ArticlePDF Available

How Does Problem Gambling Impact the Relationship Between Gambling Attitudes and Frequency?

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Individuals’ beliefs and perceptions about gambling are known to influence gambling behaviours. However, the associations between gambling attitudes, gambling frequency, and problem gambling are unclear within the existing literature. The study aimed to elucidate the relationship between gambling attitudes, gambling behaviour, and problem gambling, using responses to the 2018 Northern Territory Gambling Prevalence and Wellbeing Survey. Data from 1629 participants (Mage = 49.87 years; SD = 14.58 years; 51.63% female) who completed the Attitudes Towards Gambling Survey (ATGS), Problem Gambling Severity Index (PGSI), and reported their gambling frequency were analysed. Prior to testing for moderation of the attitude-frequency path by PGSI risk category, the measurement model of the ATGS was assessed for invariance and one item was removed to improve its psychometric properties. Problem gambling severity moderated the relationship between attitudes towards gambling and gambling frequency. The relationship between attitudes towards gambling and gambling behaviour strengthened at higher levels of problem gambling. Results indicate that the higher the risk of problem gambling, the stronger the influence of gambling attitudes on gambling frequency. These findings are discussed in relation to cognitive dissonance, rationalisation, gambling motivation, and the potential implications for problem gambling prevention strategies.
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
ORIGINAL PAPER
Accepted: 20 February 2025
© The Author(s) 2025
Mal Flack
malcolm.ack@cdu.edu.au
1 FacultyofHealth,CharlesDarwinUniversity,EllengowanDrive,Brinkin,NT0810,Australia
2 ResearchersinBehaviouralAddictions,Alcohol,andDrugs(BAAD),CharlesDarwin
University,EllengowanDrive,Brinkin,NT0810,Australia
How Does Problem Gambling Impact the Relationship
Between Gambling Attitudes and Frequency?
Kim M.Caudwell1,2· Angelica FernandezCasanova1· MalFlack1,2
Journal of Gambling Studies
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-025-10379-x
Abstract
Individuals’beliefsand perceptionsabout gamblingare knowntoinuencegamblingbe-
haviours.However,theassociationsbetweengamblingattitudes,gamblingfrequency,and
problemgambling areunclear withintheexistingliterature.Thestudy aimedtoelucidate
therelationship betweengamblingattitudes, gamblingbehaviour,andproblem gambling,
usingresponsestothe2018 NorthernTerritoryGamblingPrevalenceandWellbeingSur-
vey.Datafrom1629 participants(Mage=49.87years; SD=14.58years;51.63% female)
whocompletedtheAttitudes TowardsGamblingSurvey(ATGS),ProblemGamblingSe-
verityIndex(PGSI),andreportedtheirgamblingfrequencywereanalysed.Priortotesting
for moderation of the attitude-frequency path by PGSI risk category, the measurement
modeloftheATGSwasassessedforinvarianceandoneitemwasremovedtoimproveits
psychometric properties. Problem gambling severity moderated the relationship between
attitudes towards gambling and gambling frequency.The relationship between attitudes
towardsgamblingandgamblingbehaviour strengthenedathigherlevelsof problemgam-
bling. Results indicate that the higher the risk of problem gambling, the stronger the
inuence of gambling attitudes on gambling frequency.These ndings are discussed in
relation to cognitive dissonance, rationalisation, gambling motivation, and the potential
implicationsfor problemgamblingprevention strategies.
Keywords Gambling·Attitudes·Attitudes towardsgamblingscale·Problem
gambling· Problemgamblingseverity index· Gamblingfrequency
Gamblingisa popular activityamongAustralians,with38% ofAustralian adults report-
inggamblingatleast once a week(AustralianGamblingResearch Centre, 2023). While
atthepopulation level, mostgamblerswill not experience signicantimpactsfrom their
gambling,others–includingfamiliesand communitymembers-mayexperience negative
1 3
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Journal of Gambling Studies
eects(Goodwinetal.,2017;Langhametal.,2016).Assuch,thetermproblem gambling
isusedtorefertogamblingthatleadstonegativeconsequencesandfunctionaldisruptions
(Delfabbro,2013).Importantly,problemgamblingisnotexclusivetothoseexhibitingclini-
calsymptoms;itisestimatedthattwiceasmanypeopleexperiencenegativeconsequences
fromtheirgamblingactivitiesthanmeetclinicalthresholdsforgamblingdisorder(Austra-
lianGovernmentProductivityCommission,2010).Assuch,gambling-related harmisused
toreferto the negative consequencesofgambling activities which couldbeexperienced
morebroadly–bythegamblerthemselves,butalsobythoseconnectedtothem(Delfabbro
& King, 2019; Goodwin et al., 2017).Accordingly, gambling-related harm has attracted
considerableattentionfrom researchersattemptingto quantify theimpactof gambling at
populationlevels,toshapepoliciesandinterventionsthatcanreduceproblemgamblingand
relatedharm(Browneetal.,2021;Goodwinetal.,2017).
Attitudes Towards Gambling
Attitudes(i.e.,evaluativebeliefs)towardbehavioursareinuentialdeterminantsofbehav-
iouralengagement, andtherefore areoften primarytargetsinattemptstochangepeople’s
behaviour (Ajzen et al., 2018). Indeed, gambling advertising attempts to foster positive
attitudestowardsgambling,bynormalising,glamorising,andincentivisinggambling(Bou-
guettaya et al., 2020). On the other hand, public health and regulatory approaches may
fosterlesspositiveormorenegativebeliefsaboutgambling,byconveyingtheriskofharm
(Muñozetal.,2013).Givenattitudechangeconferssmalltomediumeectsinrelationto
healthbehaviourchange(Sheeranetal.,2016),andpromptshelp-seekingbehaviour(Rosen
etal., 2020),interventions maybringaboutconsiderablereductionsinproblem gambling
atthepopulationlevel.Similarly,attitudes towardgamblingareoftenusedasa proxyfor
publicsupportofgamblingregulationandsupportprograms(Donaldsonetal.,2016).
Generally speaking, higher positive gambling attitudes correspond with higher gam-
blingengagement(e.g.,Canaleetal.,2016;Flack&Morris,2017;Kristensenetal.,2023;
McAllister,2014).Similarly,longitudinalresearchamongadolescentshasshowngambling
attitudesbecomemorepositiveasgamblingparticipationincreases(Pallesenetal.,2016).
However,therelationshipbetweenattitudesandproblemgamblingappearsmorecomplex,
withsomestudiesreporting positive associations (e.g.,Andrà et al., 2022; Canale etal.,
2016; Zhou et al., 2019), negative associations (Donaldson et al., 2016; Dowling et al.,
2021),ornoassociationatall(Dixonetal.,2016).Whiletherearevariousattitudinalmea-
suresof gamblingthroughoutthe literature,perhaps themostwidely usedmeasure isthe
AttitudeTowardsGamblingScale (ATGS;Wardle, 2007).Arecent systematic reviewby
Kristensen et al. (2023) reviewed of the use of theATGSacross the available gambling
literature,conrming thatattitudesandgamblingfrequencyarepositivelyassociated,and
observingmixedndings inrelationto associations betweenattitudesand problemgam-
bling.Theauthorsobservethatthoseexperiencinggamblingproblemsarelikelytobemore
frequentgamblers,andmorefrequentgamblersendorsemorepositive gamblingattitudes.
