ArticlePDF Available

Gratitude interventions reduce cyber-aggression in adolescents: gender and disposition effects

Springer Nature
Scientific Reports
Authors:

Abstract and Figures

The study explores the impact of gender and gratitude disposition on the effectiveness of a classroom-based gratitude intervention aimed at reducing cyber-aggression among Polish adolescents. Cyber-aggression, linked to maladjustment and mental health issues like depression and anxiety, is a growing concern. The intervention involved 548 students, divided into a control group (399) and an experimental group (149), and lasted for seven days. Participants completed the Cyber-aggression Types Questionnaire (CATQ) and a Gratitude Questionnaire. Results showed the intervention effectively reduced overall cyber-aggression, particularly aversive controlled types, but increased appetitive impulsive aggression. Girls responded better to the gratitude exercises, and those with lower gratitude levels saw the most significant reduction in cyber-aggression. However, students with medium levels of gratitude showed increased impulsive and controlled appetitive cyber-aggression. The study concludes that gender and gratitude disposition influence the success of gratitude interventions in reducing cyber-aggression.
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
Gratitude interventions reduce
cyber-aggression in adolescents:
gender and disposition eects
Tomaszek Katarzyna 1 & Muchacka-Cymerman Agnieszka 2
The study explores the impact of gender and gratitude disposition on the eectiveness of a classroom-
based gratitude intervention aimed at reducing cyber-aggression among Polish adolescents. Cyber-
aggression, linked to maladjustment and mental health issues like depression and anxiety, is a
growing concern. The intervention involved 548 students, divided into a control group (399) and an
experimental group (149), and lasted for seven days. Participants completed the Cyber-aggression
Types Questionnaire (CATQ) and a Gratitude Questionnaire. Results showed the intervention
eectively reduced overall cyber-aggression, particularly aversive controlled types, but increased
appetitive impulsive aggression. Girls responded better to the gratitude exercises, and those with
lower gratitude levels saw the most signicant reduction in cyber-aggression. However, students with
medium levels of gratitude showed increased impulsive and controlled appetitive cyber-aggression.
The study concludes that gender and gratitude disposition inuence the success of gratitude
interventions in reducing cyber-aggression.
Keywords Classroom gratitude intervention, Cyber-aggression types, Gender, Gratitude disposition
Cyber-aggression is a complex multi-type maladaptive behavior that was recognized as one of the most
concerning public health problems aecting adolescents1,2. Despite the importance of studying distinct proles
of cyber-perpetrators related to dimensions of valence and control, research suggests the need for more attention
to nding eective ways to allow individuals to avoid such aversive experiences due to their detrimental impact
on adolescents’ development3,4. Importantly, the meta-analysis showed that the prevalence of online perpetration
among children and adolescents ranged from 5.3 to 66.2%5. To date, multiple studies have addressed the risk
factors that may inuence the rate of adolescent cyber-aggression, with the most commonly studied person-
related characteristics. Evidence indicates mixed results for gender as a risk factor3. Moreover, given the
prominent eects of gratitude intervention in promoting various benets at every developmental stage, research
on adolescents’ cyber-aggression and gratitude is scarce. Notably, some previous studies suggest that gratitude-
based practices within natural social groups may eectively reduce negative states (aggression) and behaviors
(cyber-bullying) among adolescents and adults with comparable positive eects to classical psycho-educational
programs6,7. Furthermore, previous studies have predominantly focused on a cross-sectional examination of
the role of gratitude trait on the overall cyber-aggression, with limited evidence for the diversities caused by its
various types. Hence, this project tends to provide valuable insight into the role of Classroom gratitude induction
(CGI) with attention given to investigating the inuence of personal factors, such as gender and low gratitude
disposition.
Cyber-aggression among adolescent girls and boys
Cyber-aggression among adolescent girls and boys manifests in distinct patterns, reecting dierences in
socialization, emotional expression, and online behavior. Research suggests that while both genders engage
in cyber-aggression, the motivations and methods oen dier8,9. e heterogeneity of the cyber-oenders’
motivations contributing to developing several classications of cyber-aggression10. Most commonly proposed
types are based on classical typology of pro-active (instrumental aggressive actions focused on anticipated
reward, planned and organized, with no emotional arousal dened as cold-blooded and high aective control),
and reactive aggression (an impulsive response to perceived threat associated with adverse emotional states
such as anger, tension or anxiety)11,12. e second source of online aggression diversity is related to perpetrators’
motivation i.e. aversion induced aggression acts (aggression arising from aversive conditions such as violent
conicts) or appetitive motives (aggression initiated by the need for experiencing pleasure or fun that stems
1Rzeszow University, Kraków, Poland. 2Humanitas University in Sosnowiec, ul. Kilińskiego 43, Sosnowiec 41-200,
Poland. email: agnieszka.muchacka-cymerman@humanitas.edu.pl
OPEN
Scientic Reports | (2025) 15:14602 1
| https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-025-97214-w
www.nature.com/scientificreports
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
from attacking and ghting with others)13,10. For example in respect to these typologies, Runions et al.2 proposed
four forms of cyber-attacks: aversive: impulsive and controlled; and appetitive: impulsive and controlled. More
recently DeMarsico et al.14 have postulated eight forms of cyber-aggression: (1) Social Bonding - induced by a
need to be aliated by others; (2) Social Activism – initiated by the believes that the person have to advance
or defend political/social issues; (3) Reactive Aggression – motivated by anger; (4) Interpersonal Distress – acts
induced by relationship stress; (5) Impulsivity – conducted to mitigate impulsive urges; (6) Virtual Dissociation
– motivated to take on a dierentcyber-identity; (7)rill-Seeking – initiated to experience excitement; and (8)
Vengeance – pursued to gain revenge. Noteworthy, adolescent girls are more likely to participate in relational
forms of cyber-aggression, such as spreading rumors or social exclusion, which align with their tendency to
value social connections and relationships15. On the other hand, boys are more inclined towards direct and
overt forms of cyber-aggression, including threats and insults, which may be linked to traditional notions of
masculinity and dominance16. ese gendered dierences highlight the importance of tailored interventions that
address the specic ways in which girls and boys experience and perpetrate cyber-aggression. Understanding
these distinctions is crucial for developing eective strategies to prevent and reduce cyber-aggression, as well as
to support adolescents in navigating the complex dynamics of online interactions.
Cyber-aggression among adolescent girls and boys demonstrates notable dierences in both prevalence and
expression, which are inuenced by gender-specic social dynamics and psychological factors. According to
research by León-Moreno et al.17, gender plays a signicant role in shaping how adolescents engage in cyber-
aggression, with boys generally showing higher involvement in such behaviors compared to girls. However,
the study also reveals that girls, particularly those who are rejected or neglected by their peers, may engage in
cyber-aggression as a response to feelings of anger and helplessness caused by social exclusion. is behavior
is oen facilitated by the anonymity and indirect nature of online interactions, which can make it easier for
these girls to express their frustrations without direct confrontation. Conversely, boys who are categorized as
controversial—both liked and disliked by their peers—tend to exhibit the highest levels of cyber-aggression,
suggesting that their involvement in both prosocial and antisocial behaviors in the physical world extends into
their online interactions. Research conducted by Álvarez-García et al.18 reveals that boys tend to display higher
levels of cyber-aggression than girls, with key factors such as impulsivity and antisocial behavior serving as
signicant predictors. Although girls are generally less involved in cyber-aggressive acts, they are not entirely
exempt; their participation in such behaviors is oen inuenced by social factors, particularly peer relationships
and experiences of victimization. According to Uddin and Rahman19, boys tend to exhibit higher levels of both
cyber victimization and cyber aggression compared to girls. e study also found signicant gender dierences
in emotion regulation strategies, with girls more likely to use cognitive reappraisal, which helps them manage
their emotions eectively, thereby reducing the likelihood of engaging in cyber-aggression. On the other
hand, boys were more inclined to use expressive suppression, which was positively correlated with both cyber
victimization and aggression, suggesting that boys who suppress their emotions are more prone to retaliatory
aggressive behaviors online.
Gratitude as a protective factor against cyber-aggression
e available scientic evidence advocate that positive youth development indicators that is character strengths
such as grateful mood and predisposition toward thankfulness and appreciation are associated to a decreased
likelihood of engaging in various risk behaviors (i.e. substance use, smoking, sexual risk behaviors)20. Gratitude
dened as a positive emotional state characterized by recognition for the kindness and benets received from
others, plays a crucial role in mitigating cyber-aggression, particularly among adolescents21. Consistently with
Fredrickson’s broaden-and-build theory, the positive impact of gratitude on human behaviors stems from
improving people’s monetary thought-action repertoires and the increase in accessibility of personal resources,
what in a long-term positively impacts individuals’ mental health and social interactions22. As a protective factor,
gratitude promotes emotional regulation, fosters empathy, and strengthens social connections23. ese traits
not only enhance personal well-being but also create a buer against negative emotions such as anger, envy,
or frustration—key drivers of cyber-aggressive behaviors. Consequently, gratitude can contribute to a more
respectful and empathetic online environment, reducing instances of harmful behaviors like cyber-aggression7,22.
e protective role of gratitude is especially signicant in school environments, where adolescents are highly
susceptible to peer inuences and social dynamics. Research by Oliveira et al.24 highlights the interplay between
gratitude, forgiveness, and self-regulation in enhancing adolescents’ psychological well-being. is improved
well-being fosters prosocial behaviors and reduces the likelihood of engaging in cyber-aggression. Adolescents
who practice gratitude regularly show greater resilience to social challenges and are less likely to retaliate with
aggression during online conicts or provocations. Moreover, the relationship between gratitude and cyber-
aggression is mediated by factors such as self-compassion and moral disengagement. Studies indicate that
gratitude strengthens self-compassion, which in turn reduces moral disengagement—a process oen associated
with aggressive behaviors25. is sequential mediation underscores the complex interplay of psychological traits
that gratitude can inuence to mitigate cyber-aggression. Individuals who consistently practice gratitude tend
to experience positive emotions such as empathy and compassion, which are essential for fostering healthy
interpersonal relationships online and oine. For example, DeWall et al.26 found that gratitude encourages
individuals to focus on the positive aspects of their social interactions, reducing the likelihood of engaging in
harmful online behaviors. is is particularly relevant in digital spaces where anonymity and lack of physical
presence oen lead to dehumanization, making it easier to perpetrate cyber-aggression. By promoting a sense of
connection and respect for others, gratitude discourages such behaviors. Gratitude’s inuence extends beyond
individual benets to foster a supportive school culture. A school environment that promotes gratitude and
other positive emotional practices can reduce the prevalence of cyber-aggression by creating a community
where empathy and mutual respect thrive25. Such environments not only discourage harmful behaviors but also
Scientic Reports | (2025) 15:14602 2
| https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-025-97214-w
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
prevent their escalation into more severe forms like cyberhate. Fulantelli et al.27 argue that while cyberbullying
and cyberhate dier, they share common predictors and consequences related to adolescents’ emotional well-
being. By embedding gratitude into school programs, communities can strengthen their resistance to these
harmful behaviors.
Gratitude interventions among children and adolescents
Numerous past studies have investigated the impact of increasing gratitude among adult or university students
populations, with signicant eects of such practices on decreasing adverse mental states i.e. depression,
anxiety, and increasing the positive ones i.e. well-being, social functioning28. Yet, the gratitude implementation
among children and adolescents has received less attention. Among gratitude interventions dedicated to
youth population most focused on private gratitude expressions (e.g., counting blessings, gratitude journaling,
gratitude letters)29. Scholars have induced gratitude in school through several ways i.e. Grateful Recounting
(counting simple blessings received, list three good things), Grateful Reection (reecting on the kindness of
others, participants are asked to nominate events, things or people they are grateful and which happened in a
specied period), Grateful Expression (expressing gratitude and gratitude through letters or journals), Grateful
Reappraisal (looking back at the positive side of negative events that have been experienced)30. According
to past gratitude literature the recommended duration of gratitude intervention carried out is at least two
weeks, to give time for participants to process the information they have gained and provide an opportunity
for applying what they have studied during the training30. However, signicant eects were detected also in
short-term gratitude-based interventions31 conrmed that 1-day gratitude induction increased positive aect
and psychological emotionality). According to meta-analysis performed by Kirca et al.32 intervention length
and duration from baseline to nal assessment did not signicantly moderate eect sizes across studies.
