Access to this full-text is provided by PLOS.
Content available from PLOS One
This content is subject to copyright.
RESEARCH ARTICLE
Overcoming the threat of anti-bias
interventions: Combining self-report and
psychophysiological measures to capture the
process of change
Fe
´lice van NunspeetID
1‡
*, Esmee M. VeenstraID
1‡
, Beatriz Monteiro Grac¸a Casquinho
1
,
Naomi Ellemers
1
, Daan Scheepers
1,2
, Miriam I. Wickham
1
, Elena A. M. Bacchini
1
,
Jojanneke van der Toorn
1,2
, on behalf of The Organizational Behaviour Group
¶
1Faculty of Social and Behavioural Sciences, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands, 2Department of
Social, Economic, & Organizational Psychology, Faculty of Social Sciences, Leiden University, Leiden, The
Netherlands
‡ FN and EMV are listed in alphabetical order, they contributed equally to this work.
¶ Membership of the Organizational Behaviour Group is provided in the Acknowledgments.
*f.vannunspeet@uu.nl
Abstract
Anti-bias interventions do not always have the intended results and can even backfire. In
light of research on the psychology of morality, we examined whether confronting people
with evidence of their own (group’s) bias causes a (psychophysiological) threat response,
and how to overcome this. We focused on an intervention addressing gender bias in teacher
evaluations. After assessing their own teaching evaluations, we presented student research
participants (N= 101; 71.3% female), in Part 1 of the intervention, with evidence of bias dis-
played in such teaching evaluations. This evidence either did (self-implied condition) or did
not (self not-implied condition) include participants’ own ostensibly biased evaluations. In
Part 2 of the intervention, we asked participants to reflect on the issue of gender bias, and
compared the impact of two experimental instructions. In the promotion condition, instruc-
tions referred to emphasizing how the university could try to achieve the ideal of promoting
fair and just evaluations of teachers. In the prevention condition, instructions referred to
highlighting the university’s obligation to prevent unfair and unjust teacher evaluations.
While participants verbally reflected on the intervention, during both phases (in Part 1 and
Part 2) we measured their psychophysiological responses using indices of cardiovascular
‘threat vs. challenge’. Then, we used self-report measures to examine participants’ explicit
responses to the different parts of the intervention. Results revealed that implicating the self
in the occurrence of bias (Part 1) raises a psychophysiological threat response. However,
emphasizing the future ideal of promoting fair evaluations of teachers (rather than the obli-
gation of preventing biased evaluations; Part 2) resulted in a psychophysiological challenge
response and increased perceived coping abilities to combat such bias. The implications of
these findings are discussed.
PLOS ONE
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0314813 January 13, 2025 1 / 26
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
OPEN ACCESS
Citation: van Nunspeet F, Veenstra EM, Monteiro
Grac¸a Casquinho B, Ellemers N, Scheepers D,
Wickham MI, et al. (2025) Overcoming the threat
of anti-bias interventions: Combining self-report
and psychophysiological measures to capture the
process of change. PLoS ONE 20(1): e0314813.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0314813
Editor: Katsuya Oi, Northern Arizona University,
UNITED STATES OF AMERICA
Received: January 25, 2024
Accepted: November 16, 2024
Published: January 13, 2025
Peer Review History: PLOS recognizes the
benefits of transparency in the peer review
process; therefore, we enable the publication of
all of the content of peer review and author
responses alongside final, published articles. The
editorial history of this article is available here:
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0314813
Copyright: ©2025 van Nunspeet et al. This is an
open access article distributed under the terms of
the Creative Commons Attribution License, which
permits unrestricted use, distribution, and
reproduction in any medium, provided the original
author and source are credited.
Data Availability Statement: Data is available at:
https://doi.org/10.17605/osf.io/vqh57.
Introduction
Ever since Kurt Lewin [1] made a case for action research, the social sciences in general,—and
social psychologists in particular—consider the potential significance of their work not only to
generate theory, but also to benefit society. This resonates with the increased emphasis on sci-
entific outreach, knowledge transfer and utilization, and public engagement urging academic
researchers to enhance the impact of their findings and to connect science and society [2–4].
The widely acknowledged responsibility to generate impact manifests itself in current efforts
to improve individual and societal outcomes and change behaviour by developing evidence-
based interventions (see, for example [5]).
A central aim of our research group is to contribute to such evidence-based interventions—
in particular interventions relating to behaviour that has moral implications, such as measures
aiming to enhance diversity and inclusion in organizations. As a group we therefore devote a
substantial proportion of our time and resources to developing outreach activities highlighting
the implications of key research findings for practitioners. This is intended to educate non-sci-
entific audiences about relevant insights (for instance on the persistence of (implicit) bias and
how to overcome this), while enhancing our theorizing on evidence-based interventions [6,7].
These experiences have taught us that even audiences that communicate an explicit interest in
these issues often find it difficult to accept scientific evidence for the persistence of bias. This
raises additional questions about what coping mechanisms people employ to deal with evi-
dence of bias that implicates their self-views, and how this impacts their willingness to address
bias. In the present study, we therefore take an experimental approach to compare different
versions of the same intervention. More specifically, we measure how psychophysiological
responses unfold during the intervention to capture the process of change, and we combine
this with self-reports revealing how participants explicitly reflect on their experience. This
allows us to specify intended and unintended consequences of the intervention, and systemati-
cally identify which aspects might be adapted to enhance the effectiveness of anti-bias inter-
ventions. This approach increases our understanding of psychological mechanisms that may
either undermine or enhance the likelihood that people engage with the intervention and
adapt their behaviour. Collecting such information will benefit our understanding of when
and why interventions are most likely to be effective, and how to optimize the effectiveness of
existing interventions.
We prepared this paper as the outcome of a joint project with many authors and collabora-
tors. In this way, we capture the mission and exemplify the characteristic approach of our
research group, in which many different skills and insights are brought together to develop sci-
ence as well as practice. By using scientific methods to document the impact of outreach activi-
ties that are meant to communicate about research findings, we complete the full empirical
cycle.
Measuring the effectiveness of anti-bias interventions
Interventions to make people aware of and combat bias are becoming increasingly popular. In
fact, many organizations nowadays expect members of staff in leadership and HR roles to par-
ticipate in some form of ‘(implicit) bias training’, in an attempt to foster employee diversity
and inclusion. This is even obligatory in some public institutions, for instance for universities
in the UK that apply for certification needed to qualify for research funding (Athena SWAN
Award Scheme for Gender Equality), and for individual scholars who sit in on research evalua-
tion committees for the European Science Foundation [8]. However, there is little evidence for
the diversity gains of such awareness and training efforts, prompting researchers to character-
ize exchanges of ‘best practices’ as no more than ‘best guesses’ [9–11].
PLOS ONE
Overcoming the threat of anti-bias interventions
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0314813 January 13, 2025 2 / 26
Funding: This research was supported by the
NWO-Spinoza prize awarded to N.E. by the
Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research
(NWO). https://www.nwo.nl/nwo-spinozapremie.
The funder had no role in study design, data
collection and analysis, decision to publish, or
preparation of the manuscript.
Competing interests: The authors have declared
that no competing interests exist.
A feature shared by most anti-bias interventions is creating awareness of the existence and
pervasiveness of (implicit) bias. For instance, real time demonstrations during the intervention
are often used to convince participants that they too may suffer from biases—even when they
explicitly endorse measures promoting equal opportunities (e.g., as revealed by participating
in an Implicit Association Test [12], see also [13]). There might be good reasons for adopting
this type of approach: Anti-bias training programs that actively invite participants to consider
their own biases generally elicit more positive attitudes towards disadvantaged groups and
offer more evidence of (long term) behavioural changes than more passive forms of informa-
tion transfer [14,15]. However, several other studies have documented the limited impact of
such interventions [16–22]. Hence, so far there is no consistent evidence of what determines
the effectiveness of anti-bias interventions in changing people’s behaviours towards diversity
issues [7,17,19,22]. Nor do we know whether or how demonstrating that participants in the
intervention also suffer from (implicit) bias contributes to these effects. Then again, many
studies aiming to examine the impact of anti-bias interventions simply compare pre- and post-
intervention questionnaires, rather than systematically monitoring psychological mechanisms
that might enhance or impede the impact of specific aspects of the intervention [21]. For
instance, no studies to date have examined real-time (physiological) responses documenting
the process of change while taking part in a diversity training—the approach we took in the
current study.
The motivational state of threat
In the current research, we posit that demonstrating that participants in the intervention also
suffer from bias may backfire when this raises threat responses that result in defensiveness.
Instead of increasing people’s motivation to change, threat responses are likely to prevent peo-
ple from engaging with the intervention. The possibility that the self-relevance of information
about the persistence of bias is threatening, can be derived from research showing that people
are motivated to be moral, and to appear moral in their own eyes and the eyes of others [23–
25]. Fair treatment of others is a key moral foundation [26,27]. Accordingly, prior research
has indicated that information emphasizing that people themselves, or members of their
group, have treated others unfairly is a source of self-threat and guilt [28,29]. Across many
groups and contexts such information has been found to raise defensive responses. Instead of
motivating people to repair harm done, or attempt change, they blame the victim or find ways
to justify past injustices [30–35]. Likewise, people have been found to respond in a defensive
manner to feedback indicating they themselves are biased [36–38]. The first aim of the current
research is therefore to test our hypothesis that when people are confronted with evidence of their
own bias—compared to when evidence of existing bias is not implying the self—they will display
threat and defensive responses.
The motivational state of challenge
Improving our understanding of what can be done to overcome such threat makes it possible
to enhance the effectiveness of interventions that aim to reduce (the effects of) social bias.
Interestingly, prior work suggests that defensive responses to feedback indicating that people
themselves are biased are not inevitable. In general, constructive responses to information
about implicit bias can be enhanced when people are offered a vision for improvement instead
of focusing only on problems that should be avoided [34,39–41]. Here, we extend prior
research examining how different conditions may encourage individuals to overcome
(implicit) bias, by reducing the defensive responses that characterize the experience of moral
threat. Specifically, we examine specific ways of communicating about the persistence of
PLOS ONE
Overcoming the threat of anti-bias interventions
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0314813 January 13, 2025 3 / 26
implicit bias that are likely to make it easier for people to cope with and respond to this (threat-
ening) information.