Althoughthesendingsareinformative,thereviewwasnotabletodeterminethestrength
orthevariabilityofthepositiverelationshipsbetweenattitudesandgamblingfrequencyor
probewhetherproblemgamblingseverityinuencedthisrelationship.
1 3
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Journal of Gambling Studies
Elucidatingtheroleofproblemgamblingintheattitude-frequencyrelationshipmayhelp
toinformpersuasion-basedattitudinalchangeinpublichealthandclinicalsettings,reducing
gambling-relatedharmandtheincidenceofproblemgambling.Notably,arecentmeta-anal-
ysisofglobalgamblingparticipationhasestimated8.7%ofadultsengageinany-riskgam-
bling(i.e.,engagementinanylevelofgamblingriskbehaviours),withaproportionof1.4%
engagingin problematicgambling(Tranet al.,2024). Givengambling-relatedmessaging
isintendedtopromotesafergamblingatthepopulationlevel,itisimportanttoconsiderthe
eectofgamblingattitudesongamblingfrequencyinrelation toproblemgambling(e.g.,
Newalletal.,2024).Forinstance,arecentmeta-analysisbyJoyal-Desmaraisetal.(2022)
outlineshowthepersuasivenessofmessagingisdependentonthecongruenceofthemes-
sagewithaperson’sunderlyingbeliefs.Specically,positivematchingincreasestheeec-
tivenessof persuasion(byanaverageeect sizeof r=.20),negatively matchedmessages
mayunderminepersuasiveeectsinrelationtoattitudeandsubsequentbehaviourchange–
orleadthem tobackre. Indeed,“boomerangeects” havebeenobserved inresponsible
gamblingmessagingforproblemgamblersspecically,wheremessagesappeartoincrease
normativeperceptionsofgambling(DeJansetal.,2023).Forinstance,aresponsiblegam-
blingmessagethatsuggestsothersarealsoexperiencinggambling-relatedharmmayleada
problemgamblertoassumetheirownexperiencesarecommon.
Thepresentstudythereforeaimedtoascertaintheextenttowhichthegamblingattitude-
frequencyrelationshipwasdependentonthelevelofproblemgambling,usingalarge-scale
sample.
Methods
Procedure
AsecondarydataanalysiswasconductedusingdatafromtheNorthernTerritory(NT)2018
GamblingPrevalenceandWellbeingSurvey(Stevensetal.,2019).Theoriginalsurveycol-
lecteddatafrom5000NTresidents,aged18andover,viaphoneinterviewusingarandom
selectionof phonenumbers. Priortoanalysingtherelationshipbetweenthevariablesand
testingthe moderationmodel,participants wereexcluded iftheyhadnotreported engag-
ing in gambling activities over the last year, or did not complete the problem gambling
screen.Analyseswere conductedusing IBMSPSS Statistics29.0. Missingvalue analysis
revealed<5%oftheattitudemeasuredataweremissing,withLittle’sMCARtestnon-sig-
nicant:χ2(269)=267.26,p=.519.Multipleimputationwasthereforeusedformissingval-
ues,usinganExpectation-Maximisation(EM)approach,leadingtoasampleofN=1629.
Measures
Problem Gambling
TheProblemGamblingSeverityScale(PGSI)fromtheCanadianProblemGamblingScale
(Ferris&Wynne,2001)wasusedtomeasureproblemgambling.Nineitemsarescoredon
afour-pointLikertscale(0=never1=sometimes,2=mostofthetime,3=almostalways),
withfouritemsreferringtoproblemgamblingbehaviours(e.g.,Howoftenhavedoyoubet
1 3
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Journal of Gambling Studies
morethan youcanaordtolose? )and theothervereferringtonegativeconsequences
ofgambling(e.g.,howoftenhasyourgamblingcausedyouanyhealthproblems,including
stressoranxiety?),withthetotalscorecommonlyusedtoassessproblemgamblingsever-
ity(Tsengetal.,2023).ThePGSIscoresalsoallowforclassifyingparticipantsintodier-
entriskcategories:0=norisk,1–2=lowrisk,3–7=moderaterisk,≥8=problemgambling
(Ferris&Wynne,2001).
Attitudes Towards Gambling
Attitudesweremeasuredwiththe8-itemversionoftheAttitudesTowardsGamblingScale
(ATGS-8; Canale et al., 2016). TheATGS-8 is the shorter form of the original 14-item
scale,which wasdesignedtogainabroadideaofthe attitudestowards gamblingin gen-
eralandtoassesstheopinionsongamblinginthegeneralcommunity(Wardle,2007).The
ATGS-8usesave-pointLikertscalewhereeachitemisendorsedfromstronglyagree(5)
to strongly disagree (1). Four items are phrased to expressed negative attitudes towards
gambling(e.g.,gamblingshouldbediscouraged)andtheotherfourphrasedtoexpressposi-
tiveattitudestowardsgambling(e.g.,gamblinglivensuplife);thelatteritemsarerecoded,
meaninghigherscoresindicatemorepositiveattitudes.
Gambling Frequency
Participantsindicatedthe number of timestheyparticipatedin lotteries, bingo,racetrack
betting,casinotable games,sportsbetting,raes/sweeps,keno,pokies,instantscratchies,
informal(e.g.,card-based)andnon-sportseventbetting(e.g.,awards),andothergambling
withinthe previous12 months.The responseprovided foreachactivitywasconvertedto
numberofdaysperyear,andgamblingfrequencywascalculatedbysummingthenumber
oftimesapersonengagedinalltheactivitiesoverthepreviousyear.
Results
Participants
Participants(Mage=49.87years,SDage=14.58years;51.63%female)wereresidentsofthe
NorthernTerritory,with16.82%identifyingasAboriginalorTorresStraitIslanders,andthe
majorityof theparticipants (94.17%)reportingspeakingEnglishastheirhousehold main
language.Statistics for12-month frequencybygamblingtype,acrossage,sex,andPGSI
riskstatusareincludedinTable1.
Preliminary Analyses
Thefactor structureof theATGS-8was initiallyexamined toensure consistencybetween
thoseexhibitingnoriskofproblemgambling(i.e.,PGSI=0),tothosewithanyriskofprob-
lemgambling (i.e.,PGSI≥1),consistentwithpreviousresearch(Donaldson etal., 2016).
