ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

While a large part of the deception literature focuses on lying detection, the factors contributing to one’s ability to lie remain unclear. The present study examined the contribution of Theory of Mind (ToM) and interoception on our ability to lie using a directed lie paradigm with two conditions (“Interrogation” and “Polygraph”), designed to enhance each of the two mechanisms. Given the relatively small sample size (n = 26 × 40 trials), special steps were taken to avoid false positives. Our results suggest that various facets of interoceptive abilities are positively related to the self-rated confidence in one’s own lies, especially when under the belief that bodily signals are being monitored (i.e., in the “Polygraph” condition). Beyond providing evidence for the role of the body in lying and raising interesting questions for deception science, these results carry practical implications for criminology and lie detection protocols.
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
Current Psychology (2024) 43:34215–34226
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-024-06890-w
socially malevolent prole known as the Dark Triad (Paul-
hus & Williams, 2002), such as narcissism (Zvi & Elaad,
2018) and psychopathy (Rassin et al., 2023). While often
conceptualized to be immoral and unconscionable, lying is
ubiquitous in everyday life, and being able to lie skillfully
can sometimes facilitate interpersonal relationships, help-
ing us avoid conict or causing emotional harm to others
(Levine & Lupoli, 2022). In fact, recent research shows that
certain forms of deception, such as prosocial lies (i.e., false
statements told to benet others, Levine & Lupoli, 2022),
can increase trust (Levine & Schweitzer, 2015). Moreover,
individuals who told altruistic lies were perceived as more
benevolent than those who were honest (Levine & Sch-
weitzer, 2014).
As deception requires the liar to intentionally manipulate
the beliefs of others (Burgoon & Buller, 1994; Sip et al.,
2012), a signicant line of research has been focused on the
role of theory of mind (ToM) in lying ability. ToM refers
to the ability to infer that others have mental states, such
as beliefs, emotions and intentions, distinct from ourselves
(Baron-Cohen, 1997; Lee & Imuta, 2021; Wellman et al.,
2001). The ability to tell lies, as well as their complexity,
have previously been found to be related to higher ToM
abilities (Evans & Lee, 2011; Talwar et al., 2007, 2017).
However, studies investigating the link between ToM and
Lying - the intentional attempt at instilling a false belief in
others (Sip et al., 2012) - is a prevalent phenomenon car-
rying potentially important consequences. Interestingly,
evidence suggests that the successful detection of a lying
attempt depends more on the ability of the liar, than on the
performance of the lie detector (Bond Jr & DePaulo, 2008;
Levine et al., 2011; Verigin et al., 2019). However, with
most of the deception literature focused on deception detec-
tion (Masip, 2017; Sternglanz et al., 2019; Viji et al., 2022),
the factors contributing to one’s ability to lie remain unclear.
Nevertheless, some ndings suggest a relationship between
the propensity to tell lies, and traits that characterize the
Dominique Makowski
D.Makowski@sussex.ac.uk
1 School of Psychology, University of Sussex, Brighton, UK
2 School of Social Sciences, Nanyang Technological
University, Singapore, Singapore
3 Centre for Research and Development in Learning, Nanyang
Technological University, Singapore, Singapore
4 Lee Kong Chian School of Medicine, Nanyang
Technological University, Singapore, Singapore
5 National Institute of Education, Nanyang Technological
University, Singapore, Singapore
Abstract
While a large part of the deception literature focuses on lying detection, the factors contributing to one’s ability to lie
remain unclear. The present study examined the contribution of Theory of Mind (ToM) and interoception on our ability
to lie using a directed lie paradigm with two conditions (“Interrogation” and “Polygraph”), designed to enhance each of
the two mechanisms. Given the relatively small sample size (n = 26 × 40 trials), special steps were taken to avoid false
positives. Our results suggest that various facets of interoceptive abilities are positively related to the self-rated condence
in one’s own lies, especially when under the belief that bodily signals are being monitored (i.e., in the “Polygraph” condi-
tion). Beyond providing evidence for the role of the body in lying and raising interesting questions for deception science,
these results carry practical implications for criminology and lie detection protocols.
Keywords Deception · Interoception · Theory of Mind · Polygraph · Lying Ability
Accepted: 16 October 2024 / Published online: 13 November 2024
© The Author(s) 2024
The heart can lie: a preliminary investigation of the role of
interoception and theory of mind in deception
DominiqueMakowski1· Zen J.Lau2· TamPham2· An ShuTe2· StephanieKirk2· ClaudiaLiauw2·
S. H. AnnabelChen2,3,4,5
1 3
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Current Psychology (2024) 43:34215–34226
deception have predominantly been focused on children
and neuroatypical individuals (Beaudoin et al., 2020; Bora
& Yener, 2017; Roheger et al., 2022), and its importance in
healthy adults remains to be claried.
Besides paying attention to the person we lie to, gaug-
ing whether they believe us, some attention is also directed
inwards: monitoring our own body and its reactions (e.g.,
cardiac activity and its related changes such as blushing),
which could be used as cues to infer our real intent. This
begs the question of the potential role of interoceptive abili-
ties in deception ability. Broadly dened as one’s sensitivity
to their own internal signals and bodily states (Chen et al.,
2021; Murphy et al., 2019; Weiss et al., 2014), Garnkel
et al., (2015) conceptualize interoception as a three-dimen-
sional construct comprising three distinct facets, namely,
interoceptive accuracy - the objective ability to monitor
internal bodily signals; interoceptive sensibility - the sub-
jective condence in one’s interoceptive accuracy; and
interoceptive awareness - the metacognitive ability to cor-
rectly evaluate one’s interoceptive ability. Interoception has
increasingly been tied to subjective perceptual experiences
(Connell et al., 2018; Seth et al., 2012), as well as individual
dierences in executive functions, emotional processing,
and decision-making (Barrett & Simmons, 2015; Murphy et
al., 2019; Petzschner et al., 2021).
Although few studies exist that investigate the relation-
ship between interoception and deceptive ability per se,
previous decision-making studies have demonstrated a
negative correlation between interoceptive awareness and
one’s likelihood to make risky decisions (Dunn et al., 2010;
Furman et al., 2013). This is in line with the somatic marker
hypothesis, which posits that an accurate evaluation of one’s
bodily signals facilitates the use of such interoceptive feed-
back to guide rational decision making (Damasio, 1996).
Indeed, Sugawara et al. (2020) further reported that individ-
uals who received interoceptive training were more likely
to show higher interoceptive accuracy and make reasoned
decisions. Given that deciding to lie generally involves a
consideration of the potential costs of getting caught, and
hence could also be perceived as risky behavior (Kireev et
al., 2013), interoception could be construed to be negatively
related to lying ability. However, some studies have instead
found heightened interoceptive attention (one’s self-focus
towards internal bodily signals), to predict immoral behav-
iour, such as cheating (Ditto et al., 2006; Lenggenhager et
al., 2013; Williams et al., 2016). Extending these ndings to
social cognition, Vabba et al. (2022) further reports individu-
als with lower interoception told signicantly fewer egoistic
lies when the social reputational stakes were high, whereas
individuals with higher interoception did not exhibit a sig-
nicant dierence in the number of lies told. Given the
scarce research on interoception and deception, more stud-
ies are herein needed to clarify these mixed ndings.
The aim of the present study was to explore the contri-
bution of ToM and interoception abilities on individuals’
deception skills, as indicated by their lying condence,
physiological arousal and response time. To this end, we
designed a directed-lying paradigm with two conditions
diering in the nature of their feedback cues. The Inter-
rogation condition was designed to emphasize (and pref-
erentially mobilize) ToM-related mechanisms, whereas the
Polygraph condition was designed to emphasize interocep-
tive mechanisms. In particular, we expected lying ability
(i.e., higher lie condence, shorter response time and lower
physiological arousal), to be positively predicted by indi-
viduals’ interoceptive abilities in the Polygraph condition,
and by ToM skills in the Interrogation condition. Consistent
with the cognitive load approach outlined in several theories
of deception (such as the Four-Factor Theory (Riggio et al.,
1987) and Activation-Decision-Construction Model (Walc-
zyk et al., 2014), as well as previous ndings which suggest
response time as a reliable cue to deception (Gonzalez-
Billandon et al., 2019; Walczyk et al., 2009), we regarded
shorter response times as a proxy of better lying ability.
Methods
Participants
Thirty university students from Singapore were recruited
through posters, yers, and online social media platforms,
and rewarded with study credits for their time. Four par-
ticipants were excluded as their data was not recorded due
to technical issues. The nal sample consisted of 26 par-
ticipants (Mean age = 20.9, SD = 2.0, range: [18, 25], Sex:
65.4% women, 34.6% men). The heart rate of one partici-
pant and response time of one participant were excluded
from further analysis due to extreme outlying values. To
maximize statistical power, the problematic data from these
two participants were only excluded from analyses involv-
ing those measures; all other data were retained for analyses.
