ArticlePDF Available

Propagation of interspecies sexual behaviour between Japanese macaques and Sika deer: First evidence

Authors:

Abstract

This naturalistic note presents a series of observation of interspecies sexual behaviour involving Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata yakui) and Sika deer (Cervus Nippon yakushimae) on Yakushima Island, Japan. The initial observation in 2015 revealed a male macaque's attempt to engage in sexual activity with female deer, displaying mate-guarding behaviour. Subsequent observations in 2020, 2021 and 2023 showed the continuation of this behaviour and potentially the propagation to other macaques. We categorised this rare behaviour as a case of reproductive interference and explored hypotheses regarding its functionality. While some suggest nutritional benefits for the deer, others propose learning, incomplete species recognition, or mate deprivation hypotheses. This behaviour may also be selectively neutral, offering no direct fitness benefits, but rather represents a behavioural by-product of other interactions between these two species, which may themselves be adaptive. Furthermore, we hypothesise that the observed propagation may underlie social transmission and highlight the potential cognitive capacities of Japanese macaques involving social learning mechanisms and the willingness to adopt non-instinctual behaviours.
OPEN ACCESS
Copyright: © 2023 The Author(s). This is an open-access arcle distributed under the terms of the Creave Commons Aribuon 4.0
Internaonal License (CC-BY 4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribuon, and reproducon in any medium, provided the original
author and source are credited. See hp://creavecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
Cultural Science Journal is a peer-reviewed open access journal published by Erfurt University, Tallinn University and Sciendo
1
CULTURAL SCIENCE JOURNAL 15(1)
DOI: 10.2478/csj-2023-0001
Propagaon of interspecies sexual behaviour
between Japanese macaques and Sika deer: First
evidence
DRIC SUEUR
Université de Strasbourg, France; e-mail: cedric.sueur@iphc.cnrs.fr; 0000-0001-8206-2739
ATSUYUKI OSHIMA
Atsuyuki Ohshima photography, Kyoto, Japan; e-mail: atyk12.info@gmail.com
MARIE PE
Université Catholique de Lille, France; e-mail: marie.pele@univ-catholille.fr 0000-0003-2297-5522
Keywords: tradion, culture, reproducve interference, primate, cervid behaviour
Abstract: This naturalisc note presents a series of observaon of interspecies sexual behaviour
involving Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata yakui) and Sika deer (Cervus Nippon yakushimae)
on Yakushima Island, Japan. The inial observaon in 2015 revealed a male macaque’s aempt
to engage in sexual acvity with female deer, displaying mate-guarding behaviour. Subsequent
observaons in 2020, 2021 and 2023 showed the connuaon of this behaviour and potenally
the propagaon to other macaques. We categorised this rare behaviour as a case of reproducve
interference and explored hypotheses regarding its funconality. While some suggest nutrional
benefits for the deer, others propose learning, incomplete species recognion, or mate
deprivaon hypotheses. This behaviour may also be selecvely neutral, offering no direct fitness
benefits, but rather represents a behavioural by-product of other interacons between these
two species, which may themselves be adapve. Furthermore, we hypothesise that the observed
propagaon may underlie social transmission and highlight the potenal cognive capacies of
Japanese macaques involving social learning mechanisms and the willingness to adopt non-
insnctual behaviours.
CULTURAL SCIENCE JOURNAL 15 (2023)
Cédric Sueur, Atsuyuki Ohshima, Marie Pe
2
Introducon
Animal culture refers to behaviours, pracces, and tradions that are learned socially within a species
and passed down through generaons. These behaviours, which are not inherited genecally but
through social learning, challenge the belief that culture is unique to humans and offers insights into
animal behaviour, evoluon, and conservaon (Whiten 2000; Laland and Janik 2006; Whiten and
Schaik 2007; Duboscq et al. 2016). Animals, like humans, develop and transmit cultural traits, with
different populaons exhibing disnct behaviours. For example, some chimpanzees use scks to
fish for termites, while others use stones to crack nuts, illustrang the variaon in cultural behaviours
between populaons (Whiten et al. 1999, 2005, 2007; Biro et al. 2006; Matsuzawa 2017). The
concept of animal culture originated in primatology, parcularly through Kinji Imanishi’s study of
Japanese macaques in the mid-20th century. Imanishi’s team observed the spread of potato-washing
behaviour among macaques, providing the first documented example of cultural transmission in
animals (Kawai 1965; Matsuzawa 2015). Jane Goodall’s work with chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in
the 1960s further solidified the idea of animal culture, parcularly through her discovery of tool use,
which was learned and transmied socially within groups (Goodall 1986; Whiten et al. 1999). Since
then, animal culture has been observed across various species. Humpback whales (Megaptera
novaeangliae), for instance, exhibit cultural transmission through their songs (Garland et al. 2011),
while New Caledonian crows (Corvus moneduloides) demonstrate cultural tool use (Rutz et al. 2018).
Even species like fruit flies (Drospophila sp.) display cultural tradions in mate choice (Baes et al.
2015). These findings highlight the importance of cultural diversity in animal conservaon,
emphasising that preserving cultural knowledge is as vital as protecng genec diversity (Carvalho
et al. 2022; Sueur 2022).
JAPANESE MACAQUES AND SIKA DEER CULTURES
Direct interacons between Japanese macaques and Sika deer provide a compelling example of the
evoluon of interspecies relaonships. Inially, these species coexisted closely in terms of spaal
proximity, with both benefing from this arrangement. For instance, macaques, while feeding in
trees, oen drop food, which becomes an addional energy source for the deer, enhancing their
foraging efficiency through gleaning (Tsuji et al. 2007). This interacon fosters a strong cohabitaon
dynamic between the two species. A parcularly unusual aspect of this relaonship is the deer’s
coprophagia, where deer consume the feces of macaques, especially around the laer’s sleeping
sites (Nishikawa and Mochida 2010). As their relaonship change, some macaques have been
observed grooming deer, a behaviour that suggests a deepening bond between the species. On
Yakushima Island, this interacon has advanced into a co-culture (Co-culture is the mutual evoluon
of shared behaviours between different species that influence each other through direct or indirect
interacons in a shared environment) (Sueur and Huffman 2024), with macaques even riding on the
backs of Sika deer (Pelé et al. 2017). Similar behaviours have been observed between rhesus
macaques (Macaca mulaa) and sambar deer (Rusa unicolor), suggesng that such complex
interspecies interacons may arise in other environments and among different species (Prasad et al.