AninvariancetestingapproachwasundertakenusingconrmatoryfactoranalysesinIBM
SPSSAMOS v. 28, involving testing successive restrictions on the measurement model
1 3
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Journal of Gambling Studies
Table 1 Descriptivestatisticsforgamblingfrequencybytype,acrossage,sex,andPGSIrisk(i.e.,PGSI<1,PGSI≥1)
Variable Category nPokies Racing Scratchies Keno Lotto Bingo Casinogames Sportsbetting
M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD
Agerange 18–34 274 15.54 46.42 30.63 84.85 7.35 11.67 17.24 46.81 31.53 212.81 9.11 16.53 5.34 7.41 23.82 40.14
35–49 499 17.57 31.98 18.90 43.58 10.20 34.90 22.67 52.48 23.34 34.69 13.30 34.33 3.82 6.58 22.16 36.80
50–64 596 24.26 58.75 31.89 67.38 12.64 18.62 25.92 47.65 35.50 128.35 27.58 28.83 9.25 18.77 28.23 40.13
65+ 260 25.97 35.58 34.19 83.20 28.72 38.54 30.55 50.76 36.99 42.30 43.44 54.52 2.88 3.72 11.20 11.22
Sex Male 788 23.77 53.36 42.09 80.31 16.09 37.42 30.17 56.95 35.54 117.50 9.00 19.05 6.85 12.36 25.45 37.36
Female 841 18.58 39.75 7.62 28.96 11.34 19.93 18.06 39.46 27.77 114.04 23.40 37.04 2.33 2.64 14.74 38.99
PGSIRisk Norisk 1115 15.97 37.89 19.33 50.75 13.11 23.80 20.38 40.65 31.36 138.02 20.33 33.24 4.52 11.58 23.01 36.04
Atrisk 514 25.95 53.30 39.35 81.92 13.45 33.48 30.41 60.39 32.18 45.02 24.29 40.38 6.34 9.71 24.05 39.34
Note.Frequenciesforinformalbetting,non-sportseventbetting,and‘other’gamblingareexcludedduetolowf requenciesacrosscategories
1 3
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Journal of Gambling Studies
(i.e.,congural,metric, and scalarinvariance),and assessingforreductions in modelt.
Theadoptedthreshold forinvariancewaswheretheassociateddecrementinCFIwasless
than0.010,andinRMSEAlessthan0.015(Chen,2007).Whenassessingscalarinvariance,
highstandardisedresidualcovariancesimplicatedtheATGS-8item7(i.e.,“gamblinglivens
uplife”),withat-testconrmingthatmeanscoresonthisitemwerestatisticallysignicant
fortheriskcategories(p<.001).Theinvariancetestwasconductedwiththeitemremoved,
andthemodied scale (i.e.,“ATGS-7”)was tested formodelt and invariance,deemed
acceptable1andusedinsubsequentanalyses.ModelresultsforbothATG-8andATG-7are
includedinTable2.
Descriptive statistics and correlations are shown in Table 3. Overall, the respondents
reported unfavourable attitudes towards gambling (M=21.01, SD=5.12).A correlational
analysis indicated a positive relationship between gambling attitude and gambling fre-
quency (r=.16, p<.001); no signicant correlation between PGSI score an ATG7 was
observed(p=.899).
Moderation Analyses
Totestthehypothesisthatproblemgamblingwouldmoderatetherelationshipbetweenatti-
tudestowardsgamblingandgamblingfrequency,thePROCESSv4.3Macro(Hayes,2018)
wasused(i.e.,Model1),withthePGSItotalscoreasthemoderatingvariable,andageand
genderascovariates.AsthePGSIcanbeusedtodierentiatelevelsofrisk,Helmertcod-
ingwasusedtoprobetheinteractionatthenorisk(n=1115),lowrisk(n=342),moderate
risk(n=131)andatrisk(n=41)categories,tofacilitatecomparisonofthemeansforeach
groupwiththoseordinallyhigher(Hayes&Montoya,2017).Overall,themodelaccounted
for10.87%ofthevarianceingamblingfrequency,F(9,1604)=21.74,p<.001.Resultsof
theHelmertapproachtoprobingthemoderationeectareincludedinTable4,withavisual
depictionoftheinteractionincludedinFig.1.
Discussion
Thecurrentstudyattemptedto furtherelucidatetheassociationbetweenattitudestogam-
bling and gambling frequency,taking into account the experience of problem gambling.
Specically,thestudyaimed totestthe hypothesisthatproblem gambling wouldmoder-
atetheeect ofattitudeson gambling frequency.Resultsindicatedtwo key ndings:(1)
thepositiveassociationbetweenattitudesandfrequencywereampliedinhigherproblem
gamblingriskcategories, whiletherelationship between attitudesandproblem gambling
was not signicant; (2) that when comparing no risk to any risk problem gamblers, the
ATGS-8appearsnon-invariant.Thesendingsrequirefurtherdiscussionandillustratethe
importanceofconsideringproblem gambling in relationtothemeasurement of attitudes
andwork toestablishassociations betweenattitudes andgamblingfrequency (Kristensen
etal.,2023).
1 Scalar invariance was essentially conrmed; though the CFI decreased slightly,the RMSEA, which is
arguablymoresensitivetononinvariantmodelsinlargersamples(i.e.,N>300;Chen,2007),remainedwell
withintherecommendedthreshold.
1 3
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Journal of Gambling Studies
Thending thatattitudes towardsgambling areassociated withgambling frequencyis
consistent with previous research (Canale et al., 2016; Flack & Morris, 2017; McAllis-
ter,2014).Whilethese ndings supportthe notion thatchanginggambling attitudesmay
changegamblingfrequency,thepresenceofproblemgamblingasamoderatorindicatesthat
attitude-behaviour relationships should not be investigated without considering problem
gamblingseverity.
Therearearangeofplausibleinterpretationsforthismoderationeect.Oneexampleis
cognitive dissonance(Festinger &Carlsmith,1959)–thedistress experiencedby anindi-
vidualwhentheirbeliefsandbehavioursareincongruent.Cognitivedissonanceisthought
tocreateamotivationalimpetustochangeone’sattitudesorbehaviourtoalleviatedistress
Table 2 MeasurementmodelinvariancetestscomparingthePGSI‘norisk’categorysubsampletothePGSI
‘anyrisk’subsamplefortheATGS-8andATGS-7
Sample/Invariance χ2df CFI RMSEA ∆CFI ∆RMSEA
ATGS-8 Fullsample(n=1629) 197.68 20 0.939 0.065 - -
Norisk(n= 11 1 5 ) 154.5 20 0.937 0.078 - -
Anyrisk(n=514) 69.51 20 0.937 0.069 - -
Conguralinvariance 224.01 40 0.937 0.053 - -
Metric 228.99 47 0.938 0.049 0.001 0.004
Scalar 293.85 55 0.919 0.052 0.019 0.003
ATGS-7 Fullsample 107.13 14 0.961 0.064 - -
Norisk 78.57 14 0.964 0.064 - -
Anyrisk 44.58 14 0.951 0.065 - -
Congural 123.51 28 0.961 0.046 - -
Metric 127.27 34 0.961 0.041 <0.001
Scalar 140.04 41 0.959 0.039 0.003
Note. ATGS-7=Attit udes Towards Gambling Survey 7-item; PGSI=Problem Gambli ng Severity
Index. Cr iteria for model t: CF I>0.95, RMSEA<0.08. Crite ria for invaria nce testing, CFI<−0.01;
∆RMSEA<−0.015(Chen,2007)
Table 3 Descriptivestatisticsandcorrelationsbetweenvariablesforthesample
Variable Min Max MSD 1 2 3 4
1.ATG7 7 35 18.54 4.54 0.77
2.PGSI 0 22 0.88 2.14 −0.02 0.81
3.Frequency 12706 59.10 129.31 0.16** 0.26** -
4.Age 18 92 49.87 14.58 0.02 −0.10** 0.08* -
5.Gender - - −0.19** −0.07* −0.12** −0.03
Note.ATGS-7=AttitudesTowardsGambling Survey 7-item; PGSI=Problem GamblingSeverityIndex.
