Access to this full-text is provided by Springer Nature.
Content available from Scientific Reports
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
Frequency and categorization of
presleep fantasies
Anna Szente1, Husamalddin Ali Alhour1, Eszter Áfra2, Ákos Arató1,2, Barnabás Dudás1,
AnnaSzűcs3, Réka Horváth1, Norbert Kovács1, Gergely Darnai1,2,4 & József Janszky1,4
Thecontentofpresleepthoughtshavebeenassumedtoinuencesleepqualityforalongtime,
e.g., insomnia has repeatedly been discussed to be associated with anxious thoughts before falling
asleep. However, the phenomenon of recurring, voluntary fantasizing before sleep does not appear
tobeacommonresearchtopic.Intherststageofthisstudy,weexploredthefrequencyofpresleep
fantasizingonasampleof281volunteers.Inthesecondstage,weanalyzedthecontentofca.5000
fantasy descriptions found online to discover similar patterns. Our results showed that approximately
75%oftherespondentsfantasizesbeforesleepregularlyandcoulddescribethreemaintopicsduring
categorization (‘aims and ambitions’, thinking about a ‘calming’ scene, ‘fading away and death’).
Basedonourndings,presleepfantasizingisacommonphenomenon.Toourknowledge,thisisthe
rststudytoinvestigateandcategorizepresleepfantasies.
Keywords Sleep, Fantasy, Presleep fantasy, Presleep period
Cognitive activity during the presleep period has been suggested to inuence sleep quality and thus the
contribution to insomnia for a long time1, however, the content of presleep thoughts remains on the periphery
of this research eld2,3. e shi of attention from the external world to our inner thoughts is something that we
experience regularly, most of us tend to spend 50% of our time during the day with task-unrelated thoughts4.
Fantasizing and being occupied with our own personal thoughts are oen introduced as a failure of executive
control5, therefore many disadvantages can be highlighted like sleep diculties6, worse performance and
daytime functioning7. On the contrary, there is a more positive apprehension of the phenomenon. Because it is
so common, some studies have suggested that it has a valuable function: depending on the associated feelings,
fantasizing could contribute to attentional uctuations, dishabituation (when a break from an ongoing task allows
to enhance learning potential), thinking through future plans and enhanced creativity8. In this sense, while some
studies consider it to be an interruption of an ongoing task, there is growing evidence showing that fantasizing
facilitates adaption9 and can contribute to self-development. In summary, while fantasizing has been seen as
both a cost and a benet10, its function and value remain unclear. One possible explanation is that when a certain
task or goal is considered too limited, the mind starts to explore something more advantageous11. However, it
has also been suggested, that it would be important to distinguish between intentional and unintentional forms
of this phenomenon12.
During the period of falling asleep, the perception of external stimuli is steadily reduced, allowing for voluntary
presleep fantasizing. We can nd two types of task-unrelated thoughts, based on whether the attentional shi is
consciously or unconsciously controlled, presleep fantasizing can take the conscious form, attention deliberately
shis from the external to the internal world13. Why this phase could be meaningful in sleep research is shown
by the many aspects sleep is linked to emotions. Stress can alter sleep through sleep fragmentation and increased
sleep latency14. An other study showed, that a negative feedback provided on a test to participants, resulted
in deteriorated sleep quality15. ese results pointed to the extent of how emotions play a role in determining
sleep aspects in otherwise healthy individuals. e connection is bidirectional: decreased sleep eciency alters
emotion regulation as well16. Although studies agree that there is a link between presleep cognitive activity and
sleep quality, it is hard to compare the results of these studies due to the absence of presleep fantasies’ content
descriptions17.
e power of presleep thoughts, and their inuence on sleep quality is well known, mostly related to negative
consequences: ruminative and anxious thoughts may contribute to poorer sleep quality18, repetitive thoughts of
stressful events may lead to elevated arousal, delayed sleep onset and thus diminishing sleep19. On the other hand,
the nature of presleep thoughts could be inuenced to improve sleep quality, for example by listening to relaxing
1Department of Neurology, Medical School, University of Pécs, Pécs, Hungary. 2Department of Behavioral
Sciences, Medical School, University of Pécs, Pécs, Hungary. 3Department of Behavioral Sciences, Medical School,
Semmelweis University, Budapest, Hungary. 4HUN-REN PTE Clinical Neuroscience MR Research Group, Pécs,
Hungary. email: darnai.gergely@pte.hu
OPEN
ScienticReports | (2024) 14:31975 1
| https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-83642-7
www.nature.com/scientificreports
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
words before sleep: the processing of relaxing words could activate several other brain regions contributing to
calmness20. us, we hypothesized, that a dierent character of conscious, voluntary fantasizing would help
the transition to sleep. To analyze the content of presleep fantasies, we examined online platforms dedicated to
sharing such information. In an article posted on Buzzfeed on the 14th of November, 2017., titled “34 Pre-Sleep
Fantasies at Will Make You Say “I thought I Was e Only One”” ( h t t p s : / / w w w . b u z z f e e d . c o m / a n n a b o r g e s / b e
f o r e - b e d - f a n t a s i e s ) , the author, Anna Borges asked the community to post these fantasies. e content of these
fantasies was e.g., relationship, prize winning, or fantasies inspired by movies or celebs. Online platforms may
help discover a wider range of presleep thoughts: anonymity encourages users to share sensitive information
about themselves compared to the identiable users21. In this study, our aim was to discover the prevalence of
fantasizing before falling asleep and analyzing the content of these fantasies, for similar patterns to be uncovered.
