ArticlePDF Available

Early Brain Development and Public Health

Authors:

Abstract

Early brain development research is important to public health professionals for understanding early development and strengthening systemic supports for young children's healthy brain growth. This overview describes basic processes of early brain development, including prenatal development and the "fetal programming" of brain and behavior, neural proliferation and the essential influence of experience in the creation of neural networks, the maturational timing of different brain systems and their behavioral consequences, the myelination of neural pathways and its influence on children's action and thinking, and the capacity of the brain to create new neural connections throughout life, which contributes to its continuing adaptability to new experiences. The implications of this research for public health and for strengthening support for early brain development are considered throughout this discussion.
Doi: 10.32481/djph.2024.10.03
Early Brain Development and Public Health
Ross A. Thompson, Ph.D.
Distinguished Professor Emeritus, Department of Psychology, University of California, Davis
Acknowledgement
This article is an adapted version of chapter 3, "Dispatches from the Laboratory," from The
Brain Development Revolution: Science, the Media, and Public Policy. Figure 1 is also taken
from the chapter.
Abstract
Early brain development research is important to public health professionals for understanding
early development and strengthening systemic supports for young children's healthy brain
growth. This overview describes basic processes of early brain development, including prenatal
development and the "fetal programming" of brain and behavior, neural proliferation and the
essential influence of experience in the creation of neural networks, the maturational timing of
different brain systems and their behavioral consequences, the myelination of neural pathways
and its influence on children's action and thinking, and the capacity of the brain to create new
neural connections throughout life, which contributes to its continuing adaptability to new
experiences. The implications of this research for public health and for strengthening support for
early brain development are considered throughout this discussion.
Introduction
The brain is the most complex biological system in the human body, by far. It consists of billions
of nerve cells of different cell types that are interlinked by trillions of connections, organized
into distinct neural circuits and located in regions that differ structurally and functionally.
Consequently, brain development is a prolonged process, beginning in the first two to three
weeks after conception and lasting through early adulthood. This extended maturation derives
from the complexity of neurobiological growth, of course, but it is also due to the brain's
continuous incorporation of experience. In other words, the brain's development is shaped
throughout life both by genetic guidance and by experience -- the classic nature-nurture
dynamic.
Understanding the early development of the brain is important for public health professionals. It
is important not only to inform understanding of early childhood development, but also because
the work of public health is crucial to supporting healthy brain development in the early years
and preventing and remediating many of the threats that exist. In this article, my goal is to
highlight the central processes of early brain development and their relevance to public health.
Brain Development Begins Early. Very Early.
Because its development is so foundational to the rest of human growth, the brain develops
rapidly early in life. Whereas a newborn's brain is already 26% of its adult weight, by age five,
the brain has reached 88% of its adult weight.1 The latter figure helps to account for the well-
known claim that the brain is 90% developed by the age of five. That claim is mistaken because
Doi: 10.32481/djph.2024.10.03
brain weight is not a very informative index of its maturation (after all, men's brains are 10%
heavier than women's). More important than estimating how heavy the brain has become by
various ages is understanding the foundations for life-long functioning that are established by its
early growth.
Some of the most important foundations are established prenatally. The nine months of prenatal
development witnesses the most rapid growth of the brain. Beginning early in prenatal growth,
during the third gestational week, new neurons (nerve cells) and other supportive cells are
generated at an astonishing rate -- by some estimates, several hundred thousand each minute --
and then migrate to their eventual destinations in the fetal brain.2 Throughout this process of
neural proliferation and migration, neurons continue to develop through gene expression within
the cell and by the influence of adjacent neurons. By the 23rd week of gestation, synapses
(contacts between neurons permitting the transmission of activation, a.k.a. neural
communication) begin to proliferate as the structural organization of the brain begins to form.
Before birth, the vast majority of neurons that populate the brain have been created.
