ArticlePDF Available

Using online negative emotions to predict risk-coping behaviors in the relocation of Beijing municipal government

Springer Nature
Scientific Reports
Authors:

Abstract and Figures

This article explores the use of online negative emotions to predict public risk-coping behaviors during urban relocation. Through a literature review, the paper proposes hypotheses that anticipate advanced prediction of public risk-coping behaviors based on online negative emotions. The study’s empirical focus is on the relocation of the Beijing municipal government, using time series data for Granger causality analysis in EViews 10.0 software. Data on online negative emotions is sourced from Sina Weibo. After data cleaning, 1420 pieces of data related to the relocation policy of the Beijing Municipal Government within the period from June 9, 2015 to April 28, 2019 are retained. while risk-coping behaviors are measured through public information search behaviors and the incidence of violent crimes, the data coverage is also from June 9, 2015 to April 28, 2019. The results indicated that: (1) Online negative emotions regarding the relocation policy predict public risk-coping behaviors in advance. (2) Negative comments are more effective predictors than negative feelings; (3) Negative emotions about relocation policy formulation predict risk-coping behaviors better than those related to policy effectiveness and implementation; (4) Negative emotions from individuals better predict public risk-coping behaviors than those from institutions; (5) Negative emotions from key stakeholders better predict public risk-coping behaviors than those from non-key or marginal stakeholders. It is recommended that relevant departments establish a real-time monitoring system to track negative public opinions and emotions expressed online, adopt a stakeholder-centric approach to facilitate communication, and promote transparency and educational campaigns to address the challenges of urban relocation. In future studies, methods such as expanding the sample size and adding indicators will be used to address the limitations of potential bias in sample data.
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
Using online negative emotions
to predict risk-coping behaviors in
the relocation of Beijing municipal
government
Qihui Xie1, Hongyu Wu2 & Ruwen Zhang1
This article explores the use of online negative emotions to predict public risk-coping behaviors
during urban relocation. Through a literature review, the paper proposes hypotheses that anticipate
advanced prediction of public risk-coping behaviors based on online negative emotions. The study’s
empirical focus is on the relocation of the Beijing municipal government, using time series data for
Granger causality analysis in EViews 10.0 software. Data on online negative emotions is sourced from
Sina Weibo. After data cleaning, 1420 pieces of data related to the relocation policy of the Beijing
Municipal Government within the period from June 9, 2015 to April 28, 2019 are retained. while
risk-coping behaviors are measured through public information search behaviors and the incidence
of violent crimes, the data coverage is also from June 9, 2015 to April 28, 2019. The results indicated
that: (1) Online negative emotions regarding the relocation policy predict public risk-coping behaviors
in advance. (2) Negative comments are more eective predictors than negative feelings; (3) Negative
emotions about relocation policy formulation predict risk-coping behaviors better than those related
to policy eectiveness and implementation; (4) Negative emotions from individuals better predict
public risk-coping behaviors than those from institutions; (5) Negative emotions from key stakeholders
better predict public risk-coping behaviors than those from non-key or marginal stakeholders. It is
recommended that relevant departments establish a real-time monitoring system to track negative
public opinions and emotions expressed online, adopt a stakeholder-centric approach to facilitate
communication, and promote transparency and educational campaigns to address the challenges of
urban relocation. In future studies, methods such as expanding the sample size and adding indicators
will be used to address the limitations of potential bias in sample data.
Keywords Negative emotions online, Information search behavior, Violent behavior, Granger causality
analysis, Relocation of Beijing municipal government
In the era of big data, the advent of online public opinions and the sentiments expressed by netizens have
heightened the complexity of the urban relocation policy process. e surge in negative emotions observed
online poses a challenge, potentially leading to oine public risk-coping behaviors. However, amid these
challenges, it is crucial to recognize the positive impact of online public opinions and emotions. ese digital
expressions can serve as a mirror reecting the genuine attitudes and coping mechanisms of the public during
the urban relocation process. Moreover, they oer a unique opportunity to anticipate the occurrence of mass
incidents. is study aims to validate the hypothesis that negative emotions online can predict the public’s risk-
coping behaviors in the relocation process. If conrmed, it implies that real-time data on negative emotions
can be monitored during urban relocation policy implementation, enabling timely and eective prediction of
risk-coping behaviors. is approach serves as a supplementary method towards conventional means of policy
risk assessment.
e existing research substantiates the signicant role of negative emotions in shaping public risk-coping
behaviors13. Moreover, it establishes that negative emotions possess predictive capabilities regarding the public’s
risk-coping behaviors4. However, the current body of research seldom addresses two critical questions: rst,
whether negative emotions expressed online can predict the public’s risk-coping behaviors in the context of
1Department of Public Administration, School of Law and Humanities, China University of Mining and Technology
(Beijing), Beijing 100083, China. 2School of Art and Design, Beijing Forestry University, Beijing 100083, China.
email: xieqihui@cumtb.edu.cn
OPEN
Scientic Reports | (2024) 14:28337 1
| https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-79846-6
www.nature.com/scientificreports
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
urban relocation policy; second, exploring factors that inuence the accuracy of this prediction. is study aims
to address these two research questions.
is paper posits hypotheses suggesting that negative emotions can predict the public’s risk-coping behaviors
during the Beijing Municipal Government relocation. e accuracy of prediction is proposed to be inuenced by
types, issues, participants, and stakeholders, as derived from a comprehensive literature review. ese hypotheses
are then tested using the Granger Causality Analysis of time series data. e time series data on online negative
emotions are sourced from Sina Weibo, while risk-coping behaviors are quantied through public information
search behaviors and the incidence of violent events.
Literature review and research hypotheses
The prediction of risk-coping behaviors by negative emotions
Risk-coping behaviors in urban relocation
e displacement and resettlement resulting from urban relocation5,6have garnered increasing attention from
scholars7. is phenomenon, deemed essential for enhancing sustainable urban development, concurrently
introduces social risks to urban management8. e success of relocation plans hinges on the improved quality of
life and economic well-being for the reloc ated public, along with the full lment of reas onable demands9. Successful
implementation relies on cooperative eorts among all actors, stakeholders, and the public, fostering mutual
understanding based on shared goals and visions10. is collaborative approach actively promotes the relocation
process, attains expected policy outcomes, and contributes to urban development objectives11. However, if the
living environment deteriorates post-relocation, resulting in short to medium-term livelihood stagnation and
poverty, the public may engage in various risk-coping behaviors, impacting urban sustainable development and
social stability. In situations of perceived risk, individuals undertake risk and response assessments, deciding
on actions they deem appropriate12,13. Dened as conscious, purposeful, and exible changes in emotions,
cognition, behavior, and environment in response to stress and risk, risk-coping behaviors are conscious eorts
to reduce the perceived risk of a threatening event in a given situation14,15. Although integral to individual
stress management, risk-coping behaviors also hold signicant value for eective social risk management. In
this study’s research scenario, the coping behaviors adopted by the public to safeguard their interests introduce
uncertain risks in the urban relocation process. Public risk-coping behaviors, as the focus of this paper, arise
from dissatisfaction with public demands not being met. In the context of public risk response behaviors during
urban relocation, representative actions include information-seeking behaviors and violent criminal behaviors.
Information-seeking behaviors refer to actions taken by the public to understand relevant information, policies,
and impacts of the relocation, which helps them better cope with the uncertainties and risks brought about by
the relocation. On the other hand, violent criminal behaviors, which arise in some cases due to social unrest,
dissatisfaction, or conicts triggered by the relocation, represent extreme coping mechanisms adopted by some
members of the public. Such behaviors are detrimental to both social stability and the smooth progress of the
relocation.
Lindell and Perry16identied information searching behavior as a manifestation of risk-coping behavior. In
such contexts, individuals tend to engage in information search when perceiving a genuine threat and sensing an
unacceptable level of personal risk. When presented with highly credible sources, the public may unquestionably
comply with instructions, while ambiguity may prompt increased eorts in seeking and processing information17.
In the Internet era, given the uncertainty surrounding relocation information and the perceived threat associated
with it, the public frequently turns to online platforms to acquire relevant policy information—specically,
searching for relocation policies. Hu et al18.corroborated the correlation between the public’s information search
behavior and their risk perception through a questionnaire survey. In essence, heightened information search
activity may signal increased risk perception, correlating with a greater likelihood of resistance against the
relocation.
Violent conict behavior represents another coping strategy that may introduce social risks during the
relocation process. Dissatisfaction with relocation compensation and resettlement may lead to violent protests
against the relocation, or large-scale conicts between the involved parties. Some scholars argue that certain
forms of relocation inherently involve violence. For instance, Ortega’s19research suggests that Manila, in
its pursuit of becoming a modern and investment-friendly city, forcefully relocates informal settlers to the
outskirts—an inherently violent form of suburbanization. Consequently, the public may resort to corresponding
violent actions in response. Begega and Kohler20discussed protest behavior and the mobilization process in
enterprise production relocation through case studies. Con et al21. examined relocation delays stemming from
conict behaviors in utility relocation processes. Furthermore, a decrease in poverty and quality of life resulting
from relocation may contribute to an increase in violent crimes in the relocated area. Casas et al22. argue that
areas of concentrated poverty and public housing face persistent issues with high rates of violent crime, while
relocated residents may not necessarily experience less violence or improved safety in their new communities.