In the rst stage, with an online questionnaire, we asked about the frequency of presleep fantasizing, in the
second stage, we collected descriptions of presleep fantasies online.
Methods
Study I
In study I, we assembled a questionnaire package as an assessment of sleep quality and the occurrence of presleep
fantasies. e questionnaire package contained two questionnaires (short version of Pittsburgh Sleep Quality
Index - PSQI & Glasgow Content of oughts Inventory - GCTI) and two extra questions about fantasizing and
creating stories in the presleep period.
e survey was assembled in Google Forms and shared on social media platforms, on Facebook. Participants
were instructed to read the items carefully and answer the rst thing that come to their minds. ey were informed
that all of their answers would remain anonymous. Additionally, we asked three questions on demographics
(age, gender, education). e answers were collected from the 1st of March to the 25th of March 2021.
Participants
Our questionnaire package was accessible through an online link and 281 participants (243 females and 38 males)
completed the questionnaire form. e mean age was 33.91 years (SD = 13.14). According to the education,
participants were classied as: 37% Master’s Degree, 19.5% Bachelor’s Degree, 32% High School Graduate and
11.5% less than High School.
During this study, all methods were carried out in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations. Based
on the ocial statement of the Regional Research Ethics Committee of the University of Pécs (9861 – PTE 2024)
an Ethical Approval was not necessary for this study. Using this questionnaire was considered safe, as all data
were collected anonymously. With completing the online questionnaire, applicants voluntarily took part in this
study, and an informed consent was obtained from all participants.
Pittsburgh sleep quality index (PSQI)
To measure subjective sleep quality over a one-month period, we used the Hungarian version of PSQI22. PSQI is
a widely accepted, self-rated questionnaire, which contains 19 self-reported items and 5 questions to be answered
by roommate or bed partner. e global score consists of seven components: perceived sleep quality, sleep
latency, sleep duration, sleep eciency, sleep disturbances, hypnotic medication use and daytime dysfunction.
According to our interest we removed a few components: hypnotic medication use, daytime dysfunction and
rating of bed partner/roommate, since these questions refer to the clinical aspects of sleep diculties. Although
daytime dysfunction and hypnotic medication use supposedly interfere with presleep cognitive activity, in this
study, our interest was mainly focused on the other characteristics of sleep. Each component score was calculated
from the item scores that range from zero to three (0 = no problem, 3 = severe problem).
Glasgow content of thoughts inventory (GCTI)
e 25-item self-report GCTI contains categories of presleep thoughts, which were collected from insomniac
subjects to analyze the nature and intrusiveness of their thoughts. e questionnaire items represent these
thoughts that insomniac people tend to revolve around prior to sleep. Every item should be answered on a
4-point scale (0 = never, 1 = sometimes, 3 = oen, 4 = always)23. In the lack of the validated Hungarian version,
the original GCTI was translated into Hungarian by our research team. Two extra questions were added as item
26 and item 27, reecting to presleep fantasizing and creating stories (“In the past 7 days before sleep I was
preoccupied with fantasizing”, “In the past 7 days before sleep I was preoccupied with creating stories”). Listing
both of them might help discover dierent aspects of the presleep cognitive activity. Although these questions
were listed at the end of the GCTI questionnaire, we would not include them as questionnaire scores, they were
used to indicate the frequency of fantasizing and creating stories. To get a broader picture of presleep thoughts,
two sets of the GCTI questionnaire were used indicating presleep thoughts of the past week and the past ten
years.
Statistical analysis
To perform statistical analysis, we used IBM SPSS Statistics 25 soware (IBM Corp. Released 2017. IBM SPSS
Statistics for Windows, Version 25.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp). Descriptive statistics were used to analyse
demographical data, the mean and standard deviations were calculated. For the correlational analysis, Spearman’s
correlation was used.
ScienticReports | (2024) 14:31975 2
| https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-83642-7
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Study II
e purpose of Study II was to analyze presleep fantasies shared in online forums and categorize them according
to their contents. To our best knowledge, to date, no other study has found intersubject similarities regarding the
content and motives in presleep thoughts.
e following search terms were used in Google search: “presleep fantasy”, “presleep thoughts”, “go-to fantasy”,
“falling asleep fantasy”, “presleep period”. For the purpose of the analysis, we selected two online forums. Most of
the reports were found on a Reddit forum, entitled “What is your go-to fantasy you think of before falling asleep”
(https://www. reddit.com/r /AskReddit/c omments/8z1 57b/what_is_your_goto_fantasy_you_think_of_before/).
About 9500 reports were within this range. Since many of them were not related to the original question, a
screening procedure was applied to remove unrelated reports. erefore, 5090 fantasies remained in our data
base. We also ignored those reports that were not considered a fantasy, such as planning the next day or a learnt
method to help the transition to sleep, thus 4912 reports remained.
Another forum, titled “Do you ever create fantasy stories in your head before you go to sleep?” ( h t t p s : / / f o r u
m s . d i g i t a l s p y . c o m / d i s c u s s i o n / 1 8 2 9 6 7 8 / d o - y o u - e v e r - c r e a t e - f a n t a s y - s t o r i e s - i n - y o u r - h e a d - b e f o r e - y o u - g o - t o - s l e e
p ) involved 22 comments.
