In line with these findings, increasing RWA scores were observed during
the pandemic (e.g., Golec de Zavala et al., ). Hartman et al. () even showed that as fear
of the pandemic increased, RWA had a stronger effect on the expression of nationalistic or anti-
immigrant attitudes. In Germany in particular, it has been observed that the pandemic has led to a
social division into the two camps of the unvaccinated and the vaccinated, with Decker, Kalkstein
et al. () identifying authoritarian dynamics in both. While many unvaccinated individuals
sought to restore their sense of control via projection in the form of conspiracy beliefs, a small
portion of the vaccinated—most of whom were moderate, non-authoritarian—also attempted to
do so through a sadomasochistic reaction (Decker, Kalkstein et al., , p. ), characterized
by identification with state-imposed COVID- regulations and exhibiting hostility towards rule
violators.
However, we do acknowledge that the study of conspiracy beliefs itself is a broad and independent field of research that
recognizes the phenomenon not as a form of authoritarian attitudes, but as a phenomenon sui generis.
15302415, 2025, 2, Downloaded from https://spssi.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/asap.70009 by Marius Dilling - Universitaetsbibliothek Leipzi , Wiley Online Library on [03/05/2025]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
AUTHORITARIAN CONTROL 7of25
In terms of conspiracy mentality, the idea that belief in almighty conspirators can be an attempt
to deal with an external locus of control or loss of control might appear contradictory at first
glance. However, it can be explained well by their underlying existential, epistemic and social
motivations (Douglas et al., ): Conspiracy theories give order to the world, because they divide
the world into good and evil and place external control in the hands of powerful others. Thus,
“the world loses its chaotic character” (Fromm, [], p. ). Especially during crises and
uncertain situations—for instance, at the onset of the COVID- pandemic—perceived loss of
control not only heightens risk perception but also foster greater belief in pandemic-related and
general conspiracy beliefs (Šrol et al., ; van Prooijen & Douglas, ). Conspiracy beliefs may
restore control at least on an intrapsychic level (Lamberty, ), as they provide the feeling of
“having uncovered the conspiracy” and the belief that historical processes can be controlled and
planned (Celik et al., ). Instead of feeling powerless against the “system” as a whole, they
offer a kind of abbreviated critique of capitalism while also enhancing the status of one’s own
group. In this regard, a substantial body of research suggests that a diminished sense of control
is strongly linked to a propensity for conspiracy beliefs (Abalakina-Paap et al., ; Whitson &
Galinsky, ), a finding that is complemented by evidence linking political (Bruder et al., ;
Kofta et al., ) and economic deprivation as well as insecure employment (Imhoff & Decker,
) with conspiracy mentality. On the other hand, reinforcing individuals’ sense of control may
help to reduce these tendencies (Mao et al., ; van Prooijen & Acker, ).
However, it remains unclear whether the desired control restitution, should it be sought, actu-
ally occurs. While Van Prooijen () contends that conspiracy theories exacerbate feelings of
existential threat, leading to an even stronger belief in further conspiracies and a general con-
spiracy mentality, Stojanov et al. () critique conspiracy theories as ineffective for control
restoration. They argue that such theories are not socially accepted, offer too much control,
and compete with more credible explanations. In later work (Stojanov & Halberstadt, ;Sto-
janov et al., , ), they find evidence that this effect is domain-specific and only occurs if
other, less stigmatized sources of control are unavailable. Jolley and Douglas (a, b) fur-
ther show that rather than restoring control, conspiracy theories can actually increase political
powerlessness—questioning direction of causality. Regardless of their success, however, the lit-
erature to date suggests that conspiracy beliefs can be a problematic attempt to cope with lack of
control, indicating both phenomena are positively associated.
The theoretical model and present study
Based on the literature, we argue that external control beliefs activate a need for control restitu-
tion, the satisfaction of which can be approached in a functional as well as dysfunctional ways.
This paper focuses on a dysfunctional and problematic process: Coping with lack of control
through authoritarian compensation. Previous research suggests that there is also a direct link
between external locus of control beliefs and anti-Semitism and xenophobia, however, we explic-
itly hypothesize that this effect is at least partially mediated by authoritarianism and conspiracy
mentality.
The following hypotheses can be summarized:
Hypothesis 1a/b. External LoC directly predicts conspiracy mentality/RWA.
Hypothesis 2a/b. External LoC directly predicts anti-Semitism/xenophobia.
15302415, 2025, 2, Downloaded from https://spssi.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/asap.70009 by Marius Dilling - Universitaetsbibliothek Leipzi , Wiley Online Library on [03/05/2025]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
8of25 DILLING .
Hypothesis 3a/b. Conspiracy mentality predicts anti-Semitism/xenophobia.
Hypothesis 4a/b. RWA predicts anti-Semitism/xenophobia.
Hypothesis 5a/b. External LoC is hypothesized to have an indirect effect on Xenophobia /
anti-Semitism through the mediator conspiracy mentality.
Hypothesis 6a/b. External LoC is hypothesized to have an indirect effect on Xenophobia /
anti-Semitism through the mediator RWA.
As previously mentioned, the evidence on the relationship between authoritarianism and inter-
nal control beliefs in particular is unclear. Since we build on the argument that authoritarianism
and right-wing extremism increase as a result of an experienced loss of control and that internal
control beliefs do not represent a loss of control, we would not expect a positive correlation at this
point. However, since there are ambivalent research results (Chertkova et al., ;Diakonova&
Gilgen, ), we nevertheless test the path exploratively.
METHOD
STATA . as well as IBM Statistics SPSS .. () were used for the subsequent analyses.
Sample
The probability-based dataset of the German population used in this paper was collected as part
of the Leipzig Authoritarianism Studies (Decker, Kiess et al., ). These studies have been
conducted in Germany since and aim to survey authoritarian and far-right attitudes. The
dataset, collected between March and May , was gathered while the country was still under
the impact of the COVID- pandemic. Residents of the New Federal States (Neue Bundesländer)
were overrepresented. The minimum age of participation was . The dataset was collected using
the paper-pencil method in face-to-face interviews by the independent polling firm USUMA. To
ensure randomness, Germany was divided into sample points, and households were randomly
selected via random route method. The target respondents within these households were selected
using the Kish selection grid, a method for randomly selecting respondents from households with
more than one person. In order to reduce the social desirability bias for items that are subject to
such an effect in Germany (such as classic anti-Semitism in particular), respondents completed
their questionnaire independently and were able to hand it over to the interviewers sealed in an
envelope if they wished. Informed consent was obtained by all participants, at least one parent
and/or legal guardian was informed as well in case of minors. The response rate was .%. The
samples include persons with an immigrant background and persons without German citizen-
ship. The sample consists of , individuals, with a nearly equal distribution of females (.%)
and males (.%), and a small percentage identifying as other (.%). The age range of partic-
ipants spans from to over years, with an average age of . years (SD =.). In terms
of education, .% of participants have a university entrance qualification or higher. Regard-
ing income, the average equivalent household income is €,. (SD =€.), with .%
of households earning between €, and €, per month. Unemployment in the sample is
15302415, 2025, 2, Downloaded from https://spssi.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/asap.70009 by Marius Dilling - Universitaetsbibliothek Leipzi , Wiley Online Library on [03/05/2025]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
AUTHORITARIAN CONTROL 9of25
TABLE 1 Demographic characteristics of the sample.