Genderwascoded1(male)and2(female).*p<.01.**p<.001.Cronbach’sαvaluesareincludedalongthe
principaldiagonal
Contrast(PGSIScore) b SE t p95%CI
0vs.≥1 8.90 1.98 4.50 <0.001 [5.01,12.79]
1–2 vs. ≥ 3 9.24 3.01 3.07 0.002 [3.34,15.14]
3 to 7 vs. ≥ 8 13.12 5.21 2.52 0.012 [2.90,23.35]
Note.R2= 0 . 11 , F(9,1604)=21.58,p= . 0 0 2
Table 4 Resultsofhelmertcon-
trastsforthemoderatingeectof
PGSIriskcategoryongambling
Attitude-Frequencypath
1 3
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Journal of Gambling Studies
(Cooper, 2019), which may facilitate avoiding disconrming information, and engaging
in motivated reasoning (e.g., Gillespie, 2020). Potentially,as people with positive gam-
bling attitudes experience gambling-related harm, instead of gambling less frequently,
theymight rationalisetheir gamblingexperiences tomaintain overallfavourable attitudes
towardsgambling.Indeed,rationalisationhasbeennotedasaformofdissonancemanage-
mentinqualitativeresearchwithEGMplayers(Hahmann&Monson,2021).Similarly,the
tendencytorationalisecontinuedgamblingdespiteaccruing mountinglosses mayalsobe
partly explained by the motives or expectancies underlying gambling, which are known
to dier between non-problem and problem gamblers. For instance, gambling to escape
or manage undesired emotional states is consistently associated with problem gambling
(Alaba-Ekpoetal.,2024;Leeetal.,2024).
Thoughspeculative,theexperienceofcognitivedissonanceandrationalisingcontinued
gamblingduetotheanticipatedemotionalbenetsmaymaintainpositiveattitudesathigher
levelsof problemgambling riskand isworthexploringfurtherinpublichealthandclini-
calcontexts.Whentheimmediateperceivedbenetsofgamblingoutweighthe perceived
losses,positiveattitudesmayremainentrenchedandresistanttoconventionalharmreduc-
tionmessaging(DeJansetal.,2023;Delfabbro&King,2020).Strategiesaimedatchang-
ing attitudes may do well to induce dissonance (e.g., Wan & Chiou, 2010), and educate
Fig. 1 PlotofregressionslopesatdierentPGSIriskcategories
1 3
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Journal of Gambling Studies
gamblersonthetendencyto resolve cognitive dissonance through rationalisation,which
maybedeliveredaspartofacompletementoftechniquesthatconstitutejust-in-timeadap-
tiveinterventionsthatincorporatedynamictailoring(Dowlingetal.,2023).
A dierent, but equally important nding, was that the ATGS-8 was not invariant
betweenPGSIno riskandrisk categories.Thiswas unanticipatedandhas to theauthors
knowledgenotbeenformallyexplored,andwarrantssomeconsideration.Thattheremoved
item(“gamblinglivensuplife”)improvedthetoftheATGSimpliesitissomewhatcon-
ceptuallydistinctfromotheritems(i.e.,thatarelargelyframedaroundtheextenttowhich
gamblingshouldbe controlled,orwhetherornotitisgoodorbadoverall).Itmaybethat
individualsatanylevelofriskforproblemgamblingmayinterpretanitemframedaround
improvinglifeakintoamotiveoroutcomeexpectancy;suchasanticipationthatgambling
willalleviatenegativeaect(Caudwelletal.,2024),orotherwisemanagedisorderedmood
symptoms(Vaughan&Flack,2022).WhiletheATGS-8hasbeenfoundtobegenderinvari-
ant,this study’sndingsindicateitsstructureshouldbeinvestigatedinrelevantcontexts,
especiallywithrespecttogamblingmodalityandmotives(Richardson etal.,2023). More
targetedresearchinthisspacewouldallowfortherenementoftheATGSandimproveits
relevanceandinterpretationinavarietyofresearchcontexts.
Giventheinconsistenciesingamblingattitude-problem gamblingcorrelationsthrough-
outtheextant literature(Kristensenet al.,2023),the present ndingsagainhighlight the
importance of considering contextual elements that may inuence the formation of atti-
tudes,engagementwithgamblingbehaviours,andexperienceofproblemgamblingsever-
ity,andconclusionsabouttheirinterrelationships.Forinstance,researchinyoungersamples
(e.g.,Dixonetal.,2016)mayintroduceooreectswhereindividuals arerestrictedfrom
engaginginhigherriskformsofgambling);similarly,universitystudentsamplesappearto
holdmorenegativeattitudestowardsgamblingandbeatlowerriskofgamblingproblems
thanthoseinthegeneralpopulation(Gainsburyetal.,2014).Ofnotetoo,isthatpublicly-
sampledattitudestoward gamblingmaybe generallynegativewhen participantsconate
gamblingwithproblem gambling(Delfabbro&King,2021).Thisisespeciallyimportant,
giventhe roleoftheATGSincapturingattitudesthatmaychangeasaresultof policyor
publicscrutinyongamblingregulation(e.g.,Donaldsonetal.,2016).Futureresearchmay
alsoneed toinvestigateattitude-frequency associationsinpurposive, higherrisk contexts
(e.g.,treatment-seekingsamples)orsubsamplesfrompopulation-basedresearch(e.g.,age
groups,gender),orwithfocusonpreferredgamblingtype,andpursuelongitudinaldesigns
thatmay complementthepathways approachtoproblemgambling(Billieux etal.,2022;
Noweretal.,2022).Suchresearchcaninformattitudeandbehaviourchangeinterventions
thatmaybedependentonproblemgamblingstatus.
Limitations and Future Research
Whilethisstudymakesanimportantcontributiontotheattitude-gamblingfrequencylitera-
ture,somelimitationswarrantconsideration.Firstly,thescaleofprevalencestudiesnecessi-
tatestheuseofself-reportmeasures,yetintroducesissuesinrelationtoparticipantaccuracy
of reported gambling behaviours and problem gambling retrospectively (i.e., recalling
behaviouroverthelast12months).Secondly,thestudy’scross-sectionaldesignprecludes
denitivestatementsaboutcausality.Futureresearchshouldadoptlongitudinaldesignsto
consolidateunderstandingofthepatternsofgamblingattitudedevelopment,andinuence
ongamblingbehaviour,overtime.Finally,thestudy’sndingsarebasedonapopulation-
1 3
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Journal of Gambling Studies
basedsurveyoftheNorthernTerritoryofAustralia,whichareundoubtedlyshapedbyspe-
ciccultural andlegislativedynamics,whichlimitsthegeneralisability ofthe ndingsto
other contexts. Future research, especially that involving large-scale prevalence studies,
shouldattempttoreplicatethendings–particularlythoseinrelationtheattitudemeasure-
mentinvariance–inothernationalandculturalcontexts.
Conclusion
Thepresentstudyshowsthattheinvestigationoftheassociationbetweengamblingattitudes
andfrequencyneedstoconsiderproblemgamblingrisk.Specically,theattitudesofthose
who are in higher risk problem gambling categories appear to have a more pronounced
eectongamblingfrequency.Thendingsofthisstudycarryimportantimplicationsforthe
measurementofgamblingattitudesindierentcontexts,especiallyinrelationtothemea-
surementofattitudesinproblemgamblers.Theyalsoindicatethat attitudechangeamong
problemgamblersmayneedtoaordagreaterroletocognitivedissonanceandmotivation
forgambling,andthepropensityofproblemgamblers torationalisetheirexperiencesand
maintainoverallpositiveattitudestowardgambling.