Based on the reports of the abovementioned forums, the category system consisted of three main parts: (1)
content-based categories, that were established during several meetings with our research team. Based on this,
classication of each fantasy report was decided by two of the authors (ASZ and HAA), and inconsistencies, which
occurred in 4% of all cases, were decided by a third independent investigator (EÁ), (2) commonly occurring
words to detail the fantasy, that were selected by our team aer reading through the fantasy descriptions carefully,
and their occurrence was calculated using Excel spreadsheets and (3) analyzing the following characteristics:
reality of the fantasy; presence of sensory modalities, memories; features of movies, series, books, videogames;
temporal aspects: fantasy evolving over time or existing in the past only; playing a main or minor role and being
lonely or having company in the fantasy.
e nal collection of presleep fantasies contained 4934 reports using the two forums mentioned above.
We assumed that commonalities of presleep fantasy content would occur which could provide the base to
classication.
Data used in this study was collected from publicly available online platforms. For the protection of user
privacy, our results did not contain any identiable information about the users, and we have complied with the
relevant terms and conditions of these platforms. Due to the use of anonymized, publicly available data, and the
position of Reddit, that collection of data is not prohibited for academic purposes only (i.e. non-commercial), an
ethical approval was not necessary, supported by the statement of the Regional Research Ethics Committee of the
University of Pécs (9861 – PTE 2024). Previous studies analyzing online data have followed a similar approach.
Results
Study I
Pittsburgh sleep quality index and glasgow content of thoughts inventory
e mean score of the PSQI questionnaire was 4.199 ± 2.241. Values ranged between 1 and 12. A higher global
score indicated poorer sleep quality.
GCTI was listed twice in the questionnaire package presenting the past seven days and the past ten years,
respectively. A ten-year period was chosen subjectively, since we assumed this could indicate a relevant picture
of sleep habits in the past. e mean score for GCTI showing the past seven days was 25.99 ± 12.25. Values
ranged between 0 and 65. Based on the 42-point cut-o, by the GCTI, 24 people t into the poor sleepers group.
Regarding the past 10 years, results seemed similar with 31 individuals in the poor sleepers’ group, mean score
was 28.61 ± 11.77.
Fantasizing/creating stories before sleep
Percentages of the extra two questions, respondents reporting fantasizing/creating stories before sleep are
presented in Table1. e majority of the respondents experienced presleep fantasizing on a regular basis.
Although it is questionable whether our participants could provide a reliable answer of the past 10 years, we
could not exclude the possibility, that we would gain valuable information. According to the results of the past
10 years, they were very similar to the percentages and GCTI scores listed above, therefore we can assume that
frequency of fantasizing and creating stories did not change too much during the years.
Spearman correlation analysis
PSQI, GCTI questionnaire scores and fantasizing/creating stories were correlated. e correlation between
GCTI and fantasizing/creating stories was signicant and correlated positively (R = 0.321, p<0.001; R = 0.304,
Fantasizing Creating stories
Always 12.4% (12.5%) 9.6% (9.9%)
Oen 21.7% (23.4%) 12.8% (16.5%)
Sometimes 38.8% (39.1%) 23.5% (27.8%)
Never 27.1% (25.0%) 54.1% (45.8%)
Tab le 1. Prevalence of fantasizing/creating stories during the past 7 days and 10 years. Note. Percentages
inside parenthesis refers to the frequency of fantasizing in the last 10 years.
ScienticReports | (2024) 14:31975 3
| https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-83642-7
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
p<0.001), suggesting that a higher GCTI score coincides with a higher occurrence of presleep fantasizing and
creating stories. ere was no association between fantasizing/creating stories and PSQI scores.
Study II
Content based category system
Based on content, four main categories and several subcategories (25 in total) were established. Main categories
were”: ‘aims and ambitions’, thinking about a ‘calming’ scene, ‘fading away and death’ and if the fantasy did not
t into any of these sections (mostly due to the absence of further details), an ‘other’ category was created. First,
two authors (ASZ and HAA) inserted the fantasy reports in the category system. Aer merging, in the resulting
two tables, 4% of all the categorization results were varying. Upon discussion, the dierences aected poorly
detailed fantasy reports, and their nal category was decided by a third, independent author (EÁ). Distribution
of the main categories is shown in Table2.
Majority of the presleep fantasies belonged to the category ‘aims and ambitions’. People shared more or less
ctitious wishes and goals like having superpowers, being another character, being able to change the past,
monetary reward, love and relationships, sexual fantasy, being successful or meeting celebrities. Contrary to
the calming fantasies, respondents here oen detailed an exciting, developing storyline. is category mostly
involved stories based on positive emotions, but a smaller part of them were negative, anxious or unpleasant
fantasies. Some of the reports were not straightforward and not clear enough to understand the content, therefore
we included them to more than one category, due to a possible overlap. Table3. presents the list of subcategories,
the prevalence and examples of the reports of ‘aims and ambitions’ category.
e second most frequent category was imaging a calming, relaxing situation. A calming situation appearing
in the fantasy indicated an undisturbed, peaceful scene mainly associated with loneliness. It appeared in dierent
forms, depending on whether it took place in an open (e.g. forest) or closed (e.g. cave) space, or involved a
relaxation technique („I usually imagine with every exhale I’m sinking into the ground more and more and I feel
myself becoming a part of the earth.”). Table4. shows subcategories within the calming situations.