Demographic variable N%
Sex
Female .
Male .
Other .
Age
– years .
– years .
– years .
– years .
– years .
– years .
≥ years .
Employment status
Unemployed .
Education
University entrance qualification or higher educational attainment .
Missing .
Income
≤ EURO .
>– EURO .
>– EURO .
> EURO .
Missing .
Note:N=. Missing values are indicated for some variables.
relatively low at .%. The sample is representative of the population in terms of key demographic
variables. Table above provides a more detailed overview of the demographic characteristics.
Instruments
A full overview of the following instruments, their wording and scaling, can be found in
Appendix A.
Locus of control
We use the IE-, a two-dimensional conceptualization of Kovaleva et al. (, ), which mea-
sures control beliefs using four items on a five-point-Likert scale, two of which capture internal
control (“I am my own boss.”, “If I work hard, I will succeed.”) and two that capture external con-
trol (“Whether at work or in my personal life: What I do is mainly determined by others.”, “Fate
often gets in the way of my plans.”).
15302415, 2025, 2, Downloaded from https://spssi.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/asap.70009 by Marius Dilling - Universitaetsbibliothek Leipzi , Wiley Online Library on [03/05/2025]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
10 of 25 DILLING .
Right-wing authoritarianism
We use the KSA- (Beierlein et al., ), a nine-item German modern version of the RWA Scale.
Three items each are assigned to the dimensions: authoritarian aggression, authoritarian submis-
siveness, and conventionalism. The scale was developed to address problems of traditional RWA
scales due to a) their time and context boundedness and b) the non-distinctness of their sub-
scales. Example items include “Traditions should absolutely be cultivated and maintained.” and
“We need strong leaders in order to live safely in society”. The scale has a five-point Likert format.
Conspiracy mentality
We captured conspiracy mentality (CM) using a three-item short form of the Conspiracy Men-
tality Scale (Imhoff & Bruder, ) on a seven-point Likert scale. Items are “Most people do
not recognize to what extent our life is determined by conspiracies that are concocted in secret.”
and “Politicians and other leaders are nothing but the string puppets of powers operating in the
background”.
Anti-Semitism and xenophobia
We used the Questionnaire on Right-Wing Extremist Attitudes—Leipzig Form (FR-LF) (Decker
et al., ; Heller et al., ), which captures right-wing extremism on six dimensions on a five-
point Likert Scale. Xenophobia and anti-Semitism represent two dimensions of this scale, each
measured with three items. Example items are “The foreigners only come here to exploit our
welfare state” for xenophobia and “Even today the influence of the Jews is too great” for anti-
Semitism.
RESULTS
Descriptive analysis
Descriptive statistics for every item can be found in Appendix-Table A. Skewness lies between −.
(internal LoC) and . (anti-Semitism), while kurtosis ranges from −. (Xenophobia) and .
(internal LoC).
Reliability and correlation analysis
The reliability of the scales used was tested using both Cronbach’s 𝛼and McDonald’s ω.The
indices can be seen in Table .Cronbach’s𝛼and McDonalds ωrange from . (anti-Semitism)
to . (Conspiracy Mentality). These indices indicate a good to very good internal consistency of
the scales used.
A full correlation matrix for all items can be found in Appendix-Table B. Additionally, a cor-
relation matrix of mean indices for measures used can be found in Table . The mean index of
15302415, 2025, 2, Downloaded from https://spssi.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/asap.70009 by Marius Dilling - Universitaetsbibliothek Leipzi , Wiley Online Library on [03/05/2025]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
AUTHORITARIAN CONTROL 11 of 25
TABLE 2 Mean values, standard deviations, internal consistencies, and intercorrelations of the measures.
MSD Loc-E Loc-I Xeno AS AGR SUB CONV CM RWA
Loc-E . . /
Loc-I . . −.** /
Xeno . . .** −. (.; .)
AS . . .** −.** .** (.; .)
AGR . . . . .** .** (.; .)
SUB . . .** −. .** .** .** (.; .)
CONV . . .** .*.** .** .** .** (.; .)
CM . . .** −.** .** .** .** .** .** (.; .)
RWA . . .** . .** .** .** .** .** .** (.; .)
Note:N=; due to the listwise deletion, the number of cases differs here from the total data set. The number of cases here corresponds to that used later in the SEM. Because the missing
values for each indicator do not exceed %, we assume that exclusion did not bias our results (Schafer & Graham, ).
*=p<.; ** =p<. (two-tailed); Bonferroni-adjusted significance levels. Loc-E, external locus of control; Loc-I, internal locus of control; Xeno, xenophobia; AS, anti-Semitism; AGR,
authoritarian aggression; SUB, authoritarian submission; CONV, conventionalism; CM, conspiracy mentality; RWA, mean index of the indicator itemsofSUB,AGR,CONV;Cronbach’salpha
(first) and McDonald’s omega (second) are presented in parentheses. McDonald’s omega and Cronbach’s alpha require at least three variables for estimation, so not applicable for locus of control
(see Appendix-Table Bfor pairwise correlation).
15302415, 2025, 2, Downloaded from https://spssi.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/asap.70009 by Marius Dilling - Universitaetsbibliothek Leipzi , Wiley Online Library on [03/05/2025]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
12 of 25 DILLING .
internal locus of control is not significantly correlated with the mean index of xenophobia. The
sub-dimension of conventionalism and internal locus of control are positively correlated. All other
correlation coefficients are statistically significant at p<. and their direction is in line with
the theory.
SEM analysis
Since the relationship and mediation of effects of multidimensional constructs are to be investi-
gated, the procedure of structural equation modeling (SEM) with mediator effects (Hayes, )is
used. For this purpose, the assumed dimensionality of the constructs is first examined confirma-
tory with a measurement model. Based on the theoretical considerations, the measured indicator
items are then assigned to different factors and transferred into a structural model. Since the indi-
rect effect of the mediator effect is the product of two regression coefficients, the assumption of
normal distribution of these effects in the population may be violated (Hayes, ). Additionally,
the Doornik-Hansen omnibus test rejects the null hypothesis of multivariate normality (chi()
=.; p<.). To test our indirect effects we therefore make use of a bootstrap with
replications. The results of the SEM are presented in Figure and Table .
In the model depicted in Figure , we permitted two additional correlations among the resid-
uals. Firstly, there is a correlation (rε, ε =.) between the residuals of xenophobia and
anti-Semitism latent constructs (MI =.), reflecting their position as interconnected facets
within a broader syndrome of right-wing extremist attitudes or group-focused enmity (e.g., Heller
et al., ). Additionally, we allowed for a residual correlation between internal and exter-
nal locus of control constructs, correlating rinternal, external =−. (MI =,), reflecting their
theoretically contrasting positions as suggested in the literature.
Allfactorloadingsaresignificantatp<.. The likelihood-ratio-χ-test compares our model
with a saturated model (df =), which always perfectly fits the data. Our model significantly
differs from a saturated model by χ() =. with p<.. Given the test’s sensitivity to
sample size (Bollen, ), global fit indices provide a more accurate evaluation. The CFI for the
measurement model is ., and the TLI is ., indicating a good model fit. The RMSEA is .
