ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

We have historically ignored sex/gender and conducted sex- and gender-neutral psychopathology research. There is a clear need to analyze potential differences and similarities between individuals with various sexes, genders, and sexual orientations in psychopathology research. Specifically, we need to stop ignoring sex, gender, and sexual orientation, conduct analyses that go beyond the binary, and analyze these important variables for generalizability even when the primary research question is not about sex, gender, and sexual orientation. In the current study we examined ADHD and internalizing symptoms in a community sample to compare different ways to analyze data and better understand differences and similarities across groups. We predicted that a richer understanding of sex/gender differences would emerge when we compared sex and gender minority (SGM) participants to cisgender heterosexual women (CHW) and men (CHM) rather than conducting binary analyses. Emerging adults (N = 2,938; ages 18–29 years) completed an online survey, responding to demographic items, as well as ADHD and internalizing symptoms. Binary analyses using biological sex and gender identity yielded no differences in ADHD symptoms, and the expected female preponderance in internalizing symptoms. However, when analyzed across three groups, individuals in the SGM group reported higher levels of ADHD and internalizing symptoms compared with the other two groups. Notably, no differences emerged for internalizing symptoms across CHW and CHM when the SGM group was included. This is compelling evidence that analyzing sex, gender, and sexual orientation more systematically and precisely in psychopathology research is warranted.
Vol.:(0123456789)
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment (2025) 47:18
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-024-10188-3
A Call toAnalyze Sex, Gender, andSexual Orientation
inPsychopathology Research: AnIllustration withADHD
andInternalizing Symptoms inEmerging Adults
CynthiaM.Hartung1 · ElizabethK.Leer2 · TamaraM.Abu‑Ramadan3 · AnneE.Stevens1 ·
JudahW.Serrano4 · EmilyA.Miller1 · ChristopherR.Shelton5
Accepted: 21 October 2024
© The Author(s) 2025
Abstract
We have historically ignored sex/gender and conducted sex- and gender-neutral psychopathology research. There is a clear
need to analyze potential differences and similarities between individuals with various sexes, genders, and sexual orientations
in psychopathology research. Specifically, we need to stop ignoring sex, gender, and sexual orientation, conduct analyses
that go beyond the binary, and analyze these important variables for generalizability even when the primary research ques-
tion is not about sex, gender, and sexual orientation. In the current study we examined ADHD and internalizing symptoms
in a community sample to compare different ways to analyze data and better understand differences and similarities across
groups. We predicted that a richer understanding of sex/gender differences would emerge when we compared sex and gen-
der minority (SGM) participants to cisgender heterosexual women (CHW) and men (CHM) rather than conducting binary
analyses. Emerging adults (N = 2,938; ages 18–29years) completed an online survey, responding to demographic items, as
well as ADHD and internalizing symptoms. Binary analyses using biological sex and gender identity yielded no differences
in ADHD symptoms, and the expected female preponderance in internalizing symptoms. However, when analyzed across
three groups, individuals in the SGM group reported higher levels of ADHD and internalizing symptoms compared with
the other two groups. Notably, no differences emerged for internalizing symptoms across CHW and CHM when the SGM
group was included. This is compelling evidence that analyzing sex, gender, and sexual orientation more systematically and
precisely in psychopathology research is warranted.
Keywords Sexual and gender minorities· ADHD· Depression· Anxiety· Emerging adults
Historically, psychopathology researchers have given
short shrift to understanding differences (and similari-
ties) between men’s and women’s experiences of mental
illness (Hartung & Widiger, 1998; Hartung & Lelfer 2019;
Howard etal., 2017). Hartung and Lefler (2019) argued
that studies of mental illness have suffered from a type of
sampling bias: the tendency to select samples composed
of only those most prototypical of a disorder (e.g., studies
of only boys with autism), leading to diagnostic criteria/
symptoms, theories of etiology, and treatment strategies
that may only pertain to one group or may be more rel-
evant for one group. There has also been a tendency to
conflate sex and gender terms in research studies (Har-
tung & Lefler, 2019; Howard etal., 2017), leading to: (a)
unclear conclusions about how biological/genetic and/or
societal/environmental roles contribute to the develop-
ment of mental illness and differential sex/gender preva-
lence rates; and (b) unclear treatment recommendations
for diverse individuals, among other concerns. When
researchers do include both men and women or boys and
girls (which they are doing at increasing rates), data ana-
lytic bias remains (i.e., a bias toward not running analyses
* Cynthia M. Hartung
chartung@uwyo.edu
1 University ofWyoming, Laramie, WY, USA
2 University ofOregon, Portland, OR, USA
3 Children’s National Hospital, Washington,DC, USA
4 University ofDenver, Denver, CO, USA
5 Pennsylvania State University, The Behrend College, Erie,
PA, USA
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment (2025) 47:18 18 Page 2 of 10
to compare sex or gender groups; Hartung & Lefler, 2019).
Furthermore, when sex/gender are considered in psycho-
pathology research, which is not often, a binary approach
is almost always used. When the binary approach is used,
sex/gender are often conflated, as mentioned earlier, and
this approach systematically ignores the experiences of
sexual and gender minorities (SGM1; i.e., lesbian, gay,
bisexual, transgender, queer [LGBTQ +]; National Insti-
tutes of Health [NIH], 2024).
For example, Hartung and Lefler (2019) reviewed all
studies published in the Journal of Abnormal Psychol-
ogy (now the Journal of Psychopathology and Clinical
Science) from 2010 to 2017 and found that while 80%
of studies included both males and females, only 41.7%
of those study authors conducted analyses to deter-
mine whether their findings generalized to both binary
sex groups (i.e., analyzed sex/gender as an independ-
ent, moderator, or mediator variable) whereas 44.4% of
authors ignored sex/gender in the preliminary and pri-
mary analysis, and 13.8% of authors included sex/gender
as a covariate which ignores sex/gender rather than help-
ing us determine whether our findings apply to both men
and women. This is not even to mention the erasure of
non-binary, transgender, intersex, and queer people from
psychopathology research entirely. Very few studies, with
the exception of those directly studying LGBTQ + expe-
riences, analyze results with these groups in mind, let
alone list rates of these identities in their participants'
sections. The problem is far-reaching; when we fail to
understand psychopathology in girls/women, boys/men,
and individuals with diverse sexual and gender identities,
we will fail to accurately and appropriately diagnose and
treat them.
It is clear that we need to stop ignoring sex/gender in
psychopathology research. However, possible approaches
to analyzing sex, gender, and sexual orientation (S/G/
SO) have not been extensively explored. Thus, in the cur-
rent paper we use attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder
(ADHD) and internalizing symptoms to call attention to
this problem and explore analytic options for researchers
to better capture potential S/G/SO differences (and simi-
larities) in their studies. Prior to describing our current
study in more detail, we briefly discuss what we know
about S/G/SO differences and similarities in ADHD and
internalizing symptoms.
Sex, Gender, andSexual Orientation
inADHD andInternalizing Symptoms
Both ADHD and internalizing symptoms (i.e., depression
and anxiety symptoms) have long-established sex/gender
differences. Per the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition, Text-Revision (DSM-5-TR;
American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2022), ADHD
shows a male preponderance for adults of 1.6:1, major
depressive disorder (MDD) shows a female preponder-
ance of 1.5:1 to 3:1, generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) a
female preponderance of 2:1, and panic disorder a female
preponderance of 2:1. These numbers are supported by vari-
ous worldwide meta-analyses and large-scale studies of sex/
gender differences in these common disorders. For example,
Salk etal. (2017) conducted a worldwide meta-analysis of
95 studies which confirmed that women self-reported higher
levels of depression symptoms than men. This sex/gender
difference was shown to peak in adolescence, decline in
emerging adulthood, and remain relatively stable through-
out adulthood. Next, Willcutt (2012) conducted a worldwide
meta-analysis that included 96 studies. This analysis con-
cluded that the ratio of ADHD in adults was 1.6M:1F. More
recently, a review by Hinshaw etal. (2022) suggested that
the rates of ADHD in adults might be less discrepant and
approaching a 1:1 ratio. Finally, in a study of over 20,000
participants (i.e., Collaborative Psychiatric Epidemiology
Study [CPES]) McLean etal. (2011) concluded that the life-
time female:male prevalence ratio of any anxiety disorder to
be 1.7F:1M, and found that women reported higher illness
burdens than men. This suggests that women outnumber
men in anxiety diagnoses, and may also be more impaired
by their anxiety. In all, these studies tell us that ADHD
and several common internalizing disorders have well-
documented sex/gender differences. While this amount of
empirical data on sex/gender differences in prevalence rates
is heartening, it is nonetheless limited. Indeed, these studies
may have conflated and/or poorly operationalized sex and
gender on their demographics forms, few studies account
for LGBTQ + experiences, and it is still not the norm for
research teams to include any analyses that consider sex,
gender, or sexual orientation.
