ArticlePDF Available

Analysis of the Relationships Between Heart Rate Variability (HRV) and Intuitive Thinking Skills

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Intuition is generally taken as a belief, experience, and tool for knowledge emergence, often characterized by emotional judgments, sensations or foresight and be classified into various types. Recently researchers have started to search for somatic markers for intuition using EEG and ECG. The objective of this study is to explore the correlations between measurements that serve as indicators of heart rate variability and the strength and kind of intuition based on samples self-reports. The samples are 149 students aged 19-21 at Kocaeli University. Data was collected using KYTO2935 HRV sensors, Elite HRV Bluetooth application and the Intuitive Thinking Scale. Research findings indicate correlations between intuitive thinking skills and certain sub-dimensions and specific heart rhythm indices. These correlations vary in terms of their effect sizes, but it is satisfactory to assert that certain features of ours, which are acknowledged as intuitive thinking abilities, are connected to heart rhythm indices and require more thorough investigations.
Content may be subject to copyright.
128
KOSBED, 2024, 48: 128-145
Yayın Geliş Tarihi: 2024-09-11
Yayın Onay Tarihi: 2024-11-23
DOI No: 10.35343/kosbed.1548365
İsmet ŞAHİN 1
Analysis of the Relationships
Between Heart Rate
Variability (HRV) and
Intuitive Thinking Skills
Kalp Hızı Değişkenliği (HRV) ile Sezgisel
Düşünme Becerileri Arasındaki İlişkilerin
Analizi
Abstract
Intuition is generally taken as a belief, experience, and tool for knowledge emergence, often characterized by
emotional judgments, sensations or foresight and be classified into various types. Recently researchers have
started to search for somatic markers for intuition using EEG and ECG. The objective of this study is to explore
the correlations between measurements that serve as indicators of heart rate variability and the strength and kind
of intuition based on samples self-reports. The samples are 149 students aged 19-21 at Kocaeli University. Data
was collected using KYTO2935 HRV sensors, Elite HRV Bluetooth application and the Intuitive Thinking Scale.
Research findings indicate correlations between intuitive thinking skills and certain sub-dimensions and specific
heart rhythm indices. These correlations vary in terms of their effect sizes, but it is satisfactory to assert that certain
features of ours, which are acknowledged as intuitive thinking abilities, are connected to heart rhythm indices and
require more thorough investigations.
Keywords: Intuition, Intuitive Thinking, Types of Intuition, Heart Rate Variability, Heart Brain,
Jel Codes: 2530, 3040, 3550 (APA)
Özet
Sezgi, genellikle duygusal yargılar, duyumlar veya öngörü ile karakterize edilen ve çeşitli tiplerde sınıflandırılan
bir inanç, deneyim ve bilgiye ulaşma araolarak tanımlanmaktadır. Son yıllarda araştırmacılar sezgi için somatik
bazı belirteçler aramaya başladılar. Bu amaçla EEG ve EKG kullanarak, sezginin kalp ve beyin dalgalarıyla
ilişkileri araştırılmaktadır. Bu çalışmanın amacı, kalp hızı değişkenliğinin göstergesi olarak hizmet eden ölçümler
ile sezginin gücü ve türü arasındaki korelasyonları, örneklemin kendi beyanlarına dayanarak araştırmaktır.
Örneklem, Kocaeli Üniversitesi'nde 19-21 yaşları arasındaki 149 öğrencidir. Veriler KYTO2935 HRV sensörleri,
Elite HRV Bluetooth uygulaması ve Sezgisel Düşünme Ölçeği kullanılarak toplanmıştır. Araştırma bulguları,
sezgisel düşünme becerileri ile belirli alt boyutlarıyla ve bazı kalp ritmi indeksleri arasında korelasyonlar
olduğunu göstermektedir. Bu korelasyonlar etki büyüklükleri açısından değişmektedir, ancak sezgisel düşünme
yetenekleri olarak kabul edilen belirli özelliklerimizin kalp ritmi indeksleriyle bağlantılı olduğunu ve daha
kapsamlı araştırmalar gerektirdiğini ifade etmek için yeterlidir.
Anahtar Kelimeler: Sezgi, Sezgisel Düşünme, Sezgi Tipleri, Kalp Ritim Uyumu, Kalbin Beyni.
Jel Kodları: 2530, 3040, 3550 (APA)
1İsmet Şahin, Kocaeli Üniversitesi, Eğitim Fakültesi, Eğitim Bilimleri Bölümü, ismetsahin@gmail.com, ORCID: 0000-0002-
2268-9289.
129
INTRODUCTION
Intuition is the capacity to comprehend, anticipate, and directly sense reality without depending
on prior knowledge or logical reasoning (TDK, 2024). In the field of philosophy, it is defined as a belief,
a tendency to believe, an experience similar to perception, a tool that accelerates the emergence of
knowledge, comprehending the veracity or fallacy of something without the use of reasoning and
inference,, a way of acquiring infallible knowledge as a function of the pure mind, an internal reality
that directly grasps the data of consciousness. Knowledge is defined as the capacity to comprehend
and grasp information (Akarsu, 1988; Bealer, 1998; Öktem, 2000; Haklı, 2007; Koksvik, 2011; Nado,
2014; Bibika, 2024; Gündoğan, 2024; Soyaslan, 2024).
In the field of psychology, intuition is characterized as emotionally-driven judgments that
emerge from quick, unconscious, and comprehensive associations. It is a form of perception that
operates unconsciously, generating judgments swiftly and without conscious knowledge. Intuition is
an implicit, holistic, automatic, emotional, and unconscious process. It can also be characterized as a
sensation, a cognitive capacity, prescience, or foresight that discovers solutions or directs without the
need of reasoning or logic (Jung, 1971; Otte, 1990; Bailin, 1991; Hammond, 1996; Hogarth, 2001; Gore
ve Saddler-Smith, 2011; Pretz et al., 2014; Cai Shi & Lucietto, 2022).
Intuitions, as a summary of philosophical and psychological definitions can be described as a
function of the pure mind that occurs through a rapid, sudden, unconscious, emotional, holistic,
implicit and automatic process, as well as instinctive judgments, predictions, comprehension or
understanding in finding answers or direction.
There are studies claiming that intuition is not a singular skill and trying to define different types
based on its source and function. According to Pretz et al. (2014), there are three types of intuition:
problem solving, moral and creative. Glöckner and Witteman (2010) categorized intuition into four
distinct types: relational intuition, matching intuition, cumulative intuition, and constructive intuition.
According to Cai Shi and Lucietto (2021), intuition can be divided into 3: inferential, emotional and
holistic.
Agyakwa (1988) offers a classification of four different examples of intuitive knowledge:
extrasensory perception, self-evident facts, direct grasp of specific situations, and expert insight.
According to Gore and Saddler-Smith (2011), three general domain mechanisms of intuition have been
proposed: the application of heuristics under uncertainty, the acquisition and activation of complex
domain-related schemas, and the involvement of affect in decision making.