[CI =.; .; pclose =.], also suggesting an adequate fit. Finally, the SRMR of . indi-
cates a good model fit. Overall, these results suggest a good model fit (see Appendix-Table Cfor
comparison with a model without permitted residual correlations).
Direct effects
Based on the structural equation model (Table ), hypotheses Ha/b can be accepted, as we observe
a positive direct effect of external control beliefs on conspiracy mentality (β=.) and RWA
(β=.). In contrast, the exploratively tested direct paths between internal control beliefs and
conspiracy mentality, anti-Semitism and Xenophobia are not significant. Surprisingly, internal
locus of control has a medium-strength positive direct effect on RWA, which is significant at
p=.. The direct effect of external locus of control on anti-Semitism is not significant (p=.),
therefore hypothesis Hamust be rejected. Hypothesis Hbcan be accepted: external locus of con-
trol directly predicts Xenophobia (β=.). However, this is due to full mediation (in terms of
anti-Semitism), and partial mediation (in terms of Xenophobia) by RWA and Conspiracy Men-
tality. Unsurprisingly, the direct effects of conspiracy mentality and RWA on xenophobia and
15302415, 2025, 2, Downloaded from https://spssi.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/asap.70009 by Marius Dilling - Universitaetsbibliothek Leipzi , Wiley Online Library on [03/05/2025]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
AUTHORITARIAN CONTROL 13 of 25
FIGURE 1 Path diagram of the SEM analysis.
Note:N=. Standardized factor loadings. AGR, authoritarian aggression; SUB, authoritarian submission;
CONV, conventionalism; AUT, authoritarianism/RWA; Xeno, xenophobia; AS, anti-Semitism; VM, conspiracy
mentality; Internal, internal locus of control; External, external locus of control.
anti-Semitism are positive and significant, with standardized coefficients ranging from β=.
to β=.—Hypotheses Ha/b and Ha/b can be accepted. In comparison, the effect of RWA is sig-
nificantly stronger (β=.; β=.), but the effect of conspiracy mentality on anti-Semitism (β
=.) and on xenophobia (β=.) is nevertheless not negligible.
Indirect effects
Given that the exploratively tested direct effect of internal locus of control on RWA was significant,
an indirect effect was also tested. Indeed, our model reveals two significant indirect effects of
internal locus of control on RWA, with an effect of β=. on xenophobia and β=. on anti-
Semitism. We will discuss these surprising effects in the discussion.
15302415, 2025, 2, Downloaded from https://spssi.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/asap.70009 by Marius Dilling - Universitaetsbibliothek Leipzi , Wiley Online Library on [03/05/2025]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
14 of 25 DILLING .
TABLE 3 Direct, indirect and total effects of our SEM.
→Dependent variable
Independent variable ML-estimator (standardized coefficients) SE z
Direct effects
→Conspiracy mentality
Internal LoC . . .
External LoC .** . .
→RWA
Internal LoC .** . .
External LoC .** . .
→anti-Semitism
Internal LoC −. . −.
External LoC . . .
Conspiracy mentality .** . .
RWA .** . .
→Xenophobia
Internal LoC . . .
External LoC .*. .
Conspiracy mentality .** . .
RWA .** . .
Indirect effects (bootstrap =)
I-LoC →CM →Xenophobia ., % CI [−., .] . .
I-LoC →CM →anti-Semitism ., % CI [−., .] . .
I-LoC →RWA →xenophobia .**, % CI [., .] . .
I-LoC →RWA →anti-Semitism .**, % CI [., .] . .
E-LoC →CM →xenophobia .**, % CI [., .] . .
E-LoC →CM →anti-Semitism .**, % CI [., .] . .
E-LoC →RWA →xenophobia .**, % CI [., .] . .
E-LoC →RWA →anti-Semitism .**, % CI [., .] . .
Total effects
→Xenophobia
Internal LoC .** . .
External LoC .** . .
→Anti-Semitism
Internal LoC . . .
External LoC .** . .
Note:N=. In the model, we controlled for gender ( =woman), age, university entrance qualification and the equivalent
household income. For reasons of clarity and better readability, we do not show the corresponding paths in the model. Modification
indices (MI) <. *p<., **p<..
The mediation effects (Ha/b und Ha/b) are statistically significant and, there is even a full
mediation of the effect of external locus of control on anti-Semitism through the mediators RWA
and conspiracy mentality. The indirect effects of external control beliefs mediated by conspiracy
15302415, 2025, 2, Downloaded from https://spssi.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/asap.70009 by Marius Dilling - Universitaetsbibliothek Leipzi , Wiley Online Library on [03/05/2025]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
AUTHORITARIAN CONTROL 15 of 25
mentality are β=. (xenophobia), and β=. (anti-Semitism). In the case of mediation via RWA,
the indirect effects are β=. (xenophobia) and β=. (anti-Semitism).
Total effects
The significant total effect of external control beliefs—mediated by RWA and conspiracy
mentality—is β=. for both xenophobia and anti-Semitism. The total effect of internal locus
of control, which is only mediated by RWA, is β=. for xenophobia and is significant, and
β=. for anti-Semitism. The latter total effect, however, is not significant (p=.).
DISCUSSION
We have argued that xenophobia and anti-Semitism can be seen as a possible outcome of an
authoritarian and problematic attempt to re-establish control. In line with this argumentation,
our data from a comprehensive probability-based sample showed that external LoC, mediated
by elements of the authoritarian syndrome (Decker et al., ), predicts xenophobia and anti-
Semitism. Building on this finding, it may be precisely times of crisis and uncertainty that—due
to an increase in external locus of control—can make people susceptible and vulnerable to author-
itarian and right-wing radicalization, as the example of the COVID- pandemic shows, and
require special political sensitivity. However, a loss of control may not inherently result in cop-
ing in terms of the authoritarian syndrome. Individuals can respond in different ways, with some
exhibiting authoritarian tendencies while others engaging in alternative coping strategies, such as
collective action. Certain predispositions might make individuals more likely to follow the author-
itarian path, though further research is needed to explore these mechanisms. In this context, it is
likely that groups of people who are already economically or politically deprived and are there-
fore limited in their ability to exercise control are particularly vulnerable to this dynamic. Further
research is needed to explore these mechanisms. It is particularly striking that there is no direct
effect between external locus of control and anti-Semitism (p=.) when we control for RWA
and conspiracy mentality, and the direct effect between external locus of control and xenophobia
is only slightly larger and just barely significant (p=.). This underlines all the more that it is
not the loss of control itself, but the authoritarian reaction to it, that poses a particular danger to
democracy. Thus, since a loss of control does not necessarily lead to right-wing extremist attitudes
in forms of xenophobia and anti-Semitism, future research should investigate whether, in addi-
tion to the problematic attempt to restore control discussed in our model, potentially “healthy”
and functional alternatives of (extended) primary and secondary control could counteract author-
itarian and far-right dynamics in uncertain times. In doing so, vulnerable groups should also be
identified and addressed. Strengthening primary control in another area could also be effective:
Kiess and Schmidt (), for example, report that participation, recognition and solidarity in
the workplace—also known democratic efficacy at the workplace—can strengthen democracy and
Loeys et al. () point out that the test for indirect effects has a higher power than the test for total effects. Although
it has already been discussed in the past that a significant total effect should be a prerequisite for the test of an indirect
effect, Loeys and colleagues point out that this need not be a prerequisite in studies whose research interest is primarily
focused on the mediation process. Nevertheless, for the sake of completeness, we draw attention to the non-significant
total effect.