2013). Interesngly, these interacons have even led to interspecies sexual behaviours, as observed
in both Yakushima and the Minoh Prefecture in Japan. Male macaques have been documented
engaging in sexual acvity with female Sika deer, and conversely, female macaques with male deer
(Gunst et al. 2018). These instances illustrate the capacity for highly complex and unexpected
behaviours to emerge between species living in close proximity. The evoluon of their interacons
CULTURAL SCIENCE JOURNAL 15 (2023)
Cédric Sueur, Atsuyuki Ohshima, Marie Pe
3
ranging from shared foraging and grooming to more intricate behaviours like riding and interspecies
sexual acvies  demonstrates significant social and behavioural adaptaon between these two
disnct species. These behaviours are categorised as highly complex due to the acve feedback
loops between the species, which foster reciprocal adaptaons, and the layered nature of their
interaconsfrom foraging and grooming to intricate behaviours like riding and interspecies sexual
acviesindicang cognive processes, social transmission, and behavioural innovaon that
evolve through mutual influence. They appear to learn from one another and have developed co-
cultural behaviours (Sueur and Huffman 2024).
BEHAVIOURAL ANALYSIS
Original study
During his presence on Yakushima Island, Japan, from October 10 to November 6, 2015, the
photographer Alexandre Bonnefoy observed for the first me the intriguing behaviour of an adult
male Japanese macaque (figure 1a, Video 1). A meline and a map are present in figure 2. This male,
seemingly a peripheral member or part of a group of peripheral males (Sprague 1991), exhibited a
case of heterospecific sexual behaviour, characterised by the mounter performing pelvic thrusng
(i.e. rhythmic movements of the mounter’s pelvic girdle toward the mountee’s back or hindquarter).
While this behaviour was documented for the first me in our study (Pelé et al. 2017), it does not
mean that it did not exist before our observaon. Nonetheless, our colleagues on the site had never
reported witnessing this sexual behaviour unl we shared our video findings with them. The male
lived among other males and did not appear to be part of a mul-male, mul-female group. On
November 6, the macaque male engaged in sexual acvity with at least two female Sika deer.
Notably, the macaque did not display aggression toward the deer but acvely chased away other
peripheral macaque males that approached the ‘guarded’ deer. Its inial interacon with one female
deer involved mounng and displaying sexual behaviours with pelvic thrusng, although penetraon
did not occur. These behaviours occurred at consistent intervals between the same macaque male
and the same deer female, with about ten mounts taking place approximately every 45 minutes
over the course of an hour of observaon before the animals disappeared into the forest. The male
subsequently aempted to mount another female deer, which rejected the sexual advances. She
tried to evade the macaque through movements, accelerang her pace, turning around, and
exhibing threatening gestures. Interesngly, shortly aer, a 2018 study revealed that five female
macaques exhibited similar mounng behaviour toward male deer in the Meiji Memorial Forest of
Minoh Quasi-Naonal Park, Minoh Prefecture, near Osaka, Japan  over 600 kilometres away from
Yakushima (Gunst et al. 2018).
In October 2017, we (Marie Pelé and Cédric Sueur) came back for a week to Yakushima to assess
whether the male macaque connued to display this behaviour and whether social transmission was
observed, but we only saw the male macaque close to a deer without sexual behaviour. During this
week we observed from 9 a.m. to 5 p.m. macaques ad libitum, going from group to group on the side
of the unique road along the naonal park, idenfying from three to nine groups per day. We cannot
provide detailed descripons of each group since we were unable to idenfy them specifically. Out
of the numerous groups in Yakushima, only three are closely monitored. Our knowledge is limited to
certain individual idenficaon and insights into the home range and group size of macaques, with
observaons of up to nine groups per day. The density of macaques in Yakushima is exceponally
high. Within the lowland coastal zone, ranging from 0 to 300 metres above sea level, it is esmated
that there were about 4.8 troops and between 62.4 to 99.8 monkeys per square kilometre. To
CULTURAL SCIENCE JOURNAL 15 (2023)
Cédric Sueur, Atsuyuki Ohshima, Marie Pe
4
enhance the reliability of our observaons based on video evidence provided by local inhabitants
and researchers, we have corroborated these recordings with extensive local knowledge of the area’s
wildlife and the consistent behaviour paerns of the subjects over me. This approach has allowed
us to migate some of the limitaons associated with the opportunisc nature of data collecon.
Cues indicang a propagaon
In 2020 and 2021, new videos were recorded by an inhabitant of Yakushima (Cameron Riki Joyce)
documenng interspecies sexual behaviour. No informaon had been disclosed about his daily visits
to the park, including key details such as the locaon and duraon of the observaons. On August
23, 2020, a male macaque looking like the one previously observed in 2015 and 2017 was seen riding
a female deer and engaging in sexual behaviour for nine seconds (figure 1b, Video 2). This male was
idenfied by his disnct almond-shaped eyes, though we cannot be certain, with 100% accuracy,
that he was the same individual. However, this does not affect the overall aim and conclusions of this
note. In the video, the male was not seen with any other macaques. However, on March 12, 2021,
the male was observed surrounded by other macaques, including one male, at least six females and
three juveniles (Video 3). On the same day, a young female climbed onto a male deer, quickly
groomed it, and then engaged in two sequences of sexual behaviours (with pelvic thrusng as
behavioural units lasng five and seven seconds, respecvely), followed by an extended period of
grooming the deer (figure 1c, video 3). We defined a sequence of sexual behaviour as at least three
consecuve pelvic thrusng behaviour. Later, another female, older than the first one described
(differenated through the body size and corpulence), also briefly engaged in a sequence of sexual
behaviour with a male deer before being ejected by the deer (figure 1d, video 4). On November 29,
2023, a female exhibited mounting behaviour with pelvic thrusting on a male deer. Video 5,
provided by A. Otsuyuki Ohshima, showcases activities within the national park during the week
of November 27, capturing moments from dawn until dusk. Observations were made ad
libitum, moving between groups along the roadside.