Funding OpenAccessfundingenabledandorganizedbyCAULanditsMemberInstitutions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License,
whichpermits use,sharing, adaptation,distributionandreproductionin anymedium orformat,aslongas
yougiveappropriatecredittotheoriginalauthor(s)andthesource,providealinktotheCreativeCommons
licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are
includedinthearticle’sCreativeCommonslicence,unlessindicatedotherwiseinacreditlinetothematerial.
Ifmaterialisnotincludedinthearticle’sCreativeCommonslicenceandyourintendeduseisnotpermitted
bystatutory regulationor exceedsthe permitteduse, youwill needto obtainpermission directlyfrom the
copyrightholder.Toviewacopyofthislicence,visithttp://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
References
Ajzen,I.,Fishbein,M.,Lohmann,S.,&Albarracin,D.(2018). Theinuenceof attitudesonbehavior.The
handbook of attitudes, volume 1: Basic principles(Vol.1).Routledge.
Alaba-Ekpo,O.,Caudwell,K.M.,&Flack,M.(2024).Examiningthe strengthofthe associationbetween
problemgamblingandgamblingtoescape.AsystematicreviewandMeta-Analysis.International Jour-
nal of Mental Health and Addiction. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-024-01354-5
Andrà,C.,Priolo,G.,Merlin,F.,&Chiavarino,C.(2022).Dierencesinperceivedandexperiencedstigma
betweenproblematicgamblersandNon-gamblersinageneralpopulationsurvey.Journal of Gambling
Studies,38(2),333–351.https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-021-10048-9
AustralianGamblingResearch Centre(2023).Gambling participation, experience of harm and community
views. h t t p s : / / a i f s . g o v . a u / r e s e a r c h / r e s e a r c h - s n a p s h o t s / g a m b l i n g - p a r t i c i p a t i o n - e x p e r i e n c e - h a r m - a n d - c o
m m u n i t y - v i e w s
AustralianGovernmentProductivityCommission(2010).Gambling. h t t p s : / / w w w . p c . g o v . a u / i n q u i r i e s / c o m p
l e t e d / g a m b l i n g - 2 0 1 0 / r e p o r t
Billieux,J.,Bonnaire,C.,Bowden-Jones,H.,&Clark,L.(2022).CommentaryonNoweretal:Thepathways
modelshouldapplytonon-clinicalgamblingpatterns.Addiction,117 (7),2011–2012.h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1
0 . 1 1 1 1 / a d d . 1 5 8 6 0
Bouguettaya,A.,Lynott,D.,Carter,A.,Zerhouni,O.,Meyer,S.,Ladegaard,I.,Gardner,J.,&O’Brien,K.S.
(2020).Therelationshipbetweengambling advertisingandgamblingattitudes,intentions andbehav-
iours:Acriticalandmeta-analyticreview.Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences,31,89–101.h t t p s : /
/ d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 1 0 1 6 / j . c o b e h a . 2 0 2 0 . 0 2 . 0 1 0
1 3
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Journal of Gambling Studies
Browne,M.,Rawat,V.,Tulloch,C.,Murray-Boyle,C.,&Rocklo,M.(2021).TheevolutionofGambling-
Related harm measurement: Lessons from the last decade. International Journal of Environmental
Research and Public Health,18(9),4395.https://www.mdpi.com/1660-4601/18/9/4395
Canale,N.,Vieno,A.,Pastore,M.,Ghisi, M.,&Griths,M.D. (2016).Validationofthe8-item attitudes
towards gambling scale (ATGS-8) in a British population survey. Addictive Behaviors, 54, 70–74.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2015.12.009
Caudwell,K.M.,Bacovic,I.,&Flack,M.(2024).Whatroledomaladaptivecopingandescapeexpectancies
playinthe relationshipbetweenstressandproblemgambling?Testingamoderatedmediationmodel.
International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-023-01238-0
Chen,F.F.(2007).Sensitivityofgoodnessoftindexestolackofmeasurementinvariance.Structural Equation
Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal,14(3),464–504.https://doi.org/10.1080/10705510701301834
Cooper,J.(2019).Cognitivedissonance:whereWe’vebeenandwherewe’regoing.International Review of
Social Psychology,32(1),7.https://doi.org/10.5334/irsp.277
DeJans,S.,Cauberghe,V.,Hudders, L.,&Rys,F.(2023).Anexperimental studytoexamine whetherand
howFlemishand Dutchharm prevention messageson gamblingadvertisingaect consumers’gam-
bling-relatedbeliefsand intentions.Psychology of Addictive Behaviors,37(6),771–784. h t t p s : / / d o i . o r
g/10.1037/adb0000951
Delfabbro,P.(2013).Problemandpathologicalgambling:Aconceptualreview.Journal of Gambling Busi-
ness & Economics,7(3),35–53.https://doi.org/10.5750/jgbe.v7i3.817
Delfabbro,P.,&King,D.L.(2019).ChallengesintheconceptualisationandmeasurementofGambling-Related
harm.Journal of Gambling Studies,35(3),743–755.https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-019-09844-1
Delfabbro,P.,&King,D.L.(2020).Onthelimitsandchallengesofpublichealthapproachesinaddressing
Gambling-Relatedproblems. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction,18(3), 844–859.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-020-00276-2
Delfabbro, P. H., & King, D. L. (2021). The behaviour-attitude divide: Understanding public attitudes
towardsgambling. International Gambling Studies, 21(1),153–167. h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 1 0 8 0 / 1 4 4 5 9 7 9 
5 . 2 0 2 0 . 1 8 1 9 3 6 6
Dixon,R.W.,Youssef,G.J.,Hasking,P.,Yücel,M.,Jackson,A.C.,&Dowling,N.A.(2016).Therelation-
shipbetweengamblingattitudes,involvement,andproblemsinadolescence:Examiningthemoderating
roleofcoping strategiesandparenting styles.Addictive Behaviors,58,42–46.h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 1 0 1 6
/ j . a d d b e h . 2 0 1 6 . 0 2 . 0 1 1
Donaldson, P., Rocklo, M. J., Browne, M., Sorenson, C. M., Langham, E., & Li, E. (2016). Attitudes
towards gambling and gambling reform inAustralia. Journal of Gambling Studies, 32(1), 243–259.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-015-9529-y
Dowling,N.A.,Aarsman, S.R., &Merkouris,S.S.(2021). Risk,compensatory,and protectivefactorsin
problemgambling:Theroleofpositivementalhealthcharacteristics.Addictive Behaviors,112 ,106604.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2020.106604
Dowling,N.A.,Rodda,S.N.,&Merkouris,S.S. (2023).Applyingthe just-in-timeadaptive intervention
frameworktothedevelopmentofgamblinginterventions.Journal of Gambling Studies. h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g
/10.1007/s10899-023-10250-x
Ferris,J.A.,&Wynne,H.J.(2001).Canadiancentreonsubstanceabuse.