Scholars further assert that through the systematic cultivation of gratitude, various types of benets have been
achieved i.e. school connectedness, academic success, strong peer and family relationships, higher absorption in
activities, better sleep quality33, . e most cited studies on gratitude intervention among youths were conducted
by Froh and colleagues33. Eleven classrooms - youth aged 11–14 years (6-7th grades) took part in two weeks
Counting blessings activities. Aer the intervention the experimental group presented greater optimism, life
satisfaction and gratitude disposition, experienced less negative emotions compared to their counterparts
without gratitude condition. Similar methodology was applied by Huebnar et al.34, who induced gratitude
towards school environment via 2-week intervention based on Counting good things at school. Students in
gratitude condition reported higher school satisfaction, were more interested in school activities, were more
positive towards attending school and engaging in learning. According to the authors the intervention mitigated
adverse educational appraisals, and at the same time increase emotional and social competences. A 2-weeks
positive Shamiri intervention implemented to 12–19 years old students, that included gratitude module has
revealed a positive eect on reducing internalizing symptoms and improved academic outcomes35. Similarly,
Sahar et al.36 found that the four-week improving gratitude program, which included counting blessings related
to school life, friends, studies and teachers, writing gratitude letters and loving kindness meditation increases
positive emotionality among adolescents aged 16 years. Chen et al.37 showed a signicant decline in stress and
depressive symptoms over time aer the 4-week writing of work-related gratitude diaries. Such an eect may
have preventive impact on impulsively motivated aggression, mostly derived from frustration and distress2.
Nevertheless, empirical studies on gratitude intervention and aggression among youth are scarce. One of such
gratitude- based interventions was a weekly group-based sharing gratitude and counting blessings exercises
implemented by Deng et al.21 among prisoners. Aer the 5-weeks the authors found that participants in the
gratitude sharing and blessing-counting subgroups had lower levels of aggression and higher of well-being than
the controls. Prior research also conrmed that expressing gratitude leads to the reduction behavioral aggression
in response to provocation, and to decrease the tendency of denigration of partners when confronting with
threatened feedbacks26,38. Interestingly, Cho et al.38 argued that the ameliorating eect of gratitude expression
on threatened power holders’ tendency to denigrate subordinates is mediated by increased perceptions of social
worth. Similarly, Chamizo-Nieto et al.7 emphasized that programs designed to enhance emotional intelligence
and gratitude in 12–18 aged adolescents yield promising results in mitigating cyber-aggression. ese programs
focus on fostering a deeper appreciation for positive emotions and social bonds, equipping individuals to
navigate online interactions constructively. By integrating gratitude practices into educational and community
initiatives, it is possible to develop a more resilient and empathetic digital culture that curtails the spread of
cyber-aggression.
Dierences in gratitude experiences across demographic characteristics
A review of past gratitude literature revealed that the feeling of gratitude and the ability to express this
complex emotion depends on a wide variety of contextual factors, including chronological gender (females
experience gratitude more frequently, with a greater intense, and are more sensitive to gratitude stimuli) Skalski,
Pochwatko39, age (older people are more willing to experience grateful mood and present their appreciation
towards other40, however the ndings regarding school-age children and youth are mixed. Empirical work by
O’Brien et al. (2018) suggested that older school-aged children express gratitude more oen compared to the
their younger counterparts. Chan41 observed a positive association between expressing gratitude and age during
adolescence period. In contrast, no signicant age-eect was detected by other scholars in samples aged 10–19
years old42. ese results may be related to dierent ways of grateful reactions. Baumgarten-Tramer43 proposed
four types of gratitude expressions: (a) verbal reaction, which is saying kindly thank you” to the benefactor,
without necessarily denoting any emotions; (b) concrete (repaying benefactor with something valuable for
child) (c) connective(dened as the creation of a spiritual relationship with the benefactor) (d) nalistic (child is
Scientic Reports | (2025) 15:14602 3
| https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-025-97214-w
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
repaying a favor through positive actions that assists in the objects’ attainment, or that promotes own personal
development. All those types were related to age. More specically, youth aged 11–14 more frequently expressed
verbal and connective gratitude, whereas those aged 7–10 years old its concrete and connective types. Interestingly,
the nalistic type was recognized as typical for older adolescents44 (de Lucca Freitas et al., 2011). Similarly
Morgan et al.45 found developmental and cultural dierences in relation to more sophisticated reasoning around
gratitude, such as recognition of ulterior motives. More specically, Australian adolescents were less impacted
by the ulterior motive than were their UK counterparts, and the feature of obligation/indebtedness was named
signicantly more in the UK sample than in the Australian cohort. Other results suggest that subjects from
collectivistic cultures tend to report higher levels of gratitude than those from individualistic ones45. Moreover,
gratitude was associated with more negative features in the UK than in the US46. Similarly, Parker et al.47
observed that higher self-focused and more autonomous interpersonal style people experience less gratitude.
Present study
e current research used a two-wave longitudinal design to explore the eectiveness of classroom gratitude
intervention (CGI) in reducing cyber-aggression among early adolescents from Poland. e primary objective
of this study was to examine the pre- and post-intervention levels of cyber-aggression types to identify the
distinct impact of CGI on these maladaptive behaviors. Based on ndings from past research6,7,34, the following
hypotheses were formulated:
1. Adolescents will demonstrate a decrease in cyber-aggression aer the 7-day CGI intervention.
2. e eectiveness of CGI in minimizing the frequency of youth cyber-aggression will vary based on gender
and gratitude disposition.
3. Early adolescent girls with a high level of gratitude will exhibit the greatest decrease in overt cyber-aggres-
sion.
4. Adolescents who identify as male and have low levels of gratitude will not exhibit a signicant decrease in
cyber-aggression following the CGI intervention.
Method
Participants and procedure
Approximately 548 adolescents from 7-8th grade primary school participated in the paper-pencil survey
conducted during 2023. e randomly chosen primary schools located in urban and rural areas of Poland were
invited to participate in the quasi-experimental survey. ere were no specic criteria, except attending 7-8th
grade. We randomly chose classes from those schools that agreed to participate in the project. Aer receiving
consent classes were randomly assigned to intervention vs. control conditions (there was no variation in
assignment – a similar number of schools from urban and rural areas took part in the survey). All subjects
provided assent, with counselors and parental consent, to participate in a study. e psychological assessments
in the control group were introduced by teachers, and completed in the classroom during classes. Similarly,
teachers provided information on a one-week intervention, with a measurement one week before the study and
a one-week follow-up in the experimental group. e control sample consisted of 399 participants (177 girls, 222
boys) aged 12–15 years (Mage = 13.7, SD = 0.67). e experimental sample included 149 adolescents (80girls,69
boys) aged 12–15 years (Mage = 13.18, SD = 0.55) at Time1, and 142 youth at Time2 (77 girls, 65 boys)aged 12–15
(Mage = 13.66, SD = 0.55). Seven participants from the experimental group did not complete all parts of the
survey (4.7% of the sample), due to the absence at Time 2 at school. is paper refers a part of larger project on
school burnout, personality traits and cyber-aggression among early adolescents in Poland. e controlled group
was examined only at Time 1 with measurements of Cyber-aggression (CATQ) scale, and demographic items,
whereas the experimental sample was tested twice time, that is at Time 1 we used CATQ scale and demographic
items, at Time 2 (3 weeks later) they fullled CATQ scale and gratitude (GQ) scale. e gratitude intervention
was delivered to students by trained teachers, who participated in a set of online meetings with the researchers
before the project started. e teachers were learned how to conduct the intervention, they were delivered
gratitude materials to improve their knowledge of the subjects, and they were also informed how to use an online
tool i.e. an online gratitude classroom book. One week aer the rst examination students took part in a 7-day
Classroom Gratitude Intervention (CGI), that includes enhancing knowledge on the importance of expressing
gratitude by watching a lm and discussion during lectures with teachers and classmates, gratitude exercises
such as counting blessing, gratitude journaling completed jointly with other students, gratitude challenge
completed privately. Each gratitude activity was implemented by trained teachers during classes (the sessions
lasted 45min per day during school week), who instructed each gratitude exercise by presenting “A Gratitude
Week” researchers’ PowerPoint presentations (a gratitude quiz, a link to gratitude lm, a hand of gratitude,
a gratitude self-reported level, and an example of classroom gratitude book). Aer these sessions, students
were divided into small sub-groups and completed collectively gratitude journal in the classroom setting (the
task for each group was to nd a gratitude sentence for each day and discuss it publically). Students were then
encouraged to fulll the private gratitude challenge during weekend (the implementation of this exercise was not
controlled). Prior the survey we obtained an approval from the University of National Educational Commission
Research Ethics Committee (Nb. DNa.0046.13.2023).All studies were conducted in accordance with relevant
guidelines and regulations. Informed consent was obtained from all participants and their parents based on the
information provided to them by teachers (students were informed during classes and parents during parent-
teacher evenings). e study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.
Scientic Reports | (2025) 15:14602 4
| https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-025-97214-w
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Measurements
Cyber-aggression Types Questionnaire (CATQ) developed by Runions et al.2 measures four types of online
aggression aversive impulsive and controlled, appetitive impulsive and controlled. e instruments includes29
items rated on a 4-point Likert scale (1-Very unlike me; 4-Very Like me) (e.g. Sometimes I can be mean to
people online to get what I want).e polish version of the CATQ was prepared by Tomaszek and Muchacka-
Cymerman48.e validity of the CATQ in Poland was conrmed by estimating correlations with oine
aggression (r = .42, p < .0001)28.Reliability in this study was very high (Control sample (n = 399): α /ɷ over 0.97
for CATQ Total score, and α/ɷ equal or over 0.90 for its subtype; Experimental sample (n = 149): α/ɷ over 0.90
for CATQ Total score, and α/ɷ equal or over 0.86 for its subtypes).
Gratitude Questionnaire (GQ) developed by McCullough et al.49, and adapted by Kossakowska and Kwiatek50
assess individuals’ gratitude disposition levels by using 7-point Likert scale (1-strongly disagree, 7 – strongly
agree). e subject respond on the 6 items (i.e. “I have so much in life to be thankful for”).e signicant positive
relationships between gratitude and other psychological positive measurement i.e. life satisfaction (r = .29,p < .05),
religiosity (r = .35, p < .05), ethical openness (r = .46, p < .05), and harmony (r = .35, p < .05) conrmed the validity
of the GQ scale in Poland.e GQ reliability in this study was high, i.e. α/ɷ equal to or higher than 0.88.
Demographic items– all participants were asked to report their biological gender (Male vs. Female), and age.
Data analysis
To test the eectiveness of the CGI intervention on levels of cyber-agression types, we conducted paired-
parametric and non-parametric statistics (tStudent, rankWilcoxon, U Mann-Whitney). Required minimum
sample size equal to 45 (tStudent test for matched pairs), and 47 (Wilcoxon signed-rank test – matched pairs)
(G*Power 3.1.9.7.calculator conditions: eect size = 0.50, α = 0.05, 1-ß = 0.95). To compare samples unequal in the
number of participants (experimental vs. control; girls vs. boys) we calculated the tStudentWel c h, and the U Mann-
Whitney statistics. Required sample 105 (tStudent independent sample), and 110 per group (G*Power3.1.9.7.
calculator conditions: two tails, d = 0.50, α = 0.05, 1-ß = 0.95). To examine the dierences between students
varied in the levels of gratitude disposition we divided the experimental sample into three sub-groups: Low
gratitude n = 32; medium gratitude n = 79, high gratitude n = 17 (χ2(2) = 95.39, p < .001), and used Kruskal-Wallis
statistic to examine the sub-group dierences. Required minimum sample size equal to 66 (G*Power 3.1.9.7.
calculator conditions: eect size = 0.50, α = 0.05, 1-ß = 0.95, number of groups 3). e pre-and post intervention
results within the three gratitude conditions were examined by calculating paired Wilcoxon rank test. Required
sample size reported above. Finally, we used Repeated Measures ANOVA (RM-ANOVA) to estimate interaction
eect between gender and gratitude. is procedure was used because regardingthe violations of normalityit
is robust (i.e. Type I error and power of the F-statistic are not altered) with skewness 2.31, and kurtosis 851.