It is a truism that the more rapid pace of development, the greater the vulnerability of the
organism. Consistent with this truism is the vulnerability of prenatal brain development to a
variety of hazards, including environmental toxins (such as pesticide and lead exposure), drugs
(e.g., alcohol, cannabis), and maternal viral infection (such as influenza and herpes simplex) that
can potentially have long-lasting developmental consequences, sometimes because of the
epigenetic processes (i.e., changes in gene expression) they trigger.3 Some prenatal influences
are hazardous because they provoke developmental adaptations in the fetus that may have
negative long-term consequences for health and morbidity. According to the developmental
origins of health and disease (DOHaD) theory, certain early influences, such as prenatal
undernutrition, can program changes in fetal development in anticipation of the requirements of
the postnatal environment.4 In this view, "fetal programming" of physical and neurobiological
systems derives from fetal sensitivity to aspects of the intrauterine environment that signal
conditions of life outside the womb, and fetal development adapts to enhance the chance of
survival after birth.
This was evocatively seen in the Dutch Hunger Winter of 1945, when official rations for citizens
in the occupied Netherlands were dramatically cut by Nazi authorities in reprisal for a strike on
the Dutch railways. The children born to women who were pregnant during this period have
been studied into adulthood to determine the long-term effects of this time-limited period of
profound nutritional insufficiency that was followed by the immediate restoration of an adequate
diet (when the Allies liberated the Netherlands). Researchers discovered that the children born to
mothers who were malnourished early in their pregnancy were at significantly greater risk as
adults for a variety of chronic health and mental health problems compared with children of
different gestational age or same-sex siblings.5 One explanation is that fetal metabolism, growth
rate, and organ system development early and quickly adapted to conditions of limited food
intake in preparation for encountering comparably deprived circumstances after birth. Children
were biologically unprepared for the conditions of nutritional adequacy into which they were
born, however, and this led to a range of physical and mental health problems that endured
throughout life.6
Guided by this view, researchers have been concerned not only with nutritional adequacy but
also with other prenatal influences, such as the effects of maternal stress on the developing fetus.
This concern is warranted given that 15% to 20% of pregnant women experience depression or
Doi: 10.32481/djph.2024.10.03
anxiety symptoms, and these rates increase in the context of other stresses of pregnancy, such as
difficult employment conditions, strain in close relationships, and low income.7 Chronic
maternal stress has direct effects on the fetal brain: it increases cortisol levels in the brain and
contributes to the development of heightened stress reactivity in newborns.8 These consequences
may derive from fetal programming, since prenatal exposure to elevated stress hormones is a
powerful signal of environmental adversity for which developing biological systems adapt in
preparation for birth into a challenging world. One study showed that by six months after birth,
the infants of mothers who were prenatally depressed showed weaker connections between the
amygdala and the prefrontal cortex, which suggests compromised emotion regulation beginning
early in infancy and heightened sensitivity to cues of threat and danger.9 The behavioral effects
of fetal exposure to maternal stress can be enduring, and include heightened emotional reactivity
and enhanced risk for depression or anxiety as late as adolescence.10
Developing an Adaptive Brain
Fetal programming shows that from the beginning, the brain incorporates experience into its
architecture. This is even more evident in another feature of early brain development: the
overproduction and refinement of neural connections.
As earlier noted, synapses begin to form prenatally, connecting neurons in communicative
networks. Because synapses are a basic means by which the brain develops, they are formed
continuously as the brain matures (and, indeed, throughout life). But early in life, the production
of synapses is especially rapid, and can be considered (in the technical language of
developmental neuroscientists) "exuberant." By one estimate, more than one million synapses
are formed every second in the early years.11 If this seems excessive, it is true: up to 40% more
synapses are overproduced early in life than will be retained in the mature brain.12 Consequently,
the young brain is a more active and densely packed organ than it will be at maturity.
Overproduction of synapses, which derives from the genetic guidance of brain development, is a
good thing. It affords considerable capacity for early growth because synapses can become
enlisted into developing and consolidating a potentially expansive range of skills and knowledge.
But overproduction has liabilities, including poorer signal-to-noise in neural communication
(i.e., lots of incoming transmissions reaching neurons when only a subset is important), and
some forms of mental disability are associated with atypical rates of proliferation of synaptic
connections.13 Therefore, an important subsequent step in brain development is the progressive
reduction and elimination of synapses to increase the efficiency of neural processing.