Festinger, L., & Carlsmith, J. M. (1959). Cognitive consequences of forced compliance.The Journal of
Abnormal and Social Psychology,58(2),203–210.https://doi.org/10.1037/h0041593
Flack,M.,& Morris,M.(2017). Gambling-relatedbeliefsand gamblingbehaviour: Explaining gambling
problemswiththetheoryofplannedbehaviour.International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction,
15(1),130–142.https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-015-9611-9
Gainsbury,S.M.,Russell,A.,&Blaszczynski,A.(2014).Arepsychologyuniversitystudentgamblersrep-
resentativeofNon-universitystudentsandgeneralgamblers?Acomparativeanalysis.Journal of Gam-
bling Studies,30(1),11–25.https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-012-9334-9
Gillespie,A.(2020).Disruption,self-presentation,anddefensivetacticsatthethresholdoflearning.Review
of General Psychology,24(4),382–396.https://doi.org/10.1177/1089268020914258
Goodwin,B. C.,Browne,M.,Rocklo,M., &Rose, J.(2017).Atypical problemgambleraectssixoth-
ers.International Gambling Studies,17(2),276–289.h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 1 0 8 0 / 1 4 4 5 9 7 9 5 . 2 0 1 7 . 1 3 3 1 2 5 2
Hahmann,T.,&Monson,E.(2021).Rationalizationasadissonancemanagementstrategyamongelectronic
gamblingmachineplayers.Critical Gambling Studies,2(1),76–86.h t t p s : / / c r i t i c a l g a m b l i n g s t u d i e s . c o m
/ i n d e x . p h p / c g s / a r t i c l e / v i e w / 3 2 / 4 2
Hayes, A. F. (2018). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis (2 ed.).
Guildford.
Hayes,A.F.,&Montoya,A.K.(2017).Atutorialontesting,visualizing,andprobinganinteractioninvolving
amulticategoricalvariableinlinearregressionanalysis.Communication Methods and Measures,11(1),
1–30.h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 1 0 8 0 / 1 9 3 1 2 4 5 8 . 2 0 1 6 . 1 2 7 1 1 1 6
1 3
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Journal of Gambling Studies
Joyal-Desmarais,K., Scharmer,A.K.,Madzelan, M.K., See,J.V.,Rothman,A.J.,&Snyder,M.(2022).
Appealingtomotivationtochangeattitudes,intentions,andbehavior:Asystematic reviewandmeta-
analysisof702experimentaltestsoftheeectsofmotivationalmessagematchingonpersuasion.Psy-
chological Bulletin,148(7–8),465.https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000377
Kristensen,J.H.,Trifunovic,S.,Strand,J.,KraftVistnes,K.,Syvertsen,A.,Zandi,A.,&Pallesen,S.(2023).
Asystematic literature review of studies on attitudes towards gambling using the attitudes towards
gamblingscale(ATGS). International Gambling Studies,23(3),353–386. h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 1 0 8 0 / 1 4 4
5 9 7 9 5 . 2 0 2 2 . 2 1 4 3 8 5 6
Langham,E.,Thorne,H.,Browne,M.,Donaldson,P.,Rose,J.,&Rocklo,M.(2016).Understandinggam-
blingrelatedharm:Aproposeddenition,conceptualframework,andtaxonomyofharms.Bmc Public
Health,16(1),80.https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-016-2747-0
Lee,A.,Flack,M.,&Caudwell,K.M.(2024).Excite,ortakeight??Exploringtherelationshipbetweendif-
cultieswithemotionregulation, outcomeexpectancies,andproblemgambling. Journal of Gambling
Studies. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-024-10340-4
McAllister,I.(2014).PublicopiniontowardsgamblingandgamblingregulationinAustralia.International
Gambling Studies,14(1),146–160.https://doi.org/10.1080/14459795.2013.861001
Muñoz,Y.,Chebat,J.C.,&Borges,A.(2013).Graphicgamblingwarnings:Howtheyaectemotions,cogni-
tiveresponsesandattitudechange.Journal of Gambling Studies,29(3),507–524.h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 1 0
07/s10899-012-9319-8
Newall,P.,Torrance,J.,Russell,A.M.T.,Rocklo,M.,Hing,N.,&Browne,M.(2024).Chancesareyou’re
abouttolose’:NewindependentAustraliansafergamblingmessagestestedinUKandUSAbettorsam-
ples.Addiction Research & Theory,32(6),400–408.h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 1 0 8 0 / 1 6 0 6 6 3 5 9 . 2 0 2 3 . 2 2 8 2 5 4 5
Nower,L.,Blaszczynski,A.,&Anthony,W.L.(2022).Clarifyinggamblingsubtypes:Therevisedpathways
modelofproblemgambling.Addiction,117 (7),2000–2008.https://doi.org/10.1111/add.15745
Pallesen,S.,Hanss,D.,Molde,H.,Griths,M.D.,&Mentzoni,R.A.(2016).Alongitudinalstudyoffactors
explaining attitude change towards gambling among adolescents. Journal of Behavioral Addictions,
5(1),59–67.https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.5.2016.017
Richardson,A.C.,Flack,M.,&Caudwell,K.M.(2023).TwofortheGOES:Exploringgamblingoutcome
expectanciesscoresacrossmixedandOine-Onlygamblersinrelationtoproblemgamblingrisksta-
tus.Journal of Gambling Studies. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-023-10234-x
Rosen,L.A.,Weinstock,J.,&Peter,S.C.(2020).Arandomizedclinicaltrialexploringgamblingattitudes,bar-
rierstotreatment,andecacyofabriefmotivationalinterventionamongEx-Oenderswithdisordered
gambling.Journal of Forensic Sciences,65(5),1646–1655.https://doi.org/10.1111/1556-4029.14476
Sheeran,P.,Maki,A.,Montanaro,E.,Avishai-Yitshak,A.,Bryan,A.,Klein,W.M.,Miles,E.,&Rothman,
A.J.(2016).Theimpactofchangingattitudes,norms,andself-ecacyonhealth-relatedintentionsand
behavior:Ameta-analysis.Health Psychology,35(11),1178–1188.https://doi.org/10.1037/hea0000387
Stevens,M.,Gupta,H.,&Flack,M.(2019). 2018NorthernTerritoryGamblingPrevalence andWellbeing
Survey.h t t p s : / / i n d u s t r y . n t . g o v . a u / p u b l i c a t i o n s / b u s i n e s s / p u b l i c a t i o n s / g a m b l i n g - r e s e a r c h / 2 0 1 8 - n t - g a m b l
i n g - p r e v a l e n c e - a n d - w e l l b e i n g - s u r v e y
Tran,L.T.,Wardle,H.,Colledge-Frisby,S.,Taylor,S.,Lynch,M.,Rehm,J.,&Degenhardt,L.(2024).The
prevalenceofgamblingandproblematicgambling:Asystematicreviewandmeta-analysis.The Lancet
Public Health. h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 1 0 1 6 / S 2 4 6 8 - 2 6 6 7 ( 2 4 ) 0 0 1 2 6 - 9
Tseng,C.H.,Flack,M.,Caudwell,K.M.,&Stevens,M.(2023).Separatingproblemgamblingbehaviorsand
negativeconsequences:ExaminingthefactorstructureofthePGSI.Addictive Behaviors,136,107496.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2022.107496
Vaughan,E.,&Flack,M.(2022).Depressionsymptoms,problemgamblingandtheroleofescapeandexcite-
mentgamblingoutcome expectancies.Journal of Gambling Studies,38(1),265–278.h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1
0.1007/s10899-021-10032-3
Wan,C.S.,&Chiou,W.B.(2010).Inducingattitudechangetowardonlinegamingamongadolescentplay-
ersbasedondissonancetheory:Theroleofthreatsandjusticationofeort.Computers & Education,
54(1),162–168.h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 1 0 1 6 / j . c o m p e d u . 2 0 0 9 . 0 7 . 0 1 6
Wardle,H.(2007).British gambling prevalence survey 2007.TheStationeryOce.