This study was approved by the NTU Institutional
Review Board (NTU-IRB-2020-09-007). All participants
provided their informed consent prior to participation and
were awarded with academic credits upon completion of the
study.
1 3
34216
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Current Psychology (2024) 43:34215–34226
Measures
Theory of Mind (ToM)
Two measures of ToM and its related constructs were
administered. The Yoni Task (Shamay-Tsoory & Aharon-
Peretz, 2007) is a behavioral task which assesses rst and
second-order ToM abilities in both cognitive and aective
domains. Participants were presented with the face of a
character named “Yoni”, surrounded by four colored pic-
tures of objects or faces - one in each corner of the screen.
In total, each participant completed 101 trials 49 trials
assessing their aective ToM abilities, 37 trials assessing
their cognitive ToM abilities and 15 control trials (physi-
cal TOM). During each trial, participants were given an
instruction (e.g., “Yoni is thinking of …” or “Yoni loves
…”) and a specic cue (e.g., the directions of Yoni’s eye
gaze or Yoni’s facial expressions) which they used to choose
the correct answer among the four options presented. Par-
ticipants were instructed to respond as quickly as possible
using the corresponding keys on the given keyboard. In the
control trials (physical TOM), the instruction (e.g., “Yoni is
close to …”) and the cue (e.g., physical distance between
Yoni and the options) required participants to respond based
on Yoni’s physical context. Additionally, the instructions
were changed to assess the rst and second-order abilities
for cognitive and aective TOM. In rst-order TOM tri-
als, participants were instructed to make inferences about
Yoni’s mental state with regards to the objects surrounding
it (e.g., “Yoni is thinking of…” for cognitive ToM trials or
“Yoni likes…” for aective ToM trials). In more complex
second-order TOM trials, participants had to correctly infer
the interaction between Yoni and others’ mental states (e.g.,
“Yoni is thinking of the fruit that wants” for cognitive
ToM trials or “Yoni likes the fruit that … likes” for aective
ToM trials).
The Basic Empathy Scale (BES, Jollie & Farrington,
2006), a 20-item self-report questionnaire measuring two
dimensions of empathy, namely Cognitive (
α=0.83
)
and Aective (
α=0.82
) using a 5-point Likert scale was
administered. Although ToM and empathy are regarded as
distinct psychological constructs, previous research nd-
ings point to them being closely related (Gallant et al.,
2020; Sebastian et al., 2012). Specically, empathy is often
thought to be an integral component in the aective dimen-
sion of ToM (i.e., the ability to infer what someone else is
feeling) (Shamay-Tsoory et al., 2010).
Interoception
To assess participants’ interoceptive ability, participants
completed a Heartbeat Counting Task (HCT, Schandry,
1981) while having their actual heartbeats recorded. During
the HCT task, participants were instructed to count the num-
ber of heartbeats over 5 trials with varying time intervals
(20s, 25s, 30s, 35s, 40s), the order of which was random-
ized. Interoceptive accuracy was computed from the dier-
ence between the estimated number and the real number of
heart beats. Interoceptive sensibility was estimated as the
average of the condence ratings presented at the end of
each trial. Interoceptive awareness was indexed by the cor-
relation between the objective accuracy and the subjective
condence.
Given its multidimensional nature, the MAIA-2 (Meh-
ling et al., 2012), a 37-item questionnaire using 5-point Lik-
ert scales was also administered. It measures eight distinct
facets of interoception including Noticing (e.g., I notice
when I am uncomfortable in my body;
α=0.70
), Not-Dis-
tracting (e.g., I try to ignore pain;
α=0.87
), Not-Worrying
(e.g., I can stay calm and not worry when I have feelings of
discomfort or pain;
α=0.68
), Attention Regulation (e.g., I
can refocus my attention from thinking to sensing my body;
α=0.85
), Emotional Awareness (e.g., I notice how my
body changes when I am angry;
α=0.75
), Self-Regula-
tion (e.g., I can use my breath to reduce tension;
α=0.62
), Body Listening (e.g., I listen to information from my body
about my emotional state;
α=0.88
), and Trust (e.g., I trust
my body sensations;
α=0.89
).
Deception
Using PsychoPy (Peirce et al., 2019), we implemented a
directed-lying task in which participants were instructed
to briey answer 80 questions (taken from their previously
taken Autobiographical Memory Questionnaire - AMQ,
Rubin et al., 2003) pertaining to their personal preferences
and subjective experiences, by either lying or telling the truth
(depending on whether they see “lie” or “truth” written on
the screen). Their goal was to make convincing answers, so
that truths would be judged as truths by the receiver, and lies
as lies. The nature of the receiver was dierent depending
on the condition: participants were told that for half of the
trials, they would have to convince another participant who
would be observing them from a separate room (COVID
regulations were used as a justication) via a webcam con-
nection (Interrogation condition). For the remaining tri-
als, participants were tasked to convince a “lie detection
machine” that would be assessing their behaviour through
their physiological signals (Polygraph condition). In real-
ity, there was no real “receiver” and their answers were not
judged externally (the study focused on their subjective rat-
ings and reactions). The two conditions were presented in a
counter-balanced order, and each comprised of 40 trials (20
truth; 20 lies).
1 3
34217
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Current Psychology (2024) 43:34215–34226
questionnaire that assesses four dispositional lying dimen-
sions - Ability (
α=0.92
), Frequency (
α=0.66
), Negativ-
ity (
α=0.66
), and Contextuality (
α=0.70
).
Procedure
A within-subjects design was used in the present study,
which is comprised of two sessions, to investigate the roles
interoception and ToM play in lying ability. During session
1, participants answered a brief demographic survey as well
as a questionnaire regarding their personal preferences and
subjective experiences (the AMQ), followed by a series
of psychological scales (i.e., BES, MAIA and Lie scale),
which were randomly displayed.
During session 2, performed about one week later, the
three cognitive-behavioural tasks (i.e., the deception task,
HCT and the Yoni task) were administered to participants
while their physiological signals (ECG, RSP, and EDA)
were being recorded. The physiological recording devices
were set up as follows: ECG was recorded with three elec-
trodes placed according to a modied Lead II conguration
(Takuma et al., 1995), and respiration was measured using a
respiration belt. All signals were recorded at 1000 Hz via the
BioPac MP160 system (BioPac Systems Inc., USA).
For all participants, session 2 began with the deception
task, followed by the Yoni task and the HCT, with the latter
two presented in a randomized order. In the directed-lying
task, items of the AMQ were presented as stimuli, with par-
ticipants’ recorded responses (in session 1) used to establish
the ground truth.
Data analysis
Aware of the low number of participants, we tried to take
every step to (1) maximize power by using all available data
(from individual trials) with appropriate statistical tools and
(2) ensure the robustness of results by cross-validating the
ndings across dierent measures and approaches.
Firstly, a manipulation check was carried out to ensure
that our outcome variables were sensitive to the experi-
mental manipulations, by testing the eect of the question
phrasing (direct vs. indirect) and condition (polygraph vs.
interrogation) on the outcome variables. This analysis was
performed using mixed models with the participants and
questions both entered as random factors. Marginal con-
trasts analysis (denoted by
) was also performed to clar-
ify the dierences between conditions. To allow for a better
quantication of the uncertainty associated with the eects,
as well as to increase the robustness to outliers and artefac-
tual ndings, all statistics were undertaken under the Bayes-
ian framework (Makowski et al., 2019), using informative
The sequence of each trial was the same for both condi-
tions. Participants were rst instructed to respond to a ques-
tion shown on a computer screen by verbally lying or telling
the truth (for half of the trials in each condition, i.e., n = 20).
In addition, as past studies have found associations between
lying behaviour and type of question phrasing (Walczyk &
Cockrell, 2022), each question was phrased either directly
(e.g., “What is your favourite sport?”), or indirectly (e.g.,
“Is your favourite sport Hockey?”) to reduce possible con-
founding eects. Following a short interval (0.7–1.5 s) to
allow time for response preparation, during which “Con-
necting…” was shown on the screen, a social or bio-
feedback cue (for Interrogation and Polygraph conditions
respectively) was displayed for a maximum of 10s or until a
response was given. Specically, participants had to provide
their answers verbally, and pressed the space key to signify
the end of their response. After another short interval (1.5–
2.5 s), during which “Disconnecting…” was presented on
the screen, participants were asked to rate their condence
in how convincing (i.e., likely to get judged as truthful) they
perceived their response to be on a visual analog scale.
