ArticlePDF Available

The face recall and face recognition: Description accuracy does not indicate eyewitness identification accuracy

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Eyewitness testimonies are frequently considered crucial in criminal investigations. The impact of the initial description provided by an eyewitness on subsequent identification accuracy remains an understudied area. We employed a slightly modified version of Schooler and Engstler-Schooler's experimental approach to assess whether description quantity and quality are related to lineup identification accuracy. The sample comprised 99 undergraduate students from the University of Sarajevo. More than 50% of our participants made an accurate lineup identification, whereas they recalled and described only the general physical characteristics with minimal detail. No significant relationship between the quantity/quality of descriptions and identification accuracy was observed in terms of the total number of words, total number of details in the description, and individual characteristics that participants mentioned in the description. In other words, neither an extensive nor a detailed description effectively predicts lineup identification accuracy. Finally, our findings suggest that the recognition task is significantly less challenging for participants than the recall and description tasks because descriptions provide a general impression of an individual and lack detailed information. The findings of this study indicate that a more detailed and extensive description does not serve as a reliable indicator of lineup identification accuracy.
Content may be subject to copyright.
NBP 2025, Vol. 30, Issue 2, pp.
https://doi.org/10.5937/nabepo30-54463
Original scientic paper
eISSN 2620-0406
Citation: Fazlić, A., & Deljkić, I. (2025). Face recall and face recognition:
Description accuracy does not indicate eyewitness identication accuracy. NBP.
Nauka, bezbednost, policija, 30(2), pp . https://doi.org/10.5937/nabepo30-54463
The Face Recall and Face Recognition: Description Accuracy
Does Not Indicate Eyewitness Identication Accuracy
Adnan Fazlić1, Irma Deljkić2
University of Sarajevo, Faculty of Criminal Justice and Security Studies
Bosnia and Herzegovina
Submitted: 2024-10-30 • Accepted: 2025-01-19 • Published: 2025-02-03
Abstract: Eyewitness testimonies are frequently considered crucial in criminal investigations.
e impact of the initial description provided by an eyewitness on subsequent identication ac-
curacy remains an understudied area. We employed a slightly modied version of Schooler and
Engstler-Schooler’s experimental approach to assess whether description quantity and quality are
related to lineup identication accuracy. e sample comprised 99 undergraduate students from
the University of Sarajevo. More than 50% of our participants made an accurate lineup identica-
tion, whereas they recalled and described only the general physical characteristics with minimal
detail. No signicant relationship between the quantity/quality of descriptions and identication
accuracy was observed in terms of the total number of words, total number of details in the de-
scription, and individual characteristics that participants mentioned in the description. In other
words, neither an extensive nor a detailed description eectively predicts lineup identication
accuracy. Finally, our ndings suggest that the recognition task is signicantly less challenging
for participants than the recall and description tasks because descriptions provide a general im-
pression of an individual and lack detailed information. e ndings of this study indicate that
a more detailed and extensive description does not serve as a reliable indicator of lineup identi-
cation accuracy.
Keywords: perpetrator description, lineup identication accuracy, eyewitness identication, eye-
witness testimony.
INTRODUCTION
Eyewitness testimony is frequently considered crucial in criminal investigations and may
occasionally be the only evidence for identifying perpetrators (Wells & Olson, 2003).
Eyewitness statements are commonly used as compelling evidence and historically re-
garded as the gold standard of proof (Ling et al., 2021; Albright & Garrett, 2022). Howev-
er, practitioners generally consider this evidence inaccurate and unreliable, particularly
1 Corresponding author: afazlic@n.unsa.ba • https://orcid.org/0000-0002-4419-1828 • Phone: +387 61
38 22 66
2 ideljkic@n.unsa.ba • https://orcid.org/0000-0001-5699-6095
ONLINE FIRST
NBP. Nauka, bezbednost, policija
2
NBP 2025, Vol. 30, Issue 2, pp.
for eyewitness identication (Wells, 2020). e importance of eyewitness testimony has
led to numerous studies showing that various factors can aect perpetrator description
accuracy (e.g. Berkowitz et al., 2020; Lockamyeir et al., 2020; Anakwah et al., 2020; Marr
et al., 2021; Baić et al., 2022), as well as eyewitness identication accuracy (Fazlić et al.,
2020; Bull Kovera & Evelo, 2020; Seale-Carlisle et al., 2022; Wixted & Wells, 2017). ese
ndings have resulted in best practices for conducting eyewitness interviews and line-
ups to maximise the reliability of eyewitness evidence (Wells et al., 2020). Despite these
advancements, the impact of the initial description provided by an eyewitness on subse-
quent identication accuracy remains an understudied area, warranting further research
(Fahsig et al., 2004).
Police ocers routinely collect detailed suspect descriptions from eyewitnesses, covering
attributes such as age, sex, race, height, physique, and distinctive features (Launay et al.,
2021). ese descriptions help to narrow the range of potential suspects and guide line-
ups (Mickes, 2016). However, the verbal overshadowing eect indicates that describing
a perpetrator can impair memory and reduce identication accuracy (Schooler & Engs-
tler-Schooler, 1990; Meissner & Brigham, 2001; Alogna et al., 2014). Some studies have
replicated this eect (e.g. Marmurek et al., 2021; Bacharach & Baker, 2024), while others
have not or found a reversal (Sauerland et al., 2008; Vredeveldt et al., 2015). Despite these
mixed ndings, researchers continue to explore ways to mitigate the potential negative
eects of verbal descriptions on eyewitness memory, while preserving their investigative
value.
Building on these conicting results, researchers have begun to apply alternative ap-
proaches to enquire about the inuence of verbal descriptions on eyewitness memory.