Required minimum sample size equal to between factors: 51 subjects, and within factors: 12 subjects (G*Power
3.1.9.7. calculator conditions: f = 0.50, α = 0.05, 1-ß = 0.95). e conduct statistical analysis were performed by
using the IBM SPSS-22 (IBM Corp., NY) and the Jamovi 2.3.28 free soware.
Data availability
e datasets generated and analysed during the current study are not publicly available due to ethical restrictions
related to participant privacy and the condentiality agreements required by the institutional review board.
However, the data are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Results
Descriptive statistics
e CGI intervention signicantly decreased overall level of cyber-aggression (small eect size), and its aversive
controlled type (F2) (large eect size), however, slightly increased appetitive impulsive cyber-aggression (F3)
(small eect size) (see Table1).
Control vs. experimental sample
e controlled sample scored signicantly lower in CATQTime1 (z(546) =−7.56, p < .001, eect size = 0.42 -
average, Mean dierence = 6.50), and F2 Time1 (z(546) =−14.44, p < .001, eect size = 0.77 - average; Mean
dierence = 6.67) before CGI. Aer the intervention we detected one signicant dierence in aversive controlled
aggression (F2) (z(539) =−3.18,p = .001). Particularly, CGI intervention caused the decrease in overall CATQ
score among adolescents from experimental sample, hence, the controlled and experimental group were equal
in CATQ level (Mean dierence = 3.6, eect size = 0.05). In addition, students from experimental group still
scored higher in aversive controlled aggression (F2) (Mean dierence = 1.18; Eect size = 0.17 - small), however
the dierence was much smaller than at Time 1. An in-depth analysis reecting participants gender revealed
that before the intervention boys from control and experimental sample scored higher in aversive controlled
aggression (F2) (z(289) =−10.18, p < .001, Mean dierence = 6.81), and CATQ (z(289) =−5.29, p < .001, Mean
dierence = 7.57). However, unexpectedly aer the CGI boys from experimental sample scored signicantly
higher in some CATQ indicators (CATQ z(285) = −2.45, p = .014, Mean dierence = 10.61; F1 z(285) = −2.12,
p = .034, Mean dierence = 3.62; F3 z(285) =−3.25, p = .001, Mean dierence = 2.62). Importantly, dierences in F2
between both samples at Time 2 were smaller than at Time 1 (F2: z(285) =−3.53, p < .001, Mean dierence = 6.81),
indicating a decrease in aversive controlled cyber-aggression type. In girls sample the CATQ (z(255) =−5.47,
p < .001, Mean dierence = 5.42), and aversive controlled aggression (F2) (z(255) =−10.10, p < .001, Mean
dierence = 6.51) were higher in experimental sample. Aer the intervention we detected signicantly lower
scores in Aversive Impulsive type (F1) (z(252) =−2.28, p = .023) among girls from the experimental sample.
Importantly, the scores in Aversive Controlled type (F2) (z(252) =−0.99, p = .324), and CATQ (z(252) =−1.22,
Scientic Reports | (2025) 15:14602 5
| https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-025-97214-w
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
p = .224) decreased in experimental sample, so both groups were similar in F2 (Mean dierence = 0.10) and
CATQ (Mean dierence = −2.31).
Gender dierences
Before the gratitude intervention the insignicant dierences between girls and boys were observed in almost all
cyber-aggression factors, except appetitive controlled type (F4) – boys scored slightly higher in this type of cyber-
aggression compared to girls (tWelch (147) = −2.34, p = .021, d = .39-average eect size, 95%CI[−2.45;−.20]). Aer
the intervention girls scored signicantly lower in all examined characteristics(F1: tWelch (140) = −3.33, p = .001,
d = .57, 95%CI[−8.41;−2.14]; F2: tWelch (140) = −3.06, p = .003, d = .52, 95%CI[−4.05;−.87]; F3: tWel ch(140) = −3.54,
p < .001, d = .60, 95%CI[−4.44;−1.25]; F4: tWel ch(140) = −2.73, p = .007, d = .46, 95%CI[−3.37;−.54]; CATQ Total
score: tWelch (140) = −3.31, p = .001, d = .56, 95%CI[−20.00;−5.02]).e pre-post intervention comparison in girls
sample’ revealed a signicant decrease in CATQ total score (average eect size), and its two aversive dimensions:
impulsive (F1) (small eect size), and controlled (F2) (large eect size), and insignicance of changes in both
appetitive dimensions (F3, F4). In boys’ sample we detected an increase in aversive impulsive cyber-aggression
(F1) (average eect size), and a decrease in aversive controlled cyber-aggression (F2) (large eect size), and
appetitive impulsive cyber-aggression (F3) (average eect size). e levels of appetitive controlled cyber-
aggression (F4), and CATQ total score were similar to pre-intervention ones (see Fig.1). To sum up, study
hypothesis 1 was mostly conrmed.
Gratitude disposition
e ndings suggest signicant dierences aer the gratitude intervention in all three gratitude subgroups
were in cyber-aggression total score (χ2 (2, N = 142) = 7.36, p = .025, ε2=0.06)and its three types, that is: aversive
Fig. 1. e eectiveness of CGI in reducing cyber-aggression among early adolescents’ girls and boys.
Variables
M(SD) Skewness Kurtosis
Reliability tStudent/
Wilc oxon ran k pEect sizeCyber-aggression α ɷ
Aversive
Impulsive (F1) Time 1 18.2(7.31) 1.40 1.68 0.91 0.92 −1.02 0.309 0.09
Time 2 19.1(9.51) 1.41 1.49 0.96 0.96
Controlled (F2) Time 1 15.2(3.16) −0.16 −0.41 0.90 0.91 12.9 < 0.001 6.24
Time 2 9.71(4.77) 1.42 1.48 0.91 0.91
Appetitive
Impulsive (F3) Time 1 8.30(3.49) 1.95 3.77 0.86 0.87 1299 0.020 0.29
Time 2 9.39(4.85) 1.44 1.36 0.92 0.93
Controlled (F4) Time 1 7.11(3.36) 2.04 1.48 0.88 0.90 2105 0.328 0.11
Time 2 7.88(4.26) 3.91 1.20 0.93 0.94
CATQ Total score
Time 1 49.9(13.9) 1.48 2.55 0.96 0.96 6326 0.007 0.26
Time 2 46.0(22.7) 1.45 1.55 0.98 0.98
Gratitude Time 2 27.2(9.44) 0.20 −1.00 0.88 0.89
Tab le 1. Descriptive statistics and comparison between pre-and post gratitude intervention measures of
CATQ scores in experimental group (n = 149). Paired Samples tStudent test and Wilcoxon rank test were used
for comparisons with hypothesis Time ≠ Time 2.
Scientic Reports | (2025) 15:14602 6
| https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-025-97214-w
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
impulsive (F1:χ2 (2, N = 142) = 6.18, p = .046, ε2=0.05), aversive controlled (F2: χ2 (2, N = 142) = 7.09, p = .029,
ε2=0.06); appetitive impulsive (F3: χ2(2, N = 142) = 8.69, p = .013, ε2=0.07). We also observed a dierence on the
level of statistical tendency in appetitive controlled cyber-aggression type (F4: χ² (2, N = 142) = 5.71, p = .058,
ε2=0.04).e Dwass-Steel-Critchlow-Fligner pairwise comparisons showed that participants from medium
gratitude sample scored signicantly higher in cyber-aggression, and its two types, that is F2, and F3 measured
at Time 2 compared to their low gratitude counterparts (W = 3.54, p = .033, W = 3.71, p = .024; and W = 3.43,
p = .041, respectively). A non-parametric Wilcoxon rank statistic found a signicant decrease in CATQ total
score, and aversive impulsive (F1) and controlled (F2) cyber-aggression among subjects with low gratitude
sample. e aversive controlled cyber-aggression (F2) signicantly decreased, whereas the appetitive impulsive
and controlled cyber-aggression signicantly increased aer the intervention among individuals with medium
gratitude disposition. Finally, among adolescents with high gratitude disposition we detected a signicant
decrease only in aversive impulsive cyber-aggression (see Table2; Fig.2). Considering our study hypothesis 2 we
did not nd support for the notion that a high level of gratitude disposition will have the most positive impact
on the decrease in cyber-aggression.
The interaction eect of gender and gratitude disposition
e within subjects analysis: e RM-ANOVA showed signicant interaction eects for time and gender for
cyber-aggression total score, and its three types, i.e. aversive impulsive (F1), aversive controlled (F2); appetitive
impulsive (F3). Time and gratitude interaction was signicant in cyber-aggression total score, and its type
appetitive impulsive cyber-aggression (F3). is interaction was on the level of statistical tendency (i.e. p = .057)
in aversive impulsive cyber-aggression (F1) (see Table3).
e between subjects analysis: e ndings indicate a signicant dierences in cyber-aggression between
participants with low, medium, and high gratitude disposition in both aversive types of cyber-aggression (i.e.
impulsive and controlled). e results for gender, as well as the interaction between gender and gratitude were
insignicant (see Table4).
Discussion
We investigated the eect of gender and gratitude disposition on the eectiveness of a 7-day classroom gratitude
intervention in reducing cyber-aggression among early adolescents from Poland.
e rst study hypothesis referred to the decrease in cyber-aggression aer the 7-day CGI intervention. We
observed a signicant reduction in cyber-aggression (small eect size), and its aversive controlled (F2) type
(large eect size), however, slightly increased in appetitive impulsive cyber-aggression (F3) (small eect size).
Compared to adolescents in a control group, those who completed the CGI intervention reported signicant
decrease in cyber-aggression (g = 0.42 – average eect size). Our nding aligns with prior studies that highlighted
the positive eect of gratitude interventions on youth behavioral functioning20,26. For example, Ma et al.20 found
that greater life-orientation gratitude was associated with abstinence from sexual intimacy, sexual intercourse,
likelihood of engaging in sex during primary school, and abstinence from drug/alcohol use. According to the
authors, higher appreciation leads to personally and socially productive behaviors such as avoidance of high-
risk behaviors, but also serves as a resources reservoir that can be used when young people confront with life
demands. Our ndings, partially conrm the Wood’s coping hypothesis, according to which increasing gratitude
utilizes positive coping mechanisms, and reducing avoiding behaviors (i.e. risky activities) when confronting
Variables Gratitude level
Before
intervention Aerintervention
zpHedges gM(SD) M(SD)
Aversive
Impulsive
(F1)
Low 18.63(7.11) 16.09(7.32) -2.13 0.034 0.35
Medium 17.86(6.62) 21.18(10.46) -1.69 0.090 0.38
High 16.41(5.01) 16.59(7.33) -0.05 0.964 0.03
Controlled
(F2)
Low 14.19(3.14) 8.03(3.03) -4.48 < 0.001 2.00
Medium 15.30(3.32) 10.68(5.32) -5.75 < 0.001 1.04
High 16.00(3.37) 9.29(4.82) -3.36 0.001 1.61
Appetitive
Impulsive
(F3)
Low 8.50(3.19) 8.06(3.72) -0.94 0.348 0.13
Medium 7.81(3.06) 10.68(5.31) -3.79 < 0.001 0.66
High 8.24(3.63) 8.11(3.90) -0.07 0.943 0.03
Controlled
(F4)
Low 7.06(2.14) 6.72(3.39) -1.26 0.209 0.12
Medium 6.89(3.20) 8.81(4.82) -2.46 0.014 0.61
High 6.94(3.53) 6.88(3.10) 0.00 1.00 0.02
CATQ Total score
Low 48.38(12.24) 39.91(16.92) -3.05 0.002 0.57
Medium 47.86(12.62) 51.15(25.26) -1.00 0.921 0.16
High 47.59(13.12) 40.88(18.63) -1.33 0.185 0.42
Tab le 2. Means and standard deviation among low gratitude (n = 32), mediumgratitude (n = 79, and high
gratitude (n = 17) samples before and aer gratitude intervention. Non-parametric Wilcoxon rank statistic was
used for group comparison.
