Available via license: CC BY 4.0
Content may be subject to copyright.
Vol.:(0123456789)
Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics
https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-024-02974-8
Disentangling decision errors fromaction execution inmouse‑tracking
studies: The case ofeffect‑based action control
SolveigTonn1 · MoritzSchaaf1 · WilfriedKunde2 · RolandPster1
Accepted: 9 October 2024
© The Author(s) 2024
Abstract
Mouse-tracking is regarded as a powerful technique to investigate latent cognitive and emotional states. However, drawing
inferences from this manifold data source carries the risk of several pitfalls, especially when using aggregated data rather
than single-trial trajectories. Researchers might reach wrong conclusions because averages lump together two distinct contri-
butions that speak towards fundamentally different mechanisms underlying between-condition differences: influences from
online-processing during action execution and influences from incomplete decision processes. Here, we propose a simple
method to assess these factors, thus allowing us to probe whether process-pure interpretations are appropriate. By applying
this method to data from 12 published experiments on ideomotor action control, we show that the interpretation of previous
results changes when dissociating online processing from decision and initiation errors. Researchers using mouse-tracking
to investigate cognition and emotion are therefore well advised to conduct detailed trial-by-trial analyses, particularly when
they test for direct leakage of ongoing processing into movement trajectories.
Keywords Mouse-tracking· Response-effect compatibility· Ideomotor framework· Action execution· Initial decision
errors
Introduction
Motor actions comprise more than a sequence of movements.
They are driven by certain intentions and involve manifold
cognitive and emotional processes. As recent observations
suggest that the intention behind an action heavily influ-
ences the kinematics of the ensuing movement, the analysis
of unfolding motions is regarded as a powerful tool to shed
light on cognitive and emotional processes (Ansuini etal.,
2014; Georgiou etal., 2007; Sartori etal., 2011; Song &
Nakayama, 2009; Stillman etal., 2018). This potential has
attracted the attention of behavioral scientists, especially in
cognitive psychology, a field with a long history of employ-
ing reaction-time setups to test for the speed of processing.
While conventional, chronometric setups yield essentially
one data point per correct response, movement trajectories
are able to provide multiple data points for every movement,
substantially increasing the amount of information that can
be analyzed (Bundt etal., 2018; Fischer & Hartmann, 2014;
Maldonado etal., 2019; Zgonnikov etal., 2017).
One particular prominent way to capture movement tra-
jectories is the simple and elegant means of logging the coor-
dinates of a mouse cursor across time (Freeman & Ambady,
2009; McKinstry etal., 2008). Mouse-tracking has generated
valuable insights in numerous fields, including social cat-
egorization (Dale etal., 2007; Hehman etal., 2014; Lazerus
etal., 2016; Stolier & Freeman, 2017), self-control in deci-
sion-making (Buttlar & Walther, 2019; O’Hora etal., 2016;
Stillman etal., 2017, 2018), and semantic processing (Dale
& Duran, 2011; Spivey etal., 2005; Wirth etal., 2019). Fur-
ther, it has been employed to investigate motivational topics
like approach and avoidance tendencies (Boschet etal., 2022;
Dignath etal., 2014, 2020; Wirth etal., 2016), rule-breaking
(Jusyte etal., 2017; Pfister etal., 2016; Wirth etal., 2018),
and cognitive conflict (Boschet etal., 2022; Erb etal., 2016;
Mittelstädt etal., 2023; Quétard et al., 2023; Scherbaum
etal., 2010).
Despite its widespread use, comprehensive standards for
experimental design and especially data analysis have not
* Solveig Tonn
tonn@uni-trier.de
1 Department ofPsychology, University ofTrier,
Johanniterufer 15, 54290Trier, Germany
2 Department ofPsychology, University ofWürzburg,
Würzburg, Germany
Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics
yet been established. Mouse-tracking experiments differ,
for example, regarding their starting procedure (dynamic
vs. static), response mode (terminating a response by click-
ing vs. reaching a target), or cursor speed (Kieslich etal.,
2018; Scherbaum & Kieslich, 2018). In fact, recent obser-
vations suggest that these seemingly slight differences in
the experimental procedure can lead to puzzling differences
in observed results and corresponding conclusions (Grage
etal., 2019; Kieslich etal., 2020; Schoemann etal., 2019,
2021; Wirth etal., 2020).
The only aspect of mouse-tracking analyses that enjoys
widespread consensus is the use of aggregated means instead
of individual trajectories to plot experimental results (Buttlar
& Walther, 2019; Dieciuc etal., 2019; Pfister etal., 2016;
Stillman etal., 2018; Ye & Damian, 2022). Ironically, this
consensus comes with major limitations as it neglects that
one and the same average trajectory can derive from highly
different trajectories on a by-participant or by-trial level
(e.g., Matejka & Fitzmaurice, 2017).
Single movements versusaggregate statistics
The observation that identical trajectory averages can result
from profoundly different individual movements calls for
methods to evaluate different trajectory types. Such meth-
ods include the classification via cluster-analytical methods
(Wulff etal., 2018) or graphical approaches such as heat-
maps of individual movements (Garcia-Guerrero etal.,
2022; Scherbaum etal., 2010; Vogel etal., 2018). These
highly sophisticated methods have not been adopted widely,
however.
Thus, we suggest an easy-to-implement method that aims
to reach a similar goal. Our approach provides a straight-
forward way to assess the particularly relevant distinction
of smooth, single-step movements versus multi-step move-
ments in which an initial movement is re-evaluated and
then revised along the movement trajectory. Single-step
and multi-step movements clearly involve different cognitive
processes and, therefore, this distinction must be considered
when interpreting the results of an experiment (see van der
Wel etal., 2009 and Spivey etal., 2010, for a conceptually
related discussion of discrete vs. continuous influences on
movement trajectories in a lexical decision task).
A simple criterion
Multi-step movements often come in the shape of initial
movements in the wrong direction that are corrected by
pausing and changing direction along the way. Such move-
ments reflect errors in the initial decision of where to move,
and should therefore not be considered when movement exe-
cution (rather than decision making) is targeted by an experi-
ment. Although statistical clustering (Wulff etal., 2018) is
the currently most elegant and sophisticated method to dis-
tinguish different types of movements, it may not always be
ideal for two reasons. First, it does not take the experimen-
tal design into account. This includes the display geometry
in terms of common home and target areas that define a
movement’s start and endpoint. Second, current clustering
algorithms are inherently spatial, whereas multi-step move-
ments may also come as stop-and-go movements with breaks
or decelerations along the trajectory (Dale & Duran, 2011;
Fishbach etal., 2005, 2007; Kieffaber etal., 2023).
We therefore suggest combining a simple spatial criterion
relating to initial decision errors with a validation of this
criterion in terms of a movement’s corresponding velocity.
As a spatial cutoff, we suggest using a simple vertical cutoff
line based on the display geometry of a given study. This
method allows to easily exclude movements with wrong ini-
tial decisions. Doing so focuses the analyses on trials with
completed decisions before movement start. As a spatio-
temporal method, we suggest using velocity plots to assess
whether trajectories on different sides of the spatial cutoff
do indeed come with different attributes.1
A topical example: Effect‑based action control
To demonstrate the combined application of both strategies
and to document the importance of distinguishing between
smooth single-step movements versus multi-step move-
ments, we re-evaluated a set of findings from a particular
experimental design – the response-effect compatibility
paradigm (Kunde, 2001; Pfister etal., 2014). We chose this
specific paradigm for two reasons: First, we prefer to criti-
cize (and potentially deconstruct) our own previous work
rather than the work of others. Second, and more impor-
tantly, this area of research has its historical origin in theo-
ries of motor control (Harleß, 1862; Herbart, 1825; James,
1890; for historical comments, see Pfister & Janczyk, 2012;
Stock & Stock, 2004), not in theories of decision making. As
a result, investigators utilized mouse-tracking to derive con-
clusions about action execution rather than decision mak-
ing. This aim is, of course, not applicable to all research
using mouse-tracking. As we outline in the Discussion, other
research domains are particularly interested in movements
with non-completed decisions upon movement start (e.g.,
Boschet etal., 2022; Dale & Duran, 2011; Dshemuchadse
etal., 2013). Instead of disregarding these movements, such
studies might intentionally utilize experimental designs
evoking movements with incomplete decisions. If, however,
1 Another simple spatial cutoff is related to the measure of x-flips,
i.e., the number of directional changes along the x-axis (Dale &
Duran, 2011; Duran etal., 2010). Movements with many x-flips are
commonly multi-step movements.
Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics
the underlying question is whether effect anticipations influ-
ence responses even beyond the categorical selection of an
action goal (Pfister etal., 2014), excluding influences from
(partial) errors is pivotal.
Studies using the response-effect compatibility design
typically assess the content of action representations by
coupling actions with effects that share or oppose charac-
teristics of the action. In other words, these experiments
introduce feature overlap (dimensional overlap; Kornblum
etal., 1990) between responses (i.e., body movements) and
response-contingent perceptual effects (e.g., in the agent’s
environment). In the case of mouse-tracking, this common
feature is usually manipulated through a spatial left-versus-
right arrangement of movement targets and the visual effects
that are triggered by reaching a target (Fig.1; see Pfister
etal., 2014; Schonard etal., 2021). These effects can be
compatible (i.e., when a movement to the right evokes a
visual effect on the right-hand side) or they can be incom-
patible (i.e., when a movement to the right evokes a visual
effect on the hand side). Crucially, previous studies observed
ongoing movements to be attracted towards the movement-
contingent effect. Put differently, if the completion of move-
ments triggered an effect on the other side of the screen,
movements deviated more to that side than when they trig-
gered an effect on the same side. This result was interpreted
as evidence suggesting that the visual effect was anticipated
during motor execution and, thus, plays a pivotal role for
the control of efferent activity. That is, these studies specifi-
cally sought to investigate influences on action execution,
and regarded spatial aspects of the trajectory as “postselec-
tion measures” (Hommel etal., 2017, p.825).
In-depth analyses of a recent experiment, however, chal-
lenged this interpretation by showing that such compatibility
effects may derive from a subset of movements with initial
decision errors (Tonn etal., 2023). In fact, excluding these
movements made the response-effect compatibility effect
disappear in spatial measures. Therefore, re-evaluating pre-
vious evidence (and previous interpretations) from this par-
ticular paradigm is ideal as a first estimate of how prevalent
and pervasive the influence of multi-step movements is.
Method
All mouse-tracking experiments that were included in the
present re-analyses investigated the influence of action
effects on action execution (Hommel etal., 2017; Pfister
etal., 2014; Schonard etal., 2021; Tonn etal., 2023; Wirth
etal., 2015).