In the Interrogation condition, participants had to pro-
vide their answer while receiving social feedback in the
form of a video stream of an examiner, and were informed
that the examiner would be evaluating the truthfulness
of their responses after observing them through the live
video feed. In actuality, the displayed video feed were pre-
recorded video clips of a confederate’s face (staying still
with minimal reactions, and with a medical mask, as the
experiment was run during COVID restrictions), and the
same video-clips were used with all participants. All partici-
pants reported believing that the stream was real and that the
examiner was really there during the debrieng.
In the Polygraph condition, participants had to provide
their answer while receiving bio-feedback in the form of
physiological signals (including cardiac activity - ECG, res-
piration - RSP, and electrodermal activity - EDA), of which
they were informed was live feedback of their own physi-
ological activity. In actuality, we displayed pre-recorded
video clips of a confederate’s physiological signals. All
participants reported believing that their own signals were
shown during the debrieng.
Three outcome variables were recorded for each trial
of the deception task, namely the participants’ condence
ratings that their answers (lies or truths) were convincing,
the response time (RT) between the question onset and the
participant’s key press (indicating the end of their verbal
answer), and the change in heart rate associated with the
response (within a window of 3.5 s).
Finally, on top of the deception task, we also measured
participants’ self-reported tendency to lie in their everyday
life using the Lie Scale (Makowski et al., 2021b), a 16-item
1 3
34218
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Current Psychology (2024) 43:34215–34226
leading to slower answers, regardless of whether they were
lies or truths. Given this absence of interaction with the type
of answers in any modality, this factor was not included in
subsequent analysis.
Feature reduction
The three Yoni-task dimensions and the two BES traits were
combined into a unique factor, labelled ToM (explaining
35.76% of variance). It was loaded by the cognitive (0.89),
aective (0.77), physical (0.45) Yoni dimensions, and the
aective (0.41) and cognitive (0.17) facets of the BES.
The eight MAIA dimensions and the three HCT com-
ponents were reduced to 4 factors (explaining 65.17% of
variance). The rst factor, labelled Interoception - Meta
(23.59%), was loaded primarily by Attention Regulation
(0.97), Self-regulation (0.63), Emotional awareness (0.60),
and Noticing (0.49) dimensions of the MAIA and the HCT
condence score (0.40). The second factor, labelled Intero-
ception - Listening (18.54%), was primarily loaded by the
Body Listening (0.92) and Trusting (0.53) MAIA dimen-
sions, and the Awareness (-0.60) and Condence (0.46)
HCT scores. The third factor, labelled Interoception - Focus
(12.07%), was primarily loaded by MAIA Not-Distracting
(0.87), Emotional Awareness (-0.40) and HCT Accuracy
(0.33). The fourth factor, labelled Interoception - Regula-
tion (10.97%), was primarily loaded by MAIA not-worrying
(0.71), HCT Accuracy (0.61) and MAIA Trusting (0.40).
Theory of mind
The higher composite To M score was signi-
cantly associated with a decreased condence in lies
(
β=0.19,95%CI [0.36,0.02] ,pd=98.47%
), spe-
cically in the polygraph condition. Figure 1 illustrates the
interindividual correlates of lying condence. The higher
composite To M score was also associated with slower answers
for lies (
β=0.42,95%CI [0.01,0.83] ,pd=97.67%
), spe-
cically in the polygraph condition. No signicant eect
was found with regards to dispositional lying traits, heart
rate, and RT for truths in both polygraph and interrogation
conditions.
Interoception
The higher Meta interoception score was sig-
nicantly associated with an increased con-
dence in lies, specically in the polygraph
condition (
β=0.20,95%CI [0.03,0.35] ,pd=98.98%
). It was also associated with faster answers for both lies
(
β=0.54,95%CI [0.93,0.15] ,pd=99.67%
) and
truths (
),
priors centred around 0 (
tConfidence (1,0,1)
,
tRT (1,0,3)
,
tHeartrate(1,0,8)
).
To maximize the signal-to-noise ratio, we performed a
feature reduction on our two groups of predictor variables
(namely, ToM and interoception) using factor analysis over
PCA, as the goal was to extract meaningful and consis-
tent factors, rather than merely maximizing the variance
explained. Then, we modelled the relationship between
these inter-individual composite scores (note that the analy-
sis for all individual variables is nonetheless included in the
analysis report) and the three outcome variables in interac-
tion with the condition (polygraph vs. interrogation). Finally,
we investigated the relationship between the deception scale
traits, and the ToM and interoception scores using Bayesian
correlations. All analyses and data have been made publicly
available. Therefore, in this manuscript, we will focus on
discussing signicant ndings, which - in this context - are
statistically reliable and in our opinion theoretically relevant
results.
The data analysis was carried out using R 4.2 (R Core
Team, 2022), brms (Bürkner, 2017), and the easystats col-
lection of packages (Lüdecke et al., 2019, 2021; Makowski
et al., 2019, 2020), and the physiological signal processing
was done using the default routines available in NeuroKit2
(Makowski et al., 2021a). Note that EDA was not further
analyzed as most participants did not yield any skin conduc-
tance responses - which we believe was partly caused by the
low temperature (with dry air-con air) of the experimental
room.
The analysis was not pre-registered (stemming from an
undergraduate’s nal year project), but the full reproducible
analysis script, statistical results report, and data, are avail-
able at [masked for blinding].
Results
Manipulation check
Compared to truths, lies were rated with less con-
dence (
=1.35,95%CI [1.46,1.23] ,pd=100%
), but no signicant dierence between the condi-
tions was found. On the other hand, the RT did not
dier between truths and lies, but was signicantly
slower in the polygraph condition for both conditions
(
=0.25,95%CI [0.62,0.41] ,pd=100%
). The heart rate
was signicantly more elevated during lies as compared
to truths (
=1.16,95%CI [0.57,1.73] ,pd=100%
), and
during interrogation as compared to the polygraph condi-
tion (
=4.84,95%CI [4.23,5.44] ,pd=100%
).
The indirect phrasing of the question only had a signicant
eect on RT (
β=0.36,95%CI [0.21,0.51] ,pd=100%
),
1 3
34219
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Current Psychology (2024) 43:34215–34226
(
r=0.50,95%CI [0.04,0.64] ,BF10 =3.48%
). No sig-
nicant association was found with heart rate in both
conditions.
The higher Focus interoception score was signicantly
associated with an increased condence in truths in the
polygraph (
β=0.17,95%CI [0.01,0.34] ,pd=97.16%
); a consistent pattern, although non-signicant, was
found for condence in truths in the interrogation con-
dition (
β=0.15,95%CI [0.02,0.32] ,pd=95.76%
). The Focus interoception score was also positively
correlated with the dispositional lying Ability trait
(
r=0.50,95%CI [0.22,0.74] ,BF10 =34.37%
). No sig-
nicant association was found with RT for lies and heart
rate in both conditions.
specically in the polygraph condition. No signicant asso-
ciation was found with regards to dispositional lying traits
and heart rate in both conditions.
The higher Listening interoception score was signicantly
associated with an increased condence in lies, in both the
polygraph (
β=0.43,95%CI [0.27,0.59] ,pd=100%
) and interrogation conditions
(
β=0.16,95%CI [0.01,0.32] ,pd=98.04%
). It
was also associated with faster answers for both lies
(
β=0.42,95%CI [0.82,0.03] ,pd=98.19%
) and
truths (
β=0.36,95%CI [0.76,0.03] ,pd=96.49%
), specically in the polygraph condition. The Lis-
tening interoception score was also positively corre-
lated with the dispositional lying Contextuality trait
Fig. 1 Interindividual corrrelates
of lying condence. The lines
shows the relationship (with 95%
CI), assessed via Bayesian mixed
models (*pd > 97%, **pd > 99%,
***pd > 99.9%), between the par-
ticipants’ interoceptive and ToM
composite scores and the con-
dence ratings of their responses.
Average lying condence (+/- 1
SD) for each participant within
the two experimental conditions
is displayed as points for descrip-
tive purposes as the models were
ran on individual trials
1 3
34220
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Current Psychology (2024) 43:34215–34226
interoception and ToM (Chiou & Lee, 2013; Gendolla &
Wicklund, 2009; Scadi Abbate et al., 2016; Wundrack &
Specht, 2023), our results suggest the two are negatively
linked. One possible interpretation of our ndings is that
people with stronger ToM abilities by default rely more on
their social skills and altercentric inference when lying (i.e.,
they focus on - and try to read - the other person). When
that mechanism is unavailable or unsuited (e.g., when there
is no person to lie to - but a “machine” in our case), their
corresponding lying ability decreases. However, in light of
the current eld of mixed ndings relating interoception and
ToM (Canino et al., 2022; Gao et al., 2019; Miller, 2015;
Shah et al., 2017), future studies are necessary to investi-
gate the interaction of these mechanisms in dierent social
contexts.