Popkin et al23. contend that while most relocations reduce violent crimes, the relocation of medium and high-
density communities may negatively impact domestic violence crimes.
Negative emotions online and risk-coping behaviors
Research on emotions originated in psychology, attributing emotions to physiological mechanisms such as the
impact of the prefrontal cortex on the amygdala. Sociologists have also extensively studied emotions, recognizing
their integration with social network relations, people, places, time, space, and events, giving rise to the concept
of social emotions. It is believed that the formation of social emotions relies on individuals’ perception of society.
Once formed, social sentiments inuence social behavior, with social emotions driving scal, macroeconomic,
and political behaviors, according to Prechter24.
Scientic Reports | (2024) 14:28337 2
| https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-79846-6
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
e internet’s global reach, breaking time and space constraints, signicantly enhances individual social
cognition levels, contributing to the formation of social emotions. On social media platforms, Internet users’
emotions, oen magnied innitely, shape and adjust netizens’ emotions and impact public social behavior25,26.
It is posited that internet users’ emotions inuence risk-coping behaviors, challenging the notion that social
behaviors drive emotions27.
When focusing on negative emotions, studies suggest their impact on risk-coping behaviors. For instance,
Ramli et al1. found that controlling negative emotions during group gatherings reduces aggressive and dangerous
behaviors. Lei and Yang2proposed that stronger negative emotions correlate with higher likelihoods of choosing
high-risk coping behaviors. Hu et al3. quantitatively established a positive correlation between risk perception,
negative emotions, and rumor-spreading behavior, with negative emotions playing mediating roles.
It has been shown that negative emotions have a certain correlation with public risk coping behaviours, and
that negative emotions increase the emergence of risk coping behaviours to a certain extent and increase the
number of violent incidents in society. e research object chosen for this study is online negative emotions,
and the collection of negative emotions is limited to the scope of the Internet, mainly because there are certain
dierences between online negative emotions and negative emotions. From the point of view of the scope of
dissemination, online negative emotions can be quickly spread to a wider audience through the Internet and
can cross geographical boundaries, which makes their inuence wider. On the other hand, ordinary negative
emotions are usually conned to face-to-face communication, which has a relatively small scope of inuence.
In terms of communication persistence, once posted, online negative sentiment may be permanently recorded
unless it is deleted or hidden. is makes negative content highly persistent. In contrast, negative emotions
tend to be temporary in real life and may fade over time. From the perspective of speaking anonymity, online
negative emotions can sometimes be expressed anonymously or under a pseudonym, which may prompt some
people who are afraid to speak out in real life to choose to speak out on the Internet and express their true
emotions. Finally, from the perspective of information interactivity, online negative emotions can trigger chain
reactions, such as follow-on comments and retweets by netizens, creating collective negative emotions. Whereas
oine negative emotions may also resonate with others, but the spreading speed and scope of such resonance
is relatively limited. Online negative emotions are to some extent more authentic and widespread than oine
negative emotions. With the popularity of the Internet, many researchers choose to capture online comments
and data for data analysis28,29. erefore, this paper chooses to study the impact of online negative emotions on
public risk response behavior.
Despite evidence of negative emotions driving risk-coping behaviors, there is limited research exploring
whether online negative emotions can predict public risk-coping behaviors in urban relocation processes.
Building on prior studies, this paper hypothesizes that negative emotions online can eectively predict public
risk-coping behaviors during urban relocation.
H1: Negative emotions online can predict public risk-coping behaviors in the urban relocation process.
Factors inuencing the prediction eect
According to the existing research, there is a certain correlation between negative emotions and risk coping3032.
is study further explores the dierences in the predictive eects of various dimensions of negative emotions on
risk coping behaviors. On one hand, we delve into the generation of the online text content containing negative
emotions. e types of negative emotions are categorized based on the internal formation mechanisms of those
online contents, while emotions related to dierent issues are classied according to the external problems that
evoke these contents. On the other hand, we focus on the distinctions among the subjects that express online
negative emotions. e negative emotions from dierent participants are categorized based on the organizational
attributes of those subjects, whereas the negative emotions from dierent stakeholders are classied according to
the interest attributes of those subjects. Based on the formation of contents containing negative emotions and the
dierences among subjects expressing negative emotions, this study identied four dimensions of the negative
emotions, including the dierent types of negative emotions, the negative emotions about dierent issues, the
negative emotions from dierent participants, and the negative emotions from dierent stakeholders.
Dierent types of negative emotions
Initially, emotional analysis methods were predominantly employed by computer and information science
scholars, utilizing coarse-grained analysis focused solely on positive and negative attitudes. Watson and
Tellegen33categorized self-reported emotions into positive and negative emotions, introducing the PANAS
scale for emotional measurement. Subsequent research included intensity analysis based on this coarse-grained
dichotomy. Russell et al34. developed a circular emotion model, classifying emotions according to pleasure
and intensity, resulting in four categories: high-intensity pleasure, medium-intensity pleasure, high-intensity
displeasure, and medium-intensity displeasure. As emotional analysis technology matured, scholars transitioned
from a simple dichotomy to a more systematic, ne-grained analysis of public emotional information. Ekman35,
for instance, divided emotions into six basic states: joy, sorrow, anger, fear, surprise, and disgust.
Chinese text analysis also identies various negative emotions. One approach is ne-grained, as seen in
Liu’s 36classication of negative emotions, including anxiety, sadness, anger, disgust, and fear. Another approach,
based on coarse-grained dichotomy, involves dierentiating negative emotions by degree. For instance, the
HowNet Dictionary separates negative feeling words from negative comment words37,38. In comparison to
ne-grained analysis, HowNet’s classication accentuates distinctions within negative emotions. is study
employs HowNet’s classication to analyze the inuence of dierent negative emotions on prediction eects.
Negative feelings refer to the expression of one’s subjective feelings, such as sadness, anger, or disappointment,
while negative comments refer to criticisms, accusations, or dissatisfaction expressed towards a certain event or
phenomenon based on one’s personal values. Negative emotions belong to the realm of individual psychology,
Scientic Reports | (2024) 14:28337 3
| https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-79846-6
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
emphasizing the expression of personal feelings. ese emotions may be private and subjective, and do not
necessarily directly reect or inuence external social reality. erefore, although the negative feelings expressed
by individuals online may be genuine and intense, they may not necessarily immediately or signicantly change
the opinions or behaviors of the general public39. Negative comments fall under the scope of social psychology,
emphasizing the expression of societal perspectives. Rational and well-grounded negative comments tend to
be more easily accepted by the public, as they may be based on facts, logic, or shared values, rather than mere
emotional venting40. Xie and Peng41 also conrmed that negative comments signicantly impact risk perception,
while negative feelings show no signicant impact. Given the signicant relationship between risk perception
and risk-coping behaviors, research hypothesis 2 is as follows:
H2: Negative comments online can more eectively predict public risk-coping behaviors than negative
feelings online in the urban relocation process.
Negative emotions on dierent issues
Issue setting is a form of agenda setting42, wherein the media accentuates specic matters to inuence the public’s
attitudes or understanding. It is widely acknowledged that framing issues signicantly impact public awareness
and behavior43,44. Similarly, government policy making and synergies can inuence and guide public behavior45.
erefore, the paper posits that negative emotions related to dierent issues will dier signicantly in predicting
risk-coping behaviors. Grounded in policy process theory, public policy undergoes stages such as formulation,
implementation, evaluation, and termination46. us, issues within a public policy can be classied into three
types: policy formulation, policy implementation, and policy eectiveness. e policy formulation process is
more likely to draw public attention and discussion than other stages47. e outcome of the policy formulation
will aect the policy implementation and the policy eectiveness48.us, the third hypothesis of this paper is
presented as follows:
H3: Negative emotions related to the issue of relocation policy formulation can more eectively predict
public risk-coping behaviors than those related to issues of relocation policy eectiveness and relocation policy
implementation.
Negative emotions from dierent participants
Online emotions serve as a reection of the public’s mindset and behaviors. Numerous studies have conrmed
that personal characteristics signicantly inuence behavioral judgments and public risk-coping behaviors4951.
erefore, variations in the subjects of emotions may result in dierent risk-coping behaviors. Public opinion
participants can be classied in various ways, such as categorizing them as opinion leaders, active users, or
inactive users based on the enthusiasm of their online behaviors52, or as government, media, and the public
based on their roles in online posting53. Other researchers divided public opinion participants into individuals,
groups and communities based on the perspectives of dierent groups54.
In contrast to negative emotions expressed by individual citizens, emotions emanating from government
and media are subject to “gatekeepers” and may not directly mirror public opinion55. us, this paper proposes
that emotions expressed by individuals are more eective in predicting risk-coping behaviors compared to those
from government or media, leading to the formulation of the fourth hypothesis.
H4: Negative emotions expressed by individuals are more eective in predicting public risk-coping behaviors
than those expressed by institutions in the urban relocation process.