15302415, 2025, 2, Downloaded from https://spssi.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/asap.70009 by Marius Dilling - Universitaetsbibliothek Leipzi , Wiley Online Library on [03/05/2025]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
16 of 25 DILLING .
counteract right-wing extremist tendencies. With respect to their concept of extended primary
control, Fritsche et al. () argue that group identification can also counteract the heightened
ethnocentric response when group norms endorse diversity and nonviolence.
As expected, there is no direct effect of internal control on conspiracy mentality or our mea-
sures of right-wing extremist resentments. However, it is surprising that internal locus of control,
fully mediated by RWA (not CM), also predicts xenophobia and anti-Semitism, since internal con-
trol beliefs have been mostly associated with protective factors, with exception for the findings of
Diakonova and Gilgen () and Chertkova et al. (). While we observe a direct relationship
between the latent factor of RWA and internal control beliefs in the measurement error-adjusted
SEM, the measurement error-unadjusted bivariate correlations indicate that this may be trace-
able to the sub-dimension of conventionalism, as already observed by Chertkova et al. (). The
question that arises is that of causality, as our study cannot determine whether internal control
promotes authoritarianism or whether authoritarianism can successfully restore internal con-
trol, at least temporarily, or whether the identification with external norms rather causes an even
stronger shift towards an external LoC. Future research should address this question using longi-
tudinal or experimental studies. From a more methodological point of view, a closer look to our
applied IE- scale might shed light on this phenomenon, as the internal control belief items seem
to exhibit an intersection with neoliberal beliefs (“I am my own boss” and “If I try hard, I will suc-
ceed”). This capitalistic affirmation of performance and success closely aligns with what Amlinger
and Nachtwey () describe as characteristic of a new and contemporary libertarian authori-
tarianism that submits to neoliberal norms such as their own autonomy. Such beliefs appear to
be less detached from traditional authority figures than one might initially assume. While it is
important to acknowledge potential implications for construct validity at this point, it is equally
crucial to emphasize that we have utilized a well-validated and widely established scale, align-
ing with best practices in psychological measurement. Kovaleva et al. () themselves discuss
this in the context of the construct validity of the IE-, pointing out that, in the literature, there
appear to be positive correlations between internal locus of control and the importance of various
aspects of the workplace (Borg & Noll, ). The observed overlap might not be a matter of con-
struct validity of our scale, but this overlap might indicate that both constructs are theoretically
intertwined. Future research should explore this further.
Apart from a possible overlap with neoliberal beliefs, another plausible explanation could be
that people who excessively believe they have complete control over their lives deny reality as
they fail to recognize the complexity of certain situations—especially during the pandemic in
, from which our data set originates. While they may not need the fantasy that other groups
control everything (in terms of external locus of control and conspiracy mentality), they still
require identification with a strong authority—an identification that corresponds well with the
idea of extended primary control—and subsequent devaluation of outgroups. Future research
should explore whether unhealthy and healthy forms of crisis management exist in both groups
(scoring high in internal respectively external locus of control). Person-centered approaches,
rather than variable-centered methods, could identify patterns among individuals with high and
low external and internal loci of control.
Lastly, following Decker et al. (), we assumed the relationship between the rd order fac-
tor of the authoritarian syndrome and selected latent sub-dimensions (conspiracy mentality and
RWA) of the authoritarian syndrome to be reflective. However, considering both the different
dynamics that locus of control has on these syndrome elements as well as the moderate corre-
lations between the manifest indicators of conspiracy mentality and those of sadomasochistic
authoritarianism/RWA, the question arises as to whether alternative modeling strategies, such
15302415, 2025, 2, Downloaded from https://spssi.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/asap.70009 by Marius Dilling - Universitaetsbibliothek Leipzi , Wiley Online Library on [03/05/2025]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
AUTHORITARIAN CONTROL 17 of 25
as a formative relationship between the rd order factor and its lower order constructs is also
applicable—but this has to be discussed on a theoretical level first, since causality between the
higher order factor and its lower order constructs would be reversed in such a model.
With the present study, we aim to contribute to the understanding of psychological mechanisms
underlying contemporary authoritarianism, anti-Semitism and xenophobia during societal crises.
By pointing out their roles in restoring the fundamental need for controls, we also hope to provide
a foundation for future research to address these potential threats to democracy.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We would like to thank the anonymous reviewers and Fabian Max Hess for their helpful and
valuable comments.
Open access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL.
DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT
The dataset analyzed in the current study was generated as a joint project of several different uni-
versities. Due to missing consent of all parties involved, we are unable to make the dataset publicly
available. The parts supporting the findings of this study will be provided by the corresponding
author upon reasonable request.
ORCID
Marius Dilling https://orcid.org/---
Alina Sorrentino https://orcid.org/---
Elmar Brähler https://orcid.org/---
Jörg M. Fegert https://orcid.org/---
Oliver Decker https://orcid.org/---X
REFERENCES
Abalakina-Paap, M., Stephan, W. G., Craig, T., & Gregory, W. L. (). Beliefs in conspiracies. Political Psychology,
20,–.https://doi.org/./-X.
Adorno, T. W., Frenkel-Brunswik, E., Levinson, D. J., & Sanford, R. N. (). The authoritarian personality.Harper
& Brothers.
Aho, J. (). Revisiting authoritarianism. Critical Sociology,46(), –. https://doi.org/./
Altemeyer, B. (). Right-wing authoritarianism. University of Manitoba Press.
Amlinger, C., & Nachtwey, O. (). Offended freedom: The rise of libertarian authoritarianism.Polity.
Baier, D., & Manzoni, P. (). Verschwörungsmentalität und Extremismus—Befunde aus Befragungsstudien in
der Schweiz. Monatsschrift für Kriminologie und Strafrechtsreform,103(), –. https://doi.org/./mks-
-
Baron, R. A. (). Authoritarianism, locus of control, and risk taking. Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and
Applied,68(), –. https://doi.org/./..
Beierlein, C., Kovaleva, A., Kemper, C. J., & Rammstedt, B. (). Kurzskala Autoritarismus (KSA-) [Short scale
for authoritarianism (KSA-)]. GESIS—Leibniz-Institut für Sozialwissenschaften,https://doi.org/./zis
Bollen, K. A. (). Structural equations with latent variables. Wiley.
Borg, I., & Noll, H.-H. (). Wie wichtig ist „wichtig? ZUMA-Nachrichten, 27,–.
Brehm, J. W. (). A theory of psychological reactance. Academic Press.
Bruder, M., Haffke, P., Neave, N., Nouripanah, N., & Imhoff, R. (). Measuring individual differences in generic
beliefs in conspiracy theories across cultures: Conspiracy mentality questionnaire. Frontiers in Psychology,4,
. https://doi.org/./fpsyg..