We also found that interoceptive abilities (as indicated
by the composite interoception scores) are correlated with
a higher condence in one’s lies in the polygraph condi-
tion, a condition in which the attention towards internal
reactions is fostered. Indeed, this is in line with previous
studies that found individuals with low interoception were
more averse to risk when reputational stakes were high, tell-
ing fewer egoistical lies (Vabba et al., 2022). In fact, Vabba
et al. (2022) further reported that people with high intero-
ception abilities were less likely to dier in risk-taking ten-
dencies, telling the same number of lies regardless of the
social stakes. Consistent with our results, Mohr et al. (2023)
found that individuals with high interoceptive accuracy
were more likely to make egocentric decisions. However,
in contrast to previous studies (Füstös et al., 2013; Owens et
al., 2018; Pinna & Edwards, 2020; Pollatos et al., 2007), we
did not nd any signicant relationship between individu-
als’ interoception scores and their heart rate changes during
their answers. This points toward a predominantly meta-
cognitive eect without necessarily an actual bodily regula-
tion (i.e., participants with good interoception feel that their
lies are more convincing, but do not actively attenuate their
bodily reactions).
Another possibility that should be tested in the future is
that of a mediating role of executive functions, given their
association with lying (Abe et al., 2007; e.g., Battista et al.,
2021) and interoception (Molnar-Szakacs & Uddin, 2022).
For instance, neuroscientic ndings investigating the cor-
relates of interoception have underlined the potential role
of the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and anterior insula
(AI) (Craig, 2009; Critchley et al., 2004; Khalsa et al., 2009;
Wang et al., 2019), both of which are often thought to be
activated during deception (Abe, 2011; Baumgartner et al.,
2013; Sip et al., 2008), and have been implicated in cogni-
tive processes associated with deception (such as cognitive
control, Molnar-Szakacs & Uddin, 2022; or conict detec-
tion, Kerns et al., 2004). It is thus possible that the positive
The higher Regulation interoception score was signicantly
associated with an increased condence in lies in both the
polygraph (
β=0.32,95%CI [0.14,0.51] ,pd=99.99%
) and the interrogation conditions
(
β=0.18,95%CI [0.00,0.36] ,pd=97.42%
), and with a
decreased condence in truth only in the polygraph condi-
tion (
β=0.1,95%CI [0.36,0.01] ,pd=97.16%
). No
signicant association was found with RT and heart rate in
both conditions (Fig. 2).
Discussion
The present study examined the contribution of ToM and
interoception on our ability to lie using a directed lie para-
digm with two conditions (“Interrogation” and “Polygraph”)
designed to enhance each of the two mechanisms. Inter-
estingly, we found that when participants’ responses were
perceived to be evaluated by a person (the interrogation
condition), instead of the lie detection machine (the poly-
graph condition), their response time for both lies and truths
were faster, and their heart rate was elevated. Although the
condition did not impact the subjective condence that par-
ticipants had in their answers, the pattern of results suggests
that believing one’s response is being evaluated by a person,
instead of a machine, could induce more fear, consequently
speeding up the response and increasing the physiological
arousal (Aylward et al., 2017). Alternatively, the slower
response in the polygraph condition could be explained by
the established attentional switching hypothesis, which pos-
its that an increase in attention towards internal signals and
managing one’s emotional reaction would confer less cog-
nitive resources available, thereby resulting in individuals
taking a longer time to respond (Arnold et al., 2019; Hana-
nia & Smith, 2010). While the impacts of external settings
on individuals’ responses warrant further investigation, the
results highlight how physiological responses can be easily
confounded by other factors, independent of whether one
is lying or telling the truth. For instance, the presence or
absence of the “interrogator”, or the saliency of the moral
nature of the task (e.g. Peleg et al., 2019, argues that the
polygraph test alone also acts as a “moral reminder,” fram-
ing the possibility that physiological arousal in a polygraph
context might be partially a reection of individuals’ atten-
tion directed to their own moral standards). By extension,
our study concurs with the controversial discourse sur-
rounding the use of physiological measures in deception
research (Oviatt et al., 2018; Rosky, 2013).
Furthermore, our results suggest that higher ToM abilities
were related to slower and less condent lies, but only in the
polygraph condition. While previous bodies of work have
reported mixed ndings regarding the association between
1 3
34221
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Current Psychology (2024) 43:34215–34226
with informative priors), future replication studies with
larger samples are warranted to conrm this rst investiga-
tion. Nonetheless, we believe our results to be credible as
we nd consistent patterns across various facets and mea-
sures (for instance, all interoceptive dimensions, although
distinct, share a similar trend) in line with theoretical
expectations. The statistical power could also explain the
relationship between interoceptive abilities and deception is
at least partially mediated by cognitive control abilities.
Although yielding promising results, the sample size of
this exploratory study is a source of concern. Although we
tried to mitigate it by (1) extracting more robust variables
(by combining multiple ones by means of feature reduction)
and (2) using a suited analysis approach (Bayesian statistics
Fig. 2 Summary of ndings showing the positive (green) and negative
(red) associations between interoception and theory of mind abilities
and deception skills, depending on the experimental condition. It high-
lights that ToM was related to less condent and slower lies in the
polygraph condition, and that specic interoceptive dimensions were
related to more condent lies
1 3
34222
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Current Psychology (2024) 43:34215–34226
interoception, could emerge with sucient statistical power
and valid measures.
Finally, there has been some research in the extant lit-
erature linking individual dierences in ToM and intero-
ception, as well as their neurophysiological underpinnings
(Gao et al., 2019; Ondobaka et al., 2017; Shah et al., 2017).
As such, it remains a possibility that the two constructs
interact in inuencing lying ability. However, much of this
research seems focused on emotion processing, which only
constitutes one of the hosts of cognitive processes required
to engage in deceptive behaviour (e.g., Shah et al., 2017).
Furthermore, given the overlaps in the literature surround-
ing ToM and empathy, it remains unclear whether intero-
ception works with ToM or empathy (specically aectivce
empathy) in the processing of emotions. Considering the
current gaps in literature, the present study investigates the
inuence of individual dierences in ToM and interocep-
tion on lying ability separately; this could be a useful rst
approach to delineate potential “main eects” of these pro-
cesses. Nevertheless, future studies (with a dierent design
and a larger sample) could investigate the interaction (and
possible mediation eects) between interoception and ToM
by means of, for instance, structural equation modelling.
In conclusion, this study is a rst step towards assess-
ing the contribution of ToM and interoception abilities in
deception, particularly in one’s ability to lie convincingly.
To this end, we introduced a new paradigm to delineate the
contribution of these mechanisms while remaining relevant
to applied elds of lie detection and criminology (in which
the experimental conditions nd echoing practices). Nota-
bly, our results provide some evidence that interoception
could be an important - and overlooked - process involved
in deception. Furthermore, our ndings extend and oer an
alternate perspective to the debatable use of polygraphs,
suggesting that its utility for lie detection is not only ques-
tionable, but could potentially selectively modulate decep-
tive skills depending on the cognitive and interoceptive
prole of the participant.
Acknowledgements [masked for blinding]
Funding The authors did not receive support from any organization
for the submitted work.
Data availability The material (stimuli generation code, experiment
code, raw data, analysis script with complementary gures and analy-
ses, etc.) for this research is available at [masked for blinding].
Declarations
Conflict of interest The authors declare that the research was con-
ducted in the absence of any commercial or nancial relationships that
could be construed as a potential conict of interest.
overall lack of results found in relation to heart rate, which
has a higher signal-to-noise ratio as compared to subjective
reports (such as condence scales). Additionally, one has to
note that the participants did not have strong incentive for
lying (there was no risk of losing the “reward” - i.e., student
credits), which might have further decreased the potential
eect sizes.
Another aspect to note is the strong reliance on self-
reported measures as outcome variables of lie ability (in
particular, the measure of answer condence, but also the
auto-questionnaires). This might conate meta-cognitive
abilities as well as dishonest answers. Although we tried
to include more objective measures, such as RT (although
it too was tied to the participants’ conscious decision to
press a key) and heart rate, future studies should attempt at
measuring objectively the answer (lie or truth) quality, for
instance by means of external examiners. Note that this is
not a limitation per se, as it answers a slightly dierent ques-
tion - what are the correlates of objective lying skills - rather
than of deception self-condence.