In analysing post-identication statements, two factors – description quality and amount
of detail – can predict identication accuracy with 70% identication accuracy (Short
& Dalby, 2007). Despite divergent ndings (e.g. Meissner et al., 2007; Demarchi & Py,
2009; Pozzulo et al., 2009; Sheahan et al., 2017; Areh & Walsh, 2020; Handler & Frühholz,
2021), research shows that quantity and quality of description do not consistently aect
identication accuracy. Also, Pozzulo et al. (2013) found that in this respect there are
no dierences between adults and adolescents (Generation Z). Inconsistent relationships
between descriptive characteristics and identication accuracy impede the assessment of
eyewitness reliability. e inuence of verbal descriptions on accuracy remains complex
and may depend on various factors.
is study explored the link between the quantity and quality of verbal descriptions and
eyewitness identication accuracy, with the understanding that recalling and recognising
human faces may involve dierent cognitive processes (Wells, 1984). As the rst of its kind
in Bosnia and Herzegovina, it aims to: (1) assess the quantity and quality of the description
and its relationship to the accuracy of the lineup identication; (2) explore whether the
eyewitness description features (quantity and quality of description) inuence their ability
to identify a suspect in a lineup accurately; (3) compare this study’s ndings globally, ex-
ploring cultural socializations impact on memory processes and generational dierences,
particularly the cognitive challenges digitalization poses to Generation Z.
ONLINE FIRST
NBP. Nauka, bezbednost, policija
3
NBP 2025, Vol. 30, Issue 2, pp.
METHODS
E    
e present study employed a slightly modied experimental design from Schooler and
Engstler-Schooler (1990), which comprised a series of six experiments. eir ndings
demonstrated a detrimental eect of verbal description on subsequent identication of
a target face. is phenomenon is referred to as verbal overshadowing. More specically,
the present study replicates the experimental (face verbalisation) conditions of School-
er and Engstler-Schooler’s Experiment 2. Using a between-subjects experimental design
with counterbalancing, this study explored the impact of eyewitness description quality
and quantity (independent variables) on eyewitness identication accuracy (dependent
variable). is experimental design is used to assess whether the quantity and quality of
descriptions inuence the accuracy of eyewitness identication, whilst controlling for or-
der eects by counterbalancing the order of description and the ller task. It comprised
two experiments, each consisting of four steps. In Experiment 1, participants rst watched
a 44-second mock bank robbery video and then spent ve minutes describing the robber.
Next, they completed a 20-minute ller task (an easy crossword puzzle), and nally, they
identied the robber from a photo of eight people. Experiment 2 swapped the second
(ve-minute description) and third (20-minute ller task) steps. e study used the origi-
nal Schooler and Engstler-Schooler’s (1990) critical video and test photos, but the instruc-
tions and ller task were in the Bosnian language.
P
e sample comprised undergraduate students from the University of Sarajevo – UNSA
(N = 99; 57 – male, 42 – female), with a mean age of 19.14 years (AM = 19.14 years; SD =
.808) who participated individually but in groups of up to ten (for course credit). All the
participants were rst-year undergraduate students. Participants were randomly assigned
to two experiments: Experiment 1 (N = 49) and Experiment 2 (N = 50). e characteristics
of the two groups are presented in Table 1. e experimental groups were homogeneous in
terms of sex, year of study, and age. Informed consent was obtained from all participants.
Table 1. Characteristics of the Participants
Variable Category Tota l Experiment 1 Experiment 2
N % N % N %
Gender Male 57 57.6 26 53.1 32 62.0
Age
Female 43 42.4 23 46.9 19 38.0
M 19.14 18.94 19.34
SD .808 .659 .895
Min 18 18 18
Max 22 21 22
Note. M – mean; SD – standard deviation; Min – minimum value; Max – maximum value; N – number of respondents.
ONLINE FIRST
NBP. Nauka, bezbednost, policija
4
NBP 2025, Vol. 30, Issue 2, pp.
M  P
e procedure for conducting Experiment 1 necessitated that the participants, before
commencement, be informed that the experiment comprised several tasks. Initially, they
were presented with a 44-second video depicting a simulated robbery. Subsequently, the
participants were requested to describe the appearance of the perpetrator and provide
their responses in written form on a specically designed document. A ve-minute du-
ration was allocated for the completion of these tasks. Aer three minutes, participants
in this experiment were instructed in all instances to continue describing to provide a
more comprehensive description. e second task commenced upon the expiration of the
time allocated to the aforementioned task. is task involved solving crossword puzzles
for which each participant received a copy of the crossword form. e duration allocated
for this task was 20 minutes. Upon completion of the previously mentioned task, the par-
ticipants performed the nal task, which involved identifying the person from the video
in a set of photographs. Accordingly, eight photographs (1–8) of potential perpetrators
from the video in the rst task were presented to the participants via a monitor or projec-
tor. Participants were instructed to circle one number from 1 to 8, corresponding to the
photograph that they believed depicted the perpetrator. Additionally, if any participant
believed that the perpetrator was not represented in any of the photographs, they had the
option of circling the number 9.
e procedure for Experiment 2 involved the same tasks and periods for their comple-
tion; however, this procedure included a permutation of the second (describing the per-
petrator) and third (solving the crosswords) tasks from Experiment 1. e nal phase of
Experiment 2’s procedure was identical to that outlined in the procedure for Experiment
1 and involved identifying the perpetrator from one of the eight presented photographs.
Attributes from the verbal descriptions were extracted and coded by a single researcher.
e coder was familiar with the coding procedure, study objectives, and specic criteria
for evaluating the quality and quantity of descriptions. Each description was coded ac-
cording to the established criteria (1 = correct, 2 = partly correct, 3 = incorrect) to ensure
consistency in the evaluation of the quantity and quality of the descriptions. To ensure
coding reliability and consistency, despite having one coder, several measures were imple-
mented. ese included recoding a subset of descriptions (approximately 10–20%) by the
same individual aer a time period to evaluate temporal consistency. e coder regularly
examined the rubric to maintain adherence to coding criteria and reduce potential bias.
S 
Initial data entry into the database preserved the original form of the participants’ respons-
es. ey were then classied into various distinct groups based on the perpetrator’s char-
acteristics, including hair, forehead, ears, eyebrows, eyes, nose, facial hair, mouth shape,
jaw shape, clothes, gender, build, race, face shape, and teeth. Following this, each category
of individual description for which information was gathered was evaluated against the
actual description of the perpetrator.