Scientic Reports | (2025) 15:14602 7
| https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-025-97214-w
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
with socially challenges situations i.e. conicts. e data are also in line with the notion that gratitude activates
moral self-schemas that are positively related to self-control, and manifests as a greater emotional and behavioral
regulation (i.e. reduces proactive and reactive aggression)52.
In our research, the classroom gratitude induction shared with natural counterparts was found to have a
signicant positive eect mostly its aversive controlled online acts. is type of cyber-aggression is an eect
of perceived provocation that is related to uncomfortable emotional states (e.g., shame, embarrassment). At
the same time, individuals maintain high capacities for self-control, hence aggressive acts are more likely to
take the form of calculated payback, or revenge2. e observed decline in aversive controlled cyber-aggression
may be explained through the typical gratitude-based benets i.e. an increase in positive emotions and
gratitude disposition33,53 what causes more positive attitudes towards others, and makes people less reactive to
provocations. For instance, studies conducted on a large sample of adolescents aged 12–17 years old by García-
Vázquez et al.52 showed that forgiveness and gratitude had an indirect relationship by decreasing both proactive
Variables
Aversive
impulsive Aversive
controlled Appetitive
impulsive Appetitive
controlled CATQ
total score
F η²pF η²pF η²pF η²pF η²p
Gender 0.57 0.01 0.84 0.01 2.39 0.02 2.90 0.02 1.52 0.01
Gratitude 3.05*0.05 4.12*0.06 1.45 0.02 1.91 0.03 2.86 0.05
Gender x Gratitude 1.07 0.02 1.15 0.02 1.19 0.02 0.76 0.01 1.15 0.02
Tab le 4. Between subjects eects for cyberaggression. Type 3 Sums of Squares; F statistic and Partial η² are
reported.
Variables
Aversive
impulsive Aversive
controlled Appetitive
impulsive Appetitive
controlled CATQ
total score
F η²pF η²pF η²pF η²pF η²p
Time 0.31 0.003 119.72*** 0.50 2.08 0.02 1.13 0.01 2.04 0.02
Time x Gender 7.28** 0.06 4.82*0.04 4.36*0.03 1.02 0.01 5.57*0.04
Time x Gratitude 2.94 0.05 1.82 0.03 4.56*0.07 2.56 0.04 3.39*0.05
Time x Gender x Gratitude 1.09 0.02 1.58 0.03 0.29 0.01 0.83 0.01 1.06 0.02
Tab le 3. Within subjects eects for cyber-aggression. Type 3 Sums of Squares; F statistic and Partial η² are
reported;
Fig. 2. e eectiveness of CGI in reducing Cyberaggression among individuals with dierent level of
gratitude disposition.
Scientic Reports | (2025) 15:14602 8
| https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-025-97214-w
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
and reactive aggression through their positive eects on self-control. Corresponding to the abovementioned
explanation prior research has found that practices based on cultivating gratitude activate greater generosity and
sensitivity exhibited towards others, as well as more empathetic and compassionate reactions, hence attenuating
students’ aggression21,26. According to the broaden-and-build framework, even a single positive experience (i.e.
expressing thanks for small gis) can extend and intensify due to positive creating upward spirals of emotions
and actions22. Such a positive circle may initiate the deep transformation of possessed interpersonal relationships,
hence the social environment is perceived and actually becomes more friendly and safe. e prosocial eect of
cultivating gratitude was conrmed by many scholars i.e. McCullough et al.49 posited that gratitude prompts
people to change their behaviors into more social. According to these authors the prosociality of gratitude
stems from its three core functions: (1) a moral barometer function (being the beneciary of someone’s moral
actions), (2) a moral motive function (motivation to behave morally toward the benefactor), and (3) a moral
reinforce function (being encouraged to behave morally in the future). ese gratitude functions may also
directly reduce the tendencies to behave immorally i.e. taking revenge through an online aggression. It is worth
adding, that moral disengagement mediated the relationship between gratitude and cyberpetration25. Gratitude
decreased the tendency to engage in unethical behavior without guilt or self-sanctions, because activated the
need for maintaining positive moral self-image. Noteworthy, the laboratory induced gratitude also increased the
distribution of self-resources to another54, which in turn may decrese the provocative acts from others. Another
possible mechanism underlying the eectiveness of gratitude-based interventions is related to an increase in self-
worth38. Prior studies have shown that expressing gratitude activates positive self-schemas and shis individuals’
thoughts on self-strengths, hence people are less prone to perceive conicts as threatened, and are more willing
to look for adaptive and creative ways of solving such situations via reconciliation55. For example, teams worked
under the gratitude condition experienced an increase in information elaboration and generated highly creative
ideas more than those in the neutral condition56. Dizon55 argued that participation in a gratitude journaling
by intimate partners increases adoption of more positive conict resolution styles, and decreased adherence of
negative ones.
On the contrary, the CGI was found to be ineective in appetitive impulsive cyber-aggression, named recreation.
is type of aggression refers to the self-generated violent acts by oenders, with a positive perception of violence
perpetration, recognized as fascinating and appealing2,57. According to Runions et al.2 it reects spontaneous
and immediate aggressive reactions, conducted without heed of long term consequences. is is an instrumental
and goal-oriented type of aggression, dened by the primary intrinsic enjoyment of aggressive activity58. e
origins of appetitive aggression are related to the adverse childhood experiences, that manifest through PTSD
symptoms later, produced by an ongoing climate of violence in families57. According to some scholars it can
be considered a stable and long-term adaptation to cope with an insecure and negative environment59. e
instances of impulsive-induced appetitive aggression are in the service of individuals’ protective mechanism
(attaining social status) and striving for signicance. In this context, increasing grateful moods through short-
term interventions may ineective, as this group of students may found hard to produce positive appraisals of
benets. It is possible, that this group of young people found it dicult to notice help from benefactor i.e. interpret
al.l benets as less valuable, costless or non-altruistically intended. Prior research has found that adolescents,
who experienced family abuse and school violence presented lower level of gratitude disposition60. Noteworthy,
although gratitude decreases PTSD symptoms among victims, individuals with traumatic experiences are
characterized by diminished level of gratitude disposition61. According to Kim et al.61 such a regularity is
related to negative self-attributions i.e. an increased amount of self-stigma (negative perspective and opinion
one carries about themselves) and social stigma. It is also worth to add, that the relationship between gratitude
and cyberperpetration was found to be mediated by other psychological characteristics i.e. self-compassion and
moral disengagement25. In accordance, the I3 metatheoretical model of aggression by Finkel and Hall62 posits
three orthogonal processes that causes violent reactions. e rst one is instigation, which includes immediate
environmental stimuli (e.g., provocation), the second one is impellance, situational or dispositional qualities
that decide on the intensity of aggressive response, and the third one is inhibition characterized by situational
or dispositional qualities responsible for overridden the proclivity. According to Zeng et al.63 gratitude and all
character strengths may be recognized as inhibitors of aggression, because grateful people tend to be more
compassionate and less critical towards themselves, which in turn inhibits violent acts in space and contributes
a higher tendency to develop healthy attitude towards others (i.e. all human beings deserve kindness). Finally, as
cyber-aggression may be an element of classroom in-group rivalry, the ineectiveness of CGI may be an eect of
class social hierarchy. According to social distance theory of power higher-power individuals experience higher
social distance towards others compared to low-powered ones64. In addition, those high in power are more
prone to perceive relationships in the context of social exchange. Hence, the level of engaging in group-gratitude
activities may be lower and the gratitude indebtedness may be higher among high-power people. Indeed, studies
conducted by Anicich et al.65 revealed that individuals with relatively low social power tend to be characterized
by increased feelings and expressions of gratitude aer beneting from a favor. Drawing on the above-presented
ndings, it is recommended to precede the gratitude-based intervention with a shiing perspective from rivalry
to equality, and to include activities that increase classroom positive climate.
e next three study hypothesis referred to gender and gratitude disposition as a signicant factors for the
eectiveness of CGI in minimizing the frequency of youth cyber-aggression. Our ndings conrmed that the
eect of CGI on cyber-aggression depended on participants’ gender (girls benet more from this intervention
compared to boys), and gratitude disposition (students with low gratitude levels experienced the highest
decrease in cyber-aggression, whereas the medium gratitude appeared as a risk factor for the increase in aversive
impulsive and controlled as well as appetitive controlled cyber-aggression).
Gender dierences: Gender was found to be a signicant factor that interacted with time and explained changes
in adolescent cyber-aggression, (i.e. aversive impulsive and controlled types, and appetitive impulsive type).
Scientic Reports | (2025) 15:14602 9
| https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-025-97214-w
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Additionally, we detected that boys exhibit a higher rate of cyber-aggression (before and aer the intervention)
compared to girls. e results remain a consensus with the past ndings suggesting that being an adolescent boy
is a risk factor for engaging in online perpetration6668. Moreover, the CGI intervention appeared ineective
among boys. Particularly, the changes in CATQ total score were either insignicant or indicated an increase
in some indicators - aversive and appetitive impulsive types of cyber-aggression. In contrast, we observed a
small decrease in the aversive controlled cyber-aggression type among boys’ experimental sample. e CGI
program seems to be more benecial when implemented among adolescent girls. We observed a decrease in
the overall level of cyber-aggression at pre-to-post intervention, and its all types. e comparison focused on
cyber-aggression subtypes revealed a signicant decrease in two aversive dimensions: impulsive and controlled,
but no changes in its appetitive types. e above results suggest that gratitude intervention may not be a
fruitful practice among all adolescents as its eectiveness is limited to some cyber-aggression types and varies
regarding recipients’ gender. e ndings coincide with prior notions that practitioners should dierentiate
cyber-aggression preventive strategies by sex69. Particularly, the main explanation for this suggestion stems from
diversity addressed for boys and girls in: (1) gender socialization, (i.e. a greater approval of direct – physical
aggression for boys, and an indirect – relational aggression for girls)70, (2) social norms (i.e. the experiences
of being perpetrator or victim are grounded in hegemonic masculinity - masculine dominance and gender
inequalities)69, and (3) the relatives’ normative expectations regarding resolving conict situations (i.e. the
belief that boys will attack during the confrontation, and girls will look for compromise) and building social
bonds (i.e. gender-normative girls have a greater vested interest than gender-normative boys in maintaining
friendships and resolving conicts)71. To understand more about sex diversity in cyber-perpetration it is worth
adding that gender is a signicant factor in motives, types, and consequences of this maladaptive behavior. More
interestingly, individuals with more feminine traits were found to engage in more cyber-relational aggression via
social networking sites and mobile phones, whereas those who identify themselves as more masculine carried
more oen cyber-verbal aggression through online gaming72. Moreover, boys not only report more tolerance
toward cyberbullying but also present more frequently impulsive cyber-aggression geared towards fullling their
immediate needs or heightening their social status and power (i.e. intrasexual competition between males)72,73.
In contrast, girls are mostly motivated to conduct spontaneous cyber-attacks in order to gain peer’s attention74.
erefore, the observed in our study higher decrease in cyber-aggression among girls may be related to satisfying
their need to attract attention and approval from classmates during CGI. However, the CGI practices could not
support the participants’ need for domination or power, which may explain the low eectiveness of gratitude
induction among males. Additionally, according to Kashdan74, the promptness to express positive emotions (i.e.
gratitude) is also mediated by gender. Particularly, men are less willing to feel and present their appreciation
to a benefactor, but also are more critical and suspicious of gratitude induction, thus deriving less goodness
from such practices. Moreover, gender signicantly dierentiates the preferred ways of expressing gratitude and
the urge need to reciprocate75. Consistently to these ndings, the CGI might be related to a higher feeling of
debt activated by gratitude induction among males and the preference of showing it privately to the benefactor.