According to McCraty (2015), intuition can be categorized into three distinct forms. Implicit
knowledge, energy sensitivity, and nonlocal intuition. Implicit knowledge pertains to knowledge that
has been previously acquired but subsequently forgotten or not consciously acknowledged. The brain
employs neural mechanisms to link patterns of novel problems with implicit memories from prior
experiences. Energy sensitivity refers to the ability of the nervous system to detect and react to external
stimuli, such as electromagnetic radiation. Nonlocal intuition refers to an insight or perception that
cannot be attributed to prior information or external stimuli. Instances of nonlocal intuition encompass
scenarios in which parents possess the ability to see events occurring to their children at a remote area
or when entrepreneurs exhibit astute decision-making skills in their business endeavors.
130
According to these classifications, intuition can be understood as follows; an explicit form of
comprehension in a familiar situation, based on past experience, knowledge, and reasoning, an implicit
form of understanding in unfamiliar situations, relying on mental frameworks created by past
experiences and knowledge and last a form of immediate comprehension, particularly in high-stakes
and critical situations, characterized by affect, feeling, and somatic signs. The initial two can be referred
to as sign or cue interpretation. Sensing is the act of perceiving through the recognition of distinct
signals, recognizable cues, or by tapping into subconscious patterns formed from previous experiences.
The last can be described as an extrasensory sensation, an instinctive intuition that is separate from
conscious thought, knowledge, and logical deductions.
Figure 1: Classification of Definitions of the Concept of Intuition in the Literature
Chudnoff (2019) attempted to demonstrate the resemblances and distinctions between intuition,
sensory perception, and explicit reasoning in terms of their content and process, drawing on
Kahneman's (2011) work. He states that intuition shares a similar process with sensory perception, but
has a wider reach than both sensory perception and reasoning. In contrast to Kahneman, he asserted
that sensory perception and intuition have comparable subjective experiences, however originate from
distinct cognitive mechanisms, and no specific information can be provided regarding their specific
contents.
In most efforts to explain the concept of intuition, a search for grounding it with reason and
rational thinking tools is naturally observed. Perhaps for this reason, in many definitions and
explanations, intuition is treated as a more or less implicit reading of signs as a result of the senses and
experience. As mentioned by McCraty (2015), it may be a frequency, a biological energy or electricity
sensing mechanism that are generally associated with telepathy and foresight whose center may be the
brain, heart or gut, as a potential power, gift or ability of our nature of creation or of the evolution
process that we have not yet been able to name, that we have not experienced concretely until today,
that we have not yet realized its existence. If considered from this perspective, it is thought that it is
necessary to first study whether it is, rather than what it is.
From another perspective, are our decisions based on reason and logic based on pure knowledge
and experience, or is there an intuitive intervention when making the final choice or evaluation after
processing all experience and information? A similar comment can be made regarding our senses.
Stimuli coming from our senses are not perceived in a mechanical or automatic process. Couldn't there
be an intuitive choice in the process of giving meaning to the stimuli by our brain? One of the examples
we frequently encounter on social media is about the color of a woman's dress. White gold or blue
131
black? We look at the same photo but see it differently. Another is a voice recording. While some hear
the same voice recording as Yanny, others hear it as Laural. Plato said that the real world we perceive
through our senses can only be a reflective image of a higher level of existence. Only with our minds
can we understand what the misleading image we perceive with our senses really is. According to
Plato, the mind is not just a passive recipient of sensory information but an active contributor in
shaping the perception of reality. Using logical thinking, self-reflection and philosophical inquiry, we
can transcend the misleading illusions of the material world and understand the true essence of reality.
Can we express the proposition that our senses can mislead us, we can only know the truth with our
minds, as in Plato's cave analogy, as we notice it with our intuition and make sense of it with our
minds?
The main point here is the uniqueness of our senses. Our perceptions may be formed by
comparing the stimuli coming from our senses with experience and past information in our brain, but
at some point in this process may there be an intuitive choice? In this case, it would not be wrong to
think that our intuitive potential may even have an impact on the formation of our rational decisions
and concrete senses. Therefore, the paradigms we have developed regarding logic, sensory perception
and intuition will need to be re-evaluated. Perhaps the fundamental code behind all human behavior
is an ability, capacity or power we call intuition. This may even be the determinant of what we call
intelligence. In this case, there may be sub-dimensions of intuitive ability in the capacities we call
rational, emotional or social intelligence and similar.
1.1. Heart Brain - ‘Little brain’
The heart has universally been recognized as the locus of emotions, passion, and wisdom in
nearly all civilizations, devoid of any scientific connotation (Salem, 2009). Love has evolved into a
sentiment experienced within the heart and conveyed via it. People feel their love and pain in their
hearts. People who act cruelly and callously are described as 'heartless'. Many expressions such as
loving from the heart, with my most heartfelt feelings, in the depths of my heart, heartache, breaking
the heart, touching the heart, twisting the heart express this. After extensive research, Armour and
Ardell (1994) introduced the functional concept of 'heart brain'. Armour and Ardell’s studies have
revealed that the heart has an internal nervous system consisting of 40 thousand neurites that is so
complex that it can be described as a 'little brain' in itself. Armour and Ardell’s studies have created
the suspicion that these sayings, which are used without foundation in society, may have a basis. The
heart transmits significant signals to the brain that serve to inform, as well as to command, regulate,
and guide (Lacey and Lacey, 1978). Furthermore, neurophysiologists have noted that communications
from the heart to the brain, which traverse various networks and channels, can either amplify or
diminish the electrical activity within the brain (McCraty, 2002). To summarize, the cardiac brain
influences all of our cognitive and affective functions, and in certain instances, it is said to be more
efficient than the cerebral cortex.
Scientific study has demonstrated that affective changes occur simultaneously with predictable
alterations in heart rhythm, blood pressure, respiration, and digestive systems. Put simply, the
networks located on the left side of our peripheral nervous system, specifically the Sympathetic
division of our Autonomic Nervous System, are responsible for activating us during stressful
conditions and priming us for conflict. During tranquil circumstances, the parasympathetic division of
132
our peripheral nervous system induces relaxation and a state of calmness. In summary, our peripheral
nervous system, in coordination with the brain, determines how we respond to different stimuli. (Rein
et al, 1995).The heart engages in communication with the brain through four distinct mechanisms. One
of these is the electromagnetic field. Research has revealed that the heart transmits information to the
brain and the whole body through electromagnetic field interactions (McCraty, Bradley & Tomasino,
2004). It has also been determined that there is a cell type known as 'intrinsic cardiac adrenergic' (ICA)
cells in the heart. These cells secrete noradrenaline and dopamine neurotransmitters, which were once
thought to be produced only by neurons in the brain. The brain detects these hormones secreted by the
heart and the changes they cause in the body (Cantin & Genest, 1986). Another form of communication
is heart rhythm synchronization. It shows that when heart rhythm patterns are compatible, neural
information sent to the brain facilitates cortical function. This effect is often experienced as mental
clarity, improved decision-making, and increased creativity. In addition, harmonious input from the
heart tends to facilitate the experience of positive emotional states. Thus, the heart appears to be
intimately involved in psychophysiological coherence formation (Tille et al., 1996, & McCraty, 2000).
The last and most obvious connection between the heart and the brain is neural connections. In
addition to the peripheral nervous system, it is stated that the vagus nerves directly connect the heart
and the amygdala and affect the formation of our emotions (McCraty, 2002).