Experience guides the process of synaptic pruning by strengthening neural connections when
they are activated. When the brain responds to stimulation, it activates specific synaptic
networks, and these networks become strengthened in the process. By contrast, synapses that are
rarely activated eventually wither and disappear. The brain of a Parisian baby overhearing
French spoken at home develops consolidated synaptic networks related to the perception of
French speech sounds, for example, but synapses for Russian or Chinese speech perception,
rarely activated, wither. In this manner, early experiences help to determine the retention of
connections in the developing brain (this is the basis for the well-known maxim, "use it or lose
it") and the result is a brain that consolidates skills that are relevant to the world in which the
child is living.
Doi: 10.32481/djph.2024.10.03
In fact, we can observe this process both behaviorally and biologically. Studies have shown that
during their first six months, infants can discriminate speech sounds found in languages all over
the world, including sounds that their parents cannot differentiate (a young Japanese infant can
discriminate, for example, between the sounds /l/ and /r/, even though her parents cannot readily
do so). But by the end of the first year, the baby's speech perception has become more narrowly
channeled to the sounds of the home language, and infants can no longer discriminate foreign
speech sounds.14 In other words, infants become perceptually fine-tuned to the language system
that they are overhearing and preparing to learn. (The same occurs with the child's own sounds:
by 10 months, infants have incorporated the sounds of the home language into their babbling.15)
This developmental change can also be observed by neuroimaging brain regions involved in
speech perception. One research group found that at seven months, the brain became active in
response to speech sounds from different languages. Between seven and 11 months, however,
there was an increase in activation to speech in the home language, and by the end of the first
year, infants showed neural activation only to sounds of the home language.16
This illustrates the profound adaptability, or plasticity, of the developing brain. Since the fetal
brain cannot predict whether it will be born in Paris, Kiev, Tokyo, Los Angeles, or elsewhere,
the brain at birth is prepared through the overproduction of synapses to discriminate the sounds
of any human language. But months of experience overhearing the language -- or languages --
spoken at home guide the pruning of synapses and reorganization of brain areas governing
speech perception to focus on the home language, and the capacity for universal speech
discrimination is lost. The benefits of this can be observed a few months later, in the "vocabulary
explosion" beginning at 18 months, when the rewired brain is now attuned to this language and
new words are acquired at a very rapid rate.
Retaining lots of unneeded synapses is unhelpful, therefore, since the desired developmental
outcome is efficient learning. Consequently, the dual processes of exuberant overproduction
followed by the experience-guided elimination of synapses occurs throughout the brain to
increase the efficiency of neural processing and adapt the brain's capacities to the world in which
the child lives. Importantly, the timing and pace of this process varies for different brain regions,
as shown in Figure 1 using three brain regions for illustration. The figure shows that synaptic
overproduction begins prenatally in areas relevant to basic sensory skills -- seeing and hearing --
with postnatal experience contributing to the refinement of these connections, completed by
early childhood. Language areas of the brain (such as Broca's area and the angular gyrus) have a
more extended period of synaptic pruning as language skills continue to be refined until age 11
or 12. Even longer is the developmental timetable for the prefrontal cortex, an area important to
self-regulation, which does not reach maturity until early adulthood (despite the fervent hopes of
parents).
Figure 1. Development of the Human Brain
Doi: 10.32481/djph.2024.10.03
This figure illustrates, therefore, that the timing of these dual developmental processes varies for
different brain regions and in a manner that accords with what we see behaviorally: young
children achieve sensory acuity by kindergarten (a necessary skill for the acquisition of other
abilities, such as reading), show a longer timetable for first (and second) language acquisition,
and by adolescence are still mastering capacities for self-control in thinking and behavior. Many
developmental changes in children's behavior -- growing motor coordination and fine-motor
control, advances in thinking and reasoning, skills in social and emotional understanding --
likewise follow advances in synaptic pruning in relevant brain areas. Note, however, that the
figure also illustrates how the first five years witnesses major advances in virtually all of these
developmental domains. In this respect, the importance of early childhood is underscored by the
neurobiological advances that are establishing a foundation for many developmental
achievements that are occurring in childhood and at later ages.