The sexual behaviour displayed by female macaques toward male deer differs from the typical
mang behaviours observed with male macaques, as the pelvic movements are slower, less rhythmic,
and lack the thrusng intensity usually seen in heterosexual interacons. Instead, these movements
resemble the slower pelvic moons oen observed in female-female homosexual interacons
among macaques. These observaons indicate two significant findings: firstly, if it indeed is the
original male observed in 2015, the macaque exhibited a connuaon of the original behaviour, and
secondly, these observaons suggest possible propagaon (Duboscq et al. 2016) of this behaviour to
other group members though further data and evidence are needed to confirm this transmission.
Notably, this propagaon occurred between macaques of different sexes, from one male to, at least,
two females.
The appearance of rare and unique behaviours, such as the interspecies interacons documented
here, highlights the value of non-standard methods like cizen science. As we see in this study, videos
captured by photographers and tour guides provide invaluable insights, serving as powerful
complements to rigorous scienfic methods and expanding the scope of discovery beyond standard
observaonal approaches alone (Silvertown2009; Nelson and Fijn 2013; Sueur et al. 2020; Fraisl et
al. 2022).
CULTURAL SCIENCE JOURNAL 15 (2023)
Cédric Sueur, Atsuyuki Ohshima, Marie Pe
5
Figure 1. Cases of heterospecific sexual behaviour between Japanese macaques and Sika deer. a. First
observaon made in 2015, a male macaque on a female deer. b. Second observaon of probably the
same male macaque on a female deer made in 2020. c. and d. Observaon of female macaques on male
deer indicang a social transmission.
Figure 2: meline and maps of events. 2015: The first observaon of interspecies sexual behaviour
between a male Japanese macaque and a female Sika deer on Yakushima Island was documented. The
male macaque displayed mang behaviours toward the deer without aggression, guarding the deer
against other male macaques. 2017: Cédric Sueur and Marie Pelé returned to Yakushima for a week to
assess whether the male macaque connued this behaviour and to observe potenal social
transmission. However, no further sexual behaviour was observed during this visit. 2018: Observaons
in the Meiji Memorial Forest of Minoh Quasi-Naonal Park near Osaka (over 600 km from Yakushima)
documented five female Japanese macaques displaying similar mounng behaviour toward male deer,
indicang a spread of this behaviour to other regions and individuals. 2020: A video recorded by a
Yakushima resident (Cameron Riki Joyce) captured a male macaque engaging in sexual behaviour with
a female deer for nine seconds. The male macaque was idenfied by unique physical features but could
CULTURAL SCIENCE JOURNAL 15 (2023)
Cédric Sueur, Atsuyuki Ohshima, Marie Pe
6
not be definively confirmed as the same individual observed in 2015 and 2017. 2021: Another video
captured a young female macaque climbing onto a male deer, engaging in grooming and short
sequences of mang-like behaviours, followed by grooming. Later, an older female macaque also briefly
displayed similar behaviour with a male deer, suggesng possible social transmission. 2023: On
November 29, a female macaque was observed displaying mounng behaviour with pelvic thrusng on
a male deer. This observaon further supported the possibility of social transmission, as behaviours
now involved both male and female macaques.
FUNCTIONAL ANALYSIS AND HYPOTHESES
This rare instance of heterospecific sexual interacon aempt is categorised as one of the seven
cases of reproducve interference as defined by Gröning and Hochkirch (2008). They define
reproducve interference as any interspecific interacon occurring during mate acquision that
detrimentally impacts the fitness of at least one species involved, resulng from incomplete species
recognion. They categorise reproducve interference into seven disnct types: signal jamming,
heterospecific rivalry, misdirected courtship, heterospecific mang aempts, erroneous female
choice, heterospecific mang, and hybridisaon. However, the likelihood that Japanese macaques
misrecognise sika deer as conspecific is minimal. This improbability is underscored by the long-term
coexistence and frequent interacons these two species share on Yakushima Island. Instead, the
observed interspecies interacons, including sexual behaviours, likely emerge from the close
proximity and the complex, reciprocal interspecies dynamics that have developed, rather than from
errors in species recognion.
The first observaon and second observaons involve sexual interacon between a male
Japanese macaque and a female Sika deer, although there was no penetraon. In the other
observaons, it involves female macaques and male deer. Unlike some cases of interspecific
interacons that involve sexual harassment or coercion (Cluon-Brock and Parker 1995), these
behaviours appeared different; the deer are always in the vicinity of macaques looking for food le
by the macaques or eang the feces of the monkeys (Tsuji et al. 2007; Nishikawa and Mochida 2010;
Pelé et al. 2017). The female deer’s behaviour, specifically licking the macaque’s seminal fluid,
suggests that it might see this behaviour as a source of nutrion. However, this hypothesis cannot
explain why the male deer let females climb on them. We first suggested that the hormonal surge
during the macaque breeding season, coupled with their kind-of cooperave interacons as
grooming exchange and play with Sika deer, may have contributed to this heterospecific sexual
behaviour. In our 2017 paper (Pelé et al. 2017), we proposed alternave explanaons for this
behaviour. One theory suggested it might serve as a learning mechanism for copulaon. Another
considered the possibility of incomplete species recognion which is quite improbable. Addionally,
we introduced the ‘mate deprivaon hypothesis’, which posits that males with limited access to
females might be more inclined to engage in such behaviour. This hypothesis is parcularly relevant
in species facing intense sexual compeon. Similar paerns have been observed in other sexual
behaviours among Japanese macaques, such as masturbaon (Thomsen and Sols 2004) and
homosexual behaviour (Leca et al. 2015).