In other words, improving gratitude among boys’ adolescents may be more eective when includes individual
face-to-face rather than classroom exercises. Finally, it is also worth mentioning, that past research pointed to
the adolescents’ identication with the role or motive they played in online aggressive behaviors. Particularly,
engaging in aggressive online attacks was more associated with the form of aggression rather than role (i.e.
sending aggressive posts more frequently was motivated by reactive reasons, whereas creating hostile websites
with proactive ones76. e above diversity in actions and motives underlying online aggression may also be
related to the sex dierences in CGI eectiveness (i.e. higher decrease in impulsive compared to controlled
aggression, and an increase in appetitive controlled aggression). Gratitude interventions are focused on exercises,
that enhance the willingness to express this feeling towards the benefactor and re-pay the goodness by prosocial
behaviors. erefore, such practices mostly strengthen the positive meaning of individuals’ lives (i.e. activates the
perspective of being supported and loved), and the capacity to control negative impulses (i.e. activates cognition
by initiating positive thoughts about others), however, for those who are motivated to commit aggression by
instrumental purposes (increase the power or domination over others), that is emotionally callousness proactive
aggression, typical strategy to increase prosocial emotions in order to reduce aggressive acts may be ineective
because must be preceded by an inner change of personal goals and fundamental values.
Gratitude disposition: Our study indicates that students with a low level of gratitude experienced a signicant
decrease in cyber-aggression, and in its two aversive types, whereas participants from the medium gratitude
sample scored signicantly higher in cyber-aggression, and in its two appetitive types. A decrease in aversive
impulsive cyber-aggression type was observed among medium and high gratitude adolescents. Signicant
interaction eects were detected between the gratitude disposition and the time when explaining cyber-
aggression total score and its appetitive Impulsive type. e above ndings are partly in line with McCullough’s
et al.77 resistance hypothesis, reecting that a high grateful mood inhibits the benets of discrete positive
emotional episodes because of a lack of reasons for looking new benets to be appreciated for. In fact, youth
high in gratitude disposition may believe that a grateful attitude is something obvious, and there is no need to
present privately experienced emotions jointly with the classmates as the benefactor has already received the
thanks. Additionally, low gratitude individuals may experience the “halo eect” related to a new meaningful
perspective (i.e. ‘Being thankful for all the advantages in your life makes your life better) activated by the CGI,
and thus they might start to see more positives around them, which stopped them from acting out aversive
emotions through the digital devices. It is worth highlighting that these ndings are not in line with the notion
that low gratitude adolescents typically magnify the daily adversities’ distress by blindly adopting accidental and
inappropriate coping strategies, and thus they are constantly frustrated which results in inner disturbance and
adverse states78. erefore, momentarily activated appreciation by the CGI might have initiated the process of
looking for highly valued benet, which is shared by the entire class group. In this context, the relative size of
Scientic Reports | (2025) 15:14602 10
| https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-025-97214-w
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
goodness proposed by the classmates might allow for changing the perspective regardless of the low gratitude
trait. In other words, youth may be guided by trained group leaders to be grateful for all things received from
others facilitating a positive perception of a person’s life and the owned strength and resources. Similarly to other
studies79 gratitude alleviated stress related to school events and reduced the frequency of externalized behaviors,
such as cyber-aggression.
e fact that medium gratitude students experienced an increase in cyber-aggression aer the CGI is more
dicult to explain, as somehow is inconsistent with past research52. Notably, our results suggest that gratitude
intervention is more eective in minimizing reactive cyber-aggression, that is an impulsive action initiated
by a provocation or an actual or conceived threat80. Moreover, it is hostile aggression that “is a primordial
means for establishing one’s sense of signicance and mattering, thus addressing a frustration of a fundamental
social-psychological need81. Past studies conrmed that by enhancing gratitude individuals activate cognitive
planning, emotional self-control, and emotional stability, but also increase the satisfaction of fullling extrinsic
and intrinsic needs82. us individuals are less motivated to react aggressively in response to frustration. e
detected ineectiveness of the CGI in reducing appetitive controlled online aggression may be also related to the
following characteristics: (1) most cyber-attacks are done by one or a few perpetrators, (2) cyber-aggressors are
usually from the same class group (3) cyber-attack oen lasts about a week and is committed outside of school83.
e CGI intervention was provided only for one week, inside the school, with no control for the internee’s
engagement and expectations in the proposed grouped gratitude exercises. e complex long-term adverse
relation between perpetrator and victim, and the self-identication of aggressor with the played role may be so
strong that a one-week lasting induction of gratitude perspective may not be enough to change these grounded
class-positions. erefore, although momentary cyber-aggression attacks inside school may be less frequent,
this eect may not last aer schooling. Additionally, even though gratitude motivates individuals to be more
sensitive or more focused on others’ needs, and promotes prosociality, and by doing so mitigates intentional
aggression directed to harm others, the key determinant of these benets may lay in the level of empathy26. More
specically, higher empathy mediates the positive relational behaviors conveyed by gratitude expressions, since is
a social anchor, reducing antipathic behaviors during face-to-face contact84. Enhancing empathetic skills is also
thought to be an eective strategy in reducing youths’ involvement in cyber-perpetration3,50. Nevertheless, some
evidence demonstrates that aective empathy and anti-empathy (the tendency to feel a contradictory response
to others’ emotions) signicantly accounted for aggression, but its cognitive component did not85. Knowing
the appetitive controlled cyber-aggression is weakly related to empathy12, we may conclude that CGI, which is
built on emotional induction, may not be eective as a single strategy to mitigate aggressive acts on the Internet
committed for violence lust. Importantly, gratitude is dened as an interpersonal process, directly related to
social relationships, and depends on expected social costs. For example, Oishi et al.86 found that expressing
gratitude towards someone important vs. for something good is more costly, because elicits a higher rate of
indebtedness, and adverse emotions (guilt, shame). Furthermore, feelings of elevation and indebtedness caused
by writing gratitude letters were found to be unique to socially relevant expressions of gratitude (i.e., gratitude
to a specic benefactor)87. Considering the above limitations of gratitude interventions, if the CGI activated
among perpetrators negative emotions such as guild: ‘I have received so much and I have not repaid anyone’; or
shame: ‘I have received so much and I myself do not give good things’ the recipients may try to discard them by
minimizing the meaningfulness of gratitude exercises (i.e. making jokes from those who express gratitude), or
not engaging in those gratitude activities that were supposed to be done with counterparts. As a consequence,
the eectiveness of gratitude practices may be disturbed. Another barriers, that limit the benecial eects of
gratitude interventions, are related to the suppression of emotional expression of the experienced gratitude
due to social expectations. More specically, Kumar88 pointed out that people underestimate the goodness or
overestimate the costs of showing gratitude, which creates a misplaced barrier to interpersonal interaction.
Consequently, it undermines the pro-social values in daily life and creates social distance89. Noteworthy, past
results suggest that the positive eect of gratitude intervention on persons’ mental health is rather short-lived and
small90,91. Complementing the above explanations, it is also worth adding that the ability to respond to gratitude
induction may depend on the development of social-cognitive and emotional processes (i.e. empathetic attitude
towards classmates), that were found as signicant mediators between social normative beliefs about aggression
and aggressive behaviors92. Additionally, according to the bi-factor structural theory of gratitude, it consists of
either cognitive or aective dimension, however when the emotional aspect of gratitude (i.e. the feeling of being
grateful to a benefactor) positively predicts mental health outcomes (in cross-sectional and longitudinal studies),
the cognitive gratitude (i.e. one’s appreciation for the benefaction – reecting it as goodness) did it negatively.
Considering that most GQ items refer to cognitive gratitude, one week aer the CGI intervention data collection
could not capture the accurate level of gratitude feelings, which are rather connected to day-to-day interactions.
Furthermore, the meta-analysis revealed that the vast number of cyber-aggression interventions did not show
positive eects in the long term, especially as they were focused only on social skills and implemented only
through the classroom without delivering a wider program focused on the entire school society92.
To our knowledge, no previous studies have analyzed the impact of gratitude-based intervention on
dierent types of cyber-aggression. According to our ndings, the short-term activation of grateful attitudes
among youth eectively reduces aversive, but not appetitive cyber-aggression. Next, adolescent girls benet
more from gratitude intervention than adolescent boys (among the girls’ sample we observed a decline in all
types of cyber-aggression, whereas among boys only in the aversive controlled type). Finally, the last valuable
result is related to the level of gratitude disposition, the highest decrease was observed among the low-gratitude
subgroup in aversive cyber-aggression. In summary, we must conclude that the eectiveness of Classroom
Gratitude Intervention aimed to reduce adolescent cyber-aggression is limited regarding some early adolescent
groups, therefore practitioners should adopt a broader path of preventive strategies that is not focused only
Scientic Reports | (2025) 15:14602 11
| https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-025-97214-w
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
on strengthening positive emotions, but provides students with knowledge about the phenomenon of cyber-
aggression itself and its adverse long-term consequences.
Study limitations
e study identied several important limitations that need to be considered. First, the eectiveness of the
Gratitude Classroom Intervention (CGI) was not consistent across all participant groups. In particular, boys and
students with medium levels of gratitude did not experience the same positive eects as others, and in some cases,
their levels of certain types of cyber-aggression actually increased. is raises concerns about the generalizability
of the intervention and suggests that it may not be equally eective for all groups. Next, 4.7% dropouts from
Time 1 to Time 2 in the experimental group were observed. Failing to measure the analyzed variables among
these students might impact the ndings. We thus recommend using mixed methods to improve gratitude, to
avoid possible participants attrition or boredom. Additionally, the short duration of the intervention—lasting
only one week—may have limited its potential to produce lasting behavioral changes. Interventions of longer
duration could provide more substantial eects and allow for a better understanding of how gratitude practices
inuence behavior over time. Another signicant limitation is the reliance on self-reported data through the
CATQ and GQ questionnaires. While these tools are validated, self-reported data may introduce bias, such as
social desirability or inaccurate self-assessment. Future studies could mitigate this limitation by incorporating
additional objective measures, such as behavioral obser vations or peer reports, to provide a more comprehensive
evaluation of outcomes. e study also measured outcomes only within the school setting, without considering
external inuences or behaviors that occur outside of the classroom. is limitation makes it challenging to
fully assess the overall impact of the gratitude intervention, as many relevant interactions and experiences may
have taken place in other environments. Lastly, the study’s sample was limited to a specic group of Polish
adolescents, which restricts the ability to generalize the ndings to other populations, age groups, or cultural
contexts. Broader studies that involve more diverse samples would be necessary to draw conclusions that are
applicable to a wider range of settings.
Future directions
Building on the limitations identied in the current study, future research should aim to address several key
areas. One of the main recommendations is to extend the duration of gratitude interventions. e one-week
timeframe used in this study may not have been sucient to foster lasting change in participants’ behaviors and
attitudes. Future research should consider implementing longer interventions that include follow-up activities or
booster sessions to reinforce the concepts learned and help maintain the benets over time.
Furthermore, the mixed methods of gratitude induction, including verbal, concrete, connective, and nalistic
ways of its expression seems reasonable to increase the eectiveness of such practices among early adolescents.
Another critical direction for future research is the need to tailor interventions to account for gender dierences.
e study’s ndings suggest that gender signicantly inuences how participants respond to gratitude
interventions, with girls beneting more than boys. Future programs should therefore consider oering more
personalized activities that cater to the distinct needs, motivations, and social dynamics of dierent genders.
Additionally, expanding the scope of research to include more varied and larger samples is crucial. Future
studies should aim to include participants from dierent cultural, socioeconomic, and geographic backgrounds,
as this would provide more robust and generalizable ndings regarding the eectiveness of gratitude-based
interventions. Finally, it is essential to investigate the long-term outcomes of such interventions. Conducting
longitudinal studies that track participants’ behaviors over extended periods would help determine whether the
observed eects of gratitude practices are enduring or whether they diminish over time. is type of research
would provide valuable insights into the sustained impact of gratitude interventions and inform the design of
more eective programs.
