Content uploaded by Evi Petersen
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Evi Petersen on Nov 20, 2024
Content may be subject to copyright.
Content uploaded by Evi Petersen
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Evi Petersen on Nov 20, 2024
Content may be subject to copyright.
Evi Petersen
Move(d) outdoors
Exploring the role of social relational emotions
in human–nature relationships
Dissertation for the
degree of Ph.D
Culture studies
To the evaluation committee
2024
Petersen II The role of emotions in connecting in and to nature
Scientific environment
This doctoral thesis was conducted between November 2017 and February 2024. During this
time, I was a PhD student enrolled in the Culture Studies program and employed at the
Department of Sports, Physical Education, and Outdoor Studies at the University of South-
Eastern Norway (USN). The PhD project was officially supervised by Professor Jan Ove
Tangen (USN), Professor Thomas Schubert from the University of Oslo (UiO, Department of
Psychology), and Professor Per Ingvar Haukeland (USN).
In April 2019, I received an Erasmus+ stipend to extend the data collection in collaboration
with Dr. Gunnar Liedtke at the University of Hamburg (Institute of Human Movement
Science) in Germany. Thereafter, I was a visiting scholar at Massey University (School of
Sport, Exercise and Nutrition) in New Zealand and collaborated with Professor Andy Martin
from January to July 2020. Since April 2022, I have been working as an assistant professor
at the Faculty of Educational and International Studies at Oslo Metropolitan University
(OsloMET).
Throughout the PhD project, I was a member of the USN-based research groups Research
on Friluftsliv, Society and Sustainability and Kropp, Samfunn og Bevegelseskulturer.
Furthermore, I participated in the international research group Kama Muta Lab and the
academic cluster of the Horizon Europe-funded GoGreenRoutes project. Since April 2022, I
have been a member of the research group Sustainability, Nature, Health, and Movement at
OsloMET.
Written with and in Norway, Germany, and New Zealand.
Assessment Committee:
First opponent: Professor Terry Hartig, Uppsala University
Second opponent: Professor Kirsten Kaya Roessler, University of Southern Denmark
Administrator: Professor Bengt Karlsson, University of South-Eastern Norway
Petersen III The role of emotions in connecting in and to nature
Dedication
To Ivar and Johne, who guided me towards this PhD path. Among many other things, you
taught me ting tar tid (things take time) and gjort er gjort og spist er spist (what's done is
done and what's eaten is eaten), which became two essential mental mantras throughout this
PhD journey. Your kind, passionate, and playful spirits will continue to inspire me and many
others on our friluftsliv adventures to come.
Petersen IV The role of emotions in connecting in and to nature
Preface
Standing at the top of a mountain, encountering an animal, seeing the sun rise, or simply
feeling the cool breeze from the ocean: each of these moments, some deeply imprinted in
memory, can elicit strong emotions in humans. Sometimes, such experiences change, either
immediately or over time, how we feel about our relationship with others, ourselves, and not
least, nature itself. How and when does this phenomenon occur, what are the implications of
such connecting moments, and why do people differ in their responses? Questions like these
sparked my scholarly curiosity and marked the beginning of this thesis.
Petersen V The role of emotions in connecting in and to nature
Acknowledgements
I am grateful to the Culture Studies PhD program and the Department of Sports, Physical
Education and Outdoor Studies at USN. They entrusted me with a research fellowship and
provided valuable learning opportunities through useful PhD courses and teaching
opportunities. I would like to extend my gratitude to my new and dear colleagues at the
Institute of Early Childhood Education at OsloMET for their kind support during the final
phase of this PhD.
I want to thank all the participants who shared their rich experiences with me throughout this
PhD. Along with their outdoor teachers and instructors, they made the data collection for this
project possible.
Taking the path of this doctoral thesis was an adventure. I was lucky to be accompanied by
many wonderful travel companions who provided inspiration, advice, and distraction
whenever needed. I would like to thank certain individuals who particularly influenced the
development of this thesis:
First, I would like to thank my main supervisor, Jan Ove Tangen, for inviting me to pursue my
research interests. Your open-mindedness and trust towards my ideas and approaches
inspired me to be more explorative. I am particularly thankful for your emotional support
towards the end of this journey.
Second, I am grateful to my second supervisor, Thomas Schubert. You challenged me to
become more autonomous in my research and writing. Thank you for introducing me to the
Kama Muta Research Lab and all its members, who contributed with valuable feedback and
exciting discussions over the years.
To my dear friend Alan P. Fiske, I am incredibly thankful for your endless encouragement
and our wonderful and inspiring talks at Stockfleths and online. ಥ‿ಥ
I also want to thank Helga S. Løvoll and Per Ingvar Haukeland for providing very helpful
comments and suggestions on the synopsis towards the end of this thesis.
A heartfelt thank you to my friend Annette Bischoff, with whom I have shared tears of both
joy and sadness. Your support has been present at every level throughout my entire PhD
journey.
Petersen VI The role of emotions in connecting in and to nature
Kirsten W. Houe, I could always count on your “Heia Heia Heia!”. Thanks for being such a
supportive boss, colleague, and friend.
To Andy Martin, thank you for welcoming me to Massey University, Palmy, Castle Point, and
the weekly 700 kids’ triathlon. I am grateful that your pragmatic advice prevented this thesis
from reaching the volume of a Harry Potter saga.
To Gunnar Liedtke, you have been a great friend and colleague throughout this trip. Without
you, much of my data collection would not have been possible. I am looking forward to more
green collaboration.
Heaps of thanks go to Pip and Ross, our friends and hosts during COVID-19, when New
Zealand went into lockdown and our planned overnight stay turned into several weeks.
Thank you for the inspiring conversations, garden games, and enlightening quizzes over
delicious dinners.
This journey would not have been the same without my co-stipends and friends. So, I would
like to thank the entire Bø-crew for an adventurous time and for sharing the rollercoaster ups
and downs of a PhD life over many cups of (sometimes quite bad) coffee. Special thanks go
to Maria Karlsen, who made the shut-up-and-suffer sessions less painful. Further, I want to
thank Helga Norheim for her comradeship throughout the entire PhD journey. Daniel, thanks
for the R-support from Jena. Martina and Giorgia, I would not have finished my PhD in a
million years without your support. And to Caro and Esther, thank you for your unwavering
friendship, which knows no geographical boundaries.
I also want to thank my wonderful, loving, and caring family, both in Germany and Norway. I
am especially grateful to my mother Waltraut and my stepfather Holger for always supporting
me, no matter what.
Finally, thank you so much, Iver. You have been a tremendous source of strength and
inspiration. You have provided me with love, a lot of patience, and constructive criticism
whenever needed. New chapters and adventures – here we come!
Petersen VII The role of emotions in connecting in and to nature
Abstract
Across cultures, the relationship between humans and nature affects both the well-being of
humans and the natural environment. While the concept of nature connectedness is
recognized as an important topic in this regard, little is known about the psychological
processes that establish and foster it. Positioned at the intersection of environmental
psychology and outdoor studies, this article-based thesis adopts a critical realism perspective
to explore how social relational emotions, such as kama muta (≈ being moved) and awe, are
specifically significant to the process of connecting in and to nature.
Article I is a conceptual paper that provides the theoretical foundation for the empirical
studies of the Articles II–VI. It proposes that the same emotions underpin social
connectedness and nature connectedness, and suggests that social relational emotions are
crucial to understanding how humans connect to nature. Drawing on relational models theory
(RMT), the paper specifically highlights the potential role of the emotion of kama muta in the
context of connecting to nature. It concludes by presenting five key implications for future
research and practice within this scope.
Article II explores the social relational emotion of kama muta and its role in accompanying
connecting processes in the context of nature. Follow-up interviews were conducted with
German university students six weeks after they participated in an outdoor field trip, which
took either place in Germany or Norway. The participants were asked to describe their
experiences using photographs from the trips as prompts. The thematic analysis of the
interview transcripts indicates that kama muta was elicited in the context of connecting to
nature, others, the self, and human-made materials or artifacts. Moreover, the article posits
that the method of photo elicitation serves as an effective catalyst in terms of helping
participants to recall and articulate the emotional aspects of their experiences outdoors.
Article III investigates the outdoor practice of solo – that is, being intentionally solitary in
nature. This study aims to understand how affective phenomena are part of this specific
outdoor practice, and how emotions, specifically, are linked to pathways of well-being.
Debriefing group interviews were conducted with adults from Norway, Germany, and New
Zealand, and content-analyzed by applying the PERMA-V framework in combination with a
data-driven approach. The main findings suggest that conscious experiences of emotions
and connecting processes during solo are related to both hedonic and eudaimonic well-being
pathways. The secondary findings indicate that personal (such as prior experiences),
situational (like weather), and socio-cultural (including nationality) aspects are relevant
contextual factors regarding the different well-being pathways. The article’s findings
underscore the value of solo experiences for well-being across various practical fields,
including outdoor education and nature-based therapy.
Petersen VIII The role of emotions in connecting in and to nature
Article IV examines the impact of social relational emotions, specifically awe and kama
muta, on nature connectedness during solo experiences. Data were collected and analyzed
from 132 adults in Norway, Germany, and New Zealand, who participated in one of nine
outdoor trips, using pre- and post-experience questionnaires. These questionnaires
evaluated the participants' experiences of awe and kama muta alongside other affective
states, their sense of nature connectedness, well-being, and environmental attitudes.
Employing linear mixed models to the data analysis, the findings reveal a significant increase
in nature connectedness attributable to solo experiences across groups. Moreover, the
intensity of experiencing awe or kama muta during solo was found to predict the increase in
nature connectedness. Secondary findings suggest a medium to strong positive correlation
between the two emotions, and that the influence of personal factors, beyond gender and
age, on nature connectedness merits further investigation.