Additionally to the limitations pertaining to the measure
of lying ability, some also concern the measure of the pre-
dictor constructs, namely ToM and interoception. While
we tried to include a behavioral task as well as a subjec-
tive questionnaire for each, it has to be underlined that they
are notoriously dicult concepts to measure. In particular,
objective interoceptive accuracy was assessed using the
Heartbeat Counting Task (HCT). While the HCT used to be
considered as a gold standard and remains one of the most
commonly used measures (Desmedt et al., 2022), concerns
regarding its validity have been increasingly highlighted in
several studies as more research eorts are invested into
developing novel interoception tasks (Brener & Ring, 2016;
Desmedt et al., 2018, 2022; Legrand et al., 2022; Plans et
al., 2021; Ponzo et al., 2021). Future works should further
examine the relationship between interoception and lying
ability using measures with better psychometric properties.
Moreover, although the cognitive and aective compo-
nents of ToM and empathy share overlaps in the current
literature, and there is no consensus regarding how the
two concepts should be delineated, recent evidence none-
theless suggests ToM and empathy are necessarily distinct
constructs with separable underlying mechanisms (Kanske
et al., 2015). As such, future studies are warranted to fur-
ther investigate the associations between ToM and lying
ability using validated instruments sensitive to measuring
ToM (such as the Theory of Mind Inventory, Hutchins et
al., 2021). Furthermore, our application of feature reduction
as a noise-elimination measure could have over-simplied
the data. A more complex pattern of relationships, with dif-
ferent contributions of various subdimensions of ToM and
1 3
34223
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Current Psychology (2024) 43:34215–34226
Chen, W. G., Schloesser, D., Arensdorf, A. M., Simmons, J. M., Cui,
C., Valentino, R., Gnadt, J. W., Nielsen, L., Hillaire-Clarke, C.
S., Spruance, V., et al. (2021). The emerging science of intero-
ception: Sensing, integrating, interpreting, and regulating signals
within the self. Trends in Neurosciences, 44(1), 3–16.
Chiou, W. B., & Lee, C. C. (2013). Enactment of one-to-many com-
munication may induce self-focused attention that leads to dimin-
ished perspective taking: The case of facebook. Judgment and
Decision Making, 8(3), 372–380.
Connell, L., Lynott, D., & Banks, B. (2018). Interoception: The for-
gotten modality in perceptual grounding of abstract and concrete
concepts. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Bio-
logical Sciences, 373(1752), 20170143.
Craig, A. D. (2009). How do you feel—now? The anterior insula and
human awareness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(1), 59–70.
Critchley, H. D., Wiens, S., Rotshtein, P., Öhman, A., & Dolan, R.
J. (2004). Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness.
Nature Neuroscience, 7(2), 189–195.
Damasio, A. R. (1996). The somatic marker hypothesis and the possi-
ble functions of the prefrontal cortex. Philosophical Transactions
of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences,
351(1346), 1413–1420.
Desmedt, O., Luminet, O., & Corneille, O. (2018). The heartbeat
counting task largely involves non-interoceptive processes: Evi-
dence from both the original and an adapted counting task. Bio-
logical Psychology, 138, 185–188.
Desmedt, O., Van Den Houte, M., Walentynowicz, M., Dekeyser, S.,
Luminet, O., & Corneille, O. (2022). How does heartbeat count-
ing task performance relate to theoretically-relevant mental health
outcomes? A meta-analysis. Collabra: Psychology, 8(1), 33271.
Ditto, P. H., Pizarro, D. A., Epstein, E. B., Jacobson, J. A., & Mac-
Donald, T. K. (2006). Visceral inuences on risk-taking behavior.
Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 19(2), 99–113.
Dunn, B. D., Galton, H. C., Morgan, R., Evans, D., Oliver, C., Meyer,
M., Cusack, R., Lawrence, A. D., & Dalgleish, T. (2010). Listen-
ing to your heart: How interoception shapes emotion experience
and intuitive decision making. Psychological Science, 21(12),
1835–1844.
Evans, A. D., & Lee, K. (2011). Verbal deception from late childhood
to middle adolescence and its relation to executive functioning
skills. Developmental Psychology, 47(4), 1108.
Furman, D. J., Waugh, C. E., Bhattacharjee, K., Thompson, R. J., &
Gotlib, I. H. (2013). Interoceptive awareness, positive aect, and
decision making in major depressive disorder. Journal of Aec-
tive Disorders, 151(2), 780–785.
Füstös, J., Gramann, K., Herbert, B. M., & Pollatos, O. (2013). On
the embodiment of emotion regulation: Interoceptive awareness
facilitates reappraisal. Social Cognitive and Aective Neurosci-
ence, 8(8), 911–917.
Gallant, C. M., Lavis, L., & Mahy, C. E. (2020). Developing an under-
standing of others’ emotional states: Relations among aective
theory of mind and empathy measures in early childhood. British
Journal of Developmental Psychology, 38(2), 151–166.
Gao, Q., Ping, X., & Chen, W. (2019). Body inuences on social cog-
nition through interoception. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 2066.
Garnkel, S. N., Seth, A. K., Barrett, A. B., Suzuki, K., & Critchley, H.
D. (2015). Knowing your own heart: Distinguishing interoceptive
accuracy from interoceptive awareness. Biological Psychology,
104, 65–74.
Gendolla, G. H., & Wicklund, R. A. (2009). Self-focused attention,
perspective-taking, and false consensus. Social Psychology,
40(2), 66–72.
Gonzalez-Billandon, J., Aroyo, A. M., Tonelli, A., Pasquali, D., Sciutti,
A., Gori, M., Sandini, G., & Rea, F. (2019). Can a robot catch you
lying? A machine learning system to detect lies during interac-
tions. Frontiers in Robotics and AI, 6, 64.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons
Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing,
adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format,
as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the
source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate
if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this
article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless
indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not
included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended
use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted
use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright
holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit h t t p : / / c r e a t i v e c o m m o n s . o
r g / l i c e n s e s / b y / 4 . 0 / .
References
Abe, N. (2011). How the brain shapes deception: An integrated review
of the literature. The Neuroscientist, 17(5), 560–574.
Abe, N., Suzuki, M., Mori, E., Itoh, M., & Fujii, T. (2007). Deceiv-
ing others: Distinct neural responses of the prefrontal cortex and
amygdala in simple fabrication and deception with social interac-
tions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(2), 287–295.
Arnold, A. J., Winkielman, P., & Dobkins, K. (2019). Interoception
and social connection. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 2589.
Aylward, J., Valton, V., Goer, F., Mkrtchian, A., Lally, N., Peters, S.,
Limbachya, T., & Robinson, O. J. (2017). The impact of induced
anxiety on aective response inhibition. Royal Society Open Sci-
ence, 4(6), 170084.
Baron-Cohen, S. (1997). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and the-
ory of mind. MIT Press.
Barrett, L. F., & Simmons, W. K. (2015). Interoceptive predictions in
the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16(7), 419–429.
Battista, F., Otgaar, H., Mangiulli, I., & Curci, A. (2021). The role of
executive functions in the eects of lying on memory. Acta Psy-
chologica, 215, 103295.
Baumgartner, T., Gianotti, L. R., & Knoch, D. (2013). Who is honest
and why: Baseline activation in anterior insula predicts inter-indi-
vidual dierences in deceptive behavior. Biological Psychology,
94(1), 192–197.
Beaudoin, C., Leblanc, É., Gagner, C., & Beauchamp, M. H. (2020).
Systematic review and inventory of theory of mind measures for
young children. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 2905.
Bond, C. F., Jr., & DePaulo, B. M. (2008). Individual dierences in
judging deception: Accuracy and bias. Psychological Bulletin,
134(4), 477.
Bora, E., & Yener, G. G. (2017). Meta-analysis of social cognition in
mild cognitive impairment. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and
Neurology, 30(4), 206–213.
Brener, J., & Ring, C. (2016). Towards a psychophysics of intero-
ceptive processes: The measurement of heartbeat detection.
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological
Sciences, 371(1708), 20160015.
Burgoon, J. K., & Buller, D. B. (1994). Interpersonal deception:
III. Eects of deceit on perceived communication and nonver-
bal behavior dynamics. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 18(2),
155–184.
Bürkner, P. C. (2017). Brms: An R package for bayesian multilevel
models using Stan. Journal of Statistical Software, 80(1), 1–28.
https:/ /doi.or g/10.18 637/j ss.v080.i01
Canino, S., Raimo, S., Boccia, M., Di Vita, A., & Palermo, L. (2022).
On the embodiment of social cognition skills: The inner and outer
body processing dierently contributes to the aective and cogni-
tive theory of mind. Brain Sciences, 12(11), 1423.
1 3
34224
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Current Psychology (2024) 43:34215–34226
toolbox for neurophysiological signal processing. Behavior
Research Methods, 53(4), 1689–1696.