Experimental design
The common denominator of all analyzed studies is that they
used a setup with five relevant areas, as shown in Fig.1:
Fig. 1 Experimental design and previously observed data pattern. (A)
Movements began in a starting area in the lower middle of the screen
and ended in one of two possible final movement locations. Depend-
ing on the response-effect compatibility mapping, action effects either
occurred on the same side as the final movement location (dotted cir-
cle) or on the respective other side (solid circle). (B) The previously
observed data pattern: Movements were biased towards the antici-
pated effect location, resulting in a more curved movement trajectory
with incompatible effects (solid trajectory) compared to compatible
effects (dotted trajectory)
Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics
A home area at the bottom center, two target areas in the
upper left and right corner, and two effect locations directly
above the target areas. Before the start of each trial, partici-
pants were informed about the current mapping of their own
responses to the ensuing effects (compatible vs. incompat-
ible) through visual cues in the target areas. In other words,
compatibility was varied trial-wise, and participants could
prepare for the upcoming response-effect relation. After
signaling that they were ready for the current trial by mov-
ing to the home area, an imperative stimulus instructed par-
ticipants to produce an effect at either the left or the right
location. This required a movement to the target area directly
below the desired effect location in the compatible condi-
tion whereas it required a movement to the respective other
target area in the incompatible condition. While participants
were instructed to execute the task as quickly and accurately
as possible, no explicit movement initiation deadline was
implemented. The underlying rationale for this design deci-
sion was to ensure that participants started the movement
only after completing their decision. Consequently, to isolate
influences on movement execution, the analyzed trajecto-
ries were truncated to the part between leaving the starting
area and reaching the target area. All experiments assessed
how anticipating an action effect shapes action execution
(see Fig.1B), that is, whether movements are systematically
biased towards the location of their ensuing effect. With
effect sizes ranging from dz = 0.38 to dz = 1.38, all experi-
ments consistently showed that incompatible movements
were more curved than compatible movements. However, we
argue here that these effects were mainly driven by incom-
plete decisions in a fraction of the trials.
Analyses
Basic approach
For analyzing mouse-movement trajectories, we used the R
package mousetRajectory (Pfister etal., 2024). We extracted
initiation time (IT), movement time (MT), area under the
curve (AUC), and maximum absolute deviation (MAD) from
the individual trajectories. IT was measured as the time from
the onset of the imperative stimulus until the cursor left the
starting area. MT was measured from this point in time until
the cursor arrived at the target area. AUCs were computed
as the (signed) area between the executed and the optimal
path (straight line through start and end coordinates), and
MADs were computed as the (signed) maximum orthogo-
nal deviation of the executed path from the optimal path.
Deviations towards the opposite target area were counted
as positive, whereas deviations in the other direction were
counted as negative. All movements were flipped to the
right, the coordinates of each trial were time-normalized
and re-sampledwith linear interpolation before computing
AUCs and MADs, and the resulting normalized trajectories
were used for plotting.
For our re-analyses, we used a consistent approach across
studies in terms of preprocessing and outlier correction,
which naturally leads to minor differences between the re-
analyses and the original results: For all analyses, we omit-
ted trials with downward movements of the mouse, trials
with errors, and outlier trials. Outliers were defined as trials
where any of the four measures deviated more than 2.5 SDs
from the corresponding cell mean, computed separately for
each participant and condition. For brevity, we report only
the effect of compatibility, and omit the influences of all
other experimental factors (i.e., we report F-statistics for
the main effect of compatibility in multifactorial designs and
t-statistics in unifactorial designs). Full data sets and analy-
sis scripts are available via the Open Science Framework at:
https:// osf. io/ hrpk6.
Trial‑level analyses
All studies reported a consistent impact of upcoming action
effects on movement trajectories which had previously been
taken to suggest an important role of action effect represen-
tations for motor control (Hommel etal., 2017; Pfister etal.,
2014; Schonard etal., 2021; Wirth etal., 2015). The sug-
gested spatial criterion for detecting multi-step movements
(including initial decision errors) provides a simple and
elegant tool to assess the truth value of this interpretation.
Therefore, we separated all movements previously clas-
sified as correct (i.e., movements ending on the correct
target area) into two groups: Movements directly starting
towards the correct target and movements first starting to
the wrong side and later changing the course of movement.
This excludes movements with initially wrong decisions and
puts increased focus on data points with completed decisions
upon movement start. The question was whether previously
observed effects persist when only taking these latter move-
ments into account. Our classification was implemented by
excluding all movements with x-values going below the low-
est x-value of the starting area, resulting in a vertical cutoff
line touching the starting area on the left (see Fig.2). This
criterion classified more incompatible than compatible trials
as multi-step movements, which is consistent with response-
time studies reporting more commission errors with incom-
patible action-effect mappings than with compatible map-
pings (e.g., Kunde, 2001).
Using one representative experiment, Fig.3 demonstrates
the impact of this cutoff by showing how average trajecto-
ries – as commonly used to visualize experimental results
– change when multi-step movements are excluded.
Second, we show for each experiment how inferential
statistics change, that is, how the effect evolves, when the
criterion is used and then relaxed from its original location
Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics
(i.e., on the left side of the starting area) up to the middle of
the wrong target area. In other words, we plot the standard-
ized effect size of the compatibility effect in the AUC as a
function of this cutoff criterion.
As a further check for our spatial criterion, we pro-
vide corresponding velocity information for the two
different movement types for every experiment via veloc-
ity profile plots. To account for variability in the timing
of decision changes, we identified the point of maximum
absolute deviation within each trajectory and time-nor-
malized velocities from the start of the movement up to
this timepoint as well as from this timepoint to the end of
Fig. 2 Visualization of the cutoff-criterion. Data from three example
participants. (A) Average trajectories over all trajectories (dashed,
dark gray lines), over trajectories excluded by the cutoff criterion
(solid, light gray lines), and over trajectories surviving the cutoff cri-
terion (solid, black lines). (B) Individual movements going into the
averages trajectories displayed in panel A. The vertical line visualizes
the cutoff criterion
Fig. 3 Average trajectories for one exemplary experiment (Pfister
etal., 2014, Exp. 2). Average trajectories when using all movements
(left) and when excluding multi-step movements (right). Although
participant averages (thin lines) are smooth and do not cross our
exclusion criterion, the response-effect compatibility effect in spatial
measures completely vanishes when multi-step movement trials are
excluded: Compatible (blue) and incompatible (red) average trajecto-
ries overlap for single-step movements
Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics
the movement (see Fig.4 for an explanation of this novel
procedure).
Results
Table1 provides a summary over all re-analyzed experi-
ments, highlighting how the results change when multi-step
movements are excluded with the proposed cutoff criterion.
Due to consistency in the pattern of results across all experi-
ments, we textually describe only one re-analysis in detail
in the main text. Detailed descriptions and visualizations
of the other experiments are available in the Appendix. We
focus on the Experiment 2 of Pfister etal. (2014) because
it served as the design template for all subsequent studies
investigating response-effect compatibility effects using
mouse-tracking.
Using all available data, a compatibility effect was
observed for all four measures in Experiment 2 of Pfis-
ter etal. (2014). Compatible actions had smaller AUCs
(-0.1 × 103 vs. 2.7 × 103 px2), t(19) = 3.59, p = .002, dz = 0.80,
smaller MADs (0.8 vs. 15.6 px), t(19) = 3.94, p = .001,
dz = 0.88, shorter ITs (639 vs. 693 ms), t(19) = 5.81, p < .001,
dz = 1.30, and shorter MTs (421 vs. 457 ms), t(19) = 3.12,
p = .006, dz = 0.70, than incompatible actions.
As expected, our cutoff criterion excluded fewer com-
patible than incompatible movements (9.8% vs. 17.5%),
t(19) = 4.04, p = .001, dz = 0.90. When applying this cutoff
criterion, the compatibility effect vanished in the spatial
measures of AUC, |t| < 1, and MAD, |t|< 1. In the timing
measures, the test for the ITs remained significant, with
compatible actions being initiated faster than incompatible
actions (634 vs. 706 ms), t(19) = 6.31, p < .001, dz = 1.41,
whereas the effect in MTs vanished, |t|< 1.
Panel A of Fig.5 shows that the compatibility effect
remains non-significant even when our criterion is relaxed to
permit movements to travel up to 40% of the horizontal dis-
tance towards the incorrect target area. Panel B of Fig.5 illus-
trates that this simple cutoff criterion successfully classifies the
movements into two distinct subgroups with markedly differ-
ent velocity profiles: Single-step movements display a smooth
velocity around the time of reaching MAD, whereas multi-step
movements display a deceleration that is followed by a sub-
stantial acceleration around the time of reaching MAD.
Discussion
This paper had two different aims. The first aim was to sug-
gest an easy-to-implement method that separates two groups
of mouse trajectories with different underlying processes.
The second aim was to apply this method to published
experiments within one exemplary field and to examine how
the interpretation of previous data is affected by these dif-
ferent groups of trials.
To distinguish between smooth single-step movements
and multi-step movements in which an initial, incomplete
decision is revised along the movement trajectory, a verti-
cal cutoff criterion was implemented. This spatial criterion
is straightforward to implement and effectively separates
both movement types, as evidenced by the velocity profile
plots: While single-step movements exhibited rather smooth
velocity profiles with high speeds at the point of maximum
deviation, multi-step movements exhibited a pronounced
deceleration before and rapid acceleration after the point of
maximum deviation (Vogel etal., 2018). Thus, the binary
classification (multistep: yes or no?) provided by our simple
criterion suffices to illustrate the presence of (at least) two
Fig. 4 Explanation of the velocity profile plots. For all (time-nor-
malized) trajectories, the timepoint of maximal orthogonal deviation
(MAD) from the ideal line is computed (left). Trajectories are then
separated into pre- and post-MAD parts (middle). The resulting sub-
trajectories are time-normalized again before velocities can be aver-
aged (right). The solid and dashed lines illustrate two exemplary,
individual trajectories. Note that even if two movements exhibit their
MAD at a similar point on the ideal line (left), the timepoint of the
MAD might vary substantially (middle), necessitating a per-move-
ment, temporal alignment of the velocities (right)
Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics
Table 1 Overview of the re-analyzed experiments
Exp. Goal of the experiment Experimental
manipulations
Number of
trialsa
DV REC effect without application of the cutoff
criterionb
REC effect with application of the cutoff
criterionb
Pfister etal. (2014)
Exp. 1 First experiment that investigated REC
with mouse-tracking
REC 224 AUC t(19) = 2.75, p = .013, dz = 0.61 (15.8 × 103
vs. 16.9 × 103 px2)
t(19) = 1.89, p = .073, dz = 0.42
MAD t(19) = 2.58, p = .018, dz = 0.58 (64.1 vs.
68.1 px)
t(19) = 1.17, p = .258, dz = 0.26
IT t(19) = 2.33, p = .031, dz = 0.52 (652 vs.
672 ms)
t(19) = 2.17, p = .043, dz = 0.49 (654 vs. 672 ms)
MT t(19) = 3.17, p = .005, dz = 0.71 (590 vs.