Data on SGM mental health has focused on internalizing
symptoms, substance use, and suicide risk (e.g., Marshal
etal., 2011). For example, in a meta-analysis of depres-
sion and anxiety, Ross etal. (2018) found that heterosexual/
straight-identified individuals had the lowest rates of depres-
sion/anxiety, LGB-identified people had higher rates, and
bisexual participants reported similar or even higher rates
than lesbian and gay participants. Moreover, Borgogna
etal. (2019) found that trans and gender non-conforming
(TGNC) college students had higher rates of depression and
1 For this paper, we will use both the terms SGM (NIH Sexual &
Gender Minority Research Office, 2024) and LGBTQ+to refer to
people who identify as lesbian, gay, bisexual, asexual, transgender,
Two-Spirit, queer, and/or intersex, etc. We recognize that language is
constantly evolving, and we strive to use accurate, appropriate, and
affirming terminology.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment (2025) 47:18 Page 3 of 10 18
anxiety than cisgender participants. The authors also noted
that those in emerging identity categories (e.g., demisexual,
asexual, TGNC) reported the highest rates of depression and
anxiety. Less research has been conducted on the rates of
ADHD in SGM populations. In one study by Dawson etal.
(2017), transgender participants had higher rates of ADHD
(in addition to anxiety and depression) as compared to cis-
gender participants. Likewise, Strange etal. (2014) found
that children with an ADHD diagnosis were 6.64 times more
likely than controls to have their parents indicate some gen-
der variance on their behalf. While this area of research is
growing, there is still much to learn.
It is important to study SGM samples because of these
higher rates of disorders. Indeed, minority stress theory
states that individuals with a minoritized identity (or more
than one such identity) are subject to micro- and macro-
aggressions that result in stress, and confer additional risk
for mental health concerns (Meyer, 2003). This may also
lead to internalized homophobia and/or transphobia (Hen-
dricks & Testa, 2012; Herek etal., 1997) which can cause
people in the LGBTQ + community to harbor negative
beliefs about their own sexual or gender minority status,
which can in turn lead to additional risk for internalizing
problems (Newcomb & Mustanski, 2010). This population is
also important to understand because more and more people
are identifying in these ways (Lefler etal., 2023). In fact,
22.7% of emerging adults (in this case defined as ages 18 to
25years) reported an SGM identity as compared to 1.3% of
older adults (ages 65 to 84years; Lefler etal., 2023). Taken
together, there is a need for additional research on emerging
adult mental health, with an eye toward better understand-
ing S/G/SO.
The Current Study
There is a clear need for psychopathology research that
directly investigates potential differences and similarities
between individuals with various sexes, genders, and sexual
orientations. Progress has been made in recent decades in that
more and more studies include individuals from “both” binary
sex/gender categories. However, this is still limiting for sev-
eral reasons as outlined above. What is needed next is more
S/G/SO research, less focus on only the binary, and more stud-
ies that examine these important variables even when these
identities are not thecentral research question. To this end,
in the current study we examined ADHD and internalizing
symptoms in a community sample to illustrate the importance
of finding creative ways to analyze data and better understand
differences and similarities across groups. We chose to focus
on emerging adults given that this group is more likely to
endorse SGM identities than other groups of adults (Lefler
etal., 2023). Specifically, our research question was whether
established sex/gender differences would change if SGM par-
ticipants were removed from these binary groups and analyzed
separately. We predicted that a richer understanding of sex/
gender differences would emerge when we carefully examined
SGM participants and compared them to cisgender hetero-
sexual women (CHW) and men (CHM). In the current study,
we used a large emerging adult sample (2,900 +) to examine
this research question. We conducted analyses on broad inter-
nalizing symptoms, which might be more obviously elevated
for SGM participants, but also ADHD symptoms, which have
been under-studied in this population.
Method
Participants
Participants were 2,938 emerging adults who resided in the
United States and completed an online survey via Ama-
zon’s Mechanical Turk (MTurk; https:// www. mturk. com).
Participants ranged in age from 18 to 29years (M = 24.77,
SD = 3.03) and provided demographic information regarding
age, biological sex, gender identity, sexual orientation, race/
ethnicity, and highest level of education completed.
First, we describe participants in terms of their self-
reported biological sex, gender identity, and sexual orienta-
tion. For biological sex, participants endorsed the following:
female (n = 1,801; 61.3%), male (n = 1,078; 36.7%), intersex
(n = 54; 1.8%), not listed/missing (n = 5; 0.1%). Next, for
gender identity, participants endorsed the following: female/
woman (n = 1,706; 58.1%), male/man (n = 1,059; 36.0%),
non-binary/fluid queer/gender queer (n = 143; 4.9%), not
sure/exploring (n = 16; 0.5%), not listed/missing (n = 14;
0.5%). Finally, for sexual orientation, participants endorsed
the following: heterosexual/straight (n = 2,084; 70.9%), gay/
lesbian (n = 194; 6.6%), bisexual (n = 410; 14.0%), queer
(n = 48; 1.6%), pansexual (n = 90, 3.1%), asexual (n = 41,
1.4%), not sure/exploring (n = 56; 1.9%), not listed/missing
(n = 15; 0.5%).
Next, we describe participants in terms of race/eth-
nicity, level of education, and self-reported lifetime
diagnosis of ADHD, depression, or anxiety. For race/
ethnicity,participants responded in the following ways:
Asian/Asian American (n = 377; 12.8%), Black or Afri-
can American (n = 322; 11.0%), Latinx/Hispanic (n = 316;
10.8%), Native American/American Indian/Alaska Native/
Indigenous (n = 59; 2.0%), Middle Eastern/North African
(n = 28; 1.0%), Pacific Islander/Native Hawaiian (n = 12;
0.4%), White (n = 1,744; 54.4%), multiracial (n = 70;
2.4%), and not listed /missing (n = 10; 0.3%). For level of
education, participants endorsed the following: doctoral
degree (n = 46; 1.6%), master’s degree (n = 327; 11.1%),
bachelor's degree (n = 1,123; 38.2%), associate’s degree
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment (2025) 47:18 18 Page 4 of 10
(n = 398; 13.5%), high school diploma or GED (n = 1021;
34.8%), less than high school or GED (n = 16; 0.5%), and
prefer not to answer, not listed, or missing (n = 7; 0.3%).
In terms of self-reported lifetime diagnosis (for our
variables of interest), our participants reported ADHD
(n = 552; 18.8%), depression (n = 1,016; 34.6%), and anxi-
ety (n = 1,286; 43.8%. Additionally, the sample as a whole
had an average of 18.03 (SD = 11.81) for ADHD severity
score, and an average of 24.33 (SD = 15.18) for internal-
izing total score (more on how these were calculated in
Measures below).
Measures
The Depression, Anxiety, and Stress Scale (DASS; Lovi-
bond & Lovibond, 1995) is a 21-item self-report measure
designed to assess symptoms of depression, anxiety, and
stress. Individuals reported the extent to which they expe-
rienced each symptom in the past week using a four-point
Likert scale (0 = Never to 3 = Almost Always). The DASS
produces a total score and three subscale scores. This
measure has been shown to have acceptable to excellent
reliability and validity (Antony etal., 1998) as evidenced
by strong convergent validity and the ability to distinguish
features of depression and anxiety. In the current study, we
used the total score ranging from 0 to 63as a measure of
overall internalizing symptoms, internal consistency was
excellent(α = 0.95).