Zhou,X.L.,Goernert,P.N.,&Corenblum,B. (2019).ExaminingtheecacyoftheGameSensegambling
prevention programme among university undergraduate students. International Gambling Studies,
19(2),282–295.h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 1 0 8 0 / 1 4 4 5 9 7 9 5 . 2 0 1 8 . 1 5 5 4 0 8 3
Publisher’s Note SpringerNatureremainsneutralwithregardtojurisdictionalclaimsinpublishedmapsand
institutionalaliations.
1 3
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
Terms and Conditions
Springer Nature journal content, brought to you courtesy of Springer Nature Customer Service Center
GmbH (“Springer Nature”).
Springer Nature supports a reasonable amount of sharing of research papers by authors, subscribers
and authorised users (“Users”), for small-scale personal, non-commercial use provided that all
copyright, trade and service marks and other proprietary notices are maintained. By accessing,
sharing, receiving or otherwise using the Springer Nature journal content you agree to these terms of
use (“Terms”). For these purposes, Springer Nature considers academic use (by researchers and
students) to be non-commercial.
These Terms are supplementary and will apply in addition to any applicable website terms and
conditions, a relevant site licence or a personal subscription. These Terms will prevail over any
conflict or ambiguity with regards to the relevant terms, a site licence or a personal subscription (to
the extent of the conflict or ambiguity only). For Creative Commons-licensed articles, the terms of
the Creative Commons license used will apply.
We collect and use personal data to provide access to the Springer Nature journal content. We may
also use these personal data internally within ResearchGate and Springer Nature and as agreed share
it, in an anonymised way, for purposes of tracking, analysis and reporting. We will not otherwise
disclose your personal data outside the ResearchGate or the Springer Nature group of companies
unless we have your permission as detailed in the Privacy Policy.
While Users may use the Springer Nature journal content for small scale, personal non-commercial
use, it is important to note that Users may not:
use such content for the purpose of providing other users with access on a regular or large scale
basis or as a means to circumvent access control;
use such content where to do so would be considered a criminal or statutory offence in any
jurisdiction, or gives rise to civil liability, or is otherwise unlawful;
falsely or misleadingly imply or suggest endorsement, approval , sponsorship, or association
unless explicitly agreed to by Springer Nature in writing;
use bots or other automated methods to access the content or redirect messages
override any security feature or exclusionary protocol; or
share the content in order to create substitute for Springer Nature products or services or a
systematic database of Springer Nature journal content.
In line with the restriction against commercial use, Springer Nature does not permit the creation of a
product or service that creates revenue, royalties, rent or income from our content or its inclusion as
part of a paid for service or for other commercial gain. Springer Nature journal content cannot be
used for inter-library loans and librarians may not upload Springer Nature journal content on a large
scale into their, or any other, institutional repository.
These terms of use are reviewed regularly and may be amended at any time. Springer Nature is not
obligated to publish any information or content on this website and may remove it or features or
functionality at our sole discretion, at any time with or without notice. Springer Nature may revoke
this licence to you at any time and remove access to any copies of the Springer Nature journal content
which have been saved.
To the fullest extent permitted by law, Springer Nature makes no warranties, representations or
guarantees to Users, either express or implied with respect to the Springer nature journal content and
all parties disclaim and waive any implied warranties or warranties imposed by law, including
merchantability or fitness for any particular purpose.
Please note that these rights do not automatically extend to content, data or other material published
by Springer Nature that may be licensed from third parties.
If you would like to use or distribute our Springer Nature journal content to a wider audience or on a
regular basis or in any other manner not expressly permitted by these Terms, please contact Springer
Nature at
onlineservice@springernature.com
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any citations for this publication.
Article
Full-text available
Emotional dysregulation is a transdiagnostic process associated with a range of addictive behaviours including problem gambling, with emerging research indicating that emotionally oriented reasons for gambling (i.e., excitement, escape) are associated with problem gambling. However, the relationships between difficulties with emotion regulation, reasons for gambling, and problem gambling, are unclear. The current study tested whether the association between difficulties with emotion regulation and problem gambling could be explained by escape and excitement gambling outcome expectancies. A total of 187 regular gamblers recruited via social media (50.3% male, 48.7% female) completed measures of difficulties with emotion regulation, gambling outcome expectancies, and problem gambling severity (Mage = 41.07, SD = 15.8). Analyses revealed that escape outcome expectancies partially mediated the relationship between difficulties with emotion regulation and problem gambling severity. However, the mediating effect of excitement on this relationship was not significant. The findings suggest that individuals with greater emotional regulation difficulties may engage in problem gambling to help manage aversive emotional states. The study’s findings illustrate the importance of considering emotional dysregulation and outcome expectancies in problem gambling treatment planning and public health strategies.
Article
Full-text available
Gambling to escape is often purported to play a central role in the maintenance of problem gambling. However, the strength and consistency this association is unknown. Therefore, the current systematic review and meta-analysis was conducted to explore the association between gambling to escape and problem gambling. The study protocol was registered in the PROSPERO database and protocols were developed and published studies were searched until May 2024. The PRISMA standards were adopted for screening and extraction of relevant data. Twenty-seven studies that measured gambling motives, reasons, or expectancies, related to escape, using validated multiple item measures, were included. Positive associations were found between gambling to escape and problem gambling, and this association remained after controlling for the effect of gambling for financial gain. Additional subgroup analyses were conducted to test the stability of the association by escape measure. Although the magnitude of the positive association changed depending on the scales used, the positive relationship held. The findings support the contention that problem gambling is at least, in part, maintained by the escape afforded by gambling—indicating the importance considering the emotion focused reasons in the design of initiative to reduce problem gambling behaviours.
Article
Full-text available
Stress has long been implicated in relation to problem gambling and gambling disorder. However, less is known about the psychological processes that link stress to problem gambling through other known correlates, including outcome expectancies and maladaptive coping. The current study tests a moderated mediation model whereby the effect of stress on problem gambling was hypothesized to be mediated by escape outcome expectancies, with this mediation effect moderated by maladaptive coping. Participants (N = 240; 50.2% male, Mage = 32.76 years; SDage = 11.35 years) were recruited from an online crowdsourcing platform and provided responses on the Depression, Anxiety and Stress Scale (DASS-21; Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995), the Problem Gambling Severity Index (PGSI; Ferris & Wynne, 2001), escape subscale of the Gambling Outcome Expectancies Scale (GOES; Flack & Morris, 2015) and the Brief COPE (Carver, 1997). The model was tested using Hayes’ (2018) PROCESS macro, revealing a significant moderated mediation effect of the stress-escape path by maladaptive coping, showing that the effect was significant when maladaptive coping was high. The findings provide support for escape outcome expectancies as being a potential mechanism through which the stress-problem gambling relationship may operate specifically, influenced by how gamblers are engaged in maladaptive coping generally. There is a need to further investigate the potential for combining gambling outcome expectancy challenges with methods to reduce maladaptive coping or develop more adaptive responses in the face of stress among problem gamblers.