This doctoral thesis introduces a novel interdisciplinary framework, both theoretical and
empirical, for understanding affective phenomena within the context of human-nature
relationships. It specifically investigates the function and characteristics of social relational
emotions in moments of connecting in and to nature. Among these emotions, kama muta and
awe are emphasized for their crucial roles in the process of connecting to nature. The thesis
employs various methods and approaches to studying emotions in the context of the
outdoors. By offering empirical insight into how certain emotion-related aspects of outdoor
experiences are linked to well-being and, to some extent, pro-environmental attitude, this
research provides valuable practical implications for educational outdoor programs and
nature-based therapy.
Keywords: outdoor experience, friluftsliv, human–nature relationship, nature connectedness,
social relational emotions, kama muta, awe, solo, photo elicitation, well-being, sustainability
Petersen IX The role of emotions in connecting in and to nature
Sammendrag (Norwegian)
Denne doktorgradsavhandlingen befinner seg i skjæringspunktet mellom miljøpsykologi og
friluftslivsstudier, og utforsker betydningen av sosial-relasjonelle emosjoner i menneskers
forhold til naturen. To sentrale sosial-relasjonelle emosjoner avhandlingen særlig fokuserer
på, er kama muta (≈ beveget, rørt) og awe (≈ ærefrykt).
Artikkel I introduserer et nytt teoretisk perspektiv som argumenterer for at både sosial
tiknytning og naturtilknytning er forankret i de samme emosjonene. Videre hevdes det, basert
på relational models theory (RMT), at sosial-relasjonelle emosjoner spiller en nøkkelrolle i
prosessene der mennesker knytter bånd til naturen. Denne konseptuelle artikkelen legger
det teoretiske fundamentet for de påfølgende empiriske studiene i Artiklene II–VI.
Artikkel II utforsker emosjonen kama muta i konteksten av naturtilknytning, gjennom
individuelle oppfølgingsintervjuer med tyske studenter om deres turer i naturen. Den
tematiske analysen understreker kama mutas betydning i forhold til tilknytning til naturen,
andre mennesker, seg selv, og menneskeskapte artefakter. Bruken av egne bilder viser seg
å være en effektiv metode for å erindre og rapportere om emosjonelle utendørsopplevelser.
Artikkel III undersøker hvordan sosial-relasjonelle emosjoner integreres i helhetsopplevelser
i naturen, med fokus på soloopplevelser – det å være alene i naturen. Innholdsanalyse av
gruppeintervjuer med voksne fra Norge, Tyskland og New Zealand indikerer at positive
emosjoner og tilknytningsprosesser under soloopplevelser er særlig relevant for velvære.
Artikkel IV undersøker hvorvidt opplevelser av kama muta eller ærefrykt under soloturer i
naturen kan forutsi en økning i tilknytningen til naturen. Studien, som baserer seg på
spørreskjemaer utfylt av voksne deltakere fra Tyskland, Norge og New Zealand, bruker
blandete lineære modeller for å analysere data både før og etter turene. Resultatene
indikerer at disse to emosjonene, som varierer fra middels til sterk korrelasjon, kan forutsi en
forsterket naturtilknytning.
Denne avhandlingen foreslår en tverrfaglig teoretisk tilnærming for å forstå emosjoner som er
fundamentale for menneske-natur-relasjoner. Den tilbyr metodologiske anbefalinger for
forskning på emosjoner utendørs og gir innsikt i hvordan emosjonelle aspekter ved
utendørsopplevelser, spesielt i solo kontekster, er koblet til velvære og miljøspørsmål, med
praktiske implikasjoner for friluftsliv i utdannings-, fritids- og terapeutiske sammenhenger.
Stikkord: Friluftsliv, menneske-natur-forhold, naturtilknytning, sosial-relasjonelle emosjoner,
kama muta, ærefrykt, solo i naturen, velvære, bærekraft
Petersen X The role of emotions in connecting in and to nature
List of publications1
Article I
Petersen, E., Fiske, A. P., & Schubert, T. W. (2019). The role of social relational emotions for
human–nature connectedness. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, Article 2759.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02759
Article II
Petersen, E., & Martin, A. J. (2021). Kama muta (≈ being moved) helps connect people in
and to nature: A photo elicitation approach. Ecopsychology, 13(1), 37-47.
https://doi.org/10.1089/eco.2020.0032
Article III
Petersen, E., Bischoff, A., Liedtke, G., & Martin, A. J. (2021). How does being solo in nature
affect well-being? Evidence from Norway, Germany and New Zealand. International
Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 18(15), Article 7897.
https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18157897
Article IV
Petersen, E. (XXXX). Connecting with nature in solitude – Awe and kama muta predict
nature-connectedness during nature solo. In preparation for submission to Current
Research in Ecological and Social Psychology.
____________
1 Reprints were made with permission from the respective publishers.
Petersen XII The role of emotions in connecting in and to nature
List of figures
Figure 1. Philosophical framework for the PhD project, derived from critical realism ............37
Figure 2. Illustration based on the stratified ontology of critical realism .................................39
Figure 3. Illustration of the overall methodological approach of this thesis ............................43
Figure 4. Overview of outdoor field trips’ included data (Articles II–IV) .................................47
Figure 5. Prerequisites for identifying kama muta in interview transcripts (Article II) .............52
Figure 6. Integrating the findings: a hypothetical conceptual model ......................................67
Petersen XIII The role of emotions in connecting in and to nature
Abbreviations
AR
Authority ranking
CS
Communal sharing
EM
Equality matching
EON
Experience of nature
ICF
Informed consent form
MP
Market pricing
LMMs
Linear mixed models
MCAR
Missing completely at random
NESH
Norwegian Committee for Research Ethics in the Social Sciences
and the Humanities
NSD
The Norwegian Social Science Data Service
QCA
Qualitative content analysis
PANAS
Positive and Negative Affect Schedule
PERMA-V
Positive Emotion, Engagement, Relationships, Meaning, Accomplishment
(or Achievement) - Vitality
PWB
Psychological well-being
RMT
Relational models theory
SWB
Subjective well-being
TA
Thematic analysis
USN
University of South-Eastern Norway
WEIRD
Western, educated, industrialized, rich, and democratic
Petersen XIV The role of emotions in connecting in and to nature
Table of Contents
Scientific environment ........................................................................................................ II
Dedication ............................................................................................................................III
Preface ................................................................................................................................ IV
Acknowledgements ............................................................................................................. V
Abstract ............................................................................................................................. VII
Sammendrag (Norwegian) ................................................................................................. IX
List of publications ............................................................................................................. X
List of tables ....................................................................................................................... XI
List of figures .................................................................................................................... XII
Abbreviations ................................................................................................................... XIII
Table of Contents ............................................................................................................. XIV
1 Introduction .................................................................................................................. 1
1.1 Research scope and aims ....................................................................................... 2
1.2 Thesis structure ....................................................................................................... 4
1.3 Personal and academic stance ............................................................................... 5
2 Theoretical background: human–nature relationships ............................................. 8
2.1 Conceptualizing nature and the experience of it ...................................................... 8
2.2 Factors that shape one’s relationship with nature ...................................................10
2.3 Nature engagement and well-being (flourishing) ....................................................14
2.4 Nature connectedness – concepts, measures, and research .................................16
2.5 The role of emotions in connecting to nature: The literature gap ............................20
2.5.1 Emotions as outcome variables of nature engagement ..........................................20
2.5.2 Emotions as predictors of well-being and pro-environmental aspects .....................21
2.5.3 Conceptual proposals linking emotions and nature connectedness ........................22
3 Theoretical framework: Emotion approach to human–nature relationships ..........24
3.1 Connecting with nature as psychological process ..................................................24
3.2 Notion of emotion: nature or nurture? .....................................................................27
3.3 Functions of social relational emotions ...................................................................30
3.4 Relational models theory ........................................................................................32
3.5 Kama muta .............................................................................................................33
3.6 Awe ........................................................................................................................35
4 The thesis’s research approach .................................................................................36
4.1 Philosophical stance...............................................................................................36
4.2 Methodology ..........................................................................................................42
4.3 Conceptual approach in Article I .............................................................................44
4.4 Empirical approaches in Articles II-IV .....................................................................45
4.4.1 Recruitment, sampling, and participants .................................................................45
4.4.2 Research sites and contexts ..................................................................................46
4.5 Data collection methods .........................................................................................47
Petersen XV The role of emotions in connecting in and to nature
4.5.1 Interviews ...............................................................................................................47
4.5.2 Survey questionnaire ..............................................................................................49
4.6 Data analysis..........................................................................................................50
4.6.1 Qualitative data analysis ........................................................................................51
4.6.1.1 Thematic analysis ...........................................................................................51
4.6.1.2 Qualitative content analysis .............................................................................52
4.6.2 Quantitative data analysis ......................................................................................53
4.7 Methodological considerations: strengths and limitations .......................................54
4.7.1 Truth value .............................................................................................................55
4.7.2 Applicability ............................................................................................................56
4.7.3 Consistency ...........................................................................................................56
4.7.4 Neutrality ................................................................................................................57
4.8 Ethical considerations ............................................................................................58
5 Summary of findings ..................................................................................................61
5.1 Article I ...................................................................................................................61
5.2 Article II ..................................................................................................................62
5.3 Article III .................................................................................................................63
5.4 Article IV .................................................................................................................64
5.5 Integration of findings .............................................................................................65
6 Discussion ...................................................................................................................68
6.1 The role of social relational emotions in connecting in and to nature ......................68
6.1.1 Interplay between social relational emotions ..........................................................71
6.1.2 Commonalities and variations in participants’ responses ........................................73
6.2 Researching emotions in outdoor settings ..............................................................78
6.2.1 Memory-experience gap .........................................................................................78
6.2.2 Lexical fallacy .........................................................................................................81
6.2.3 Context complexity .................................................................................................82
6.3 Solo in nature as context for emotion elicitation and connecting .............................83
6.3.1 Contrast between solo in nature and daily routines ................................................84
6.3.2 Multi-sensuous engagement during solo ................................................................85
6.4 Implications for future research and practice ..........................................................87
6.4.1 Theoretical implications ..........................................................................................87
6.4.2 Methodological implications ....................................................................................90
6.4.3 Implications for practice ..........................................................................................91
7 Conclusion ..................................................................................................................93
References ..........................................................................................................................94
Article I .............................................................................................................................. 118
Article II ............................................................................................................................. 125
Article III ............................................................................................................................ 139
Article IV............................................................................................................................ 161
Appendices ....................................................................................................................... 195
Petersen 1 The role of emotions in connecting in and to nature
1 Introduction
For many people, life in contemporary societies unfolds in cities or urban areas and is
embedded in digital technological daily routines. This way of living can be characterized by
the impression of an accelerated pace of living (Rosa, 2010), shaping how humans
experience and interact with their environments, including the natural ones. Related to this
phenomenon, many individuals, particularly in Western societies, find their engagement with
nature diminishing, although some individuals experience a contrary effect, and feel more
drawn to the outdoors. This divergence in engagement with nature has become particularly
apparent at both the individual and national level since the outbreak of the COVID-19
pandemic (Nigg et al., 2023).