628 ms)
t(19) = 2.77, p = .012, dz = 0.62 (578 vs. 598 ms)
%CC t(19) = 3.19, p = .005, dz = 0.71 (5.9% vs.
10.3%)
Exp. 2 In contrast to Exp. 1, participants did not
have to start their movement upwards
and thus, were able to take the direct
path
REC 224 AUC t(19) = 3.59, p = .002, dz = 0.80 (-0.1 × 103
vs. 2.7 × 103 px2)
|t|< 1
MAD t(19) = 3.94, p = .001, dz = 0.88 (0.8 vs.
15.6 px)
|t|< 1
IT t(19) = 5.81, p < .001, dz = 1.30 (639 vs.
693 ms)
t(19) = 6.31, p < .001, dz = 1.41 (634 vs. 706 ms)
MT t(19) = 3.12, p = .006, dz = 0.70 (421 vs.
457 ms)
|t|< 1
%CC t(19) = 4.04, p = .001, dz = 0.90 (9.8% vs.
17.5%)
Wirth etal. (2015)
Exp. 1 Where is the locus of the mouse-tracking
REC effect?
Dual tasking, “locus of slack” logic
Task 1: pitch discrimination
Task 2: REC with mouse-tracking
REC × stimulus
onset asynchrony
348 AUC F(1, 15) = 7.63, p = .015, ηp
2 = .34 (0.7 × 103
vs. 2.8 × 103 px2)
F < 1
MAD F(1, 15) = 8.19, p = .012, ηp
2 = .35 (4.2 vs.
15.0 px)
F < 1
IT F(1, 15) = 26.50, p < .001, ηp
2 = .64 (880 vs.
950 ms)
F(1, 15) = 27.17, p < .001, ηp
2 = .64 (879 vs.
950 ms)
MT F(1, 15) = 13.58, p = .002, ηp
2 = .48 (375 vs.
399 ms)
F(1, 15) = 1.70, p = .212, ηp
2 = .10
%CC t(15) = 4.30, p = .001, dz = 1.08 (9.4% vs.
17.1%)
Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics
Table 1 (continued)
Exp. Goal of the experiment Experimental
manipulations
Number of
trialsa
DV REC effect without application of the cutoff
criterionb
REC effect with application of the cutoff
criterionb
Exp. 2 Dual tasking, “effect propagation” logic:
Task 1: REC with mouse-tracking
Task 2: pitch discrimination
REC × stimulus
onset asynchrony
348 AUC F(1, 15) = 4.53, p = .050, ηp
2 = .23 F < 1
MAD F(1, 15) = 6.40, p = .023, ηp
2 = .30 (22.8 vs.
27.9 px)
F(1, 15) = 1.00, p = .334, ηp
2 = .06
IT F(1, 15) = 3.55, p = .079, ηp
2 = .19 F(1, 15) = 3.87, p = .068, ηp
2 = .21
MT F(1, 15) = 9.78, p = .007, ηp
2 = .39 (1015 vs.
1091 ms)
F(1, 15) = 7.66, p = .014, ηp
2 = .34 (988 vs. 1053
ms)
%CC t(15) = 2.26, p = .039, dz = 0.56 (13.0% vs.
16.6%)
Hommel etal. (2017)
Exp. 1 Do REC effects stem from sensory or
affective compatibility? Is this differen-
tially affected by whether actions are free
vs. forced choice?
sensory
RECc × affective
RECc × free vs.
forced choice
240 AUC F(1, 34) = 10.41, p = .003, ηp
2 = .23
(3.2 × 103 vs. 5.3 × 103 px2)
F(1, 34) = 5.45, p = .026, ηp
2 = .14 (-0.7 × 103
vs. 0.3 × 103 px2)
MAD F(1, 34) = 9.77, p = .004, ηp
2 = .22 (17.7 vs.
27.6 px)
F(1, 34) = 4.76, p = .036, ηp
2 = .12 (-2.4 vs. 1.5
px)
IT F(1, 34) = 7.19, p = .011, ηp
2 = .17 (496 vs.
507 ms)
F(1, 34) = 5.38, p = .027, ηp
2 = .14 (497 vs. 507
ms)
MT F(1, 34) = 5.69, p = .023, ηp
2 = .14 (339 vs.
347 ms)
F(1, 34) = 5.24, p = .028, ηp
2 = .13 (317 vs. 324
ms)
%CC t(34) = 4.82, p < .001, dz = 0.81 (16.5% vs.
22.3%)
Schonard etal. (2021)
Exp. 1 Can mouse-tracking REC effects be repli-
cated in a simplified setting?
REC × free vs.
forced choice
240 AUC F(1, 19) = 23.22, p < .001, ηp
2 = .55
(22.7 × 103 vs. 33.7 × 103 px2)
F(1, 19) = 3.05, p = .097, ηp
2 = .14
MAD F(1, 19) = 23.00, p < .001, ηp
2 = .55 (32.4
vs. 51.7 px)
F(1, 19) = 3.02, p = .098, ηp
2 = .14
IT F(1, 19) = 3.77, p = .067, ηp
2 = .17 F(1, 19) = 3.50, p = .077, ηp
2 = .16
MT F(1, 19) = 30.65, p < .001, ηp
2 = .62 (544 vs.
572 ms)
F(1, 19) = 6.43, p = .020, ηp
2 = .25 (535 vs. 552
ms)
%CC t(19) = 5.52, p < .001, dz = 1.23 (7.1% vs.
16.1%)
Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics
Table 1 (continued)
Exp. Goal of the experiment Experimental
manipulations
Number of
trialsa
DV REC effect without application of the cutoff
criterionb
REC effect with application of the cutoff
criterionb
Exp. 2 Is the mouse-tracking REC effect subject
to sequential modulation?
REC × previous
RECd
240 AUC t(39) = 4.37, p < .001, dz = 0.69 (63.2 × 103
vs. 72.7 × 103 px2)
t(39) = 1.02, p = .312, dz = 0.16
MAD t(39) = 4.22, p < .001, dz = 0.67 (92.5 vs.
108.1 px)
|t|< 1
IT t(39) = 5.15, p < .001, dz = 0.81 (602 vs.
645 ms)
t(39) = 5.80, p < .001, dz = 0.92 (604 vs. 658 ms)
MT t(39) = 4.75, p < .001, dz = 0.75 (633 vs.
661 ms)
t(39) = 3.14, p = .003, dz = 0.50 (613 vs. 628 ms)
%CC t(39) = 5.11, p < .001, dz = 0.81 (11.1% vs.
17.4%)
Exp. 3 Excludes dimensional overlap between
stimuli and effects
REC × free vs.
forced choice
240 AUC F(1, 39) = 8.89, p = .005, ηp
2 = .19
(13.8 × 103 vs. 19.3 × 103 px2)
F(1, 39) = 5.42, p = .025, ηp
2 =.12(7.2 × 103 vs.
9.7 × 103 px2)
MAD F(1, 39) = 8.63, p = .006, ηp
2 = .18 (20.7 vs.
30.3 px)
F(1, 39) = 4.94, p = .032, ηp
2 = .11 (9.5 vs. 13.0
px)
IT F(1, 39) = 10.66, p = .002, ηp
2 = .21 (633 vs.
653 ms)
F(1, 39) = 11.69, p = .001, ηp
2 = .23 (634 vs.
657 ms)
MT F(1, 39) = 2.90, p = .097, ηp
2 = .07 F < 1
%CC t(39) = 2.78, p = .008, dz = 0.44 (8.6% vs.
11.8%)
Tonn etal. (2023)
Exp. 1 Can REC effects be observed for actions
that prevent (instead of produce) sensory
effects? Typical actions that produce sen-
sory effects serve as baseline condition
REC × effect-
preventing vs.
effect-producing
actions
312 AUC F(1, 42) = 13.98, p = .001, ηp
2 = .25
(3.5 × 103 vs. 6.2 × 103 px2)
F < 1
MAD F(1, 42) = 17.48, p < .001, ηp
2 = .29 (16.5
vs. 30.6 px)
F < 1
IT F(1, 42) = 34.62, p < .001, ηp
2 = .45 (632 vs.
673 ms)
F(1, 42) = 48.99, p < .001, ηp
2 = .54 (634 vs.
683 ms)
MT F(1, 42) = 47.93, p < .001, ηp
2 = .53 (506 vs.
562 ms)
F(1, 42) = 30.12, p < .001, ηp
2 = .42 (481 vs.
524 ms)
%CC t(42) = 5.01, p < .001, dz = 0.76 (13.7% vs.
21.2%)
Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics
Exp. Goal of the experiment Experimental
manipulations
Number of
trialsa
DV REC effect without application of the cutoff
criterionb
REC effect with application of the cutoff
criterionb
Exp. S1 In contrast to Exp. 1, to-be-produced and
to-be-prevented effects were no longer
associated with monetary gains/losses
REC × effect-
preventing vs.
effect-producing
actions
312 AUC F(1, 40) = 14.91, p < .001, ηp
2 = .27
(3.7 × 103 vs. 6.5 × 103 px2)
F < 1
MAD F(1, 40) = 17.08, p < .001, ηp
2 = .30 (16.9
vs. 30.9 px)
F < 1
IT F(1, 40) = 40.36, p < .001, ηp
2 = .50 (588 vs.
635 ms)
F(1, 40) = 52.36, p < .001, ηp
2 = .57 (591 vs.
646 ms)
MT F(1, 40) = 31.24, p < .001, ηp
2 = .44 (423 vs.
461 ms)
F(1, 40) = 9.30, p = .004, ηp
2 = .19 (401 vs. 422
ms)
%CC t(40) = 6.14, p < .001, dz = 0.96 (12.7% vs.
21.9%)
Exp. S2 In contrast to Exp. 2, unsuccessful effect-
preventing actions were no longer associ-
ated with unpleasant auditive effects
REC × effect-
preventing vs.
effect-producing
actions
312 AUC F(1, 40) = 28.88, p < .001, ηp
2 = .42
(3.6 × 103 vs. 7.1 × 103 px2)
F < 1
MAD F(1, 40) = 30.86, p < .001, ηp
2 = .44 (18.0
vs. 35.4 px)
F < 1
IT F(1, 40) = 96.42, p < .001, ηp
2 = .71 (538 vs.
579 ms)
F(1, 40) = 72.79, p < .001, ηp
2 = .65 (541 vs.
589 ms)
MT F(1, 40) = 85.02, p < .001, ηp
2 = .68 (449 vs.
491 ms)
F(1, 40) = 28.89, p < .001, ηp
2 = .42 (423 vs.
448 ms)
%CC t(40) = 8.55, p < .001, dz = 1.34 (15.9% vs.