Celik, K., Dilling, M., & Kiess, J. (). Im Schattender Individualisierung: Jugend als Krise, Verschwörungsglaube
als Heil? In J. Kiess, J. Preunkert, M. Seeliger, & J. Steg (Eds.), Krisen und soziologie (pp. –). Beltz Juventa.
15302415, 2025, 2, Downloaded from https://spssi.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/asap.70009 by Marius Dilling - Universitaetsbibliothek Leipzi , Wiley Online Library on [03/05/2025]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
18 of 25 DILLING .
Chertkova, Y. D., Egorova, M. S., & Fominykh, A. Y. (). Authoritarianism and attribution of responsibility. In
S. B. Malykh, & E. V. Nikulchev (Eds.), Psychology and education—ICPE , vol . European proceedings of
social and behavioural sciences (pp. –). Future Academy. https://doi.org/./epsbs...
Christie, R. (). Authoritarianism and related constructs. In J. P. Robinson, P. R. Shaver, & L. S. Wrightsman
(Eds.), Measures of social psychological attitudes, Vol. 1. Measures of personality and social psychological attitudes
(pp. –). Academic Press. https://doi.org/./B--- -.-
Decker, O., Hinz, A., Geißler, N., & Brähler, E. (). Questionnaire on right-wing extremist attitudes—Leipzig
form (FR-LF). In O. Decker, J. Kiess, & E. Brähler (Eds.), Right-wing extremism of the center. A social psychological
diagnosis of the present (pp. –). Psychosozial-Verlag.
Decker, O., Kalkstein, F., Schuler, J., Celik, K., Brähler, E., Clemens, V., & Fegert, J. M. (b). Polarisierung
und autoritäre Dynamiken während der Pandemie. Ein sozialpsychologischer und medizinsoziologischer Blick
auf die gegenwärtigen Herausforderungen. In O. Decker, J. Kiess, A. Heller, & E. Brähler (Eds.), Autoritäre
Dynamiken in unsicheren Zeiten. Neue Herausforderungen—alte Reaktionen? (pp. –). Psychosozial Verlag.
Decker, O., Kiess, J., & Brähler, E., Eds. (). The dynamics of right-wing extremism within German society. Escape
into authoritarianism. Routledge.
Decker, O., Schuler, J., Yendell, A., Schließler, C., & Brähler, E. (). Das autoritäre Syndrom: Dimension
und Verbreitung der Demokratie-Feindlichkeit. In O. Decker, & E. Brähler (Eds.), Alte Ressentiments — Neue
Radikalität: Leipziger Autoritarismus Studie 2020 (pp. –). Psychosozial-Verlag.
Diakonova, N., & Gilgen, A. (). Right-wing authoritarianism relative to traditional western thinking and locus
of control. Psychological Reports,82,–.https://doi.org/./pr....
Dilling, M., Kiess, J., & Brähler, E. (). Flucht in die Projektion. Zum Zusammenhang von Autoritarismus,
Verschwörungsmentalität und rechtsextremer Einstellung. ZRex—Zeitschrift für Rechtsextremismusforschung,
3(), –. https://doi.org/./zrex.vi.
Đorđević, J. M., Žeželj, I., & Đurić, Ž. (). Beyond general political attitudes: Conspiracy mentality as a global
belief system predicts endorsement of international and local conspiracy theories. JournalofSocialandPolitical
Psychology,9(), –. https://doi.org/./jspp.
Douglas, K. M., Uscinski, J. E., Sutton, R. M., Cichocka, A., Nefes, T., Ang, C. S., & Deravi, F. (). Understanding
conspiracy theories. Political Psychology,40,–.https://doi.org/./pops.
Duckitt, J. (). Locus of control and racial prejudice. Psychological Reports,54(), –. https://doi.org/.
/pr....
Duckitt, J. (). A dual-process cognitive-motivational theoryof ideology and prejudice. Advances in Experimen-
tal Social Psychology,33(October), –. https://doi.org/./s-()-
Duckitt, J. (). Differential effects of right wing authoritarianism and social dominance orientation on outgroup
attitudes and their mediation by threat from and competitiveness to outgroups. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin,32(), –. https://doi.org/./
Duckitt, J. (). Authoritarianism: Conceptualization, research, and new developments. In D. Osborne, & C. Sib-
ley (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of political psychology (pp. –) (Cambridge Handbooks in Psychology,
Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/./.
Duckitt, J., & Sibley, C. (). Personality, ideology, prejudice, and politics: A dual-process motivational model.
Journal of Personality,78, –. https://doi.org/./j.-...x
Eicker, J. (). Erklärungsansätze für Rechtsextremismus und ihre Systematisierung. Eine Untersuchung
zum Stand der Theoriebildung in der Rechtsextremismusforschung. ZRex–Zeitschrift für Rechtsextremismus-
forschung,1(), –.
Feldman, S. (). Enforcing social conformity: A theory of authoritarianism. Political Psychology,24(), –.
Fritsche, I., Jonas, E., & Fankhänel, T. (). The role of control motivation in mortality salience effects on
ingroup support and defense. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,95(), –. https://doi.org/.
/a
Fritsche, I., & Jonas, E., & Kessler, T. (). Collective reactions to threat: Implications for intergroup conflict and
for solving societal crises. Social Issues and Policy Review, . https://doi.org/./j.-...x
Fromm, E. (). Escape from freedom. Farrar & Rinehart.
Fromm, E. (). Sozialpsychologischer teil. In M. Horkheimer (Ed.), Studien über Autorität und Familie.
Forschungsberichte aus dem Institut für Sozialforschung (pp. –) [Studies on authority and family. Research
reports from the Institute for Social Research] Dietrich zu Klampen Verlag.
15302415, 2025, 2, Downloaded from https://spssi.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/asap.70009 by Marius Dilling - Universitaetsbibliothek Leipzi , Wiley Online Library on [03/05/2025]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
AUTHORITARIAN CONTROL 19 of 25
Funke, F. (). Die Dimensionale Struktur von Autoritarismus [The dimensional structure of authoritar-
ianism]. Doctoral dissertation, Friedrich-Schiller Universität Jena. https://www.db-thueringen.de/servlets/
MCRFileNodeServlet/dbt_derivate_/diss_funke.pdf
Goertzel, T. (). Belief in conspiracy theories. Political Psychology,15(), –.
Golec de Zavala, A., Bierwiaczonek, K., Baran, T., Keenan, O., & Hase, A. (). The COVID- pandemic, author-
itarianism, and rejection of sexual dissenters in Poland. Psychology of Sexual Orientation and Gender Diversity,
8(), –. https://doi.org/./sgd
Grzesiak-Feldman, M. (). Are the high authoritarians more prone to adopt conspiracy theories? The role of
right-wing authoritarianism in conspiratorial thinking. In M. Bilewicz, A. Cichocka, & W. Soral (Eds.), The
psychology of conspiracy (pp. –). Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.
Grzesiak-Feldman, M., & Irzycka, M. (). Right-wing authoritarianism and conspiracy thinking in a Polish
sample. Psychological Reports,105(), –. https://doi.org/./pr...-
Haase, A., Asbrock, F., & Rohmann, A. (). Right-wing authoritarianism and majority members’ preferences
for minority acculturation. European Journal of Social Psychology,50,–.https://doi.org/./ejsp.