Exploring the relationship between humans and nature is crucial to addressing two pressing
issues in modern society: human well-being and environmental conservation. Research
increasingly highlights the significant role that specific natural settings, landscapes, and
features play in enhancing human health and well-being (e.g., Hartig et al., 2014; McMahan
& Estes, 2015), while anthropogenic climate change and biodiversity loss present profound
societal concerns (e.g., Whitmee et al., 2015). This dynamic between human well-being and
environmental conservation emphasizes the need for a fundamental shift in behavior at both
the individual and the collective level. Consequently, stakeholders from various sectors,
including individuals, institutions, and policymakers, are actively seeking advice from a wide
array of experts. This diverse group spans researchers, landscape planners, wilderness
therapists, outdoor educators, artists, and recently, virtual reality specialists, among others.
Together, they explore how to improve access to natural environments and understand
which types of nature-based experiences can act as catalysts for the necessary shifts at both
personal and societal levels.
As for the contribution of research in this regard, different disciplines, including environmental
psychology, outdoor studies, ecophilosophy, and human geography, have made fruitful
contributions to understanding how experiences in nature can benefit both humans and
natural environments. Among others, the concept of nature connectedness (≈ the notion of a
subjectively perceived connection to nature) has been identified as a central psychological
concept in the pursuit of discovering pathways through which contact with nature fosters
health and well-being benefits (e.g., Capaldi et al., 2014; Pritchard et al., 2019). Additionally,
recent studies have shown that nature connectedness is related to pro-environmental
attitudes and behaviors (e.g., Mackay & Schmitt, 2019; Sierra-Barón et al., 2023; Whitburn et
al., 2019). Although there is solid evidence that nature connectedness is influenced by
experiences with nature (Lengieza & Swim, 2021; Sheffield et al., 2022; Tam, 2013a), little
research has examined the specific factors that contribute to this phenomenon in depth.
Petersen 2 The role of emotions in connecting in and to nature
While it is crucial to identify nuanced outcomes and benefits of nature connectedness for
different societal groups (Barragan‐Jason et al., 2021; Ives et al., 2018), equally important is
understanding how nature connectedness is elicited and facilitated. Recent approaches have
begun to explore the psychological processes of connecting to nature on an individual level,
highlighting the role of emotion-related mechanisms (e.g., Jacobs & McConnell, 2022;
Lengieza & Swim, 2021; Moreton, 2018a). Yet, much of the existing research has treated
emotions primarily as outcomes, such as components of hedonic well-being, overlooking
their critical function in establishing, regulating, and sustaining relationships (see Subsection
2.5). This oversight presents a significant knowledge gap, which this doctoral dissertation
aims to address.
1.1 Research scope and aims
The scientific scope of this PhD project primarily lies at the intersection between
environmental psychology and outdoor studies. Underpinned by a critical realist approach, as
detailed in Section 4.1, this thesis contributes to an emerging body of theoretical and
empirical work exploring the human-nature relationship. It specifically focuses on how
outdoor leisure experiences, especially the experience of solitude in nature, can foster a
psychological connection with nature. Building on emerging research that underscores the
crucial role of certain emotions in interpersonal connection processes, this project seeks to
investigate to which extent these processes may similarly facilitate connections in and to
nature. As such, the main research question of this thesis is the following:
What role do social relational emotions play in the process of establishing and fostering
connections in and to nature?
To explore this main question, the thesis considers four sub-questions, investigated through
one conceptual and three empirical studies. Specifically, this thesis aims to investigate which
emotions influence how people (adults) connect in and to nature, how emotions are
entangled with aspects of the overall nature experience, and how emotions are related to
outcomes like an increase in well-being and pro-environmental attitudes and behavioral
choices. The overall research design for this thesis features an emergent exploratory
sequential approach (Creswell & Clark, 2018), in which different (qualitative and quantitative)
research methods are applied. Table 1 provides a brief overview of the four research articles.
An in-depth account of the philosophical stance, methodology, and methods is provided in
Chapter 4.
Petersen 3 The role of emotions in connecting in and to nature
Table 1. Overview of research articles in the PhD project
Study
purpose
To gain a deeper and more holistic understanding of the emotional mechanisms
involved in the psychological process of connecting in and to nature, and their
influence on outcomes such as well-being and environmental attitude.
Title
Article I Article II Article III Article IV
The role of social
relational emotions
for human–nature
connectedness
Kama muta
(≈ being moved)
helps connect
people in and to
nature: A photo
elicitation approach
How does being
solo in nature
affect well-being?
Evidence from
Norway, Germany,
and New Zealand
Connecting with
nature in solitude –
Awe and kama
muta predict
nature-
connectedness
during nature solo
Published
in Journal
Frontiers in
Psychology (2019)
Ecopsychology
(2021; ahead of
print: 2020)
International
Journal of
Environmental
Research and
Public Health
(2021)
In preparation for
submission to
Current Research
in Ecological and
Social Psychology
(XXXX)
Sub-
research
questions
What is the
potential role of
social relational
emotions for
connecting to
nature?
How does the
social relational
emotion of kama
muta link to
processes of
connecting in and
to nature?
What are the
pathways through
which solo affects
well-being across
different national
samples?
Do the social
relational emotions
of kama muta and
awe predict an
increase in nature
connectedness?
Study
design
Theoretical,
conceptual
approach
Empirical,
qualitative
approach
Empirical,
qualitative
approach
Empirical,
quantitative
approach
Sample Peer-reviewed
empirical and
conceptual articles
University students
(N = 27) from
Germany
Adults (N = 40)
from Norway,
Germany, and New
Zealand
Adults (N = 132)
from Norway,
Germany, and
New Zealand
Data Database and key-
word search
(+ preliminary data,
research group
discussions)
Follow-up
photograph elicited
semi-structured
interviews
Group-based
debriefing
interviews
Survey
questionnaires
(PANAS, Nature
Connectedness,
kama muta, awe;
SWB, environm.
attitude)
Data
analysis
Explicit abductive
reasoning
Reflexive thematic
analysis
(following Braun &
Clarke, 2006)
Qualitative content
analysis
(following Rädiker
& Kuckartz, 2020)
Statistical analysis
(correlations, linear
mixed models)
Due to the explorative and data-driven approach of this research and the years it took to
develop this thesis, an attentive reader will recognize two content-related shifts in the thesis's
focus while reading the first to the fourth paper. The first shift occurred while broadening the
perspective from solely connecting to nature in the first paper to connecting in and to nature
Petersen 4 The role of emotions in connecting in and to nature
in the second paper. In nature refers to situations, in which participants did not experience
kama muta (≈ being moved) while connecting to elements of nature but instead while
connecting to others, the self, and human-made materials or artifacts. In this context, I would
also like to highlight that, from Article II onwards, I frame emotions as positively and
negativity perceived instead of using the terms positive and negative emotions. This is
because I came to recognize that it would be misleading to describe an emotion as positive
or negative. For instance, shame, guilt or embarrassment are generally experienced as
rather unpleasant, but these emotions serve a useful function by helping an individual to
recognize their positioning in a social community (Bastin et al., 2016). The second shift
occurs between Article II and Article III. In the beginning, I focused on collecting data from
German and Norwegian participants regarding their overall outdoor experiences, which
included various forms of outdoor activities and settings. Later, I narrowed this focus to
specifically examining their experiences of being solitary in nature (solo). This change in
focus was based on the results of Article II, which showed that participants were more likely
to establish connections to nature (or at least were aware of it) when they were alone.
Narrowing the perspective to the solo context provided me with the opportunity to consider a
culturally and nationally diverse sample by additionally collecting data in New Zealand, which
in turn lead to a broader applicability of the findings. Although my planned data collection in
New Zealand was compromised by the COVID-19 lockdowns, I was able to gather a portion
of the planned dataset, which are included in the Articles III and IV.
1.2 Thesis structure
This article-based thesis integrates the four research articles into the synopsis (kappe).