However, these new observaons do not confirm these hypotheses. The male connued
displaying interspecies sexual behaviour despite appearing to be dominant within his group, as
shown in the video where he is seen chasing and threatening other group members. This behaviour
may also be selecvely neutral, offering no direct fitness benefits, but rather represents a behavioural
by-product of other interacons between these two species, which may themselves be adapve.
CULTURAL SCIENCE JOURNAL 15 (2023)
Cédric Sueur, Atsuyuki Ohshima, Marie Pe
7
COGNITIVE ANALYSIS AND HYPOTHESES
Regarding the interspecies sexual behaviour
Sexual behaviours observed among species can be aributed to their close proximity and frequent
interacons. Indeed, complex interspecific interacons have been observed in various species,
ranging from mixed-species groups to mutual exchanges of services between different species. These
interacons have been documented in different contexts and can have various benefits (Axelrod and
Hamilton 1981). For example, mixed-species groups can benefit from proximity to members of other
species, leading to indirect advantages such as increased vigilance against predators or access to
dropped fruits from trees (Tsuji et al. 2007; Nishikawa and Mochida 2010). In the case of Japanese
macaques and Sika deer on Yakushima Island, direct interacons occur and may have conducted to
these remarkable sexual behaviours between the two species. Indeed, the macaques drop food from
trees, which serves as an energy source for the deer, enhancing their foraging efficiency. This results
in a close cohabitaon between the two species. Addionally, the deer engage in coprophagia by
consuming the feces of Japanese macaques, parcularly around macaque sleeping sites. Some
macaques have even been observed grooming the deer and aempng to ride them without sexual
behaviour involved. Similar interacons have been reported between rhesus macaques and sambar
deer in India (Prasad et al. 2013). The sexual behaviour may have emerged from the close proximity
and exisng rodeo-like behaviours at a parcular moment.
Regarding the potenal social transmission
The observaon of interspecies sexual behaviour between Japanese macaques and Sika deer has
revealed complex interacons that point to both cognive capabilies and the potenal for social
transmission of behaviours. The objecve here is to clarify the differences between the cognive
processes implied by these behaviours and the hypothesis regarding social transmission (Whiten
2000; Hoppi and Laland 2013; Duboscq et al. 2016). Regarding cognive capacies and behavioural
innovaon, the inial sighngs in 2015 suggested a novel aspect of macaque behaviour, although it
is unlikely to be the first occurrence of this innovaon given the absence of systemac, long-term
data collecon and similar behaviours observed in other populaons. However, this was the first
documented instance, as other colleagues regularly present in the field had never previously
observed it. These innovaons illustrate the macaques' ability to adapt and innovate by exploring
mang opportunies outside their species, an indicator of behavioural flexibility and curiosity
(Vancatova 2008; Aplin et al. 2015). Addionally, the guarding behaviour observed may reflect a level
of understanding in managing compeon for mates. This guarding may serve not only as a
reproducve strategy but also as a way to protect the deer as a social or play-related resource rather
than as a direct mang partner (Vasey 2004; Gunst et al. 2018). The different responses observed in
female macaques engaging in reproducve interference with male deer also raise intriguing
quesons about their ability to discern between potenal mang partners. This behaviour implies
that some level of cognive processing may be influencing their decision-making processes in these
interacons (De Petrillo and Rosa 2021).
The social transmission hypothesis is supported by observaons from 2020 to 2023, during which
macaques of both sexes exhibited similar interspecies interacons. These findings may align with the
hypothesis that social learning and transmission are at play, as the spread of this behaviour across
different individuals, potenally from males to females, suggests an advanced form of social learning.
CULTURAL SCIENCE JOURNAL 15 (2023)
Cédric Sueur, Atsuyuki Ohshima, Marie Pe
8
This behaviour indicates that macaques may be open to adopng behaviours culturally transmied
in this species (Zhang and Watanabe 2007, Zhang et al. 2007, Huffman et al. 2010).
We recognise the limitaons posed by the opportunisc nature of our observaons, including the
potenal for previous unrecorded instances of this behaviour among females before 2015. The
absence of long-term, connuous observaon precludes definive conclusions about the origins and
spread of this behaviour among macaques. Despite these limitaons, our interpretaon of the data
is suggesve evidence of behaviour in mulple individuals within the same social group, a paern
that aligns with documented instances of social learning in non-human primates. While we cannot
definively rule out individual learning or independent innovaon as explanaons for the observed
behaviour, the simultaneous occurrence in closely associated individuals suggests a social
component to the behaviour’s acquision. Our suggeson of social transmission is also informed by
comparave analysis with documented cases of social learning in primates, where behaviours spread
through observaon and imitaon within social groups. We also acknowledge the possibility of
alternave explanaons for the observed behaviour, including individual learning or a shared
environmental influence as some studies have not found any evidence of observaonal learning or
imitaon in primates (Nagell et al. 1993, Tomasello and Call 1994, Call et al 2005, Tennie et al 2006,
Clay and Tennie 2018, Neadle et al. 2021).
To fully understand the learning mechanisms underlying this behaviour, addional data would be
required on several fronts, as noted by reviewers. First, establishing the ming of behaviour
innovaon and duraon within the group is crical; although we noted the first recorded
occurrence in 2015, this likely does not reflect the initial appearance of deer mounting. Second,
without continuous monitoring, it remains unclear if other individuals practised this behaviour
prior to the second observation, when females were noted engaging in deer mounting. We,
therefore, lack data on the original innovator of the behaviour and the potential pathways of
social transmission within the group, if any. It is equally possible that all members began
engaging in this behaviour independently, with no social learning involved. To rigorously assess
the role of learning, controlled experimental data would be essential, ideally involving naïve
individuals exposed to social stimuli in a controlled setting. These experiments would require
testing individuals confirmed to be naïve to deer mounting and systematically introducing the
behaviour to discern the mechanisms behind its emergence. Given that this behaviour has also
been observed in a separate group of macaques (Gunst et al. 2018), a simpler assumption may
be that complex social learning mechanisms are not required for this behaviour to manifest;
rather, exposure to the stimulusin this case, the deermay be suicient for its appearance.