Negative emotions from dierent stakeholders
Stakeholders, initially an economic concept, refer to individuals or entities with claims to a company’s cash
ow. In the realm of social science or management research, stakeholders encompass any entity aected by
organizational decisions and actions in the external environment56. As per stakeholder theory, numerous risk
factors inuencing social stability oen escalate into signicant social conicts not solely due to the actual
magnitude of the risk, which can be challenging to control and resolve. Instead, these conicts are largely rooted
in varying risk perceptions among dierent stakeholders57.
From a relevance perspective, stakeholders can be categorized into three groups: key stakeholders, non-key
stakeholders, and marginal stakeholders58,59. Key stakeholders are essential resources that institutions rely on for
survival and development; their support crucial for the institution’s continued existence and growth60. While the
existence and development of institutions are intertwined with the support of non-key stakeholders, the degree
of dependence on them for enterprise survival and development is notably lower than that of key stakeholders.
Marginal stakeholders, on the other hand, are not indispensable for the institution’s existence and development.
Numerous studies have explored the dierences in risk perception among various stakeholders61. e majority
of research ndings indicate that the risk perception of key stakeholders surpasses that of other stakeholders62,63.
As a result, risk-coping behaviors originating from key stakeholders are anticipated to be higher than those from
other stakeholders. erefore, this paper proposes Hypothesis 5.
H5: Negative emotions expressed by key stakeholders are more eective in predicting public risk-coping
behaviors than those expressed by non-key stakeholders or marginal stakeholders in the urban relocation
process.
Materials and methods
Case study
Beijing’s population had already exceeded 20million in 2012, as shown in Fig.1. If not controlled, overpopulation
will lead to a series of megacity-related issues, such as trac congestion, a decline in service levels, resource
shortages, and the destruction of the ecological environment. In this context, both the central government
Scientic Reports | (2024) 14:28337 4
| https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-79846-6
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
and the Beijing government are contemplating the possibility of seeking a breakthrough. e relocation policy
implemented by the Beijing Municipal Government is a public policy aimed at evacuating the population.
Figure2 summarizes the policy process of the Beijing Government Relocation. Since the beginning of 2015,
rumors about the relocation by the Beijing Municipal Government had been circulating among the public.
Many people initially reacted with disbelief, but those who were well-informed began to purchase houses in the
Tongzhou District of Beijing. By June 9 2015, Beijing’s stance on the implementation of the Beijing-Tianjin-Hebei
Cooperative Development Plan was adopted, conrming the government’s policy of relocating to Tongzhou. On
November 25, 2015, during the Eighth Plenary Session of the Eleventh Beijing Municipal Committee of the
Communist Party of China, it was announced that all municipal administrative institutions in Beijing would
collectively or partially move to the Tongzhou Administrative Vice-Center in 2017, with the relocation schedule
being determined for the rst time.
Since December 2015, the negative eects of the policy have surfaced one aer another. e most immediate
consequence of the policy is the surge in housing prices in Tongzhou and its neighboring areas. e spike in house
prices has led to sudden wealth for some individuals, while others have experienced a loss of income, resulting
in social injustice and exacerbating social conicts. For instance, on November 8–9, 2015, four consecutive
intentional injury cases related to demolition and relocation took place in Xinzhuang Village, Yanjiao Town,
Sanhe City, Hebei Province64.
Although fraught with numerous risks, the relocation of the Beijing Municipal Government proceeded.
By the end of December 2017, only a few departments had adhered to the relocation schedule outlined in
November 2015, and the initial batch of ministries and commissions commenced the move to Tongzhou. On
January 11, 2019, four groups comprising the Beijing Municipal Committee of the Communist Party of China,
the Beijing Municipal People’s Government, the Beijing People’s Congress, and the Beijing Political Consultative
Conference collectively relocated to Tongzhou, ocially implementing the policy.
Spanning nearly four years from its initial announcement to implementation, the policy had been widely
discussed on the Internet since the emergence of rumors. Simultaneously, risk-related issues such as rising
housing prices, education, and healthcare garnered public interest, with the repercussions of policy risks still
ongoing. is paper selects this policy as a representative case of relocation policies to explore solutions for
various challenges arising from China’s rapid economic development and investigate the relationship between
online negative emotions and public risk perception. Based on the case analysis, the data for analysis was
obtained from June 9, 2015 to April 28, 2019.
e reason that we choose the period of data collection is: rumors about the relocation by the Beijing
Municipal Government had been circulating among the public in June 9 and the Beijing Municipal Government
Fig. 2. Policy progress.
Fig. 1. Population Change in Beijing (Ten thousands).(Source: China National Bureau of Statistics).
Scientic Reports | (2024) 14:28337 5
| https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-79846-6
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
relocation policy was ocially introduced in July 2015, while the Beijing Municipal Government ocially
relocated into Tongzhou in January 2019 and the Beijing Municipal Government have settled down in Tongzhou
in April 28. is study is concerned with whether there is any public opposition to this policy and negative
emotions in cyberspace during this four-year period, which ultimately led to the policy not being implemented,
so it is considered that the ocial relocation in 2019 means that the policy has already been implemented and
has produced eects. is paper will investigate the public’s negative emotions on the internet during these four
years.
Variables of negative emotions
Acquisition of analytical data
Presently, Sina Weibo remains as the most popular and inuential microblogging platform in China; thus, it
was chosen as the sample source. Using keywords such as “Beijing government relocation,” “Beijing relocation
to Tongzhou,” “Beijing government eastward relocation,” and “Tongzhou New Town,” searches were conducted
on Sina Weibo from June 9, 2015 to April 28, 2019. Over 20,000 public posts about this policy were collected.
rough the emotional judgment system developed by Boryou Technology (https://www.boryou.com/, accessed
on August 12, 2022), 3,133 posts on Sina Weibo related to negative emotions were identied. Aer manual data
cleaning, which involved removing information unrelated to the policy, eliminating duplicates, and excluding
non-negative emotions, 1420 posts related to the relocation policy of the Beijing Municipal Government were
retained. e time series data for the number of posts containing negative emotions were organized to align
with the data on public risk-coping behaviors. e data used in this study were all open data and were obtained
from the Internet. e data on online negative emotions and risk-coping behaviors are shown in the Figs.3 and
4, containing a total of 1420 data between June 9,2015 and April 28,2019.To make the data presentation more
scientic and aesthetically pleasing, this study has undergone data normalization.
Ethical approval and consent to participate
All procedures performed in studies involving human participants followed the ethical standards of the
institutional and national research committee along with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments
or comparable ethical standards. e study protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of the school of Law
and Humanities, China University of Mining and Technology (Beijing). All participants provided voluntary
informed consent upon survey completion. Furthermore, our data underwent anonymization procedures to
ensure participant privacy.
Manual coding of variables
Table 1shows the list of variable codes. To ensure the validity of the coding, two coders with backgrounds
in linguistics and public administration were selected and trained to perform the coding independently.
If the repetition rate between the two coders exceeds 90%, the condence level of the results is considered
acceptable65,66. In this study, the coding of the data was examined through the Holsti reliability test, and the
overall reliability coecients of the two coders were calculated to be between 0.90 and 1.0, which shows that the
results of the reliability between the 2 coders are acceptable.
Measurement of public risk-coping behaviors
Based on the literature review, representative risk-coping behaviors in the relocation process include information-
seeking behaviors and violent criminal behaviors. is paper assesses public risk-coping behaviors in the urban
relocation process through an examination of these two dimensions.
Fig. 3. Online Negative Emotions data.
Scientic Reports | (2024) 14:28337 6
| https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-79846-6
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Information searching behaviors
is paper employed the Baidu Index to gauge public information searching behaviors. e time series data
capturing the public’s search volume related to the “Beijing government relocation” policy were obtained through
the Baidu search index (http://index.baidu.com, accessed on June 1, 2022).
Violent crime behaviors
In previous studies, violent crimes have typically been derived from ocial statistics. e violent crimes
investigated in this paper require time ser ies statistical analysis in conjunction with online public opinion. Online
data can be captured at specic intervals, even down to the minute or second, while oine ocial statistics are
typically reported annually or quarterly, posing challenges for correlation studies. erefore, this paper utilizes
big data mining to acquire daily data on violent crimes. Davenport et al67. employed news reports to study public
behaviors. Traditional newspaper media, unlike new media, tends to track crisis events, oering insights into
the essence of these events. Hence, this paper initially identies violent crime behaviors in the relocation area
using the “newspaper database” from the China National Knowledge Infrastructure (CNKI). Keywords such as
“Beijing Tongzhou,” “violence incident,” “mass incident,” “petition,” and “crime” were used in the search, and the
data on behaviors were manually assessed and ltered.