We build on regulatory focus theory [42], distinguishing a focus on promoting positive out-
comes and ideals from the focus on preventing negative outcomes that violate important obli-
gations [43]. Previous research suggests that encouraging a focus on promoting positive
outcomes rather than preventing negative outcomes can help avoid defensive responses. This
also seems to be the case when evidence of pervasive bias is presented. Shifting people’s focus
on the pursuit of ideals (promotion-focus) as opposed to obligations (prevention-focus) can
help to combat defensive reactions and make people more receptive to (potentially) threaten-
ing information [39,40]. Specifically, results from prior research examining how student par-
ticipants responded to statistics documenting labour discrimination demonstrated that
focusing on ideals (i.e., a promotion-focus; as opposed to obligations, indicating a prevention-
focus) elicits responses indicating relatively more challenge and less threat [39,40]. These
results suggest that focusing on ideals to be achieved (inducing a promotion focus) as opposed
to obligations to be met (prompting a prevention focus), can diminish threat responses and
improve people’s ability to engage with attempted change. Our second aim with the current
research is therefore to test whether,and further examine why,promoting the ideal of fairness
and equal treatment—rather than the obligation to prevent unfairness and unequal (i.e., biased)
treatment—elicits a challenge response.
Furthermore, past research has shown that offering people feedback that highlights poten-
tial future opportunities, for instance to reveal their moral character or behavioural intentions
instead of past (immoral) events, raises their perceived ability to cope with the situation and
allows them to engage with opportunities for change [44]. In a similar vein, emphasizing that
past failures may indicate a lack of competence instead of moral inadequacy makes people
more confident that they will be able to improve their behaviour and social image [45,46]. Our
third aim with the current research is therefore to explore whether and how the effect of the pro-
motion (vs.prevention) focus is associated with people’s perceived coping ability.
The biopsychosocial model of challenge and threat
The biopsychosocial model of challenge and threat (BPS-CT) specifies cardiovascular (CV)
markers of the motivational states of threat and challenge during so-called motivated perfor-
mance situations (e.g., athletic performance, negotiating, doing a math test, giving a speech
[47–52]). In addition, the BPS-CT characterizes validated CV response-profiles that mark
challenge rather than threat. In the context of the BPS-CT, four CV measures are typically
used: Heart rate (HR), Pre-Ejection Period (PEP), Cardiac Output (CO) and Total Peripheral
Resistance (TPR). A defining aspect of motivated performance is a certain level of task engage-
ment which is at the physiological level marked by increased HR and decreased PEP [48,51,
53]. That is, the heart starts pumping faster, and with more force. Given task-engagement,
challenge and threat can in turn be distinguished based on CO and TPR. In terms of reactivity
compared to baseline levels (so-called absolute patterns of challenge and threat [48,54]), chal-
lenge is marked by increased CO and decreased TPR, which facilitates the efficient mobiliza-
tion and transportation of energy during motivated performance. Threat, by contrast, is
marked by increased TPR and stable CO, which is a less efficient CV response profile during
motivated performance [48].
In addition to these absolute patterns of challenge and threat, in more recent formulations
of the BPS-CT more stress is placed on relative differences in challenge and threat between
experimental conditions. Here, challenge is indicated by relatively high CO and low TPR, and
threat by relatively high TPR and low CO. As a further elaboration of these relative differences
PLOS ONE
Overcoming the threat of anti-bias interventions
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0314813 January 13, 2025 4 / 26
in challenge and threat motivational states, CO and TPR can also be combined in a single
threat-challenge index (TCI [55–60]).
The current research
In the current research, we combine insights from psychological theory and experimental
research to examine how different versions of an anti-bias intervention impact on participants’
thoughts, feelings and experiences. Because of our aim to examine a real-life intervention, stu-
dent research participants were confronted with existing evidence on a form of bias relevant to
them: gender bias in student evaluations of teachers. This bias indicates that students systemat-
ically give lower teaching evaluations to female as opposed to male teachers—irrespective of
objective differences in teaching effectiveness [61,62]. Except for selecting a theme that would
be relevant to the target group, the procedure we followed is very similar to how we typically
present evidence of bias displayed by one’s own group in anti-bias interventions we conduct in
organizations (e.g., during public outreach activities).
Prior to taking part in the intervention (which was embedded in an interactive webinar in
the lab about measuring teaching effectiveness), student participants were invited to complete
an online questionnaire and evaluate teachers of recently attended courses. During the webi-
nar, it was first explained that student evaluations of teachers, which are regarded as one of the
most important indicators of teaching effectiveness, impact on the career progression of teach-
ers. Then the question was raised whether these student evaluations indeed reflect teaching
effectiveness, and research into the teacher evaluation bias was introduced. In the remainder
of the webinar, participants were educated about the nature, pervasiveness, and impact of this
form of gender bias, based on the growing body of evidence that evaluations of teachers tend
to reveal gender bias instead of accurately reflecting teaching effectiveness [61,63,64].
After this general introduction, in Part 1 of the intervention, participants in the self-implied
condition were presented with results allegedly indicating that their own evaluations of teach-
ers were not immune to gender bias. Specifically, they received feedback specifying that they
and their fellow students had shown a gender bias in the evaluation of teachers to the disad-
vantage of female teachers, ostensibly based on the online questionnaire they had completed
prior to the experiment. We compared this to a condition with no such self-implication, in
which participants only received the general information on the likelihood that such biases
emerge, in the webinar. Subsequently, in Part 2 of the intervention, we randomly exposed
research participants to one of two versions of our anti-bias intervention, with the promotion
version emphasizing the ideal of promoting fair and just evaluations of teachers, and the pre-
vention version highlighting the obligation of preventing biased evaluations of teachers.
In both parts of the intervention, we invited participants to verbally reflect on the existence
of the bias in teaching evaluations (Speech 1) and the promotion of fair (vs. the prevention of
unfair/biased—Speech 2) treatment of both male and female teachers, while measuring their
cardiovascular responses. Subsequently, we asked participants to complete several self-report
questionnaires assessing their affective responses to the experimental parts of the intervention,
and their behavioural intentions after having completed the intervention. By examining their
underlying motivational states (i.e., real-time cardiovascular indices of threat vs. challenge), as
well as more traditional self-reported reactions, we were able to assess the added value of more
implicit indicators of participants’ responses to their explicit evaluations of various aspects of
the intervention.
In sum, this study was designed to test the following hypotheses:
1. Being confronted with self-implied (vs. self not-implied) evidence of the existence of gender
bias in teacher evaluations, will elicit a threat response among students (H1).
PLOS ONE
Overcoming the threat of anti-bias interventions
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0314813 January 13, 2025 5 / 26
2. Inviting students to promote the fair and just (instead of requesting them to prevent biased)
treatment of both male and female teachers will elicit a challenge response among students
(H2).
3. Occurrence of the challenge response elicited by the promotion focus will be mediated by
students’ perceived coping ability to combat the teacher evaluation bias (H3).
In addition to testing these hypotheses, we explored how the (more implicit motivational)
psychophysiological responses relate to (more explicit) self-reported reactions to the interven-
tion–which are typically used in standard evaluations of such interventions.
Method
Ethics statement
This entire research project, including the hypotheses and the exploratory analyses described
above, was approved by the local Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Social and Behavioural
Sciences of the university. Research participants were recruited and data was collected between
July 14—September 16, 2018. All participants gave their written informed consent.
Participants and design
Based on prior research [40] in which cardiovascular responses were measured and—com-
parable to our promotion vs. prevention manipulation—moral ideals vs. obligations were
emphasized, a power analysis was conducted. The smallest effect size reported of a cardio-
vascular effect comparing a promotion and prevention condition (i.e., an emphasis on
moral ideals vs. moral obligations [40]) was ηp
2
= 0.12, Cohen’s f= 0.37. Based on this effect
size and our aim to compare the effects of two different manipulations (i.e., self-implied vs.
self not-implied evidence, and a promotion vs. prevention frame) on cardiovascular mark-
ers for threat and challenge in a one-way ANOVA, a power analysis using G*Power indi-
cated that we needed to include around 84 participants in total to obtain a power of 80%.
We recruited additional participants to account for unintended data loss at the start of the
data collection, due to technical errors, and participants not completing the experiment or
not strictly following the procedure. Specifically, 108 participants took part, who were all
students at a Dutch university and who were financially compensated for their time or
received course credit for participation. Seven participants were excluded for not complet-
ing the experiment or not strictly following the procedure. For 18 participants, the monitor-
ing of their cardiovascular measures was partially affected due to technical errors in the
initial phase of the data collection. We therefore decided for each analysis, to include only
the participants of whom enough good-quality data remained, resulting in small differences
in sample sizes across analyses.
Of the remaining 101 participants (M
age
= 23.40 years, SD = 3.00, age range = 18–39 years),
71.3% indicated being female, 27.7% male and 1% ‘other’. Most participants (53.5%) were
Master students, 43.6% of participants were Bachelor students (5% in year 1, 8.9% in year 2,
and 29.70% in year 3/4), and 3% of participants were working towards another type of degree.
The native language of most participants was Dutch (57.4%), English for 9.9%, and a different
native language for 32.7%. Participants were randomly assigned to one of four conditions in a
2 (Self-implied evidence: Yes vs. No) ×2 (Frame: Promotion vs. Prevention) between-partici-
pants design.
PLOS ONE
Overcoming the threat of anti-bias interventions
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0314813 January 13, 2025 6 / 26
Procedure
A complete overview of all phases of the experimental session is visualized in Fig 1. Partici-
pants were recruited for ‘a study on the effectiveness of teaching evaluations’ and agreed to
take part in a psychophysiological lab study. The study began, on average, five days prior to the
lab session (M
days
= 4.72, SD = 4.46) when participants completed an online survey in which
they were asked to evaluate four different teachers of courses they had recently attended
(details of this assessment, as well as the results, are reported in S1 Fig in S1 Appendix).
During the lab session, participants were informed that they would be shown a webinar on
measuring teaching effectiveness—presented by a (white, female) professor in social psychol-
ogy at the participants’ university (who is a member of our research team and an expert on
diversity and inclusion), and that their cardiovascular responses would be measured. One of
seven different experimenters (all of whom were White and female) applied the sensors for the
cardiovascular measurements (see below). Importantly, none of the researchers (i.e., the exper-
imenters and the university professor who presented in the webinar), was included in partici-
pants’ teaching evaluations as these evaluations concerned courses outside of the researchers’
teaching responsibilities. Independence between the researchers and the research participants
was thus assured. Once participants were connected, they were further informed about the dif-
ferent parts of the experimental session: It was explained that they would be asked twice to ver-
bally reflect on the message that was presented to them in the webinar, and that they would be
asked to complete several questionnaires at the end of the session (after the entire webinar).
Participants then watched a 5-minute relaxing video during which we measured their baseline
heart rate, which was followed by the start of the webinar.