Following the introduction and presentation of the research aim and scope, Chapter 1 will
continue by detailing my personal stance as a PhD candidate and providing insights into my
pre-understanding when approaching and conducting this PhD project. Chapter 2 presents
the background and current literature on the human–nature relationship and offers a
rationale for the integration of the emotion perspective into the human-nature relationship
literature. Chapter 3 outlines the theoretical and conceptual framework used to approach the
phenomena of emotions. Chapter 4 presents the research approach for this thesis, detailing
the philosophical stance, methodological approach, and specific methods and procedures for
each article. It also provides critical reflections on the strengths and limitations of the
methodological approaches across articles, as well as an elaboration on ethical
considerations. In Chapter 5, the main findings from Articles I – IV are summarized, followed
by an integration of the findings. Chapter 6 encompasses the discussion of the findings from
the four articles regarding current research literature on the topic, and it outlines the
theoretical, methodological, and practical implications of the thesis’s contribution. Finally,
Chapter 7 provides some concluding remarks.
Petersen 5 The role of emotions in connecting in and to nature
1.3 Personal and academic stance
"And into the forest I go, to lose my mind and find my soul." (John Muir)
The hilly path of this doctoral journey led me, literally and metaphorically, into the forest and
beyond. I can relate to Muir's poem, as I encountered several moments in which I felt that I
was losing my mind and eventually had some epiphanies along the way. Although the
experiences explored in this PhD project belong to the study participants, their experiences
shaped mine. Thus, on the one hand, this thesis is a scientific exploration of affective
phenomena in the context of the outdoors. On the other hand, it is also a narrative of the
paths that I took to complete this doctoral project. By consciously focusing on the latter in this
section, I aim to provide readers with some insight on how the questions addressed,
methods used, and findings interpreted in this thesis were colored by my personal and
professional socio-cultural background, preconceptions, and motivations. By reflexively
engaging with the personal knowledge that I gained along the way, I acknowledge the notion
that a researcher can never be unbiased or completely objective and that perspectives are
always partial and contextual, as they shift during the research process (Gadamer, 2013).
This form of reflexivity writing can be used as a tool to improve the quality, rigor, and validity
of research and is especially crucial whenever subjective interpretation is at the core of a
data analysis (Creswell & Creswell, 2018). First, I outline my socio-cultural background and
consider how I developed an interest in the thesis topic. Then, I draw on my experiences and
realizations as a novice researcher, gaining a nuanced perspective throughout the research
process.
"Of all the paths you take in life, make sure a few of them are dirt." (John Muir)
I did not follow a straightforward path to becoming a PhD candidate, enrolling in Culture
Studies, and researching emotions within the scope of outdoor experiences. In hindsight,
however, I can retrace some of the crossroads that led me in this direction. By the time I
submit this dissertation, I will be in the middle of my thirties. I am a white European female
academic immigrant with a middle-class background who lives in Norway. My move from
Germany to Norway six years ago informed this thesis (especially the data collection) and
indicates a position of privilege due to my mobility. Focusing on nature-based experiences
within a leisure context also portrays me as an individual with the privilege of leisure time and
access to nature landscapes, as the narratives will further reveal.
Growing up in the northern part of Germany, I lived close to the North Sea, which provided
many early physical interactions with what I call nature. From swimming in the ocean at high
tide to crawling through the mudflats at low tide, picking mushrooms at the dike, and playing
in the nearby forest, many of my childhood paths were made of dirt; the natural environment I
Petersen 6 The role of emotions in connecting in and to nature
grew up in ensured this. I was excited about physical activities, and those that took place
outdoors especially filled me with joy. Following my passion for such activities, I chose to
study human movement science with a minor in psychology at the University of Hamburg.
During my undergraduate studies, I had the opportunity to participate in an Erasmus
exchange to Kristiansand, Norway in 2010. While studying friluftsliv (≈ outdoor studies), I
noticed some differences in how outdoor activities were practiced in Norway and in
Germany. Consequently, I began to develop a sense for and an interest in the socio-cultural
domain of outdoor experiences. I quickly realized that friluftsliv, as a societal phenomenon,
occupies a central position in Norwegian culture (e.g., Goksøyr, 1994; Gurholt, 2008;
Nedrelid, 1991). At the same time, I recognized the positive effects of this bodily practice on
the mental well-being of myself and others and how its impact could differ according to a
person's socio-cultural background, prior experiences, and expectations. Later, I enrolled in a
master's program in Hamburg and specialized in health research. During my master's
degree, I traveled to Nanaimo, Canada in 2013 to study sustainable leisure management.
This experience widened my perspective on how outdoor practices are situated in broader
socio-cultural settings and how the human–nature connection is not only a matter of human
health and well-being but also linked to environmental concerns. My academic and personal
interest in outdoor studies gradually grew, as did my interest in psychological phenomena
and culture, which eventually let me pursue this doctoral thesis.
To some extent, my academic interest in psychology, health, and sustainability are all
reflected in the questions that underpin this thesis. Through my academic mobility, I had the
opportunity to encounter different socio-cultural notions of being outdoors. In particular, the
First Nations culture in British Columbia (Canada) challenged my anthropocentric approach
to the concept of nature, as did my later experiences with Māori culture in New Zealand.
Otherwise, my personal and professional socio-cultural development has been profoundly
influenced by Western culture, which shaped both my understanding of and relationship with
nature. While positively perceived childhood memories experienced in nature certainly
contributed to my interest in nature-based activities in adulthood, my physical engagement
during childhood experiences builds on an understanding of nature as an accessible and
relatively safe playground for pleasure, adventure, and enjoyment. Such descriptions
resonate with many of my Generation Y peers and have even been acknowledged as part of
outdoor leisure attitudes and behaviors among women in the Scandinavian context (Gurholt,
1999). As demonstrated by my choice of Muir quotations in this section, I have inherited
Western romantic ideas about nature and a conservationist attitude towards it. For instance, I
am aware that I value typical aesthetic and awe-inducing elements of nature that elicit the
notion of freedom, which is prevalent in the modern romantic view of nature (Jay & Schraml,
2009). These preferences undoubtedly motivated my later experiences of journeying in wild
or sublime landscapes on my own and with groups, which again, contributed to my decision
to conduct this research.
Petersen 7 The role of emotions in connecting in and to nature
In retrospect, I wonder if I was not reflected enough about my privilege to view and treat the
investigation of nature experiences as activities with the opportunity for restoration and
enjoyment throughout the research process. In fact, I paid little attention to the historically
speaking often less-romantic side of human–nature relationships and the continuing
struggles that people experience in coping with natural threats and disasters, such as
earthquakes, tornados, and flooding. Nevertheless, due to my personal leisure perspective, I
share socio-cultural similarities with many of the project's participants, and my personal
experiences with and in nature may not significantly differ from those of the participants,
especially the Germans and Norwegians. In Section 4.7, I reflect on how this circumstance
may has benefitted and, in some ways, challenged the data analysis.
My doctoral journey officially began in November 2017 and took me to different geographical
locations and intellectual spaces. Initially, I was intrigued by emerging research that aimed to
empirically demonstrate how nature affects human health and well-being. Approaching my
project like a quest, I was motivated to identify the mechanism by which connecting to nature
affects the human mind. Engaging with the scientific literature for a while led me realize that
this phenomenon was not attributable to one mechanism. Instead, I discovered an intricate
interplay of ecological, physiological, psychological, and sociocultural factors, all contributing
to the complexity of human-nature relationships. Largely due to my engagement with the
Kama Muta Lab (emotion research group), I understood that affective phenomena play a
crucial role in interpersonal relationships but are highly underexplored in human–nature
relationships. I figured that exploring emotion mechanisms by focusing on the subjective
level could be a crucial first step in investigating connecting moments in nature. In my
theoretical approach, I am mainly influenced by disciplines in the social sciences, mostly due
to my academic background. I did not explicitly draw on theories from academic disciplines
within the humanities, such as emotional geography (e.g., Davidson et al., 2007),
ecophilosophy (e.g., Naess, 1977) anthropology (e.g., Ingold, 2000; Milton, 2002, 2005), and
phenomenology (e.g., Merleau-Ponty, 1962). Nevertheless, my engagement with this
literature shaped my research approach for this dissertation, as it particularly influenced my
consideration of the embodied, emplaced, and culturally situated aspects of the project's
findings and discussions, especially concerning solo experiences in nature (Section 6.3).
Lastly, I want to acknowledge that I wrote this thesis under the umbrella of the Culture
Studies program at the University of South-Eastern Norway. Debates in the interdisciplinary
field of culture studies principally view the human body as marked by history—that is, socially
and culturally mediated or produced (Jane & Barker, 2016). Therefore, researchers in culture
studies sometimes dismiss physiological and psychological explanations of human behavior
as biological reductionism and prefer to take a stance within cultural constructionism. By
contrast, the critical realist perspective adopted in this thesis (see Section 4.1) illustrates a
relatively atypical approach to the scope of the Culture Studies program by offering
explanations and thought experiments that suggest the integration of culture studies and
environmental and social psychology in a productive alliance. Such an alliance collapses
Petersen 8 The role of emotions in connecting in and to nature
several dualistic distinctions such as the mind and the body, culture and biology, and
cognition and emotion (see Section 3.2). Consequently, this thesis contributes to debates in
the field of culture studies in at least two ways: (1) by understanding emotions as both nature
and nurture and (2) by foregrounding the socio-cultural components of outdoor practice.
2 Theoretical background: human–nature
relationships
In the introduction, I established why human–nature relationships are at the center of current
societal discourses on both well-being and environmental issues. Without doubt, human–
nature relationships are complex and can be explored from a range of perspectives. Thus, to
explore and discuss how emotions are relevant to establishing and fostering such
relationships, this chapter provides a contextual foundation for this endeavor. Specifically, I
present relevant perspectives, theoretical concepts, current research approaches, and
studies in environmental psychology and outdoor studies related to the phenomenon of
humans connecting to nature, primarily in the context of leisure. Presenting the concept of
nature connectedness and its relationship to well-being and pro-environmental attitude and
behavior will expand the scope of Article I. The final section (2.5) positions this thesis within
the existing body of literature by reviewing the studies that have empirically and conceptually
investigated the relationship between emotions and connecting to nature, and by identifying
the associated knowledge gaps that give rise to the initial research questions of this thesis.