1.2. Intuition and Heart
Some researchers state that there is a deep connection, relationship or harmony between our
instincts, emotions and our heart (Otte, 1990; Holzer, 2022; Damasio, 1994; Dunn et al., 2010). The heart,
on the other hand, is often associated with emotions and empathy. It is considered the center of love,
compassion and intuition. When we say "follow your heart" we are usually referring to the guidance
gained from our emotional and intuitive senses. It is believed that when we connect deeply with our
hearts and are in tune with our emotions, our intuition becomes clearer and more accessible. This
alignment allows us to make decisions, make connections, and navigate life in a more authentic and
meaningful way (McCraty, 2015; Salem, 2009).
McCraty et al. (2004a) study, 30 calm and 15 emotionally arousing images were shown to 26
individuals in two experimental conditions: a baseline condition representing normal
psychophysiological functioning and a condition promoting physiological consistency. The main
measurements used are (electroencephalogram) EEG and (heartbeat-evoked potentials,
electrocardiogram) ECG to obtain heart decelerations/accelerations. The researchers summarized the
findings as follows: Contrary to expectations, the heart responds to intuitive information, a significant
decrease in heart rate is observed before encountering emotionally charged stimuli, and gender
differences are observed in the processing of information before the stimuli are presented.
McCraty et al. (2004b) conducted another study where they measured brain response (EEG) and
heart-rhythm activity (ECG) while participants were shown randomly selected photographs that were
either emotionally arousing or calming. The study was based on Radin's protocol, which aimed to elicit
an emotional response. The researchers discovered that both the brain and heart received information
about the upcoming emotional picture approximately 4 to 5 seconds before it was randomly selected
by the computer. Interestingly, the heart received this information about 1.5 seconds before the brain
did.
133
Essentially, the relationship between intuition and the heart is a profound connection that spans
our emotional, intuitive, and spiritual abilities. By maintaining a connection with our emotions and
being attuned to our instinctive understanding, we may tap into profound depths of wisdom and make
decisions that are in harmony with our own identity.
1.3. Heart Rate Variability (HRV)
Heart rate variability (HRV) refers to the beat-to-beat variation in heart rate or duration of the
R-R interval (heart period) (Billman, 2011 and Cygankiewicz & Zareba, 2013).
It is a measure of the level of coordination and harmony in the heart's activity. Similarly, heart
rate coherence or cardiac coherence refers to a state in which the heart rhythm becomes more
synchronized and consistent and may reflect changes in cardiac autonomic regulation (McCraty et al.,
2009). Physiological coherence refers to the regularity in the oscillatory outputs of regulatory systems
at any given time period. It represents the measure of stability and harmony. Physiological coherence
is associated with increased heart rate variability (HRV) and is linked to self-regulatory capacity,
emotion regulation, social interactions, and cognitive performance. The autonomic nervous system
(ANS) plays a role in regulating cardiac coherence, as stated by Tiller et al. (1996) and McCraty and
Zayas (2014). Reduced heart rate variability (HRV) is linked to a poorer state (physically, emotionally,
psychologically) in various medical disorders, but maintaining R-R compliance within the normal
range generally indicates excellent health and well-being..
The objective of this study is to explore the correlations between measurements that serve as
indicators of heart rate variability and other indices of heart rate and the strength and kind of intuition
based on individuals' self-reported experiences. Hence, the investigation will ascertain the potential
correlation between our intuition, which is purportedly influential in our decision-making and
learning endeavors, and the functioning of the heart and the resulting sinus rhythm. If these links are
discovered, a biological indicator for human intuition may be acquired, offering a fresh outlook on the
processes of learning and decision-making.
2. METHODS AND MATERIALS
The research was carried out with the endorsement of the ethics committee of Kocaeli
University's Social and Human Sciences department, as well as the consent of the dean of the faculty
where the research took place (Decision no: 20, made during the meeting on 07.06.2024, and numbered
2024/07). For writing the report of the research, the Quillbot artificial intelligence application was
utilized to a limited extent, specifically for tasks such as literature review, paraphrasing, and
translation.
2. 1. Research Design
The study is a descriptive research conducted using a relational approach. It involved analyzing
the relationship between the HRV (Heart Rate Variability) indicators, measured during rest, and the
scores obtained from various dimensions of the intuitive thinking scale developed by Berkant et al
(2022).
134
2. 2. Population and samples of the research
The research focuses on the population of students enrolled in the Faculty of Education at
Kocaeli University. The research sample consists of 149 male and female students, primarily in the 1st
and 2nd grades, aged 19-21, from various departments. These students were told about the research
and chose to participate voluntarily. The abbreviated version of the general health scale, as created by
Demiral et al. (2006), was used on the samples with proper authorization. Any samples that exhibited
health levels below the reference values and were diagnosed with psychological or other problems
were excluded. Analyses were performed on the remaining 105 individuals. In addition, certain data
points that deviated significantly from the norm were excluded from the analyses. Additionally, some
outliers were removed from the analyses. 56 of these students were fasting and 45 were not. The
number of students who were not fasting or menstruate is 33.
2. 3. Heart Rate Indices
ECG recordings are commonly used to observe heart rate. Every stage of this sinus rhythm
corresponds to a distinct function and phase.
135
Figure 2: Sinus Rhythm
Source: Wikipedia
Table 1: Heart Rate Indices
Time Domain Indices
RMSSD: Root mean square of successive RR interval differences
SDNN: Standard deviation of NN intervals
lnRMSSD: Natural Logaritm of Root mean square of successive RR interval
differences
PNN50: Percentage of successive RR intervals that differ by more than 50ms
MRRINT: Mean of interbeat intervals between all successive heartbeats.
Frequency Domain Indices
ToPow: Total Power is the signal energy found within a frequency band
LF/HF: Ratio of LF-to-HF power
LFPow: Relative power of the low-frequency band (0.040.15Hz) in normal units
HFPow: Relative power of the high-frequency band (0.150.4Hz) in normal units
LFPeak: Peak frequency of the low-frequency band (0.040.15Hz)
HFpeak: Peak frequency of the high-frequency band (0.150.4Hz)
Heart Rate Indices
HRMean: Heart rate mean
HRVmean: Heart rate variability mean
Schafer and Ginsberg (2017)
136
2. 4. Data Collection Tools
2. 4. 1. Kyto2935 HRV Sensors
The sensor used to measure heart rate variability is Kyto2935, the operation frequency is 2402
2480 MHz, the modulation type is GFSK, Bluetooth version 4.0, the bitrate of the transmitter is 1 Mbps,
and it has 40 channels. Shenzhen Asia Test Technology Co., Ltd. tested the device for validity and
reliability. It is found to accomplish FCC standards, part 15.247 (FCC ID: 2ALC3KYT02935). The report
number is ATT-2017SZ0217856F (the link for the report is in reference section). The summary of the
report mentions that the device described above has been tested by ATT, and the test results show that
the equipment under test (EUT) is in compliance with the FCC requirements. The test procedure is
ANSI C63.10-2013. It passed all standard sections like ‘Conducted Emission’, ‘6dB Bandwidth’, ‘Peak
Output Power’, ‘Radiated Spurious Emission’, ‘Power Spectral Density’, ‘Band Edge Emission', and
‘Antenna Requirement’. And last, the level of confidence was found to be 95%. In the literature review,
many research papers on HRV were found using the Kyto2935 device, like Cheng et al. (2019) and
Laurman (2023).