Responses were categorised according to their level of accuracy in describing the oend-
er’s actual appearance. A correct description was assigned a code of 1, a partly correct de-
ONLINE FIRST
NBP. Nauka, bezbednost, policija
5
NBP 2025, Vol. 30, Issue 2, pp.
scription was given a code of 2, and an incorrect description was coded as 3. Participants
who failed to provide any description were deemed unable to characterise certain aspects
of the oender and were omitted from the analysis. In the photo identication lineup, the
correct position is 6.
Statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics (Version 21.0) (IBM Corp,
2012). Descriptive statistics for each variable/category was calculated, including the mean,
standard deviation (SD), minimum (Min), and maximum (Max). e analysis focused on
the descriptions provided by participants regarding the individual depicted in the video.
Specically, the term “words pertaining to facial features” refers to all words used to de-
scribe the facial appearance of the person shown, regardless of whether the description
was accurate. To assess the number of correct details, a exible approach was adopted. For
example, since the individual in the video had dark brown hair and eyes, any description
approximating the hair colour (e.g., “dark hair”, “dark brown hair”, “black hair”, “brown
hair”) or eye colour (e.g., “dark eyes, “brown eyes”) was considered correct. In contrast,
incorrect descriptions were those that did not reasonably describe the persons appearance
(e.g., “light hair”, “blonde hair”, “grey hair”, “blue eyes”, “green eyes”). is approach was
consistently applied when analysing the descriptions in both experiments.
An independent samples t-test was used to compare the mean values of the quantity of
descriptions between the groups of participants who made accurate and inaccurate lineup
identications. In this context, the mean values of the total number of words in the de-
scription, the total number of words in the description pertaining to the facial features,
the total number of correct details in the description, and the total number of incorrect
details in the description were considered. Additionally, a chi-square test of independence
was used to examine the relationship between the accuracy of the description and lineup
identication.
RESULTS
Descriptive statistical analysis was specically directed towards the quantication of de-
scriptions in terms of four key parameters: a) the number of words in the description, b)
the number of words pertaining to facial features, c) the number of accurate details, and d)
the number of inaccurate details. e results revealed that, on average, participants used
35.1 words to describe the individual, with 18.1 words, on average, dedicated to the facial
characteristics of the person. In terms of accuracy, the mean number of correct details
was 7.6, while the mean number of incorrect details was 10.5. ese ndings indicate that
approximately half of the words in the descriptions were related to facial characteristics.
Moreover, the data suggest that participants tended to include slightly more incorrect de-
tails than correct ones in their descriptions (see Table 2).
ONLINE FIRST
NBP. Nauka, bezbednost, policija
6
NBP 2025, Vol. 30, Issue 2, pp.
Table 2. Quantitative Analysis of the Description
Description quantity MSD Min Max
e number of words in the description 35.1 18.9 6 79
e number of words in the description
pertaining to the facial features 18.1 12.1 0 60
e number of accurate details 7.6 5.3 0 37
e number of inaccurate details 10.5 9.3 0 50
Note. M – mean; SD – standard deviation; Min – minimum value; Max – maximum value.
Furthermore, as part of the analysis of the descriptions obtained in the experiment, atten-
tion was also given to the facial features most frequently included in the descriptions by
participants from the experimental groups. e analysis revealed that participants most
oen described the following ve characteristics: gender, hair, facial hair, clothing, and
eyes (see Table 3).
Table 3. Description Accuracy
Variable
Frequency Accuracy (%)
N % Correct Partly
correct Incorrect
Hair 89 89.9 39.3 58.4 2.2
Forehead 6 6.1 66.7 33.3 0.0
Ears 3 3.0 0.0 33.3 66.7
Eyebrows 15 15.2 40.0 40.0 20.0
Eyes 41 41.4 58.5 31.7 9.8
Nose 14 14.1 14.3 50.0 35.7
Facial hair 83 83.8 50.6 42.2 7.2
Mouth shape 5 5.1 60.0 20.0 20.0
Jaw shape 2 2.0 50.0 0.0 50.0
Clothes 81 81.8 11.1 74.1 14.8
Gender 97 98.0 100.0 0.0
Build 12 12.1 58.3 8.3 33.3
Race 6 6.1 83.3 0.0 16.7
Face shape 9 9.1 11.1 44.4 44.4
Teeth 0 0
Note. N = number of descriptions including corresponding attribute.
ONLINE FIRST
NBP. Nauka, bezbednost, policija
7
NBP 2025, Vol. 30, Issue 2, pp.
An independent samples t-test was used to compare the total word count in descriptions
with the lineup identication accuracy. e dierence in word count between participants
who made accurate identications (M = 31.9; SD = 18.1) and those who made inaccurate
identications (M = 38.4; SD = 19.3) was not statistically signicant (t [97] = –1.72, p >
0.05). Similarly, no signicant dierence was found in the word count for facial charac-
teristics between the accurate (M = 17.7; SD =12.9) and inaccurate groups (M = 18.6;
SD = 11.3) (t [97] = –.37, p > 0.05). For the qualitative aspect of the verbal description, no
signicant dierence was found in the correct details (t [97] = –1.06, p > 0.05) between
those who accurately identied the target (M = 7.1; SD = 5.8) and those who did not
(M = 8.2; SD = 4.9). Additionally, there was no signicant dierence in incorrect details
(t [97] = .09, p > 0.05; correct identication: M = 10.6; SD = 9.9; incorrect identication:
M = 10.4; SD = 8.7). ese results indicate that the quantity and quality of verbal descrip-
tions did not dier signicantly between accurate and inaccurate identications.
e statistical relationship between the description and lineup identication accuracy was
subsequently examined. Specically, a chi-square test of independence was conducted to
ascertain whether correct, partly correct, or incorrect descriptions of certain characteris-
tics could serve as indicators of correct or incorrect identication in the lineup. e results
did not reveal a statistically signicant relationship (Table 4).