Moreover, ocial microblog accounts serve as extensions of government functions, and each piece of
information released through them can be considered credible. us, government microblogs are selected
as sources of data on violent crime behaviors. Given that the “Beijing Municipal Government Relocation
policy primarily impacted Beijing and Hebei, the ocial microblog accounts of the Beijing Municipal Public
Security Department, “@ Ping An Beijing,” and the Hebei Provincial Public Security Department, “@ Hebei
Factors Variables Manual coding
Types A1:Negative comments If the content of the text is expressions of dierent views about the public policy, and the opposing attitude is more moderate, then
code 1. If not, code 0
A2:Negative feelings If the content of the text is expressions of strong dissatisfaction and strong opposition, then code 1. If not, code 0
Issues
B1:Policy formulation If the content of the text is to express opinions of policy making (such as agenda setting, policy plan, etc.), then code 1. If not, code 0
B2:Policy implementation If the content of the text is to express opinions of policy implementation (e.g. group incidents, rumors, etc.), then code 1. If not,
code 0
B3:Policy eectiveness If the content of the text is to express opinions of the policy eect (for example the policy caused the rise of housing prices, the
policy caused the problems of education, medical care, etc.), then code 1. If not, code 0
Participants C1: Institutions If the publisher is media or government, the code is 1, if not 0
C2: Individuals If the publisher is an ordinary person, the code is 1, if not 0
Stakeholders
D1: Key Stakeholder If the negative information is published in Beijing, the code is 1. If not, the code is 0
D2:Non-Key stakeholder If the negative information is published in Tianjin or Hebei, the code is 1. If not, the code is 0
D3:Marginal stakeholders If the negative information is not published in Beijing, Tianjin or Hebei, the code is 1. If not, the code is 0
Tab le 1. Variables coding list.
Fig. 4. Risk-coping Behaviors data(information searching and violent crime behaviors).
Scientic Reports | (2024) 14:28337 7
| https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-79846-6
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Public Security Network Spokesman,” were chosen to gather crime data. Keywords such as “violent events,
“mass events,” “petitions,” and “crimes” were used in the search, and manual judgment was applied to determine
whether the behavior constituted a violent crime. By combining newspaper data and government microblog
data, a time series dataset of violent crime behaviors was compiled.
Granger causality analysis
Granger causality analysis, pioneered by Clive W. J. Granger, the 2003 Nobel Prize winner in economics, has been
widely employed by scholars to examine the predictive relationship between variables68,69. is test was initially
developed to analyze Granger causality in economic variables. Granger dened Granger causality as dependence
on the variance of the best least squares prediction using all the information at some point in the past.
e original hypothesis of the Granger causality test is as follows:
Hypothesis 0 Variable x cannot Granger cause variable y;
Hypothesis 1 Variable x can Granger cause variable y.
e calculation formula is presented in Formula 1. If the p-value is less than 0.05, it indicates that H1 is
established; if the p-value is greater than 0.05, it means H0 is established.
Formula 1. Granger causality analysis.
is paper employs the Granger causality analysis method to examine the predictive impact of online
negative emotions on public risk perception, through three steps: unit root test, VAR model construction, and
establishment of the optimal lag date.
Results
e analysis was conducted using EViews 10.0 soware. Data cleaning by using the Excel soware. e negative
emotions data section includes the variables A1-D3 in Table1, and the data in the Public Risk-coping behaviors
includes two parts, respectively, the public information searching behaviors and the number of violent incidents.
e data of public information searching behaviors are derived from Baidu search index, the data of violent
events is the sum of dierent violence reports in microblog news, newspaper news and website news in the same
day.
Verication of the prediction of public risk perception by negative emotions
Unit root test
Only stationary series can be used for the Granger test. e Unit Root Test was employed to assess whether the
data represent stationary series. e time series data for negative emotions and risk perception underwent the
Augmented Dickey-Fuller (ADF) test in EViews. Table2 shows that at a 1% signicance level, the variables of
negative emotions, information searching behaviors (ISB), and violent crime behaviors (VCB) are stationary and
meet the conditions for modeling. Consequently, an unconstrained VAR model was established.
VAR model construction
e VAR model was estimated using the AR root estimation method to test the stationarity of the results. AR
root estimation is based on the principle that if the reciprocal of all root modules of the VAR model is less than 1,
indicating that all root modules are within the unit circle, then the model is stable. Conversely, if the reciprocal
of all root modules of the VAR model is greater than 1, meaning that all root modules are outside the unit
circle, then the model is unstable. is paper constructed two VAR models: negative emotion and information
search behavior (Model 1), negative emotion and violent criminal behavior (Model 2). Figures5 and 6 display
t-Statistic Prob.*
Test critic al values
1% level 5% level 10% level
Negative emotions –24.92146 0.0000 –3.964525 –3.412980 –3.128488
ISB –8.230661 0.0000 –3.964534 –3.412985 –3.128490
VCB –3.599641 0.0059 –3.434779 –2.863383 –2.567800
Tab le 2. Augmented Dickey-Fuller test of negative emotions and risk-coping behaviors.
Scientic Reports | (2024) 14:28337 8
| https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-79846-6
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
the Inverse roots results of the two VAR models. As shown in the Figures, no root is outside the unit circle,
conrming that the estimated VAR models satisfy the stability condition.
Granger causality test and optimal lag date establishment
As depicted in Figure 3, all test results, with the exception of the Final Prediction Error (FPE), converge upon a
conclusion that the optimal lag order for both Model 1 and Model 2 is 3.First of all, AIC (Akaike Information
Criterion) and SC (Schwarz Criterion) are usually used as the core observation indicators of the optimal lag order
in the VAR model70,71, which should be considered mainly. Secondly, if the lag order above order 3 is selected for
model 1 according to the selection method of HPE, the model will need to estimate more parameters, and then
lose more sample size. In conclusion, according to the principle of the most concise lag order, the third-order lag
was selected to be included in the construction of VAR model.
Fig. 6. Inverse roots of AR Characteristic Polynomial in model 2 (negative emotion and violent criminal
behavior).
Fig. 5. Inverse roots of AR Characteristic Polynomial in model 1(negative emotion and information search
behavior).
Scientic Reports | (2024) 14:28337 9
| https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-79846-6
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
e optimal lag time, determined by both the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) and Schwarz Information
Criterion (SC) in models 1 and 2, was found to be 3. erefore, VAR (Lag = 3) was selected for the test, and the
results are presented in Table4.
e probability that negative emotion does not have Granger Causality of information search behavior is
only 0.0000. Hence, negative emotions can predict information search behaviors in advance. Additionally, the
probability that negative emotion does not have Granger Causality of violent crime behavior is only 0.0189
(< 0.05). erefore, negative emotions can predict violent crime behaviors in advance. e research hypothesis
1 was conrmed in both models.
Verication of the factors inuencing the prediction eect
Table 5 Indicates that at the 1% signicance level, all variables related to types, issues, participants, and
stakeholders are stationary and meet the conditions for modeling. Unconstrained VAR models were constructed
for each variable and risk perception, and the AR root estimation method was employed to test the stationarity
of the estimated results of the VAR model. Aer the test, no root was found outside the unit circle, conrming
that the estimated VAR model satises the stability condition.
To assess Hypotheses 2 to 5, Granger causality analysis was conducted to evaluate the overall predictive
impact of the variables representing ten inuencing factors on public risk-coping behavior.
Table 6 presents the analysis results for public risk-coping behavior, as indicated by information search
behavior. e ndings revealed that among the two variables representing negative emotion types, negative
comments (A1, 8.30***) and negative feelings (A2, 4.11**), the probability that negative comments were not the
Factors Variables t-Statistic Prob.*
Test critic al values
1% level 5% level 10% level
Types Negative comments –17.24982 0.0000 –3.434624 –2.863315 –2.567763
Negative feelings –36.97040 0.0000 –3.434618 –2.863312 –2.567762
Issues
policy formulation –17.24982 0.0000 –3.434624 –2.863315 –2.567763
policy implementation –36.74461 0.0000 –3.434618 –2.863312 –2.567762
policy eectiveness –18.88733 0.0000 –3.434624 –2.863315 –2.567763
Participants Institution emotions –17.61006 0.0000 –3.434624 –2.863315 –2.567763
Individual emotions –26.63745 0.0000 –3.434618 –2.863312 –2.567762
Stakeholders
Key stakeholders –25.65244 0.0000 –3.434618 –2.863312 –2.567762
Non-key stakeholders –6.016446 0.0000 –3.434664 –2.863333 –2.567773
Marginal stakeholders –18.71895 0.0000 –3.434624 –2.863315 –2.567763
Tab le 5. Augmented Dickey-Fuller test of the factors.
Null Hypothesis F-Statistic Prob. Lag
Model1: Negative emotions does not Granger Cause Information Searching Behaviors 33.6063*** 0.0000 3
Model 2: Negative emotions does not Granger Cause Violent Crime Behaviors 3.33127* 0.0189 3
Tab le 4. Granger Causality Test. Model1: Max lag(= 3) selected according to Akaike information criterion
(AIC) by building VAR model. AIC(Lag = 1),17.29692; AIC(Lag = 2),17.28203 AIC(Lag = 3),17.23450;
SC(Lag = 1), 17.31872; SC(Lag = 2),17.31836; SC(Lag = 3),17.28536. Model 2: Max lag(= 3) selected according
to Akaike information criterion (AIC) by building VAR model. AIC(Lag = 1), 6.938467; AIC(Lag = 2), 6.818072;
AIC(Lag = 3), 6.774964; SC(Lag = 1), 6.960723; SC(Lag = 2), 6.855167; SC(Lag = 3), 6.826896. *signicant at 0.05;
**signicant at 0.01; ***signicant at 0.001.
Lag LogL LR FPE AIC SC HQ
Model 1
0 –12905.96 NA 327279.8 18.37432 18.38180 18.37712
1 –12058.90 1690.512 68565.69 17.29692 17.31872 17.18261
2 –12041.07 35.53914 96643.81 17.28203 17.31836 17.16850
3 –11994.31 93.04316 90937.43 17.23450* 17.28536* 17.11323*
Model 2
0 –5229.338 NA 5.517510 7.383681 7.391100 7.386453
1 –4909.904 637.5160 3.534997 6.938467 6.960723 6.946782
2 –4820.604 177.9689 3.134025 6.818072 6.855167 6.831930
3 –4786.062 68.74312* 3.001793* 6.774964* 6.826896* 6.794366*
Tab le 3. Result of optimal lag order test. *Indicates lag order selected by criterion.