The figure also illustrates another important aspect of early brain development: the myelination
of neural connections. Myelin is a fatty protein sheath covering neurons that provides insulation
and speeds the conduction of neural activation (in some areas of the brain, over 100-fold).17
Whereas synaptic overproduction and pruning refine the brain's neural networks, myelination
contributes speed and efficiency to them. The timetable for myelination is similar to the one
described for synapse development, with earlier advances in sensory and motor regions and
much later development in brain regions governing complex information processing and self-
regulation. One can observe the effects of myelination in children's behavior, from the growing
speed of coordinated movement to the increased quickness and dexterity of thinking. Although
neural transmission can occur in unmyelinated neurons, the efficiency of myelinated neural
networks is significantly greater, and it is myelination that significantly advances the efficiency
of neural transmission.
Doi: 10.32481/djph.2024.10.03
Neural Networks
Before proceeding further, a clarification. The discussion thus far has referred to neural
"networks" because that is how neuroscientists think about brain functioning. Specifically, the
brain processes governing most behavior involve connections between neurons that are often
widely distributed throughout the brain. This is especially true of the complex behaviors that are
most interesting, such as those associated with learning or emotion. The view that the brain
functions in neural networks contrasts with the more common locationist view of the brain that
attributes specific behaviors to particular brain structures (e.g., the amygdala is where fear
responding occurs; the hippocampus is where memory occurs). By contrast with the locationist
view, the brain processes involved with emotions like fear network systems associated with
sensory evaluation, cognitive appraisal, short- and long-term memory, and other processes that
are widely distributed throughout the brain.
This is important because a view of brain functioning in neural networks not only underscores
the amazing complexity of how the brain functions, but also provides greater insight into the
development and remediation of behavioral problems. The development of emotion regulation is
not, for example, primarily due to maturation of the amygdala, but due to a range of other brain
systems we don't usually see as relevant to emotion, such as areas of the prefrontal cortex that
support self-regulation and which take much longer to mature. Understanding the network of
brain systems involved in emotion regulation might give parents and practitioners greater
patience for understanding the challenges of young children in emotion management, and also
provide insights that can help in remedial and therapeutic interventions when problems of
emotion management are more severe and enduring.
Two Kinds of Experience
At this point it is possible to answer a question that is often posed about brain development: why
isn't it all in the genes? In other words, given its importance, why isn't the entire development of
the brain encoded in the human genome?
One reason is that the entire genome is simply inadequate for mapping the trillions of
connections in a typical adult brain. Another reason, however, is that a "genetic blueprint" would
make brain development inflexible, and render children poorly suited to any of the incredibly
diverse environments in which people live. As research has shown, by incorporating experience
into the architecture of brain development, individual brains become tailored to the young child's
lived experience in their individual environments.
There are, however, two kinds of "experience" relevant to early brain development.
The first kind of experience contributes to experience-expectant development. These are
experiences that are pervasive in life and, as a consequence, the developing brain "expects" to
encounter these experiences as part of its development. Examples include exposure to patterned
light, gravity, the sound of human voices and language, touch on skin surfaces, social contact,
and others. Although these are common, universal experiences, brain development is built on
these exposures occurring when relevant neurobiological systems are maturing. Their failure to
occur, or to occur at the right time, can result in the loss of relevant brain capacities.
Consequently, experience-expectant development is often viewed in terms of sensitive periods
during which certain environmental exposures must occur if brain development is to proceed
normally.