Table 4. Relationship Between the Description and Lineup Identication Accuracy
Described
feature
Description
accuracy
Identication accuracy
Test of dierences
Total
Accurate
N (%)
Inaccurate
N (%)
Hair
Correct 19 (21.3) 16 (18.0)
χ2(2) = 2.23, p = .33Partly correct 27 (30.3) 25 (28.1)
Incorrect 2 (2.2)
Forehead
Correct 2 (33.3) 2 (33.3)
χ2(1) = 0.94, p = .76Partly correct 2 (33.3)
Incorrect
Ears
Correct
a
Partly correct 1 (33.3)
Incorrect 2 (66.7)
Eyebrows
Correct 1 (6.7) 5 (33.3)
χ2(2) = 5.97, p = .05Partly correct 2 (13.3) 4 (26.7)
Incorrect 3 (20.0)
Eyes
Correct 11 (26.8) 13 (31.7)
χ2(2) = 1.65, p = .44Partly correct 5 (12.2) 8 (19.5)
Incorrect 3 (7.3) 1 (2.4)
ONLINE FIRST
NBP. Nauka, bezbednost, policija
8
NBP 2025, Vol. 30, Issue 2, pp.
Nose
Correct 1 (7.1) 1 (7.1)
χ2(2) = 0.58, p = .97Partly correct 3 (21.4) 4 (28.6)
Incorrect 2 (14.3) 3 (21.4)
Facial hair
Correct 18 (21.7) 24 (28.9)
χ2(2) = 1.48, p = .48Partly correct 19 (22.9) 16 (19.3)
Incorrect 2 (2.4) 4 (4.8)
Mouth shape
Correct 1 (20.0) 2 (40.0)
χ2(2) = 2.22, p = .33Partly correct 1 (20.0) 0 (0.0)
Incorrect 1 (20.0)
Jaw shape
Correct 1 (50.0)
χ2(1) = 0.00, p = 1.00Partly correct
Incorrect 1 (50.0)
Clothes
Correct 4 (4.9) 5 (6.2)
χ2(2) = 2.22, p = .33
Partly correct 29 (35.8) 31 (38.3)
Incorrect 8 (9.9) 4 (4.9)
Gender Correct 48 (49.5) 49 (50.5) a
Incorrect
Build
Correct 5 (41.7) 2 (16.7)
χ2(2) = 2.01, p = .37Partly correct 1 (8.3)
Incorrect 2 (16.7) 2 (16.7)
Race
Correct 1 (16.7) 4 (66.7)
χ2(1) = 0.00, p = 1.00Partly correct
Incorrect 1 (16.7)
Face shape
Correct 1 (11.1)
χ2(2) = 1.12, p = .57Partly correct 2 (22.2) 2 (22.2)
Incorrect 3 (33.3) 1 (11.1)
Note. N – number of respondents; a – the lack of variability in respondents’ answers precluded the possibility of
examining discrepancies in the frequency of answers in the identication accuracy.
No statistically signicant relationship between the description and lineup identication
accuracy was observed, even when the results were analysed through the lenses of Experi-
ments 1 and 2. e temporal interval between description and photo lineup identication,
as well as the delay between viewing the video and providing the description, did not
demonstrate a statistically signicant inuence on the outcomes.
ONLINE FIRST
NBP. Nauka, bezbednost, policija
9
NBP 2025, Vol. 30, Issue 2, pp.
DISCUSSION
e primary nding of the present study indicates that participants found the human
face recognition task to be easier than the recall and description tasks. is observation
supports the idea that face recall and face recognition are distinct cognitive processes
with diering demands. While recall requires the active generation and retrieval of facial
identity, recognition relies on a familiarity judgment of the currently viewed face (Grin
& Motta-Mena, 2021). ese results rearm that the human face is a highly informative,
complex, and non-verbal stimulus. Describing a face involves recalling detailed features
and translating them into words, a process that is cognitively demanding and constrained
by memory capacity. In contrast, human faces are processed holistically, meaning they are
perceived as unied wholes rather than as a collection of individual features. is holistic
processing enables the brain to quickly and subconsciously assess familiarity, making rec-
ognition easier. Unlike recall, recognition relies on matching a face to an existing mem-
ory, even when that memory lacks precise details. In the present study, just over 50% of
participants accurately identied the perpetrator in the lineup, while their descriptions
contained only general physical characteristics with minimal detail. Given this relatively
low descriptive detail and the fact that participants were describing an unfamiliar face, it
is reasonable to infer that they relied on relative judgment processing rather than absolute
processing during the lineup identication task (Wells, 1984). is suggests that recogni-
tion tasks engage dierent and less eortful cognitive processes compared to recall and
description tasks.
e perpetrator descriptions in our study primarily reect a general impression, with only
a small portion of the descriptions focusing on details specically related to the perpetra-
tor’s identity. is nding complements previous research (e.g., Fahsig et al., 2004; Pozzu-
lo et al., 2018). Despite a slightly increased word count in the descriptions, the additional
quantity did not result in higher-quality or more detailed descriptions. ese results are
consistent with prior studies showing no correlation between the quantity and quality of
descriptions (e.g., Demarchi & Py, 2009). Importantly, our ndings suggest that neither
the extensiveness nor the detail of a description signicantly inuences lineup identica-
tion accuracy. is lack of relationship between the quantity or quality of descriptions and
identication accuracy persists both in terms of the total number of words and the specic
characteristics mentioned in the descriptions. ese ndings may be explained by the
verbal overshadowing eect (Schooler & Engstler-Schooler, 1990), which occurs when de-
scribing a human face impairs subsequent face recognition, irrespective of the quality or
quantity of the description provided. Additionally, the attractiveness and distinctiveness
of a face may play a signicant role in face recognition. For example, Yamaguchi and Sug-
imori (2024) found that attractive and distinctive faces are remembered and recognized
more eectively than faces lacking these traits. In our study, participants did not comment
on the perceived attractiveness or distinctiveness of the perpetrator’s face. Future research
should incorporate these factors to better understand their impact on both facial descrip-
tions and identication accuracy.