In the first part of the webinar, the professor explained the relevance for and impact of stu-
dents’ teaching evaluations on teachers’ career progression. She also presented several exam-
ples of published research evidence suggesting that such evaluations are often biased against
female teachers. Participants in the self not-implied condition did not receive any additional
information. Participants in the self-implied condition were also presented with a brief sum-
mary indicating they and their fellow students had shown a gender bias in their teacher evalua-
tions–allegedly based on the online questionnaire they had filled out before they had come to
the lab. This feedback was not presented within the webinar (i.e., by the professor), but rather
provided as a separate (additional) section of the experiment. After this first part of the inter-
vention, participants were asked to give their first speech (Speech 1), during which
Fig 1. The (parts of the) experimental intervention.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0314813.g001
PLOS ONE
Overcoming the threat of anti-bias interventions
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0314813 January 13, 2025 7 / 26
cardiovascular responses were registered. They were then shown the second part of the webi-
nar, which focused on the implications of this gender bias for the university and the univer-
sity’s reaction to the research findings. Depending on their random assignment to
experimental condition, participants either saw a version which was framed in terms of a pro-
motion focus or a version framed in terms of a prevention focus. Then, participants were
asked to verbalize their thoughts about this second part of the intervention (Speech 2), after
which they completed the self-report questionnaires. Finally, they were fully debriefed, com-
pensated, and thanked for their participation. In the debriefing, it was explicitly emphasized
that bogus evidence of bias had been presented and that this did not reflect participants’ actual
answers given in their teacher evaluations.
Instruments and measures
Self-implication manipulation. Participants were randomly assigned to either the self-
implied or the self not-implied condition. In the self-implied condition, participants were pre-
sented with bogus results, ostensibly based on responses to the teacher evaluation survey they
had participated in, demonstrating that they and their fellow students showed a bias in the
evaluation of teachers to the disadvantage of female teachers. In the self not-implied condition,
these results were not shown. After this, participants were asked to give a speech (Speech 1) in
which they verbally reflected on the question: “Where do you think the bias in the evaluation
of teachers comes from?” Participants had one minute to prepare, and then two minutes to
subsequently deliver their speech.
Framing manipulation. Participants watched one of two versions of the second part of
the webinar depending on whether they had been randomly assigned to the promotion or pre-
vention condition. In this part of the webinar, the professor talked about the reaction of the
students’ university to the research on the teacher evaluation bias and how the university and
the student council wanted to respond to it. In the promotion condition, it was emphasized
that the research findings offered opportunities to improve the situation and helped the univer-
sity to achieve its ideal of promoting fair and just evaluations of teachers. Conversely, in the
prevention condition, the professor stressed that the research findings pointed out a situation
that the university wanted to avoid in order to meet its obligation of preventing unfair and
unjust evaluations of teachers. This promotion vs. prevention perspective was also reflected in
the reflection assignment participants received for the second speech (Speech 2). In the pro-
motion condition they were asked: “What ideas can you come up with to promote the fair eval-
uation of teachers? Please generate ideas to achieve the ideal of fair and just evaluations of
teachers”. In the prevention condition they were asked: “What measures can you come up
with to prevent the unfair evaluation of teachers? Please list measures to meet the obligation to
prevent biased evaluations of teachers.” Again, participants received one minute to prepare
and two minutes to give the speech.
Cardiovascular measures. Throughout the experiment we continuously measured CV-
markers using a Biopac MP160 system [65]. Specifically, we used the NICO100C module to
measure impedance-cardiography (ICG), the ECG100 module to measure electrocardiography
measures (ECG), and the NIBP100D module to measure blood pressure (BP). We followed the
procedures previously reported in [66]. Physiological data were stored using Acqknowledge
software [65] and scored using the physiodata toolbox [67]. Specifically, ECG, ICG, and BP
signals were first visually inspected for artifacts; complexes that were non-scorable or other-
wise of low quality were rejected. The r-peaks of the ECG were automatically scored which
yielded a measure of heart rate (HR). The BP signal was also automatically scored which
yielded a measure of mean arterial pressure (MAP). The ECG and ICG were then ensemble-
PLOS ONE
Overcoming the threat of anti-bias interventions
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0314813 January 13, 2025 8 / 26
averaged for the three epochs we were interested in: The last minute of the baseline, and one
minute of the first and the second speech task (seconds 10 through 70), respectively. The Q, B,
and X points were then manually scored in the ensemble-averaged signals; the Q-point was
scored as ‘r-onset’ [68] and the B- and X-point were scored following [69]. This yielded the
pre-ejection period (PEP, which is a measure of ventricular contractility) and cardiac output
(CO). Finally, in SPSS, TPR was calculated from CO and MAP using the following formula:
TPR = (MAP/CO) x 80 [70].
Also in line with standard practice, individual reactivity scores were created for the four
measures by subtracting the baseline scores from each of the six task scores. Univariate out-
liers (defined as 3.3SD above/below the mean) were assigned a value of 1% higher/lower
than the adjacent non-extreme value [55,71,72]. Finally, we calculated combined Threat-
Challenge Indices (TCI) by calculating Z-scores of CO and TPR reactivity, then multiplying
TPR with -1 and summing the result with the CO Z-score [55,56,60]. Higher scores on the
resulting indices—which maximize the reliability of the cardiovascular measures [60]—
indicate a greater challenge motivational state, whereas lower scores indicate a greater
threat motivational state.
Self-report measures. At the end of the webinar (i.e., following participants’ second
speech), we used self-report questionnaires to assess participants’ explicit responses. We did
this to test the subjective equivalence of experimental conditions, and to be able to rule out
alternative explanations, as well as to explore relations between these standard self-reported
evaluations and more implicit cardiovascular indicators of participants’ responses to the inter-
vention. Here we asked them to indicate how they had experienced the first and/or second
part of the intervention, and to register their attitudes towards the general topic of the inter-
vention (i.e., gender bias). All items were rated on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree–
7 = strongly agree), unless otherwise indicated.
Manipulation checks. To examine whether the self-implication manipulation evoked
explicit defensive reactions, participants in the self-implied condition indicated their agree-
ment with statements adapted from the work on defensive responses to feedback regarding
individual’s implicit biases [73,74]. The total defensiveness scale of nine items had a relatively
low reliability (α= .65) and was hence subjected to a Principal Components Analysis with Var-
imax rotation, which yielded three orthogonal components. The first factor represents the
degree to which the participants rejected that the feedback represented their deliberate,true
preferences (α= .85), and included three items such as: “The results from the online question-
naire that I and my fellow students filled out:. . .do not reflect anything about my true thoughts
or feelings”. The second factor represents the degree to which the participants rejected the feed-
back being corrupt (α= .66) and included four items such as: “The results:. . .are misleading”.
The third factor represents the degree to which the participants rejected that the feedback
reflected their unconscious,automatic preferences (two items, r= .80), with an example item
being: “The results:. . .reflect something about my automatic thoughts or feelings” (reverse
coded). The three-factor solution explained 68.8% of the variance.
To examine the effect of the framing manipulation, six items were included to assess
whether participants were focused on approaching opportunities (promotion focus) or avoid-
ing risks (prevention focus) during Speech 2. Five items based on the regulatory strategy scale
[75] were used, each with a promotion strategy at one end and a prevention strategy at the
other end of the scale. For example, participants responded to the following contrasting ten-
dency: “During the second speech, I was focused on:. . .taking risks” versus “acting cautiously”.
Participants had to answer using a 7-point scale, in which a rating of 4 would indicate that
both prevention and promotion strategies fit to the same extent and lower values would indi-
cate a better fit of promotion strategies. We added the item: “. . .taking opportunities” versus
PLOS ONE
Overcoming the threat of anti-bias interventions
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0314813 January 13, 2025 9 / 26
“taking responsibility” (six items, α= .34). To improve the internal consistency, we decided to
exclude one item (acting thoroughly/superficially) from the total scale (α= .64).
Perceived coping abilities. Related to our third hypothesis, we examined participants’ ability
to cope with our requests to verbally reflect on the two issues raised in the webinar. We did
this using two items [adapted from 76] assessing perceived demands and resources in a partic-
ular situation. In addition, one item was added to incorporate the perceived difficulty of our
request. That is, regarding Speech 1 (α= .69), participants responded to the items: “In prepara-
tion of and during the first speech, . . . it was stressful to come up with an answer to the ques-
tion where the bias in the evaluation of teachers comes from” (measuring demands, reverse
coded) “. . .I felt I was able to reflect on the bias in the evaluation of teachers” (measuring
resources), and “. . .it was difficult to come up with a response to the question where bias in the
evaluation of teachers comes from” (measuring difficulty, reverse coded).
Similarly, regarding Speech 2 (α= .76), participants responded to the items: “In preparation
of and during the second speech, . . . it was stressful to come up with ideas to promote the fair
evaluation of teachers / measures to prevent the unfair evaluation of teachers” (measuring
demands, reverse coded) “. . . I was able to generate ideas to achieve the ideal of fair and just
evaluations of teachers / list measures to meet the obligation to prevent biased evaluations of
teachers” (measuring resources), and “. . .it was difficult to come up with ideas to promote the
fair evaluation of teachers / measures to prevent the unfair evaluation of teachers” (measuring
difficulty, reverse coded).
Additional self-report measures. In addition to the manipulation checks and the role of cop-
ing, we also explored effects of the experimental manipulations on participants’ intention to
act and motivation to change.
Willingness to take action: Participants were asked whether they wanted to be involved in a
student panel to talk about the future of student evaluations of teachers. They were given the
following seven options for concrete choices they might make (percentages are given in paren-
theses): “Yes,I would like to engage in a conversation with the student council to find out how I
can contribute” (15.3%),"Yes,I would like to attend one of the information sessions to hear what
the student council expects from students” (20.4%), “Yes,I would like to stay informed on the
composition of this panel by subscribing to the monthly newsletter of the student council”
(19.4%), "No,I would like to forget about the bias in the evaluation of teachers and its conse-
quences” (1%), “No,I rather not get mixed up in discussions about the bias in the evaluation of
teachers to the disadvantage of female teachers” (5.1%), “No, I cannot devote my time to this, as
I have other important things to do” (33.7%), and “Other,namely. . .”(5.1%).
Preliminary analyses using post-hoc z-scores were used to examine the specific differences in
response patterns to the provided options [77]. This revealed no significant differences between
the first two answers (p’s >0.05), which could both be considered active ways of contributing
to the panel, and which were therefore combined into one category—indicating active involve-
ment. Results neither revealed significant differences between the fourth and fifth answers
(p’s >0.05), which could both be considered active ways of not contributing to the panel, and
which were therefore also combined into one category—indicating active distancing. This thus
resulted in the following four categories: ‘active involvement’, ‘passive involvement’, ‘active dis-
tancing’, and ‘passive distancing’. Further analyses are discussed in the results section.
Future intentions to regulate bias: Three items, inspired by [38], assessed participants’ future
intentions to monitor and control their possible teacher evaluation bias. Example items are: “I
am concerned I will exhibit gender bias in the future”, and “After what I learned today,I will be
more on guard for gender biased behaviour” (α= .62).