Hartman, T. K., Stocks, T. V. A., McKay, R., Gibson-Miller, J., Levita, L., Martinez, A. P., Mason, L., McBride,
O., Murphy, J., Shevlin, M., Bennett, K. M., Hyland, P., Karatzias, T., Vallières, F., & Bentall, R. P. (). The
authoritarian dynamic during the COVID- pandemic: Effects on nationalism and anti-immigrant sentiment.
Social Psychological and Personality Science,12(), –. https://doi.org/./
Hayes, A. F. (). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis. A regression-based
approach (nd ed.). The Guilford Press.
Heidemeyer, P., Sorrentino, A., Hoffmann, L., Quinten, L., Grozeva, N., & Banse, R. in preparation. The California
F scale revisited: On the predictive power of its omitted facets [Manuscript in preparation]. University of Bonn.
Heller, A., Decker, O., & Brähler, E. (). Right-wing extremism—A unitary construct? A contribution to the
question of operationalization using the Questionnaire on Right-Wing Extremism-Leipzig Form (FR-LF). In A.
Heller, O. Decker, & E. Brähler (Eds.), Precarious cohesion. The threat to democratic togetherness in Germany.
Psychosozial-Verlag.
Henkelmann, K., Jäckel, C., Stahl, A., Wünsch, N., & Zopes, B., Eds. (). Konformistische rebellen: Zur Aktualität
des autoritären Charakters. Verbrecher Verlag.
Holtmann, D., & Görl, T. (). Right-wing extremism, xenophobia, anti-Semitism. In D. H. Sturzbecher, & D.
Holtmann (Eds.), Values, family, politics, violence—What moves youth? Recent results of a survey (pp. –).
https://publishup.uni-potsdam.de/frontdoor/index/index/docId/
Horkheimer, M. (). [] Studien über Autorität und Familie. Forschungsberichte aus dem Institut für Sozial-
forschung [Studies on authority and family. Research reports from the Institute for Social Research], Dietrich zu
Klampen Verlag.
Imhoff, R., & Bruder, M. (). Speaking (un-)truth to power: Conspiracy mentality as a generalised political
attitude. European Journal of Personality,28(), –. https://doi.org/./per.
Imhoff, R., & Decker, O. (). Verschwörungsmentalität als Weltbild. In O. Decker, E. Brähler, & J. Kiess (Eds.),
Rechtsextremismus der Mitte (Orig.-Ausg ed., pp. –). Psychosozial-Verlag.
Inglehart, R., & Norris, P. (). Trump, Brexit, and the rise of populism: Economic have nots and cultural
backlash. HKS Faculty Research Working Paper Series RWP16.
Iser, J. (). prejudices: On the role of personality, values, general attitudes, and threat. Basic human values theory,
authoritarianism, and social dominance theory as explanatory approaches to prejudice: An integrative comparison
of theories. Dissertation. Giessen.
Jäger,D.W.G.F.().Dialektik der Deprivation: Zu Idee und Praxis der Entsagung als Element des Autoritarismus
und ihre Rekonfiguration in der Gegenwart [Dialectics of deprivation: On the idea and practice of renunciation
as an element of authoritarianism and its reconfiguration in the present]. (Doctoral dissertation, University of
Groningen). University of Groningen.
Jolley, D., & Douglas, K. M. (a). The social consequences of conspiracism: Exposure to conspiracy theories
decreases intentions to engage in politics and to reduce one’s carbon footprint. British Journal of Psychology,105,
–. https://dx.doi.org/./bjop.
Jolley, D., & Douglas, K. M. (b). The effects of anti-vaccine conspiracy theories on vaccination intentions. PLoS
ONE,9(), e. https://doi.org/./journal.pone.
15302415, 2025, 2, Downloaded from https://spssi.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/asap.70009 by Marius Dilling - Universitaetsbibliothek Leipzi , Wiley Online Library on [03/05/2025]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
20 of 25 DILLING .
Kay, A., & Gaucher, D., & Napier, J., & Callan, M., & Laurin, K. (). God and the government: Testing a com-
pensatory control mechanism for the support of external systems. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
95,–.https://doi.org/./-...
Kay, A. C., Whitson, J. A., Gaucher, D., & Galinsky, A. D. (). Compensatory control: Achieving order through
the mind, our institutions, and the heavens. Current Directions in Psychological Science,18(), –. https://
doi.org/./j.-...x
Kiess, J., & Schmidt, A. (). The political spillover of workplace democratization: How democratic efficacy at
the workplace contributes to countering right-wing extremist attitudes in Germany. Economic and Industrial
Democracy,0(). https://doi.org/./X
King, V. (). Autoritarismus als regression [Authoritarianism as regression]. WestEnd: Neue Zeitschrift für
Sozialforschung,18(), –.
Kofta, M., Soral, W., & Bilewicz, M. (). What breeds conspiracy antisemitism? The role of political uncontrol-
lability and uncertainty in the belief in Jewish conspiracy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,118(),
–. https://doi.org/./pspa
Kovaleva, A., Beierlein, C., Kemper, C. J., & Rammstedt, B. (). A short scale for measuring locus of control: The
Internal-External locus of control- (IE-) scale. (GESIS Working Papers, /). Mannheim: GESIS—Leibniz
Institute for the Social Sciences. https://nbn-resolving.org/urn:nbn:de:-ssoar-
Kovaleva, A., Beierlein, C., Kemper, C. J., & Rammstedt, B. (). Eine Kurzskala zur Messung von Kontrol-
lüberzeugungen: Die Skala Internale-Externale-Kontrollüberzeugung- (IE-) [A short scale for measuring
locus of control: The scale of internal-external locus of control- (IE-)]. GESIS—Leibniz-Institut für Sozial-
wissenschaften.https://doi.org/./zis
Krampe, H., Danbolt, L. J., Haver, A., Stålsett, G., & Schnell, T. (). Locus of control moderates the association
of COVID- stress and general mental distress: Results of a Norwegian and a German-speaking cross-sectional
survey. BMC Psychiatry [Electronic Resource],21(). https://doi.org/./s---
Kreis, J. (). Zur Messung von rechtsextremer Einstellung: Probleme und Kontroversen am Beispiel zweier Studien:
Arbeitsheft aus dem Otto-Strammer-Zentrum Nr. 12.https://refubium.fu-berlin.de/bitstream/handle/fub/
/ahosz.pdf?sequence=&isAllowed=y
Lamberty, P. (). Don’t trust anyone: Verschwörungsdenken als Radikalisierungsbeschleuiniger? Journal EXIT-
Deutschland. Zeitschrift für Deradikalisierung und demokratische Kultur,5,–.
Landau, M. J., Kay, A. C., & Whitson, J. A. (). Compensatory control and the appeal of a structured world.
Psychological Bulletin,141(), –. https://doi.org/./a
Loeys, T., Moerkerke, B., & Vansteelandt, S. (). A cautionary note on the power of the test for the indirect effect
in mediation analysis. Frontiers in psychology,5.https://doi.org/./fpsyg..
Mao, J. Y., Yang, S. L., & Guo, Y. Y. (). Are individuals from lower social classes more susceptible to conspiracy
theories? An explanation from the compensatory control theory. Asian Journal of Social Psychology,23(), –
.