EVOLUTIONARY CONSIDERATIONS
Interspecific interacons in animals can have both negave and posive outcomes. Negave
interacons are exemplified by prey-predator relaonships (Freedman and Waltman 1984), where
one species benefits at the expense of the other. Posive interacons are seen in mutualism (Axelrod
and Hamilton 1981), where both species benefit. Heterospecific sexual behaviour, which involves
sexual interacons between individuals of different species, has been mainly described in closely
related species and has been studied in terms of its effects on fitness, hybridisaon, and species
survival (Burdfield-Steel 2012; Kyogoku 2015; Shuker et al. 2015). There have been few documented
cases of heterospecific mang between individuals of distantly related species, and these instances
have primarily occurred in capve or human-animal interacon sengs (Beetz 2004; Gröning and
Hochkirch 2008; Beirne 2009). One study reports sexual harassment of king penguins by Antarcc
CULTURAL SCIENCE JOURNAL 15 (2023)
Cédric Sueur, Atsuyuki Ohshima, Marie Pe
9
fur seals, where some penguins were killed and eaten at the end of their mang behaviour (de Bruyn
et al. 2008; Haddad et al. 2015). This note underscores the relave scarcity of research on
interspecies sexual behaviour, parcularly among distant species in natural sengs. It also highlights
the potenal consequences and complexies associated with such interacons, which can range
from fitness implicaons to survival challenges, depending on the circumstances and species
involved (Sueur and Huffman 2024). The rarity of heterospecific sexual interacons between distant
species makes our observaons significant, providing insights into the evoluon of interspecific
mang behaviour in the animal kingdom and its potenal relevance to understand zoophilia in
humans (Beetz 2004; Beirne 2009).
This interspecies sexual behaviour, if it is socially transmied, which seems probable given past
cultural behaviours observed in macaques, may represent a form of cultural transmission among the
macaques. This cultural transmission can have profound implicaons for the evoluonary trajectory
of a group (van Schaik et al. 2012; Maei 2014; Henrich 2017). It may allow for the spread of novel
behaviours that provide adapve advantages. Cultural behaviours are well documented in Japanese
macaques (Bonnefoy et al. 2016), with some serving praccal funcons, such as the use of hot
springs for bathing (Zhang et al. 2007), washing potatoes (Matsuzawa 2015), or forming strong
aggregaons known as ‘saru-dango.’ However, the funconality of certain other behaviours, such as
the stone-handling behaviour (Leca et al. 2007; Huffman et al. 2010) or these heterospecific sexual
interacons, remains a topic of ongoing debate. These behaviours, nonetheless, illuminate the
intricate social and cultural lives of Japanese macaques. As researchers, it is imperave that we delve
deeper into our observaons and analyses of these behaviours.
CONCLUSION
In conclusion, this note on interspecies interacons between Japanese macaques and Sika deer on
Yakushima Island reveals a complex dynamic that extends beyond mere coexistence. The
documented instances of macaques engaging in heterospecific behaviours with deer, including
grooming, riding, and sexual behaviours, suggest significant cognive and social dimensions. These
interacons highlight the adaptability and behavioural innovaon within macaque populaons,
which may stem from close proximity and regular interspecies encounters. The possibility of social
transmission of these behaviours among macaques implies that such interacons could become
culturally embedded within specific groups, similar to previously documented cultural behaviours.
While heterospecific sexual behaviour is rare and has largely been observed in closely related
species, its occurrence here offers insights into the cognive and social flexibility of macaques.
However, these findings remain preliminary due to the opportunisc nature of observaons. Further
research is needed to determine the mechanisms underlying these behaviours, their evoluonary
implicaons, and the potenal for social learning among individuals. This note adds a unique
dimension to our understanding of animal culture and cross-species interacons, urging a deeper
exploraon of how such behaviours evolve and persist in animal sociees.
Acknowledgements
Funding: This research was funded by an internaonal research project (IRP Comp²A) between the
CNRS and Kyoto University.
CULTURAL SCIENCE JOURNAL 15 (2023)
Cédric Sueur, Atsuyuki Ohshima, Marie Pe
10
Acknowledgments: We thank the Kagoshima Prefecture for the authorisaon of presence in the
Yakushima Naonal Park. We thank Hideki Sugiura for his help on this research. We thank Alexandre
Bonnefoy (Edions Issekinicho) and Cameron Riki Joyce (Yakushima Experience) for their courtesy to
use their pictures and videos.
Conflicts of interest/Compeng interests: The authors are not aware of any conflicts of interest that
may have influenced the material presented in this arcle.
Availability of material:
Video 1: hps://youtu.be/l8HFDnM7Sdw?si=qdaFyeCqfRTRh9pP;
Video 2: hps://youtube.com/shorts/wfDasKQiCb0?si=85BG8v5F7PxvMbmK ;
Video 3: hps://youtube.com/shorts/qK1SEW106D8?si=aNxzkhZ_uXIdIhBH ;
Video 4: hps://youtu.be/peYCBIa0fpk?si=L_5M_BoEzRTborHw ;
Video 5: hps://youtu.be/Df0lw_tBRu8
Authors’ contribuons: Data curaon: AO, CS & MP. Visualisaon: CS & MP. Original dra: CS. Wring,
review and eding: AO, CS & MP.