Belief in present gender discrimination: Six items from the Contemporary Gender Discrimi-
nation Scale [78] were included to examine participants’ belief in present gender
PLOS ONE
Overcoming the threat of anti-bias interventions
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0314813 January 13, 2025 10 / 26
discrimination. For example, “Although it is more subtle than it used to be,women still experi-
ence discrimination.”and “Women still need to work harder than men to achieve the same
things.”. Because three of these items were reverse-coded, we first conducted a factor analysis
to determine the scale’s dimensionality. We used principal axis factoring with a Principal
Components Analysis with Varimax rotation. Results indicated that all items loaded on one
factor (48.19% variance explained). The scale had sufficient reliability (α= .76).
Besides the measures described above, participants were also presented with self-report
measures regarding the perceived stability of bias, threat to one’s social identity, positive and
negative reactions to teacher evaluation bias, attention checks and experiences in the experi-
ment. Furthermore, we analysed the speeches that participants had to produce in terms of con-
tent. Because these measures go beyond the scope of our main manuscript, but do not alter
(the interpretation of) our findings, details are included in S1–S3 Appendices.
Results
Manipulation checks
The self-report measures collected at the end of the intervention asked participants in the self-
implied condition to reflect back on the feedback they had received—presenting (ostensible)
evidence that they themselves and their group members had shown a gender bias in their
teacher evaluations. Results indicated that participants did not believe that the feedback repre-
sented their true values or reflected their true thoughts or feelings (i.e., Msignificantly higher
than scale midpoint, M
diff
= 0.55, t[49] = 2.35, p= .023, 95% CI [0.079, 1.027], Hedges’s g
s
=
0.654). However, participants did not invalidate the feedback (i.e., Msignificantly lower than
scale midpoint, M
diff
= -0.53, t[49] = -3.31, p= .002, 95% CI [-0.852, -0.208], Hedges’s g
s
= -
0.922). This indicates that they thought the feedback about their own (group’s) bias was based
on fact and not misleading or exaggerated. Moreover, interestingly, participants did not fully
reject the feedback (i.e., Msomewhat lower than scale midpoint, M
diff
= = -.39, t[49] = -1.89, p
= .064, 95% CI [-0.804, -0.024], Hedges’s g
s
= -0.526, suggesting that they saw the results as
reflecting something about their automatic thoughts or feelings. These findings were not mod-
erated by the framing manipulation (all p’s >687). Together, these results reveal that partici-
pants believed the feedback to be factual, but indicative of an implicit, rather than explicit,
gender bias.
We also examined whether participants in the promotion and prevention conditions delib-
erately intended to approach opportunities or to avoid risks while they came up with ways to
address gender bias in the future—during their second speech. Results of this self-report mea-
sure indicated that participants in the promotion condition did not think they had been partic-
ularly focused on approaching opportunities (M= 4.00, SD = 0.93), t(52) = 0, p= 1.00), nor
did participants in the prevention condition indicate being particularly focused on avoiding
risks (M= 3.78, SD = 0.84), t(47) = -1.85, p= .071). This suggests that participants’ explicit
recall of their deliberate intentions during the second speech did not differ depending on
experimental condition, F(1,97) = 1.59, p= .21. We now continue by testing our hypotheses
about the psychophysiological responses during each of the phases of the intervention.
Hypothesis testing
Before testing our hypotheses regarding participants’ cardiovascular responses, we first verified
that any differences found between conditions can be attributed to the experimental manipula-
tions in the intervention, rather than to participants’ task engagement. We therefore examined
participants’ behavioural task engagement by inspecting the number of words spoken and the
number of hesitance words they used during both speeches, and their cardiovascular signs of
PLOS ONE
Overcoming the threat of anti-bias interventions
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0314813 January 13, 2025 11 / 26
task engagement during both speeches, by inspecting their CV-reactivity in indices of HR and
PEP compared to baseline. Results regarding the cardiovascular task engagement indicated
that participants were more engaged during both speeches compared to the baseline period
(for HR: t’s11.07, p’s .001; for PEP: t’s -6.1, p’s .001). Moreover, regardless of the con-
dition participants were in, they showed similar levels of behavioural (all t’s 1.72, p’s .09)
and cardiovascular task engagement (all t’s1.94, p’s .055) during the speeches.
To test our hypothesis regarding the psychophysiological measures, we first calculated
mean levels of HR, PEP, CO and TPR for the last minute of the baseline period, as well as the
first minute of the two speech tasks, respectively, and checked for between-condition differ-
ences. We found a significant interaction between self-implication and framing on baseline
HR, F(1, 95) = 4.82, p= 0.031. In line with standard practice, we therefore included baseline
HR as a covariate in the confirmatory analyses on the cardiovascular data reported below.
However, it should be noted that the results are similar when this covariate is not included. No
other main or interaction effects were found, F’s1.84, p’s .179.
Effects of self (not) implying evidence (Speech 1). Our first hypothesis stated that being
confronted with self-implied (vs. self not-implied) evidence of the existence of gender bias in
teaching evaluations, should elicit a threat response. To test this prediction, we examined the
relative patterns of challenge and threat by performing an ANOVA on the separate cardiovas-
cular indicators TPR and CO, as well as on the combined TCI as an overall indicator of relative
challenge vs threat—all measured during the first speech—and included our experimental
manipulation of self-implied vs. self not-implied evidence of bias as the independent variable.
Results showed no between-condition differences in TPR, CO, and TCI (F’s2.41, p’s.12).
However, in line with common practice [e.g., 54], we also examined absolute patterns of
cardiovascular (CV) reactivity as changes in TPR and CO during the first speech task com-
pared to baseline levels. Consistent with the typical pattern related to threat, results revealed a
significant increase in TPR in the self-implied condition, t(42) = 2.42, p= .020, while TPR was
stable in the self not-implied condition, t(39) = 0.28, p= .784. Additionally, although CO was
increased in both the self-implied (t[42] = 2.70, p= .010) and the self not-implied condition
compared to baseline (t[43] = 4.69, p<.001)—whereas the typical pattern related to threat is
associated with CO remaining stable—, the increase in the self-implied condition was smaller
(M
diff
= 0.25, SD = 0.61) than the increase in the self not-implied condition (M
diff
= 0.45,
SD = 0.64). These results thus indicate that, in line with Hypothesis 1, reflecting on evidence of
bias that implies the (group-) self yields CV reactivity indicative of threat (i.e., higher TPR and
relatively lower CO). The mean levels of CO, TPR, and TCI during Speech 1 (relative to base-
line) are displayed in Table 1.
Effects of a promotion vs. Prevention frame (Speech 2). Our second hypothesis was that
a promotion (vs. a prevention) frame, for how to address the equal treatment of teachers,
should elicit a challenge response. To test this prediction, we examined the relative patterns of
challenge and threat by performing ANOVA’s on the separate cardiovascular indicators TPR
and CO, as well as on the combined TCI as an overall indicator of relative challenge vs threat
—measured during the second speech. Since the second speech was performed after the whole
intervention had been completed, we included both experimental manipulations (promotion
vs. prevention frame, as well as self-implied vs. self not-implied evidence of bias) as indepen-
dent variables. Results showed no effects of the framing, nor the self-implication manipulation,
on TPR during the second speech, Fs<1.80, ps>.182. Regarding CO, we found no main
effect of self-implication, F(1, 82) = 0.86, p= .357, nor an interaction effect between self-impli-
cation and framing, F(1, 82) = 0.89, p= .349. However, in line with our hypothesis, there was a
main effect of promotion vs prevention frame on CO, F(1, 82) = 4.39, p= .039, indicating
higher CO in the promotion (M= 0.16, SD = 0.51) than in the prevention condition (M=
PLOS ONE
Overcoming the threat of anti-bias interventions
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0314813 January 13, 2025 12 / 26
-0.04, SD = 0.40). Even more interesting, results showed a main effect for frame on the com-
bined TCI, indicating that participants in the promotion condition were relatively more chal-
lenged (M= 0.35, SD = 1.64), than participants in the prevention condition, who were
relatively more threatened (M= -0.37, SD = 1.72), F(1, 78) = 4.21, p= .043. Neither the main
effect of the self-implication manipulation, nor the interaction effect between self-implication
and framing were significant, F’s <1.49, p’s>.226. Mean difference levels of CO, TPR, and
TCI reactivity during the second speech, as a function of self-implication and framing, are dis-
played in Table 2. The pattern for the combined TCI is displayed in Fig 2.
Perceived coping abilities. Finally, we tested our third hypothesis, that occurrence of the
challenge response when reflecting on the promotion of the fair and just (instead of the pre-
vention of unfair or biased) treatment of both male and female teachers, would be mediated by
students’ perceived coping abilities. To do this, we first examined whether there were any
Table 1. Cardiovascular reactivity as a function of self (not) implying evidence during Speech 1.
Self-implied Self not-implied
M
diff
SD M
diff
SD
CO 0.25*0.61 0.45*** 0.64
TPR 177.24*480.22 24.96 571.21
TCI -0.27 1.62 0.29 1.76
Note. Condition means were tested against zero to determine significant increases or decreases from the baseline for
CO = Cardiac Output, and TPR = Total Peripheral Resistance. TCI = Threat-Challenge Index, reflected in Z-scores,
with lower scores indicating relatively more threat, higher scores more challenge.
*p<.05.
** p<.01.
*** p<.001.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0314813.t001
Table 2. Cardiovascular reactivity as a function of self-implication and frame during Speech 2.
Promotion Prevention Total
M
diff
SD M
diff
SD M
diff
SD
CO
Self-implied 0.09 0.58 -0.05 0.37 0.02 0.49
Self not-implied 0.23*0.44 -0.02 0.43 0.10 0.44
Total 0.16*0.51 -0.04 0.40 0.06 0.47
TPR
Self-implied 218.70** 352.36 495.69** 733.01 350.90 577.08
Self not-implied 206.89 571.29 207.08*414.92 206.98** 494.71
Total 213.21** 461.21 358.60*** 613.44 283.28 541.54
TCI
Self-implied 0.20 1.60 -0.62 1.90 -0.19 1.78
Self not-implied 0.52 1.71 -0.11 1.51 0.22 1.63
Total 0.35 1.64 -0.37 1.72 0.00 1.71
Note. All condition means were tested against zero to determine significant increases or decreases from the baseline; CO = Cardiac Output, TPR = Total Peripheral
Resistance, TCI = Threat-Challenge Indices, these reflect Z-scores, with lower scores indicating relatively more threat, higher scores more challenge.
*p<.05.
** p<.01.
*** p<.001.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0314813.t002
PLOS ONE
Overcoming the threat of anti-bias interventions
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0314813 January 13, 2025 13 / 26
differences between conditions on participants’ self-reported coping abilities regarding both
the first and the second speeches. Results indicated there were none (all F’s 2.66, all p’s
.11). Due to the absence of a main effect of framing on participants perceived coping abilities
in their second speech, we could not test for mediation.