Scientic Reports | (2024) 14:28337 10
| https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-79846-6
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Granger cause of risk-coping behavior was less than 0.001. In contrast, the probability that negative feelings were
not the Granger cause of risk-coping behavior was less than 0.01. us, negative comments can better predict
public risk-coping behavior, conrming Hypothesis 2.
Regarding the analysis of issues, policy formulation (B1, 10.36***) was found to predict risk-coping behavior,
while policy implementation and policy eectiveness were not identied as Granger causes of risk-coping
behavior. e probabilities were 0.446 (B1) and 0.503 (B2). erefore, public risk-coping behavior cannot be
predicted by negative emotions related to these two issues, supporting Hypothesis 3.
Analysis of participants indicated that negative emotions from institutions (C1, 0.33) could not predict public
risk-coping behavior, while negative emotions from individuals (C2, 16.38***) might be the cause of public risk-
coping behavior. us, Hypothesis 4 was validated.
In the variables related to stakeholders, the probability that negative emotions from key stakeholders (D1,
14.01 ***) were not the Granger cause of public risk-coping behavior was less than 0.001. is suggests that
negative emotions from key stakeholders can predict risk-coping behavior, while negative emotions from
non-key stakeholders (D2, 0.048) and marginal stakeholders (D3, 0.17) cannot predict risk-coping behavior.
erefore, Hypothesis 5 was conrmed.
Table7 presents the analysis results for public risk-coping behavior represented by violent crime behavior.
e outcomes indicated that among the two variables representing negative emotion types, negative comments
(A1, 9.09***) and negative feelings (A2, 1.90), the probability that negative comments were not the Granger
cause of risk-coping behaviors was less than 0.001. In contrast, the probability that negative feelings were not the
Granger cause of risk-coping behavior was greater than 0.05. erefore, negative comments can better predict
public risk perception, thus conrming Hypothesis 2.
Concerning the three variables related to relocation issues, the predictive eect of policy formulation (B1,
8.54***, p < 0.001) was superior to policy implementation (B2, 3.28**, p < 0.01) and policy eectiveness (B3,
2.85*, p < 0.05). e probability that negative emotions on the policy formulation issue were not the Granger
cause of risk-coping behavior was lower, validating Hypothesis 3.
Among the two variables representing negative emotions from dierent participants, emotions from
institutions (C1, 3.66**) and emotions from individuals (C2, 10.36***), the probability that negative emotions
from individuals were not the Granger cause of risk-coping behavior was less than 0.001. In contrast, the
probability that negative emotions from institutions were not the Granger cause of risk-coping behavior was less
than 0.01. us, negative emotions from individuals can better predict public risk-coping behavior, conrming
Hypothesis 4.
In the variables related to stakeholders, the probability that negative emotions from key stakeholders (D1,
7.54***, P = 6.E-08) were not the Granger cause of public risk perception was the least. is suggests that negative
emotions from key stakeholders can better predict risk-coping behavior than negative emotions from non-key
stakeholders (D2, 1.92*, P < 0.05) and marginal stakeholders (D3, 5.33***, P = 7.E-05). erefore, Hypothesis 5
was veried.
Discussions and conclusions
Summary of Major ndings and contributions
e present research veried the impact of negative emotions on public risk-coping behaviors14, but fewer
studies focused on the prediction of negative emotions on public risk-coping behaviors. is study veried that
negative emotions can eectively predict public information searching behaviors and violent crime behaviors
using the case of relocation policy of Beijing Municipal Government. Our ndings further explored the factors
inuencing the prediction eect. e results indicated that: (1) e publics’ negative emotions expressed online
towards the relocation policy can predic t the public risk-coping behaviors in advance; (2) e negative comments
A1 A2 B1 B2 B3 C1 C2 D1 D2 D3
Lag 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 6 16 5
SC 6.67 3.097 6.04 3.47 4.83 5.46 5.92 6.16 3.08 5.15
F 9.09*** 1.90 8.54*** 3.28** 2.85* 3.66** 10.36*** 7.54*** 1.92* 5.33***
P 2.E-08 0.0909 6.E-08 0.0060 0.0144 0.0027 9.E-10 6.E-08 0.0156 7.E-05
Tab le 7. Granger Causality Tests of dierent emotion variables and violent crime behaviors. *signicant at
0.05; **signicant at 0.01; ***signicant at 0.001.
A1 A2 B1 B2 B3 C1 C2 D1 D2 D3
Lag 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3
SC 17.19 13.64 16.61 14.03 15.38 16.02 16.41 16.63 13.80 15.75
F 8.30*** 4.11** 10.36*** 0.89 0.78 0.33 16.38*** 14.01*** 0.048 0.17
P 0.000 0.007 0.000 0.446 0.503 0.803 0.000 0.000 0.986 0.914
Tab le 6. Granger Causality Tests of dierent emotion variables and information search behaviors. *signicant
at 0.05; **signicant at 0.01; ***signicant at 0.001.
Scientic Reports | (2024) 14:28337 11
| https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-79846-6
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
can better predict the public risk-coping behaviors than the negative feelings; (3) e negative emotions on issue
of relocation policy formulation can better predict the public risk-coping behaviors than on issues of relocation
policy eectiveness and relocation policy implementation. (4) e negative emotions from individuals can better
predict public risk-coping behaviors than from institutions. (5) e negative emotions from key stakeholder can
better predict the public risk-coping behaviors than from non-key stakeholder or marginal stakeholder.
e theoretical contributions of this study encompass the following points. 1)Validation of the predictive
relationship between negative emotions and public risk-coping behaviors. is nding is consistent with existing
literature on the impact of emotions on decision-making and behavior, as exemplied by Lerner & Keltner, who
argued that emotional states can signicantly inuence an individual’s information processing and decision-
making processes72. 2) Revealing dierences in predictive eectiveness among dierent types of emotions and
emotional issues. is aligns with Schwarz & Clore’s “Emotion as Information” hypothesis, which posits that
dierent types of emotions carry distinct information and subsequently aect an individual’s cognition and
behavior73. 3) Emphasizing the role of information sources (individuals vs. institutions) in prediction. is echoes
Granovetter’s theory of the strength of weak ties, suggesting that weak connections may have greater inuence
in the dissemination of information, albeit with a focus on the emotional inuence aspect74. 4) Highlighting the
importance of key stakeholders in predicting risk-coping behaviors. is discovery oers a novel perspective
on stakeholder theory, underscoring the signicance of identifying and prioritizing key stakeholders in public
policy and risk management, which is consistent with Mitchell, Agle & Wood’s discussions on stakeholder
classication and inuence75.
ese ndings have two practical contributions: (1) is study promotes the prediction of public risk-coping
behaviors in the relocation policy. In the process of the relocation policy, in order to eectively reduce the public
risk perception, which determines the successful implementation of the policy, the relocation policy related
departments should pay attention to the negative public opinion on the network in a timely manner, because the
negative emotions can eectively predict the pubic risk-coping behaviors. If the content of the policy is adjusted
according to the negative emotions, the policy risk can be reduced. In the practice of relocation policy, we
should pay attention on the negative emotions from the key stakeholders and individuals, the negative emotions
on issue of policy formulation and the negative comments. (2) is study adds a case of the relocation policy
of Beijing Municipal Government to the research elds of urban relocation policies in China. In the process of
rapid economic development, China has to face the problems of urban relocation and it is of great signicance
that the relocation policies are successfully implemented. is study also further improves on our understanding
of relocation policy from the perspective of risk-coping behaviors. Simultaneously, it provides a case reference
for comparisons with urban relocations in other cities both in China and globally.
Based on the above ndings and contributions, three policy recommendations can be formulated as follows:
Firstly, to enhance the prediction and management of public risk-coping behaviors during relocation policies,
relevant departments should establish a real-time monitoring system for negative public opinions and emotions
expressed online. By promptly addressing these negative sentiments, especially those from key stakeholders and
individuals, as well as those related to policy formulation and negative comments, policy risks can be mitigated.
Secondly, to improve the eectiveness of relocation policies, policy-makers should incorporate a stakeholder-
centric approach, prioritizing engagement and communication with key stakeholders to gather insights and tailor
policies that better align with their concerns. Finally, policymakers should strive for transparency and education
campaigns to build trust and reduce negative emotions, thereby fostering a more positive environment for
successful relocation policies. By integrating these recommendations, China can better navigate the challenges
of urban relocation amidst rapid economic development and ensure the successful implementation of relocation
policies.