References
Aplin, L.M.; Farine, D.R.; Morand-Ferron, J. et al. 2015. Experimentally induced innovaons lead to
persistent culture via conformity in wild birds. Nature 518:538
Axelrod, R.; Hamilton, W. 1981. The evoluon of cooperaon. Science 211:13901396.
hps://doi.org/10.1126/science.7466396
Baes, M.; Pasquarea C.; Moreno, C. et al. 2015. Ecology of informaon: social transmission
dynamics within groups of non-social insects. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B:
Biological Sciences 282:20142480. hps://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2014.2480
Beetz, A.M. 2004. Besality/zoophilia: A scarcely invesgated phenomenon between crime,
paraphilia, and love. Journal of Forensic Psychology Pracce 4:136
Beirne, P. 2009. Confronng animal abuse: Law, criminology, and human-animal relaonships.
Rowman & Lilefield Publishers
Bonnefoy, A.; Sueur, C.; Pelé, M. et al. 2016. Saru - Singes du Japon. Issekinicho, Schilgheim
Burdfield-Steel, E.R. 2012. The causes and consequences of reproducve interference in the
Lygaeidae
Call, J.; Carpenter, M.; Tomasello, M. 2005. Copying results and copying acons in the process of
social learning: chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and human children Homo sapiens). Anim Cogn
8:151163. hps://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-004-0237-8
Carvalho, S.; Wessling, E.G.; Abwe, E.E. et al. 2022. Using nonhuman culture in conservaon requires
careful and concerted acon. Conservaon Leers 15:e12860.
hps://doi.org/10.1111/conl.12860
Clay, Z.; Tennie, C. 2018. Is overimitaon a uniquely human phenomenon? Insights from human
children as compared to Bonobos. Child Development 89:15351544.
hps://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12857
CULTURAL SCIENCE JOURNAL 15 (2023)
Cédric Sueur, Atsuyuki Ohshima, Marie Pe
11
Cluon-Brock, T.H.; Parker, G.A. 1995. Sexual coercion in animal sociees. Animal Behaviour
49:13451365
de Bruyn, P.N.; Tos h C .A.M;. Bester, M.N. 2008. Sexual harassment of a king penguin by an Antarcc
fur seal. Journal of Ethology 26:295297
De Petrillo, F.; Rosa, A.G. 2021. Variaon in primate decision-making under uncertainty and the
roots of human economic behaviour. Philosophical Transacons of the Royal Society B
376:20190671
Duboscq, J.; Romano, V.; MacIntosh, A.; Sueur, C. 2016. Social Informaon Transmission in Animals:
Lessons from Studies of Diffusion. Froners in Psychology 7:
Fraisl, D.; Hager, G.; Bedessem, B. et al. 2022. Cizen science in environmental and ecological
sciences. Nat Rev Methods Primers 2:120. hps://doi.org/10.1038/s43586-022-00144-4
Freedman, H.; Waltman, P. 1984. Persistence in models of three interacng predator-prey
populaons. Mathemacal Biosciences 68:213231
Garland, E.C.; Goldizen, A.W.; Rekdahl, M.L. et al. 2011. Dynamic horizontal cultural transmission of
humpback whale song at the ocean basin scale. Current Biology 21:687691.
hps://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2011.03.019
Goodall, J. 1986. Social rejecon, exclusion, and shunning among the Gombe chimpanzees. Ethology
and Sociobiology 7:227236
Gröning, J.; Hochkirch, A. 2008. Reproducve interference between animal species. The Quarterly
Review of Biology 83:257282
Gunst, N.; Vasey, P.L.; Leca, J-B. 2018. Deer mates: a quantave study of heterospecific sexual
behaviours performed by Japanese macaques toward sika deer. Archives of Sexual Behavior
47:847856
Haddad, W.A.; Reisinger, R.R.; Sco, T. et al. 2015. Mulple occurrences of king penguin
(Aptenodytes patagonicus) sexual harassment by Antarcc fur seals (Arctocephalus gazella). Polar
Biology 38:741746
Henrich, J. 2017. The secret of our success: how culture is driving human evoluon, domescang
our species, and making us smarter. Princeton University Press
Hoppi, W.; Laland, K.N. 2013. Social learning: an introducon to mechanisms, methods, and
models. Princeton University Press
Huffman, M.A.; Leca, J-B.; Nahallage, C.A. 2010. Cultured Japanese macaques: a muldisciplinary
approach to stone handling behaviour and its implicaons for the evoluon of behavioural
tradion in nonhuman primates. In: The Japanese Macaques, Nakagawa, N., Nakamichi, M.,
Sugiura, H. Springer, Tokyo, pp 191219
Kawai, M. 1965. Newly-acquired pre-cultural behaviour of the natural troop of Japanese monkeys
on Koshima Islet. Primates 6:130
Kyogoku, D. 2015. Reproducve interference: ecological and evoluonary consequences of
interspecific promiscuity. Populaon Ecology 57:253260
Laland, K.N.; Janik, V.M. 2006. The animal cultures debate. Trends in Ecology & Evoluon 21:542
547. hps://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2006.06.005
Leca, J-B.; Gunst, N.; Huffman, M.A. 2007. Japanese macaque cultures: inter-and intra-troop
behavioural variability of stone handling paerns across 10 troops. Behaviour 144:251281.
hps://doi.org/10.1163/156853907780425712
CULTURAL SCIENCE JOURNAL 15 (2023)
Cédric Sueur, Atsuyuki Ohshima, Marie Pe
12
Leca, J-B.; Gunst, N.; Vasey, P.L. 2015. Comparave Development of Heterosexual and Homosexual
Behaviors in Free-Ranging Female Japanese Macaques. Arch Sex Behav 44:12151231.
hps://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-014-0437-5
Matsuzawa, T. 2015. Sweet-potato washing revisited: 50th anniversary of the Primates arcle.