e cultural background of witnesses plays a signicant role in shaping memory reports
(Anakwah et al., 2020). erefore, the ndings of our study were interpreted within the
cultural context of Bosnia and Herzegovina. While there is no consensus in the literature,
ONLINE FIRST
NBP. Nauka, bezbednost, policija
10
NBP 2025, Vol. 30, Issue 2, pp.
some studies suggest that Bosnia and Herzegovina retains the elements of collectivism, a
legacy of its socialist past in the former Yugoslavia, distinguishing it from predominantly
individualistic cultures in Europe (Klarin et al., 2012). Based on this, we hypothesized
that the results of our study might exhibit unique cultural characteristics. However, our
ndings did not dier signicantly from the studies conducted in individualistic cultures.
It is important to note that our participants were the members of Generation Z, and fu-
ture research should explore potential generational dierences, particularly between older
generations and Generation Z in Bosnia and Herzegovina.
e generational aspect was also considered in interpreting our ndings. Previous re-
search indicates that Generation Z, as digital natives, may possess specic cognitive traits
(Szymkowiak et al., 2021). We hypothesised that participants from this generation might
display unique patterns in their responses. However, our results align with those of Poz-
zulo et al. (2013), particularly regarding the average number of details in descriptions and
the characteristics most frequently mentioned (e.g., clothing, hair). Similarly, earlier stud-
ies involving older generations (e.g., Kuehn, 1974) reported comparable results, suggest-
ing that generational aliation may not signicantly inuence eyewitness descriptions.
is study has notable limitations that must be addressed. One limitation is the experi-
mental structure, particularly the shorter time delays compared to real-life scenarios. In
police investigations, eyewitness identication oen occurs aer substantial temporal de-
lays – measured in days, weeks, or even months – between the crime, the description, and
the lineup identication. In our study, all participants provided descriptions and complet-
ed identications within 25 minutes of viewing the video. While our analysis found no sig-
nicant eect of the time interval, these short delays do not reect real-world conditions,
leaving open the possibility that results might dier with longer, more realistic intervals.
Additionally, the method of data collection poses a limitation. Unlike police interviews,
our study did not involve questioning participants to elicit descriptions, which may have
inuenced the level of detail provided. Future research should address these limitations
by incorporating longer time delays and methods more closely aligned with actual police
practices. Such adjustments could oer deeper insights into the reliability and accuracy of
eyewitness descriptions and identications in real-world contexts.
CONCLUSIONS
is study provides valuable insights into the cognitive processes underlying face recall
and recognition and highlights their distinct demands. e ndings revealed that face
recognition tasks are signicantly easier than recall and description tasks, supporting the
notion that these are separate cognitive processes. Human faces, as complex non-verbal
stimuli, are processed holistically, enabling rapid familiarity assessments during recogni-
tion. Participants in the experiment appeared to rely on relative judgment processing dur-
ing lineup identication, engaging in less eortful cognitive strategies than those required
for recall and description tasks.
e study also found no signicant inuence of the quantity or quality of descriptions on
lineup identication accuracy. Although conducted in Bosnia and Herzegovina with Gen-
eration Z participants, the ndings did not dier signicantly from those of the studies
ONLINE FIRST
NBP. Nauka, bezbednost, policija
11
NBP 2025, Vol. 30, Issue 2, pp.
conducted in other cultures or older generations. is suggests that cultural and gener-
ational factors may have less of an impact on eyewitness descriptions and identications
than previously assumed.
ese ndings have signicant implications for law enforcement and the criminal justice
system, emphasising the complex nature of eyewitness testimony. ese results suggest
that caution should be exercised when using detailed witness descriptions as predictors of
lineup identication accuracy. Instead, such descriptions may be utilised more eectively
to narrow the pool of suspects and construct lineups. Understanding the limitations of
eyewitness accounts and their underlying cognitive processes is critical for developing
more eective eyewitness identication procedures. is approach has the potential to
enhance the accuracy of suspect identication and reduce the risk of wrongful convictions
resulting from unreliable eyewitness testimonies.
Despite the limitations mentioned earlier, this study advances our understanding of the
cognitive processes involved in face recognition, recall, and description, as well as their
implications for eyewitness testimony. Future research should explore factors such as face
attractiveness, distinctiveness, and potential cultural and generational dierences in larger
and more diverse samples. Such eorts will rene understanding of these complex pro-
cesses and enhance their practical applications in investigative contexts.
ACKNOWLEDGMENT
Authors declare that they have no conicts of interest.
REFERENCES
Albright, T. D., & Garrett, B. L. (2022). e law and science of eyewitness evidence. Boston
University Law Review, 102(2), 511–630. http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3675055
Alogna, V. K., Attaya, M. K., Aucoin, P., Bahník, Š., Birch, S., Birt, A. R., Bornstein, B. H.,
Bouwmeester, S., Brandimonte, M. A., Brown, C., Buswell, K., Carlson, C., Carlson, M., Chu,
S., Cislak, A., Colarusso, M., Collo, M. F., Dellapaolera, K. S., Delvenne, J.-F., … Zwaan, R.
A. (2014). Registered replication report: Schooler and Engstler-Schooler (1990). Perspec-
tives on Psychological Science, 9(5), 556–578. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691614545653
Anakwah, N., Horselenberg, R., Hope, L., Amankwah-Poku, M., & van Koppen, P. J.
(2020). Cross-cultural dierences in eyewitness memory reports. Applied Cognitive Psy-
chology, 34(2), 504–515. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3637
Areh, I., & Walsh, D. (2020). Own-gender bias may aect eyewitness accuracy of perpe-
trators’ personal descriptions. Revija za kriminalistiko in kriminologijo, 71(4), 247–255.
http://dx.doi.org/10.6084/m9.gshare.13703806
Bacharach, V. R., & Baker, M. A. (2024). Verbal overshadowing and decision criterion
eects on recognition memory for faces. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 36(8), 881–897.
https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2024.2419888
ONLINE FIRST
NBP. Nauka, bezbednost, policija
12
NBP 2025, Vol. 30, Issue 2, pp.