The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Dis-
orders-Fifth Edition (DSM-5) ADHD symptom checklist
is a self-report measure containing the 18 ADHD items
from the DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association
[APA], 2013). This measure includes 9 inattention and
9 hyperactivity/impulsivity items. Participants indicated
whether they experienced each item on a 4-point Likert
scale (0 = Never or rarely to 3 = Very often) based on the
past six months. By virtue of this list of items being pulled
directly from the DSM-5, it is considered a valid measure-
ment of DSM-5-defined ADHD. For the current study, a
total severity score was calculated ranging from 0 to 54,
and internal consistency was excellent (α = 0.93).
Procedure
All study procedures were approved by the Institutional
Review Board at the first author’s university. Participation
in this study occurred through MTurk, an online crowd-
sourcing platform. Participants completed an online survey
created using the Qualtrics Research Suite. After providing
informed consent, participants completed the demographic
items, the DSM-5 ADHD Checklist, and the DASS-21. The
survey consisted of a total of 56 items, including two atten-
tion check items, and was took 3–5min to complete. When
a participant failed either attention check item (2.5%), their
participation was automatically discontinued. Participants
who completed the study were compensated with $0.20,
which is considered fair compensation for MTurk surveys
of this length (Moss etal., 2023).
Data Preparation & Analytic Plan
Prior to analysis, the dataset was screened for missing
data, duplicate participation, and for potential issues with
univariate normality and outliers. Data from participants
who had any data missing on the variables of interest was
deleted from the dataset. The final sample was N = 2,938.
For the current study, we compared levels of ADHD and
internalizing symptoms across two sex/gender groups and
three sex/gender/sexual orientation groups. We conducted
two, two-group analyses; one for biological sex and one for
gender identity. For our first two-group analysis, we com-
pared biological females (n = 1,801) to biological males
(n = 1,078; Table1), and for our second two-group analy-
sis we compared those who identified as woman/female
(n = 1,706) to those who identified as men/male (n = 1,059;
Table2). For the three-group approach, we compared cis-
gender heterosexual women (CHW; n = 1,168), cisgender
heterosexual men (CHM; n = 895), and a sexual and gender
minority (SGM; n = 869) group. The SGM group consisted
of individuals who endorsed: (a) intersex on the biological
sex item; (b) non-binary/fluid queer/gender/queer, not sure/
exploring, or not listed/missing on the gender identity item;
(c) gay/lesbian, bisexual, queer, pansexual, asexual, not sure/
exploring, or not listed/missing on the sexual orientation
Table 1 Two-Group Analysis:
Biological Sex
Notes. N = 2,879; ADHD range = 0–54; Internalizing range = 0–63; For Cohen’s d, 0.2 to 0.4 is a small, 0.5
to 0.7 is a medium, and >= 0.8 is a large effect. The sample size for this table is smaller than the full sam-
ple because only binary respondents were included
Females Males t p Cohen’s d
n = 1,801 n = 1,078 (1, 2877)
M SD M SD
ADHD dimension 18.04 11.94 17.66 11.49 0.84 .401 -.03 (-.04-.11)
Internalizing dimension 25.16 15.20 22.47 15.00 4.62 < .001 -.18 (.10-.25)
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment (2025) 47:18 Page 5 of 10 18
item; (d) female on the biological sex item and male on the
gender identity item; or (e) male on the biological sex item
and female on the gender identity item. When three group
analyses were significant, we conducted pairwise post-hoc
Games-Howell analyses. This procedure was chosen because
the equal variance assumption was violated in some of these
analyses. Power analyses conducted using G-Power 3.1.9.7
(Faul etal., 2009) indicated that at least 64 participants per
group were needed to achieve adequate power (0.80) to
detect a medium effect (d = 0.50) with a standard signifi-
cance level (p = 0.05) using independent samples t-tests. For
one-way ANOVAs with three groups, at least 80 participants
per group were needed to achieve adequate power (0.80) to
detect a medium effect (f = 0.25) with a standard significance
level (p = 0.05). Thus, all of our analyses were adequately
poweredto detect medium effects.
Results
Two‑Group Analyses
Biological Sex We first conducted two independent samples
t-tests to examine potential differences by biological sex in
ADHD and internalizing symptoms based on self-report
(Table1). For ADHD symptoms, the mean for biological
females was 18.04 (SD = 11.94), and the mean for biologi-
cal males was 17.66 (SD = 11.49). This difference was not
statistically significant (t = 0.84, p = .401, d = 0.03). For
internalizing symptoms, the mean for biological females
was 25.16 (SD = 1.36), and the mean for biological males
was 22.47 (SD = 15.00). This difference was statistically sig-
nificant (t = 4.62, p < .001, d = 0.18) and approaching a small
effect with females reporting more internalizing symptoms
than males.
Gender Identity Next, we conducted two independent
samples t-tests to examine potential differences by gen-
der identity in ADHD and internalizing symptoms based
on self-report (Table2). For ADHD symptoms, the mean
for those who identified as women was 17.73 (SD = 11.89),
and the mean for those who identified as men was 17.66
(SD = 11.57). This difference was not statistically signifi-
cant (t = 0.16, p = .872, d = 0.01). For internalizing symp-
toms, the mean for those who identified as women was 24.57
(SD = 15.12) and those who identified as men was 22.44
(SD = 15.11). This difference was statistically significant
(t = 3.77, p < .001, d = 0.15) and approached a small effect,
with women reporting more internalizing symptoms than
men.
Three‑Group Analyses
We conducted two one-way univariate analyses of vari-
ance tests (i.e., 3 × 1 ANOVAs) to examine potential sex/
gender/sexual orientation differences in ADHD and inter-
nalizing symptoms (Table3). For ADHD symptoms, the
mean for CHW was 15.93 (SD = 11.61), the mean for CHM
was 16.72 (SD = 11.24), and the mean for SGM was 22.19
Table 2 Two-Group Analysis:
Gender Identity
Notes. N = 2,879; ADHD range = 0–54; Internalizing range = 0–63; For Cohen’s d, 0.2 to 0.4 is a small, 0.5
to 0.7 is a medium, and >= 0.8 is a large effect. The sample size for this table is smaller than the full sam-
ple because only binary respondents were included
Women Men t p Cohen’s d
n = 1,706 n = 1,059 (1, 2763)
M SD M SD
ADHD dimension 17.73 11.89 17.66 11.57 0.16 .872 -.01 (-.07-.08)
Internalizing dimension 24.67 15.12 22.44 15.11 3.77 < .001 -.15 (.07-.22)
Table 3 Three-Group Analysis:
Sex/Gender/Sexual Orientation
Notes. N = 2,932; CHM = cisgender heterosexual men; CHW = cisgender heterosexual women; SGM = sex-
ual and gender minority; ADHD range = 054; Internalizing range = 0–63; For partial eta squared, .01 to
.05 is a small, .06 to .13 is a medium, and > = .14 is a large effect. Intersex individuals are included in the
SGM group
CHW
n = 1,168
CHM
n = 895
SGM
n = 869
F
(2, 2929)
pPartial η2
M SD M SD M SD
ADHD dimension 15.93a11.61 16.72a11.24 22.19b11.59 82.33 < .001 .05 (.04-.07)
Internalizing dimension 22.20a14.81 21.15a14.87 30.50b14.18 110.60 < .001 .07 (.06-.09)
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment (2025) 47:18 18 Page 6 of 10
(SD = 11.59). This difference was statistically significant
(F = 82.33, p < .001, partial η2 = 0.05) and corresponded
to a small effect. Post-hoc Games-Howell paired compari-
sons demonstrated that there was no significant difference
between CHW and CHM (p = .260); however, there were
significant differences between CHW and SGM (p < .001)
and between CHM and SGM (p < .001) such that the SGM
group reported higher ADHD symptoms.
For internalizing symptoms, the mean for CHW was 22.20
(SD = 14.81), the mean for CHM was 21.15 (SD = 14.87),
and the mean for SGM was 30.50 (SD = 14.18). This dif-
ference was statistically significant (F = 110.60, p < .001,
partial η2 = 0.07) and corresponded to a medium effect.