Doi: 10.32481/djph.2024.10.03
Ordinarily there is no reason for concern about this in normal circumstances. But when young
children live in profound deprivation early in life, essential experience-expectant exposures may
be lacking. When a 13-year-old girl with the pseudonym Genie was rescued by Los Angeles
County child welfare authorities in 1970, the authorities were astonished at the grossly neglectful
conditions in which she had lived her entire life, lacking normal human contact, language
exposure, and even opportunities for normal motor development. Many of her developmental
deficits proved to be remediable with therapeutic assistance, but the psychologists assisting in
her care concluded differently with respect to language. Although Genie's vocabulary
development surpassed researchers' expectations, she never acquired mastery of basic grammar
and language pragmatics. Since her rescue occurred when she was at the close of the period of
synaptic pruning in regions of the brain relevant to language development (see Figure 1), her
irreversible deficits in certain domains of language use were not unexpected.18
The second kind of experience contributes to experience-dependent development. These are
experiences affecting the brain that can vary across individuals. Their influence can occur at any
age (not just during an early sensitive period) and they can be virtually any kind of experience
that leads to the formation of new synapses or modification of existing synaptic connections in
the brain. Needless to say, the range of potential experiences contributing to this kind of
development is extremely broad: learning how to play the piano, indulging an interest in poetry,
joining a support group, or developing deeper understanding of American history are examples.
Experience-expectant development constitutes life-long brain plasticity, and helps to account for
the increasingly individualized character of brain growth as the years proceed. Although the
brain completes its maturation by early adulthood (see Figure 1), evidence that the brain
continues to be capable of changing throughout life attests to the life-long importance of
experience-dependent development. Neurobiological studies with adults suggest, for example,
that new neurons and synaptic connections develop in the hippocampus, a brain structure
important for learning and memory, in response to experiences promoting new learning.19
The profound developmental potential afforded by experience-expectant development is one
reason the human brain is such a powerful learning organ, and it helps to account for the
astonishingly early and rapid mental advances of early childhood. This developmental potential
is one reason that parents who can afford them enlist enrichment experiences for their young
children, such as art, music, or other kinds of classes or tutoring, even though there is little or no
evidence that such experiences can accelerate early brain development beyond normal limits.20
There is, however, abundant evidence that deprivation and adversity can undermine healthy
brain development. Unfortunately, experience-dependent development is undiscriminating with
respect to the kinds of experiences affecting the brain's growth: it incorporates harm as well as
enrichment into its developing architecture.21 Deprivation of important supports (such as
nurturant care) can also significantly undermine the developing brain through the loss of
opportunities that would otherwise strengthen important neural networks.
Implications for Public Health Practitioners
A quarter-century ago, the American public was rocked by a revolution in their understanding of
early childhood development that focused on early brain development and its implications.22 In
the years since, developmental science has modified but not fundamentally changed the account
that parents and others learned then. Early brain development is foundational to the growth of
life-long capacities, and many of these foundations are established prenatally. Early experiences
Doi: 10.32481/djph.2024.10.03
are important to the development and refinement of neural connections in the early years,
although the hazards of adverse experiences are much clearer than are the benefits of
enrichment. The brain's adaptability, or plasticity, is one of its superpowers for tailoring its
development to the child's experience in a specific environment, and this continues to be a
strength beyond early childhood as the brain continues to develop in later life. The brain's
adaptability can be a liability, however, when it incorporates into its architecture the effects of
abuse, deprivation, and trauma. In light of the brain's rapid growth, identifying and remediating
developmental problems early in life is important before these problems become consolidated,
and doing so can be far more cost-effective than trying to remediate them later.
Understanding the implications of developmental neuroscience for parents, practitioners, and
policymakers has been as important as understanding the science, and the past 25 years have
witnessed slow progress on important science-based initiatives for children, as well as the
excesses of overpromising and the liabilities of ignoring important needs.22 For public health
practitioners, however, the implications of the science may be self-evident. Improving the
prenatal care of expectant mothers, with particular attention to the hazards to fetal brain
development of nutritional inadequacy, maternal stress, and toxic exposures. Strengthening the
supports for young children within their families, especially in the availability of nurturant
relationships, adequate nutrition, safe environments, and learning opportunities. Early
intervention when young children's environments are unsafe or neglectful. Expanding programs
to address the multiple threats deriving from childhood poverty, a multisystemic stressor with
documented effects on developing children's brains and behavior.23 Early detection of
developmental problems, coupled with state-of-the-science interventions to remediate them.