Limitations and suggestions for Future Research
e main limitations of this study include the following aspects. On the one hand, the primary limitation of this
study lies in the inherent constraints associated with manual coding. e identication of negative emotion types
and issues related to negative emotions relies on human judgment, introducing the possibility of deviations. To
address this concern, the study implemented a rigorous approach by training two independent coders. However,
the potential for subjectivity remains. Future research endeavors could explore the application of big data analysis
methods to achieve more objective identications, mitigating the limitations associated with manual coding.
On the other hand, although this paper veried the predictive relationship between online negative emotions
and risk coping behaviors through a panel Granger causality test, which concluded that the causality between
the two in a statistical sense is not equal to the actual causality, the perspective of this study is relatively novel
and still has some reference value. However, there is still an endogeneity problem to be solved in this study, and
further validation by using instrumental variables and other methods can be attempted in future studies. In
addition, whether there exists some potential commonality and interaction eect among dierent indicators is
also a topic worthy of in-depth study, and further useful additions can be made regarding indicators related to
negative public sentiment, such as economic indicators, information dissemination speed, media attention, and
public opinion bias. In future research, we will expand the sample size of social media data, increase comparisons
with other national or regional studies, and adopt both qualitative and quantitative research methods to reduce
data bias.
Data availability
Online negative emotions data are publicly available from the Boryou Technology (https://www.boryou.com/).
Public information searching behaviors data are publicly available from the Baidu search index ( h t t p : / / i n d e x . b
a i d u . c o m ) . Violent crime behaviors data obtained from Weibo accounts such as “@ Ping An Beijing,” and the
Scientic Reports | (2024) 14:28337 12
| https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-79846-6
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Hebei Provincial Public Security Department, “@ Hebei Public Security Network Spokesman”. e datasets used
and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Received: 7 May 2024; Accepted: 12 November 2024
References
1. Ramli, N. et al. Bayesian Network Model of Crowd Emotion and Negative Behavior. In Proceedings of the 3rd International
Conference on Quantitative Sciences and Its Applications (ICOQSIA), Kedah, MALAYSIA, 12–14, 867–874 (2014).
2. Lei, T. & Yang, N. D. A multifactoria l study on the inuence of personal emotions and behavioral decisions in the context of sudden
disasters. Seeker. 114–118 (2016).
3. Hu, W., Wang, Q., Chen, H. L. & Jiang, Y. H. e relationship between public risk perceptions and unintentional/intentional
rumor-mongering behaviors in the Newcastle pneumonia outbreak: the mediating role of negative emotions. Chin. J. Clin. Psychol.
28, 734–738 (2020).
4. Ford, B. Q. & Feinberg, M. Coping with politics: the benets and costs of emotion regulation. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 34, 123–128
(2020).
5. Terminski, B. De velopment-Induced displacement and resettlement: theoretical frameworks and Current challenges. Development.
10, 101 (2013).
6. Wet, C. de. Development-induced displacement: problems, policies and people. Berghahn Books, (2006).
7. Ruming, K. & Melo Zurita, M. de. L. Care, urban regeneration and forced tenant relocation: the case of Ivanhoe social housing
estate, Sydney. Hous. Stud. 39(3), 812–830 (2024).
8. Gamble, S. House relocation: a redevelopment tool for rapidly changing urban environments. Cities. 154, 105233 (2024).
9. Tang, J., Xu, Y. & Qiu, H. Integration of migrants in poverty alleviation resettlement to urban China. Cities. 120, 103501 (2022).
10. Kaida, N. & Miah, T. M. Rural-urban perspectives on impoverishment risks in development-induced involuntary resettlement in
Bangladesh. Habitat Int. 50, 73–79 (2015).
11. Lin, Q., Luo, X., Lin, G., Yang, T. & Su, W. Impact of relocation and reconstruction policies on the upgrading of urban industrial
structure in old industrial districts. Front. Environ. Sci. Eng. 10, 1002993 (2022).
12. Ganster, D. C. & Rosen, C. C. Work stress and employee health: a multidisciplinary review. J. Manag. 39(5), 1085–1122 (2013).
13. Aven, T. Risk assessment and risk management: review of recent advances on their foundation. Eur. J. Oper. Res. 253(1), 1–13
(2016).
14. Lazarus, R. S. & Folkman, S. Stress,Appraisal, and Coping Springer Publishing Company, (1984).
15. Compas, B. E., Connor-Smith, J. K., Saltzman, H., omsen, A. H. & Wadsworth, M. E. Coping with stress during childhood and
adolescence: problems, progress, and potential in theory and research. Psychol. Bull. 127(1), 87–127 (2001).
16. Lindell, M. K. & Perry, R. W. e protective action decision model: theoretical modications and additional evidence. Risk Anal.
32(4), 616–632 (2011).
17. Huang, S. K., Lindell, M. K., Prater, C. S., Wu, H. C. & Siebeneck, L. K. Household Evacuation decision making in response to
Hurricane Ike. Nat. Hazards Rev. 13(4), 283–296 (2012).
18. Hu, X. N., Guo, X. S., Lian, C. H. & Zhao, H. Z. Study on the risk information seeking and Processing of Public Policy: a Case Study
of Disaster Resettlement Policy in Southern Shaanxi. J. Intell. 38, 172–180 (2019).
19. Ortega, A. A. C. Exposing Necroburbia: Suburban Relocation, Necropolitics, and violent geographies in ManilaMga susing salita.
Antipode. 52(4), 1175–1195 (2020).
20. Begega, S. G. & Kohler, H. D. Workforces and local communities against corporate restructuring: a comparative case study of
resistance to plant closures in Northern Spain. Soc. Mov. Stud. 21(3), 355–371 (2022).
21. Con, G. J., Wang, J. & Howard, I. L. Utility relocation concept Plan for Mitigation of Utility Relocation Delays. J. Pipel Syst. Eng.
Pract. 13(1), 05021013 (2022).
22. Casas, A. et al. Housing Relocation Policy and Violence: A literature review. Trauma. Violence Abus. 17(5), 601–610 (2016).
23. Popkin, S. J. et al. Public housing transformation and crime: making the case for responsible relocation. Cityscape. 14, 137–160
(2012).
24. Prechter, R. R. Pioneering Studies in Socionomics Gainesville, (2003).
25. Verma, A., Islam, S., Moghaddam, V. & Anwar, A. Digital emotion regulation on Social Media. Computer. 57(6), 82–89 (2024).
26. Peng, L., Wang, J., Zheng, N. & Guo, X. Traversing emotional spaces: social media aordances and emotion regulation in Times of
physical isolation. Soc Med. Soc 10(1), (2024).
27. Gu, M., Guo, H., Zhuang, J., Du, Y. & Qian, L. Social Media user Behavior and emotions during Crisis events. Int. J. Environ. Res.
Public. Health. 19(9), 5197 (2022).
28. Gang, R. & Taeho, H. Examining the relationship between specic negative emotions and the perceived helpfulness of online
reviews. Inf. Process. Manage. 56(4), 1425–1438 (2019).
29. Lelieveld, G. J. & Hendriks, H. e interpersonal eects of distinct emotions in online reviews. Cogn. Emot. 35(7), 1257–1280
(2021).
30. Chen, J. et al. Inuences of PM2.5 pollution on the public’s negative emotions, risk perceptions, and coping behaviors: a cross-
national study in China and Korea. J. Risk Res. 26(4), 367–379 (2023).
31. Wang, T. et al. e determinants aecting the intention of urban residents to prepare for ood risk in China. Nat. Hazards Earth
Syst. Sci. 22(6), 2185–2199 (2022).
32. Zhao, Y., Jiang, Y., Zhang, W. & Zhu, Y. Relationship between risk perception, emotion, and coping behavior during Public Health
emergencies: a systematic review and Meta-analysis. Systems. 11(4), 181 (2023).
33. Watson, D. & Tellegen, A. Toward a consensual structure of mood. Psychol. Bull. 98, 219–235 (1985).
34. Russell, J. A., Lewicka, M. & Niit, T. A cross-cultural study of a circumplex model of aect. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 57, 848 (1989).
35. Ekman, P. Facial expression and emotion. Am. Psychol. 48(4), 384–392 (1993).
36. Liu, L. Research on Emotion Recognition of College Students Based on Forum Posts. Doctoral dissertation, Central China Normal
University (2016).
37. Qi, F. C. et al. Openhownet: An open sememe-based lexical knowledge base. arXiv:1901.09957 (2019).
38. Dong, Z. D. & Dong, Q. HowNet - A hybrid language and knowledge resource. In Proceedings of the International Conference on
Natural Language Processing and Knowledge Engineering, Beijing. Peoples R China. 820–824 (2003).
39. Catford, J. C. A Linguistic eory of Translation: An Essay in Applied Linguistics Oxford University Press, (1965).
40. Wang, Y. & Huang, J. Chinese localization and evaluation of reliability and validity of Perceived Stigmatization Questionnaire.
Chin. Nurs. Res. 32(21), 3414–3417 (2018).
41. Xie, Q. H. & Peng, Z. C. An empirical study on Netizens’ negative emotions and psychological risk perception-from the perspec tive
of major decision-making and social stability risk assessment. Sci. Decis. Mak. 36–53 (2018).
42. Kiousis, S. & Shields, A. Intercandidate agenda-setting in presidential elections: issue and attribute agendas in the 2004 campaign.
Public. Relat. Rev. 34, 325–330 (2008).