Exploratively, we then examined whether combined TCI during the second speech (for
which we found the main effect of frame as reported above) correlated with perceived coping
abilities. Results showed no significant overall correlation between TCI during the second
speech and perceived coping ability, although it was in the expected (positive) direction r(83)
= .18, p= .11. In line with our hypothesis, an additional test of the within-cell correlations
showed a positive correlation between coping ability and the TCI in the promotion condition,
r(43) = .33, p= .029, while in the prevention condition this relation was not significant, r(40) =
.12, p= .468. Thus, enhanced perceived coping abilities during the speech in the promotion
condition, were related to a stronger challenge response, while there was no reliable relation
between these two variables in the prevention condition.
Additional self-report measures
Willingness to take action. We explored whether participants would be more willing to
contribute to a student panel to deal with the issue of gender bias in teacher evaluations,
depending on the confrontation with (self-implying) evidence of bias or the promotion versus
prevention frame. To this end, we conducted a chi-square test with the self-implication and
framing manipulation as the independent variables and the four answer-categories (i.e., active
or passive involvement in the student panel, and active or passive distancing from contributing
Fig 2. Threat-Challenge Index (TCI) as a function of evidence (self-implied vs. Self not-implied) and framing (promotion vs. Prevention)
during Speech 2.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0314813.g002
PLOS ONE
Overcoming the threat of anti-bias interventions
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0314813 January 13, 2025 14 / 26
to the student panel) as the dependent variable. The analysis only yielded a significant result
for the self-implication manipulation (X
2
= 10.64, p= .014, phi = 0.34). Post-hoc analyses
using z-scores [77] indicated that participants who had been confronted with self-implying
evidence of bias tended more towards active involvement in how to deal with the issue of gen-
der bias in teacher evaluations (by taking part in the panel) or to passively distance themselves
from the issue (by indicating having other things to do). However, participants who were not
confronted with evidence of their own (group’s) bias tended more towards passive involve-
ment in how to deal with the issue (by subscribing to a newsletter) or to actively distance them-
selves from the issue (by indicating to not wanting to get mixed-up in discussions like these
(all p’s <.05). There were no significant differences between conditions in how often partici-
pants said yes or no (p’s >0.05), nor did we find any effects for the promotion vs. prevention
framing (X
2
= 0.57, p= .903, phi = 0.08).
Future intentions to regulate bias. We also explored whether participants’ deliberate
intentions to regulate (their own) bias were different depending on the confrontation with
(self-implying) evidence of bias or the promotion versus prevention frame. A 2 (Self-implied:
Yes vs. No) ×2 (Frame: Promotion vs. Prevention) ANOVA revealed no significant main
effect of self-implication, F(1,97) = 0.24, p= .624, ωp
2
= -0.007, framing, F(1,97) = 0.11, p=
.740, ωp
2
= -0.009, or interaction, F(1,97) = 1.41, p= .238, ωp
2
= 0.004 on self-stated intentions.
This indicates that participants’ stated concerns about (potentially) exhibiting gender bias or
monitoring their own (potentially biased) behaviour were not affected by the experimental
manipulations in our intervention.
Belief in present gender discrimination. To examine whether participants in the promo-
tion condition reported greater belief in present gender discrimination than participants in the
prevention condition and whether this differed across the self-implication conditions, we con-
ducted a two-way ANOVA. Results revealed no main effect of the self-implied manipulation, F
(1, 97) = 3.06, p= .083, ωp
2
= 0.02, nor an interaction effect, F(1, 97) = .23, p= .630, ωp
2
=
-0.007. Interestingly however, there was a main effect of the framing manipulation, F(1, 97) =
3.27, p= .074, ωp
2
= 0.022, indicating that participants in the promotion condition reported
greater belief in present gender discrimination (M= 5.49, SD = 0.86) than participants in the
prevention condition (M= 5.13, SD = 1.01). These findings thus reveal that a promotion focus
on how to address gender bias in teacher evaluations may help to open one’s eyes to the persis-
tence of the problem of social bias.
Discussion
In the current research, we examined the effects of a gender bias intervention in which people
were confronted with disconcerting evidence about this ongoing problem in society. We con-
sider this intervention an example of how results of scientific research are typically communi-
cated, in order to influence people’s attitudes and behaviours: It is common practice to present
evidence of the occurrence of the problem—and to emphasize how the self is implied in the
persistence of these problems—as a way to convey the relevance and urgency of particular
issues. Nevertheless, we hypothesized that confronting people in this way is also likely to raise
a threat response which gives rise to defensiveness, denial, avoidance, or passivity. In addition
to examining the occurrence of threat, we therefore also examined whether and how people
can be helped to overcome such initial stress responses, framing the intended outcome of the
intervention differently to increase its impact. In this way, we aimed to gain a better under-
standing of the psychological mechanisms that are activated when people are confronted with
evidence of ongoing problems in society–in this case the existence of gender bias in teaching
evaluations. To achieve this aim, we first tested the prediction that people feel threatened when
PLOS ONE
Overcoming the threat of anti-bias interventions
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0314813 January 13, 2025 15 / 26
presented with evidence of their own group’s bias (by means of a self-implication manipula-
tion, in the first part of the intervention). Second, we tested whether a promotion focus (vs.
prevention focus; framing manipulation, in the second part of the intervention) to address the
issue of gender bias, would help to elicit a motivational state of challenge. A visualization of the
different parts of the intervention and our main findings are displayed in Fig 3.
We found evidence in line with predictions. First, participants who were confronted with
self-implying evidence of gender bias (as compared to participants who saw evidence not
implying the self) showed an increased initial psychophysiological threat response—as indi-
cated by higher Total Peripheral Resistance (TPR) and relatively lower Cardiac Output (CO).
This extends prior research revealing the threat of being reminded of the moral shortcomings
of oneself or one’s group–which is greater than the threat of being reminded of past compe-
tence failures [44]. Further, previous research has shown that people feel particularly threat-
ened and show defensive responses when confronted with information about the moral
inadequacy of another ingroup member, compared to when they are confronted with moral
failures of outgroup members [34]. In general, when the occurrence of threat and defensive
Fig 3. Overview of the findings.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0314813.g003
PLOS ONE
Overcoming the threat of anti-bias interventions
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0314813 January 13, 2025 16 / 26
responses is observed, people become less motivated to act and engage in initiatives to address
the topic [36,38]. However, our study is the first to show how common intervention strate-
gies–in which research evidence of ongoing problems is presented as a way to motivate people
towards change–can raise a threat response that can have counterproductive effects.
The second main finding of our study is therefore of utmost importance. In line with our
hypothesis, results on the Threat-Challenge Index (TCI) showed how the initial threat
response can be overcome. That is, we observed a reliable difference in the cardiovascular
responses of participants in the promotion condition and participants in the prevention condi-
tions. Thus, once participants were asked to verbally reflect on ways to pursue the ideal of pro-
moting fair evaluations of teachers, they showed relatively more challenge than those who were
asked to address the issue by listing potential efforts to meet the obligation of preventing unfair
evaluations—who showed relatively more threat during this task. The promotion condition
also made them more likely to acknowledge that gender bias in teaching evaluations continues
to be a problem. This observation extends previous research in which students were asked to
reflect on evidence of ongoing labour discrimination, about which their responses differed
depending on whether the moral implications of cultural diversity were framed in terms of
ideals or obligations [39]. In line with that work, we demonstrate that encouraging a focus on
promoting positive outcomes and ideals can make people more receptive of information docu-
menting one’s own role and responsibility in the persistence of social problems [34,39,40,42].
This suggests that the counterproductive effects of presenting scientific results to communicate
the severity, urgency and self-relevance of a societal problem can be overcome. That is, explic-
itly adopting a focus on the promotion of ideal and desired outcomes (rather than focusing on
the obligation to prevent problems) highlights future opportunities for change. This helps peo-
ple to acknowledge the severity of ongoing problems and stimulates them to consider and
develop strategies for (behavioural) improvement (see also [34,39,40])).
Related to the third aim and hypothesis of our research, we examined whether the state of
challenge was related to self-reported coping abilities. Our findings showed that–only in the
promotion condition–participants who displayed a stronger cardiovascular challenge response
also reported feeling more able to cope with the situation. This extends prior work which doc-
umented that helping people to focus on future improvements, rather than past failures—as
well as presenting their past failures as indicating a lack of competence rather than stemming
from a lack of moral intentions [46]—enhances their perceived coping abilities and reduces
defensive responses. We observed a similar effect when we invited participants—after comple-
tion of the whole intervention—to consider what they might do differently in the future. Here
we found that those who had received self-implying evidence of gender bias in teaching evalua-
tions (but regardless of whether they were presented with a promotion or prevention frame to
deal with the issue in the future) were more willing to take action, for instance by becoming
involved in a student panel to talk about the future of teaching evaluations. We think this
extends prior work and shows how important it is for interventions to both highlight the sever-
ity and urgency of particular problems—for instance by emphasizing how the self is implied in
the persistence of these problems—, as well as to help people look forward to possible solutions
and opportunities to change their future behaviours. That is, our findings indicate that in the-
ory, research evidence of ongoing problems, as well as people’s own role in perpetuating these
problems, can communicate the urgency, severity and relevance of such problems, and
enhance their willingness to take action. However, our findings also show the dangers of this
approach–when documenting gender bias. That is, examining cardiovascular response pat-
terns at different stages of the intervention, allowed us to reveal an initial threat response when
being confronted with self-implying evidence of bias–which in practice can raise defensive,
avoidant, and passive responses [36,37,38]. Thus, we emphasize the importance of the second
PLOS ONE
Overcoming the threat of anti-bias interventions
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0314813 January 13, 2025 17 / 26
part of our intervention, containing our second experimental manipulation. Here we demon-
strated that explicitly emphasizing the promotion of desired outcomes—in this case the ideal
of fair evaluations—caused participants to display a physiological challenge response, which
was associated with greater self-perceived coping abilities. Furthermore, only in this condition
were participants inclined to accept and embrace the evidence shown to them, and acknowl-
edge the perseverance of gender bias as an ongoing problem.
Theoretical and practical implications
To optimize the impact of interventions that rely on the communication of research findings
and provide participants with self-implying evidence of the recurring problem, it is important
to examine when and why such interventions may and may not work, and how they can be
made more effective. The findings from the current research contribute to these insights by
showing how specific aspects of an anti-bias intervention impacts on the experiences of people
taking part in the intervention. Thus far, most of the anti-bias interventions that are offered
are not systematically evaluated for their impact [6]. And if evaluations are made, these usually
consist of proximal subjective ratings, asking individual participants to indicate retrospectively
whether they liked taking part in the intervention [79,80]. Yet from other types of interven-
tions (e.g., aiming to improve people’s lifestyle or health behaviours), we know that subjective
likeability of the intervention does not necessarily predict the probability of behavioural
improvement over time [16,19,81]. Studies that do address more distal and objective indica-
tors tend to rely on macro-level archival data (e.g., observed changes over time in organiza-
tional level hiring percentages of groups targeted in the intervention [82]. In studies such as
these, little information is available about the underlying process of change or the psychologi-
cal mechanisms through which specific interventions result in these outcomes. The current
study thus complements prior efforts by directly examining the process of change participants
undergo at different stages of an the intervention. Further, in addition to measuring partici-
pants’ explicit retrospective evaluations of the intervention by means of self-report question-
naires, we assessed their more implicit responses to different parts of the intervention in real-
time. That is, we measured participants’ physiological states while they reflected on the con-
frontation with (self-implying) evidence of bias, and while they considered ways to either pre-
vent such bias from occurring or to promote unbiased evaluations.