Enlarging the availability of early learning opportunities, but with an important proviso. Early
childhood programs to support young children's learning need to be buttressed by strong primary
education opportunities to build on that foundation. The research is clear that early education
cannot alone lead to high school graduation and other later achievements.
There are other implications of the research on early brain development. The insights on the
developing brain that have been emerging during the past quarter century provide continuing
catalysts for the responsibilities of those of us committed to young children's healthy
development.
Dr. Thompson may be contacted at rathompson@ucdavis.edu.
References
1. Dekaban, A. S., & Sadowsky, D. (1978, October). Changes in brain weights during the span
of human life: Relation of brain weights to body heights and body weights. Annals of
Neurology, 4(4), 345–356. PubMedhttps://doi.org/10.1002/ana.410040410
2. Cowan, W. M. (1979, September). The development of the brain. Scientific American,
241(3), 112–133. PubMedhttps://doi.org/10.1038/scientificamerican0979-112
3. Kundakovic, M., & Jaric, I. (2017, March 18). The epigenetic link between prenatal adverse
environments and neurodevelopmental disorders. Genes, 8(3), 104.
PubMedhttps://doi.org/10.3390/genes8030104
Doi: 10.32481/djph.2024.10.03
4. Doi, M., Usui, N., & Shimada, S. (2022, March 15). Prenatal environment and
neurodevelopmental disorders. Frontiers in Endocrinology (Lausanne), 13, 860110.
PubMedhttps://doi.org/10.3389/fendo.2022.860110
5. Lumey, L. H., Stein, A. D., Kahn, H. S., van der Pal-de Bruin, K. M., Blauw, G. J., Zybert,
P. A., & Susser, E. S. (2007, December). Cohort profile: The Dutch Hunger Winter families
study. International Journal of Epidemiology, 36(6), 1196–1204.
PubMedhttps://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dym126
6. Schulz, L. C. (2010, September 28). The Dutch Hunger Winter and the developmental
origins of health and disease. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
United States of America, 107(39), 16757–16758.
PubMedhttps://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1012911107
7. Dunkel Schetter, C., & Tanner, L. (2012, March). Anxiety, depression and stress in
pregnancy: Implications for mothers, children, research, and practice. Current Opinion in
Psychiatry, 25(2), 141–148. PubMedhttps://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0b013e3283503680
8. Monk, C., Lugo-Candelas, C., & Trumpff, C. (2019). Prenatal developmental origins of future
psychopathology: Mechanisms and pathways. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 15,
16.1-16.28.
9. Posner, J., Cha, J., Roy, A. K., Peterson, B. S., Bansal, R., Gustafsson, H. C., . . . Monk, C.
(2016, November 1). Alterations in amygdala-prefrontal circuits in infants exposed to
prenatal maternal depression. Translational Psychiatry, 6(11), e935.
PubMedhttps://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2016.146
10. Davis, E. P., & Thompson, R. A. (2014). Prenatal foundations: Fetal programming of health
and development. Zero to Three, 34, 6–11.
11. Center for the Developing Child. (2024). Brain architecture. Retrieved July 8, 2024 from
https://developingchild.harvard.edu/science/key-concepts/brain-architecture/
12. Huttenlocher, P. R., & Dabholkar, A. S. (1997, October 20). Regional differences in
synaptogenesis in human cerebral cortex. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 387(2),
167–178. PubMedhttps://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19971020)387:2<167::AID-
CNE1>3.0.CO;2-Z
13. Lepeta, K., Lourenco, M. V., Schweitzer, B. C., Martino Adami, P. V., Banerjee, P.,
Catuara-Solarz, S., . . . Seidenbecher, C. (2016, September). Synaptopathies: Synaptic
dysfunction in neurological disorders - A review from students to students. Journal of
Neurochemistry, 138(6), 785–805. PubMedhttps://doi.org/10.1111/jnc.13713
14. Kuhl, P. K. (2004, November). Early language acquisition: Cracking the speech code.
Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 5(11), 831–843. PubMedhttps://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1533
15. de Boysson-Bardies, B., Halle, P., Sagart, L., & Durand, C. (1989, February). A
crosslinguistic investigation of vowel formants in babbling. Journal of Child Language,
16(1), 1–17. PubMedhttps://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900013404
16. Rivera-Gaxiola, M., Silva-Pereyra, J., & Kuhl, P. K. (2005, March). Brain potentials to
native and non-native speech contrasts in 7- and 11-month-old American infants.