25.3%)
Exp. S3 Effect-producing actions only REC 156 AUC t(45) = 3.84, p < .001, dz = 0.57 (6.1 × 103
vs. 9.1 × 103 px2)
t(45) = 1.44, p = .156, dz = 0.21
MAD t(45) = 4.12, p < .001, dz = 0.61 (26.8 vs.
43.0 px)
t(45) = 1.62, p = .111, dz = 0.24
IT t(45) = 5.56, p < .001, dz = 0.82 (573 vs.
609 ms)
t(45) = 5.23, p < .001, dz = 0.77 (580 vs. 621 ms)
MT t(45) = 7.06, p < .001, dz = 1.04 (506 vs.
561 ms)
t(45) = 4.16, p < .001, dz = 0.61 (475 vs. 515 ms)
%CC t(45) = 5.88, p < .001, dz = 0.87 (17.3% vs.
25.4%)
DVdependent variable; REC response effect compatibility; ITinitiation time, MTmovement time; AUCarea under the curve; MADmaximum absolute distance; %CCpercentage of trials that
were classified by the cutoff criterion and excluded as multistep movements. For significant differences, descriptive values for the compatible (first descriptive value) and incompatible (second
descriptive value) condition are provided
a Number of trials denotes the total number of trials for each participant, prior to any exclusions. In all experiments, participants worked through an equal amount of compatible and incompatible
trials
b For brevity, only the effect of compatibility is reported, and influences of all other experimental factors are omitted. Thus, in multifactorial designs, F-statistics for the main effect of compatibil-
ity are reported, whereas in unifactorial designs, t-statistics are reported
c Sensory compatibility denotes whether the location of an effects corresponds to the movement direction (i.e., “typical” spatial dimensional overlap as in the other experiments). Affective com-
patibility denotes whether a positive or negative event must be approached by the movement
d We omitted the factor “REC in the previous trial”’ and treated the data like a unifactorial design
Table 1 (continued)
Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics
markedly different trajectory types that are mixed up when
relying solely on average statistics.
In fact, the re-analyses of previous experiments revealed
that a major proportion of the systematic variance in spatial
measures resulted from multi-step movements, which con-
stituted only a minor portion of the trials. In 10 out of 12
experiments, the compatibility effect in AUCs and MADs
completely vanished after excluding movements starting
in the wrong direction. This supports a recent speculation
suggesting that these initial movements towards the wrong
location may be the driving factor for influences on spatial
trajectory markers in response-effect compatibility setups
(Tonn etal., 2023), and therefore speaks against previous
interpretations which ascribed these influences to a continu-
ous activation of the anticipated perceptual effects (Hommel
etal., 2017; Pfister etal., 2014). It is important to note that
across all experiments, the response-effect compatibility
effect for initiation times remained significant (and some-
times even increased in magnitude) after excluding trials
with initial decision errors. Thus, while our analyses yielded
no evidence for an influence on motor execution, temporal
measures indicated a strong influence of response-effect
compatibility on decision making.
But why differentiate between different types of move-
ments and investigate where the compatibility effect origi-
nates from? The question of whether an anticipated action
effect is represented is indeed not affected by this distinction.
However, in the ideomotor framework, mouse-tracking was
specifically employed to make precise inferences on when
and how the anticipated action effect influences movements
(Hommel etal., 2017; Pfister etal., 2014), that is, which
action stages are affected by effect anticipations. In other
words, observed influences were ascribed to a specific phase
within a movement, to action execution, which in this con-
text referred to the efferent activity that follows a completed
decision. Thus, identifying the impact of movements ini-
tially starting into the wrong direction is critical because,
in these trials, the decision phase was not completed before
movement start. From our analyses we can conclude that the
effects in the average trajectories do not primarily originate
from movements starting with a completed decision towards
the correct location. Therefore, the observed deviations in
incompatible trajectories do not yield evidence for a continu-
ous activation of the anticipated perceptual effects of the
movement during its execution: This pattern mainly reflects
decision errors during action selection instead.
Consequently, we suggest that researchers implement
design features that hamper decision changes within the
mouse-tracking paradigm or, alternatively, cross-validate
results through other approaches when aiming to make infer-
ences on movement execution rather than action selection.
However, is it even possible to unequivocally separate action
execution from decision making? At a broad level, one might
argue that movements without changes-of-mind represent
“pure” motor execution. On a more nuanced level, how-
ever, movements inherently involve a decision-making and
planning component, as the system continuously “decides”
whether (and how) to adjust the execution of an action. Con-
sequently, a movement may contain decisional influences
even when excluding all initial decision errors. Conversely,
not all discontinuities (e.g., rapid speed or direction changes)
necessarily indicate an erroneous initial decision but could
likewise result from a correct decision where the execution
failed at any point (e.g., due to muscle twitches). Neverthe-
less, in either scenario it can be argued that discontinui-
ties in the movements reflect decision making while acting
(Netick & Klapp, 1994; Vogel etal., 2024), irrespective of
whether an erroneous decision or an erroneous execution is
corrected.
Of course, mouse-tracking is not always utilized to make
inferences on “pure” motor execution without leakage of
decisional processes. Rather, various fields of research
employ methods that aim at increasing (instead of decreas-
ing) the temporal overlap of action selection and action
execution to tightly couple cognitive and motor processes.
Such approaches may include instructing participants to
start moving swiftly (Freeman & Ambady, 2011; Hehman
etal., 2015; Stolier & Freeman, 2016) or displaying
imperative stimuli only after movement onset (e.g., Kies-
lich etal., 2020; Scherbaum & Kieslich, 2018; Schoemann
etal., 2019). Thus, movements with incomplete or erro-
neous decisions are the primary research target there and
should not be excluded from the analyses. However, as
many studies are based on the assumption that cognitive
processes manifest in the movement at the point in time
where they occur (Dshemuchadse etal., 2013; Stillman
etal., 2018), analyzing velocity profiles, as demonstrated
here, provides vital new insights: It has recently been stated
that interruptions in the form of pauses in the movement
decouple the cognition-movement connection (Schoemann
etal., 2021). Consequently, there are efforts to exclude
such trials by eliminating the respective data points (e.g.,
Schonard etal., 2021) or by introducing design features
that make pauses less likely, for example, by reducing
the overall time limit (e.g., Boschet etal., 2022; Garcia-
Guerrero etal., 2022). While this is a promising start-
ing point towards more straightforward interpretations,
it might only exclude a fraction of the movements where
the cognition-movement connection is decoupled: Error
research suggests that neural correlates of error processing
can start even before the erroneous response is initiated
(Yeung etal., 2004). Consequently, errors can be canceled
extremely quickly or are even corrected on the fly (Foer-
ster etal., 2022a, b). As error correction times are very
short (e.g., Cooke & Diggles, 1984), the time until the
corrective movement is initiated might be shorter than the
Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics
time required to overcome the mass-inertia of the hand
executing the erroneous movement. Therefore, the phases
of decelerating the movement in the direction of the wrong
response and accelerating the movement in the direction
of the correct response might overlap, raising the ques-
tion whether the two components are indeed always sepa-
rated by a complete stop. Consequently, researchers are
well advised to explicitly check not only for pauses, but
also for dips in the velocity profiles because any kind of
velocity change (e.g., a notable deceleration of the move-
ment) might indicate a decoupling of the cognition-move-
ment connection and thereby hide processes taking place.
A more detailed look into the origin of observed effects
regarding velocities within a movement, trajectories on
a by-participant and by-trial level, and a specification of
whether these effects depict errors or a deliberate strategy
to postpone a decision (Wong & Haith, 2017), can advance
the interpretations drawn from mouse-tracking experiments
in various fields.
How do the current methodological considerations inform
ideomotor theorizing? Influences of effect anticipations on
action execution have not only been investigated by evidently
metric movement trajectories, but also by metric aspects of
seemingly discrete keypress movements such as duration
or force of the executed keypress. This raises the question
of whether the current results also have implications for
the interpretation of these experiments. In other words, do
experimental setups in which anticipated effects manifest
in the parameters of executed actions generally reflect only
decisional influences? We believe that this is not the case.
In mouse-tracking setups, participants have ample time to
correct an incorrect response during its execution. Most
keypress experiments, however, do not provide this oppor-
tunity (notable exceptions are mainly found in literature
on error processing; e.g., Crump & Logan, 2013; Rabbitt,
1966). Instead, participants usually know that the onset of a
keypress immediately categorizes their movement as either
correct or wrong. If an initially incorrect response exceeds
the key’s activation threshold, the trial is directly classified
as error, regardless of whether the correct key is pressed
afterwards. In these designs, the onset of a keypress serves
as a natural barrier beyond which a change of mind can no
longer be implemented. Therefore, if anticipated effects still
influence action execution after the onset of the keypress,
this suggests that the observed influences go beyond a deci-
sional component.
We recently investigated keypress durations (i.e., the time
between pressing and releasing a key; Pfister etal., 2023;
Shin etal., 2023) in such a design and indeed found the
duration of keypresses to be biased towards the duration of
irrelevant auditory effects (Tonn etal., 2023). Similarly, in
a study investigating motor sequences, execution times (i.e.,
the time between the onsets of the first and the last keypress)
showed assimilative influences of temporal effect anticipa-
tions (Brown etal., 2022). Together, these results suggest
that response-effect compatibility effects in action execution
Fig. 5 Results from Pfister etal. (2014), Experiment 2. (A) Compat-
ibility effect in area under the curve (AUC) as a function of the used
cutoff criterion. The x-axis indicates the allowed horizontal move-
ment between the center of starting area and the center of the wrong
target area, normalized to percentage. The y-axis indicates the stand-
ardized effect size. The dashed vertical line indicates the cutoff used
in the text. (B) Velocity profiles for all movements (left), and move-
ments classified with the cutoff criterion. The light-gray line depicts
movements excluded from the analysis while the black line depicts
movements remaining in the analysis. The solid vertical lines mark
the point of maximal deviation from an ideal trajectory, with times on
the x-axis normalized to percentage from start up this point to as well
as from this point to reaching the target
Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics
are not generally driven by categorical decision making.
Interestingly, actions have not only been reported to align
with features of the ensuing effects, but also to diverge from
them. For instance, contrast effects were observed when par-
ticipants were specifically instructed to press a key for either
a short or a long duration (Kunde, 2003), or with low or high
force (Kunde etal., 2004; Thébault etal., 2020), resulting in
tones of varying lengths or intensities. Unlike experiments
that found assimilation effects, these experiments involved
task-relevant action features. Thus, it is conceivable that for
task-relevant features, participants intuitively counteracted
the natural tendency to align their actions with the antici-
pated effect to prevent errors.
Conclusion
Decision errors are pervasive in mouse-tracking studies.
Understood in the context of a single response, such decision
errors relate to a response that is initiated in a wrong direc-
tion but might be corrected later during the movement. If an
experiment seeks to measure any influence of an experimen-
tal condition, such decision errors do not pose a concern.