Based on commonalities, these categories could be divided into further sections: some respondents placed
the relaxing fantasy in a post-apocalyptic situation, representing complete isolation (e.g., “Zombie outbreak
starts and I gather the family and meet my loved ones at the bunker/armory.”). We created a subcategory, where
loneliness is especially noted: ‘last person on Earth’ with the extinction of all other humans on the planet (e.g.,
“I would the last person on earth and hop on any car id like and drive alone on the freeways for hours at a time
and have some fun alone.”). If the fantasy took place somewhere closed, it represented not only calmness but also
safety (e.g., „My go-to fantasy for some reason has always been just me sipping on hot chocolate late at night in
Category Frequency Example
Being someone else
Love, relationship 1108
546 “It’s okay. My fantasy is being Tony Stark.”
“So true it hurts. I imagine my ideal relationship”
Specic wish 495 “Playing football for my favorite team.”
Superpower 473 “Sometimes what I would do if I had superpowers”
Desires in general 408 “Being happy”
Sexual fantasy 259 “Porn. e acting is mind numbing which actually helps me fall asleep.”
Receiving an award or monetary reward 230 “Winning the lottery!”
Accomplishment, success 197 “Being an NBA player”
Aggression, violence 191 “I fantasize of me beating some annoying youngster to oblivion.”
Negative, unpleasant situation 145 “Getting rejected by everyone I like”
Meeting celebrities 78 “Usually, some kind of relationship involving my celebrity crush of the day.”
Being able to change the past 80 “at I will wake up the next morning ten years old again in 1993 with all of the knowledge I currently have.”
Powerful, inuential position 71 “Being a God in another world. I love being a god.”
Plans for the future 58 “What the house my future family and I have will look like; I imagine it being very cozy!”
Freezing the moment 35 “I like to imagine all people on earth get paused, except for me. en, I go explore.”
People reacting to the narrator’s funeral 21 “How would people react if I died.”
Meeting deceased people 14 “Seeing my dad one last time….”
Tab le 3. Subcategories of category aims and ambitions, prevalence and examples.
Category Number of presleep fantasies
Aims and ambitions 4409
Calming fantasies 1039
Fading away and death 174
Others 382
Tab le 2. Main categories of presleep thoughts based on content.
ScienticReports | (2024) 14:31975 4
| https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-83642-7
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
the O’Hare International Airport all alone. Usually, I think of soothing jazz music playing too.”), and it involved
a class where a danger source also occurred (Fig.1). Origin of the danger appeared in dierent forms: it could
be a raging thunderstorm, dinosaurs, the space or a stormy sea. Manifestation of safety varied too: it appeared to
be a cave, tent, space pod or submarine (e.g., “e veranda on a log cabin in the forest, by a lake. It’s raining. We
have an outdoor heater burning, wrapped in blankets.”).
A less frequent, but separable topic included death-related thoughts and fading away. ese fantasies were
mostly short, straightforward thoughts, or could be more complex fantasies (e.g.,” (…) I just get this creeping
cold down my spine and feel like death and think about how when we die there is nothing and everything is
worthless and then I snap back to reality, and either do it again or pass out”). Interestingly, some of these death-
related thoughts were not associated with negative feelings, but with peaceful ones (“I dream of a small asteroid
the size of a bus speeding towards earth. it loses most of its size in the atmosphere and when its hits earth, it
hits exactly me and only me. Everything I own and was eradicated in an instant and I felt nothing. e thought
calms me.”).
If none of the categories mentioned above were suitable for the presleep thoughts, we put them into an ’other’
category. Most of these reports were too shortly dened to be understood (“Dogs. Literally just dogs or ants.”).
List of commonly occurring words describing presleep fantasies
Besides the categories, we also calculated the occurrence of a few selected words to describe a fantasy content.
We selected words which are aligned with the content-based category system. erefore, we reviewed the context
of the inspected word, and removed it if the respondent did not use it in a certain meaning (e.g., ‘space’ can
describe a location, that can be indenite and the cosmic space too, which we were looking for). ‘Space’ was the
most frequently occurring word, mostly to explain a relaxing, isolated situation far from everyone else. ‘Hero’
was also used commonly to express ambitions such as playing the main role in a heroic action, being able to get
revenge or constructing an exciting storyline (“Being a Robin Hood type anti-hero in a superhero story where
I save the world by robbing banks. It’s kinda lame but it’s fun to imagine.”). e word ‘y’ appeared in dierent
Fig. 1. Schematic presentation of subcategory ‘outside danger’ belonging to the rst-order category, ‘calming
fantasies’. Note: is gure demonstrates examples of danger sources and safe place choices within the
subcategory ‘outside danger’.
Category Number of presleep fantasies
Open space 313
Relaxation 189
Isolation 175
Outside danger 162
Post apocalypse 110
Closed space 43
Survival 28
Last person on Earth 19
Tab le 4. Subcategories of calming presleep fantasies.