Baić, V., Oljača, M., & Tasić, M. (2022). e Inuence of the period of retention on the re-
liability of episodic memory in the context of testimony. NBP. Nauka, bezbednost, policija,
26(3), 7–24. https://doi.org/10.5937/nabepo26-35280
Berkowitz, S. R., Garrett, B. L., Fenn, K. M., & Lous, E. F. (2020). Convicting with con-
dence? Why we should not over-rely on eyewitness condence. Memory, 30(1), 10–15.
https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2020.1849308
Bull Kovera, M., & Evelo, A. J. (2021). Eyewitness identication in its social context. Journal
of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 10(3), 313–327. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
jarmac.2021.04.003
Demarchi, S., & Py, J. (2009). A method to enhance person description: A eld study. In
R. Bull, T. Valentine, & T. Williamson (Eds.), Handbook of psychology of investigative in-
terviewing: Current developments and future directions (pp. 241–256). John Wiley & Sons.
Fahsig, I. A., Ask, K., & Granhag, P. A. (2004). e man behind the mask: Accuracy and
predictors of eyewitness oender descriptions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89(4), 722–
729. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.89.4.722
Fazlić, A., Deljkić, I., & Bull, R. (2020). Gender eects regarding eyewitness identi-
cation performance. Criminal Justice Issues, 20(5), 31–41. https://doi.org/10.51235/
kt.2020.20.5.31
Grin, J. W., & Motta-Mena, N. V. (2021). Face recall. In T. K. Shackelford, & V. A.
Weekes-Shackelford (Eds.), Encyclopedia of evolutionary psychological science (pp. 2884–
2887). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-19650-3_3454
Handler, A., & Frühholz, S. (2021). Eyewitness memory for person identication: Pre-
dicting mugbook recognition accuracy according to person description abilities and sub-
jective condence of witnesses. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, Article 675956. https://doi.
org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.675956
IBM Corp. (2012). IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows (Version 21.0) [Computer soware].
IBM Corp. https://www.ibm.com/products/spss-statistics
Klarin, M., Pororoković, A., Šašić, S. Š., & Arnaudova, V. (2012). Some characteristics of
social interactions among adolescents in Croatia, Bosnia and Herzegovina, and Macedo-
nia. Psychology Research and Behavior Management, 5, 163–172. https://doi.org/10.2147/
PRBM.S36389
Kuehn, L. L. (1974). Looking down a gun barrel: Person perception and violent crime. Per-
ceptual and Motor Skills, 39(3), 1159–1164. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1974.39.3.1159
Launay, C., Py, J., Brunel, M., & Demarchi, S. (2021). Beyond investigation-relevant in-
formation: A content analysis of police questioning. Police Practice and Research, 22(4),
1341–1355. https://doi.org/10.1080/15614263.2020.1869002
Ling, S., Kaplan, J., & Berryessa, C. M. (2021). e importance of forensic evidence for
decisions on criminal guilt. Science & Justice, 61(2), 142–149. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
scijus.2020.11.004
Lockamyeir, R. F., Carlson, C. A., Jones, A. R., Carlson, M. A., & Weatherford, D. R. (2020).
e eect of viewing distance on empirical discriminability and the condence–accuracy
ONLINE FIRST
NBP. Nauka, bezbednost, policija
13
NBP 2025, Vol. 30, Issue 2, pp.
relationship for eyewitness identication. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 34(5), 1047–1060.
https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3683
Marmurek, H. H. C., Rusyn, R., Zgardau, A., & Zgardau, A. M. (2021). Verbal overshad-
owing at an immediate Task-Test delay is independent of Video-Task delay. Journal of
Cognitive Psychology, 34(2), 243–248. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2021.1981916
Marr, C., Otgaar, H., Sauerland, M., Quaedieg, C. W. E. M., & Hope, L. (2021). e eects
of stress on eyewitness memory: A survey of memory experts and laypeople. Memory &
Cognition, 49(3), 401–421. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-020-01115-4
Meissner, C. A., & Brigham, J. C. (2001). A meta-analysis of the verbal overshadowing
eect in face identication. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 15(6), 603–616. https://doi.
org/10.1002/acp.728
Meissner, C. A., Sporer, S. L., & Schooler, J. W. (2007). Person descriptions as eyewitness
evidence. In R. L. C. Lindsay, D. F. Ross, J. D. Read, & M. P. Toglia (Eds.), e handbook of
eyewitness psychology: Vol. 2. Memory for people (pp. 1–34). Routledge.
Mickes, L. (2016). e eects of verbal descriptions on eyewitness memory: Implications
for the real-world. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 5(3), 270–276.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2016.07.003
Pozzulo, J., Bennell, C., & Forth, A. (2018). Forensic psychology (5th ed.). Pearson Canada.
Pozzulo, J. D., Dempsey, J. L., Crescini, C., & Lemieux, J. M. T. (2009). Examining the rela-
tion between eyewitness recall and recognition for children and adults. Psychology, Crime
& Law, 15(5), 409–424. https://doi.org/10.1080/10683160802279625
Pozzulo, J. D., Dempsey, J., & Pettalia, J. (2013). e Z generation: Examining perpetrator
descriptions and lineup identication procedures. Journal of Police and Criminal Psychol-
ogy, 28(1), 63–74. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11896-012-9107-5
Sauerland, M., Holub, F. E., & Sporer, S. L. (2008). Person descriptions and person identi-
cations: Verbal overshadowing or recognition criterion shi? European Journal of Cogni-
tive Psychology, 20(3), 497–528. https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440701728417
Seale-Carlisle, T. M., Grabman, J. H., & Dodson, C. S. (2022). e language of accurate
and inaccurate eyewitnesses. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 151(6), 1283–
1305. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001152
Schooler, J. W., & Engstler-Schooler, T. Y. (1990). Verbal overshadowing of visual mem-
ories: Some things are better le unsaid. Cognitive Psychology, 22(1), 36–71. https://doi.
org/10.1016/0010-0285(90)90003-M
Sheahan, C. L., Pica, E., Pozzulo, J. D., & Nastasa, C. (2017). Eyewitness recall and identi-
cation abilities of adolescent and young-adults. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychol-
ogy, 53, 86–95. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2017.09.008
Short, J. L., & Dalby, J. T. (2007). Analysis of identication accuracy: Determining the ac-
curacy of eyewitness identications using statement analysis. Europes Journal of Psycholo-
gy, 3(3). https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v3i3.406
ONLINE FIRST
NBP. Nauka, bezbednost, policija
14
NBP 2025, Vol. 30, Issue 2, pp.