They are of substantial concern, however, if researchers
intend to specifically investigate how completed decisions
are put into motion: Not accounting for changes of mind
during the execution of an action leads to erroneous inter-
pretation of aggregate statistics. Thus, when investigating
what movement kinematics reveal about human cognition,
researchers are well advised to take full advantage of the
rich information inherent in every single trajectory instead
of interpreting the shape of average trajectories.
Appendix
This appendix discusses the usage of an additional velocity
criterion and provides the complete results and visualiza-
tions for all experiments which were not described in detail
in the main body.
Velocities asadditional criterion
Although spatial information is an important characteristic
of the multi-step movements we aim to exclude (i.e., trials
that initially start into the wrong direction), a spatial cut-
off-criterion might fail to identify some multi-step move-
ments and conversely, might classify some smooth move-
ments as multi-step. To address this, we supplemented our
analyses with velocity profile plots and visualizations of
the empirical effect as function of different cutoff values.
The velocity profile plots demonstrate that our criterion
can partition the movements into two disjunct sets, each
displaying the acceleration characteristics expected of
either multi-step or single-step movements. Additionally,
the visualizations of the adjusted cutoff-values show that
the pattern of results remains stable regardless of whether
the cutoff is positioned exactly at the border of the start-
ing area. Although such adjustments do not enhance the
inherent accuracy of the classifier itself, making the cut-
off more lenient or more stringent can alter the balance
between false-positives and false-negatives. Improving
overall accuracy could be possible by, for example, using
the velocity around the point of reaching MAD as an addi-
tional criterion.
In the current re-analyses, however, this approach comes
with a substantial drawback: Because the velocities around
the point of reaching the MAD were used as means to vali-
date the spatial criterion, using them as additional quantita-
tive criterion would undermine the validation process. Yet,
if another validation method is available – such as manual
labeling of movements – or if validation is not a major
concern, then velocity profiles could indeed serve as sup-
plementary quantitative criterion to enhance classification
accuracy. Specifically, we propose using the velocity at the
point of reaching MAD, relative to some characteristic of
the trial’s velocity distribution, such as maximum or median
speed. Similarly, exploring other movement characteristics
around the point of reaching MAD, for example the rate
of change in movement direction (angular velocity), could
also be beneficial. Incorporating multiple criteria into an
ensemble classifier might improve overall categorization
performance, potentially offering a more accurate approach
for distinguishing between multi-step and single-step move-
ments. For researchers particularly interested in the meth-
odology of mouse tracking per se, velocity-based classifica-
tions could be of substantial benefit. We added an exemplary
script to the OSF repository that, for the main experiment of
this article, compares our spatial criterion with a criterion
taking velocity around MAD into account.
Nonetheless, we believe that the main strength and unique
advantage of our current criterion lies in its simplicity,
especially when compared to other methods that are more
advanced and thus more difficult to implement. In our view,
its performance is quite satisfying, and the additional effort
required for more complex methods may pose a significant
barrier for researchers that consider mouse tracking to be
just one part of a broader toolkit for investigating other
research areas. For such researchers, our criterion likely
provides a good balance of efficiency and effectiveness.
Pfister etal. (2014)
Using all available data, a compatibility effect was observed
for all four measures. Compatible actions had smaller AUCs
(15.8 × 103 vs. 16.9 × 103 px2), t(19) = 2.75, p = .013, dz
Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics
= 0.61, smaller MADs (64.1 vs. 68.1 px), t(19) = 2.58, p =
.018, dz = 0.58, shorter ITs (652 vs. 672 ms), t(19) = 2.33, p
= .031, dz = 0.52, and shorter MTs (590 vs. 628 ms), t(19) =
3.17, p = .005, dz = 0.71, than incompatible actions.
As expected, our cutoff criterion excluded fewer compat-
ible than incompatible movements (5.9% vs. 10.3%), t(19)
= 3.19, p = .005, dz = 0.71. When applying our cutoff crite-
rion, the compatibility effect vanished in the spatial meas-
ures of AUC, t(19) = 1.89, p = .073, dz = 0.42, and MAD,
t(19) = 1.17, p = .258, dz = 0.26. In contrast, it remained
present only in the timing measures: Compatible actions had
shorter ITs (654 vs. 672 ms), t(19) = 2.17, p = .043, dz =
0.49, and shorter MTs (578 vs. 598 ms), t(19) = 2.77, p =
.012, dz = 0.62, than incompatible actions.
Figure6 shows the results for Experiment 1 in Pfister
etal. (2014).
Wirth etal. (2015)
Using all available data of Experiment 1, a compatibil-
ity effect was observed for all four measures. Compatible
actions had smaller AUCs (0.7 × 103 vs. 2.8 × 103 px2),
F(1, 15) = 7.63, p = .015, ηp
2 = .34, smaller MADs (4.2 vs.
15.0 px), F(1, 15) = 8.19, p = .012, ηp
2 = .35, shorter ITs
(880 vs. 950 ms), F(1, 15) = 26.50, p < .001, ηp
2 = .64, and
shorter MTs (375 vs. 399 ms), F(1, 15) = 13.58, p = .002,
ηp
2 = .48, than incompatible actions. Note that the original
paper did not analyze MAD.
As expected, our cutoff criterion excluded fewer compat-
ible than incompatible movements (9.4% vs. 17.1%), t(15) =
4.30, p = .001, dz = 1.08. When applying our cutoff criterion,
the compatibility effect vanished in the spatial measures of
AUC, F < 1, and MAD, F < 1. In the timing measures, the
test for the ITs remained significant, with compatible actions
initiated faster than incompatible actions (879 vs. 950 ms),
F(1, 15) = 27.17, p < .001, ηp
2 = .64, whereas the effect in
MTs vanished, F(1, 15) = 1.70, p = .212, ηp
2 = .10.
Using all available data of Experiment 2, no effect was
observed for AUCs, F(1, 15) = 4.53, p = .050, ηp
2 = .23,
but compatible actions had smaller MADs than incompat-
ible actions (22.8 vs. 27.9 px), F(1, 15) = 6.40, p = .023,
ηp
2 = .30. In the timing measures, ITs did not differ, F(1,
15) = 3.55, p = .079, ηp
2 = .19, but MTs were shorter with
compatible than with incompatible actions (1015 vs. 1091
ms), F(1, 15) = 9.78, p = .007, ηp
2 = .39.
As expected, our cutoff criterion excluded fewer compat-
ible than incompatible movements (13.0% vs. 16.6%), t(15)
= 2.26, p = .039, dz = 0.56. When applying our cutoff crite-
rion, the compatibility effect vanished in the spatial meas-
ures of AUC, F < 1, and MAD, F(1, 15) = 1.00, p = .334,
Fig. 6 Results from Pfister etal. (2014), Experiment 1. (A) Compat-
ibility effect in AUC as a function of the used cutoff criterion. The
x-axis indicates the allowed horizontal movement between the center
of starting area and the center of the wrong target area, normalized
to percentage. The y-axis indicates the standardized effect size. The
dashed vertical line indicates the cutoff used in the text. (B) Velocity
profiles for all movements (left), and movements classified with the
cutoff criterion. The light-grey line depicts movements excluded from
the analysis while the black line depicts movements remaining in the
analysis. The solid vertical lines mark the point of maximal deviation
from an ideal trajectory, with times on the x-axis normalized to per-
centage from start up this point to as well as from this point to reach-
ing the target
Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics
ηp
2 = .06. In the timing measures, ITs did not differ, F(1,
15) = 3.87, p = .068, ηp
2 = .21, but MTs were shorter with
compatible than with incompatible actions (988 vs. 1053
ms), F(1, 15) = 7.66, p = .014, ηp
2 = .34.
Figure7 shows the results for Experiment 1 and Experi-
ment 2 in Wirth etal. (2015).
Hommel etal. (2017)
Using all available data, a compatibility effect was observed
for all four measures. Compatible actions had smaller AUCs
(3.2 × 103 vs. 5.3 × 103 px2), F(1, 34) = 10.41, p = .003, ηp
2
= .23, smaller MADs (17.7 vs. 27.6 px), F(1, 34) = 9.77, p =
Fig. 7 Results from Wirth et al. (2015), Experiment 1 and 2. (A)
Compatibility effect in AUC as a function of the used cutoff criterion.
The x-axis indicates the allowed horizontal movement between the
center of starting area and the center of the wrong target area, nor-
malized to percentage. The y-axis indicates the standardized effect
size. The dashed vertical line indicates the cutoff used in the text.
(B) Velocity profiles for all movements (left), and movements clas-
sified with the cutoff criterion. The light-grey line depicts movements
excluded from the analysis while the black line depicts movements
remaining in the analysis. The solid vertical lines mark the point of
maximal deviation from an ideal trajectory, with times on the x-axis
normalized to percentage from start up this point to as well as from
this point to reaching the target
Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics
.004, ηp
2 = .22, shorter ITs (496 vs. 507 ms), F(1, 34) = 7.19,
p = .011, ηp
2 = .17, and shorter MTs (339 vs. 347 ms), F(1,
34) = 5.69, p = .023, ηp
2 = .14, than incompatible actions.
As expected, our cutoff criterion excluded fewer compat-
ible than incompatible movements (16.5% vs. 22.3%), t(34)
= 4.82, p < .001, dz = 0.81. In this experiment, the pattern
of significance remained the same after applying our cutoff
criterion: Compatible actions still had smaller AUCs (-0.7 ×
103 vs. 0.3× 103 px2), F(1, 34) = 5.45, p = .026, ηp
2 = .14,
smaller MADs (-2.4 vs. 1.5 px), F(1, 34) = 4.76, p = .036,
ηp
2 = .12, shorter ITs (497 vs. 507 ms), F(1, 34) = 5.38, p
= .027, ηp
2 = .14, and shorter MTs (317 vs. 324 ms), F(1,
34) = 5.24, p = .028, ηp
2 = .13, than incompatible actions.
Figure8 shows the results for Hommel etal. (2017).
Schonard etal. (2021)
Using all available data of Experiment 1, a compatibility
effect was found for spatial measures. Compatible actions
had smaller AUCs (22.7 × 103 vs. 33.7 × 103 px2), F(1,
19) = 23.22, p < .001, ηp
2 = .55, and smaller MADs (32.4
vs. 51.7 px), F(1, 19) = 23.00, p < .001, ηp
2 = .55, than
incompatible actions. In the timing measures, ITs did not
differ, F(1, 19) = 3.77, p = .067, ηp
2 = .17, but MTs were
shorter with compatible than with incompatible actions (544
vs. 572 ms), F(1, 19) = 30.65, p < .001, ηp
2 = .62.