Scientic Reports | (2024) 14:28337 13
| https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-79846-6
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
43. Dunaway, J., Branton, R. P. & Abrajano, M. A. Agenda Setting Public Opinion, and the issue of Immigration Reform. Soc. Sci. Q.
91(2), 359–378 (2010).
44. Iyengar, S. & Simon, A. F. News coverage of the Gulf crisis and public opinion: a study of agenda-setting, priming, and framing.
Commun. Res. 20, 365–383 (1993).
45. Li, C., Liu, J. & Wang, L. Research on the inuence mechanism of public green behavior guiding policy eect: from the perspective
of policy synergy system. Environ Dev. Sustain (2024).
46. Dye, T. R. Understanding public policy, 14th ed., Pearson: Boston (2013).
47. Zhang, Y. Q., Loh, C., Louie, P. K. K., Liu, H. & Lau A.K.H. e roles of scientic research and stakeholder engagement for
evidence-based policy formulation on shipping emissions control in Hong Kong. J. Environ. Manage. 223, 49–56 (2018).
48. Hansson-Forman, K., Reimerson, E., Bjärstig, T. & Sandström, C. A view through the lens of policy formulation: the struggle to
formulate Swedish moose policy. J. Environ. Pol. Plan. 23(4), 528–542 (2021).
49. Iravani, M. R. A social work study high-risk behavior among teenagers. Manage. Sci. 2, 313–320 (2012).
50. Neblett, R. C., Davey-Rothwell, M., Chander, G. & Latkin, C. A. Social Network Characteristics and HIV sexual risk behavior
among urban African American women. J. Urban Health. 88, 54–65 (2011).
51. Gallant, A., Pelot, A., Mazerolle, M. P., Sonier, R. P. & Roy-Charland, A. e role of emotion-related individual dierences in
enjoyment and masking smile judgment. BMC Psychol. 11, 132 (2023).
52. Weeks, B. E., Ardèvol-Abreu, A. & Gil de Zúñiga, H. Online inuence? Social Media Use, Opinion Leadership, and political
persuasion. Int. J. Public. Opin. Res. 29, 214–239 (2015).
53. Dong, L. F. Research on the main body of online public opinion formation stage based on SD evolutionary game theory. Inf. Sci.
36, 24–44 (2018).
54. Adikari, A. et al. Emotions of COVID-19: content analysis of self-reported information using Articial Intelligence. J. Med. Internet
Res. 23(4), e27341 (2021).
55. McCarthy, J. D., Smith, J. & Zald, M. N. Accessing public, media, electoral, and governmental agendas. J. Pediatr (1996).
56. Quintelier, K. J., van Hugten, J., Parmar, B. L. & Brokerhof, I. M. Humanizing stakeholders by Rethinking Business. Front. Psychol.
12, 687067 (2021).
57. Wang, F., Hu, X. & Liu, P. Empirical studies of anxiety, risk perception and NIMBY conict-landll sites in Beijing as an example.
J. Beijing Instit Technol. Soc. Sci. Ed. (6), 61–67 (2014).
58. Famiyeh, S. Socially responsible mining using project stakeholder identication and management: an exploratory study. J. Glob
Responsib. 8, 151–168 (2017).
59. Feuvre, M. L., Medway, D., Warnaby, G., Ward, K. & Goatman, A. Understanding stakeholder interactions in urban partnerships.
Cities. 52, 55–65 (2016).
60. Lengyel, A. Spatial perspectives on sustainability priorities: key stakeholders’ insights. J. Clean. Prod. 420, 138341 (2023).
61. Shen, Y., Luo, Z., Song, X. & Liu, C. Research on the evolution of cross-platform online public opinion for public health emergencies
considering stakeholders. PLoS ONE. 19(6), e0304877 (2024).
62. Williams, B. L., Brown, S., Greenberg, M. & Kahn, M. A. Risk perception in context: the Savannah River Site Stakeholder Study.
Risk Anal. 19, 1019–1035 (1999).
63. Vasi, I. B. & King, B. G. Social Movements, Risk perceptions, and Economic outcomes. Am. Sociol. Rev. 77, 573–596 (2012).
64. CNR, Four intentional injury cases in Yanjiao (Continued). 12 people were arrested and the reconstruction of the village involved
was not started. http: //ne ws. cn r.cn/n at ive/gd/2 0151113/t 2 0151113_ 520491798.shtml (2015). (accessed 25 May 2024).
65. Fleiss, J. L. & Cohen, J. e equivalence of weighted kappa and the intraclass correlation coecient as measures of reliability. Educ.
Psychol. Meas. 33(3), 613–619 (1973).
66. Hayes, A. F. & Krippendor, K. Answering the call for a standard reliability measure for coding data. Commun. Methods Meas.
1(1), 77–89 (2007).
67. Davenport, C., Soule, S. A. & Armstrong, D. A. Protesting while black? e dierential policing of American activism, 1960 to
1990. Am. Sociol. Rev. 76, 152–178 (2011).
68. Gelper, S., Lemmens, A. & Croux, C. Consumer sentiment and consumer spending: decomposing the Granger causal relationship
in the time domain. Appl. Econ. 39, 1–11 (2007).
69. Gök, R. & Çankal, E. Granger causal relationship between bond yield changes and equity returns through wavelets analysis: the
case of Turkey. Ege Acad. Rev. 20, 301–317 (2020).
70. Akaike, H. Fitting autoregressive models for prediction. Ann. Inst. Stat. Math. 21, 243–247 (1969).
71. Abrigo, M. R. M. & Love, I. Estimation of Panel Vector Autoregression in Stata: a Package of Programs. Stata J. 16(3), 778–804
(2016).
72. Lerner, J. S. & Keltner, D. Beyond valence: toward a model of emotion-specic inuences on judgment and choice. Cogn. Emot.
14(4), 473–493 (2000).
73. Schwarz, N. & Clore, G. L. Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: informative and directive functions of aective
states. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 45(3), 513–523 (1983).
74. Granovetter, M. reshold models of collective behavior. Am. J. Sociol. 83(6), 1420–1443 (1978).
75. Mitchell, R. K., Agle, B. R. & Wood, D. J. Toward a theory of stakeholder identication and salience: dening the principle of who
and what really counts. Acad. Manage. Rev. 22(4), 853–886 (1997).
Author contributions
Conceptualization, Q.X.; methodology, Q.X. and H.W.; soware, Q.X. and R.Z.; validation, Q.X., H.W. and R.Z.;
formal analysis, Q.X.; investigation, Q.X. and H.W.; resources, Q.X.; writing—original dra preparation, Q.X.;
writing—review and editing, Q.X. and R.Z.; visualization, Q.X.; supervision, Q.X.; All authors have read and
agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding
is research was funded by the National Social Science Fund of China, grant number 21CGL045.
Declarations
Competing interests
e authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to Q.X.
Reprints and permissions information is available at www.nature.com/reprints.
Scientic Reports | (2024) 14:28337 14
| https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-79846-6
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and
institutional aliations.
Open Access is article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives
4.0 International License, which permits any non-commercial use, sharing, distribution and reproduction in
any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide
a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if you modied the licensed material. You do not have
permission under this licence to share adapted material derived from this article or parts of it. e images or
other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated
otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence
and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to
obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit h t t p : / / c r e a t i v e c o m m o
n s . o r g / l i c e n s e s / b y - n c - n d / 4 . 0 / .
© e Author(s) 2024
Scientic Reports | (2024) 14:28337 15
| https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-79846-6
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
Terms and Conditions
Springer Nature journal content, brought to you courtesy of Springer Nature Customer Service Center GmbH (“Springer Nature”).
Springer Nature supports a reasonable amount of sharing of research papers by authors, subscribers and authorised users (“Users”), for small-
scale personal, non-commercial use provided that all copyright, trade and service marks and other proprietary notices are maintained. By
accessing, sharing, receiving or otherwise using the Springer Nature journal content you agree to these terms of use (“Terms”). For these
purposes, Springer Nature considers academic use (by researchers and students) to be non-commercial.
These Terms are supplementary and will apply in addition to any applicable website terms and conditions, a relevant site licence or a personal
subscription. These Terms will prevail over any conflict or ambiguity with regards to the relevant terms, a site licence or a personal subscription
(to the extent of the conflict or ambiguity only). For Creative Commons-licensed articles, the terms of the Creative Commons license used will
apply.
We collect and use personal data to provide access to the Springer Nature journal content. We may also use these personal data internally within
ResearchGate and Springer Nature and as agreed share it, in an anonymised way, for purposes of tracking, analysis and reporting. We will not
otherwise disclose your personal data outside the ResearchGate or the Springer Nature group of companies unless we have your permission as
detailed in the Privacy Policy.
While Users may use the Springer Nature journal content for small scale, personal non-commercial use, it is important to note that Users may
not:
use such content for the purpose of providing other users with access on a regular or large scale basis or as a means to circumvent access
control;
use such content where to do so would be considered a criminal or statutory offence in any jurisdiction, or gives rise to civil liability, or is
otherwise unlawful;
falsely or misleadingly imply or suggest endorsement, approval , sponsorship, or association unless explicitly agreed to by Springer Nature in
writing;
use bots or other automated methods to access the content or redirect messages
override any security feature or exclusionary protocol; or
share the content in order to create substitute for Springer Nature products or services or a systematic database of Springer Nature journal
content.