Conclusions
e ndings of this study indicate that gratitude-based interventions hold promise as a tool for reducing cyber-
aggression among early adolescents, but their eectiveness is not universal and is inuenced by several factors,
including gender and participants’ initial levels of gratitude. While the intervention was successful in reducing
aversive controlled typ es of cyber-aggression, it paradoxically led to an increase in impulsive appetitive aggression
in some cases. ese results underscore the complexity of using gratitude as a prevention strategy and highlight
the importance of tailoring interventions to the specic needs and characteristics of the target audience. e
study suggests that while gratitude can be a valuable component in eorts to curb cyber-aggression, it should be
integrated into more comprehensive approaches that combine emotional regulation strategies with educational
programs focused on the broader issue of cyber-aggression and its consequences. Overall, the study emphasizes
the need for more nuanced and exible approaches to addressing cyber-aggression, ones that take into account
the diverse factors that inuence adolescent behavior.
Data availability
e datasets generated and analysed during the current study are not publicly available due to ethical restrictions
related to participant privacy and the condentiality agreements required by the institutional review board.
However, the data are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Received: 28 October 2024; Accepted: 3 April 2025
Scientic Reports | (2025) 15:14602 12
| https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-025-97214-w
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
References
1. Zhu, C., Huang, S., Evans, R. & Zhang, W. Cyberbullying among adolescents and children: A comprehensive review of the global
situation, risk factors, and preventive measures. Front. Public. Health. 9, 634909. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2021.634909
(2021).
2. Runions, K. C., Bak, M. & Shaw, T. Disentangling functions of online aggression: e Cyber-Aggression typology questionnaire
(CATQ). Aggressive Behav. 43 (1), 74–84. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21663 (2017).
3. Xiao, B., Parent, N., Bond, T., Sam, J. & Shapka, J. Developmental trajectories of Cyber-Aggression among early adolescents in
Canada: e impact of aggression, gender, and time spent online. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public. Health. 21, 429. h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 3 3
9 0 / i j e r p h 2 1 0 4 0 4 2 9 (2024).
4. Hollá, K., Sender, B. & Kosovac, S. Empathy in the prevention of Cyber-aggression. J. Educ. Teach. Social Stud. 5, 59–69. h t t p s : / / d
o i . o r g / 1 0 . 2 2 1 5 8 / j e t s s . v 5 n 3 p 5 9 (2023).
5. Camerini, A. L., Marciano, L., Carrara, A. & Schulz, P. J. Cyberbullying perpetration and victimization among children and
adolescents: A systematic review of longitudinal studies. Telematics Inform. 49, 101362 (2020).
6. Deng, Y. et al. Counting blessings and sharing gratitude in a Chinese prisoner sample: Eects of gratitude-based interventions on
subjective well-being and aggression. J. Posit. Psychol. 14 (3), 303–311. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2018.1460687 (2018).
7. Chamizo-Nieto, M. T., Rey, L. & Pellitteri, J. Gratitude and emotional intelligence as protective factors against Cyber-Aggression:
Analysis of a mediation model. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 12, 4475. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17124475 (2020).
8. Rubio Hurtado, M. J., Donoso Vázquez, T. & VilàBaños, R. Factors related to gender cyber-victimization in social networks among
Spanish youth. Civilizar: CienciasSociales Y Humanas. 21 (40), 83–100 (2021).
9. Foody, M., McGuire, L., Kuldas, S. & O’Higgins Norman, J. Friendship quality and gender dierences in association with
cyberbullying involvement and psychological Well-Being. Front. Psychol. 10, 1723. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01723
(2019).
10. Lewon, M., Houmanfar, R. A. & Hayes, L. J. e will to ght: Aversion-Induced aggression and the role of motivation in intergroup
conicts. Perspect. Behav. Sci. 42 (4), 889–910. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40614-019-00221-2 (2019).
11. Dodge, K. A. & Coie, J. D. Social-information-processing factors in reactive and proactive aggression in children’s peer groups. J.
Personal. Soc. Psychol. 53 (6), 1146–1158. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.53.6.1146 (1987).
12. Euler, F., Steinlin, C. & Stadler, C. Distinct proles of reactive and proactive aggression in adolescents: Associations with cognitive
and aective empathy. Child. Adolesc. Psychiatry Mental Health. 11, 1. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13034-016-0141-4 (2017).
13. Elbert, T., Moran, J. & Schauer, M. Appetitive aggression. In (ed Bushman, B. J.) Aggression and Violence: A Social Psychological
Perspective (119–135). Routledge, London. (2017).
14. DeMarsico, D., Bounoua, N., Miglin, R. & Sadeh, N. Aggression in the digital era: Assessing the validity of the cyber motivations
for aggression and deviance scale. Assessment 29 (4), 764–781. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191121990088 (2022).
15. Centifanti, L. C., FantiKA, omson, N. D., Demetriou, V. & Anastassiou-Hadjicharalambous, X. Types of relational aggression
in girls are dierentiated by Callous-Unemotional traits, peers and parental overcontrol. Behav. Sci. (Basel Switzerland). 5 (4),
518–536. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs5040518 (2015).
16. Wright, M. F. & Wachs, S. Adolescents’ cyber victimization: e inuence of technologies, gender, and gender stereotype traits. Int.
J. Environ. Res. Public. Health. 17 (4), 1293. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17041293 (2020).
17. León-Moreno, C., Musitu Ochoa, G., Cañas Pardo, E., Estévez López, E. & CallejasJerónimo, J. E. Relationship between school
integration, psychosocial adjustment, and cyber-aggression among adolescents. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public. Health. 18 (1), 108.
https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18010108 (2021).
18. Álvarez-García, D., Núñez, J. C., García, T. & Barreiro-Collazo, A. Individual, family, and community predictors of cyber-
aggression among adolescents. Eur. J. Psychol. Appl. Legal Context. 10, 79–88. https://doi.org/10.5093/ejpalc2018a8 (2018).
19. Uddin, M. K. & Rahman, J. Cyber victimization and cyber aggression among high school students: Emotion regulation as a
moderator. Cyberpsychology: J. Psychosocial Res. Cyberspace. https://doi.org/10.5817/CP2022-2-4 (2022).
20. Ma, M., Kibler, J. L. & Sly, K. Gratitude is associated with greater levels of protective factors and lower levels of risks in African
American adolescents. J Adolesc.. 36(5), 983–991. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2013.07.012 (2013).
21. Deng, Y. et al. Counting blessings and sharing gratitude in a Chinese prisoner sample: Eects of gratitude-based interventionson
subjective well-being and aggression. J. Posit. Psychol. 14 (3), 303–311. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2018.1460687 (2019).
22. Fredrickson, B. L. e role of positive emotions in positive psychology: the broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. Am.
Psychol. 56, 218–226. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066x.56.3.218 (2001).
23. Sansons, R. A. & SansoneLA Gratitude and well-being: e benets of appreciation. Psychiatry (Edgmont). 7, 18–22 (2010).
24. Oliveira, W. A., Esteca, A. M. N., Wechsler, S. M. & Menesini, E. Bullying and cyberbullying in school: Rapid review on the roles
of gratitude, forgiveness, and Self-Regulation. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 21, 839. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph21070839
(2024).
25. Zeng, P., Wang, P., Nie, J., Ouyang, M. & lei, L. Gratitude and cyberbullying perpetration: e mediating role of self-compassion
and moral disengagement. Child Youth Serv. Rev. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2020.105608 (2020)., 119, Article 105608.
26. DeWall, C. N., Lambert, N. M., Pond, R. S. Jr., Kashdan, T. B. & FinchamFD A grateful heart is a nonviolent heart: Cross-sectional,
experience sampling, longitudinal, and experimental evidence. Social Psychol. Personality Sci. 3 (2), 232–240. h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 1
1 7 7 / 1 9 4 8 5 5 0 6 1 1 4 1 6 6 7 5 (2012).
27. Fulantelli, G., Taibi, D., Scifo, L., Schwarze, V. & Eimler, S. C. Cyberbullying and Cyberhate as two interlinked instances of Cyber-
Aggression in adolescence: A systematic review. Front. Psychol. 13, 909299. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.909299 (2022).
28. Froh, J. F. & Bono, G. Gratitude in youth: A review of gratitude inter ventions and some ideas for applications. Communique 39 (5),
26–28 (2011).
29. Wong, Y. J., Pandelios, A. L., Carlock, K. & ielmeyer, A. M. B. Stronger together: Perspectives on gratitude social processes in
group interventions for adolescents. Front. Psychol. 15, 1476511. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1476511 (2024).
30. Fadil, A. & Ayriza, Y. Eectiveness of gratitude interventions in schools to increase sense of belonging to high school students. Int.
J. Social Sci. Res. 4 (9), 1–10. https://doi.org/10.46799/ijssr.v4i9.941 (2024).
31. Gajadien, J. & Hendriks, T. Grantangi: A feasibility study of a One-Day gratitude intervention in Suriname. Caribb. J. Psychol. 15
(1), 109–136. https://doi.org/10.37234/CJP.2023.1501.A005 (2023).
32. Kirca, A., Malou, J. & Meynadier, J. e eect of expressed gratitude interventions on psychological wellbeing: A Meta-Analysis
of randomised controlled studies. Int. J. Appl. Posit. Psychol. 8, 63–86. https://doi.org/10.1007/s41042-023-00086-6 (2023).
33. Froh, J. F., Seck, W. J. & Emmons, R. A. Counting blessings in early adolescents: An experimental study of gratitude and subjective
well-being. J. Sch. Psychol. 46, 213–233 (2008).
34. Huebner, E. S., Drane, W. & Valois, R. F. Levels and demographic correlates of adolescent life satisfaction reports. School Psychol.
Int. 21 (3), 281–292. https://doi.org/10.1177/0143034300213005 (2000).
35. Osborn, T. L. et al. Single-session digital intervention for adolescent depression, anxiety, and well-being: outcomes of a randomized
controlled trial with Kenyan adolescents. J. Consult Clin. Psychol. 88 (7), 657–668 (2020).
36. Sahar, N. U., Lea-Baranovich, D. & arbe, I. H. A. Promoting positive emotions among adolescents through school based
gratitude interventions: A preliminary study. GESJ: Educ. Sci. Psychol. 3 (60), 35–49 (2021).
37. Cheng, S. T., Tsui, P. K. & Lam, J. H. Improving mental health in health care practitioners: Randomized controlled trial of a
gratitude intervention. J. Consult Clin. Psychol. 83, 177–186. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037895 (2015).
Scientic Reports | (2025) 15:14602 13
| https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-025-97214-w
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
38. Cho, Y. & Fast, N. J. Power, defensive denigration, and the assuaging eect of gratitude expression. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 48, 778–782
(2012).
39. Skalski, S. & Pochwatko, G. Gratitude is female. Biological sex, socio-cultural gender versus gratitude and positive orientation.
Curr. Issues Personality Psychol. 8 (1), 1–9. https://doi.org/10.5114/cipp.2020.93624 (2020).
40. Chopik, W. J. et al. Changes in optimism and pessimism in response to life events: Evidence from three large panel studies. J. Res.
Pers. 88, 103985. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2020.103985 (2020).
41. Chan, D. W. Life satisfaction among highly achieving students in Hong Kong: do gratitude and the ‘good-enough mindset’ add to
the contribution of perfectionism in prediction? Educational Psychol. 32 (5), 613–626. h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 1 0 8 0 / 0 1 4 4 3 4 1 0 . 2 0 1 2 . 6 8 5
4 5 1 (2012).
42. Obeldobel, C. A. & Kerns, K. A. A literature review of gratitude, parent–child relationships, and well-being in children. Dev. Rev.
61, 100948. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2021.100948 (2021).
43. Baumgarten-Tramer, F. Gratefulness in children and young people. J. Genet. Psychol. 53, 53–66 (1938).
44. de Lucca Freitas, L., Pieta, M. A. M. & Tudge, J. R. H. Beyond politeness: e expression of gratitude in children and adolescents.
Psicol. Reex Crit. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79722011000400016 (2011).
45. Morgan, B., Gulliford, L. & Waters, L. Taking thanks for granted: A Cross-Cultural exploration of gratitude in the UK and Australia.
Cross-Cultural Res. 56 (2–3), 185–227. https://doi.org/10.1177/10693971211067048 (2022).
46. Morgan, B., Gulliford, L. & Kristjánsson, K. Gratitude in the UK: A new prototype analysis and a cross-cultural comparison. J.
Posit. Psychol. 9 (4), 281–294 (2014).