Post-hoc Games-Howell paired comparisons demonstrated
that there was no significant difference between CHW and
CHM (p = .247); however, there were significant differences
between CHW and SGM (p < .001) and between CHM and
SGM (p < .001) such that the SGM group reported higher
internalizing symptoms.
Exploratory Analyses: Four‑Group Analyses
Given that the SGM group reported much higher levels of
ADHD and internalizing symptoms than either of the cis-
gender heterosexual groups, we conducted additional explor-
atory analyses to examine subgroups of individuals within
the SGM group. For these four-group analyses, we com-
paredparticipants who identified as: (a) cisgender and heter-
osexual (men and women combined due to no significant dif-
ferences in previous analyses; n = 2,317), (b) cisgender and
gay/lesbian (n = 141), (c) cisgender and bisexual (n = 365),
and (d) TGNC and any sexual orientation (n = 115). There
were two reasons that we did not divide the TGNC group
into smaller groups based on sexual orientation. First, the
distinction between heterosexual and gay/lesbian is based on
a binary that does not apply to those who identify as non-
binary. Second, we conducted an additional power analysis
for one-way ANOVAs with four groups. We found that at
least 90 participants per group were needed to achieve ade-
quate power (0.80) to detect a medium effect (f = 0.25) with
a standard significance level (p = .05) Table4.
For these exploratory analyses, we again conducted
two one-way univariate analyses of variance tests (i.e.,
4 × 1 ANOVAs) to examine S/G/SO differences in ADHD
and internalizing symptoms. For ADHD symptoms, the
mean for the cisgender and heterosexual group was 16.88
(SD = 11.60), the mean for the cisgender and gay/lesbian
group was 21.92 (SD = 11.84), the mean for the cisgender
and bisexual group was 22.10 (SD = 11.67), and the mean for
the TGNC group was 23.48 (SD = 11.12). This ANOVA was
statistically significant (F = 36.28, p < .001, partial η2 = 0.04)
and corresponded to a small effect. Again, we conducted
post-hoc Games-Howell analyses; all three post-hoc paired
comparisons between the cisgender/heterosexual group and
each of the SGM groups were significant (ps < .001) such
that SGM participants reported higher levels of ADHD
symptoms than their cisgender/heterosexual peers. None
of the comparisons among the three SGM groups were
significant.
For internalizing symptoms, the mean for the cisgender
and heterosexual group was 22.75 (SD = 15.11), the mean for
the cisgender and gay/lesbian group was 28.00 (SD = 14.66),
the mean for the cisgender and bisexual group was 30.64
(SD = 14.23), and the mean for the TGNC group was 31.75
(SD = 11.74). This ANOVA was also statistically significant
(F = 43.15, p < .001, par tial η2 = 0.04) and corresponded to a
small effect. Again, all three post-hoc Games-Howell paired
comparisons between the cisgender/heterosexual group and
each of the SGM groups were significant (ps < .001) such
that SGM participants reported higher levels of internal-
izing symptoms than their cisgender/heterosexual peers.2
None of the comparisons among the three SGM groups were
significant.
Table 4 Four-Group Analysis: Sex/Gender/Sexual Orientation
Notes. TGNC = transgender or gender non-conforming; SO = sexual orientation; N = 2,938; ADHD range = 0–54; Internalizing range = 0–63; For
partial eta squared, .01 to .05 is a small, .06 to .13 is a medium, and > = .14 is a large effect
Cisgender +
Heterosexual
n = 2,317
Cisgender +
Gay/Lesbian
n = 141
Cisgender +
Bisexual
n = 365
TGNC
+ Any SO
n = 115
F
(3, 2934)
pPartial η2
M SD M SD M SD M SD
ADHD dimension 16.88a11.60 21.92b11.86 22.10b11.67 23.48b11.12 36.28 < .001 .04 (.02-.05)
Internalizing dimension 22.75a15.11 28.00b14.66 30.64b14.23 31.75b11.74 43.15 < .001 .04 (.03-.05)
2 We repeated all analyses (i.e., Tables 1-4) while controlling for
the other DV (i.e., controlled for INT in the ADHD analyses and
controlled for ADHD in the INT analyses). For INT symptoms,
the results always remained the same in terms of significance and
effect size. For ADHD, there were two instances where the ANOVA
became significant (i.e., both binary analyses) and two instances
where the results stayed the same. Thus, the females/women being
higher on ADHD is not a result of comorbid internalizing symptoms.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment (2025) 47:18 Page 7 of 10 18
Discussion
In psychopathology research, we have historically ignored
sex/gender and conducted sex- and gender-neutral research
(Hartung & Lefler, 2019; Howard etal., 2017; Lefler etal.,
2023). In most studies, we do not analyze sex, gender, or
sexual orientation. For studies in which sex/gender are
analyzed, a binary approach is almost always used (Hyde
etal., 2019; Keyes & Platt, 2023). The goal of the cur-
rent study was to compare several different approaches to
conducting psychopathology analyses with S/G/SO in an
emerging adult sample.
When two-group (i.e., binary) analyses were con-
ducted, with both sex and gender, there was no significant
between-group difference for ADHD symptoms. Although
ADHD has traditionally been shown to be more common
in boys and men, our results are consistent with more
recent studies suggesting that the sex ratio in adults is
closer to 1:1 (Hinshaw etal., 2022), and this is especially
true when examining symptom levels, rather than preva-
lence rates, in emerging adult and college samples (e.g.,
Fedele etal., 2012). Next, regarding the two-group analy-
ses for internalizing symptoms (with both binary sex and
gender identity analyses), females/women reported higher
levels than males/men. This finding is consistent with past
research suggesting that women are more likely to be diag-
nosed with depression and anxiety, and that they report
higher levels of depression symptoms (Keyes & Platt,
2023; McLean etal., 2011; Salk etal., 2017).
When three-group analyses were conducted, we found
that the SGM group reported significantly higher levels of
both ADHD and internalizing symptoms, and there was
no difference between cisgender-heterosexual (cis-hetero)
men and women. For both symptom dimensions, the SGM
group was significantly higher than both the CHW and
CHM with medium effects. Although research has dem-
onstrated that SGM individuals report higher levels of
anxiety and depression than their cis-hetero peers (Fish &
Pasley, 2015; Kattari etal., 2020; King, etal., 2008), there
has been very little research on ADHD in SGM individu-
als. For example, in a systematic review of neurodevel-
opmental disorders in individuals with gender dysphoria,
Thrower etal. (2020) found that autism spectrum disorders
occur more frequently in this group; however, they did not
find sufficient studies examining ADHD.
In the current study, the differences between the SGM
and cis-hetero groups appeared to be practically and
clinically significant. For example, the mean severity of
ADHD symptoms for the SGM group was 22.19, and the
means for the CHW and CHM groups were 15.93 and
16.72, respectively. These severity scores were based
on a 4-point Likert scale (0 = Never or rarely to 3 = Very
often). Clinically, a score of 2 (Often) or 3 (Very often) is
typically interpreted to mean that the symptom is present.
Thus, a mean of 22.19 would be consistent with endorsing
11 of 18 symptoms as occurring often or 7 of 18 symptoms
as occurring very often; whereas a mean of 15.93 would be
consistent with endorsing 8 of 18 symptoms as occurring
often or 5 of 18 symptoms as occurring very often. Thus,
individuals in the SGM group are endorsing 2 to 3 more
symptoms, on average, than members of either cis-hetero
group. Given that a diagnosis of ADHD requires 5 of 9
symptoms of inattention and/or 5 of 9 symptoms of hyper-
activity/impulsivity in adulthood, and diagnostic decisions
often come down to just a symptom or two, this difference
is clinically significant. Although our INT measure does
not lend itself to a symptom analysis, the magnitude of the
differences for ADHD and INT were similar. Specifically,
those in the SGM group reported 33–39% more ADHD
and 37–44% more INT than either cis-hetero group.
These findings for the SGM group are consistent with
minority stress theory, which would suggest that individuals
in the SGM group reported higher levels of ADHD and INT
symptoms because of the additional stress that they expe-
rience in our society as members of this group. Although
we expected this finding for INT, we were surprised at the
magnitude of the difference in a highly heritable, neurode-
velopmental disorder like ADHD (Brikell etal., 2015).