Mays, A., Hambauer, V., & Gold, V. (). Social and emotional dispositions of AfD supporters. In A. Mays, A.
Dingelstedt, V. Hambauer, S. Schlosser, F. Berens, J. Leibold, & J. K. Höhne (Eds.), Foundations—Methods—
Applications in the social sciences. Festschrift for Steffen-M. Kühnel (pp. –). Springer.
McCollaum, B., & Lester, D. (). Authoritarianism, anti-authoritarianism, and locus of control. Psychological
Reports,76(), –. https://doi.org/./pr....
Moscovici, S. (). The conspiracy mentality. In C. F. Graumann, & S. Moscovici, Eds. (Hrsg.), Changing
conceptions of conspiracy (pp. –). Springer Verlag.
Nicol, A. A. M. (). Social dominance orientation, right-wing authoritarianism, and their relation with alien-
ation and spheres of control. Personality and Individual Differences,43(), –. https://doi.org/./j.paid.
..
Oesterreich, D. (). Flucht in die Sicherheit: Zur Theorie des Autoritarismus und der autoritären Reaktion.
Opladen, Leske +Budrich.
Rees, J., & Lamberty, P. (). Mitreißende Wahrheiten: Verschwörungsmythen als Gefahr für den
gesellschaftlichen Zusammenhalt. In A. Zick, B. Küpper, & W. Berghan (Eds.), Verlorene Mitte—Feindselige
Zustände. Rechtsextreme Einstellungen in Deutschland 2018/19 (pp. –). Dietz.
Richey, S. (). A birther and a truther: The influence of the authoritarian personality on conspiracy beliefs.
Politics & Policy,45(), –. https://doi.org/./polp.
15302415, 2025, 2, Downloaded from https://spssi.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/asap.70009 by Marius Dilling - Universitaetsbibliothek Leipzi , Wiley Online Library on [03/05/2025]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
AUTHORITARIAN CONTROL 21 of 25
Rippl, S., & Baier, D. (). Das Deprivationskonzept in der Rechtsextremismusforschung. KZfSS Kölner Zeitschrift
für Soziologie und Sozialpsychologie,57(), –. https://doi.org/./s---
Rothbaum, F., Weisz, J. R., & Snyder, S. S. (). Changing the world and changing the self: A two-process model
of perceived control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,42(), –. https://doi.org/./-.
..
Rotter, J. B. (). The role of the psychological situation in determining the direction of human behavior. In M.
R. Jones (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation: 1955 (pp. –). University of Nebraska Press.
Rotter, J. B. (). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychological
Monographs: General and Applied,80(), –. https://doi.org/./h
Rotter, J. B. (). Some problems and misconceptions related to the construct of internal versus external control
of reinforcement. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,43(), –. https://doi.org/./h
Russo, S., Roccato, M., & Mosso, C. (). Authoritarianism, societal threat, and preference for antidemocratic
political systems. TPM—Testing, Psychometrics, Methodology in Applied Psychology,26(), –. https://doi.
org/./TPM..
Schafer, J. L., & Graham, J. W. (). Missing data: Our view of the state of the art. Psychological methods,7(),
–.
Skinner, E. A. (). A guide to constructs of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,71(), –.
https://doi.org/./-...
Spier, T. (). Modernisierungsverlierer? die WählerschDaft rechtspopulistischer Parteien in Westeuropa.VSVerlag
für Sozialwissenschaften.
Šrol, J., Mikušková, E. B., & Cavojova, V., (). When we are worried, what are we thinking? Anxiety, lack of
control, and conspiracy beliefs amidst the COVID- pandemic. https://doi.org/./acp.
Stagner, R. (). Fascist attitudes: An exploratory study. The Journal of Social Psychology,7, –.
Stojanov, A. &, Bering, J., & Halberstadt, J. (). Perceived lack of control and conspiracy theory beliefs in the
wake of political strife and natural disaster. Psihologija,55,–.https://doi.org/./PSIS
Stojanov, A., Bering, J. M., & Halberstadt, J. (b). Does Perceived Lack of Control Lead to Conspiracy The-
ory Beliefs? Findings from an online MTurk sample. PLoS ONE,15(), e. https://doi.org/./journal.
pone.
Stojanov, A., & Halberstadt, J. (). Does lack of control lead to conspiracy beliefs? A meta-analysis. European
journal of social psychology,50,S,–.https://doi.org/./ejsp.
Stojanov, A., Halberstadt, J., Bering, J. M., & Kenig, N. (). Examining a domain-specific link between perceived
control and conspiracy beliefs: A brief report in the context of COVID-. Current psychology (New Brunswick,
N.J.),42(), –. https://doi.org/./s---
Stollberg, J., Fritsche, I., Barth, M., & Jugert, P. (). Extending control perceptions to the social self: Ingroups
serve the restoration of control. In M. Bukowski, I. Fritsche, A. Guinote, & M. Kofta (Eds.), Coping with lack of
control in a social world (pp. –). Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.
Sturzbecher, D., Landua, D., & Heyne, M. (). Political attitudes and right-wing extremism among East Ger-
man youth. In D. Sturzbecher (Ed.), Youth in East Germany: Life situations and delinquency. VS Verlag für
Sozialwissenschaften. https://doi.org/./----_
Tagini, S., Brugnera, A., Ferrucci, R., Mazzocco, K., Pievani, L., Priori, A., Ticozzi, N., Compare, A., Silani, V.,
Pravettoni, G., & Poletti, B. (). Attachment, Personality and Locus of Control: Psychological Determinants
of Risk Perception and Preventive Behaviors for COVID-. Frontiers in Psychology, . https://doi.org/./
fpsyg..
Tooze, A. (). Welcome to the world of the polycrisis. Financial Times. https://www.ft.com/content/e-
b-b-cd-ddcde
Van Prooijen, J. W. (). An Existential Threat Model of Conspiracy Theories. European Psychologist,25(), –.
https://doi.org/./-/a
van Prooijen, J.-W., & Acker, M. (). The Influence of Control on Belief in Conspiracy Theories: Conceptual and
Applied Extensions. Applied Cognitive Psychology,29,–.https://doi.org/./acp.
Van Prooijen, J.-W., & Douglas, K. M. (). Conspiracy theories as part of history: The role of societal crisis
situations. Memory Studies,10(), –. https://doi.org/./
van Prooijen, J. W., Krouwel, A., & Pollet, T. (). Political Extremism Predicts Belief in Conspiracy Theories.
Social Psychological and Personality Science,.https://doi.org/./
15302415, 2025, 2, Downloaded from https://spssi.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/asap.70009 by Marius Dilling - Universitaetsbibliothek Leipzi , Wiley Online Library on [03/05/2025]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
22 of 25 DILLING .
Whitson, J. A., & Galinsky, A. D. (). Lacking control increases illusory pattern perception. Science (New York,
N.Y.),322(), –. https://doi.org/./science.
Wood, M. J., Douglas, K. M., & Sutton, R. M. (). Dead and alive: Beliefs in contradictory conspiracy theories.
Social Psychological and Personality Science,3(), –.
Würtzen, H., Clausen, L. H., Andersen, P. B., Santini, Z. I., Rkmen, J., & Pedersen, H. F. (). Mental well-being,
health, and locus of control in Danish adults before and during COVID-. Acta Neuropsychiatrica,https://doi.
org/./neu..