47. Parker, S. C., Majid, H., Stewart, K. L. & Ahrens, A. H. No thanks! Autonomous interpersonal style is associated with less experience
and valuing of gratitude. Cogn. Emot. 31 (8), 1627–1637 (2017).
48. Tomaszek, K. & Muchacka-Cymerman, A. Is it just a matter of impulse control?’ A cross-cultural study in oine and online
aggression among Japanese and European burnout students. Kwartalnikpedagogiczny 3 (269), 111–137. h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 3 1 3 3 8 / 2
6 5 7 - 6 0 0 7 . k p . 2 0 2 3 - 3 . 6 (2023).
49. McCullough, M. E., Emmons, R. A. & Tsang, J. A. e grateful disposition: A conceptual and empirical topography. J. Personality
SocialPsychology. 82 (1), 112–127. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.82.1.112 (2002).
50. Kossakowska, M. & Kwiatek, P. Polska Adaptacja Kwestionariusza do Badania wdzięczności GQ-6. Przegląd Psychologiczny. 57 (4),
503–514 (2014).
51. Blanca, M. J., ArnauJ, García-Castro, F. J., Alarcón, R. & B ono, R. Non-normal data in repeated measures ANOVA: Impact on type
I error and power. Psicothema 35 (1), 21–29. https://doi.org/10.7334/psicothema2022.292 (2023).
52. García-Vázquez, F. I., Valdés-Cuervo, A. A. & Parra-Pérez, L. G. e eects of forgiveness, gratitude, and Self-Control on reactive
and proactive aggression in bullying. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public. Health. 17 (16), 5760. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17165760
(2020).
53. DeSteno, D., Li, Y., Dickens, L. & Lerner, J. S. Gratitude: A tool for reducing economic impatience. Psychol. Sci. 25 (6), 1262–1267.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614529979 (2014).
54. Tsang, J. A. Gratitude and prosocial behaviour: An experimental test of gratitude. Cogn. Emot. 20 (1), 138–148. h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0
. 1 0 8 0 / 0 2 6 9 9 9 3 0 5 0 0 1 7 2 3 4 1 (2006).
55. Dizon, M.T.S. e Eect of Gratitude Journaling on Conict Resolution in Intimate Dyadic Relationships.Philippine Journal of
Psychology. 53(1), 117–144. https://doi.org/10.31710/pjp/0053.05 (2020).
56. Pillay, N., Park, G., Kim, Y. K. & Lee, S. anks for your ideas: Gratitude and team creativity. Organ. Behav. Hum Decis. Process.
156, 69–81. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2019.11.005 (2020).
57. Augsburger, M., Meyer-Parlapanis, D., Bambonye, M., Elbert, T. & A Appetitive aggression and adverse childhood experiences shape
violent behavior in females formerly associated with combat. Front. Psychol. 6, 1756. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01756
(2015).
58. Hemmings, S. M. J. et al. Appetitive and reactive aggression are dierentially associated with the STin2 genetic variant in the
serotonin transporter gene. Sci. Rep. 8 (1), 6714. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-25066-8 (2018).
59. Crombach, A. & Elbert, T. e benets of aggressive traits: A study with current and former street children in Burundi. Child.
Abuse Negl. 38, 1041–1050. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2013.12.003 (2014).
60. Choi, J. H. & Yu, M. Correlates of gratitude disposition in middle school students: gender dierences. Technol. Health Care. 22 (3),
459 –466 https://doi.org/10.3233/THC-140804 (2014).
61. Kim, J. N. Dierences in the Protective Relation between Social Support, Gratitude, and PTSD across Sexual and Non-Sexual Trauma
473 (UVM Honors College Senior eses, 2022).
62. Finkel, E. J. & Hall, A. N. e I3 model: A metatheoretical framework for Understanding aggression. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 19,
125–130. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.03.013 (2018).
63. Zeng, P., Wang, P., Nie, J., Ouyang, M. & Lei, L. Gratitude and cyberbullying perpetration: e mediating role of self-compassion
and moral disengagement. Child Youth Serv. Rev. 119, 105608. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2020.105608 (2020).
64. Magee, J. C. & Smith, P. K. e social distance theory of power. Personality Social Psychol. Rev. 17 (2), 158–186. h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 .
1 1 7 7 / 1 0 8 8 8 6 8 3 1 2 4 7 2 7 3 2 (2013).
65. Anicich, E. M., Lee, A. J. & Liu, S. anks, but no thanks: Unpacking the relationship between relative power and gratitude. Pers.
Soc. Psychol. Bull. 48 (7), 1005–1023. https://doi.org/10.1177/01461672211025945 (2022).
66. Sourander, A. et al. Psychosocial riskfactors associated with cyberbullying among adolescents: A population-based study. Arch.
Gen. Psychiatry. 67, 720–728. https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.79 (2010).
67. Sun, S., Fan, X. & Du, J. Cyberbullying perpetration: A meta-analysis of gender dierences. Int. J. Internet Sci. 11, 61–81 (2016).
68. Eriksson, L., McGee, T. R., Rosse, V., Bond, C. & Horstman, N. When cyberaggression is personal: Gender dierences in threats
and betrayals of partners and friends. J. Aggress. Con. Peace Res. 15 (2), 95–108. https://doi.org/10.1108/JACPR-10-2021-0647
(2023).
69. Pérez-Rodríguez, P. et al. Peer cybervictimization and cyberaggression as a function of developmental stage during adolescence: A
preliminary study. Acta. Psychol. 246, 104280. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2024.104280 (2024).
70. Feijóo, S. et al. Sex dierences in adolescent bullying behaviours. Psychosocial Intervention. 30 (2), 95–100. h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 5 0 9 3
/ p i 2 0 2 1 a 1 (2021).
71. Wright, M. F. e role of technologies, behaviors, gender, and gender stereotype traits in adolescents’ cyber aggression. J. Interpers.
Violence. 35 (7–8), 1719–1738. https://doi.org/10.1177/0886260517696858 (2020).
72. Antipina, S. S. & Bakhvalova EV, Miklyaeva, A. Gender dierences in adolescent cyber-aggression. e Herzen University
Conference on Psychology in Education. Conference paper. https://doi.org/10.33910/herzenpsyconf-2019-2-7 [accessed Aug 24
2024]. (2019).
73. Lapierre, K. R. & Dane, A. V. Cyberbullying, cy ber aggression, and cy ber victimization in relation to adolescents’ dating and sexual
behavior: An evolutionary perspective. Aggressive Behav. 46 (1), 49–59. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21864 (2020).
74. Kashdan, T. B., Mishra, A., Breen, W. E. & JJ Gender dierences in gratitude: Examining appraisals, narratives, the willingness to
express emotions, and changes in psychological needs. J. Pers. 77 (3), 691–730. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00562.x
(2009).
75. Preś, J. E. et al. What regulates gratitude response of women and men?? e role of the received good, psychosocial factors, and
repayment. Psychol. Rep. 123 (2), 395–419. https://doi.org/10.1177/0033294118811620 (2020).
Scientic Reports | (2025) 15:14602 14
| https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-025-97214-w
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
76. Law, D. M., Shapka, J. D., Domene, J. F. & Gagné, M. H. Are cyberbullies really bullies? An investigation of reactive and proactive
online aggression. Comput. Hum. Behav. 28 (2), 664–672. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2011.11.013 (2012).
77. McCullough, M. E., Tsang, J. A. & Emmons, R. A. Gratitude in intermediate aective terrain: Links of grateful moods to individual
dierences and daily emotional experience. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 86 (2), 295–309. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.86.2.295
(2004).
78. Sun, P., Sun, Y., Jiang, H., Jia, R. & Li, Z. Gratitude and problem behaviors in adolescents: e mediating roles of positive and
negative coping styles. Front. Psychol. 10, 1547. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01547 (2019).
79. Webb, M. S., Whitmire, J. B., Hills, K. J. & Huebner, E. S. Gratitude buers against the eects of stressful life events on adolescents’
externalizing behavior but not internalizing behavior. Contemp. School Psychol. Adv. Online Publication. h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 1 0 0 7 / s
4 0 6 8 8 - 0 2 4 - 0 0 4 9 7 - 5 (2024).
80. Dodge, K. A., Lochman, J. E., Harnish, J. D., Bates, J. E. & Pettit, G. Reactive and proactive aggression in school children and
psychiatrically impaired chronically assaultive youth. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 106, 37–51. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.106.1.37
(1997).
81. Kruglanski, A. W. et al. Frustration–aggression hypothesis reconsidered: e role of signicance quest. Aggressive Behav. 49 (5),
445–468. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.22092 (2023).
82. BoggioPS, Giglio, A. C. A. et al. Writing about gratitude increases emotion-regulation ecacy. J. Posit. Psychol. 15 (6), 783–794.
https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2019.1651893 (2020).
83. Smith, P. K. et al. Cyberbullying: Its nature and impact in secondary school pupils. J. Child. Psychol. Psychiatry. 49 (4), 376–385.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01846.x (2008).
84. Salem, A. A. M. S. et al. Empathic skills training as a means of reducing cyberbullying among adolescents: An empirical evaluation.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public. Health. 20 (3), 1846. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20031846 (2023).
85. Dry burgh, N. S. J. & Vachon, D. D. Relating sex dierences in aggression to three forms of empathy. Pers. Indiv. Dier. 151, 109526.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2019.109526 (2019).
86. Oishi, S., Koo, M., Lim, N. & Suh, E. M. When gratitude evokes indebtedness. Appl. Psychology: Health Well-Being. 11 (2), 286–303.
https://doi.org/10.1111/aphw.12155 (2019).
87. Regan, A., Walsh, L. C. & Lyubomirsky, S. Are some ways of expressing gratitude more benecial than others?? Results from a
randomized controlled experiment. Aect. Sci. 4 (1), 72–81. https://doi.org/10.1007/s42761-022-00160-3 (2023).
88. Kumar, A. Some things aren’t better le unsaid: Interpersonal barriers to gratitude expression and prosocial engagement. Cur r.
Opin. Psychol. 43, 156–160. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2021.07.011 (2022).
89. Davis, D. E. et al. ankful for the little things: A meta-analysis of gratitude interventions. J. Couns. Psychol. 63 (1), 20–31. h t t p s : /
/ d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 1 0 3 7 / c o u 0 0 0 0 1 0 7 (2016).
90. Cregg, D. R. & Cheavens, J. S. Gratitude interventions: Eective self-help? A meta-analysis of the impact on symptoms of depression
and anxiety. J. Happiness Studies: Interdisciplinary Forum Subjective Well-Being. 22 (1), 413–445. h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 1 0 0 7 / s 1 0 9 0 2 - 0 2
0 - 0 0 2 3 6 - 6 (2021).
91. Swit, C. S. & Harty, S. C. Normative beliefs and aggression: e mediating roles of empathy and anger. Child. Psychiatry Human
Dev. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-023-01558-1 (2023).
92. Cantone, E. et al. Interventions on bullying and cyberbullying in schools: A systematic review. Clin. Practices Epidemiol. Mental
Health. 11 (Suppl 1 M4), 58–76. https://doi.org/10.2174/1745017901511010058 (2015).
Author contributions
K.T. and A.M-C. wrote the main manuscript text and all prepared gures and tables. All authors reviewed the
manuscript.
Declarations
Competing interests
e authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Supplementary Information e online version contains supplementary material available at h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1
0 . 1 0 3 8 / s 4 1 5 9 8 - 0 2 5 - 9 7 2 1 4 - w .
Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to M.-C.A.
Reprints and permissions information is available at www.nature.com/reprints.
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and
institutional aliations.
Open Access is article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives
4.0 International License, which permits any non-commercial use, sharing, distribution and reproduction in
any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide
a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if you modied the licensed material. You do not have
permission under this licence to share adapted material derived from this article or parts of it. e images or
other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated
otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence
and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to
obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit h t t p : / / c r e a t i v e c o m m o
n s . o r g / l i c e n s e s / b y - n c - n d / 4 . 0 / .
© e Author(s) 2025
Scientic Reports | (2025) 15:14602 15
| https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-025-97214-w
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
Terms and Conditions
Springer Nature journal content, brought to you courtesy of Springer Nature Customer Service Center GmbH (“Springer Nature”).