2.1 Conceptualizing nature and the experience of it
The term nature is at the core of this thesis. As a concept, nature is notoriously difficult to
define, as its meaning changes depending on the historical and socio-cultural context in
which it is used (Glacken, 1992). Moreover, the term nature is not consistently translated
between languages and, within any language, its meaning depends on the speaker and the
specific discourse (Ducarme & Couvet, 2020). Nevertheless, a particularly dominant notion in
Western cultures, and often adopted in social sciences (Dove, 2013), is that the meaning of
nature encompasses a contrast with whatever we see as human or human-made. This is
reflected in thoughts of dualisms such as culture versus nature or humans versus the natural
world. In this sense, the term often refers to perceivable aspects of a physical world, such as
flora and fauna, qualities of air, water and weather, including the landscapes that comprise
these and which show the influence of geological processes (Hartig et al., 2014, p. 208). In
acknowledgement that the current geological epoch is increasingly labeled as the
Anthropocene (Crutzen, 2002), the idea of nature as a pure and timeless place characterized
by the absence of humans has become problematic and unrealistic (Lorimer, 2015).
Furthermore, the ecological foundation of human existence and our inseparable relationship
Petersen 9 The role of emotions in connecting in and to nature
with the physical elements of nature (e.g., air, water, and solar radiation) are undeniable
(Collado et al., 2017). These realizations have led to the recognition of alternative terms such
as the more-than-human world, coined by David Abram (1996). Abram advocates a
theoretical perspective that views humans as an inseparable part of the natural world.
However, there is an argument to be made that using such a term makes a semantic
distinction between humans and the natural world and thereby inadvertently perpetuates the
very dichotomy it seeks to unify. Thus, due to the wide semantic diversity in the use of the
term nature, which is both historically and geographically situated, any definition of nature
must rely on contextual mental representations (models) and theoretical concepts (Demeritt,
2002; Ellen, 1996). Using nature as a scientific construct, I explain how it and related terms
are applied in this thesis.
To a large extent, the definition of nature used in this thesis relies on the meaning that
individual participants in the empirical studies (Articles II–IV) ascribed to their ideas and
understandings of nature (mental models). According to Kaplan and Kaplan (1989), the
experience of nature (EON) is what is relevant to focus on, rather than the objective definition
of the term nature. Experience can be understood as “(the process of getting) knowledge or
skills from doing, seeing or feeling things" or “something that happens to you that affects how
you feel” (Cambridge Dictionary, n.d.). Consequently, experiences of nature incorporate an
active element of change and should be differentiated from mere contact with nature
(Clayton et al., 2016) or nature exposure (Tomasso & Chen, 2022). Subjective perceptions
and evaluations of nature likely vary over time, in part because humans are affected by their
socio-cultural environment (Hartig, 1993; Proctor, 1998). For instance, the practice and value
of friluftsliv is an essential element in the identity of Nordic countries (Gelter, 2000), which
influences subjective experiences of nature, especially among Norwegians (Article III).
Although most of my study participants were situated within a Western cultural context, their
experiences of what they perceived as nature were diverse and marked by differences in
personal preferences and prior experiences. Interestingly, participants often did not regard
nature as separate from visible human influence; some of the environmental areas that
participants perceived as natural showed substantial human influence through the presence
of rural neighborhoods and cultivated agricultural landscapes, such as the German
landscape in Article II. However, especially among the Norwegians, the experience of being
in nature was linked to spending time in vast areas of remote and somewhat uncultivated
land with minimal visible human influence, which could be referred to as wilderness in
English (Gurholt, 2014, p. 234).
While the term nature area is generally used to describe nature on a large scale, green
space is more often used by policymakers in reference to urban parks or trees along streets
and gardens (van den Berg et al., 2018, p. 58). Thus, descriptions of nature also
substantially intersect with natural environments, another term that is often
interchangeably use in the literature (Hartig et al., 2014). For the purpose of this thesis,
natural environments are operationalized as "areas containing elements of living systems
Petersen 10 The role of emotions in connecting in and to nature
that include plants and non-human animals across a range of scales and degrees of human
management, from a small urban park through to relatively pristine wilderness" (Bratman et
al., 2012, p. 120), as it is often done in conservation research. Kaplan and Kaplan (1989)
offered some, for this thesis suitable, specific examples of what could be considered nature:
Nature includes parks and open spaces, meadows and abandoned fields, street trees
and backyard gardens. We are referring to places near and far, common and unusual,
managed and unkempt, big, small, and in-between, where plants grow by human
design or even despite it. We are referring to areas that would often be described as
green, but they are also natural when the green is replaced by white or brown or red
and yellow. Nature includes plants and various forms of vegetation. (p. 2)
Moreover, other biophysical elements of nature considered in this thesis include fauna,
celestial bodies (e.g., the sun, the moon, and stars), and weather-related phenomena (e.g.,
wind, rain, and clouds), and water-containing entities (e.g., rivers, lakes, and the ocean). In
addition, non-biophysical qualities such as fresh air are often described as crucial elements
of the nature experience. Although a robust body of research has shown that people can
experience nature by simply viewing natural elements or landscapes from a building, in
photographs or documentaries, or through virtual reality (see Hartig et al., 2014), such
experiences are not covered within the scope of this thesis. Instead, encounters with forms of
nature were realized in diverse modes of being in physical natural environments, often while
being physically active, of which hiking was one of the most prominent forms. Spending time
in nature has become a valued leisure phenomenon, particularly in Western, educated,
industrialized, rich, and democratic (WEIRD) societal contexts. To emphasize the leisure
context, I use the outdoors, which is an internationally recognized term within the outdoor
leisure research community referring to outdoor places and spaces where people can enjoy
nature (Humberstone et al., 2016). Other culture-specific concepts, such as friluftliv will be
explored as part of the next section.
2.2 Factors that shape one’s relationship with nature
Approximately 55% of the global human population lives in urban areas, and this percentage
is likely to increase (World Bank, 2020). The urbanization of human life, along with factors
such as rapid digitalization, may contribute to an overall decrease in frequent direct contact
with nature (Soga & Gaston, 2016). Experiences of physical disconnection feed into a
popular scientific and non-scientific environmental discourse on psychological disconnection
from nature. In this scope, terms and concepts such as extinction of experience (Pyle, 1978)
and nature deficit disorder (Louv, 2005) have been proposed to highlight that a lack of
regular physical contact with nature challenges the health and well-being of humans and the
planet (for an overview, see Gaston & Soga, 2020).
Petersen 11 The role of emotions in connecting in and to nature
However, previous research has also emphasized that this relationship may be more
complex and nuanced than initially thought. For instance, in a study on urban Singapore, Oh
et al. (2020) used a survey approach to investigate whether experiences of nature,
opportunities to experience nature, and the emotional connection between people and nature
had changed over a period of 22 years. The researchers found that emotional connection
with nature increased among both users and non-users of green spaces, although the
frequency and duration of visits to green spaces had remained the same. Such findings
could support arguments proposed by the sociologist Adrian Franklin (2002), who applied a
social theory approach to posit that connections with nature have not disappeared but rather
reshaped and relocated and involve new social objects and cultural interest and practices. In
other words, human–nature relationships are embedded in historical settings, which are
determined by socio-cultural and environmental processes, therefore, their interpretation is
dynamic.
The bio-psycho-evolutionary – Nevertheless, from the perspective of biological evolution, the
observation that humans have begun to live far away from natural environments is a
comparably recent phenomenon. Human psyche may have been shaped by interactions with
natural environments, which resulted in behavioral and attitudinal tendencies that increased
people's likelihood of survival (Ulrich, 1983; Ulrich et al., 1991). For instance, phobias of
animals and threatening aspects of natural environments are cross-cultural (LeBeau et al.,
2010), and specific biophobic responses to animals, such as snakes or spiders, often
manifest more frequently and quickly than other phobias (Öhman, 2009). Furthermore,
people show strong tendencies to prefer views of nature over views of urban environments
(Joye, 2007; Ulrich, 1981, 1993), especially when the natural environment contains water
elements (e.g., Kaplan & Kaplan, 1989) or provides semi-open and safe spaces (savannah-
type landscapes; Orians, 1980). Thus, from a psycho-evolutionary perspective, it could be
argued that humans inherited a predisposition towards certain biophobic responses, which
helped them to avoid potential natural dangers, and biophilic responses, which encouraged
and motivated them to approach non-threatening and beneficial environments (Ulrich, 1993).
However, it should be noted that most empirical studies on natural landscape preferences
have been conducted with urban Western populations, and more recent studies have
revealed that such preferences are much more diverse, especially when comparing Western
and non-Western samples (Hägerhäll et al., 2018). Furthermore, the biophilia hypothesis,
which was initially proposed by E.O. Wilson (1984) and explored by Kellert and Wilson
(1993) in greater depth, postulates that humans have a profound biological need to affiliate
with and feel connected to the broader natural world. Despite the widespread recognition of
the biophilia hypothesis, this psycho-evolutionary approach falls short to explain individual
and cultural differences in preferences and benefits experienced from human–nature
relationships. Thus, to better understand humans’ relationship with nature, some frequently
applied evolutionary assumptions would benefit from critical revision and adjustment through
the inclusion of other aspects related to personal and socio-cultural dimensions (e.g., Hartig
et al., 2011; Joye & Van den Berg, 2011). For instance, in a literature review on
Petersen 12 The role of emotions in connecting in and to nature
environmental attitudes and actions, Gifford and Nilsson (2014) found that various personal
and social factors, such as knowledge about environmental issues and personality traits
(e.g., openness to experience), determine an individual's degree of preference and
engagement with environmental topics and behaviors.