2. 4. 2. Elite HRV Bluetooth APP
The Elite HRV Bluetooth App computes several HRV indicators by directly extracting the R-R
intervals, which are the time intervals between consecutive heartbeats, from compatible devices. There
are scholarly investigations that examine the accuracy and consistency of the application. Furthermore,
it has been observed that it has been employed in numerous research investigations.
Chhetri, et al. (2022) and Ramon et al. (2022) evaluated the Elite HRV Bluetooth app's accuracy
in measuring time-domain heart rate variability (HRV) indices during rest, comparing it to the Polar
V-800 heart rate monitor. The study found that the Elite HRV application provided reliable data that
aligned with the data from the Polar V800 heart rate monitor. Perrotta et al. (2017) investigated the
correlation and agreement between rMSSDln obtained from Elite HRV and Kubios HRV 2.2 using
Pearson product-moment correlation and a Bland-Altman Plot. They found a highly significant and
strong correlation between the two. Himariotis et al. (2022) found no significant differences in
lnRMSSD data between the software in the seated position and inconsequential differences in the
supine position when artifact correction was not used. However, when using Very Low, Low, or
Automatic artifact-correction filters, the data did not show any discernible changes in either the seated
or supine positions. Ramon et al. (2022) conducted a comparative analysis of heart rate variability data
obtained from an ECG, Elite HRV, and Welltory. They found no differences in supine or seated
positions, strong to almost perfect correlation levels, and no discernible distinctions between measures
of short duration (5 minutes) and observations of ultra-short duration (1 minute). Both smartphone
applications can be used to monitor HRV in elite endurance athletes using short and ultra-short
readings. Guzik et al. (2017) found that the "ELITE HRV" app yields divergent outcomes compared to
traditional HRV methods, suggesting caution in its usage.
2. 4. 3. Intuitive Thinking Scale
The scale was established by Berkant et al (2022) and has four distinct dimensions:
Determination, inner certainty, emotional literacy, and implicit knowledge. The factor loadings for the
first factor range from 0.67 to 0.57, for the second factor they range from 0.83 to 0.64, for the third factor
137
they range from 0.54 to 0.68, and for the fourth factor they range from 0.71 to 0.58. The Cronbach's
alpha reliability coefficient for the complete scale was determined to be 0.80. The authors were
contacted to gain the required permissions.
2. 5. Data Collection
Prior to the data collecting procedure, all people were administered the abbreviated version of
the General Health Inventory, as established by Demiral et al. (2006). Due to the inability to promptly
assess their health condition, all participants underwent heart rhythm measures and thereafter
completed Berkant et al.'s (2022) Intuitive Thinking Scale. Data for those who were below reference
health values and had a diagnosed condition were excluded from the analyses.
HR recordings were collected by measuring the right ear using KYTO2935 finger and ear sensors
for 5 minutes in a specially prepared unoccupied room. Participants were instructed to assume a
relaxed posture, breathe effortlessly, and maintain their typical body position and breathing pattern.
The measurements were primarily taken during the daytime, specifically between the hours of 11 and
17. Throughout the procedure, the measurements of each person were documented following a brief
period of acclimation lasting 10-15 seconds. Throughout the procedure, no practices that could divert
or draw the attention of persons were permitted or deliberated.
The values obtained as a result of heart rate measurements were instantly recorded on the forms
and then the sample was asked to fill out the Intuitive Thinking Scale. The entire data collection process
took approximately 30 minutes for each individual. Then, heart rhythm values and intuitive thinking
ability data were transferred to the computer and cross-verified by two individuals to ensure accurate
data entry. Analyzes with these data were carried out using the SPSS statistical program, version 27.
3. RESULTS
3. 1. HRV and Intuitive Thinking Skill Relationships with All Student (Healthy) Data
Table 2: Statistics for healthy individuals, fasting and non-fasting, and the entire sample with and without
menstruation.
Intuitive Thinking
Total Points
Frequency Domain:
Low Frequency Total Power (LF Power)
r=
.221*
Sig.
.023
N
105
LF (Low frequency) refers to the magnitude of low frequency waves generated by the
sympathetic nervous system. Low-frequency power is linked to the functioning of the baroreflex. The
baroreflex can be conceptualized as a mechanism that regulates heart rate by decreasing it in response
to high blood pressure, and increasing it by reducing baroreflex activity when blood pressure is low.
Reduced blood pressure leads to a decrease in baroreflex activation, resulting in an increase in heart
rate and the restoration of blood pressure levels (Rahman et al., 2011).
138
The results indicate a significant correlation between the overall scores of intuition and the
scores of determination subscale, and the low frequency power (LF power) in the low and medium
level band, based on the Cohen effect size standards. Put simply, as the total score for Intuitive
Thinking and the scores for the determination subscale increase, there is also an increase in low
frequency power. To summarize, a rise in intuitive thinking ability is accompanied by an increase in
baroreflex function and a decrease in heart rhythm. The following list comprises the items that pertain
to the determination sub-dimension of Berkant et al.'s (2022) intuitive thinking scale.
1. I act with my logic in the face of events.
2. I take different opinions into account when making a decision on an issue.
3. I believe that decisions should be made based on evidence.
4. I analyze the events happening around me by considering the evidence.
When these items are examined, it can be seen that they describe rational, logical and scientific
thinking. Since these items are reverse coded in the scale, it can be said that as the individual's logical,
rational and scientific thinking skills decrease, baroreflex function (LF power) increases and heart
rhythm decreases. In this case, the assumed intuitive thinking skill (at least for the determination sub-
dimension ) seems to be inversely related to the rational and logical thinking skill.
3. 2. The Relationship between HRV and Intuitive Thinking Skill with data from Non-Fasting
(Healthy) Samples
Table 3: Statistics with the data from non-fasting healthy samples
Intuitive Thinking:
Determination
Intuitive Thinking:
Implicit Knowledge
lnRMSSD:
r
.335*
.319*
Sig.
.026
.033
N
44
45
Mean R-R interval
r
.347*
.338*
Sig.
.021
.023
N
44
45
LF Power
r
.317*
.273
Sig.
.036
.069
N
44
45
Heart Rate Mean (a minute)
r
-.326*
-.338*
Sig.
.031
.023
N
44
45
RMSSD represents short-term rapid changes in heart rate, which can only occur under the
influence of the parasympathetic nervous system (Schafer and Ginsberg, 2017). Researchers agree that
RMSDD is a measure of vagus-mediated control of the heart. The activity of the vagus nerve reduces
heart rate and increases blood flow to the heart.
139
InRMSSD is the natural logarithm of this value. It is stated that it is used to bring the data into a
more understandable range. It would not be wrong to expect a perfect correlation between RMSSD
and InRMSSD.
In healthy and non-fasting individuals, determination subdimension scores increase
significantly as (InRMSSD) the square root (natural logarithm) of the mean of short-term rapid changes
in heart rate increases (r=.335, n=44, p<.05). Considering that the scores are reverse coded, it can be
said that as the individual moves away from rational, logical thinking skills, the InRMSSD value
increases, parasympathetic system activity increases and heart rate decreases. As the InRMSSD value
increases, the implicit understanding in intuitive thinking also increases (r=.319, n=45, p<.05). If
intuitive thinking is considered an implicit understanding, it has a positive correlation with medium
effect size with InRMSSD, that is, short-term changes in heart rate. In other words, if the intuitive
thinking skill is considered the opposite of rational and logical thinking, it is related to the average of
short-term changes in heart rate.