Primates 56:285287
Maei, T.A. 2014. Interdisciplinary benefits of a theory of cultural evoluon centered at the group-
level: The emergence of macro-neuroeconomics and social evoluonary game theory. Behavioral
and Brain Sciences 37:264265. hps://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X13002914
Nagell, K.; Olguin, R.S.; Tomasello, M. 1993. Processes of social learning in the tool use of
chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and human children (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparave
Psychology 107:174186. hps://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.107.2.174
Neadle, D.L.; Chappell, J.; Clay, Z.; Tennie, C. 2021. Even under majority influence, great apes fail to
copy novel acons
Nelson, X.J.; Fijn, N. 2013. The use of visual media as a tool for invesgang animal behaviour. Animal
Behaviour 85:525536. hps://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.12.009
Nishikawa, M.; Mochida, K. 2010. Coprophagy-related interspecific nocturnal interacons between
Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata yakui) and sika deer (Cervus nippon yakushimae). Primates
51:9599. hps://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-009-0182-x
Pelé, M.; Bonnefoy, A.; Shimada, M.; Sueur, C. 2017. Interspecies sexual behaviour between a male
Japanese macaque and female sika deer. Primates 58:14. hps://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-016-
0593-4
Prasad, T.R.; Rao, R.; Sanjay, M.; Sharma, R. 2013. Grooming of sambar (Rusa unicolor) by rhesus
macaque (Macaca mulaa) in Sariska Tiger Reserve, Rajasthan, India. CURRENT SCIENCE 104:296
Rutz, C.; Hunt, G.R.; Clair, J.J.H.S. 2018. Corvid technologies: How do New Caledonian crows get their
tool designs? Current Biology 28: R1109R1111. hps://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.08.031
Shuker, D.M.; Currie, N.; Hoole, T.; Burdfield-Steel, E.R. 2015. The extent and costs of reproducve
interference among four species of true bug. Populaon Ecology 57:321331
Silvertown, J. 2009. A new dawn for cizen science. Trends in Ecology & Evoluon 24:467471.
hps://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2009.03.017
Sprague, D.S. 1991. Mang by nontroop males among the Japanese macaques of Yakushima Island.
Folia Primatologica 57:156158
Sueur, C. 2022. Non-human animal cultures, co-cultures and conservaon. Cultural Science 14:93
102. hps://doi.org/10.2478/csj-2022-0013
Sueur, C.; Forin-Wiart, M-A.; Pelé, M. 2020 Do they really try to save their buddy?
Anthropomorphism about animal epimelec behaviours.
hps://doi.org/10.20944/preprints202008.0711.v1
Sueur, C.; Huffman, M.A. 2024. Co-cultures: exploring interspecies culture among humans and other
animals. Trends in Ecology & Evoluon. hps://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2024.05.011
Tennie, C.; Call, J., Tomasello, M. 2006. Push or pull: Imitaon vs. emulaon in great apes and human
hildren. Ethology 112:11591169. hps://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2006.01269.x
Thomsen, R.; Sols, J. 2004 Male masturbaon in free-ranging Japanese macaques. Internaonal
journal of primatology 25:10331041
Tomas e l l o, M.; Call, J. 1994. Social cognion of monkeys and apes. American Journal of Physical
Anthropology 37:273305. hps://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.1330370610
CULTURAL SCIENCE JOURNAL 15 (2023)
Cédric Sueur, Atsuyuki Ohshima, Marie Pe
13
Tsuji , Y.; Shimoda-Ishiguro, M.; Ohnishi N.; Takatsuki, S. 2007. A friend in need is a friend indeed:
feeding associaon between Japanese macaques and sika deer. Acta Theriol 52:427434.
hps://doi.org/10.1007/BF03194240
Van Schaik, C.P.; Isler, K.; Burkart, J.M. 2012. Explaining brain size variaon: from social to cultural
brain. Trends in Cognive Sciences 16:277284. hps://doi.org/10.1016/j.cs.2012.04.004
Vancatova, М. 2008 Creavity and innovave behaviour in primates on the example of picture-
making acvity of apes. Creavity and innovave behaviour in primates on the example of
picture-making acvity of apes
Vas ey, P.L. 2004. Sex differences in sexual partner acquision, retenon, and harassment during
female homosexual consortships in Japanese macaques. American Journal of Primatology: Official
Journal of the American Society of Primatologists 64:397409
Whiten, A. 2000. Primate culture and social learning. Cognive Science 24: 477508
Whiten, A.; Goodall, J.; McGrew, W.C. et al. 1999. Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature 399:682685
Whiten, A.; van Schaik, C.P. 2007. The evoluon of animal ‘cultures’ and social intelligence. Phil Trans
R Soc B 362:603620. hps://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2006.1998
Zhang, P.; Watanabe, K. 2007. Extra-large cluster formaon by Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata)
on Shodoshima Island, central Japan, and related factors. American Journal of Primatology
69:11191130. hps://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20419
Zhang, P.; Watanabe, K.; Eishi, T. 2007. Habitual hot-spring bathing by a group of Japanese macaques
(Macaca fuscata) in their natural habitat. American journal of primatology 69:14251430
Author informaon
Cédric Sueur is Full Professor at the University of Strasbourg. He is mainly working on animal
behaviour and specifically on social networking, culture and decision-making in animal groups
at the Institut Pluridisciplinaire Hubert Curien. He is also member of the Institut Universitaire
de France (http://www.iufrance.fr).
Atsuyuki Oshima is a professional wildlife and nature photographer based in Kyoto, Japan.
Renowned for capturing the essence of wildlife, particularly monkeys, his stunning work
showcases nature's beauty. He was awarded Wildlife Photographer of the Year in 2023.
Marie Pelé is a researcher at the Anthropo-Lab, ETHICS Laboratory of the Catholic University of
Lille, specializing in animal and human ethology. Her work focuses on animal cognition and
human-animal relationships, exploring societal perspectives on the management of individuals
and species conservation. By integrating human disciplines such as ethics, sociology, and
psychology with animal-centered fields like ethology, ecology, and biology, she develops an
approach known as "ethological engineering," which combines observation and
experimentation to foster a sustainable human-animal relationship.
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any citations for this publication.