The eco-individual – Also, recurring findings from both qualitative and quantitative studies
suggest that direct physical experiences with nature during childhood and youth play an
essential role in shaping people’s relationship with nature and nature-based activity behavior
(Bischoff, 2012; Broch, 2018; Calogiuri, 2016; Chawla, 2007; Fasting et al., 2022; Rosa et
al., 2018; Tam, 2013a; Thompson et al., 2007; Van Heel et al., 2023). Within this scope,
phenomenologically inspired perspectives of the human body have been applied to explore
human–nature relationships by conceptualizing humans as embodied (embodiment; see
Merleau-Ponty, 1962) and emplaced (emplacement; see Casey, 1993) in a reciprocal
relationship with the world and nature (Ingold, 2000). Embodied experiences of nature-based
physical activity are influenced by opportunities for action afforded by the environment, such
as walking paths in nature (Bischoff, 2012). To understand the entanglement between the
active organism and the surrounding environment, the concept of affordances (originally
proposed by Gibson, 1977) has begun to establish itself as a frequently applied theoretical
framework in both outdoor studies and environmental psychology (e.g., by applying and
expanding on the affordance approach to explain nature’s positive influence on health;
Araújo et al., 2019; Brymer et al., 2020). Affordances can be understood as the functionally
significant properties of the environment that are identified through active information
perception. They encompass characteristics of both the environment and the interacting
individual. Thus, affordances vary between individuals and across different socio-
demographic groups (Kyttä, 2002, p. 109).
The proximate social – While some people’s personal histories include solitary experiences
in nature, most experiences occur in social settings with close others, such as family and
friends, and as members of social groups or communities. In this sense, the proximate socio-
cultural embeddedness of nature experiences influences how people encounter, understand,
value, establish, and maintain their relationship with nature (Olivos & Clayton, 2017).
According to the environmental psychologist James J. Gifford (2014, p. 542), human life is
constantly embedded in cultural practices. Thus, although participants in this study spent
time alone in nature (Articles II, III, and IV), the values and belief systems that they share
with others guide their solo practice.
The broader socio-cultural – Moreover, cultural values and narratives at a societal (national)
level present another influential dimension that shapes a person’s relationship with nature
and affects how the different national samples featured in this thesis engaged with nature in
their leisure time. For instance, in Norway, official policies explicitly stress the importance of
friluftsliv (often translated to English as outdoor life; Gelter, 2000; Gurholt & Haukeland,
Petersen 13 The role of emotions in connecting in and to nature
2019) and its embeddedness in socio-cultural layers of society, such as the school system,
public health, and environmental questions (Ministry of Climate and Environment, 2016).
Friluftsliv is a Scandinavian-specific way of engaging in outdoor practice; it is popular across
all age groups and is a recognized component of Norwegian national identity (Gurholt, 2008).
The Norwegian ecophilosopher Arne Naess (1973) coined the term ecological self to
describe an individual’s interconnectedness with the earth and sense of inclusion in nature.
Naess was also an outdoor enthusiast who deeply influenced the Norwegian discourse
around outdoor recreation and promoted an idealistic prototype of a slow, nature-focused
way of being outdoors. Such interpretations have continued to shape Norwegians’
understanding of outdoor practice in many ways (Gurholt, 2008). In Germany, cultural
influences at the national level appear to be more implicit and less systematically embedded
in different socio-cultural layers of society, although they are clearly marked by ideas related
to romanticism. For instance, the country's long history with hydrotherapy, which is typically
traced back to Sebastian Kneipp (1821–1897), has generated an enduring fascination with
water, emphasizing the notion of nature’s healing powers. Traces of this cultural influence
can be found in traditional spas (German: Kurbäder) across Germany. However, perhaps the
most relevant nature-related national symbol is the German forest, which has inspired social
phenomena such as the youth movement Wandering Bird (German: Wandervogel) from
1896 to 1933. Today, nature hikes and walks are still the most frequent type of outdoor
activity across different age groups in Germany (Wilson, 2019). Lastly, outdoor participants in
New Zealand often experience a fusion of different cultural understandings of what it means
to have a meaningful relationship with nature (Lynch & Dibben, 2016). On the one hand,
adventure and personal development-inspired outdoor approaches from the United Kingdom,
the United States, and the educational tradition surrounding Kurt Hahn (1886–1974) appear
to have inspired Kiwis to enjoy tramping in the bush, along with a range of other activities,
which suggests an understanding of nature as arena for personal joy and challenge. On the
other hand, such Western cultural ideas have increasingly merged with interpretations of
human–nature relationships in Māori culture, in which belief systems suggest an
understanding of the earth and the self as one (Cosgriff et al., 2012). Indeed, the New
Zealand government has begun to recognize and promote these multiple ways of
understanding and representing the human–nature relationship by incorporating the notion
that natural environments are intrinsically linked to people’s well-being into official public
health guidelines (Ministry for the Environment, 2021).
In summary, this section proposes that different perspectives shed light on various nuances
of the human–nature relationship. Through the entanglement of these different layers,
human–nature relationships are established and maintained; therefore, they are also best
investigated and understood by considering the specific context that they are embedded in.
The critical realism approach that underpins this thesis supports a more complex eco-social
and socio-behavioral viewpoint (for detailed overview, see Tomasso & Chen, 2022) in which
the bio-psycho-evolutionary, eco-individual, and socio-cultural dimensions are interrelated.
Petersen 14 The role of emotions in connecting in and to nature
In the following section, I briefly introduce the relevant literature on the role of nature in well-
being and flourishing, which is especially relevant for Article III.
2.3 Nature engagement and well-being (flourishing)
Human well-being has been a topic of scholarly investigation in philosophy and religion for
more than a millennium. However, the systematic beginnings of empirical research on well-
being in the social sciences, particularly psychology, are often traced to the 1970s (Olivos &
Clayton, 2017). Despite the popularity of well-being as a concept among scholars from
various disciplines, there is a lack of consensus on what it consists of and how it should be
defined. Many researchers view well-being as a multi-dimensional construct but
operationalize it differently (Disabato et al., 2016; Huta & Waterman, 2014). This results from
at least one of two related but distinct traditions of thought: a hedonic tradition and an
eudaimonic tradition. Hedonic approaches (feeling good) usually focus on constructs such as
happiness and positive and negative affect in combination with satisfaction with life (e.g.,
early approaches: Bradburn, 1969; Diener, 1984; Kahneman et al., 1999). Eudaimonic
approaches (functioning well) are primarily used to describe positive psychological
functioning and human development (e.g., early approaches: Rogers, 1961; Ryff, 1989;
Waterman, 1993). Although people can engage in activities that provide only hedonia or
eudaimonia (Vittersø, 2016), many eudaimonic activities are positively related to hedonia
(Waterman, 2008).
In recognition of the relevance of both the hedonic and the eudaimonic dimensions, well-
being has often been studied with a conceptual focus on subjective well-being (SWB; defined
by Diener, 1984), psychological well-being (PWB; defined by Ryff, 1989), and sometimes
social well-being (defined by Keyes, 1998), which indicates the complexity of well-being. In
Article IV, I am specifically interested in SWB, which is a reliable predictor of important life
outcomes such as health, longevity, and disease (Diener et al., 2017). SWB is sometimes
referred to as happiness and mainly associated with the hedonic aspects of well-being,
although it is constituted by three complementary aspects: life satisfaction, positive affect,
and negative affect (Diener, 1984; Diener, 2009). PWB, on the other hand, is often linked to
eudaimonic aspects of well-being, such as the meaning of life or one's sense of purpose in
the world. Yet, it should be noted that some psychological interpretations of well-being
incorporate SWB as a second dimension (see Diener et al., 2018). Lastly, social well-being
focuses on components such as feeling connected to others and valued by one's community
(Keyes, 2007).
Acknowledging the complexity of well-being and the relevance (and interrelatedness) of
different hedonic and eudaimonic components (Huta & Waterman, 2014), we addressed
well-being through the concept of flourishing in Article III. Flourishing has been proposed as
Petersen 15 The role of emotions in connecting in and to nature
a holistic approach to well-being and encompasses a combination of hedonic (related to
positive emotional states such as happiness and vitality) and eudaimonic facets (related to
positive functioning, such as purpose in life, competence, and social connections; (Huppert &
So, 2013; Keyes, 2002, 2007; VanderWeele, 2017). In this context, Seligman (2011)
suggested that positive emotion, engagement, relationships, meaning, purpose, and
achievement/accomplishment (PERMA) represent the relevant (pleasant, engaging, and
meaningful) building blocks of flourishing. Therefore, we used an extended version of
PERMA: PERMA-V (with V for vitality) as a framework for investigating the well-being
(flourishing) pathways of the solo in nature practice (see Article III).
As concepts, flourishing and well-being are closely linked to the introduction of the positive
psychology framework, which aims to understand and strengthen the elements that enable
individuals, communities, and societies to thrive (Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi, 2000).