MRRINT, the average of R-R intervals in heart rate, is also correlated to determination sub-
dimension in intuitive thinking (r=.347, n=44, p<.05) and implicit understanding scores (r=.338, n=44,
p<.05). It has a moderate positive significant correlation. In other words, if the average of the R-R beat
intervals is high, the heart beat intervals are wide and the number of heart beats per minute is low. It
means as the number of heart beats per minute decreases, determination subscale score in intuitive
thinking and implicit comprehension subscale scores increase. As observed for all healthy individuals
in the previous section, low frequency power also increases as determination subscale scores increases
among non-fasting individuals (r=.317, n=44, p<.05). In other words, when baroreflex function
increases and heart rhythm decreases, determination in intuitive thinking increases. The average heart
rate per minute (HR mean) shows a negative correlation with medium effect size and determination
subscale in intuitive thinking and implicit understanding. This finding is naturally consistent with
previous findings on R-R interval mean, INRMSSD, and RMSSD.
3. 3. Relationships Between HRV and Intuitive Thinking Skills with the data from Non-Fasting and
Non-Menstrual (Healthy) Samples
Table 4: Statistics with the Data from Non-fasting and Non-menstrual Healthy Samples
Intuitive Thinking:
Determination
Intuitive Thinking:
Implicit Knowledge
RMSSD:
r
.398*
.387*
Sig
.024
.026
N
32
33
lnRMSSD:
.445*
.416*
Sig.
.011
.016
N
32
33
Mean R-R Interval
r
.387*
.520**
Sig.
.028
.002
N
32
33
140
LF Power
r
.379*
.316
Sig.
.032
.073
N
32
33
HF Peak
r
-.328
-.358*
Sig.
.067
.041
N
32
33
Heart Rate Mean
r
-.338
-.513**
Sig.
.058
.002
N
32
33
In the analyzes conducted with the data of healthy individuals who do not fast or menstruate, it
can be seen that the degree and type of relationships increase. RMSSD, the mean of the square roots of
consecutive time differences between heartbeats and InRMSSD, the natural logarithm of the mean of
the square roots of consecutive time differences between heartbeats and heart rate mean have
significant relationships with medium and large effect sizes with both the determination subscale and
the implicit comprehension scores. As in previous analyses, low frequency power have significant
relationships with medium effect size only with the determination sub-dimension. In healthy, non-
fasting and non-menstrual individuals, significant negative relationships of medium effect size were
also observed between the high frequency peak value and heart rate mean with the implicit
comprehension sub-dimension. As the heart rate average and high frequency peak value decrease, the
implicit comprehension scores increases.
All this information mostly shows that intuitive thinking skills improve as the heart rhythm
becomes regular and calm and it is consistent with the findings of McCraty et al. (2004a). It is also
observed that irregularities such as fasting and menstruation cause a decrease in some sub-dimensions
of intuitive thinking skills in healthy individuals.
3. 4. Analysis of Differences between HRV and Intuitive Thinking Skills of Fasting and Non-
Fasting (Healthy) Samples
In the analyzes carried out to see whether fasting caused a difference in HRV values, significant
differences were observed in the Mean R-R Interval and Heart Rhythm Average (HR) values. For the
mean R-R Interval, the average of 56 people who fasted was 760.55, and the average of 45 people who
did not fast was 719.91. The obtained t=-2.105 for the significance of the differences, the probability is
.038 (p<.05) at 99 degrees of freedom. For the effect size, Cohen's d -.421 shows an effect size close to
the medium level. For heart rate mean (HR mean), the average of 56 people who fasted was 80.43, and
the average of 45 people who did not fast was 85.78. The t=2.516 obtained for the significance of the
differences has a the probability of .013 (p<.05) at 99 degrees of freedom. For effect size, Cohen's d .504
indicates a medium effect size. While the average heart rate intervals of fasting people are higher than
those who are not fasting, the average heart rate per minute is lower. Fasting causes a decrease in heart
rate and an increase in its intervals. However, heart rates are low and the increase in intervals does not
show a relationship between intuitive intelligence and its sub-dimensions as expected. Only low and
141
high frequency rates show a low-medium level significant relationship with the determination sub-
dimension in intuitive thinking (r=.280, n=55, p<.05).
SUMMARY AND DISCUSSION
Low frequency power (LF power) has a notable and consistent positive correlation with the sub-
dimension of determination in intuitive thinking across all individuals, including those who are not
fasting and those who are not fasting and menstruation. When individuals who are not fasting are
chosen, positive correlations with medium effect size between the dimensions of determination in
intuitive thinking and implicit understanding, and InRMSSD and Mean R-R Interval, in addition to
low frequency power are observed. Furthermore, notable inverse correlations were found between
determination and implicit understanding scores and the mean heart rate (HR) in non-fasting
participants. In addition to all the above relationships increase in individuals who do not fast and do
not menstruate, the high frequency peak value also shows significant, medium effect size relationships
with the implicit understanding sub-dimension. The findings of this research, decrease in heart rate
and increase in R-R interval improves intuitive thinking skills, is consistent with the study of McCraty
et al. (2004a) in respect to significant decrease in heart rate is observed before encountering emotionally
charged stimuli.
Research findings indicate that there are some correlations between intuitive thinking skills and
certain sub-dimensions, as reported by individuals, and specific heart rhythm indices. These
correlations vary in terms of their effect sizes. These associations do not provide sufficient evidence to
assert that the heart is the source of intuitive thinking or that the heart has an impact on our intuitive
thinking abilities. Nevertheless, it is satisfactory to assert that certain features of ours, which are
characterized or acknowledged as intuitive thinking abilities, are connected to heart rhythm indices
and require a more thorough investigation.
All findings show that as the average heart rhythm decreases in healthy individuals, intuitive
thinking skills based on the individual's self-report improve. Relationships improve when situations
that disrupt the individual's routine, such as fasting and menstruation, are eliminated.
Although there is no agreed definition in the literature on intuition. However, it can be
categorized as either an explicit form of interpreting signs and understanding in familiar situations
based on knowledge and reasoning, or as an implicit understanding in unfamiliar situations aided by
mental frameworks formed from past experiences and knowledge. Additionally, intuition can be
described as an affective, emotional, or bodily sensation, particularly in high-risk and crucial
circumstances.
The first of them can be understood as a sophisticated demonstration of logical reasoning and
rational thought. The second can be expressed as a kind of association based on past lives and
experiences. The third can be characterized as a sensation, an intuitive perception, that cannot be
rationalized by logic and reason, and is unaffected by knowledge and experience.
Thus, due to the absence of a precise definition of intuition and objective measures for it,
researchers have created scales that rely on one or more of these measures. These scales mostly rely on
individuals' self-reports. The individual responds to inquiries regarding a notion that he lacks a precise
definition of and possesses incomplete knowledge of. Moreover, it is possible that there could be
142
disparities between an individual's self-perception and the way they label their feelings, as well as their
true identity and emotional state. The conceptual level raises questions about the validity and
reliability of the intuition measures, making them a subject of debate. Further investigation is required
to fully comprehend and quantify the phenomenon of intuition. Prior to anything else, it is deemed
necessary to conduct further empirical investigation into the nature of intuition, including its origins
and many manifestations. Furthermore, it is believed that tests incorporating several performance
indicators are necessary in conjunction with declaration-based scales for accurate measurement.