ScienticReports | (2024) 14:31975 5
| https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-83642-7
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
forms. It could be a part of a relaxing (e.g., “at my bed is ying through the night sky. As I dri closer and
closer into sleep, I can actually start feeling the sensation of weightlessness and propulsion underneath me, it’s
pretty awesome: )”) or a more thrilling fantasy (e.g. “For my entire life, I’ve always gone to sleep with the thought
of being able to y, swooping down and saving people from danger superman-style…”). Table5. presents the
most frequently occurring words.
Category system based on commonalities
An additional thematic analysis was conducted to isolate the most common traits of fantasy reports as well: (1)
whether the content could be real or ctional (2) sensory modalities involved (3) fantasy based on memories (4)
fantasy based on movies, series, books or videogames (5) fantasy evolving over time (6) fantasy takes place in the
company of others (7) whether the person describing the fantasy played the main role (Table6).
Discussion
Presleep fantasizing might be a conscious eort to facilitate the process of falling asleep and a part of a normal
routine. However, it has not been systematically investigated before, particularly, the content of those fantasies.
Category Number of presleep fantasies Description
Real 2458 Parts of the fantasy could occur in the real world.
Fictional 2309 Details of the fantasy could only happen in an imaginary world.
Sensory modalities ere are senses mentioned within the description of the fantasy.
1. Sound 58 Noises are highlighted in fantasy descriptions, usually linked to a peaceful
scene.
2. Touch 130 Respondents imagine something aecting their body.
3. Smell 12 Smells are appearing in the fantasy.
Based on memories 81 Important components of the fantasy are memories, oen from childhood.
Based on movies, series, books or videogames 816 Structure of the imaginary scene is taken from an existing story.
Fantasy evolves over time 315 Presleep fantasizing have been present for several years and respondents
add further details to their fantasy.
Used to fantasize 66 Respondents usually remember presleep fantasies that are not present
anymore.
Loneliness 639 Presleep fantasy revolves around only the respondent.
Company 1655 Other people are involved in the fantasy as well.
Main role 4710 Respondent is the central gure of the narrative.
Not part of the fantasy 119 Describer is not part of the fantasy storyline.
Tab le 6. Classication system of presleep fantasies based on commonalities.
Word Prevalence
Space 172
Hero 137
Fly 133
Lotto/lottery 129
Apocalyp’ 112
Kill 104
Ship 102
Zombie 101
Surviving/survival 96
Forest 74
Planet 74
Dark 68
Mountain 63
Alone 63
Beach 60
Rain 57
Tow n 54
Island 53
Tab le 5. Prevalence of the most commonly used words to describe presleep fantasies.
ScienticReports | (2024) 14:31975 6
| https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-83642-7
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
In these two studies our aim was to investigate presleep fantasizing. In the rst study, we assessed the
occurrence of fantasizing before sleep in a sample of 281 participants. We found that approximately 75% of
our sample fantasized or created stories before sleep regularly. Similar results were found in a study of more
than 500 individuals, examining the link between cultural beliefs and sleep, where approximately 75% reported
daydreaming before sleep, which was also found to be linked with longer sleep duration24. Among our results,
there was a positive association between frequency of presleep fantasizing and the GCTI questionnaire score,
which represents presleep thoughts insomniac subjects oen experience. We did not have information about the
contents of these fantasies, but previous studies showed that anxious and guilty characteristics of task-unrelated
thoughts might facilitate insomnia in opposition to positive-vivid topics25. e vast majority of participants were
women. Previous research has shown that ruminative coping strategies, as a response to stressful events, showed
gender dierences as early as in adolescence, with women generally more prone to developing depression26,27.
is may be a possible explanation for the association between the severity of insomnia and the frequency of
fantasizing: since our participants were mostly women, the nature of presleep thoughts may be ruminative.
In the second study, we analyzed almost 5000 presleep fantasy descriptions that were available online. Based
on the similar features of them, we isolated distinctive categories. e most common theme of these fantasies was
‘aims and ambitions’, which was about a broad range of underlying desires and reaching more or less reasonable
goals. is category contained fantasies that were generally more expressive in terms of storyline, mostly
pointing towards personal fulllment. ey may be an imaginative expansion of the current life situation, with
practical elements indicating problem solving and plans for the future. Previous research has shown that mental
imagery of a desired scene can be benecial if it contains real-life diculties to be solved, not only the achievable
award28. Romantic and sexual fantasies were reported commonly, which could benet the relationship29, and
were found to be acceptable, as a social norm, regardless of gender30. A lower subset of this category involved
aggressive thoughts, which could be associated with rumination and lower well-being31, revenge fantasies might
be even destructing32.
Based on our fantasy descriptions, the second most common category involved relaxing and peaceful scenes
where they can feel safe and unharmed. is sort of fantasy descriptions referred to self–soothing directly (e.g.,
“Since I was a kid, I have had a hard time falling asleep (…) I imagine I’m falling into my bed, like you’re on your
back in the pool and sink down to the bottom. But it is just darkness and I’m oating downward and I see the
bed and the hole I made above me growing distant. at is when I enter my dreams.”) or indirectly, suggesting
the contributing role to sleep quality.
In our sample, negative fantasies were illustrated less frequently, although smaller but not negligible fantasies
were classied under the sub-theme ‘fading away and death’. is category included fantasies, in which death
also illustrated peaceful content, and not necessarily just suicidal thoughts. is is supported by research ndings
that suicidal fantasies are associated with detailed suicidal dispositions rather than vague fantasies33.