Moreover, these concepts are frequently applied in the field of environmental psychology
(Olivos & Clayton, 2017). For instance, a robust body of evidence has affirmed the positive
effects of natural environments and engagement with nature on people's health and well-
being. Specifically, studies have found that contact with nature provides health-related
benefits for well-being (for umbrella review, see Yang et al., 2021), positive affect or
emotions specifically (for an overview, see McMahan & Estes, 2015), and flourishing and
meaning in life-related benefits (for an overview, see Capaldi et al., 2015). Trying to explain
the mechanisms behind these findings, various biophysical, psychological, and social
concepts and theories have been proposed to explain the many health, well-being-(and
flourishing-) related benefits gained from living close to nature, contact with nature, and
engagement in nature activities. Hartig et al. (2014) summarized that contact with nature
leads to enhanced health and well-being via better air quality, increased physical activity,
social relations, and stress reduction. Regarding the latter, the most established theories
used to study the impact of nature on well-being focus on the restorative capacity of nature,
such as attention restoration theory (ART; Kaplan & Kaplan, 1989; Kaplan, 1995) and
psycho-evolutionary theory (conventionally often referred to as stress-recovery theory, SRT;
Ulrich, 1983; Ulrich et al., 1991). A recent paper by Iqbal and Mansell (2021) provided a
comprehensive overview of theoretical approaches to explaining how engagement with
nature contributes to different facets of well-being. Besides exploring the relevance of
biodiversity (Marselle et al., 2021), researchers have recently begun to study and discuss the
role of nature connectedness to explain the well-being-related effects of nature. In this scope,
a growing body of empirical research has shown that nature connectedness is significantly
linked to and can predict various facets of well-being (for recent systematic overview and
meta analysis, see McNeil et al., 2022; Wu & Jones, 2022).
Regarding the latter, the next section presents an overview of how scholars have
approached the identification and quantification of human–nature relationships at an
individual level through the operationalization of different conceptualizations of nature
connectedness.
Petersen 16 The role of emotions in connecting in and to nature
2.4 Nature connectedness – concepts, measures, and
research
Humans construe their relationship with nature or elements of it in various ways, and some
people or groups appear to have stronger bonds with nature than others. Investigating how
and why connections between humans and nature are shaped differently has become a
central topic in environmental psychology, particularly in the branches of ecopsychology and
conservation psychology. In comparison to the theoretical perspectives of place attachment
(Scannell & Gifford, 2010, 2017) and topophilia (Ogunseitan, 2005; Tuan, 1990), which focus
on affective bounds between people and specific places or environmental settings, the
concept of nature connectedness is underpinned by the notion of connecting to a more
abstract form of nature, which encompasses, for instance, flora, fauna, celestial phenomena,
etc.,. Despite various vernacular labels in the literature (e.g., being in touch with, being
connected to, being part of, or experiencing oneness with nature), contemporary theoretical
contributions primarily describe the human–nature relationship from an anthropocentric
perspective in which humans incorporate relatively unspecific and abstract notions of nature
into their psychological image of themselves (Olivos & Clayton, 2017). To differentiate such
subjective levels from group-based levels, I adopt the term human–nature connectedness
(see e.g., Riechers et al., 2021) to highlight the construct’s relevance at the societal level.
Furthermore, I use nature connectedness as an umbrella term for a subjectively perceived
connection to nature that can be described at a trait and state level.
Schultz (2002, p. 68) suggested that nature connectedness incorporates three psychological
components: cognitive (perception of the self as part of nature), affective (an affective bond
with nature), and behavioral (e.g., taking care of nature). These are expressed through the
structural components of connectedness, caring, and commitment. While researchers
generally agree that nature connectedness involves these psychological, structural
components, the literature provides a range of concepts and corresponding measures to
capture different nuances of an individually perceived nature connectedness (Tam, 2013a).
Keaulana et al. (2021) provided a comprehensive systematic review of nature, land, and
environmental connectedness and relatedness measures across disciplines. Additionally,
Tiscareno-Osorno et al. (2023) published a recent systematic review of explicit instruments
measuring nature connectedness.
Table 2 is inspired by Capaldi’s overview on measures of human-nature relationships (2020,
pp. 21-23), and focuses on briefly summarizing the most frequently mentioned constructs
and their corresponding scales in environmental psychology. The third column highlights how
the theoretical and conceptual approach of the scales were inspired by previous work on
interpersonal relations and a general notion of connecting as a psychological process.
Petersen 17 The role of emotions in connecting in and to nature
Table 2. Frequently applied concepts and measures of nature connectedness
Concept
(abbreviation;
authors)
Operationalizing measure Links to the conceptual idea of
connecting as a fundamental
psychological process
Emotional Affinity
Toward Nature
(EAN; Kals et al.,
1999)
Degree of agreement with 16 statements
that assess feeling good, free, and safe in
nature; feeling oneness with nature; and
feelings associated with love of nature;
trait state version and shortened 11-item
version available
Recognizes that emotional
responses to nature are tied to a
general sense of relatedness,
acknowledges indirectly the
relevance of the self-concept for
environmental attitudes
New Ecological
Paradigm (NEP;
Dunlap et al.,
2000)
Degree of agreement with 15 statements
that assess basic environmental beliefs
about humanity’s ability to upset the
balance of nature, the existence of limits
to growth for human societies, and
humanity’s right to rule over the rest of
nature; trait measure
Revised version of the 12-item
NEP by Dunlap and Van Liere
(2014), first grounded in social
psychological theories of attitude
structure (Stern et al., 1995), and
associated with fundamental
ideas of people’s belief systems
(Rokeach, 1968)
Inclusion of
Nature in Self
(INS; Schultz,
2001, 2002)
Graphical metaphor that displays seven
distinct pairs of circles labeled self and
nature with different degrees of overlap
between them. Selecting pair of circles
that best represents one’s own perceived
relationship with nature (trait version
available)
Builds on the item desire for
belonging and connection, which
is part of the self-determination
theory; and explicitly adopts the
Inclusion of Other in Self Scale
(Aron et al., 1992) for
interpersonal relationships
Environmental
Identity (EID;
Clayton, 2003)
Degree of agreement with 24 items that
assess components of environmental
identity: interaction with nature, whether
nature is important, thinking of oneself as
part of nature, and whether natural
environments evoke positive emotions.
Further items include competence,
autonomy, belonging, and engaging in
outdoor activities; shortened 11-item
version (Clayton, 2012), cross-culturally
tested 14-item trait version available
(Clayton et al., 2021)
Builds on research from collective
social identities; the construct of
environmental identity is viewed
as similar to other collective
identities and can therefore
provide a sense of connection or
belonging to a larger whole
Connectedness to
Nature (CNS;
Mayer & Frantz,
2004)
Degree of agreement with 14 items that
assess perceptions and cognitive beliefs
of belonging to and feeling kinship with
nature, viewing oneself as an egalitarian
member of nature, and thinking of
personal well-being as dependent on
environmental well-being; trait and state
version available (Mayer et al., 2009)
Builds on social psychological
research on self-expansion,
interpersonal closeness and
empathy in interpersonal
relationships
Nature
relatedness (NR;
Nisbet et al.,
2009)
Degree of agreement with 21 items that
assess affective, cognitive, and
experiential components of one's
relationship with nature; captures people’s
identification with nature, nature-related
worldviews, and familiarity with and
appreciation of being in nature; trait-like
measure, shortened six-item version
available (Nisbet & Zelenski, 2013)
Encompasses elements of self-
determination theory: desire for
belonging and
connection.Inspired by the notion
of ecological identity or ecological
self (Naess, 1973)
Petersen 18 The role of emotions in connecting in and to nature
As Table 2 indicates, empirical measures of nature connectedness predominantly rely on
psychometric scales that contain different sets of item statements, which respondents are
asked to indicate their agreement with on a Likert scale. However, exceptions have been
proposed by Schultz and colleagues, for instance, the Inclusion of Nature in Self scale (INS)
which is a graphical single-item measure, or the Nature Implicit Association Test (IAT-
Nature), which is an implicit measure based on reaction time (Bruni & Schultz, 2010; Schultz
et al., 2004). Tam (2013a) aimed to identify the similarities and differences between many of
the above introduced measures. Despite some differences, the author concluded that the
measures could be understood as indicators of the same underlying construct, as they were
strongly correlated. These findings were echoed in a recent meta-analysis by Balundė et al.
(2019), who found that the constructs of connectedness with nature, environmental identity,
and environmental self-identity were strongly correlated. Tam (2013a) found that measures
that conceptualize nature connectedness as multidimensional (cognitive and emotional
components) outperformed other measures. Further, Tam also highlighted the relevance of
Ashmore et al. (2004) work on collective identity and suggested that including a social
dimension in the measure of nature connectedness would reflect the concepts socio-cultural
embeddedness.
The measures listed in Table 2 have also inspired the development of modified, revised
versions and new measures, which indicates a growing academic interest in the topic of
nature connectedness. For example, Dutcher et al. (2007) developed the Connectivity with
Nature (CWN) scale, which, similar to the INS, is a combination of a graphical item and four
items that contain statements based on agreement ratings. Other closely linked concepts
and scale measures that have emerged include the Allo-Inclusive Identity–Natural World (AI-
Nature; Leary et al., 2008), the Commitment to Nature concept (COM; Davis et al., 2009), the
Love and Care for Nature (LCN; Perkins, 2010), and the Dispositional Empathy with Nature
(DEN; Tam, 2013b). Moreover, scales have been proposed to measure nature
connectedness among children, such as the Connection to Nature Index (CNI; Cheng &
Monroe, 2010) and the Nature Connectedness Parental Self-Efficacy scale (NCPSE; Barnes
et al., 2021). Most empirical studies on nature connectedness that featured one of these
abovementioned measures used a correlational cross-sectional design that targeted the
relationship between humans and nature at a trait level and psychological and behavioral
outcomes related to well-being and sustainability (for recent meta-analyses, see Barragan‐
Jason et al., 2021; Pritchard et al., 2019).