Makowski, D., Pham, T., Lau, Z. J., Raine, A., & Chen, S. (2021b).
The structure of deception: Validation of the lying prole ques-
tionnaire. Current Psychology, 42(5), 4001–4016.
Masip, J. (2017). Deception detection: State of the art and future pros-
pects. Psicothema, 29(2), 149–159.
Mehling, W. E., Price, C., Daubenmier, J. J., Acree, M., Bartmess, E.,
& Stewart, A. (2012). The multidimensional assessment of intero-
ceptive awareness (MAIA). PloS One, 7(11), e48230.
Miller, J. E. (2015). The connections between self-monitoring and
theory of mind.
Molnar-Szakacs, I., & Uddin, L. Q. (2022). Anterior insula as a gate-
keeper of executive control. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral
Reviews, 139, 104736.
Murphy, J., Catmur, C., & Bird, G. (2019). Classifying individual dif-
ferences in interoception: Implications for the measurement of
interoceptive awareness. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26(5),
1467–1471.
Ondobaka, S., Kilner, J., & Friston, K. (2017). The role of interoceptive
inference in theory of mind. Brain and Cognition, 112 , 64–68.
Oviatt, S., Schuller, B., Cohen, P. R., Sonntag, D., Potamianos, G.,
& Krüger, A. (2018). The handbook of multimodal-multisensor
interfaces: Signal processing, architectures, and detection of
emotion and cognition-volume 2. Association for Computing
Machinery; Morgan & Claypool.
Owens, A. P., Friston, K. J., Low, D. A., Mathias, C. J., & Critchley, H.
D. (2018). Investigating the relationship between cardiac intero-
ception and autonomic cardiac control using a predictive coding
framework. Autonomic Neuroscience, 210, 65–71.
Paulhus, D. L., & Williams, K. M. (2002). The dark triad of personal-
ity: Narcissism, machiavellianism, and psychopathy. Journal of
Research in Personality, 36(6), 556–563.
Peirce, J., Gray, J. R., Simpson, S., MacAskill, M., Höchenberger, R.,
Sogo, H., Kastman, E., & Lindeløv, J. K. (2019). PsychoPy2:
Experiments in behavior made easy. Behavior Research Methods,
51(1), 195–203.
Peleg, D., Ayal, S., Ariely, D., & Hochman, G. (2019). The lie deator-
the eect of polygraph test feedback on subsequent (dis) honesty.
Judgment & Decision Making, 16(6), 728–738.
Petzschner, F. H., Garnkel, S. N., Paulus, M. P., Koch, C., & Khalsa,
S. S. (2021). Computational models of interoception and body
regulation. Trends in Neurosciences, 44(1), 63–76.
Pinna, T., & Edwards, D. J. (2020). A systematic review of associations
between interoception, vagal tone, and emotional regulation:
Potential applications for mental health, wellbeing, psychologi-
cal exibility, and chronic conditions. Frontiers in Psychology,
11, 1792.
Plans, D., Ponzo, S., Morelli, D., Cairo, M., Ring, C., Keating, C.
T., Cunningham, A., Catmur, C., Murphy, J., & Bird, G. (2021).
Measuring interoception: The phase adjustment task. Biological
Psychology, 165, 108171.
Pollatos, O., Herbert, B. M., Matthias, E., & Schandry, R. (2007).
Heart rate response after emotional picture presentation is modu-
lated by interoceptive awareness. International Journal of Psy-
chophysiology, 63(1), 117–124.
Ponzo, S., Morelli, D., Suksasilp, C., Cairo, M., & Plans, D. (2021).
Measuring interoception: The CARdiac elevation detection task.
Frontiers in Psychology, 12.
R Core Team. (2022). R: A language and environment for statistical
computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. h t t p s : / / w w w
. R - p r o j e c t . o r g /
Rassin, E., Sergiou, C., van der Linden, D., & van Dongen, J. (2023).
Psychopathy as a predisposition to lie hedonistically. Psychology
Crime & Law, 1–8.
Hanania, R., & Smith, L. B. (2010). Selective attention and attention
switching: Towards a unied developmental approach. Develop-
mental Science, 13(4), 622–635.
Hutchins, T. L., Lewis, L., Prelock, P. A., & Brien, A. (2021). The
development and preliminary psychometric evaluation of the
theory of mind inventory: Self report—adult (ToMI: SR-adult).
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 51, 1839–1851.
Jollie, D., & Farrington, D. P. (2006). Development and validation of
the basic empathy scale. Journal of Adolescence, 29(4), 589–611.
Kanske, P., Böckler, A., Trautwein, F. M., & Singer, T. (2015). Dissect-
ing the social brain: Introducing the EmpaToM to reveal distinct
neural networks and brain–behavior relations for empathy and
theory of mind. Neuroimage, 122, 6–19.
Kerns, J. G., Cohen, J. D., MacDonald, I. I. I., Cho, A. W., Stenger, R.
Y., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Anterior cingulate conict monitoring
and adjustments in control. Science, 303(5660), 1023–1026.
Khalsa, S. S., Rudrauf, D., Feinstein, J. S., & Tranel, D. (2009). The
pathways of interoceptive awareness. Nature Neuroscience,
12(12), 1494–1496.
Kireev, M., Korotkov, A., Medvedeva, N., & Medvedev, S. (2013).
Possible role of an error detection mechanism in brain processing
of deception: PET-fMRI study. International Journal of Psycho-
physiology, 90(3), 291–299.
Lee, J. Y. S., & Imuta, K. (2021). Lying and theory of mind: A meta-
analysis. Child Development, 92(2), 536–553.
Legrand, N., Nikolova, N., Correa, C., Brændholt, M., Stuckert, A.,
Kildahl, N., Vejlø, M., Fardo, F., & Allen, M. (2022). The heart
rate discrimination task: A psychophysical method to estimate the
accuracy and precision of interoceptive beliefs. Biological Psy-
chology, 168, 108239.
Lenggenhager, B., Azevedo, R. T., Mancini, A., & Aglioti, S. M.
(2013). Listening to your heart and feeling yourself: Eects of
exposure to interoceptive signals during the ultimatum game.
Experimental Brain Research, 230(2), 233–241.
Levine, E. E., & Lupoli, M. J. (2022). Prosocial lies: Causes and con-
sequences. Current Opinion in Psychology, 43, 335–340.
Levine, E. E., & Schweitzer, M. E. (2014). Are liars ethical? On the
tension between benevolence and honesty. Journal of Experimen-
tal Social Psychology, 53, 107–117.
Levine, E. E., & Schweitzer, M. E. (2015). Prosocial lies: When decep-
tion breeds trust. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision
Processes, 126, 88–106.
Levine, T. R., Serota, K. B., Shulman, H., Clare, D. D., Park, H. S.,
Shaw, A. S., Shim, J. C., & Lee, J. H. (2011). Sender demeanor:
Individual dierences in sender believability have a powerful
impact on deception detection judgments. Human Communica-
tion Research, 37(3), 377–403.
Lüdecke, D., Waggoner, P., & Makowski, D. (2019). Insight: A unied
interface to access information from model objects in R. Journal
of Open Source Software, 4(38), 1412. h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 2 1 1 0 5 / j
o s s . 0 1 4 1 2
Lüdecke, D., Ben-Shachar, M., Patil, I., Waggoner, P., & Makowski,
D. (2021). Performance: An R package for assessment, compari-
son and testing of statistical models. Journal of Open Source Soft-
ware, 6(60), 3139. https:/ /doi.or g/10.21 105/j oss.03139
Makowski, D., Ben-Shachar, M. S., & Lüdecke, D. (2019). bayest-
estR: Describing eects and their uncertainty, existence and sig-
nicance within the bayesian framework. Journal of Open Source
Software, 4(40), 1541.
Makowski, D., Ben-Shachar, M., Patil, I., & Lüdecke, D. (2020).
Methods and algorithms for correlation analysis in R. Journal of
Open Source Software, 5(51), 2306. h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 2 1 1 0 5 / j o s
s . 0 2 3 0 6
Makowski, D., Pham, T., Lau, Z. J., Brammer, J. C., Lespinasse, F.,
Pham, H., Schölzel, C., & Chen, S. (2021a). NeuroKit2: A python
1 3
34225
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Current Psychology (2024) 43:34215–34226
American Journal of Emergency Medicine, 13(5), 514–517. h t t p s
: / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 1 0 1 6 / 0 7 3 5 - 6 7 5 7 ( 9 5 ) 9 0 1 6 0 - 4
Talwar, V., Gordon, H. M., & Lee, K. (2007). Lying in the elementary
school years: Verbal deception and its relation to second-order
belief understanding. Developmental Psychology, 43(3), 804.