Importantly, our real-time physiological measures revealed a threat response caused by the
confrontation with self-implying evidence of bias, as well as a challenge response when consid-
ering how to promote more fair evaluations. Importantly, this information on how partici-
pants were affected by the intervention could not be captured from the more traditional and
deliberate self-report measures. Yet, we did observe the impact of the intervention on partici-
pants’ self-stated willingness to acknowledge the perseverance of the problem, and their will-
ingness to address it. Thus, this demonstrates the added value of including implicit measures
of psychophysiological responses, and capturing changes in the associated motivational states,
in response to different aspects of the intervention.
The self-report measures in our study revealed that participants who were confronted with
evidence of gender bias in their own and their fellow group members’ teaching evaluations,
thought this evidence was based on fact and not misleading or exaggerated. However, these
participants also reported not to believe that this self-implying evidence of gender bias repre-
sented their true values or reflected their true thoughts or feelings. These findings are in line
with previous research indicating that bias is often implicit [83,84], which makes it difficult to
confront people with such bias in explicit ways. Yet, many anti-bias interventions are focused
on making people aware of such biases to elicit more positive attitudes and behavioural
PLOS ONE
Overcoming the threat of anti-bias interventions
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0314813 January 13, 2025 18 / 26
changes towards disadvantaged groups [14,15]. Importantly, our psychophysiological findings
indicated that this self-implying evidence was associated with a motivational state of threat.
Other research has revealed that this may result in less motivation to act due to different inter-
pretations of the evidence of (one’s own, or one’s group) social bias, depending on people’s
perspective and ideology and how they view the source of the message. In other words, just
giving the facts is not enough to get people to change [18,85]. The results of the current study
therefore not only show the potential downsides of procedures that try to engage people who
attend an intervention by confronting them with evidence of (their own) bias, they also reveal
that alleviating such threat further helps people to consider possibilities to address the issue.
In terms of practical implications, the findings of the current study are relevant to the
notion that ‘best practices’ regarding diversity and inclusion initiatives are not characterized
only by what, for instance, organizations do to combat (implicit) bias but also depends on how
they do this. That is, even though many anti-bias interventions are based on raising awareness
about people’s own (implicit) biases, our findings show that being confronted with this kind of
evidence can trigger threat responses. Moreover, we have shown that reflecting on what ideals
to pursue to address the issue of social bias is associated with a motivational state of challenge
and greater perceived coping abilities. In other words, anti-bias interventions may include a
direct way of making people aware of the presence of bias in their own group, but such evi-
dence is preferably accompanied by the opportunity to address the issue in terms of one’s
(shared) ideals. For instance, by reflecting on ways to strive for the promotion of fair judge-
ments, rather than in terms of what one is obliged to do by reflecting on one’s obligation to
prevent bias.
Limitations and suggestions for future research
Our findings, based on both explicit self-reports as well as more implicit real-time psychophys-
iological indices of motivational states, underline the importance and added value of having a
multi-level approach to examine the responses of participants to anti-bias interventions. That
is, consistent with the design of prior research, our self-report measures were assessed after the
entire intervention—including the presentation of self-implied vs. self not-implied evidence of
bias, and the promotion vs. prevention frame for addressing the bias. They thereby indicate
how people reflect on the issue of the existence of gender bias, and how to address it, in relative
retrospect. In contrast, our psychophysiological measures are indicative of people’s real-time
implicit responses, which provided our findings of motivational states of threat and challenge
during the intervention—which at times diverged from our self-report findings. Then again,
we may have found different results if we had included part of the self-reports after the first
experimental manipulation in which participants had only been confronted with their own
(group’s) bias, without having had the opportunity (yet) to think about how to deal with the
issue. A suggestion for future research would be to examine the effects of confronting people
with their own group’s bias, by also including self-report measures of acceptance of that evi-
dence and potential defensiveness towards that evidence, directly after the evidence is pre-
sented. This would allow for a more thorough investigation of the extent to which becoming
aware of the presence of bias in one’s own group is related to participants’ automatic psycho-
physiological threat response as well as their subsequent deliberate reflection on the issue.
Relatedly, decided not to include a post-measure of participants’ teaching evaluations to test
the effectiveness of our intervention, as has been done in prior work on gender bias in teaching
evaluations [86,87]. This was outside the scope of the current research, as we intentionally
focused on investigating the underlying psychological mechanisms that may contribute to the
varying outcomes of such interventions (as well as the mixed results of anti-bias interventions
PLOS ONE
Overcoming the threat of anti-bias interventions
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0314813 January 13, 2025 19 / 26
in various other contexts [e.g., 14,15,88]. Further, we note that the impact of our intervention
could be broader than that (e.g., by motivating students to take action against survey methods
to evaluate teaching quality due to the realization that these are subject to bias). Nevertheless,
future work could include a post-measurement of bias to more specifically assess whether and
how these types of awareness raising interventions help to eliminate bias.
Additionally, our current examination addressed a single-session intervention. Future
research might explore how this works in a more elaborate approach, where multiple sessions
are used to address specific phases and aspects of the intervention, and each are evaluated
separately.
Furthermore, future work could extend intervention materials and session characteristics
beyond the webinar paradigm we used in the current study. That is, we asked participants to indi-
vidually view a webinar about gender bias in teacher evaluations and to verbally reflect on this
gender bias via a computer screen. An alternative way of presenting such an intervention is dur-
ing an actual group event, allowing for real time interactions or workgroup sessions with other
attendees. This is another option to explore in future research, where multiple sessions can be
used to compare the impact of different types of interventions (see also [89]). Nevertheless, con-
sidering that many of such events have been replaced by webinars or other online meetings since
the Covid-19 pandemic, our experimental paradigm nicely reveals how such a ‘distant’ approach
to anti-bias interventions can still have impact and be effective. In fact, we think this approach
can be extended to other topics and issues where research findings are used as an intervention to
convey the severity and urgency of ongoing social problems. Indeed, the results of our investiga-
tion indicate that the social impact and benefits of research findings can be enhanced when pay-
ing more attention to the psychological effects of science communications on its recipients.
Conclusion
In conclusion, our research shows that anti-bias interventions that include self-implying evidence
of the presence of bias may enhance the perceived urgency and relevance of the problem that
might benefit participants’ willingness to take action. However, we also demonstrated that this is
likely to elicit a threat response, which is associated with denial, avoidance, and other maladaptive
responses. Importantly therefore, this work elucidates the added value of presenting research evi-
dence of bias in combination with the invitation to think about ways to promote ideal outcomes.
Indeed, an intervention that combines these two aspects is most likely to result in a motivational
state of challenge, where people are willing to acknowledge that gender discrimination is an ongo-
ing problem, but also experience the ability to cope with this problem by taking action.
Supporting information
S1 Appendix. Teacher evaluation survey.
(DOCX)
S2 Appendix. Additional measures lab session.
(DOCX)
S3 Appendix. Speech data.
(DOCX)
Acknowledgments
We want to thank the following (former) members of the Organizational Behavior group for
their help with data collection, data preparation, and/or preliminary data analyses (in
PLOS ONE
Overcoming the threat of anti-bias interventions
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0314813 January 13, 2025 20 / 26
alphabetical order): Jamie Breukel, Nadia Buiter, Martine Kloet, Wiebren Jansen, Inga Ro¨sler,
Jeanette van der Lee, and Marleen van Stokkum. In addition, we thank Ronny Ramos Delgado
for his help with the preparation of the manuscript for submission. Furthermore, we want to
thank other (former) members of the Organizational Behavior Group with whom we discussed
the idea, conceptualization, methodology and write-up of this research (in alphabetical order):
Tatiana Chopova, Tessa Coffeng, Florien Cramwinckel, Piet Groot, Onur Sahin, Elianne van
Steenbergen, and Melissa Vink. The current Organizational Behavior Group membership list
can be found here: https://www.uu.nl/en/research/organisational-behaviour/our-team. Lead
author for this group is Naomi Ellemers (contact: n.ellemers@uu.nl).
Author Contributions
Conceptualization: Fe
´lice van Nunspeet, Esmee M. Veenstra, Naomi Ellemers, Daan Schee-
pers, Miriam I. Wickham, Jojanneke van der Toorn.
Data curation: Esmee M. Veenstra, Beatriz Monteiro Grac¸a Casquinho.
Formal analysis: Fe
´lice van Nunspeet, Esmee M. Veenstra, Beatriz Monteiro Grac¸a Cas-
quinho, Daan Scheepers, Miriam I. Wickham, Elena A. M. Bacchini, Jojanneke van der
Toorn.
Funding acquisition: Naomi Ellemers.
Investigation: Esmee M. Veenstra, Miriam I. Wickham.
Methodology: Fe
´lice van Nunspeet, Esmee M. Veenstra, Naomi Ellemers, Daan Scheepers,
Miriam I. Wickham, Jojanneke van der Toorn.
Project administration: Esmee M. Veenstra.
Supervision: Fe
´lice van Nunspeet, Naomi Ellemers.
Validation: Fe
´lice van Nunspeet, Esmee M. Veenstra, Beatriz Monteiro Grac¸a Casquinho,
Miriam I. Wickham, Elena A. M. Bacchini.
Visualization: Fe
´lice van Nunspeet, Esmee M. Veenstra.
Writing – original draft: Fe
´lice van Nunspeet, Esmee M. Veenstra, Beatriz Monteiro Grac¸a
Casquinho, Naomi Ellemers, Daan Scheepers, Miriam I. Wickham, Elena A. M. Bacchini,
Jojanneke van der Toorn.
Writing – review & editing: Fe
´lice van Nunspeet, Esmee M. Veenstra, Beatriz Monteiro
Grac¸a Casquinho, Naomi Ellemers, Daan Scheepers, Miriam I. Wickham, Elena A. M. Bac-
chini, Jojanneke van der Toorn.
References
1. Lewin K. Action research and minority problems. In: Lewin GW, editors. Resolving social conflicts:
Selected papers on group dynamics. New York, NY: Harper & Row; 1948. p. 201–216)
2. Esses VM, Dovidio JF. Social Psychology, Social Issues, and Social Policy: What Have We Learned?
Social Issues & Policy Review. 2011; 5:1–7. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-2409.2011.01023.x
3. Gunn A, Mintrom M. Higher Education Policy Change in Europe: Academic Research Funding and the
Impact Agenda. European Education. 2016; 48: 241–57. https://doi.org/10.1080/10564934.2016.