During the developmental phase of these scales or tests, it would be more advantageous to employ
various instruments to scrutinize each assumption and diverse performance measurements to
substantiate them, rather than assessing numerous interpretations of the presumed notion of intuition
using a solitary scale.
ETHICAL STATEMENT
The author(s) declare that all processes of the study comply with research and publication ethics,
adhering to ethical rules and principles of scientific citation.
Prior to conducting the research, ethical approval and institutional permission were obtained
from the Ethics Committee of Social and Human Sciences at Kocaeli University with decisions dated
07/03/2024 (No. 3) and 07/06/2024 (No. 7). Written and verbal consent was obtained from all
participants before the study commenced.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
Since the study has a single author, the author's contribution rate is 100%.
FINANCIAL SUPPORT
This study has not received any financial support.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
There is no conflict of interest regarding the study
References
Agyakwa, K. (1988). Intuition, Knowledge and Education. The Journal of Educational Thought
(JET) / Revue de La Pensée Éducative, 22(3), 161177. http://www.jstor.org/stable/23768999
Akarsu, B. (1988). Felsefe terimleri sözlüğü. İstanbul: İnkılap Kitapevi. Cited in Wikipedia
retrieved on 13.04.2024 from https://www.wikiwand.com/tr/Sezgicilik
Armour, J.A. and Ardell, J.L. (1994). Neurocardiology, Oxford University Press: New York.
ATT-2017SZ0217856F (2017) report page https://fccid.io/2ALC3KYTO2935/Test-Report/Test-
Report-3317070
Bailin, S. (1991). Rationality and Intuition. Paideusis, 4(2), 1726.
https://doi.org/10.7202/1073380ar
Bealer, G. (1998). Intuition and the Autonomy of Philosophy. In Michael Raymond DePaul &
William M. Ramsey (eds.), Rethinking Intuition: The Psychology of Intuition and its Role in Philosophical
Inquiry. Rowman & Littlefield Publishers. pp. 201-240.
143
Berkant, G. H., Bahadır, S., Şanal, N. (2022). Sezgisel Düşünme Ölçeğinin Geliştirilmesi: Geçerlik
ve Güvenirlik Çalışması. Elektronik Sosyal Bilimler Dergisi. 21 (84) 1569 1592.
https://doi.org/10.17755/esosder.1110971
Bibika M. U. (2024). Gerçekliğin Bütününe Ancak Sezgi Yoluyla Ulaşırız- Henri Bergson. Destek
Yayınları. ISBN: 9786256608689
Billman G.E: (2011, November). Heart rate variability: a historical perspective. Frontiers in
Physiology, 29(2), 86. https://doi.org/10.3389/fphys.2011.00086
Cantin, M. and Genest, J. (1988). The heart as an endocrine gland. Pharmacol Res Commun,. 20
Suppl 3: p. 1-22. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0031-6989(88)80103-6
Cai Shi, & Lucietto, A.M. (2022). The preference of the use of intuition over other methods of
problem solving by undergraduate students. The European Educational Researcher, 5(3), 253275.
https://doi.org/10.31757/euer.532
Cai Shi., Azevedo, T. M., & Lucietto, A. M. (2021, July). Assessing Intuition Used Among
Undergraduate Engineering Technology and Engineering Students. In 2021 ASEE Virtual Annual
Conference Content Access.
Cheng, K.S., Croarkin, P.E. & Lee, P.F. (2019). Heart Rate Variability of Various Video-Aided
Mindful Deep Breathing Durations and Its Impact on Depression, Anxiety, and Stress Symptom
Severity. Mindfulness 10(10), 20822094. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-019-01178-8
Chhetri, P., Shrestha, L., & Mahotra, N. B. (2022). Validity of Elite-HRV Smartphone Application
for Measuring Heart Rate Variability Compared to Polar V800 Heart Rate Monitor. Journal of Nepal
Health Research Council, 19(4), 809813. https://doi.org/10.33314/jnhrc.v19i04.3949
Chudnoff, E. (2019). In Search of Intuition. Australasian Journal of Philosophy, 98(3):1-16 DOI:
10.1080/00048402.2019.1658121
Cygankiewicz, I., & Zareba, W. (2013). Heart rate variability. Handbook of Clinical Neurology, 117,
379393. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-444-53491-0.00031-6
Demiral, Y., Ergor, G., Unal, B., Semin, S., Akvardar, Y., Kivircik, B., & Alptekin, K. (2006).
Normative data and discriminative properties of short form 36 (SF-36) in Turkish urban population.
BMC public health, 6, 247. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-6-247
Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ Error: Emotion Reason, and the Human Brain. New York: Avon
Books.
Dunn, B. D., Galton, H. C., Morgan, R., Evans, D., Oliver, C., Meyer, M., Cusack, R., Lawrence,
A. D., & Dalgleish, T. (2010). Listening to Your Heart: How Interoception Shapes Emotion Experience
and Intuitive Decision Making. Psychological Science, 21(12), 1835-1844.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610389191
Glöckner, A., & Witteman, C. (2010). Beyond dual-process models: A categorisation of processes
underlying intuitive judgement and decision making. Thinking & Reasoning, 16(1), 125.
https://doi.org/10.1080/13546780903395748
Gore, J., & Sadler-Smith, E. (2011). Unpacking Intuition: A Process and Outcome Framework.
Review of General Psychology, 15(4), 304-316. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025069
Guzik, P., Piekos, C., Fenech, N., Pierog, O., Krauze, T., Piskorski, J., and Wykretowicz, A.
(June 2017). Heart Rate Variability by mobile app 'ELITE HRV' is not the same as computed from an
ECG, EP Europace, Volume 19, Issue suppl_3, 257259, https://doi.org/10.1093/ehjci/eux155.006
144
Gündoğan, A. O. (2024). Sezgicilik. Sosyal Bilimler Ansiklopedisi Tubitak.
https://ansiklopedi.tubitak.gov.tr/ansiklopedi/sezgicilik
Haklı, Ş., (2007). İbn Sînâ epistemolojisinde bir bilgi kaynağı olarak “Sezgi.” Hitit Üniversitesi
İlahiyat Fakültesi Dergisi, 6(11), 3552.
https://dergipark.org.tr/en/pub/hititilahiyat/issue/7694/100805
Hammond, K. R. (1996). Human judgment and social policy. Irreducible uncertainty, inevitable
error, unavoidable injustice. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. İn Pretz 2014
Himariotis, A. T., Coffey, K. F., Noel, S. E., & Cornell, D. J. (2022). Validity of a Smartphone
Application in Calculating Measures of Heart Rate Variability. Sensors (Basel, Switzerland), 22(24),
9883. https://doi.org/10.3390/s22249883
Hogarth, R.M. (2001). Educating intuition. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. In Pretz, et al.
(2014). Development and validation of a new measure of intuition: The Types of Intuition Scale. Journal
of Behavioral Decision Making, 27(5), 454467. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.1820
Holzer, P. (2022). Gut signals and gut feelings: science at the interface of data and beliefs.
Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 16. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2022.929332
Jung, C. G. (1971). Psychological types. Princeton, NJ: University of Princeton Press. In Pretz, et al.