Additional analyses showed that our findings held for ADHD
when we controlled for INT and vice versa. Nonetheless,
future research (and clinical work) should examine, via clini-
cal interviews, whether our participants might be endors-
ing ADHD symptoms that would be better accounted for
by other factors (e.g., endorsing difficulty paying attention
as an ADHD symptom which may actually be due to anxi-
ety, depression, or stress; endorsing disorganization as an
ADHD symptom which may be caused by general cognitive
overload).
Another finding that was surprising was that there were
no sex/gender differences between CHW and CHM for
INT symptoms. Higher rates of anxiety and depression
diagnoses and symptoms in women is a well-established
finding (McLean etal., 2011; Salk etal., 2017). The differ-
ences between our two- and three-group findings for INT
suggest that three-group analyses are potentially impor-
tant. As expected, females/women reported higher levels
of INT symptoms than males/men when binary analyses
were conducted. However, in the three-group analyses, the
SGM group was statistically and meaningfully significantly
higher than either of the cis-hetero groups, and the differ-
ences that were previously attributed to binary sex/gender
disappeared. Given that a higher percentage of biological
females identify as SGM than biological males (Lefler
etal., 2023), it is possible that higher rates of INT in SGM
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment (2025) 47:18 18 Page 8 of 10
have been misattributed to sex/gender. However, given that
identification with SGM status is increasing generationally,
with individuals in younger cohorts being much more likely
to identify as SGM than individuals in older cohorts, it is
unlikely that higher rates of INT in females/women would
have completely been accounted for by SGM historically.
This certainly warrants more research attention.
Given that our three-group analyses produced statistically
and clinically significantly higher endorsement of ADHD
and INT in the SGM group and that we had a large enough
sample, we conducted additional exploratory analyses with
four groups to determine whether there were any differ-
ences among more specific SGM identities. We found that
all three SGM groups (i.e., cisgender + gay/lesbian, cisgen-
der + bisexual, and TGNC) reported significantly higher lev-
els of ADHD and INT than their cis-hetero peers with small
to medium effects. Furthermore, there were no statistically
significant differences among the three SGM groups. Thus,
minority stress may be impacting gay/lesbian, bisexual, and
TGNC individuals at similar levels, though more research
is needed to confirm this, especially given the paucity of
research on and the anti-transgender laws that target TGNC
individuals.
Limitations andFuture Directions
As with any paper, the current study had limitations that
should be considered. First, it is important that researchers
consider not only S/G/SO more systematically but also vari-
ables related to race and ethnicity. It was beyond the scope
of this project for us to also address race and ethnicity, but
similar problems exist with this other set of demographic var-
iables (Eaton, 2019). That is, race and ethnicity are often con-
flated as are sex and gender, and researchers tend to ignore
these variables as they often do with sexual orientation (i.e.,
race/ethnicity are more often included in participants sections
to describe the sample than are sex/gender/sexual orienta-
tion; however, similar to the latter, they are rarely analyzed;
Eaton, 2019). In the current study, we focused on S/G/SO
and examined several approaches to analyzing these individ-
ual difference variables. Future research aimed at doing the
same for race and ethnicity variables is certainly warranted,
especially given the disparities in people of color accessing
mental health services (Lu etal., 2021).
Second, even in our attempts to analyze groups beyond
the binary (i.e., our three- and four-group analyses), we still
grouped participants together in ways that have limitations.
For example, in our three-group analyses, we grouped all
SGM individuals together in a way that might suggest that
they are a homogeneous group when they certainly are not.
Likewise, in our four-group analyses, we separated the SGM
individuals into three groups, but we still grouped all cis-gen-
der gay and lesbian participants and all TGNC participants
together. This might ignore important nuances, such as
whether transwomen have substantially different experiences
from transmen or how trans individuals who “pass” might be
impacted, both of which are certainly areas for further study.
While we maintain that these groupings are superior to ignor-
ing SGM participants and their experiences, we understand
that some nuance is lost. In the future, researchers should
over-select for specific SGM subpopulations.
Next, in the current study we focused on only two sets of
symptoms (i.e., ADHD and internalizing), used only online
self-report of these symptoms in a community sample, and
did not assess diagnosis. While we made these choices to
provide a simple demonstration of our point regarding S/G/
SO analyses with a large sample, we understand that these
are limitations. We are unable to comment on how this
would play out with evidence-based assessment of ADHD,
depression, and anxiety, and we recognize that self-report
of symptoms in a quick, online survey may be suspect. For
instance, a person in our sample may have endorsed symp-
toms such as frequent worry and poor concentration for a
myriad of reasons. Future researchers may want to test our
analytic approach in other ways.
Finally, we acknowledge that the terminology in this
area is quickly changing. While we attempted to use up-
to-date terms, we recognize that these may be out of date
in just a matter of months. For example, when we designed
the study we used “biological sex” as a category instead of
“sex assigned at birth.” In the intervening months we have
learned more and have updated our own language, and we
will continue to do so. We encourage researchers to carefully
consider the language in their demographics sections with
an emphasis on inclusion, and we point to several excellent
resources (Beischel etal., 2022; Broussard etal., 2018; Hyde
etal., 2019; Lowik etal., 2022).
Research Recommendations
We recommend that psychopathology researchers use
a three-group approach (i.e., including an SGM group)
when sample size allows. This may change conclusions
about binary sex differences, including those that are
well-established in the literature. The utility of analyzing
data with a separate SGM group has become even more
pertinent given that more individuals are identifying as
SGM in younger generations (Lefler etal., 2023). We also
recommend that researchers conduct preliminary analyses
with S/G/SO. These preliminary analyses, and possible
follow-up analyses, should be conducted even if there is
not a specific hypothesis related to S/G/SO. The goal is to
determine whether results generalize across S/G/SO and
to avoid sex- and gender-neutral research. When the pre-
liminary analyses show no differences by S/G/SO on study
variables, then no additional analyses are needed; however,
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment (2025) 47:18 Page 9 of 10 18
researchers should note whether there was enough power
to rule out S/G/SO differences.
When the preliminary analyses show S/G/SO differ-
ences, then researchers should consider whether to: (a)
include S/G/SO as an independent, mediator, or moderator
variable in their primary analyses (covarying lacks eco-
logical validity and erases group identities) or (b) con-
duct exploratory analyses with S/G/SO. Finally, if S/G/
SO from primary or exploratory analyses are significant,
researchers should discuss these results using context from
previous findings in the literature which may not have been
reviewed in the introduction due to the lack of specific
hypotheses about S/G/SO. Finally, if S/G/SO analyses
demonstrate SGM differences, researchers should consider
analyses that divide this group into more specific groups
(e.g., sexual minorities, gender minorities) if they have a
large enough sample to do so.
Insufficient power to detect potential differences is an
important consideration, and may preclude three-group
analyses. Thus, when sample sizes are small, as is often
the case in clinical studies, we recommend: (a) describing
S/G/SO in the participants section, (b) conducting pre-
liminary analyses with biological sex and gender identity
for males/females and men/women, respectively, and (c)
mentioning the importance of examining the findings in
SGM group(s) as a limitation and future direction. If pre-
liminary analyses show sex or gender differences, then we
recommend the procedure described above.
Overall Conclusion
In the current study, we demonstrated the importance of
analyzing psychopathology data by sex, gender, and sexual
orientation. In a large community sample of emerging adults
who self-reported ADHD and internalizing symptoms, we
found increasing levels of complexity and nuance at each
level of data analysis. Specifically, our data had very little
nuance when we ignored sex and gender, more when we
analyzed using binary sex and gender groups, and even more
when we considered SGM status.In conclusion, the three-
group approach, although not ideal in terms of lumping sex-
ual and gender minorities, is superior to the traditional sex-
and gender-neutral and binary approaches. Furthermore, the
three-group approach may represent current best practice
given that: (a) few samples will have a large enough gender
minority group to separate from the sexual minority group
and (b) these preliminary results did not show differences
between sexual and gender minorities.
Funding Open access funding provided by the University of Wyoming
Libraries. The first author was supported by an Institutional Develop-
ment Award (IDeA) from the NationalInstitute of General Medical
Sciences (2P20GM103432).