Article
Full-text available
Current industry-developed safer gambling messages such as ‘Take time to think’ and ‘Gamble responsibly’ have been criticized as ineffective slogans. As a result, Australia has recently introduced seven independently-developed safer gambling messages. The UK Government intends to introduce independently- developed messages from 2024 onwards, and this measure could be similarly appropriate for the US states where sports betting has been legalized and gambling advertising has become pervasive. Given this context, the current study recruited race and sports bettors from the UK and USA to elicit their perceptions of the seven Australian safer gambling messages. Participants (N=1865) rated on a Likert-scale seven newly introduced messages and two existing ones (‘Take time to think’ and ‘Gamble responsibly’) using seven evaluative statements. Participants also reported their levels of problem gambling severity. For most statements in both jurisdictions, the new messages performed significantly better than the existing ones. Specifically, the new messages were deemed more attention grabbing, applicable on a personal level, helpful to gamblers, and more likely to encourage cutbacks in gambling. The message that included a specific call to action (‘What are you prepared to lose today? Set a deposit limit’) was one of the best performing messages. Interaction effects observed in relation to jurisdiction, age, gender, and problem gambling severity were generally small enough to counteract the argument that different populations might benefit from substantially different messages. These findings add to previous research on the independent design of effective safer gambling messages.
Article
Full-text available
Objective: Despite the widespread use of harm prevention messages on gambling advertising, it is unclear whether such messages achieve their goal of encouraging safer gambling behavior and preventing gambling harms. The current research therefore investigates whether and how existing harm prevention messages implemented on gambling advertisements in Belgium and the Netherlands affect consumers’ gambling-related beliefs and intentions. Method: Two experimental studies (Nexperiment 1 = 169; Nexperiment 2 = 212) investigate (a) the influence of message prominence and (b) the impact of message framing by comparing the effectiveness of the message “Gamble in moderation” with different types of Flemish and Dutch harm prevention messages (that are currently used in Belgium and the Netherlands) on people’s gambling-related beliefs and intentions. Results: The results of the first experiment show that although the size of a harm prevention message may increase message recognition, it does not affect the message’s efficacy. In addition, the second experiment shows that the harm prevention message “Gamble in moderation” increases normative perceptions of gambling (vs. no message), and even enhances gambling intentions among at-risk gamblers compared to no message and other commonly used harm prevention messages. In contrast, exposure to the harm prevention message “What does gambling cost you? Stop in time” makes at-risk gamblers think most about the harms of gambling. Conclusions: Current harm prevention messages on gambling advertising often promote the concept of responsible gambling, but can have opposite effects than intended, especially among at-risk gamblers.
Article
Full-text available
Just-In-Time Adaptive Interventions (JITAIs) are emerging “push” mHealth interventions that provide the right type, timing, and amount of support to address the dynamically-changing needs for each individual. Although JITAIs are well-suited to the delivery of interventions for the addictions, few are available to support gambling behaviour change. We therefore developed GamblingLess: In-The-Moment and Gambling Habit Hacker, two smartphone-delivered JITAIs that differ with respect to their target populations, theoretical underpinnings, and decision rules. We aim to describe the decisions, methods, and tools we used to design these two treatments, with a view to providing guidance to addiction researchers who wish to develop JITAIs in the future. Specifically, we describe how we applied a comprehensive, organising scientific framework to define the problem, define just-in-time in the context of the identified problem, and formulate the adaptation strategies. While JITAIs appear to be a promising design in addiction intervention science, we describe several key challenges that arose during development, particularly in relation to applying micro-randomised trials to their evaluation, and offer recommendations for future research. Issues including evaluation considerations, integrating on-demand intervention content, intervention optimisation, combining active and passive assessments, incorporating human facilitation, adding cost-effectiveness evaluations, and redevelopment as transdiagnostic interventions are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
As online gambling becomes more prevalent, understanding the motives of online gamblers has become a key focus for research and practice. The aim of this study was to understand differences in gambling-related outcome expectancies between mixed (both online and offline) gamblers and offline-only gamblers, by incorporating gambling harm risk categories from the problem gambling severity index (PGSI). This study comprised a secondary data analysis of the 2015 Northern Territory Gambling Prevalence and Wellbeing Survey. A sample of 1207 individuals in the Northern Territory who had reported gambling at least once in the previous 12 months were used in the analyses. General linear and structural equation modelling were used to ascertain differences in gambling outcome expectancies, in relation to gambling modality (i.e., mixed, offline-only) and PGSI scores. Mixed gamblers tended to score higher on all outcome expectancies than their offline-only counterparts. Outcome expectancy scores were higher in individuals in higher-risk PGSI categories. The escape outcome expectancy was dependent on both modality and risk category. Invariance testing of a low and problem gambling risk subsample revealed differential relationships for both the escape and excitement outcome expectancies for mixed and offline-only gamblers. The results provide an important contribution to the existing literature regarding motivation and outcome expectancies in relation to gambling modality and problem gambling severity. The findings highlight the importance of considering both gambling outcome expectancies and modality when considering problem gambling.
Article
Full-text available
Message matching refers to the design and distribution of persuasive messages such that message features (e.g., the themes emphasized) align with characteristics of the target audience (e.g., their personalities). Motivational message matching is a form of this technique that seeks to enhance persuasion by matching specifically to differences in motivational characteristics (e.g., salient goals, needs, values). Despite widespread use of motivational matching, there is little understanding of how and when to use it. We conducted a preregistered (PROSPERO CRD42019116688; osf.io/rpjdg) systematic review and three-level meta-analysis of 702 experimental studies on motivational matching (synthesizing 5,251 effect sizes from N = 206,482). Studies were inclusive of publications until December 2018, and primarily identified using APA PsycInfo, MEDLINE, and Scopus. We evaluate moderation using meta-regressions, and provide bias assessments (sensitivity analyses, funnel plots). Motivational matching increases persuasion by an average of r = .20 (95% CI: .18, .22) as assessed by differences in attitudes, intentions, self-reported behavior, and observed behavior, relative to comparison conditions. This effect is larger than previously observed for other message matching approaches (e.g., message tailoring, message framing) which usually average r < .10. Although motivational matching can effectively improve persuasion, its effects are also marked by meaningful heterogeneity. Notably, motivational matching effects are largest when matching to contextual factors (than to individual differences), when compared to messages that conflict with people’s motivations, and when target characteristics are manipulated rather than assessed. Through this review, we develop and evaluate theoretical propositions that inform the optimization of motivational matching.
Article
Full-text available
Several studies have investigated attitudes toward gambling using the Attitudes Towards Gambling Scale (ATGS), however, their findings have not previously been synthesized or systematically reported. Thus, we conducted a systematic literature review on studies employing the ATGS to summarize the current evidence. Database searches were conducted in January 2022 in Cinahl, Embase, PsycInfo, Pubmed, Web of Science, GreyNet, and Google Scholar. Papers were included if they presented data based on the ATGS and were published in a European language. Twenty-six papers presenting the results from 23 unique studies met the inclusion criteria. Two reviewers independently extracted the data and assessed the risk of bias. Most of the studies were cross-sectional and used the short (8-item) version of ATGS. The synthesis indicates an overall incline towards negative attitudes. More positive attitudes were associated with being male, younger age, and higher gambling frequency. Studies were divergent in findings concerning problem gambling and gambling attitudes, which could be due to variance in problem gambling severity in the samples. The current evidence base is encumbered by limitations in study quality and designs. Future research should emphasize longitudinal designs, include non-western samples, and investigate the directionality and causality of variables associated with attitudes towards gambling. ARTICLE HISTORY