(2014). Development and validation of a new measure of intuition: The Types of Intuition Scale. Journal
of Behavioral Decision Making, 27(5), 454467. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.1820
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, Fast and Slow, New York: In Chudnoff, E. (2019) In Search of
Intuition. Australasian Journal of Philosophy, 98(3):1-16 DOI: 10.1080/00048402.2019.1658121
Koksvik, O., (2011). Intuition. Dissertation, Australian National University
Lacey, B. C. & Lacey, J. I. (1978). Two-way communication between the heart and the brain.
Significance of time within the cardiac cycle. The American psychologist, 33(2), 99113.
https://doi.org/10.1037//0003-066x.33.2.99
Laurman, J. (2023). Assessing the efficacy of an intervention aimed at increasing heart rate variability
and decreasing perceived stress in individuals with personality disorders - A single case experimental design
pilot study. Master Thesis Dissertaion. Faculty of Behavioural, Management and Social Sciences,
University Twente
McCraty, R. (2000). Psychophysiological Coherence: A Link Between Positive Emotions, Stress
Reduction, Performance and Health Paper presented at the American Psychological Association 109th
Annual Convention, Symposium on Gratitude and Positive Emotionality as Links Between Social and
Clinical Science, San Francisco, CA,
McCraty, R. (2002). Influence of cardiac afferent input on heart-brain synchronization and cognitive
performance. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 45(1-2): p. 72-73.
McCraty, R., Atkinson, M., & Bradley, R. T. (2004a). Electrophysiological evidence of intuition:
Part 1. The surprising role of the heart. Journal of alternative and complementary medicine (New York, N.Y.),
10(1), 133143. https://doi.org/10.1089/107555304322849057
McCraty, R., M. Atkinson, M., and Bradley, R.T. (2004b). Electrophysiological evidence of
intuition: Part 2. A system-wide process? Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, (New York,
N.Y.) 10(2): p. 325-336. https://doi.org/10.1089/107555304322849057
McCraty, R., Atkinson, M., Tomasino, D. & Bradley, R.T. (2009, December 1). The Coherent Heart
HeartBrain Interactions, Psychophysiological Coherence, and the Emergence of System-Wide Order.
Integral Review, 5(2). 10-115
145
McCraty, R. & Zayas, M.A. (2014, September 29). Cardiac coherence self-regulation autonomic
stability and psychosocial wellbeing. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1090
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01090
McCraty, R. (2015). Science of the Heart, Volume 2 Exploring the Role of the Heart in Human
Performance. An Overview of Research Conducted by the HeartMath Institute.
https://doi.org/10.13140/RG.2.1.3873.5128
Nado, J. (2014). Why intuition? Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 89(1), 1541.
https://www.jstor.org/stable/24672978
Otte, M. (1990). Intuition and Logic. For the Learning of Mathematics Vol. 10, No. 2 (Jun., 1990), pp.
37-43
Öktem, Ü. (2000). Descartes Kant Bergson ve Husserl’de Sezgi. Ankara Üniversitesi Dil Ve Tarih
Corafya Fakültesi Dergisi, 40(12), 159188. https://doi.org/10.1501/Dtcfder_0000000592
Perrotta, A. S., Jeklin, A. T., Hives, B. A., Meanwell, L. E., & Warburton, D. E. R. (2017). Validity
of the Elite HRV Smartphone Application for Examining Heart Rate Variability in a Field-Based
Setting. Journal of strength and conditioning research, 31(8), 22962302.
https://doi.org/10.1519/JSC.0000000000001841
Pretz, J. E., Brookings, J. B., Carlson, L. A., Humbert, T. K., Roy, M., Jones, M., & Memmert, D.
(2014). Development and validation of a new measure of intuition: The Types of Intuition Scale. Journal
of Behavioral Decision Making, 27(5), 454467. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.1820
Rahman, F., Pechnik, S., Gross, D. R., Sewell, L., & Goldstein, D. S. (2011). Low frequency power
of heart rate variability reflects baroreflex function, not cardiac sympathetic innervation. Clinical
Autonomic Research, 21(3), 133-141. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10286-010-0098-y
Ramon, M. M., March, M. M., González, I. P., Zabala, I. P. and Javaloyes, A. (2022). Validity
and reliability of different smartphones applications to measure HRV during short and ultra-short
measurements in elite athletes. Computer Methods and Programs. Biomedicine Volume 217, Issue C,
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cmpb.2022.106696
Rein, G., Atkinson, M. & McCraty R. (1995). The Physiological and Psychological Effects of
Compassion and Anger. Journal of Advancement in Medicine. 1995; 8(2): 87-105.
Salem, M. (2009). The Heart, Mind and Spirit. The Arab Journal of Psychiatry Vol.20 No.2 Page
(161-168) http://arabpsynet.com/Journals/ajp/ajp20.2-P160.pdf
Salovey, P., & Mayer, J. D. (1990). Emotional intelligence. Imagination, cognition and personality,
9(3), 185-211. https://doi.org/10.2190/DUGG-P24E-52WK-6CDG
Shaffer, F., & Ginsberg, J.P. (2017). An Overview of Heart Rate Variability Metrics and Norms.
Frontiers in Public Health, 5:258 DOI: 10.3389/fpubh.2017.00258
Sgoifo, A., Braglia, F., Costoli, T., Musso, E., Meerlo, P., Ceresini, G., & Troisi, A. (2003, January).
Cardiac autonomic reactivity and salivary cortisol in men and women exposed to social stressors:
relationship with individual ethological profile. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 27(12), 179188.
https://doi.org/10.1016/s0149-7634(03)00019-8
Soyarslan, S. (2024). Spinoza: Intuition. Philpapers (Open Access Bibliography of Philosophy)
retrieved 10th April 2024 from https://philpapers.org/browse/spinoza-intuition
TDK (2024). Türkçe sözlük. Ankara: Türk Dil Kurumu. https://sozluk.gov.tr/
Tiller, W.A., McCraty, R. & Atkinson, M. (1996, January). Cardiac coherence: a new, noninvasive
measure of autonomic nervous system order. Alternative Therapies in Health and Medicine, 2(1), 5265.
... The use of ECG to detect intuitive behavior has not been extensively studied in recent years. However, a recent study [15] involving a large sample of 149 participants examined Heart Rate Variability (HRV) and found that an increase in intuitive thinking ability was associated with enhanced baroreflex function and a decrease in heart rhythm variability. ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Artificial Intelligence (AI) systems frequently exhibit systematic blind spots, often referred to as hallucinations in Large Language Models (LLMs), posing risks in high-stakes applications such as autonomous systems, security, and military operations. This paper explores how human intuitive responses, along with conscious reasoning processes, can be integrated to mitigate AI blind spots and enhance decision-making effectiveness within Human-AI teams. We introduce Human-Guided Artificial Intelligence (HGAI) as a framework for achieving this goal. Specifically, we examine the role of System-1 intuitive processing, as captured through physiological signals such as electroencephalography (EEG) and electrocardiography (ECG), System-2 reasoning-based decision-making, and Multi-Modal Fusion (MMF) mechanisms while assessing the knowledge required to develop more reliable, context-aware, and ethically aligned intelligent decision-making systems for highly complex environments.