Declarations
Conflict of Interest The authors have no relevant financial or non-fi-
nancial interests to disclose.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attri-
bution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adapta-
tion, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long
as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source,
provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes
were made. The images or other third party material in this article are
included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated
otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in
the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not
permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will
need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a
copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
References
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical
manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). https:// doi. org/ 10. 1176/ appi.
books. 97808 90425 596
American Psychiatric Association (2022). Diagnostic and statistical
manual of mental disorders (5th ed., text rev.). https:// doi. org/ 10.
1176/ appi. books. 97808 90425 787
Antony, M. M., Bieling, P. J., Cox, B. J., Enns, M. W., & Swinson, R.
P. (1998). Psychometric properties of the 42-item and 21-item ver-
sions of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales in clinical groups
and a community sample. Psychological Assessment, 10(2), 176–
181. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/ 1040- 3590. 10.2. 176
Beischel, W. J., Schudson, Z. C., Hoskin, R. A., & van Anders, S.
M. (2022, February 24). The Gender/Sex 3×3: Measuring and
Categorizing Gender/Sex Beyond Binaries. Psychology of Sexual
Orientation and Gender Diversity. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/ sgd00
00558
Borgogna, N. C., McDermott, R. C., Aita, S. L., & Kridel, M. M.
(2019). Anxiety and depression across gender and sexual minori-
ties: Implications for transgender, gender nonconforming, pansex-
ual, demisexual, asexual, queer, and questioning individuals. Psy-
chology of Sexual Orientation and Gender Diversity, 6(1), 54–63.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/ sgd00 00306
Brikell, I., Kuja-Halkola, R., & Larsson, H. (2015). Heritability of
attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in adults. American
Journal of Medical Genetics Part b: Neuropsychiatric Genetics,
168(6), 406–413. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1002/ ajmg.b. 32335
Broussard, K. A., Warner, R. H., & Hope, A. R. D. (2018). Too many
boxes, or not enough? Preferences for how we ask about gender in
cisgender, LGB, and gender-diverse samples. Sex Roles, 78(9–10),
606–624. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/ s11199- 017- 0823-2
Dawson, A. E., Wymbs, B. T., Gidycz, C. A., Pride, M., & Figueroa,
W. (2017). Exploring rates of transgender individuals and men-
tal health concerns in an online sample. International Journal of
Transgenderism, 18(3), 295–304. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1080/ 15532
739. 2017. 13147 97
Eaton, N. R. (2019). Measurement and mental health disparities: Psy-
chopathology classification and identity assessment. Personality
and Mental Health, 14(1), 76–87. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1002/ pmh.
1449
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A.-G. (2009). Statisti-
cal power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and
regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 1149–1160.
Fedele, D. A., Lefler, E. K., Hartung, C. M., & Canu, W. H. (2012).
Sex differences in the manifestation of ADHD in emerging adults.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment (2025) 47:18 18 Page 10 of 10
Journal of Attention Disorders, 16(2), 109–117. https:// doi. org/
10. 1177/ 10870 54710 374596
Fish, J. N., & Pasley, K. (2015). Sexual (minority) trajectories, mental
health, and alcohol use: A longitudinal study of youth as they
transition to adulthood. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 44(8),
1508–1527. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/ s10964- 015- 0280-6
Hartung, C. M., & Lefler, E. K. (2019). Sex and gender in psycho-
pathology: DSM–5 and beyond. Psychological Bulletin, 145(4),
390–409. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/ bul00 00183
Hartung, C. M., & Widiger, T. A. (1998). Gender differences in the
diagnosis of mental disorders: Conclusions and controversies of
DSM-IV. Psychological Bulletin, 123, 260–278.
Hendricks, M. L., & Testa, R. J. (2012). A conceptual framework for
clinical work with transgender and gender nonconforming cli-
ents: An adaptation of the Minority Stress Model. Professional
Psychology: Research and Practice, 43(5), 460–467. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 1037/ a0029 597
Herek, G. M., Cogan, J. C., Gillis, J. R., & Gunt, E. K. (1997). Cor-
relates of internalized homophobia in a community sample of
lesbian and gay men. Journal of the Gay and Lesbian Medical
Association, 2, 17–25.
Hinshaw, S. P., Nguyen, P. T., O’Grady, S. M., & Rosenthal, E. A.
(2022). Annual research review: Attention-deficit/hyperactivity
disorder in girls and women: Underrepresentation, longitudinal
processes, and key directions. The Journal of Child Psychology
and Psychiatry, 63(4), 484–496. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1111/ jcpp.
13480
Howard, L. M., Ehrlich, A. M., Gamlen, F., & Oram, S. (2017). Gen-
der-neutral mental health research is sex and gender biased. The
Lancet. Psychiatry, 4(1), 9–11. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/ S2215-
0366(16) 30209-7
Hyde, J. S., Bigler, R. S., Joel, D., Tate, C. C., & van Anders, S. M.
(2019). The future of sex and gender in psychology: Five chal-
lenges to the gender binary. American Psychologist, 74(2), 171–
193. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/ amp00 00307
Kattari, S. K., Kattari, L., Johnson, I., Lacombe-Duncan, A., & Misi-
olek, B. A. (2020). Differential experiences of mental health
among trans/gender diverse adults in Michigan. International
Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(18),
6805. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3390/ ijerp h1718 6805
Keyes, K. M., & Platt, J. M. (2023). Annual research review: Sex,
gender, and internalizing conditions among adolescents in the 21st
century - trends, causes, consequences. Journal of Child Psychol-
ogy and Psychiatry, 65(4), 384–407.
King, M., Semlyen, J., Tai, S. S., Killaspy, H., Osborn, D., Popelyuk,
D., & Nazareth, I. (2008). A systematic review of mental disor-
der, suicide, and deliberate self harm in lesbian, gay and bisexual
people. BMC Psychiatry, 8(70), 1–17. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1186/
1471- 244X-8- 70
Lefler, E. K., Tabler, J., Abu-Ramadan, T. M., Stevens, A. E., Ser-
rano, J. W., Shelton, C. R., & Hartung, C. M. (2023). Sex, gender,
and sexual orientation in psychological research: Exploring data
trends and researcher opinions. Psychological Reports, Advance
online publication. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1177/ 00332 94123 11999 59
Lovibond, P., & Lovibond, S. (1995). The structure of negative emo-
tional states: Comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales
with the Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 33(3), 335–343. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/
0005- 7967(94) 00075-U
Lowik, A., Cameron, J. J., Dame, J., Ford, J., Pulice-Farrow, L., Sal-
way, T., van Anders, S., & Shannon, K. (2022). Gender and Sex
in Methods & Measurement - Research Equity Toolkit. University
of British Columbia.
Lu, W., Todhunter-Reid, A., Mitsdarffer, M. L., Munoz-Laboy, M.,
Yoon, A. S., & Xu, L. (2021). Barriers and facilitators for mental
health service use among racial/ethnic minority adolescents: A
systematic review of the literature. Frontiers in Public Health, 9.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 3389/ fpubh. 2021. 641605
Marshal, M. P., Dietz, L. J., Friedman, M. S., Stall, R., Smith, H. A.,
McGinley, J., .. & Brent, D. A. (2011). Suicidality and depression
disparities between sexual minority and heterosexual youth: A
meta-analytic review. Journal of Adolescent Health, 49, 115–123.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. jadoh ealth. 2011. 02. 005
McLean, C. P., Asnaani, A., Litz, B. T., & Hofmann, S. G. (2011).
Gender differences in anxiety disorders: Prevalence, course of
illness, comorbidity and burden of illness. Journal of Psychiatric
Research, 45(8), 1027–1035. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. jpsyc hires.
2011. 03. 006
Meyer, I. H. (2003). Prejudice, social stress, and mental health in
lesbian, gay, and bisexual populations: Conceptual issues and
research evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 129(5), 674.