Szymkowiak, A., Melović, B., Dabić, M., Jeganathan, K., & Kundi, S. G. (2021). Infor-
mation technology and Gen Z: e role of teachers, the internet, and technology in the
education of young people. Technology in Society, 65(6), 101565. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
techsoc.2021.101565
Vredeveldt, A., Tredoux, C. G., Kempen, K., & Nortje, A. (2015). Eye remember what hap-
pened: Eye-closure improves recall of events but not face recognition. Applied Cognitive
Psychology, 29(2), 169–180. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3092
Wells, G. L. (1984). e psychology of lineup identications. Journal of Applied Social Psy-
chology, 14(2), 89–103. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.1984.tb02223.x
Wells, G. L. (2020). Psychological science on eyewitness identication and its impact
on police practices and policies. American Psychologist, 75(9), 1316–1329. https://doi.
org/10.1037/amp0000749
Wells, G. L., Kovera, M. B., Douglass, A. B., Brewer, N., Meissner, C. A., & Wixted, J.
T. (2020). Policy and procedure recommendations for the collection and preservation of
eyewitness identication evidence. Law and Human Behavior, 44(1), 3–36. https://doi.
org/10.1037/lhb0000359
Wells, G. L., & Olson, E. A. (2003). Eyewitness testimony. Annual Review of Psychology,
54(1), 277–295. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145028
Wixted, J. T., & Wells, G. L. (2017). e relationship between eyewitness condence and
identication accuracy: A new synthesis. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 18(1),
10–65. https://doi.org/10.1177/1529100616686966
Yamaguchi, M., & Sugimori, E. (2024). Perceived facial attractiveness and distinctive-
ness aect face recognition. Psychologia, 66(2), 162–178. https://doi.org/10.2117/psys-
oc.2023-A224
ONLINE FIRST
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any citations for this publication.
Article
Full-text available
If a witness to a crime provides a description of a perpetrator prior to identifying the perpetrator, the witness’s recognition accuracy will be compromised—this phenomenon is referred to as verbal overshadowing. The present study reports two recognition memory studies testing the criterion shift hypothesis of verbal overshadowing. In Experiment 1, we found evidence of verbal overshadowing using a decision task (2AFC) that mitigates criterion shift effects on recognition decisions. In Experiment 2, we experimentally crossed a criterion shift manipulation with a memory strength manipulation intended to produce verbal overshadowing. The experiment employed a single interval forced choice yes/no detection task in which the dependent variable, the conditional probability of a “yes” response, was treated using logistic regression. We found evidence of verbal overshadowing related to memory strength. We also found that the criterion shift manipulation influenced recognition memory, but criterion shift was not related to verbal overshadowing.
Article
Full-text available
The paper presents a study that aimed to examine the reliability of episodic memory by varying the retention time from the moment of initial observation of two incidents to the moment of communicating what was actually remembered. Ninety (90) students aged 21-26 from the University of Criminal Investigation and Police Studies participated in the research (50 males and 40 females). The interval of retention presented an independent variable on four levels: immediately after watching a recording, after 1 month, after 3 months and after 6 months. The accuracy of recall, or remembering one aspect of the incident (characteristics of the event, characteristics of the perpetrators and characteristics of the victim) was a dependent variable. Results suggest that memory accuracy declines dramatically after a month, but also after three and six months, when the downward trend in memory accuracy is less pronounced. Regarding the degree of confidence in memory accuracy, the pattern of results is practically identical to that of memory accuracy. As a whole, the results indicate that the retention interval strongly affects both accuracy and certainty in memory accuracy, regardless of the type of crime and the characteristics being evaluated.
Article
Full-text available
This paper uses machine-learning techniques to examine people’s use of verbal expressions of confidence. Across the field of academic psychology, it is often assumed that such statements reflect the same underlying information as numeric confidence ratings. We show that verbal confidence is not redundant with numeric confidence, but instead contributes unique diagnostic value in predicting the accuracy of a response. We use eyewitness confidence in a lineup identification as our model paradigm. There is potentially great applied value in developing a machine-learning algorithm that can predict eyewitness identification accuracy, such as by reducing false convictions. To this end, we applied a machine-learning methodology to investigate the natural language of accurate and inaccurate eyewitnesses. This method revealed that verbal confidence statements provide rich diagnostic information about the likely accuracy of eyewitness identifications. Moreover, verbal confidence statements provide unique diagnostic information that traditional indicators of identification accuracy such as numeric confidence ratings and response times do not provide. However, the diagnostic value of an eyewitness confidence statement depends in part on the face recognition ability of the eyewitness: the natural language of strong face recognizers is more diagnostic than the natural language of weak face recognizers. These results are theoretically interesting, but from an applied perspective, this machine-learning methodology may prove useful to those in the criminal justice system that must evaluate eyewitnesses’ verbal confidence statements.