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of the current study was to determine the concurrent validity of the Elite HRV smartphone application when calculating heart rate variability (HRV) metrics in reference to an independent software criterion. A total of 5 minutes of R–R interval and natural log of root mean square of the successive differences (lnRMSSD) resting HRV data were simultaneously collected using two Polar H10 heart rate monitors (HRMs) in both the seated and supine positions from 22 participants (14 males, 8 females). One H10 HRM was paired with a Polar V800 watch and one with the Elite HRV application. When no artifact correction was applied, significant, but small, differences in the lnRMSSD data were observed between the software in the seated position (p = 0.022), and trivial and nonstatistically significant differences were observed in the supine position (p = 0.087). However, significant differences (p > 0.05) in the lnRMSSD data were no longer identifiable in either the seated or the supine positions when applying Very Low, Low, or Automatic artifact-correction filters. Additionally, excellent agreements (ICC3,1 = 0.938 − 0.998) and very strong to near-perfect (r = 0.889 − 0.997) relationships were observed throughout all correction levels. The Elite HRV smartphone application is a valid tool for calculating resting lnRMSSD HRV metrics.
Article
Full-text available
Sezgi, insanoğlunun varoluşunun özünde yer alan bilginin, kaynağını ve ilhamını yaşamsal faaliyetlerden alarak genellikle zamandan ve mekândan bağımsız bir biçimde açığa çıkması şeklinde tanımlanabilir. Sezgisel düşünme, nitelikleri açısından bir bütünlük oluşturmakla birlikte diğer tüm düşünme biçimlerinin kapısını aralama görevini de üstlenmektedir. Bu açıdan eğitim-öğretim sürecinde bireylere akademik başarı, keşfetme, yaratıcılık gibi birçok özelliğin kazandırılmasında sezgisel düşünme becerileri oldukça önemlidir. Bu araştırma, lise öğrencilerinin sezgisel düşünme özelliklerini tespit etmeye yönelik Likert tipi bir ölçek geliştirmenin amaçlandığı tarama modelinde nicel bir çalışmadır. Araştırmada ölçek geliştirme sürecine yönelik madde havuzu oluşturulduktan sonra dil ve kapsam geçerliği çalışmaları yürütülmüştür. Araştırmanın verileri Kahramanmaraş ili Onikişubat ilçesinde yer alan devlet okullarının lise kademesinde öğrenim gören, pilot çalışma için 414, AFA için 588 ve DFA için 264 öğrenciden toplanmıştır. AFA sonucunda elde edilen dört boyutlu yapı DFA ile test edilmiş olup, 22 maddeden oluşan ölçeğin uyum değerleri χ²/df= 1.55; GFI= .90; RMSA= 0.46; SRMR= 0.77; GFI= .95; CFI= .94; NFI= .87; NNFI(TLI)= .94; IFI= .95 olarak ve boyutların Cronbach alpha güvenirlik katsayıları ise kararlılık için 0.735, içsel kesinlik için 0.799, duygusal okur-yazarlık için 0.840, kavrayışın örtüklüğü için 0.802 ve ölçeğin geneli için 0.808 olarak hesaplanmıştır. Yapılan analizler sonucunda, geliştirilen Sezgisel Düşünme Ölçeği’nin geçerli ve güvenilir bir ölçek olduğu sonucuna ulaşılmıştır.
Article
Full-text available
Background: With technological advancement, it has become very easy to obtain heart rate variability data with handy heart rate monitors as well as smartphone applications. This study was conducted to assess the validity of Elite HRV smartphone application to measure time domain heart rate variability indices at rest, in comparison to Polar V-800 heart rate monitor. Methods: Heart rate variability data were acquired from twenty apparently healthy male medical students in supine position after simultaneously connecting Polar V800 and Elite HRV to chest-worn Polar H7 sensor. Time domain indices viz. Mean RR intervals, mean heart rate, standard deviation of normal to normal intervals and root mean square of successive differences of interval were taken for comparison. Difference, validity and agreement were assessed using students t-test, intraclass correlation coefficient and Bland Altman plot and limits of agreement. Results: Students t-test showed that there was no significant difference between the data obtained from Elite HRV and Polar V-800 in all the parameters viz. mean RR intervals, mean heart rate, standard deviation of normal to normal intervals and root mean square of successive differences of interval (p>0.05). Correlation was excellent as shown by Intra-class correlation coefficient of >0.999 in all the parameters. Bland Altman analysis revealed small bias and narrow limits of agreement with all the parameters: mean heart rate [-0.003 (0.05 - 0.04)], mean RR [0.01 (-0.58 - 0.60)], standard deviation of normal to normal intervals [-0.01 (-0.32 - 0.30)] and root mean square of successive differences of interval [-0.05 (-0.89 - 0.79)]. Conclusions: Elite HRV smartphone application provided reliable time-domain heart rate variability data consistent with the data obtained from validity tested Polar V800 heart rate monitor.
Book
This book introduces a new topic; a critical researched-based analysis of the role of human judgment in social policy formation. It applies what has been learned from research on human judgment to specific examples - from the Challenger disaster to present-day debates on health care. Human judgment can be a source of both hope and fear in the creation of social policy. Yet this important process has rarely been examined because research on human judgment has been scarce. Now, however, the results of 50 years of empirical work offer an unprecedented opportunity to examine human judgment and the basis of our hopes and fears. Numerous examples from law, medicine, engineering, and economics are used throughout to demonstrate these and other features of human judgment in action.
Article
Background and objective : Heart rate variability (HRV) has been proposed as a useful marker that can show the performance adaptation and optimize the training process in elite athletes. The development of wearable technology permits the measurement of this marker through smartphone applications. The purpose of this study is to assess the validity and reliability of short and ultra-short HRV measurements in elite cyclists using different smartphone applications. Method : Twenty-six professional cyclists were measured at rest in supine and in seated positions through the simultaneous use of an electrocardiogram and two different smartphone applications that implement different technologies to measure HRV: Elite HRV (with a chest strap) and Welltory (photoplethysmography). Level of significance was set at p < 0.05. Results : Compared to an electrocardiogram, Elite HRV and Welltory showed no differences neither in supine nor in seated positions (p > 0.05) and they showed very strong to almost perfect correlation levels (r = 0.77 to 0.94). Furthermore, no differences were found between short (5 min) and ultra-short (1 min) length measurements. Intraclass correlation coefficient showed good to excellent reliability and the standard error of measurement remained lower than 6%. Conclusion : Both smartphone applications can be implemented to monitor HRV using short- and ultra-short length measurements in elite endurance athletes.
Article
Autonomic nervous system (ANS) activity is a core and central component of emotion and motivated action. The myriad social and cognitive challenges faced by humans require flexible modulation of ANS activity for different contexts. In this study, simultaneous activity of the parasympathetic and sympathetic nervous system was measured using respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA) and pre-ejection period (PEP), respectively. Samples combined four previous studies (N = 325, 63% female, aged 15–55) in which RSA and PEP were collected continuously during a resting baseline and an acute stressor, the Trier Social Stress Task. The concurrent relation between RSA and PEP responses was modelled in order to determine the extent to which SNS and PNS activity is correlated across the task within and between participants, and whether this correlation was moderated by age, race, sex, or baseline RSA and PEP. Overall, RSA and PEP were reciprocally coupled. However, recovery from a stressor was characterized by coactivation. Individuals also vary in the extent to which their SNS and PNS are reciprocally coupled; women, younger adults, and individuals with higher baseline RSA showed more reciprocal coupling than men, older adults, and those with lower baseline RSA, respectively, reflecting greater coordination of physiological responding in the former group.