Moss, A. J., Rosenzweig, C., Robinson, J., Jaffe, S. N., & Litman,
L. (2023). Is it ethical to use Mechanical Turk for behavioral
research? Relevant data from a representative survey of MTurk
participants and wages. Behavior Research Methods, 55, 4048–
4067. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3758/ s13428- 022- 02005-0
National Institutes of Health (2024, May 15). Sexual and Gender
Minority Research Office. https:// dpcpsi. nih. gov/ sgmro
Newcomb, M. E., & Mustanski, B. (2010). Internalized homophobia
and internalizing mental health problems: A meta-analytic review.
Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 1019–1029. https:// doi. org/ 10.
1016/j. cpr. 2010. 07. 003
Ross, L. E., Salway, T., Tarasoff, L. A., MacKay, J. M., Hawkins, B.
W., & Fehr, C. P. (2018). Prevalence of depression and anxiety
among bisexual people compared to gay, lesbian, and heterosexual
individuals: A systematic review and meta-analysis. The Journal
of Sex Research, 55(4–5), 435–456. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1080/ 00224
499. 2017. 13877 55
Salk, R. H., Hyde, J. S., & Abramson, L. Y. (2017). Gender differences
in depression in representative national samples: Meta-analyses of
diagnoses and symptoms. Psychological Bulletin, 143, 783–822.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/ bul00 00102
Strang, J. F., Kenworthy, L., Dominska, A., etal. (2014). Increased
Gender Variance in Autism Spectrum Disorders and Attention
Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 43,
1525–1533. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/ s10508- 014- 0285-3
Thrower, E., Bretherton, I., Pang, K. C., Zajac, J. D., & Cheung, A.
S. (2020). Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder and attention-
deficit hyperactivity disorder amongst individuals with gender
dysphoria: A systematic review. Journal of Autism and Devel-
opmental Disorders, 50(3), 695–706. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/
s10803- 019- 04298-1
Willcutt, E. G. (2012). The prevalence of DSM–IV attention-deficit/
hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analytic review. Neurotherapeu-
tics, 9, 490–499. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/ s13311- 012- 0135-8
Publisher's Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to
jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
Terms and Conditions
Springer Nature journal content, brought to you courtesy of Springer Nature Customer Service Center GmbH (“Springer Nature”).
Springer Nature supports a reasonable amount of sharing of research papers by authors, subscribers and authorised users (“Users”), for small-
scale personal, non-commercial use provided that all copyright, trade and service marks and other proprietary notices are maintained. By
accessing, sharing, receiving or otherwise using the Springer Nature journal content you agree to these terms of use (“Terms”). For these
purposes, Springer Nature considers academic use (by researchers and students) to be non-commercial.
These Terms are supplementary and will apply in addition to any applicable website terms and conditions, a relevant site licence or a personal
subscription. These Terms will prevail over any conflict or ambiguity with regards to the relevant terms, a site licence or a personal subscription
(to the extent of the conflict or ambiguity only). For Creative Commons-licensed articles, the terms of the Creative Commons license used will
apply.
We collect and use personal data to provide access to the Springer Nature journal content. We may also use these personal data internally within
ResearchGate and Springer Nature and as agreed share it, in an anonymised way, for purposes of tracking, analysis and reporting. We will not
otherwise disclose your personal data outside the ResearchGate or the Springer Nature group of companies unless we have your permission as
detailed in the Privacy Policy.
While Users may use the Springer Nature journal content for small scale, personal non-commercial use, it is important to note that Users may
not:
use such content for the purpose of providing other users with access on a regular or large scale basis or as a means to circumvent access
control;
use such content where to do so would be considered a criminal or statutory offence in any jurisdiction, or gives rise to civil liability, or is
otherwise unlawful;
falsely or misleadingly imply or suggest endorsement, approval , sponsorship, or association unless explicitly agreed to by Springer Nature in
writing;
use bots or other automated methods to access the content or redirect messages
override any security feature or exclusionary protocol; or
share the content in order to create substitute for Springer Nature products or services or a systematic database of Springer Nature journal
content.
In line with the restriction against commercial use, Springer Nature does not permit the creation of a product or service that creates revenue,
royalties, rent or income from our content or its inclusion as part of a paid for service or for other commercial gain. Springer Nature journal
content cannot be used for inter-library loans and librarians may not upload Springer Nature journal content on a large scale into their, or any
other, institutional repository.
These terms of use are reviewed regularly and may be amended at any time. Springer Nature is not obligated to publish any information or
content on this website and may remove it or features or functionality at our sole discretion, at any time with or without notice. Springer Nature
may revoke this licence to you at any time and remove access to any copies of the Springer Nature journal content which have been saved.
To the fullest extent permitted by law, Springer Nature makes no warranties, representations or guarantees to Users, either express or implied
with respect to the Springer nature journal content and all parties disclaim and waive any implied warranties or warranties imposed by law,
including merchantability or fitness for any particular purpose.
Please note that these rights do not automatically extend to content, data or other material published by Springer Nature that may be licensed
from third parties.
If you would like to use or distribute our Springer Nature journal content to a wider audience or on a regular basis or in any other manner not
expressly permitted by these Terms, please contact Springer Nature at
onlineservice@springernature.com
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any citations for this publication.
Article
Full-text available
Internalizing conditions of psychopathology include depressive and anxiety disorders; they most often onset in adolescence, are relatively common, and contribute to significant population morbidity and mortality. In this research review, we present the evidence that internalizing conditions, including depression and anxiety, as well as psychological distress, suicidal thoughts and self‐harm, and fatal suicide, are considerably increasing in adolescent populations across many countries. Evidence indicates that increases are currently greatest in female adolescents. We present an epidemiological framework for evaluating the causes of these increases, and synthesize research on whether several established risk factors (e.g., age of pubertal transition and stressful life events) and novel risk factors (e.g., digital technology and social media) meet conditions necessary to be plausible causes of increases in adolescent internalizing conditions. We conclude that there are a multitude of potential causes of increases in adolescent internalizing conditions, outline evidence gaps including the lack of research on nonbinary and gender nonconforming populations, and recommend necessary prevention and intervention foci from a clinical and public health perspective.
Article
Full-text available
To understand human behavior, social scientists need people and data. In the last decade, Amazon’s Mechanical Turk (MTurk) emerged as a flexible, affordable, and reliable source of human participants and was widely adopted by academics. Yet despite MTurk’s utility, some have questioned whether researchers should continue using the platform on ethical grounds. The brunt of their concern is that people on MTurk are financially insecure, subject to abuse, and earn inhumane wages. We investigated these issues with two representative probability surveys of the U.S. MTurk population (N = 4094). The surveys revealed: (1) the financial situation of people on MTurk mirrors the general population, (2) most participants do not find MTurk stressful or requesters abusive, and (3) MTurk offers flexibility and benefits that most people value above other options for work. People reported it is possible to earn more than 10perhourandsaidtheywouldnottradetheflexibilityofMTurkforlessthan10 per hour and said they would not trade the flexibility of MTurk for less than 25 per hour. Altogether, our data are important for assessing whether MTurk is an ethical place for research.
Article
Full-text available
Critiques of gender/sex measurement tend to focus on the questions researchers ask, including their binaristic, static nature, or overfocus on/erasure of transgender/cisgender status. The questions matter, as does the way gender/sex responses may be categorized, which has received less focus. In this article, we report on the “Gender/Sex 3×3,” which we developed via two studies to produce a novel framework for conceptualizing, measuring, and categorizing gender/sex. It represents two intersecting dimensions: “gender trajectory” (cisgender/transgender/allogender [i.e., neither cisgender nor transgender]) and “binary relation” (binary/nonbinary/allobinary [i.e., neither binary nor nonbinary]). In Study 1, we created gender/sex questions structured by the Gender/Sex 3×3 and asked 737 gender/sex-diverse participants to directly evaluate them. Descriptive quantitative feedback indicated the comprehensibility and inclusivity of the questions; qualitative feedback suggested wording changes. In Study 2, we tested these revised questions with 317 gender/sex-diverse participants and again found high levels of comprehensibility and inclusivity. We conclude by providing recommendations for questions about gender/sex that are inclusive of all nine locations in the Gender/Sex 3×3 and useful for flexible categorizations of gender/sexes.