1237703
4. Pinheiro R, Langa PV, Pausits A. The institutionalization of universities’ third mission: Introduction to
the special issue. European Journal of Higher Education. 2015; 5: 227–32. https://doi.org/10.1080/
21568235.2015.1044551
PLOS ONE
Overcoming the threat of anti-bias interventions
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0314813 January 13, 2025 21 / 26
5. Sassenberg K, Vliek ML. W. Social Psychology in Action: Evidence-Based Interventions From Theory
to Practice. Cham: Springer; 2019. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-13788-5
6. Ellemers N, Şahin O, Jansen WS, van der Toorn J. Naar effectief diversiteitsbeleid: Het bouwen van
bruggen tussen wetenschap en praktijk. Gedrag en Organisatie. 2018; 31:409–28. https://doi.org/10.
5117/2018.031.004.006
7. Moss-Racusin CA, van der Toorn J, Dovidio JF, Brescoll VL, Graham MJ, Handelsman J. Scientific
Diversity Interventions. Science. 2014; 343:615–6. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1245936 PMID:
24503840
8. Schmidt Kalpazidou, Schmidt EK, Ovseiko PV, Henderson LR, Kiparoglou V. Understanding the
Athena SWAN award scheme for gender equality as a complex social intervention in a complex system:
analysis of Silver award action plans in a comparative European perspective. Health Research Policy
and Systems. 2020; 18:1–21. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12961-020-0527-x PMID: 32059678
9. Kalev A, Dobbin F, Kelly E. Best practices or best guesses? Assessing the efficacy of corporate affirma-
tive action and diversity policies. American Sociological Review. 2006; 71:589–617. https://doi.org/10.
1177/000312240607100404
10. Dover TL, Kaiser CR, Major B. Mixed Signals: The Unintended Effects of Diversity Initiatives. Social
Issues and Policy Review. 2020; 14:152–81. https://doi.org/10.1111/sipr.12059
11. Kraus MW, Ivuoma Onyeador, Ezgi Ozgumus, Portocarrero S, Rattan A, Torrez B, et al. The Promises
and Pitfalls of Diversity and Inclusion in Organizations and Society. Academy of Management Annual
Meeting Proceedings. 2021 Jan 1; 2021(1):1. https://doi.org/10.5465/AMBPP.2021.12524symposium
12. Greenwald AG, Banaji MR. Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psycho-
logical Review. 1995 Jan 1; 102(1):4–27. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.102.1.4 PMID: 7878162
13. Lai CK, Teachman BA, Frazier RS, Hawkins CB, Schaefer HS, Sriram N, et al. Reducing implicit racial
preferences: I. A comparative investigation of 17 interventions. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
General. 2014 Jan 1; 143(4):1765–85. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036260 PMID: 24661055
14. Carnes M, Devine PG, Manwell LB, Byars-Winston A, Fine E, Ford CE, et al. The Effect of an Interven-
tion to Break the Gender Bias Habit for Faculty atOne Institution: A Cluster Randomized, Controlled
Trial. Academic Medicine. 2015 Feb 1; 90(2):221–30. https://doi.org/10.1097/ACM.
0000000000000552 PMID: 25374039
15. Devine PG, Forscher PS, Austin AJ, Cox WTL. Long-term reduction in implicit race bias: A prejudice
habit-breaking intervention. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2012 Nov 1; 48(6):1267–78.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2012.06.003 PMID: 23524616
16. Chang EH, Milkman KL, Massey C, Gromet DM, Rebele RW, Duckworth AL, et al. The mixed effects of
online diversity training. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of
America. 2019 Apr 16; 116(16):7778–83. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1816076116 PMID: 30936313
17. Dobbin F, Kalev A. Why Doesn’t Diversity Training Work? The Challenge for Industry and Academia.
Anthropology Now. 2018 May 4; 10(2):48–55. https://doi.org/10.1080/19428200.2018.1493182
18. Handley IM, Brown ER, Smith JL, Moss-Racusin CA. Quality of evidence revealing subtle gender
biases in science is in the eye of the beholder. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
United States of America. 2015 Oct 27; 112(43):13201–6. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1510649112
PMID: 26460001
19. Lai CK, Skinner AL, Cooley E, Murrar S, Brauer M, Devos T, et al. Reducing implicit racial preferences:
II. Intervention effectiveness across time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2016 Aug 1;
145(8):1001–16. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000179 PMID: 27454041
20. Moss-Racusin CA, Molenda AK, Cramer CR. Can evidence impact attitudes? Public reactions to evi-
dence of gender bias in STEM fields. Psychology of Women Quarterly. 2015 Jun 1; 39(2):194–209.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0361684314565777
21. Paluck EL, Green DP. Prejudice Reduction: What Works? A Review and Assessment of Research and
Practice. Annual Review of Psychology. 2009 Feb 1; 60(1):339–67. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.
psych.60.110707.163607 PMID: 18851685
22. Williamson S, Foley M. Unconscious Bias Training: The ‘Silver Bullet’ for Gender Equity? Australian
Journal of Public Administration. 2018 Sep 1; 77(3):355–9. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8500.12313
23. Ellemers Naomi. Morality and the Regulation of Social Behavior: Groups As Moral Anchors. Basing-
stoke: Routledge; 2017.
24. Van Nunspeet F, Ellemers N, Derks B, Nieuwenhuis S. Moral concerns increase attention and response
monitoring during IAT performance: ERP evidence. Social Cognitive & Affective Neuroscience. 2014
Feb 1; 9(2):141–9. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nss118 PMID: 23175679
25. Van Nunspeet F, Derks B, Ellemers N, Nieuwenhuis S. Moral Impression Management: Evaluation by
an In-Group Member During a Moral IAT Affects Perceptual Attention and Conflict and Response
PLOS ONE
Overcoming the threat of anti-bias interventions
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0314813 January 13, 2025 22 / 26
Monitoring. Social Psychological and Personality Science. 2015 Mar 13; 6(2):183–92. https://doi.org/
10.1177/1948550614548076
26. Haidt J. The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psycho-
logical Review. 2001 Oct 1; 108(4):814–34. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.108.4.814 PMID:
11699120
27. Greene J, Haidt J. How (and where) does moral judgment work? Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2002
Jan 1; 6(12):517–23. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(02)02011-9 PMID: 12475712
28. Ellemers N, Pagliaro S, Barreto M. Morality and behavioural regulation in groups: A social identity
approach. European Review of Social Psychology. 2013 Dec 1; 24(1):160–93. https://doi.org/10.1080/
10463283.2013.841490
29. Branscombe NR, Doosje B, McGarty C. Antecedents and consequences of collective guilt. In: Mackie
DM, Smith ER, editors. From prejudice to intergroup emotions: Differentiated reactions to social groups.
Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press; 2002.p. 49–66.
30. Doosje B, Branscombe NR, Spears R, Manstead ASR. Guilty by association: When one’s group has a
negative history. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1998 Oct 1; 75(4):872–86. https://doi.
org/10.1037/0022-3514.75.4.872
31. Hornsey MJ, Philpot CR, Wohl MJA. Collective Apologies and Their Effects on Forgiveness: Pessimistic
Evidence but Constructive Implications. Australian Psychologist. 2015 Apr 1; 50(2):106–14. https://doi.
org/10.1111/ap.12087
32. Leach CW, Iyer A, Pedersen A. Anger and Guilt About Ingroup Advantage Explain the Willingness for
Political Action. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2006 Sep 1; 32(9):1232–45. https://doi.org/
10.1177/0146167206289729 PMID: 16902242
33. Ta
¨uber S, van Zomeren M, Kutlaca M. Should the moral core of climate issues be emphasized or down-
played in public discourse? Three ways to successfully manage the double-edged sword of moral com-
munication. Climatic Change. 2015 Jun 30; 130(3):453–64. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-014-1200-6
34. Van der Toorn J, Ellemers N, Doosje B. The threat of moral transgression: The impact of group mem-
bership and moral opportunity. European Journal of Social Psychology. 2015 Aug 1; 45(5):609–22.
https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2119
35. Van Zomeren M, Postmes T, Spears R. On conviction’s collective consequences: Integrating moral
conviction with the social identity model of collective action. British Journal of Social Psychology. 2012
Mar 1; 51(1):52–71. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8309.2010.02000.x PMID: 22435846
36. Howell JL, Ratliff KA. Not your average bigot: The better-than-average effect and defensive responding
to Implicit Association Test feedback. British Journal of Social Psychology. 2017 Mar 1; 56(1):125–45.
https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12168 PMID: 27709628
37. Legault L, Gutsell JN, Inzlicht M. Ironic effects of antiprejudice messages: How motivational interven-
tions can reduce (but also increase) prejudice. Psychological Science. 2011 Jan 1; 22(12):1472–7.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611427918 PMID: 22123778
38. Parker LR, Monteith MJ, Moss-Racusin CA, Van Camp AR. Promoting concern about gender bias with
evidence-based confrontation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2018 Jan 1; 74:8–23.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2017.07.009
39. Does S, Derks B, Ellemers N. Thou shalt not discriminate: How emphasizing moral ideals rather than
obligations increases Whites’ support for social equality. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology.
2011 Jan 1; 47(3):562–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2010.12.024
40. Does S, Derks B, Ellemers N, Scheepers D. At the heart of egalitarianism: How morality framing shapes
cardiovascular challenge versus threat in whites. Social Psychological and Personality Science. 2012
Nov 1; 3(6):747–53. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550612438924
41. Moss-Racusin CA, van der Toorn J, Dovidio JF, Brescoll VL, Graham MJ, Handelsman J. A “Scientific
Diversity” Intervention to Reduce Gender Bias in a Sample of Life Scientists. CBE—Life Sciences Edu-
cation. 2016 Sep 1; 15(3). https://doi.org/10.1187/cbe.15-09-0187 PMID: 27496360
42. Higgins ET. Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist. 1997 Dec 1; 52(12):1280–300. https://
doi.org/10.1037//0003-066x.52.12.1280 PMID: 9414606
43. Higgins ET, Shah J, Friedman R. Emotional responses to goal attainment: Strength of regulatory focus
as moderator. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1997 Mar 1; 72(3):515–25. https://doi.org/
10.1037//0022-3514.72.3.515 PMID: 9120782
44. Van der Lee R, Ellemers N, Scheepers D. Mastering moral misery: Emotional and coping responses to
intragroup morality (vs. competence) evaluations. Cognition & Emotion. 2016 Jan 1; 30(1):51–65.
https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2015.1050357 PMID: 26192008
PLOS ONE
Overcoming the threat of anti-bias interventions
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0314813 January 13, 2025 23 / 26
45. Pagliaro S, Di Cesare C, Ellemers N, Barreto M. Once dishonest, always dishonest? The impact of per-
ceived pervasiveness of moral evaluations of the self on motivation to restore a moral reputation. Fron-
tiers in Psychology. 2016 Jan 1;7 https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00586 PMID: 27199821
46. Ro
¨sler IK, van Nunspeet F, Ellemers N. Don’t tell me about my moral failures but motivate me to
improve: Increasing effectiveness of outgroup criticism by criticizing one’s competence. EuropeanJour-
nal of Social Psychology. 2021 Apr 1; 51(3):597–609. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2764
47. Blascovich J. Challenge, threat, and health. In: Shah JY, Gardner WL, editors. Handbook of motivation
science. New York: Guilford Press; 2008.p. 481–493.