In general, nature connectedness scores have been found to vary across an individual's
lifespan, with a distinct drop from the ages of 10 to 15 (Hughes et al., 2018; Hughes et al.,
2019; Richardson et al., 2019). Furthermore, Richardson et al. (2019) found modest variation
across socio-economic groups and significantly higher nature connectedness scores among
women than men in their population surveys. Although previous studies have primarily
investigated trait levels of nature connectedness, some researchers have also applied state
measures and showed that participation in different types of nature activities, such as walks,
can increase state nature connectedness (e.g., McEwan et al., 2019; Passmore & Holder,
Petersen 19 The role of emotions in connecting in and to nature
2017; Richardson & Sheffield, 2017). According to Mena-García et al. (2020), these results
indicate that a person’s relationship with nature is dynamic, reciprocal, and can fluctuate.
Thus, state levels of nature connectedness can be used to investigate the factors that situate
variations in nature connectedness responses on a momentary basis and are, therefore,
more relevant to the questions explored in this thesis compared to trait levels.
While brief exposure to natural environments has been found to increase nature
connectedness (Mayer et al., 2009), it has been suggested that more time spent in nature
has a stronger effect on nature affiliation and connectedness (e.g., Nisbet et al., 2009; Soga
et al., 2020). However, a recent study from the United Kingdom, which was based on a large,
nationally representative cross-sectional dataset, showed that noticing nature through
activities that involve active sensory engagement with it (e.g., smelling flowers) explained
differences in nature connectedness levels statistically better than the actual amount of time
spent in nature (Richardson, Hamlin, et al., 2021; Richardson, Passmore, et al., 2021).
Furthermore, in a comprehensive narrative review based on 85 research papers, Lengieza
and Swim (2021) gathered knowledge on how changes in nature connectedness are related
to situational contexts, individual difference predictors and internal psychological states
(mindfulness, self-awareness and affect). The review highlights a lack of theoretical
frameworks that explain how connectedness is initiated and calls for nuanced understanding
of effects regarding contact with nature, self-awareness, and affect, embedded within a
cross-cultural perspective. Additionally, a recent review with meta-analysis by Sheffield et al.
(2022) based on 36 papers, examined factors that could account for increased nature
connectedness in adults, incorporating (quasi-)experiments and field interventions. The
results revealed a significant positive medium mean effect of manipulations on nature
connectedness in the short term (pre-post), with similar results observed in 12-week follow-
up measures (based only on 12 studies). Interestingly, the authors found no significant
differences in effect sizes regarding the type of nature interaction (direct or indirect), the
quality of engagement (active vs. passive), or the timing of engagement (single session,
repeated practice, or residential). Sheffield et al. highlighted similar gaps in the literature as
Lengieza and Swim (2021), but also pointed out the need for future studies to investigate the
impact of social aspects of nature-based activities and changes in nature connectedness.
In summary, nature connectedness can be evaluated at both the state and trait level, and it
can be quantified using various measures. Incorporating both cognitive and affective aspects
has shown to generate more reliable findings. As a trait, nature connectedness fluctuates
across the lifespan and can differ between socio-economic groups and gender. Although
there exists solid evidence that state (and trait) nature connectedness can change through
experiences with and in nature, little research has examined the factors that contribute to this
phenomenon in depth. In this context, emotions have been declared as relevant element,
however, there is little understanding of how and in what ways they may be fundamental to
nature connectedness. The next section elaborates on this literature gap.
Petersen 20 The role of emotions in connecting in and to nature
2.5 The role of emotions in connecting to nature: The
literature gap
The following literature review aims to situate the thesis's primary research question within
the scope of current academic discourses on leisure-based nature engagement. It does so
by summarizing relevant empirical and conceptual studies on the relationship between
emotion phenomena and nature connectedness and pointing out the gaps in current
research. The relationship between nature connectedness and affective phenomena,
especially positively perceived emotions, is a growing topic of interest, primarily in the scope
of environmental psychology. Most current research indicates a robust association between
nature engagement, nature connectedness, and positively perceived emotions. However,
views on the direction of causality within this relationship vary.
2.5.1 Emotions as outcome variables of nature engagement
As outlined in the introduction to this thesis, previous research has mainly focused on
emotions or mood as outcomes of engaging with or being exposed to nature (e.g., as
positive versus negative emotional state of hedonic well-being; see Bratman et al., 2021).
Additionally, some studies investigated the affective level in the form of improved emotion
regulation (Bakir-Demir et al., 2019; Johnsen & Rydstedt, 2013; Richardson, 2019).
Furthermore, nature connectedness has frequently been suggested as a mediating or
moderating factor in the relationship between nature engagement and the subsequent
positive emotion or mood outcomes (e.g., Capaldi et al., 2014; McMahan et al., 2018; Mena-
García et al., 2020; Wu & Jones, 2022). Contrary to positively perceived emotions, negatively
perceived emotions have been found to have a mixed relationship with nature
connectedness. While some studies suggested that experiencing negatively perceived
emotions (e.g., anger, shame, and fear) correlates with a decrease in nature connectedness
(e.g., Chen et al., 2022), others indicated that negatively perceived emotions (e.g.,
compassion with suffering animals) may enhance nature connectedness by promoting a
deeper understanding and appreciation of the complexities and vulnerabilities of nature (e.g.,
Tam, 2013b).
Research over the past decades has shown that emotions and other affective phenomena
are powerful in explaining and predicting attitudes and behaviors (Dukes et al., 2021).
Similarly, the emotions that people experience during outdoor leisure activities are likely
significant motivators for engaging with nature and partly responsible for experiencing well-
being-related benefits from this engagement or changes in their understanding of the human-
nature relationship. Therefore, treating emotions merely as outcome variables falls short to
capture the full extent of their influence and role in these processes.
Petersen 21 The role of emotions in connecting in and to nature
2.5.2 Emotions as predictors of well-being and pro-environmental
aspects
Some recent studies have investigated emotions, either as distinct emotions or groups of
emotions, as predictors of environmental attitudes and behaviors. For instance, Moreton,
Arena, Hornsey, et al. (2019) conducted two empirical studies on positively perceived self-
transcendent emotions (see Section 3.3 for explanation of this group of emotions) in the
context of environmental attitude, using a university student sample and an online
convenience sample. The authors were specifically interested in how moral elevation,
induced by videos, could enhance individuals' nature connectedness and environmental
attitude (e.g., intention to purchase environmentally friendly products). While the first study
did not indicate that moral elevation directly or indirectly led to change in pro-environmental
attitude through nature connectedness, the second study identified a significant indirect
effect of moral elevation through self-transcendent positive emotions and connectedness to
nature on environmental attitude. Systematic overviews and meta-analyses on the role of
nature connectedness in human well-being and sustainable behavior and attitude are
available (see Barragan-Jason et al., 2023; Barragan‐Jason et al., 2021).
In contrast to the above-described inconclusive link between moral elevation and pro-
environmental attitudes, the emotion of awe has been associated with various aspects of
pro-environmental behavior intentions. Examples of these include willingness to make self-
sacrifices for environmental protection (Zhao et al., 2018), intentions to purchase
environmentally friendly goods, and supporting environmental policies (Bethelmy & Corraliza,
2019), as well as intentions toward pro-environmental actions, such as recycling or taking
shorter showers (Yang et al., 2018). The latter study found that the relationship between awe
and ecological behavior was mediated by nature connectedness.
Similar to awe, the elicitation of compassion has been linked to increased intentions and
environmental behaviors like recycling, energy conservation, and sustainable purchasing
(Pfattheicher et al., 2016), as well as support for climate change mitigation actions by
governments (Lu & Schuldt, 2016). Interestingly, individuals who showed greater
compassion for others also showed more willingness to alter their lifestyles for climate
change mitigation (Dickinson et al., 2016).
Research on gratitude and sustainability-related attitude and behavior is more limited,
however, a study from Japan showed that students who expressed gratitude towards nature
also showed heightened environmental care and intentions (Naito et al., 2010), and another
more recent study with undergraduate students from the United States found that gratitude,
unlike general positive affect, induced by a recall task, led participants to use fewer
resources in a task simulating natural resource dilemmas (Kates & DeSteno, 2021).
Interestingly, Tam (2022) found, based on four correlational and two experimental studies (of
which five were conducted in China and one with participants from the United States), that
Petersen 22 The role of emotions in connecting in and to nature
gratitude can be conceptually distinguished at both state and trait levels, with the trait version
correlating more strongly with nature connectedness.
In their effort to investigate a group of emotions, Jacobs and McConnell (2022) conducted
three correlational studies to compare whether self-transcendent emotions (awe,
compassion, love) versus self-interested emotions (joy, contentment, pride, amusement)
would predict sustainable behaviors, a change in values, and higher nature connectedness.
In two online questionnaire-based studies with undergraduates and one experimental study
observing recycling behaviors, they found that self-transcendent emotions uniquely predicted
increased nature connectedness, pro-environmental behaviors, biospheric concern, and
increased sustainable values, as well as a decrease in political conservatism. In contrast,
self-interested emotions were linked to greater egoistic concern, less pro-environmental
behavior, and more self-enhancement values and conservative beliefs. The third study
exemplified that self-transcendent emotions, unlike self-interested ones, could predict actual
recycling behavior weeks later.
In recent studies, researchers have also investigated the predictive or mediating role of
emotions on well-being. For example, as described in Article I, Anderson et al. (2018)
conducted a field study with military veterans participating in white-water rafting and
demonstrated that experiencing awe during this nature-based activity significantly improved
well-being a week later. Additionally, in a second study, young people from underserved
communities engaged in daily interactions with nature, and the researchers found that the
emotions of awe, gratitude, and contentment each played a mediating role in enhancing daily
life satisfaction. Løvoll et al. (2020) further emphasized that positively perceived emotions,
when experienced in nature, are linked to general well-being outcomes, and Løvoll and
Sæther (2022) specifically highlighted the potential relevance of experiencing awe in nature
for spiritual well-being. In summary, the number of studies exploring the predictive potential
of emotions on pro-environmental attitude and behavior, and especially well-be