As expected, our cutoff criterion excluded fewer com-
patible than incompatible movements (7.1% vs. 16.1%),
t(19) = 5.52, p < .001, dz = 1.23. When applying our cutoff
criterion, the compatibility effect vanished for the spatial
measures of AUC, F(1, 19) = 3.05, p = .097, ηp
2 = .14,
and MAD, F(1, 19) = 3.02, p = .098, ηp
2 = .14. In the tim-
ing measures, ITs did not differ, F(1, 19) = 3.50, p = .077,
ηp
2 = .16, but MTs were shorter with compatible than with
incompatible actions (535 vs. 552 ms), F(1, 19) = 6.43, p
= .020, ηp
2 = .25.
Using all available data of Experiment 2, a compatibility
effect was found for all four measures. Compatible actions
had smaller AUCs (63.2 × 103 vs. 72.7 × 103 px2), t(39) =
4.37, p < .001, dz = 0.69, smaller MADs (92.5 vs. 108.1
px), t(39) = 4.22, p < .001, dz = 0.67, shorter ITs (602 vs.
645 ms), t(39) = 5.15, p < .001, dz = 0.81, and shorter MTs
(633 vs. 661 ms), t(39) = 4.75, p < .001, dz = 0.75, than
incompatible movements.
Fig. 8 Results from Hommel etal. (2017). (A) Compatibility effect in
AUC as a function of the used cutoff criterion. The x-axis indicates
the allowed horizontal movement between the center of starting area
and the center of the wrong target area, normalized to percentage.
The y-axis indicates the standardized effect size. The dashed vertical
line indicates the cutoff used in the text. (B) Velocity profiles for all
movements (left), and movements classified with the cutoff criterion.
The light-grey line depicts movements excluded from the analysis
while the black line depicts movements remaining in the analysis.
The solid vertical lines mark the point of maximal deviation from an
ideal trajectory, with times on the x-axis normalized to percentage
from start up this point to as well as from this point to reaching the
target. Note that in this experiment, the data logging rate was substan-
tially higher than the polling rate of the mouse. To prevent edge arti-
facts, data without updated position information had to be excluded
via a custom script prior to time-normalization (for details see https://
osf. io/ hrpk6)
Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics
Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics
As expected, our cutoff criterion excluded fewer compat-
ible than incompatible movements (11.1% vs. 17.4%), t(39) =
5.11, p < .001, dz = 0.81. When applying our cutoff criterion,
the compatibility effect vanished in the spatial measures of
AUC, t(39) = 1.02, p = .312, dz = 0.16, and MAD, |t| < 1. In
contrast, it remained present in the timing measures: Compat-
ible actions had shorter ITs (604 vs. 658 ms), t(39) = 5.80, p
< .001, dz = 0.92, and shorter MTs (613 vs. 628 ms), t(39) =
3.14, p = .003, dz = 0.50, than incompatible actions.
Using all available data of Experiment 3, a compatibil-
ity effect was found for the spatial measures. Compatible
actions had smaller AUCs (13.8 × 103 vs. 19.3 × 103 px2),
F(1, 39) = 8.89, p = .005, ηp
2 = .19, and smaller MADs
(20.7 vs. 30.3 px), F(1, 39) = 8.63, p = .006, ηp
2 = .18,
than incompatible actions. In the timing measures, ITs were
shorter for compatible actions than for incompatible actions
(633 vs. 653 ms), F(1, 39) = 10.66, p = .002, ηp
2 = .21, but
MTs did not differ, F(1, 39) = 2.90, p = .097, ηp
2 = .07.
As expected, our cutoff criterion excluded fewer compat-
ible than incompatible movements (8.6% vs. 11.8%), t(39)
= 2.78, p = .008, dz = 0.44. In this experiment, the pattern
of significance remained the same after applying our cutoff
criterion: Compatible actions still had smaller AUCs (7.2
× 103 vs. 9.7 × 103 px2), F(1, 39) = 5.42, p = .025, ηp
2 =
.12, smaller MADs (9.5 vs. 13.0 px), F(1, 39) = 4.94, p =
.032, ηp
2 = .11, and shorter ITs (634 vs. 657 ms), F(1, 39)
= 11.69, p = .001, ηp
2 = .23, than incompatible actions, and
MTs again did not differ, F < 1.
Figure9 shows the results for Experiment 1, Experiment
2, and Experiment 3 in Schonard etal. (2021).
Tonn etal. (2023)
Using all available data of Experiment 1, a compatibility
effect was found for all four measures. Compatible actions
had smaller AUCs (3.5 × 103 vs. 6.2 × 103 px2), F(1, 42) =
13.98, p = .001, ηp
2 = .25, smaller MADs (16.5 vs. 30.6 px),
F(1, 42) = 17.48, p < .001, ηp
2 = .29, shorter ITs (632 vs.
673 ms), F(1, 42) = 34.62, p < .001, ηp
2= .45, and shorter
MTs (506 vs. 562 ms), F(1, 42) = 47.93, p < .001, ηp
2 = .53,
than incompatible actions.
As expected, our cutoff criterion excluded fewer com-
patible than incompatible movements (13.7% vs. 21.2%),
t(42) = 5.01, p < .001, dz = 0.76. When applying our cut-
off criterion, the compatibility effect vanished in the spatial
measures of AUCs, F < 1, and MAD, F < 1. In contrast,
it remained present in the timing measures: Compatible
actions had shorter ITs (634 vs. 683 ms), F(1, 42) = 48.99,
p < .001, ηp
2 = .54, and shorter MTs (481 vs. 524 ms), F(1,
42) = 30.12, p < .001, ηp
2 = .42, than incompatible actions.
Using all available data of Experiment S1, a compatibility
effect was found for all four measures. Compatible actions
had smaller AUCs (3.7 × 103 vs. 6.5 × 103 px2), F(1, 40) =
14.91, p < .001, ηp
2 = .27, smaller MADs (16.9 vs. 30.9 px),
F(1, 40) = 17.08, p < .001, ηp
2 = .30, shorter ITs (588 vs.
635 ms), F(1, 40) = 40.36, p < .001, ηp
2 = .50, and shorter
MTs (423 vs. 461 ms), F(1, 40) = 31.24, p < .001, ηp
2 = .44,
than incompatible actions.
As expected, our cutoff criterion excluded fewer com-
patible than incompatible movements (12.7% vs. 21.9%),
t(40) = 6.14, p < .001, dz = 0.96. When applying our cutoff
criterion, the compatibility effect vanished in the spatial
measures of AUC, F < 1, and MAD, F < 1. In contrast,
it remained present in the timing measures: Compatible
actions had shorter ITs (591 vs. 646 ms), F(1, 40) = 52.36,
p < .001, ηp
2 = .57, and shorter MTs (401 vs. 422 ms), F(1,
40) = 9.30, p = .004, ηp
2 = .19, than incompatible actions.
Using all available data of Experiment 2, a compatibility
effect was found for all four measures. Compatible actions
had smaller AUCs (3.6 × 103 vs. 7.1 × 103 px2), F(1, 40) =
28.88, p < .001, ηp
2 = .42, smaller MADs (18.0 vs. 35.4 px),
F(1, 40) = 30.86, p < .001, ηp
2 = .44, shorter ITs (538 vs.
579 ms), F(1, 40) = 96.42, p < .001, ηp
2 = .71, and shorter
MTs (449 vs. 491 ms), F(1, 40) = 85.02, p < .001, ηp
2 = .68,
than incompatible trials.
As expected, our cutoff criterion excluded fewer com-
patible than incompatible movements (15.9% vs. 25.3%),
t(40) = 8.55, p < .001, dz = 1.34. When applying our cut-
off criterion, the compatibility effect vanished in the spatial
measures of AUC, F < 1, and MAD, F < 1. In contrast,
it remained present in the timing measures: Compatible
actions had shorter ITs (541 vs. 589 ms), F(1, 40) = 72.79,
p < .001, ηp
2 = .65, and shorter MTs (423 vs. 448 ms), F(1,
40) = 28.89, p < .001, ηp
2 = .42, than incompatible actions.
Using all available data of Experiment S3, a compatibility
effect was found for all four measures. Compatible actions
had smaller AUCs (6.1 × 103 vs. 9.1 × 103 px2), t(45) =
3.84, p < .001, dz = 0.57, smaller MADs (26.8 vs. 43.0 px),
t(45) = 4.12, p < .001, dz = 0.61, shorter ITs (573 vs. 609
ms), t(45) = 5.56, p < .001, dz = 0.82, and shorter MTs (506
vs. 561 ms), t(45) = 7.06, p < .001, dz = 1.04, than incom-
patible movements.
As expected, our cutoff criterion excluded fewer compat-
ible than incompatible movements (17.3% vs. 25.4%), t(45)
Fig. 9 Results from Schonard etal. (2021), Experiment 1, 2, and 3.
(A) Compatibility effect in AUC as a function of the used cutoff crite-
rion. The x-axis indicates the allowed horizontal movement between
the center of starting area and the center of the wrong target area,
normalized to percentage. The y-axis indicates the standardized effect
size. The dashed vertical line indicates the cutoff used in the text.
(B) Velocity profiles for all movements (left), and movements clas-
sified with the cutoff criterion. The light-grey line depicts movements
excluded from the analysis while the black line depicts movements
remaining in the analysis. The solid vertical lines mark the point of
maximal deviation from an ideal trajectory, with times on the x-axis
normalized to percentage from start up this point to as well as from
this point to reaching the target
◂
Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics
= 5.88, p < .001, dz = 0.87. When applying our cutoff crite-
rion, the compatibility effect vanished in the spatial meas-
ures of AUC, t(45) = 1.44, p = .156, dz = 0.21, and MAD,
t(45) = 1.62, p = .111, dz = 0.24. In contrast, it remained
present in the timing measures: Compatible actions had
shorter ITs (580 vs. 621 ms), t(45) = 5.23, p < .001, dz =
0.77, and shorter MTs (475 vs. 515 ms), t(45) = 4.16, p <
.001, dz = 0.61, than incompatible actions.
Figure10 shows the results for Experiment 1 and Experi-
ment S1 in Tonn etal. (2023).
Figure11 shows the results for Experiment S2 and Exper-
iment S3 in Tonn etal. (2023).
Fig 10 Results from Tonn etal. (2023), Experiment 1, S1. (A) Com-
patibility effect in AUC as a function of the used cutoff criterion. The
x-axis indicates the allowed horizontal movement between the center
of starting area and the center of the wrong target area, normalized
to percentage. The y-axis indicates the standardized effect size. The
dashed vertical line indicates the cutoff used in the text. (B) Velocity
profiles for all movements (left), and movements classified with the
cutoff criterion. The light-grey line depicts movements excluded from
the analysis while the black line depicts movements remaining in the
analysis. The solid vertical lines mark the point of maximal deviation
from an ideal trajectory, with times on the x-axis normalized to per-
centage from start up this point to as well as from this point to reach-
ing the target
Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics
Acknowledgements This research was supported by the Deutsche
Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG; PF 853/10-1 and PF 853/11-1).