Doi: 10.32481/djph.2024.10.03
Developmental Science, 8(2), 162–172. PubMedhttps://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-
7687.2005.00403.x
17. Johnson, M. H., & de Haan, M. (2015). Developmental cognitive neuroscience: An
introduction (4th Ed.). New York: Wiley-Blackwell.
18. Rymer, R. (1994). Genie: A scientific tragedy (2nd Ed.). New York: Harper Perennial.
19. Erickson, K. I., Voss, M. W., Prakash, R. S., Basak, C., Szabo, A., Chaddock, L., . . .
Kramer, A. F. (2011). Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves
memory. PNAS Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 3017-3022.
20. Tierney, A. L., & Nelson, C. A., III. (2009, November 1). Brain development and the role of
experience in the early years. Zero to Three, 30(2), 9–13. PubMed
21. Kim-Spoon, J., Herd, T., Brieant, A., Peviani, K., Deater-Deckard, K., Lauharatanahirun,
N., . . . King-Casas, B. (2021, April). Maltreatment and brain development: The effects of
abuse and neglect on longitudinal trajectories of neural activation during risk processing and
cognitive control. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 48, 100939.
PubMedhttps://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2021.100939
22. Thompson, R. A. (2023). The brain development revolution: Science, the media, and public
policy. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
23. Evans, G. W., Chen, E., Miller, G. E., & Seeman, T. (2012). How poverty gets under the
skin: A life course perspective. In R. King & V. Mahalmes (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of
Poverty and Child Development (pp. 13-36). New York: Oxford University Press.
Copyright (c) 2024 Delaware Academy of Medicine / Delaware Public Health Association.
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Non-Commercial
License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/) which permits unrestricted non-commercial use,
distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any citations for this publication.
Article
Full-text available
The internal and external environment of the mother during the developmental stages of the fetus affects the offspring’s health. According to the developmental origins of health and disease (DOHaD) theory, environmental factors influence the offspring and also affect health in adulthood. Recently, studies based on this theory have gained attracted attention because of their clinical utility in identifying the risk groups for various diseases. Neurodevelopmental disorders (NDDs) such as autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) can be caused by exposure to certain prenatal environments during pregnancy. This review describes the latest findings on the effect of prenatal environment on the onset mechanism of NDDs based on the DOHaD theory. Unravelling the molecular mechanisms underlying the pathogenesis of NDDs is important, because there are no therapeutic drugs for these disorders. Furthermore, elucidating the relationship between the DOHaD theory and NDDs will contribute to the popularization of preventive medicine.
Article
Full-text available
The profound effects of child maltreatment on brain functioning have been documented. Yet, little is known about whether distinct maltreatment experiences are differentially related to underlying neural processes of risky decision making: valuation and control. Using conditional growth curve modeling, we compared a cumulative approach versus a dimensional approach (relative effects of abuse and neglect) to examine the link between child maltreatment and brain development. The sample included 167 adolescents (13-14 years at Time 1, 53% male), assessed annually four times. Risk processing was assessed by blood-oxygen-level-dependent responses (BOLD) during a lottery choice task, and cognitive control by BOLD responses during the Multi-Source Interference Task. Cumulative maltreatment effects on insula and dorsolateral anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) activation during risk processing were not significant. However, neglect (but not abuse) was associated with slower developmental increases in insula and dACC activation. In contrast, cumulative maltreatment effects on fronto-parietal activation during cognitive control were significant, and abuse (but not neglect) was associated with steeper developmental decreases in fronto-parietal activation. The results suggest neglect effects on detrimental neurodevelopment of the valuation system and abuse effects on accelerated neurodevelopment of the control system, highlighting differential effects of distinct neglect versus abuse adverse experiences on neurodevelopment.