In line with the restriction against commercial use, Springer Nature does not permit the creation of a product or service that creates revenue,
royalties, rent or income from our content or its inclusion as part of a paid for service or for other commercial gain. Springer Nature journal
content cannot be used for inter-library loans and librarians may not upload Springer Nature journal content on a large scale into their, or any
other, institutional repository.
These terms of use are reviewed regularly and may be amended at any time. Springer Nature is not obligated to publish any information or
content on this website and may remove it or features or functionality at our sole discretion, at any time with or without notice. Springer Nature
may revoke this licence to you at any time and remove access to any copies of the Springer Nature journal content which have been saved.
To the fullest extent permitted by law, Springer Nature makes no warranties, representations or guarantees to Users, either express or implied
with respect to the Springer nature journal content and all parties disclaim and waive any implied warranties or warranties imposed by law,
including merchantability or fitness for any particular purpose.
Please note that these rights do not automatically extend to content, data or other material published by Springer Nature that may be licensed
from third parties.
If you would like to use or distribute our Springer Nature journal content to a wider audience or on a regular basis or in any other manner not
expressly permitted by these Terms, please contact Springer Nature at
onlineservice@springernature.com
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any citations for this publication.
Article
Full-text available
Objective To explore the different processes of the themes and emotional evolution of various stakeholders in the network public opinion of sudden public health emergencies at different stages of the public opinion evolution lifecycle. Methods This paper proposes a cross-platform analysis method for online public opinion during the public health emergencies based on stakeholders. Firstly, data from multiple platforms are collected and integrated. Secondly, stakeholders are categorized and the stages of public opinion evolution are determined based on stakeholder theory and lifecycle theory. Finally, the Latent Dirichlet Allocation (LDA)+Word2vec model and Convolutional Neural Network (CNN) model are used to analyze the themes and emotional evolution of stakeholders during different stages of public opinion evolution. Results There are differences in the evolution patterns of different types of stakeholders. The evolution process of stakeholders’ focus points exhibits a two-stage transition from concentration to divergence. The focus points of stakeholders are closely associated with their respective social domains. The emotions of the public undergo a three-stage process of positive-negative-positive change. Conclusions This study can provide a reference for the government to have a more comprehensive understanding of the development trend of public opinion and reduce the negative impact of public opinion.
Article
Full-text available
People’s emotional life is increasingly interwoven with social media. Against the background of the COVID-19 pandemic and lockdown in China, this study investigated social media affordances enacted during people’s emotion regulation processes. By analyzing in-depth interviews with 27 Shanghai residents going through a 2-month lockdown, we identified intersections between social media affordances and different emotion regulation processes: selectivity and intrapersonal emotion regulation; visibility and emotional self-presentation; bandwidth and interpersonal emotion regulation; connectivity and collective emotion regulation. This study extended our knowledge of mediated emotion regulation processes and enriched our understanding of the dynamic relations between users, media technologies, and emotional and social contexts.
Article
Full-text available
The public has become an important group of carbon emissions. Strengthening the synergy of guiding policies to promote the public to practice green behavior effectively is significant for achieving the “carbon peak and carbon neutrality” goals. This paper constructs a policy synergy framework based on the two stages of “formulation-implementation” and explores the impact mechanism of synergy on policy effectiveness. Firstly, this article quantifies the public green behavior guiding policy texts released from 2010 to 2021 and analyzes the policy synergy effects in the formulation stage. Secondly, based on the Bayesian network, this paper clarifies the obstacles of multi-agent collaboration in the implementation stage from the perspective of the public, central and local governments. The research finds that: (1) the implementation effect of green behavior guidance policies for the public in China varies depending on the synergistic effect of policy types. The overall coordination of the three policy measures and the coordination between policy departments have a significant positive promoting effect on promoting public practice of green behavior. (2) The impact of each parameter on the degree of collaboration of the policy execution system varies. The improvement of central government synergy indicators has a more significant impact on system synergy. (3) The improvement of the coordination degree of the policy execution system requires the synchronization of three subsystems, and only improving the coordination degree of individual subsystems cannot effectively improve the overall coordination effect of the policy execution system. In addition, enhancing the synergy between the central and local governments can most effectively enhance the overall system's synergy. Based on the objective of optimizing the policy system to encourage the public to practice green behavior, this paper provides a theoretical basis for formulating the public green behavior guiding policy.
Article
Full-text available
Background While some research indicates that individuals can accurately judge smile authenticity of enjoyment and masking smile expressions, other research suggest modest judgment rates of masking smiles. The current study explored the role of emotion-related individual differences in the judgment of authenticity and recognition of negative emotions in enjoyment and masking smile expressions as a potential explanation for the differences observed. Methods Specifically, Experiment 1 investigated the role of emotion contagion (Doherty in J Nonverbal Behav 21:131–154, 1997), emotion intelligence (Schutte et al. in Personality Individ Differ 25:167–177, 1998), and emotion regulation (Gratz and Roemer in J Psychopathol Behav Assess 26:41–54, 2004) in smile authenticity judgment and recognition of negative emotions in masking smiles. Experiment 2 investigated the role of state and trait anxiety (Spielberger et al. in Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory, Consulting Psychologists Press, Palo Alto, 1983) in smile authenticity judgment and recognition of negative emotions in the same masking smiles. In both experiments, repeated measures ANOVAs were conducted for judgment of authenticity, probability of producing the expected response, for the detection of another emotion, and for emotion recognition. A series of correlations were also calculated between the proportion of expected responses of smile judgement and the scores on the different subscales. Results Results of the smile judgment and recognition tasks were replicated in both studies, and echoed results from prior studies of masking smile judgment: participants rated enjoyment smiles as happier than the masking smiles and, of the masking smiles, participants responded “really happy” more often for the angry-eyes masking smiles and more often categorized fear masking smiles as “not really happy”. Conclusions Overall, while the emotion-related individual differences used in our study seem to have an impact on recognition of basic emotions in the literature, our study suggest that these traits, except for emotional awareness, do not predict performances on the judgment of complex expressions such as masking smiles. These results provide further information regarding the factors that do and do not contribute to greater judgment of smile authenticity and recognition of negative emotions in masking smiles.
Article
Full-text available
Complex mechanisms exist between public risk perception, emotions, and coping behaviors during health emergencies. To unravel the relationship between these three phenomena, a meta-analytic approach was employed in this study. Using Comprehensive Meta-Analysis 3.0, 81 papers were analyzed after selection. The results of the meta-analysis showed that (1) risk perception (perceived severity, perceived susceptibility) and negative emotions (especially fear) are both correlated with coping behaviors; (2) risk perception is strongly correlated with fear and moderately correlated with anxiety; and (3) anxiety predicts the adoption of coping behaviors. The existing research provided an empirical basis for implementing effective coping behavior interventions and implied that management decisionmakers need to consider reasonable interventions through multiple channels to maintain the public’s risk perception and emotions within appropriate levels. Finally, future research directions are suggested.
Article
Full-text available
Old industrial bases (municipal districts) play a decisive role in the process of China’s economic development. This study focuses on the upgrading of urban industrial structure. Based on the panel data of 120 old industrial bases (municipal districts) in China from 2010 to 2018, a double difference model was established to empirically analyze the impact of pilot relocation and reconstruction policies on the upgrading of urban industrial structure in old industrial districts. The results demonstrate that 1) The pilot relocation and reconstruction policies play a significant role in promoting the upgrading of urban industrial structure in old industrial districts; 2) Upgrading of urban industrial structure is significantly affected by the urban economic development level, urban population density, land marketization level, industrial enterprise agglomeration degree and infrastructure construction; 3) The impact of relocation and reconstruction policies on the upgrading of urban industrial structure in old industrial districts exhibits certain regional heterogeneity. Finally, some targeted suggestions are proposed for future formulation of effective policies for the relocation and reconstruction of old industrial areas and optimal allocation of urban industrial structure.
Article
We present a summary of the latest research on digital emotion regulation (DER) within social media applications and an overview of recent research on ER interventions for social media. We discuss how social media platforms are utilized as instruments to shape others’ emotional states.
Article
This study is a cross-national study and aimed to investigate how perception of PM2.5 air pollution influences public’s avoidant and protective behaviors, whether there are correlations between the public’s PM2.5 negative emotions, risk perception and coping behaviors, and whether there are significant differences between China and Korea. To better understand the mechanisms of coping behaviors related to the risk of PM2.5 exposure, the study divided coping behaviors into avoidant behaviors, which reduce exposure to PM2.5, and protective behaviors, which reduce the health hazards of PM2.5, for more specific analysis. Three hypotheses were proposed. The results of a comparative analysis showed differences in the mechanisms mediating the relationship between PM2.5 risk perceptions and coping behaviors, between the Chinese and Korean participants. The positive correlation between risk perception and coping behaviors was partially supported, and the ‘negative emotions-risk perception-risk coping behavior’ explanatory pathway was partially supported as well. The results of this study provide valuable insight into the psychology of the public affected by PM2.5 and aid in better communicating the risks of and effective response to the impact of PM2.5 pollution.