Article
Full-text available
Mugbook searches are conducted in case a suspect is not known and to assess if a previously convicted person might be recognized as a potential culprit. The goal of the two experiments reported here was to analyze if prior statements and information about the suspect can aid in the evaluation if such a mugbook search is subsequently advised or not. In experiment 1, memory accuracy for person descriptors was tested in order to analyze, which attributes could be chosen to down-scale the mugbook prior to testing. Results showed that age was the most accurate descriptor, followed by ethnicity and height. At the same time self-assessed low subjective accuracy of culprit descriptions by the witness seemed to be divergent to the objective actual performance accuracy. In experiment 2, a mugbook search was conducted after participants viewed a video of a staged crime and gave a description of the culprit. Results showed that accuracy in mugbook searches correlated positively with the total number of person descriptors given by the witness as well as with witness’ description of external facial features. Predictive confidence (i.e., subjective rating of own performance in the subsequent mugbook search), however did not show any relation to the identification accuracy in the actual mugbook search. These results highlight the notion that mugbooks should not be conducted according to the subjective estimation of the witness’ performance but more according to the actual statements and descriptions that the witness can give about the culprit.
Article
Full-text available
Much of the literature on eyewitness identification neglects the social context in which identifications are made. As the number of cognitive psychologists conducting eyewitness research increased so did the use of signal detection theory and ROC analyses. With the resulting need for larger sample size, researchers moved toward conducting studies on internet platforms that allow for crowd-sourcing research participants. These methods make it next to impossible to ask research questions that explore the ways in which social interactions influence the identifications made by witnesses. Yet, the possibility of social context effects on witness memory are prevalent in applied contexts and research supports their existence. In addition, some eyewitness identifications may not be governed by memory at all. We argue that a consideration of social context effects is required to fully explore the reliability of witness identifications and propose a number of avenues for future research.
Article
Full-text available
Journal of Criminal Investigation and Criminology, 71(4), 247-256. -------------------- Eyewitness testimony remains an important element in resolving criminal investigations. Despite a great deal of research on the subject, the understanding of the effects that witness gender might have on their testimony remains rather limited. The aim of the present study was to examine own-gender bias in recalling personal descriptions of perpetrators. Participants, who were invited to take part in the study, were told they were providing real-life assistance to criminal investigators and potentially helping them to verify hypotheses concerning an actual crime. The participants comprised 256 undergraduate volunteers, who were placed in one of the four groups. Each group observed one of the four crime scenarios that were varied only in terms of gender of either the victim or the perpetrator. All scenarios were designed as footage taken by a CCTV surveillance camera and involved two-minute videos of a staged assault and robbery. After seven days, participants completed a survey on the basis of which the accuracy and quantity of perpetrators’ personal descriptions were checked. Results revealed that participants’ accuracy of memory recall was modest. It was found that gender had a significant main effect on the accuracy of personal descriptions in all four research conditions, while an own-gender bias was also established. The highest accuracy of perpetrators’ personal descriptions was found when male participants reported details of a male perpetrator attacking a female victim, and when female participants described a female perpetrator attacking a female victim. The lowest accuracy was established when female participants described the male perpetrator molesting the male victim. However, no effect of gender on the amount of reported personal descriptions was found.
Article
We investigated the influence of perceived impression on the recognition of strangers’ faces. Two experiments were conducted to examine whether face recognition distortion varies depending on perceived attractiveness and distinctiveness. Participants judged the attractiveness or distinctiveness of a target face presented in a video. Subsequently, they were shown a set of five images in which attractiveness or distinctiveness were manipulated and asked to select the original target face from the five images. Results showed that when participants initially judged a target face to be highly attractive, the more attractive image in the set was recognised as the target face than when participants judged the target face to be low in attractiveness. Furthermore, when participants judged a target face to be highly distinct, the more distinctive image in the set was recognised as the target face than when participants judged a target face to be low in distinctiveness. These findings were discussed in terms of the labelling effect and the verbal overshadowing effect.
Article
We sought to identify the sources of effect size differences in replications of the verbal overshadowing effect: the negative effect of verbally describing a face on later recognition of the face (Schooler & Engstler-Schooler, 1990 Schooler, J. W., & Engstler-Schooler, T. Y. (1990). Verbal overshadowing of visual memories: Some things are better left unsaid. Cognitive Psychology, 22(1), 36–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(90)90003-M[Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®] , [Google Scholar]). Comparisons of the original findings with those in a registered replication report (Alogna et al., 2014 Alogna, V. K., Attaya, M. K., Aucoin, P., Bahník, Š, Birch, S., Birt, A. R., Bornstein, B. H., Bouwmeester, S., Brandimonte, M. A., Brown, C., & Buswell, K. (2014). Registered Replication Report: Schooler and Engstler-Schooler (1990). Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9(5), 556–578. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691614545653[Crossref], [Web of Science ®] , [Google Scholar]) showed differences in the patterns of recognition in the criterial conditions defining the verbal overshadowing effect. The review indicates that although verbal overshadowing is strongest when the recognition task immediately follows the verbal description task, that delay variable is confounded with the delay between the encoding of the face and the verbal description task. We varied the delay between face encoding and the verbal description task under conditions where the recognition test immediately followed the description task. The verbal overshadowing effect was independent of the delay between face encoding and verbal description of the face.
Article
Technological progress has influenced the way we acquire knowledge and learn. On the other hand, the Internet provides fast access to information technology in different fields and thus, improves efficiency and saves time. The importance of online technology is especially emphasized in new methods for learning and education. This is particularly important among Generation Z ("Gen Z ′′), which derives knowledge from the Internet and is focused on a quick search of information. The aim of this paper is to determine how technology and the Internet affect the acquisition of knowledge by Generation Z, and which forms of knowledge acquisition this generation prefers. In our paper, the problem concerns various forms of learning and acquiring knowledge. Research was conducted among 498 young people actively using an online peer-to-peer knowledge-sharing community. The results of ANOVA analysis showed the respondents being more partial towards learning via mobile applications and video content over the traditional form. It also discovered that the students tended to emulate their teachers who integrated modern technologies into their curriculum and used it outside classroom hours for learning. The results of this research make several important theoretical contributions to the gap in knowledge acquisition by Gen Z, and provide several important practical recommendations for educators.