Emotions associated with presleep thoughts might inuence the development of insomnia, although only
a few studies analyzed the quality of presleep cognitive activity. Most of the literature focuses on how anxiety,
rumination and worry at bedtime lead to cognitive hyperarousal and have a negative impact on sleep. us, these
ruminative thoughts are strongly associated with insomnia. Insomniac people tend to have worrisome thoughts
before sleep, they are oen concerned about their general mental state or even about the sleep process34. Presleep
thoughts of insomniac people revolve around topics like trivial thoughts, bodily sensations and concerns about
work, family and sleep35. Further discovery of these ndings, inuencing cognitive activity and transform
thoughts to positive emotions, could be an ecient way to prevent or relieve symptoms of insomnia2.
Internet use before bedtime in adolescents is associated with poorer sleep quality36. e causal relationship
between technology use and sleep problems has been shown in many studies, but the underlying mechanism
is still uncertain37. Internet use might interfere with presleep cognition, inuences the content of fantasies and
therefore disturbs the normal process of falling asleep, this could be a new way to approach sleep diculties.
ere are limitations of our studies that should be considered. Controversies of terminology makes it dicult
to compare ndings. In the literature we found several terms (such as mind wandering, daydreaming or fantasy)
to the occurrence of task-unrelated thoughts that are disengaged from the external stimuli. ey are oen
used as synonyms, and it is not clear if there is any dierence between them38. In the last decade, the word
“mind wandering” started to appear more frequently in scientic papers, slowly replacing “daydreaming” and
“fantasy”39. In this article we refer to the above-mentioned phenomena as a collective term, “fantasy”, as a more
conscious form of thoughts, as opposed to mind wandering which reects a more spontaneous dri of thoughts.
Future research should also focus on using a consistent terminology. In Study II, we collected presleep fantasy
descriptions and it was not part of the original question whether the fantasizing is common so it could be
possible that people’s fantasies alter depending on external factors such as a stressful day, which we unfortunately
could not assess. is would be avoidable with supplementary questionnaires. It is also complicated to separate
presleep fantasies, since they mostly overlap in topic, therefore it is dicult to choose the right category for
them. In this study, three individuals of our research team evaluated independently the content of the fantasies,
however, future studies would be able to strengthen the category system.
We hypothesized that positive thoughts may contribute to a better sleep quality, so a future direction could
be to instruct patients to actively fantasize about a particular content, which is strengthened by the result of no
association between the PSQI score and the frequency of presleep fantasizing. However, this assumption needs
to be conrmed by future studies, as other studies on fantasy analysis are scarce.
In conclusion, we found that cognitive activity before sleep in a form of fantasizing seems to be a common
phenomenon. As the results of Study II suggest, a categorization spreadsheet could be created based on the
similar fantasy patterns of dierent people, it would be practical to assess the relationship between dierent
categories and sleep quality. It may be important to distinguish between positivity-lled and negative fantasies
that may contribute to insomnia, as this aspect is rarely addressed in insomnia studies.
ScienticReports | (2024) 14:31975 7
| https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-83642-7
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Data availability
e data that support the ndings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable
request.
Received: 19 March 2024; Accepted: 16 December 2024
References
1. Lichstein, K. L. & Rosenthal, T. L. Insomniacs’ perceptions of cognitive versus somatic determinants of sleep disturbance. J.
Abnorm. Psychol. 89, 105–107 (1980).
2. L emyre, A., Belzile, F., Landry, M., Bastien, C. H. & Beaudoin, L. P. Pre-sleep cognitive activity in adults: A systematic review. Sleep.
Med. Rev. 50, 1–13 (2020).
3. Wuyts, J. et al. e inuence of pre-sleep cognitive arousal on sleep onset processes. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 83, 8–15 (2012).
4. McMillan, R., Kaufman, S. B. & Singer, J. L. Ode to positive constructive daydreaming. Front. Psychol. 0, 626 (2013).
5. Kane, M. J. & McVay, J. C. What mind wandering reveals about executive-control abilities and failures. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 21,
348–354 (2012).
6. Carciofo, R., Du, F., Song, N. & Zhang, K. Mind wandering, sleep quality, aect and chronotype: An exploratory study. PLoS One.
9, e91285 (2014).
7. Lindquist, S. I. & McLean, J. P. Daydreaming and its correlates in an educational environment. Learn. Individ Dier. 21, 158–167
(2011).
8. Schooler, J. W. et al. Meta-awareness, perceptual decoupling and the wandering mind. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 319–326 (2011).
9. Baars, B. J. Spontaneous repetitive thoughts can be adaptive: Postscript on mind wandering. Psychol. Bull. 136, 208–210 (2010).
10. Mooneyham, B. W. & Schooler, J. W. e costs and benets of mind-wandering: A review. Can. J. Exp. Psychol. 67, 11–18 (2013).
11. Shepherd, J. Why does the mind wander? Neurosci. Conscious. (2019).
12. S eli, P., Risko, E. F., Smilek, D. & Schacter, D. L. Mind-wandering with and without intention. Trends Cogn. Sci. 20, 605–617 (2016).
13. Giambra, L. M. A Laboratory method for investigating inuences on switching attention to task-unrelated imagery and thought.
Conscious. Cogn. 4, 1–21 (1995).