Author contribution Conceptualization: ST, RP; Methodology: ST;
Formal analysis: ST, MS; Visualization: ST, MS; Writing – Original
Draft: ST; Writing – Review and Editing: ST, MS, WK, RP; Project
administration: ST; Funding acquisition: RP.
Funding Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt
DEAL.
Open practices (including availability of data, materials, and code) All
data, analyses, and figure scrips are publicly available via the Open
Science Framework (https:// osf. io/ hrpk6).Preregistrations are based on
Fig. 11 Results from Tonn et al. (2023), Experiment S2, S3. (A)
Compatibility effect in AUC as a function of the used cutoff criterion.
The x-axis indicates the allowed horizontal movement between the
center of starting area and the center of the wrong target area, nor-
malized to percentage. The y-axis indicates the standardized effect
size. The dashed vertical line indicates the cutoff used in the text.
(B) Velocity profiles for all movements (left), and movements clas-
sified with the cutoff criterion. The light-grey line depicts movements
excluded from the analysis while the black line depicts movements
remaining in the analysis. The solid vertical lines mark the point of
maximal deviation from an ideal trajectory, with times on the x-axis
normalized to percentage from start up this point to as well as from
this point to reaching the target
Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics
the original authors’ publications. Three experiments in the publication
of Tonn etal. (2023) were preregistered.
Declarations
Conflicts of interest The authors have no relevant financial or non-
financial interests to disclose.
Ethics approval, consent to participate, consent for publication Based
on the original authors’ publications.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attri-
bution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adapta-
tion, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long
as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source,
provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes
were made. The images or other third party material in this article are
included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated
otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in
the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not
permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will
need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a
copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
References
Ansuini, C., Cavallo, A., Bertone, C., & Becchio, C. (2014). The vis-
ible face of intention: Why kinematics matters. Frontiers in Psy-
chology, 5, 815. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3389/ fpsyg. 2014. 00815
Boschet, J. M., Scherbaum, S., & Pittig, A. (2022). Costly avoidance of
pavlovian fear stimuli and the temporal dynamics of its decision
process. Scientific Reports, 12(1), 6576. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1038/
s41598- 022- 09931-1
Brown, R. M., Friedgen, E., & Koch, I. (2022). The role of action
effects in motor sequence planning and execution: Exploring
the influence of temporal and spatial effect anticipation. Psy-
chological Research, 86(4), 1078–1096. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/
s00426- 021- 01525-2
Bundt, C., Ruitenberg, M. F. L., Abrahamse, E. L., & Notebaert, W.
(2018). Early and late indications of item-specific control in a
stroop mouse-tracking study. PLoS ONE, 13(5), e0197278. https://
doi. org/ 10. 1371/ journ al. pone. 01972 78
Buttlar, B., & Walther, E. (2019). Dealing with the meat paradox:
Threat leads to moral disengagement from meat consumption.
Appetite, 137, 73–80. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. appet. 2019. 02. 017
Cooke, J. D., & Diggles, V. A. (1984). Rapid error correction during
human arm movements: Evidence for central monitoring. Journal
of Motor Behavior, 16(4), 348–363. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1080/ 00222
895. 1984. 10735 326
Crump, M. J. C., & Logan, G. D. (2013). Prevention and correction in
post-error performance: An ounce of prevention, a pound of cure.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(3), 692–709.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/ a0030 014
Dale, R., & Duran, N. D. (2011). The cognitive dynamics of negated
sentence verification. Cognitive Science, 35(5), 983–996. https://
doi. org/ 10. 1111/j. 1551- 6709. 2010. 01164.x
Dale, R., Kehoe, C., & Spivey, M. J. (2007). Graded motor responses
in the time course of categorizing atypical exemplars. Memory
& Cognition, 35(1), 15–28. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3758/ BF031 95938
Dieciuc, M. A., Roque, N. A., & Boot, W. R. (2019). The spatial
dynamics of mouse-tracking reveal that attention capture is stimu-
lus-driven rather than contingent upon top-down goals. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,
45(10), 1285–1290. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/ xhp00 00671
Dignath, D., Eder, A. B., Steinhauser, M., & Kiesel, A. (2020). Conflict
monitoring and the affective-signaling hypothesis—An integrative
review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 27(2), 193–216. https://
doi. org/ 10. 3758/ s13423- 019- 01668-9
Dignath, D., Pfister, R., Eder, A. B., Kiesel, A., & Kunde, W. (2014).
Something in the way she moves—Movement trajectories reveal
dynamics of self-control. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(3),
809–816. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3758/ s13423- 013- 0517-x
Dshemuchadse, M., Scherbaum, S., & Goschke, T. (2013). How deci-
sions emerge: Action dynamics in intertemporal decision making.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(1), 93–100.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/ a0028 499
Duran, N. D., Dale, R., & McNamara, D. S. (2010). The action dynam-
ics of overcoming the truth. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,
17(4), 486–491. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3758/ PBR. 17.4. 486
Erb, C. D., Moher, J., Sobel, D. M., & Song, J.-H. (2016). Reach tracking
reveals dissociable processes underlying cognitive control. Cogni-
tion, 152, 114–126. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. cogni tion. 2016. 03. 015
Fischer, M. H., & Hartmann, M. (2014). Pushing forward in embodied
cognition: May we mouse the mathematical mind? Frontiers in
Psychology, 5, 1315. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3389/ fpsyg. 2014. 01315
Fishbach, A., Roy, S. A., Bastianen, C., Miller, L. E., & Houk, J. C.
(2005). Kinematic properties of on-line error corrections in the
monkey. Experimental Brain Research, 164(4), 442–457. https://
doi. org/ 10. 1007/ s00221- 005- 2264-3
Fishbach, A., Roy, S. A., Bastianen, C., Miller, L. E., & Houk,
J. C. (2007). Deciding when and how to correct a movement:
Discrete submovements as a decision making process. Experi-
mental Brain Research, 177(1), 45–63. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/
s00221- 006- 0652-y
Foerster, A., Moeller, B., Huffman, G., Kunde, W., Frings, C., & Pfis-
ter, R. (2022a). The human cognitive system corrects traces of
error commission on the fly. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
General, 151(6), 1419–1432. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/ xg e00 01139
Foerster, A., Steinhauser, M., Schwarz, K. A., Kunde, W., & Pfister,
R. (2022b). Error cancellation. Royal Society Open Science, 9(3),
210397. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1098/ rsos. 210397
Freeman, J. B., & Ambady, N. (2009). Motions of the hand expose
the partial and parallel activation of stereotypes. Psychological
Science, 20(10), 1183–1188. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1111/j. 1467- 9280.
2009. 02422.x
Freeman, J. B., & Ambady, N. (2011). Hand movements reveal the
time-course of shape and pigmentation processing in face catego-
rization. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(4), 705–712. https://
doi. org/ 10. 3758/ s13423- 011- 0097-6
Garcia-Guerrero, S., O’Hora, D., Zgonnikov, A., & Scherbaum, S.
(2022). The action dynamics of approach-avoidance conflict dur-
ing decision-making. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychol-
ogy, 76(1), 160–179. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1177/ 17470 21822 10876 25
Georgiou, I., Becchio, C., Glover, S., & Castiello, U. (2007). Different
action patterns for cooperative and competitive behaviour. Cogni-
tion, 102(3), 415–433. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. cogni tion. 2006.
01. 008
Grage, T., Schoemann, M., Kieslich, P. J., & Scherbaum, S. (2019).
Lost to translation: How design factors of the mouse-tracking pro-
cedure impact the inference from action to cognition. Attention,
Perception & Psychophysics, 81(7), 2538–2557. https:// doi. org/
10. 3758/ s13414- 019- 01889-z
Harleß,E. (1862). Die elementaren Funktionen der kreatürlichen Seele.
Fleischmann's Buchhandlung.
Hehman, E., Ingbretsen, Z. A., & Freeman, J. B. (2014). The neu-
ral basis of stereotypic impact on multiple social categorization.
NeuroImage, 101, 704–711. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. neuro image.
2014. 07. 056
Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics
Hehman, E., Stolier, R. M., & Freeman, J. B. (2015). Advanced mouse-
tracking analytic techniques for enhancing psychological science.
Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 18(3), 384–401. https://
doi. org/ 10. 1177/ 13684 30214 538325
Herbart,J.F. (1825). Psychologie als Wissenschaft: Neu gegründet
auf Erfahrung, Metaphysik und Mathematik. August Wilhelm
Unzer.
Hommel, B., Lippelt, D. P., Gurbuz, E., & Pfister, R. (2017). Contri-
butions of expected sensory and affective action effects to action
selection and performance: Evidence from forced- and free-choice
tasks. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24(3), 821–827. https://
doi. org/ 10. 3758/ s13423- 016- 1139-x
James,W. (1890). The principles of psychology, Vol. 2. Henry Holt and
Company. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/ 11059- 000
Jusyte, A., Pfister, R., Mayer, S. V., Schwarz, K. A., Wirth, R., Kunde,
W., & Schönenberg, M. (2017). Smooth criminal: Convicted rule-
breakers show reduced cognitive conflict during deliberate rule
violations. Psychological Research, 81(5), 939–946. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 1007/ s00426- 016- 0798-6
Kieffaber, P. D., Osborne, J., Norton, E., & Hilimire, M. (2023). Decon-
structing the functional significance of the error-related negativ-
ity (ERN) and midline frontal theta oscillations using stepwise
time-locking and single-trial response dynamics. NeuroImage,
274, 120113. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. neuro image. 2023. 120113
Kieslich,P., Henninger,F., Wulff,D., Haslbeck,J., & Schulte-Mecklen-
beck,M. (2018). Mouse-tracking: A practical guide to implemen-
tation and analysis. In M. Schulte-Mecklenbeck, A. Kuehberger,
& J. G. Johnson (Eds.), A Handbook of Process Tracing Methods
(2nd ed., pp.111–129). Routledge. https:// doi. org/ 10. 31234/ osf.
io/ zuvqa
Kieslich, P., Schoemann, M., Grage, T., Hepp, J., & Scherbaum, S.
(2020). Design factors in mouse-tracking: What makes a differ-
ence? Behavior Research Methods, 52(1), 317–341. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 3758/ s13428- 019- 01228-y
Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional over-
lap: Cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility–a model
and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97(2), 253–270. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 1037/ 0033- 295X. 97.2. 253
Kunde, W. (2001). Response-effect compatibility in manual choice
reaction tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human
Perception and Performance, 27(2), 387–394. https:// doi. org/ 10.