Springer Nature supports a reasonable amount of sharing of research papers by authors, subscribers and authorised users (“Users”), for small-
scale personal, non-commercial use provided that all copyright, trade and service marks and other proprietary notices are maintained. By
accessing, sharing, receiving or otherwise using the Springer Nature journal content you agree to these terms of use (“Terms”). For these
purposes, Springer Nature considers academic use (by researchers and students) to be non-commercial.
These Terms are supplementary and will apply in addition to any applicable website terms and conditions, a relevant site licence or a personal
subscription. These Terms will prevail over any conflict or ambiguity with regards to the relevant terms, a site licence or a personal subscription
(to the extent of the conflict or ambiguity only). For Creative Commons-licensed articles, the terms of the Creative Commons license used will
apply.
We collect and use personal data to provide access to the Springer Nature journal content. We may also use these personal data internally within
ResearchGate and Springer Nature and as agreed share it, in an anonymised way, for purposes of tracking, analysis and reporting. We will not
otherwise disclose your personal data outside the ResearchGate or the Springer Nature group of companies unless we have your permission as
detailed in the Privacy Policy.
While Users may use the Springer Nature journal content for small scale, personal non-commercial use, it is important to note that Users may
not:
use such content for the purpose of providing other users with access on a regular or large scale basis or as a means to circumvent access
control;
use such content where to do so would be considered a criminal or statutory offence in any jurisdiction, or gives rise to civil liability, or is
otherwise unlawful;
falsely or misleadingly imply or suggest endorsement, approval , sponsorship, or association unless explicitly agreed to by Springer Nature in
writing;
use bots or other automated methods to access the content or redirect messages
override any security feature or exclusionary protocol; or
share the content in order to create substitute for Springer Nature products or services or a systematic database of Springer Nature journal
content.
In line with the restriction against commercial use, Springer Nature does not permit the creation of a product or service that creates revenue,
royalties, rent or income from our content or its inclusion as part of a paid for service or for other commercial gain. Springer Nature journal
content cannot be used for inter-library loans and librarians may not upload Springer Nature journal content on a large scale into their, or any
other, institutional repository.
These terms of use are reviewed regularly and may be amended at any time. Springer Nature is not obligated to publish any information or
content on this website and may remove it or features or functionality at our sole discretion, at any time with or without notice. Springer Nature
may revoke this licence to you at any time and remove access to any copies of the Springer Nature journal content which have been saved.
To the fullest extent permitted by law, Springer Nature makes no warranties, representations or guarantees to Users, either express or implied
with respect to the Springer nature journal content and all parties disclaim and waive any implied warranties or warranties imposed by law,
including merchantability or fitness for any particular purpose.
Please note that these rights do not automatically extend to content, data or other material published by Springer Nature that may be licensed
from third parties.
If you would like to use or distribute our Springer Nature journal content to a wider audience or on a regular basis or in any other manner not
expressly permitted by these Terms, please contact Springer Nature at
onlineservice@springernature.com
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any citations for this publication.
Article
Full-text available
Most gratitude interventions for adolescents focus on private experiences of gratitude (e.g., gratitude journaling), dyadic expressions of gratitude (e.g., writing a gratitude letter to another person), or group-based psychoeducation about gratitude. By contrast, group interventions that emphasize gratitude social processes (GSPs)—interpersonal or group processes that involve or are directly triggered by the disclosure or expression of gratitude to other group members—provide an ideal forum for adolescents to reap the full benefits of gratitude experiences. In this perspective article, we propose a typology of five GSPs—disclosing, expressing, receiving, responding to, and witnessing gratitude in relation to other group members—that operate synergistically to produce positive effects for adolescents. In turn, we theorize that these GSPs likely produce superior outcomes, as compared to other gratitude interventions, through five psychosocial mechanisms of change: observational learning, group cohesion, vicarious gratitude, group-based gratitude, and collective gratitude. Overall, we encourage researchers and practitioners to incorporate GSPs in their gratitude interventions with adolescents.
Article
Full-text available
A sense of school belonging that tends to be low has an impact on problematic behavior at school, so effective psychological intervention is needed. One form of intervention carried out is gratitude intervention. This study aims to determine the effectiveness of gratitude interventions in increasing the sense of school belonging of high school students. This study uses a quantitative approach with the type of experimental research, and the design used is a pre-experimental one group pretest-posttest design. The research was conducted on students of SMA Negeri 5 Soppeng with 34 students. The determination of research participants was using the purposive sampling technique. Data were obtained by filling out the Psychological Sense of School Membership (PSSM) scale before and after the gratitude intervention. The validity of the PSSM scale was calculated using the Aiken's V test and obtained a coefficient of 0.88, while the reliability test used Cronbach's Alfa, with a reliability coefficient value of 0.86. The data analysis technique uses the N-Gain score test. The results showed that the average score of sense of school belonging before the intervention was 40.15, while after the intervention it increased to 73.5, the results of the hypothesis test using the difference index (N Gain Score) recorded an increase of 55.9. Thus, the hypothesis in this study is accepted, which means that gratitude interventions in schools are quite effective in increasing the sense of school belonging of high school students.
Article
Full-text available
This study aims to assist decision-making in anti-bullying interventions by highlighting the importance of positive factors such as gratitude, forgiveness, and self-regulation in mitigating the negative impacts of bullying/cyberbullying. The objective was to examine and synthesize available evidence on the impact of gratitude, forgiveness, and self-regulation practices in the school context regarding bullying/cyberbullying phenomena. Three databases were consulted (Web of Science, Scopus, and Scielo), and the results include 14 articles. The three character strengths were associated with psychological well-being, life and school satisfaction, improved mental health, increased likelihood of engaging in pro-social behavior, and reduced involvement in bullying/cyberbullying situations. These strengths have the potential to enhance overall well-being and decrease risk behaviors, leading to more positive outcomes in experiences of violence. These results underscore the importance of considering students’ individual strengths and the possible interventions to promote healthy school environments.
Article
Full-text available
The objective of the present study was to examine developmental trajectories of cyber-aggression in early adolescence, as well as their relationship with predictive factors related to cyber-aggression (e.g., overt aggression, gender, and time spent online). Participants were 384 adolescents from the Lower Mainland of British Columbia, Canada who were in grade six and grade seven at Time 1 of the study (192 boys, Mage = 13.62 years, SD = 0.74 year). Three years of longitudinal data on cyber-aggression, overt aggression, and time spent online were collected via online self-report questionnaires. Findings indicated three different trajectories of cyber-aggression: (a) a low-increasing (85.7% of the sample), (b) a stable trajectory (9.3% of the sample), and (c) a high-decreasing trajectory (4.9% of the sample). Adolescents who reported higher scores on overt aggression and spent more time online were more likely to be in the stable or high-decreasing groups. These findings highlight the importance of studying subgroups regarding the developmental course of cyber-aggression in early adolescence. The implications of present study findings give insight into gender differences and overt aggression among youth to inform cyber-aggression intervention and prevention.
Article
Full-text available
The identification of psychological strengths that foster healthy development in youth has become a major topic of exploration in the field of positive psychology. Gratitude is a trait-like characteristic with qualities indicative of a potential psychological strength that may serve as a protective factor for early adolescents in the face of stressful life events (SLEs). This two-wave longitudinal study utilized data from a sample of 830 middle school students from the Southeastern United States. Path analysis was employed to investigate gratitude’s role as a moderator in the relations between prior SLEs and early adolescents’ frequencies of externalizing and internalizing coping behaviors. The interaction between SLEs and gratitude significantly predicted early adolescents’ subsequent frequencies of externalizing behaviors, but not internalizing behaviors. The results provided support for gratitude as a key psychological strength in early adolescents. The results also implied the benefits of promoting youths’ gratitude in efforts to prevent externalizing behavior.
Article
Full-text available
The development of life skills is an important factor in the prevention of cyber-aggression. The most important skill is the ability of empathize. Empathy leads to greater sensitivity to other people, altruism, care, tolerance, and acceptance of differences. This ability is thus rightly desirable in a pluralistic cyberspace, allowing people to function together and express opinions without mutual vilification, humiliation, or abuse. The generation of “screenagers” is significantly threatened by modern socio-pathological phenomena. Emotional laxity and failure of emotional life are to blame for all of this. The study presents the results of research on the verification of the effectiveness of preventive activities aimed at the empathetic experience of pupils in the natural and digital environment. The research sample consisted of 30 pupils aged 9 to 10 years. Half of the pupils (N=15) represented the experimental group and the other half (N=15) the control group. The ratio of girls and boys was approximately equal. The research results showed that the experimental group reported a significantly higher level of empathy than the control group after completing the preventive activities (p = 0.04 > ? = 0.05). The preventive program used in our research included self-experience activities, which, according to our expectations, increased the ability of empathy in the experimental group. At the same time, our findings confirm previous research verifying the mutual correlation between the level of empathy and experiences with cyber-aggression.
Article
Full-text available
This study examined a two-mediator model with both empathy and anger as mediators in the association between children’s normative beliefs about aggression and forms (relational and physical) and functions (reactive and proactive) of aggressive behavior. Ninety-eight children (54% males, Mage=46.21months, SD = 8.84months) reported their approval of relationally and physically aggressive behaviors depicted in iconic (animation) and enactive (toy figurines) hypothetical scenarios. Children’s aggression, empathy and anger were measured using teacher reports. No main effects of normative beliefs about aggression on the corresponding aggressive behavior were found. Normative beliefs about aggression were negatively associated with empathy and empathy was significantly associated with relational aggression, suggesting that developing social emotional processes mediate the relation between social cognitions and aggression. Anger was associated with aggression, but not normative beliefs about aggression. The findings provide support for the distinction between subtypes of aggressive behavior in young children and the developing social-cognitive and affective processes that influence these behaviors.
Article
Full-text available
One of the oldest scientific theories of human aggression is the frustration-aggression hypothesis, advanced in 1939. Although this theory has received considerable empirical support and is alive and well today, its underlying mechanisms have not been adequately explored. In this article, we examine major findings and concepts from extant psychological research on hostile aggression and offer an integrative conception: aggression is a primordial means for establishing one's sense of significance and mattering, thus addressing a fundamental social-psychological need. Our functional portrayal of aggression as a means to significance yields four testable hypotheses: (1) frustration will elicit hostile aggression proportionately to the extent that the frustrated goal serves the individual's need for significance, (2) the impulse to aggress in response to significance loss will be enhanced in conditions that limit the individual's ability to reflect and engage in extensive information processing (that may bring up alternative, socially condoned means to significance), (3) significance-reducing frustration will elicit hostile aggression unless the impulse to aggress is substituted by a nonaggressive means of significance restoration, (4) apart from significance loss, an opportunity for significance gain can increase the impulse to aggress. These hypotheses are supported by extant data as well as novel research findings in real-world contexts. They have important implications for understanding human aggression and the conditions under which it is likely to be manifested and reduced.
Article
Abundant evidence has demonstrated a strong relationship between aggressive behaviour, one of the most common problems among students at every stage of education all over the world, and a trait for impulsiveness. Impulsiveness is also connected to diverse and enduring psychopathology. However, studies that tested burnout syndrome, impulsivity, and aggression are scarce. Accordingly, the current study examined the role of academic burnout and cross- cultural differences (Japanese vs European culture) in the association between trait impulsivity and online and offline aggression among university students (N = 291; Mage = 22.66 years; SD = 4.35; 31% males). The participants filled in self-report measures of Academic Burnout (MBI-SS); Impulsivity (BIS-15); Offline Aggression (Buss-Perry AQ), and Cyber-Aggression Types (CATQ). The results revealed that academic burnout, impulsivity, and both aggression indicators are positively interrelated. Moreover, the interaction effect between academic burnout and culture (Japanese vs European) was significant for almost all aggression characteristics. The significance of the interaction between impulsivity, academic burnout, and culture altogether was confirmed for offline aggression and the rage/cyber aggression type. The main implications and suggestions for future research are related to the importance of academic burnout and culture as crucial factors related either to offline aggression or cyber aggression levels.