48. Blascovich J, Mendes WB. Social psychophysiology and embodiment. In: Gilbert D, Fiske S, Lindzey
G, editors. Handbook of social psychology, 5th Edition. New York: Wiley; 2010.p.194–227.
49. Blascovich J, Tomaka J. The biopsychosocial model of arousal regulation. In: Advances in experimental
social psychology, Vol 28. San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 1996. p. 1–51. https://doi.org/10.1016/
S0065-2601(08)60235-X
50. Mendes WB, Park J. Neurobiological concomitants of motivation states. In: Elliot AJ, editors. Advances
in motivation science. Waltham, MA: Academic Press; 2014.p. 233–270.
51. Seery MD. Challenge or threat? Cardiovascular indexes of resilience and vulnerability to potential stress
in humans. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2011 Jan 1; 35(7):1603–10. https://doi.org/10.
1016/j.neubiorev.2011.03.003 PMID: 21396399
52. Wormwood JB, Khan Z, Dy J, Siegel E, Lynn SK, Barrett LF, et al. Physiological indices of challenge
and threat: A data-driven investigation of autonomic nervous system reactivity during an active coping
stressor task. Psychophysiology. 2019 Dec 1; 56(12). https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13454 PMID:
31407813
53. Richter M, Gendolla GHE, Wright RA. Three decades of research on motivational intensity theory: What
we have learned about effort and what we still don’t know. In: Advances in motivation science. San
Diego, CA: Elsevier Academic Press; 2016. p. 149–86. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.adms.2016.02.001
54. Blascovich J, Mendes WB, Hunter SB, Lickel B, Kowai-Bell N. Perceiver threat in social interactions
with stigmatized others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2001 Feb 1; 80(2):253–67.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.80.2.253 PMID: 11220444
55. Blascovich J, Seery MD, Mugridge CA, Norris RK, Weisbuch M. Predicting athletic performance from
cardiovascular indexes of challenge and threat. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2004 Sep
1; 40(5):683–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2003.10.007
56. Kassam KS, Koslov K, Mendes WB. Decisions Under Distress: Stress Profiles Influence Anchoring and
Adjustment. Psychological Science (0956–7976). 2009 Nov 1; 20(11):1394–9. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.
1467-9280.2009.02455.x PMID: 19843261
57. Lamarche VM, Seery MD, Kondrak CL, Saltsman TL, Streamer L. Clever girl: Benevolent sexism and
cardiovascular threat. Biological Psychology. 2020 Jan 1; 149. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.
2019.107781 PMID: 31618662
58. Moore LJ, Vine SJ, Wilson MR, Freeman P. Examining the antecedents of challenge and threat states:
The influence of perceived required effort and support availability. International Journal of Psychophysi-
ology. 2014 Aug 1; 93(2):267–73. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.05.009 PMID: 24867434
59. Peters BJ, Reis HT, Jamieson JP. Cardiovascular indexes of threat impair responsiveness in situations
of conflicting interests. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2018 Jan 1; 123:1–7. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2017.12.005 PMID: 29253512
60. Seery MD, Weisbuch M, Hetenyi MA, Blascovich J. Cardiovascular measures independently predict
performance in a university course. Psychophysiology. 2010 May 1; 47(3):535–9. https://doi.org/10.
1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00945.x PMID: 20030765
61. Mengel F, Sauermann J, Zo
¨litz U. Gender Bias in Teaching Evaluations. Journal of the European Eco-
nomic Association. 2019 Apr 1; 17(2):535–66. https://doi.org/10.1093/jeea/jvx057
62. Sprague J, Massoni K. Student evaluations and gendered expectations: What we can’t count can hurt
us. Sex Roles: A Journal of Research. 2005 Dec 1; 53(11–12):779–93. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-
005-8292-4
63. Boring A, Ottoboni K, Stark P. Student Evaluations of Teaching (Mostly) Do Not Measure Teaching
Effectiveness. ScienceOpen Research. 2016 Jan 1. https://doi.org/10.14293/s2199-1006.1.sor-edu.
aetbzc.v1
64. Wagner N, Rieger M, Voorvelt K. Gender, ethnicity and teaching evaluations: Evidence from mixed
teaching teams. Economics of Education Review. 2016 Oct 1; 54:79–94. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
econedurev.2016.06.004
65. Biopac Systems Inc., Goleta, CA, USA
PLOS ONE
Overcoming the threat of anti-bias interventions
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0314813 January 13, 2025 24 / 26
66. Scheepers D, Ro
¨ell C, Ellemers N. Unstable Power Threatens the Powerful and Challenges the Power-
less: Evidence from Cardiovascular Markers of Motivation. Frontiers in Psychology. 2015 May 1; 6.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00720 PMID: 26074860
67. Sjak-Shie EE. PhysioData Toolbox (Version 0.4) [Computer software]. 2018. Available from https://
PhysioDataToolbox.leidenuniv.nl
68. Berntson G.G., Lozano D.L., Chen Y., & Cacioppo J.T. (2004). Where to Q in PEP. Psychophysiology,
41(2):333–7. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2004.00156.x PMID: 15032999
69. Seery MD, Kondrak CL, Streamer L, Saltsman T, Lamarche VM. Preejection period can be calculated
using R peak instead of Q. Psychophysiology. 2016 Aug 1; 53(8):1232–40. https://doi.org/10.1111/
psyp.12657 PMID: 27080937
70. Sherwood A, Allen MT, Fahrenberg J, Kelsey RM, Lovallo WR, van Doornen LJ. Methodological guide-
lines for impedance cardiography. Psychophysiology. 1990 Jan 1; 27(1):1–23. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.
1469-8986.1990.tb02171.x PMID: 2187214
71. Mendes W. B., Blascovich J., Lickel B., & Hunter S. (2002). Challenge and threat during social interac-
tions with White and Black men. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(7), 939–952.
72. Seery M. D., West T. V., Weisbuch M., & Blascovich J. (2008). The effects of negative reflection for
defensive pessimists: Dissipation or harnessing of threat?. Personality and Individual Differences, 45
(6), 515–520.
73. Howell JL, Ratliff KA, Gaither SE. Caught in the Middle: Defensive Responses to IAT Feedback Among
Whites, Blacks, and Biracial Black/Whites. Social Psychological and Personality Science. 2015 May; 6
(4):373–81. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550614561127
74. Howell JL, Redford L, Pogge G, Ratliff KA. Defensive responding to iat feedback. Social Cognition.
2017 Oct 1; 35(5):520–62. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2017.35.5.520
75. Sassenberg K., Jonas K. J., Shah J. Y., & Brazy P. C. (2007). Why some groups just feel better: The
regulatory fit of group power. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(2), 249. https://doi.org/
10.1037/0022-3514.92.2.249 PMID: 17279848
76. Tomaka J, Blascovich J, Kelsey RM, Leitten CL. Subjective, physiological, and behavioral effects of
threat and challenge appraisal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1993 Aug 1; 65(2):248–
60. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.65.2.248
77. Sharpe D. Your Chi-Square Test Is Statistically Significant: Now What? Practical Assessment,
Research & Evaluation. 2015 Apr 1; 20(8). https://doi.org/10.7275/tbfa-x148
78. Rosell MC, Hartman SL. Self-presentation of beliefs about gender discrimination and feminism. Sex
Roles: A Journal of Research. 2001 Jun 1; 44(11–12):647–59. https://doi.org/10.1023/
A:1012294129547
79. Paluck EL. Diversity Training and Intergroup Contact: A Call to Action Research. Journal of Social
Issues. 2006 Sep 1; 62(3):577–95. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.2006.00474.x
80. Cahn PS. Recognizing and Reckoning With Unconscious Bias: A Workshop for Health Professions
Faculty Search Committees. MedEdPORTA. 2017 Feb 1; 13. https://doi.org/10.15766/mep_2374-
8265.10544 PMID: 30800746
81. Ford JK, Baldwin TT, Prasad J. Transfer of training: The known and the unknown. Annual Review of
Organizational Psychology and Organizational Behavior. 2018 Jan 1; 5:201–25. https://doi.org/10.
1146/annurev-orgpsych-032117-104443
82. Devine PG, Cox WTL, Forscher PS, Kaatz A, Carnes M, Sheridan J. A gender bias habit-breaking inter-
vention led to increased hiring of female faculty in STEMM departments. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology. 2017 Nov 1; 73:211–5. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2017.07.002 PMID: 29249837
83. Devine PG. Implicit prejudice and stereotyping: How automatic are they? Introduction to the special sec-
tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2001 Nov 1; 81(5):757–9. https://doi.org/10.1037//
0022-3514.81.5.757 PMID: 11708554
84. Lai CK, Hoffman KM, Nosek BA. Reducing Implicit Prejudice. Social & Personality Psychology Com-
pass. 2013 May 1; 7(5):315–30. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12023
85. Zhu L, Aquino K, Vadera AK. What makes professors appear credible: The effect of demographic char-
acteristics and ideological beliefs. Journal of Applied Psychology. 2016 Jun 1; 101(6):862–80. https://
doi.org/10.1037/apl0000095 PMID: 26949817
86. Peterson D. A., Biederman L. A., Andersen D., Ditonto T. M., & Roe K. (2019). Mitigating gender bias in
student evaluations of teaching. PloS One, 14(5), e0216241. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.
0216241 PMID: 31091292
87. Genetin B., Chen J., Kogan V., & Kalish A. (2022). Mitigating implicit bias in student evaluations: A ran-
domized intervention. Applied Economic Perspectives and Policy, 44(1), 110–128. https://doi.org/10.
1002/aepp.13217
PLOS ONE
Overcoming the threat of anti-bias interventions
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0314813 January 13, 2025 25 / 26
88. FitzGerald C., Martin A., Berner D., & Hurst S. (2019). Interventions designed to reduce implicit preju-
dices and implicit stereotypes in real world contexts: a systematic review. BMC Psychology, 7, 1–12.
https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-019-0299-7 PMID: 31097028
89. Coffeng T, Van Steenbergen EF, De Vries F, Ellemers N. Quality of group decisions by board members:
A hidden-profile experiment. Management Decision. 2021 Jan 1; 59(13):38–55. https://doi.org/10.1108/
MD-07-2020-0893
PLOS ONE
Overcoming the threat of anti-bias interventions
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0314813 January 13, 2025 26 / 26