Zick, A., & Mokros, N. (). Rechtsextreme Einstellungen in der Mitte. In Die distanzierte Mitte. Rechtsextreme
und demokratiegefährdende Einstellungen in Deutschland / (Hrsg. Andreas Zick, Beate Küpper, Nico
Mokros), S. –. Verlag J. H. W. Dietz Nachf. GmbH: Bonn.
How to cite this article: Dilling, M., Sorrentino, A., Brähler, E., Fegert, J. M., & Decker,
O. (). The authoritarian syndrome as an attempt to restore control and its mediating
role in anti-semitism and xenophobia in Germany. Analyses of Social Issues and Public
Policy,25, e. https://doi.org/./asap.
APPENDIX A: DESCRIPTIVE STATISTICS AND TRANSLATED ENGLISH WORDING
OF THE ITEMS
Constructs
Indicator variables at the individual level MSDMin Max
Conspiracy mentality
Most people do not recognize to what extent our life
is determined by conspiracies that are concocted in
secret.
. . “strongly
disagree”
“strongly
agree”
There are secret organizations that have great
influence on political decisions.
. . “strongly
disagree”
“strongly
agree”
Politicians and other leaders are nothing but the
string puppets of powers operating in the
background.
. . “strongly
disagree”
“strongly
agree”
Authoritarian aggression
Outsiders and under-performers in society should be
severely punished.
. . “strongly
disagree”
“strongly
agree”
Troublemakers should clearly feel the effects of the
fact that they are unwanted in the society.
. . “strongly
disagree”
“strongly
agree”
Social rules should be enforced without compassion. . . “strongly
disagree”
“strongly
agree”
Authoritarian submission
We need strong leaders in order to live safely in
society.
. . “strongly
disagree”
“strongly
agree”
People should leave important decisions to those in
charge/their leaders.
. . “strongly
disagree”
“strongly
agree”
We should be grateful for leaders who tell us exactly
what we should to do.
. . “strongly
disagree”
“strongly
agree”
(Continues)
15302415, 2025, 2, Downloaded from https://spssi.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/asap.70009 by Marius Dilling - Universitaetsbibliothek Leipzi , Wiley Online Library on [03/05/2025]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
AUTHORITARIAN CONTROL 23 of 25
Constructs
Indicator variables at the individual level MSDMin Max
Conventionalism
Traditions should absolutely be cultivated and
maintained.
. . “strongly
disagree”
“strongly
agree”
Established conducts should not be questioned. . . “strongly
disagree”
“strongly
agree”
It’s always best to do things in the usual way. . . “strongly
disagree”
“strongly
agree”
Xenophobia
Foreigners only come here to abuse the welfare
system.
. . “strongly
disagree”
“strongly
agree”
When jobs are scarce, foreigners should be sent
home.
. . “strongly
disagree”
“strongly
agree”
Germany is losing its identity because of the large
number of foreigners.
. . “strongly
disagree”
“strongly
agree”
anti-Semitism
The influence of the Jews is still too strong. . . “strongly
disagree”
“strongly
agree”
More than other people, the Jews use dirty tricks to
achieve their goals.
. . “strongly
disagree”
“strongly
agree”
The Jews just have something peculiar about them
and don’t really fit in with us.
. . “strongly
disagree”
“strongly
agree”
External locus of control
What I do is mainly determined by others. . . “strongly
disagree”
“strongly
agree”
Fate often gets in the way of my plans. . . “strongly
disagree”
“strongly
agree”
Internal locus of control
I am my own boss. . . “strongly
disagree”
“strongly
agree”
If I work hard, I will succeed. . . “strongly
disagree”
“strongly
agree”
15302415, 2025, 2, Downloaded from https://spssi.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/asap.70009 by Marius Dilling - Universitaetsbibliothek Leipzi , Wiley Online Library on [03/05/2025]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
24 of 25 DILLING .
APPENDIX B: CORRELATION MATRIX
AGR1 AGR2 AGR3 SUB1 SUB2 SUB3 CONV1 CONV2 CONV3 XENO1 XENO2 XENO3 VM1 VM2 VM3 AS1 AS2 AS3 ILoC1 ILoC2 ExLoc1 ExLoC2
AGR
AGR .***
AGR .*** .***
SUB .*** .*** .***
SUB .*** .*** .*** .***
SUB .*** .*** .*** .*** .***
CONV .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .***
CONV .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .***
CONV .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .***
XENO .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .***
XENO .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .***
XENO .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .***
VM .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .***
VM .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .***
VM .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .***
AS .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .***
AS .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .***
AS .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .***
ILoC .*.*** . . . −. .*** .*. −. −.*−. −.*** −.*** −.*** −.*−.** −.***
ILoC .*.*** . . −. −. .*** . . −. −.** −. −.*** −.*** −.*** −.** −.*** −.*** .***
ExLoC .** −. . .** .*** .*** . . .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** −.*** −.***
ExLoC . −. . .*** .*** .*** .*.*.** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** −.*** −.*** .***
Note:N=2,380.
*p<., ** p<., *** p<.. Bonferroni-adjusted significance levels.
15302415, 2025, 2, Downloaded from https://spssi.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/asap.70009 by Marius Dilling - Universitaetsbibliothek Leipzi , Wiley Online Library on [03/05/2025]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
AUTHORITARIAN CONTROL 25 of 25
APPENDIX C: MODEL COMPARISON
Model CFI TLI RMSEA SRMR AIC BIC
Model (with residual correlations) . . . . . .
Model (no residual correlations) . . . . . .
AUTHOR BIOGRAPHIES
Marius Dilling is a research assistant at the Else-Frenkel-Brunswik-Institute for Democracy
Research at Leipzig University and an associate at the Competence Center for Research on
Right-Wing Extremism and Democracy at Leipzig University. He studied sociology and con-
ducts research on political attitudes with a focus on authoritarianism and right-wing extremist
attitudes. He is currently enrolled as a PhD candidate at the University of Hannover.
Alina Sorrentino studied philosophy and psychology at the University of Bonn and special-
ized in political psychology during her studies. She works as a clinical psychologist in a hospital
in Euskirchen.
Elmar Brähler, University of Leipzig, University of Mainz. He was head of the Department
of Medical Psychology and Medical Sociology at the University of Leipzig until . He is a
visiting scientist at the Clinic and Polyclinic for Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy
at the Mainz University Medical Center.
Jörg M. Fegert, University Hospital Ulm, is Medical Director of the Department for Child and
Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy at Ulm University Hospital, Past
President and Congress President (Ulm ) of the German Society for Child and Adolescent
Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy (DGKJP) as well as President (-) of
the European Society for Child and Adolescent Psychiatry (ESCAP).
Oliver Decker, is Professor of Social Psychology at Sigmund Freud University, Mem-
ber of the Department of Medical Psychology and Medical Sociology and Director of the
Else Frenkel-Brunswik Institute, Leipzig University. Since , he has headed the Leipzig
Authoritarianism Studies together with Elmar Brähler.
15302415, 2025, 2, Downloaded from https://spssi.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/asap.70009 by Marius Dilling - Universitaetsbibliothek Leipzi , Wiley Online Library on [03/05/2025]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License