Talwar, V., Crossman, A., & Wyman, J. (2017). The role of execu-
tive functioning and theory of mind in children’s lies for another
and for themselves. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 41,
126–135.
Vabba, A., Porciello, G., Panasiti, M. S., & Aglioti, S. M. (2022).
Interoceptive inuences on the production of self-serving lies in
reputation risk conditions. International Journal of Psychophysi-
ology, 177, 34–42.
Verigin, B. L., Meijer, E. H., Bogaard, G., & Vrij, A. (2019). Lie preva-
lence, lie characteristics and strategies of self-reported good liars.
PloS One, 14(12), e0225566.
Viji, D., Gupta, N., & Parekh, K. H. (2022). History of deception
detection techniques. Proceedings of International Conference
on Deep Learning, Computing and Intelligence, 373–387.
von Mohr, M., Finotti, G., Esposito, G., Bahrami, B., & Tsakiris, M.
(2023). Social interoception: Perceiving events during cardiac
aerent activity makes people more suggestible to other people’s
inuence. Cognition, 238, 105502.
Walczyk, J. J., & Cockrell, N. F. (2022). To err is human but not decep-
tive. Memory & Cognition, 50(1), 232–244.
Walczyk, J. J., Mahoney, K. T., Doverspike, D., & Grith-Ross, D. A.
(2009). Cognitive lie detection: Response time and consistency of
answers as cues to deception. Journal of Business and Psychol-
ogy, 24, 33–49.
Walczyk, J. J., Harris, L. L., Duck, T. K., & Mulay, D. (2014). A social-
cognitive framework for understanding serious lies: Activation-
decision-construction-action theory. New Ideas in Psychology,
34, 22–36.
Wang, X., Wu, Q., Egan, L., Gu, X., Liu, P., Gu, H., Yang, Y., Luo, J.,
Wu, Y., Gao, Z., et al. (2019). Anterior insular cortex plays a criti-
cal role in interoceptive attention. Elife, 8, e42265.
Weiss, S., Sack, M., Henningsen, P., & Pollatos, O. (2014). On the
interaction of self-regulation, interoception and pain perception.
Psychopathology, 47(6), 377–382.
Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of
theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child
Development, 72(3), 655–684.
Williams, E. F., Pizarro, D., Ariely, D., & Weinberg, J. D. (2016). The
valjean eect: Visceral states and cheating. Emotion, 16(6), 897.
Wundrack, R., & Specht, J. (2023). Mindful self-focus–an interaction
aecting theory of mind? Plos One, 18(2), e0279544.
Zvi, L., & Elaad, E. (2018). Correlates of narcissism, self-reported lies,
and self-assessed abilities to tell and detect lies, tell truths, and
believe others. Journal of Investigative Psychology and Oender
Proling, 15(3), 271–286.
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to juris-
dictional claims in published maps and institutional aliations.
Riggio, R. E., Tucker, J., & Widaman, K. F. (1987). Verbal and nonver-
bal cues as mediators of deception ability. Journal of Nonverbal
Behavior, 11, 126–145.
Roheger, M., Brenning, J., Riemann, S., Martin, A. K., Flöel, A., &
Meinzer, M. (2022). Progression of socio-cognitive impairment
from healthy aging to alzheimer’s dementia: A systematic review
and meta-analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 140,
104796.
Rosky, J. W. (2013). The (f) utility of post-conviction polygraph test-
ing. Sexual Abuse, 25(3), 259–281.
Rubin, D. C., Schrauf, R. W., & Greenberg, D. L. (2003). Belief and
recollection of autobiographical memories. Memory & Cogni-
tion, 31, 887–901.
Scadi Abbate, C., Boca, S., & Gendolla, G. H. (2016). Self-aware-
ness, perspective-taking, and egocentrism. Self and Identity,
15(4), 371–380.
Schandry, R. (1981). Heart beat perception and emotional experience.
Psychophysiology, 18(4), 483–488.
Sebastian, C. L., Fontaine, N. M., Bird, G., Blakemore, S. J., De Brito,
S. A., McCrory, E. J., & Viding, E. (2012). Neural processing
associated with cognitive and aective theory of mind in ado-
lescents and adults. Social Cognitive and Aective Neuroscience,
7(1), 53–63.
Seth, A. K., Suzuki, K., & Critchley, H. D. (2012). An interoceptive
predictive coding model of conscious presence. Frontiers in Psy-
chology, 2, 395.
Shah, P., Catmur, C., & Bird, G. (2017). From heart to mind: Linking
interoception, emotion, and theory of mind. Cortex; a Journal
Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 93,
220.
Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., & Aharon-Peretz, J. (2007). Dissociable pre-
frontal networks for cognitive and aective theory of mind: A
lesion study. Neuropsychologia, 45(13), 3054–3067.
Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Harari, H., Aharon-Peretz, J., & Levkovitz, Y.
(2010). The role of the orbitofrontal cortex in aective theory of
mind decits in criminal oenders with psychopathic tendencies.
Cortex; a Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System
and Behavior, 46(5), 668–677.
Sip, K. E., Roepstor, A., McGregor, W., & Frith, C. D. (2008).
Detecting deception: The scope and limits. Trends in Cognitive
Sciences, 12(2), 48–53.
Sip, K. E., Skewes, J. C., Marchant, J. L., McGregor, W. B., Roep-
stor, A., & Frith, C. D. (2012). What if i get busted? Deception,
choice, and decision-making in social interaction. Frontiers in
Neuroscience, 6, 58.
Sternglanz, R. W., Morris, W. L., Morrow, M., & Braverman, J.
(2019). A review of meta-analyses about deception detection. The
Palgrave Handbook of Deceptive Communication, 6, 303–326.
Sugawara, A., Terasawa, Y., Katsunuma, R., & Sekiguchi, A. (2020).
Eects of interoceptive training on decision making, anxiety, and
somatic symptoms. BioPsychoSocial Medicine, 14, 1–8.
Takuma, K., Hori, S., Sasaki, J., Shinozawa, Y., Yoshikawa, T., Handa,
S., Horikawa, M., & Aikawa, N. (1995). An alternative limb
lead system for electrocardiographs in emergency patients. The
1 3
34226
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
Terms and Conditions
Springer Nature journal content, brought to you courtesy of Springer Nature Customer Service Center GmbH (“Springer Nature”).
Springer Nature supports a reasonable amount of sharing of research papers by authors, subscribers and authorised users (“Users”), for small-
scale personal, non-commercial use provided that all copyright, trade and service marks and other proprietary notices are maintained. By
accessing, sharing, receiving or otherwise using the Springer Nature journal content you agree to these terms of use (“Terms”). For these
purposes, Springer Nature considers academic use (by researchers and students) to be non-commercial.
These Terms are supplementary and will apply in addition to any applicable website terms and conditions, a relevant site licence or a personal
subscription. These Terms will prevail over any conflict or ambiguity with regards to the relevant terms, a site licence or a personal subscription
(to the extent of the conflict or ambiguity only). For Creative Commons-licensed articles, the terms of the Creative Commons license used will
apply.
We collect and use personal data to provide access to the Springer Nature journal content. We may also use these personal data internally within
ResearchGate and Springer Nature and as agreed share it, in an anonymised way, for purposes of tracking, analysis and reporting. We will not
otherwise disclose your personal data outside the ResearchGate or the Springer Nature group of companies unless we have your permission as
detailed in the Privacy Policy.
While Users may use the Springer Nature journal content for small scale, personal non-commercial use, it is important to note that Users may
not:
use such content for the purpose of providing other users with access on a regular or large scale basis or as a means to circumvent access
control;
use such content where to do so would be considered a criminal or statutory offence in any jurisdiction, or gives rise to civil liability, or is
otherwise unlawful;
falsely or misleadingly imply or suggest endorsement, approval , sponsorship, or association unless explicitly agreed to by Springer Nature in
writing;
use bots or other automated methods to access the content or redirect messages
override any security feature or exclusionary protocol; or
share the content in order to create substitute for Springer Nature products or services or a systematic database of Springer Nature journal
content.
In line with the restriction against commercial use, Springer Nature does not permit the creation of a product or service that creates revenue,
royalties, rent or income from our content or its inclusion as part of a paid for service or for other commercial gain. Springer Nature journal
content cannot be used for inter-library loans and librarians may not upload Springer Nature journal content on a large scale into their, or any
other, institutional repository.
These terms of use are reviewed regularly and may be amended at any time. Springer Nature is not obligated to publish any information or
content on this website and may remove it or features or functionality at our sole discretion, at any time with or without notice. Springer Nature
may revoke this licence to you at any time and remove access to any copies of the Springer Nature journal content which have been saved.