14. Kim, E. J. & Dimsdale, J. E. e eect of psychosocial stress on sleep: A review of polysomnographic evidence. Behav. Sleep. Med.
5, 256–278 (2007).
15. Vandekerckhove, M. et al. e role of presleep negative emotion in sleep physiology. Psychophysiology 48, 1738–1744 (2011).
16. Fairholme, C. P., & Manber, R. Sleep emotions, and emotion regulation. In sleep and aect 45–61 (Elsevier, h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 1 0 1
6 / B 9 7 8 - 0 - 1 2 - 4 1 7 1 8 8 - 6 . 0 0 0 0 3 - 7 . (2015).
17. Wicklow, A. & Espie, C. A. Intrusive thoughts and their relationship to actigraphic measurement of sleep: Towards a cognitive
model of insomnia. Behav. Res. er. 38, 679–693 (2000).
18. Guastella, A. J. & Moulds, M. L. e impact of rumination on sleep quality following a stressful life event. Pers. Individ Dif. 42,
1151–1162 (2007).
19. Morin, C. M., Rodrigue, S. & Ivers, H. Role of stress, arousal, and coping skills in primary insomnia. Psychosom. Med. 65, 259–267
(2003).
20. Beck, J., Loretz, E. & Rasch, B. Exposure to relaxing words during sleep promotes slow-wave sleep and subjective sleep quality.
Sleep 44, (2021).
21. Peddinti, S. T., Ross, K. W. & Cappos, J. ‘On the internet, nobody knows you’re a dog’: A twitter case study of anonymity in social
networks. In Proceedings of the second ACM conference on Online social networks 83–93 doi: (2014). h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 1 1 4 5 / 2 6 6 0 4
6 0 . 2 6 6 0 4 6 7
22. Takács, J., Bódizs, R., Ujma, P. P. & Horváth, K. Reliability and validity of the Hungarian version of the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality
Index (PSQI-HUN): Comparing psychiatric patients with control subjects. Sleep. Breath. 1045–1051. h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 1 0 0 7 / s 1 1 3
2 5 - 0 1 6 - 1 3 4 7 - 7 (2016).
23. Harvey, K. J. & Espie, C. A. Development and preliminary validation of the Glasgow Content of thoughts Inventory (GCTI): A new
measure for the assessment of pre-sleep cognitive activity. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 43, 409–420 (2004).
24. Arslan, S. Cultural beliefs aecting sleep duration. Sleep. Biol. Rhythms. 13, 287–296 (2015).
25. Starker, S. & Hasenfeld, R. Daydream styles and sleep disturbance. J. Nerv. Ment Dis. 163, 391–400 (1976).
26. Jose, P. E. & Brown, I. When does the gender dierence in rumination begin? Gender and age dierences in the use of rumination
by adolescents. J. Youth Adolesc. 37, 180–192 (2008).
27. Mezulis, A. H., Abramson, L. Y. & Hyde, J. S. Domain specicity of gender dierences in rumination. J. Cogn. Psychother. 16,
421–434 (2002).
28. Oettingen, G. & Psychology, K. R. S. and P. & undened. e power of prospection: Mental contrasting and behavior change. Wiley
Online Library 10, 591–604. (2016).
29. Birnbaum, G. E., Kanat-Maymon, Y., Mizrahi, M., Recanati, M. & Orr, R. What fantasies can do to your relationship: e eects of
sexual fantasies on couple interactions. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 45, 461–476 (2019).
30. Busch, T. M. Perceived acceptability of sexual and romantic fantasizing. Sex. Cult. 24, 848–862 (2020).
31. Poon, K. T. & Wong, W. Y. Stuck on the train of ruminative thoughts: e eect of aggressive fantasy on subjective well-being. J.
Interpers. Violence. 36, 6390–6410 (2021).
32. Lillie, M. & Strelan, P. Careful what you wish for: Fantasizing about revenge increases justice dissatisfaction in the chronically
powerless. Pers. Individ. Dif.. 94, 290–294 (2016).
33. Crabb, P. B. e material culture of suicidal fantasies. J. Psychol. 139, 211–220 (2005).
34. Fichten, C. S. et al. Role of thoughts during nocturnal awake times in the insomnia experience of older adults. Cogn. erapy Res.
25, 665–692 Preprint at (2001). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1012963121729
35. Watts, F. N., Coyle, K. & East, M. P. e contribution of worry to insomnia. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 33, 211–220 (1994).
36. van den Eijnden, R. J. J. M., Geurts, S. M., Bogt, T., van der Rijst, T. F. M., Koning, I. M. & V. G. & Social media use and adolescents’
sleep: A longitudinal study on the protective role of parental rules regarding internet use before sleep. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public.
Health. 18, 1–13 (2021).
37. Gradisar, M. et al. e sleep and technology use of Americans: Findings from the National Sleep Foundation’s 2011 Sleep in
America Poll. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 9, 1291–1299 (2013).
38. Christo, K. Undirected thought: Neural determinants and correlates. Brain Res. 1428, 51–59 (2012).
39. Callard, F., Smallwood, J., Golchert, J. & Margulies, D. S. e era of the wandering mind? Twenty-rst century research on self-
generated mental activity. Front. Psychol. 4, 891 (2013).
Acknowledgements
None.