1037/ 0096- 1523. 27.2. 387
Kunde, W. (2003). Temporal response-effect compatibility. Psy-
chological Research, 67(3), 153–159. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/
s00426- 002- 0114-5
Kunde, W., Koch, I., & Hoffmann, J. (2004). Anticipated action effects
affect the selection, initiation, and execution of actions. The
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57A(1), 87–106.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1080/ 02724 98034 30001 43
Lazerus, T., Ingbretsen, Z. A., Stolier, R. M., Freeman, J. B., & Cikara,
M. (2016). Positivity bias in judging ingroup members’ emotional
expressions. Emotion, 16(8), 1117–1125. https:// doi. or g/ 10. 1037/
emo00 00227
Maldonado, M., Dunbar, E., & Chemla, E. (2019). Mouse-tracking
as a window into decision making. Behavior Research Methods,
51(3), 1085–1101. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3758/ s13428- 018- 01194-x
Matejka,J., & Fitzmaurice,G. (2017). Same stats, different graphs:
Generating datasets with varied appearance and identical statistics
through simulated annealing. In Conference on Human Factors
in Computing Systems. Symposium conducted at the meeting of
Association for Computing Machinery, New York. https:// doi. org/
10. 1145/ 30254 53. 30259 12
McKinstry, C., Dale, R., & Spivey, M. J. (2008). Action dynamics
reveal parallel competition in decision making. Psychological
Science, 19(1), 22–24. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1111/j. 1467- 9280. 2008.
02041.x
Mittelstädt, V., Leuthold, H., & Mackenzie, I. G. (2023). Motor
demands influence conflict processing in a mouse-tracking Simon
task. Psychological Research, 87(6), 1768–1783. https:// doi. org/
10. 1007/ s00426- 022- 01755-y
Netick, A., & Klapp, S. T. (1994). Hesitations in manual tracking: A
single-channel limit in response programming. Journal of Experi-
mental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(4),
766–782. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/ 0096- 1523. 20.4. 766
O’Hora, D., Carey, R., Kervick, A., Crowley, D., & Dabrowski, M.
(2016). Decisions in motion: Decision dynamics during inter-
temporal choice reflect subjective evaluation of delayed rewards.
Scientific Reports, 6, 20740. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1038/ srep2 0740
Pfister, R., & Janczyk, M. (2012). Harleß’ Apparatus of Will: 150 years
later. Psychological Research, 76(5), 561–565. https:// doi. org/ 10.
1007/ s00426- 011- 0362-3
Pfister, R., Janczyk, M., Wirth, R., Dignath, D., & Kunde, W. (2014).
Thinking with portals: Revisiting kinematic cues to intention.
Cognition, 133(2), 464–473. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. cogni tion.
2014. 07. 012
Pfister, R., Neszmélyi, B., & Kunde, W. (2023). Response durations: A
flexible, no-cost tool for psychological science. Current Directions
in Psychological Science, 32(2), 160–166. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1177/
09637 21422 11416 92
Pfister, R., Tonn, S., Schaaf, M., & Wirth, R. (2024). mousetRajectory:
Mouse trajectory analyses for behavioral scientists. The Quantita-
tive Methods for Psychology, 20(3), 217–237. https:// doi. org/ 10.
20982/ tqmp. 20.3. p217
Pfister, R., Wirth, R., Schwarz, K. A., Steinhauser, M., & Kunde, W.
(2016). Burdens of non-conformity: Motor execution reveals cog-
nitive conflict during deliberate rule violations. Cognition, 147,
93–99. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. cogni tion. 2015. 11. 009
Quétard, B., Spatola, N., Parris, B. A., Ferrand, L., & Augustinova,
M. (2023). A mouse-tracking study of the composite nature of
the stroop effect at the level of response execution. PLoS ONE,
18(1), e0279036. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1371/ journ al. pone. 02790 36
Rabbitt, P. M. (1966). Error correction time without external error signals.
Nature, 212(5060), 438. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1038/ 21243 8a0
Sartori, L., Becchio, C., & Castiello, U. (2011). Cues to intention: The
role of movement information. Cognition, 119(2), 242–252. https://
doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. cogni tion. 2011. 01. 014
Scherbaum, S., Dshemuchadse, M., Fischer, R., & Goschke, T. (2010).
How decisions evolve: The temporal dynamics of action selection.
Cognition, 115(3), 407–416. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. cogni tion.
2010. 02. 004
Scherbaum, S., & Kieslich, P. J. (2018). Stuck at the starting line: How
the starting procedure influences mouse-tracking data. Behavior
Research Methods, 50(5), 2097–2110. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3758/
s13428- 017- 0977-4
Schoemann, M., Lüken, M., Grage, T., Kieslich, P. J., & Scherbaum,
S. (2019). Validating mouse-tracking: How design factors influ-
ence action dynamics in intertemporal decision making. Behavior
Research Methods, 51(5), 2356–2377. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3758/
s13428- 018- 1179-4
Schoemann, M., O’Hora, D., Dale, R., & Scherbaum, S. (2021). Using
mouse cursor tracking to investigate online cognition: Preserving
methodological ingenuity while moving toward reproducible sci-
ence. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 28(3), 766–787. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 3758/ s13423- 020- 01851-3
Schonard, C., Proctor, R. W., Xiong, A., & Janczyk, M. (2021). Examina-
tion of a response–effect compatibility task with continuous mouse
movements: Free - versus forced-choice tasks and sequential mod-
ulations. The American Journal of Psychology, 134(4), 415–439.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 5406/ amerj psyc. 134.4. 0415
Shin,Y.K., Choe,S., & Kwon,O.-S. (2023). Strong evidence for ideo-
motor theory: Unwilled manifestation of the conceptual attribute in
Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics
movement control. Frontiers in Psychology, 14. https:// doi. org/ 10.
3389/ fpsyg. 2023. 10668 39
Song, J.-H., & Nakayama, K. (2009). Hidden cognitive states revealed in
choice reaching tasks. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(8), 360–366.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. tics. 2009. 04. 009
Spivey, M. J., Dale, R., Knoblich, G., & Grosjean, M. (2010). Do curved
reaching movements emerge from competing perceptions? A reply
to van der Wel etal. (2009). Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Human Perception and Performance, 36(1), 251–254. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 1037/ a0017 170
Spivey, M. J., Grosjean, M., & Knoblich, G. (2005). Continuous attrac-
tion toward phonological competitors. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102(29),
10393–10398. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1073/ pnas. 05039 03102
Stillman, P. E., Medvedev, D., & Ferguson, M. J. (2017). Resisting temp-
tation: Tracking how self-control conflicts are successfully resolved
in real time. Psychological Science, 28(9), 1240–1258. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 1177/ 09567 97617 705386
Stillman, P. E., Shen, X., & Ferguson, M. J. (2018). How mouse-tracking
can advance social cognitive theory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,
22(6), 531–543. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. tics. 2018. 03. 012
Stock, A., & Stock, C. (2004). A short history of ideo-motor action. Psy-
chological Research, 68(2–3), 176–188. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/
s00426- 003- 0154-5
Stolier, R. M., & Freeman, J. B. (2016). Neural pattern similarity reveals
the inherent intersection of social categories. Nature Neuroscience,
19(6), 795–797. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1038/ nn. 4296
Stolier, R. M., & Freeman, J. B. (2017). A neural mechanism of social
categorization. The Journal of Neuroscience, 37(23), 5711–5721.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1523/ JNEUR OSCI. 3334- 16. 2017
Thébault, G., Pfister, R., Michalland, A.-H., & Brouillet, D. (2020). Flexi-
ble weighting of body-related effects in action production. Quarterly
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 73(9), 1360–1367. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 1177/ 17470 21820 911793
Tonn, S., Schaaf, M., Kunde, W., & Pfister, R. (2023). Action representa-
tions in prevention behavior: Evidence from motor execution. Cogni-
tion, 234, 105370. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. cogni tion. 2023. 105370
van der Wel, R. P. R. D., Eder, J. R., Mitchel, A. D., Walsh, M. M., &
Rosenbaum, D. A. (2009). Trajectories emerging from discrete ver-
sus continuous processing models in phonological competitor tasks:
A commentary on Spivey, Grosjean, and Knoblich (2005). Journal
of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,
35(2), 588–594. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/ 0096- 1523. 35.2. 588
Vogel, D., Kunde, W., Herbort, O., & Scherbaum, S. (2024). Ideonamic:
An integrative computational model of ideomotor learning and
effect-based action control. Psychological Review, 131(1), 79–103.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/ rev00 00460
Vogel, D., Scherbaum, S., & Janczyk, M. (2018). Dissociating decision
strategies in free-choice tasks – A mouse-tracking analysis. Acta
Psychologica, 190, 65–71. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. actpsy. 2018.
06. 012
Wirth, R., Dignath, D., Pfister, R., Kunde, W., & Eder, A. B. (2016).
Attracted by rewards: Disentangling the motivational influence of
rewarding and punishing targets and distractors. Motivation Science,
2(3), 143–156. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/ mot00 00037
Wirth, R., Foerster, A., Herbort, O., Kunde, W., & Pfister, R. (2018).
This is how to be a rule breaker. Advances in Cognitive Psychology,
14(1), 21–37. https:// doi. org/ 10. 5709/ acp- 0235-2
Wirth, R., Foerster, A., Kunde, W., & Pfister, R. (2020). Design choices:
Empirical recommendations for designing two-dimensional finger-
tracking experiments. Behavior Research Methods, 52(6), 2394–
2416. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3758/ s13428- 020- 01409-0
Wirth, R., Kunde, W., & Pfister, R. (2019). How not to fall for the white
bear: Combined frequency and recency manipulations diminish
negation effects on overt behavior. Journal of Cognition, 2(1), 11.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 5334/ joc. 62
Wirth, R., Pfister, R., Janczyk, M., & Kunde, W. (2015). Through the
portal: Effect anticipation in the central bottleneck. Acta Psycho-
logica, 160, 141–151. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. actpsy. 2015. 07. 007
Wong, A. L., & Haith, A. M. (2017). Motor planning flexibly optimizes
performance under uncertainty about task goals. Nature Communi-
cations, 8, 14624. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1038/ ncomm s14624
Wulff,D., Haslbeck,J., Kieslich,P., Henninger,F., & Schulte-Mecklen-
beck,M. (2018). Mouse-Tracking: Detecting Types in Movement
Trajectories. In M. Schulte-Mecklenbeck, A. Kuehberger, & J. G.
Johnson (Eds.), A Handbook of Process Tracing Methods (2nd ed.,
pp.131–145). Routledge.
Ye, W., & Damian, M. F. (2022). Exploring task switch costs in a color-
shape decision task via a mouse-tracking paradigm. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,
48(1), 8–20. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/ xhp00 00975
Yeung, N., Botvinick, M. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). The neural basis of
error detection: Conflict monitoring and the error-related negativ-
ity. Psychological Review, 111(4), 931–959. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/
0033- 295X. 111.4. 931
Zgonnikov, A., Aleni, A., Piiroinen, P. T., O’Hora, D., & Di Bernardo,
M. (2017). Decision landscapes: Visualizing mouse-tracking data.
Royal Society Open Science, 4(11), 170482. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1098/
rsos. 170482
Publisher's Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to
jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.