Article
Full-text available
Attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) – and its underlying behavioral dimensions of inattention and hyperactivity–impulsivity – have been understudied in females. We first cover the conceptual issues of prevalence, diagnostic practices, diversity, comorbidity, and causal factors, plus forces limiting awareness of ADHD in females. After a narrative review of cross‐sectional and longitudinal findings, we conclude the following. (a) Girls meet diagnostic criteria for ADHD at just under half the rates of boys, a ratio that becomes much closer to equal by adulthood. (b) Girls and women with ADHD show a predominance of inattention and associated internalizing problems; boys and men display greater levels of hyperactive–impulsive symptoms and associated externalizing problems. (c) Sex differences in ADHD symptoms and related outcomes depend heavily on the clinical versus nonreferred nature of the samples under investigation. (d) Females with ADHD experience, on average, serious impairments, with a particularly heightened risk for problems in close relationships and engagement in self‐harm. (e) Clinicians may overlook symptoms and impairments in females because of less overt (but still impairing) symptom manifestations in girls and women and their frequent adoption of compensatory strategies. Our review of predictors and mediators of adult outcomes highlights (a) the potential for heterotypically continuous pathways in females with childhood ADHD and (b) developmental progressions to self‐harm, intimate partner violence, unplanned pregnancy, and comorbid psychopathology. Focusing on ADHD in females is necessary to characterize causal and maintaining mechanisms with accuracy and to foster responsive interventions, as highlighted in our closing list of clinical implications and research priorities.
Article
Full-text available
Introduction: Mental disorders represent serious public health concerns in the U.S. Compared with Whites, racial/ethnic minority adolescents are more likely to be affected by mental disorders but less likely to use mental health services. This systematic review aimed to summarize factors related to mental health service use among minority adolescents in the U.S. as identified in previous research. Methodology: Following the PRISMA guideline, we systematically searched seven databases for peer reviewed articles related to barriers and facilitators of mental health service use among racial/ethnic minority adolescents. Results: Thirty-two quantitative studies met our inclusion criteria, among which 12 studies (37.5%) sampled mostly Blacks or African Americans, 6 studies (18.7%) focused primarily on Hispanics or Latin/a/x, including Mexican Americans and Puerto Ricans, and 4 studies (12.5%) were mostly Asian Americans (e.g., Chinese, Vietnamese). Based on the socio-ecological framework, 21 studies (65.6%) identified adolescent-related barriers and facilitators of mental health service use, including biological (e.g., age, gender), clinical (e.g., symptom severity), behavioral (e.g., drug/alcohol use), and psychological characteristics (e.g., internal asset) of minority youth. Ten studies (31.3%) identified parents-related factors that influenced minority adolescent mental health service use, including parental perceptions and beliefs, family and parenting issues, and demographic characteristics. Primary factors at the therapist level included ethnic match between patient and practitioner, relationship with healthcare practitioners, and patient-therapist co-endorsement of etiological beliefs. Fifteen studies (46.9%) identified factors influencing minority adolescent mental health service use at the contextual/structural level, including household income, insurance status, and family structure. Lastly, acculturation and school experiences were major factors at the social/cultural level that influence minority adolescent service use. Conclusion: More empirical studies are needed to understand the mechanism underlying minority adolescents' unmet mental health service needs. Culturally competent interventions are warranted to engage minority adolescents with mental disorders into treatment.
Article
Full-text available
Transgender and gender diverse individuals experience high rates of health disparities, as compared with their cisgender (non-transgender) counterparts. One area in which these disparities is most grave is that of mental health, with some studies indicating transgender and gender diverse individuals as having a 40% rate of lifetime suicide attempts and similarly high rates of depression, anxiety, and suicidal ideation. These rates vary further within this population, with differential rates seen across sociodemographic factors, including race/ethnicity, gender, sexual orientation, disability status, education level, and income. This study explores mental health experiences across different social identities, using data from the 2018 Michigan Trans Health Survey (N = 659), a community-based participatory action research project collected in Michigan, United States, analyzed using chi-square tests of independence and logistic regressions. Findings indicate incredibly high rates of mental health concerns; 72.2% had been diagnosed with depression in their lifetime and 73.0% had been diagnosed with anxiety in their lifetime. In the past year, 49.9% had had non-suicidal self-injury (NSSI) thoughts, 45.4% had suicidal thoughts, 26.3% engaged in NSSI, and 7.7% had attempted suicide. Bivariate regressions showed some nuanced experiences of rates of mental health diagnoses and experiences, such as greater odds of experiencing all mental health disparities among those with disabilities, and differential odds across gender in regard to ever having a depression diagnosis, non-suicidal self-injury thoughts and engaging in non-suicidal self-injury behavior. This indicates a need for social workers, counselors, therapists, and other human services professionals to act more intentionally and with an intersectional lens when it comes to exploring the mental health of transgender and gender diverse persons.
Article
Full-text available
Autism spectrum disorders (ASD) and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) can compromise health and may be more prevalent amongst individuals with gender dysphoria (GD). Symptoms such as attention or social difficulties can impact assessment of GD, understanding of health information, and engagement in clinical care. To ensure neurodevelopmental conditions are adequately considered in gender health services, we aimed to systematically review the literature examining the prevalence of ASD and ADHD amongst individuals with GD. In this systematic review based on the PRISMA guidelines. MEDLINE and PsycINFO databases were searched for studies examining the prevalence of ASD and/or ADHD in individuals with GD or investigated the rate of GD in cohorts with ASD or ADHD. All English peer-reviewed publications were included. The search strategy identified 179 studies. After applying exclusion criteria, a total of 30 studies were identified, 22 studies which examined the prevalence of ASD or ADHD in people with GD. A further 8 studies examined the reverse; prevalence of GD in people with ASD. The few studies employing diagnostic criteria for ASD suggest a prevalence of 6–26% in transgender populations, higher than the general population, but no different from individuals attending psychiatry clinics. Few studies examine prevalence of ADHD. Low-level evidence exists to suggest a link between ASD and GD. Further population-based and controlled studies using diagnostic criteria for ASD and ADHD are required.
Article
Psychology researchers have historically neglected variables related to sex, gender, and sexual orientation, leading to the erasure of sex, gender, and sexual orientation in research, which limits the generalizability of psychological findings. We argue that these important variables need to be considered more consistently by researchers across psychology subdisciplines. In Study 1 we found that 15.1% of a large MTurk sample (i.e., 8500+) identified as a sexual or gender minority (SGM; e.g., lesbian, gay, bisexual, transgender, queer [LGBTQ+]). In addition, data from Study 1 showed that our youngest cohort (i.e., aged 18-25 years) reported significantly higher rates of LGBTQ+ identification (22.7%) than our oldest cohort (i.e., 65-84 years; 1.3%), suggesting that endorsement of these idnetities is increasing. Next, in Study 2 we found that psychology researchers (N = 135) tended to rate expansive sex, gender, and sexual orientation demographic variables as important in general, but were much less likely to report actually using these variables in their own studies. Moreover, younger faculty and faculty who identified as women rated these variables as more important than their colleagues. Based on our findings, we conclude that psychology researchers should use expansive sex, gender, and sexual orientation items in their studies, report these demographic variables consistently, and analyze their data by these important variables when possible. Because a substantial and growing proportion of individuals identify as LGBTQ+, and because SGM identity is related to additional life stressors, it is imperative to better understand these individuals. Various resources are offered and challenges are discussed.
Article
Mental health disparities research compares groups of individuals with regard to their mental health status, and numerous studies have reported significant and burdensome disparities across populations. This literature is based almost entirely on traditional mental disorder constructs (e.g. major depressive disorder and borderline personality disorder) as defined by official nosologies, but these disorders are associated with numerous problems of reliability and validity. Advances in psychopathology classification research have converged on an alternative conceptualization of mental disorder structure, which comprises a set of transdiagnostic dimensions that cut across traditional diagnostic boundaries and overcome the limitations of traditional diagnoses. The application of these dimensions, particularly those of the hierarchical taxonomy of psychopathology model, holds great promise for mental health disparities research. Measurement considerations associated with these transdiagnostic dimensions, as well as participants' intersecting identities, are discussed. Incorporation of these measurement advances with statistical advances allows for the generation and testing of unique hypotheses related to minority stressors that may give rise to observed disparities.