To the fullest extent permitted by law, Springer Nature makes no warranties, representations or guarantees to Users, either express or implied
with respect to the Springer nature journal content and all parties disclaim and waive any implied warranties or warranties imposed by law,
including merchantability or fitness for any particular purpose.
Please note that these rights do not automatically extend to content, data or other material published by Springer Nature that may be licensed
from third parties.
If you would like to use or distribute our Springer Nature journal content to a wider audience or on a regular basis or in any other manner not
expressly permitted by these Terms, please contact Springer Nature at
onlineservice@springernature.com
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any citations for this publication.
Article
Full-text available
Our judgements are often influenced by other people’s views and opinions. Interoception also influences decision making, but little is known about its role in social influence and particularly, the extent to which other people may influence our decisions. Across two experiments, using different forms of social influence, participants judged the trustworthiness of faces presented either during the systolic phase of the cardiac cycle, when baroreceptors convey information from the heart to the brain, or during diastolic phase, when baroreceptors are quiescent. We quantified the extent to which participants changed their minds (as an index of social influence) following the social feedback, in order to compare two competing hypotheses. According to the Arousal- Confidence Hypothesis, cardiac signals create a context of heightened bodily arousal that increases confidence in perceptual judgements. People should, therefore, be less subject to social influence during systole. By contrast, according to the Uncertainty-Conformity Hypothesis, cardiac signals increase neural noise and sensory attenuation, such that people should display greater effects of social influence during systole, as they then underweight private interoceptive signals in favour of the external social information. Across two studies that used different kind of social interactions, we found that participants changed their minds more when faces were presented at systole. Our results, therefore, support the Uncertainly-Conformity hypothesis and highlight how cardiac afferent signals contribute to shape our social decision-making in different types of social interactions.
Article
Full-text available
Is thinking about oneself helpful or harmful for understanding other people? The answer might depend on how a person thinks about themself. Mindfulness is one prominent construct that seems to affect the quality and content of a person’s thoughts about themselves in the world. Thus, we hypothesize that the relationship between self-focus and Theory of Mind (ToM) is moderated by mindfulness. We evaluate our hypothesis with a large cross-sectional dataset (N = 543) of native and non-native German and English speakers using OLS and MM-estimated robust multiple regression analysis. We found a small but robust self-focus × mindfulness interaction effect on ToM so that there was a significant positive relation between self-focus and ToM for more mindful individuals and no significant relation for less mindful individuals. The findings support our hypothesis that mindfulness moderates the relationship between self-focus and ToM performance. We discuss the limitations and differences between the present study and previous findings.
Article
Full-text available
Despite its controversial status, the lie detection test is still a popular organizational instrument for credibility assessment. Due to its popularity, we examined the effect of the lie-detection test feedback on subsequent moral behavior. In three studies, participants could cheat to increase their monetary payoff in two consecutive phases. Between these two phases the participants underwent a mock polygraph test and were randomly given Deception Indicated (DI) or No Deception Indicated (NDI) assigned feedback. Then, participants engaged in the second phase of the task and their level of dishonesty was measured. Study 1 showed that both NDI and DI feedback (but not the control) reduced cheating behavior on the subsequent task. However, Study 2 showed that the mere presence of the lie-detection test (without feedback) did not produce the same effect. When the role of the lie detector as a moral reminder was cancelled out in Study 3, feedback had no effect on the magnitude of cheating behavior. However, cheaters who were given NDI feedback exhibited a lower level of physiological arousal than cheaters who were given DI feedback. These results suggest that lie detection tests can be used to promote honesty in the field, and that, while feedback type does not affect the magnitude of cheating, NDI may allow people to feel better about cheating.
Article
Full-text available
A specific interpretation of embodiment assigns a central role to the body representations (BR) in cognition. In the social cognition domain, BR could be pivotal in representing others’ actions and states. However, empirical evidence on the relationship between different BR and social cognition, in terms of Theory of Mind (ToM), in the same sample of participants is missing. Here, this relationship was explored considering individual differences in the action-oriented BR (aBR), nonaction-oriented BR (NaBR), and subjective predisposition toward internal bodily sensations (interoceptive sensibility, ISe). Eighty-two healthy adults were given behavioral measures probing aBR, NaBR, ISe, and affective/cognitive ToM. The results suggest that NaBR, which mainly relies on exteroceptive signals, predicts individual differences in cognitive ToM, possibly because it can allow differentiating between the self and others. Instead, the negative association between affective ToM and ISe suggests that an alteration of the internal body state representation (i.e., over-reporting interoceptive sensations) can affect emotional processing in social contexts. The finding that distinct aspects of the body processing from within (ISe) and from the outside (NaBR) differently contribute to ToM provides empirical support to the BR role in social cognition and can be relevant for developing interventions in clinical settings.
Article
Full-text available
Executive control is a complex high-level cognitive function that relies on distributed brain circuitry. We propose that the anterior insular cortex plays an under-appreciated role in executive processes, acting as a gatekeeper to other brain regions and networks by virtue of primacy of action and effective connectivity. The flexible functional profile of the anterior insular subdivision renders it a key hub within the broader midcingulo-insular ‘salience network’, allowing it to orchestrate and drive activity of other major functional brain networks including the medial frontoparietal ‘default mode network’ and lateral frontoparietal ‘central executive network’. The microanatomy and large-scale connectivity of the insular cortex positions it to play a critical role in triaging and integrating internal and external multisensory stimuli in the service of initiating higher-order control functions. Multiple lines of evidence scaffold the novel hypothesis that, as a key hub for integration and a lever of network switching, the anterior insula serves as a critical gatekeeper to executive control.
Article
Background Trajectories of decline across different socio-cognitive domains in healthy older adults and in pathological aging conditions have not been investigated. This was addressed in the present systematic review and meta-analysis. Methods MEDLINE, Web of Science Core Collection, CENTRAL, and PsycInfo were searched for studies investigating social cognition across four domains (Theory of Mind, ToM; emotion recognition, ER; Social-decision making, SD; visual perspective taking, VPT) in healthy older individuals, individuals with subjective and mild cognitive impairment (SCD, MCI) and Alzheimer’s disease (AD). Random-effects meta-analyses were conducted. Results Of 8,137 screened studies, 132 studies were included in the review. ToM and ER showed a clear progression of impairment from normal aging to AD. Differential patterns of decline were identified for different types of ToM and ER. Conclusion This systematic review identified progression of impairment of specific socio-cognitive abilities, which is the necessary pre-requisite for developing targeted interventions. We identified a lack of research on socio-cognitive decline in different populations (e.g., middle age, SCD and MCI-subtypes) and domains (SDM, VPT). Registration CRD42020191607, https://www.crd.york.ac.uk/prospero/
Chapter
Deception among humans has always existed. It has now become a part of our nature and has led to the research of various deception detection techniques. The longing for lie detection has been a never-ending age-old practice. The techniques have varied from measuring physical values of the person’s body (heart rate, body temperature) to analysing the small expressions that the eyes made to micro-changes of the facial muscles, frequency of the utterance of the words, etc. In our survey, we have covered various techniques that have been employed in professional practice to techniques which are still developing and can show promising results in the near future. These techniques are widely useful in legal and lawsuits, police offices, crime and court trails. From the study of polygraph which dominated the twentieth century to facial and speech imaging techniques which are now widely used for various domains, the paper presents a comprehensive study of all the recognized techniques in lie detection and comparison among the different approaches. Finally, the paper summarizes the deception techniques and provides discussion on the existing problems and trends.KeywordsLie detectionPolygraphMultimodal deception detectionPsychology
Article
Bodily signals influence high-order cognitive and emotional processes, including social decision making. Here, we examined whether individual differences in the capacity to read signals from inside (interoception) and outside the body (exteroception) predicted participants' (dis)honesty. Deceptive behavior was measured in a card game where participants were tempted to lie to another person for financial gain in two conditions, i.e., under high vs. low risk of being seen by the other player (reputation risk). Participants completed the Heartbeat Counting Task (cardiac interoception) and a variation of the Body-Scaled Action Task (visual exteroception). Overall, when participants believed their reputation was at risk (i.e., the other player knew they lied) they told significantly less egoistic lies compared to when their choices were secret. This effect was significantly moderated by cardiac interoception. While low interoceptive participants told less egoistic lies when their reputation was at risk, high cardiac interoceptive participants did not change their behavior depending on the reputation risk conditions. We also found that cardiac interoception and visual exteroception did not correlate. Together our findings suggest that although integrated, interoception and exteroception constitute distinct facets of corporal awareness, and that high cardiac interoception shapes moral behavior by making people less concerned about their social reputation during spontaneous lies.