ScienticReports | (2024) 14:31975 8
| https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-83642-7
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Author contributions
JJ, ASz, GD, RH and ASz were involved in developing the hypothesis and study design. ASz, GD and ÁA assem-
bled the questionnaire packages and summarized the results. ASz, EÁ and AHA evaluated the fantasy descrip-
tions and their categorization. ASz wrote the main manuscript body. JJ, NK, RH, ASz, BD and DG revised the
manuscript.
Declarations
Competing interests
e authors declare no competing interests.
Ethical approval
During this study, all methods were carried out in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations. Based
on the ocial statement of the Regional Research Ethics Committee of the University of Pécs (9861 – PTE
2024) an Ethical Approval was not necessary for this study. Using this questionnaire was considered safe, as all
data were collected anonymously. With completing the online questionnaire, applicants voluntarily took part in
this study, and an informed consent was obtained from all participants. Regarding Study II, data was collected
from publicly available online platforms. For the protection of user privacy, our results did not contain any
identiable information about the users, and we have complied with the relevant terms and conditions of these
platforms. Due to the use of anonymized, publicly available data, and the position of Reddit, that collection of
data is not prohibited for academic purposes only (i.e. non-commercial), an ethical approval was not necessary,
supported by the statement of the Regional Research Ethics Committee of the University of Pécs (9861 – PTE
2024). Previous studies analyzing online data have followed a similar approach.
Additional information
Supplementary Information e online version contains supplementary material available at h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1
0 . 1 0 3 8 / s 4 1 5 9 8 - 0 2 4 - 8 3 6 4 2 - 7 .
Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to G.D.
Reprints and permissions information is available at www.nature.com/reprints.
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and
institutional aliations.
Open Access is article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives
4.0 International License, which permits any non-commercial use, sharing, distribution and reproduction in
any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide
a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if you modied the licensed material. You do not have
permission under this licence to share adapted material derived from this article or parts of it. e images or
other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated
otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence
and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to
obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit h t t p : / / c r e a t i v e c o m m o
n s . o r g / l i c e n s e s / b y - n c - n d / 4 . 0 / .
© e Author(s) 2024
ScienticReports | (2024) 14:31975 9
| https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-83642-7
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
Terms and Conditions
Springer Nature journal content, brought to you courtesy of Springer Nature Customer Service Center GmbH (“Springer Nature”).
Springer Nature supports a reasonable amount of sharing of research papers by authors, subscribers and authorised users (“Users”), for small-
scale personal, non-commercial use provided that all copyright, trade and service marks and other proprietary notices are maintained. By
accessing, sharing, receiving or otherwise using the Springer Nature journal content you agree to these terms of use (“Terms”). For these
purposes, Springer Nature considers academic use (by researchers and students) to be non-commercial.
These Terms are supplementary and will apply in addition to any applicable website terms and conditions, a relevant site licence or a personal
subscription. These Terms will prevail over any conflict or ambiguity with regards to the relevant terms, a site licence or a personal subscription
(to the extent of the conflict or ambiguity only). For Creative Commons-licensed articles, the terms of the Creative Commons license used will
apply.
We collect and use personal data to provide access to the Springer Nature journal content. We may also use these personal data internally within
ResearchGate and Springer Nature and as agreed share it, in an anonymised way, for purposes of tracking, analysis and reporting. We will not
otherwise disclose your personal data outside the ResearchGate or the Springer Nature group of companies unless we have your permission as
detailed in the Privacy Policy.
While Users may use the Springer Nature journal content for small scale, personal non-commercial use, it is important to note that Users may
not:
use such content for the purpose of providing other users with access on a regular or large scale basis or as a means to circumvent access
control;
use such content where to do so would be considered a criminal or statutory offence in any jurisdiction, or gives rise to civil liability, or is
otherwise unlawful;
falsely or misleadingly imply or suggest endorsement, approval , sponsorship, or association unless explicitly agreed to by Springer Nature in
writing;
use bots or other automated methods to access the content or redirect messages
override any security feature or exclusionary protocol; or
share the content in order to create substitute for Springer Nature products or services or a systematic database of Springer Nature journal
content.
In line with the restriction against commercial use, Springer Nature does not permit the creation of a product or service that creates revenue,
royalties, rent or income from our content or its inclusion as part of a paid for service or for other commercial gain. Springer Nature journal
content cannot be used for inter-library loans and librarians may not upload Springer Nature journal content on a large scale into their, or any
other, institutional repository.
These terms of use are reviewed regularly and may be amended at any time. Springer Nature is not obligated to publish any information or
content on this website and may remove it or features or functionality at our sole discretion, at any time with or without notice. Springer Nature
may revoke this licence to you at any time and remove access to any copies of the Springer Nature journal content which have been saved.
To the fullest extent permitted by law, Springer Nature makes no warranties, representations or guarantees to Users, either express or implied
with respect to the Springer nature journal content and all parties disclaim and waive any implied warranties or warranties imposed by law,
including merchantability or fitness for any particular purpose.
Please note that these rights do not automatically extend to content, data or other material published by Springer Nature that may be licensed
from third parties.
If you would like to use or distribute our Springer Nature journal content to a wider audience or on a regular basis or in any other manner not
expressly permitted by these Terms, please contact Springer Nature at
onlineservice@springernature.com
Available via license: CC BY-NC-ND 4.0
Content may be subject to copyright.