Article
Full-text available
Animal culture involves the transmission of behaviours through social learning, which plays a crucial role in species' survival and adaptability to changing environments. Co-cultures, which describe the mutual cultural evolution between species, underscore the importance of interspecies interactions in maintaining ecological balance and resilience. The loss of these cultural practices can lead to a decline in population viability, reduced genetic diversity, and destabilised ecosystems, ultimately impacting the services these ecosystems provide to human health and well-being. The preservation of cultural traits is essential for maintaining biodiversity, ensuring species' adaptive capacities, and supporting the overall health of ecosystems, which are critical for the survival of both non-human animals and humans. Integrating the conservation of animal cultures and co-cultures into broader conservation and One Health strategies is necessary to protect the delicate balance of life on Earth and ensure the well-being of all species.
Article
Full-text available
Discussions of how animal culture can aid the conservation crisis are burgeoning. As scientists and conservationists working to protect endangered species, we call for reflection on how the culture concept may be applied in practice. Here, we discuss both the potential benefits and potential shortcomings of applying the animal culture concept, and propose a set of achievable milestones that will help guide and ensure its effective integration existing conservation frameworks, such as Adaptive Management cycles or Open Standards.
Article
Full-text available
Uncertainty is a ubiquitous component of human economic behaviour, yet people can vary in their preferences for risk across populations, individuals and different points in time. As uncertainty also characterizes many aspects of animal decision-making, comparative research can help evaluate different potential mechanisms that generate this variation, including the role of biological differences or maturational change versus cultural learning, as well as identify human-unique components of economic decision-making. Here, we examine decision-making under risk across non-human primates, our closest relatives. We first review theoretical approaches and current methods for understanding decision-making in animals. We then assess the current evidence for variation in animal preferences between species and populations, between individuals based on personality, sex and age, and finally, between different contexts and individual states. We then use these primate data to evaluate the processes that can shape human decision-making strategies and identify the primate foundations of human economic behaviour. This article is part of the theme issue ‘Existence and prevalence of economic behaviours among non-human primates’.
Article
Full-text available
Recent research shows that New Caledonian crows can incorporate information from researcher-made objects into objects they subsequently manufacture. This 'mental template matching' is one of several possible - mutually compatible - mechanisms for the cultural transmission of tool designs among wild crows.
Book
Confronting Animal Abuse presents a powerful examination of the human-animal relationship and the laws designed to protect it. Piers Beirne, a leading scholar in the growing field of green criminology, explores the heated topic of animal abuse in agriculture, science, and sport, as well as what is known, if anything, about the potential for animal assault to lead to inter-human violence. He convincingly shows how from its roots in the Irish plow-fields of 1635 through today, animal-rights legislation has been primarily shaped by human interest and why we must reconsider the terms of human-animal relationships. Beirne argues that if violations of animals' rights are to be taken seriously, then scholars and activists should examine why some harms to animals are defined as criminal, others as abusive but not criminal and still others as neither criminal nor abusive. Confronting Animal Abuse points to the need for a more inclusive concept of harms to animals, without which the meaning of animal abuse will be overwhelmingly confined to those harms that are regarded as socially unacceptable, one-on-one cases of animal cruelty. Certainly, those cases demand attention. But so, too, do those other and far more numerous institutionalized harms to animals, where abuse is routine, invisible, ubiquitous and often defined as socially acceptable. In this pioneering, pro-animal book Beirne identifies flaws in our traditional understanding of human-animal relationships, and proposes a compelling new approach.
Article
Citizen science is an increasingly acknowledged approach applied in many scientific domains, and particularly within the environmental and ecological sciences, in which non-professional participants contribute to data collection to advance scientific research. We present contributory citizen science as a valuable method to scientists and practitioners within the environmental and ecological sciences, focusing on the full life cycle of citizen science practice, from design to implementation, evaluation and data management. We highlight key issues in citizen science and how to address them, such as participant engagement and retention, data quality assurance and bias correction, as well as ethical considerations regarding data sharing. We also provide a range of examples to illustrate the diversity of applications, from biodiversity research and land cover assessment to forest health monitoring and marine pollution. The aspects of reproducibility and data sharing are considered, placing citizen science within an encompassing open science perspective. Finally, we discuss its limitations and challenges and present an outlook for the application of citizen science in multiple science domains. Contributory citizen science is a method in which non-professional participants contribute to data collection in whole or in part to advance scientific research. This Primer outlines the use of citizen science in the environmental and ecological sciences, discussing participant engagement, data quality assurance and bias correction.
Book
Many animals, including humans, acquire valuable skills and knowledge by copying others. Scientists refer to this as social learning. It is one of the most exciting and rapidly developing areas of behavioral research and sits at the interface of many academic disciplines, including biology, experimental psychology, economics, and cognitive neuroscience. This book provides a comprehensive, practical guide to the research methods of this important emerging field. It defines the mechanisms thought to underlie social learning and demonstrate how to distinguish them experimentally in the laboratory. It presents techniques for detecting and quantifying social learning in nature, including statistical modeling of the spatial distribution of behavior traits. It also describes the latest theory and empirical findings on social learning strategies, and introduces readers to mathematical methods and models used in the study of cultural evolution. This book is an indispensable tool for researchers and an essential primer for students.
Preprint
In this study, we asked participants to answer an online questionnaire about videos showing animal epimeletic behaviours: an individual (a sparrow, an elephant and a macaque) displayed behaviours towards an inanimate conspecific who suddenly got back to conscious at the end of the footage. A fourth video showed a dog-robot kicked by an engineer to demonstrate its stability. After each video, questions were asked to score the degree of anthropomorphism of participants, from mentophobia (no attribution whatever the species) to full anthropomorphism and to measure how close participants are to biological reality (actual scientific knowledge). A first important result is that there is a negative correlation (about 61%) between the anthropomorphism score (AS) and the biological reality one (BRS) showing a wrong statement. The heterogeneity of responses proved that all levels of anthropomorphism are covered from mentaphobia to full anthropomorphism. However, the scores participants attributed to animals differ according to the species shown in the video and to human characteristics. Understanding how one can play with these factors can conduct to better relationships with animals as encourage human-robot interactions. Finally, such reflective anthropomorphism can lead to an increase of human empathy and sociality, finally increasing our humanity.