Article
Full-text available
Synapses are essential components of neurons and allow information to travel coordinately throughout the nervous system to adjust behavior to environmental stimuli and to control body functions, memories, and emotions. Thus, optimal synaptic communication is required for proper brain physiology, and slight perturbations of synapse function can lead to brain disorders. In fact, increasing evidence has demonstrated the relevance of synapse dysfunction as a major determinant of many neurological diseases. This notion has led to the concept of synaptopathies as brain diseases with synapse defects as shared pathogenic features. In this review, which was initiated at the 13th International Society for Neurochemistry Advanced School, we discuss basic concepts of synapse structure and function, and provide a critical view of how aberrant synapse physiology may contribute to neurodevelopmental disorders (autism, Down syndrome, startle disease, and epilepsy) as well as neurodegenerative disorders (Alzheimer and Parkinson disease). We finally discuss the appropriateness and potential implications of gathering synapse diseases under a single term. Understanding common causes and intrinsic differences in disease-associated synaptic dysfunction could offer novel clues toward synapse-based therapeutic intervention for neurological and neuropsychiatric disorders. In this Review, which was initiated at the 13th International Society for Neurochemistry (ISN) Advanced School, we discuss basic concepts of synapse structure and function, and provide a critical view of how aberrant synapse physiology may contribute to neurodevelopmental (autism, Down syndrome, startle disease, and epilepsy) as well as neurodegenerative disorders (Alzheimer's and Parkinson's diseases), gathered together under the term of synaptopathies. Read the Editorial Highlight for this article on page .
Article
Full-text available
The developmental origins of health and disease hypothesis applied to neurodevelopmental outcomes asserts that the fetal origins of future development are relevant to mental health. There is a third pathway for the familial inheritance of risk for psychiatric illness beyond shared genes and the quality of parental care: the impact of pregnant women's distress—defined broadly to include perceived stress, life events, depression, and anxiety—on fetal and infant brain–behavior development. We discuss epidemiological and observational clinical data demonstrating that maternal distress is associated with children's increased risk for psychopathology: For example, high maternal anxiety is associated with a twofold increase in the risk of probable mental disorder in children. We review several biological systems hypothesized to be mechanisms by which maternal distress affects fetal and child brain and behavior development, as well as the clinical implications of studies of the developmental origins of health and disease that focus on maternal distress. Development and parenting begin before birth. Expected final online publication date for the Annual Review of Clinical Psychology Volume 15 is May 7, 2019. Please see http://www.annualreviews.org/page/journal/pubdates for revised estimates.
Article
Full-text available
Prenatal adverse environments, such as maternal stress, toxicological exposures, and viral infections, can disrupt normal brain development and contribute to neurodevelopmental disorders, including schizophrenia, depression, and autism. Increasing evidence shows that these short- and long-term effects of prenatal exposures on brain structure and function are mediated by epigenetic mechanisms. Animal studies demonstrate that prenatal exposure to stress, toxins, viral mimetics, and drugs induces lasting epigenetic changes in the brain, including genes encoding glucocorticoid receptor (Nr3c1) and brain-derived neurotrophic factor (Bdnf). These epigenetic changes have been linked to changes in brain gene expression, stress reactivity, and behavior, and often times, these effects are shown to be dependent on the gestational window of exposure, sex, and exposure level. Although evidence from human studies is more limited, gestational exposure to environmental risks in humans is associated with epigenetic changes in peripheral tissues, and future studies are required to understand whether we can use peripheral biomarkers to predict neurobehavioral outcomes. An extensive research effort combining well-designed human and animal studies, with comprehensive epigenomic analyses of peripheral and brain tissues over time, will be necessary to improve our understanding of the epigenetic basis of neurodevelopmental disorders.
Article
Full-text available
Prenatal exposure to maternal depression is common and puts offspring at risk for developing a range of neuropsychiatric disorders. Despite its prevalence and adverse associations, neurobiological processes by which prenatal maternal depression (PMD) confers risk remain poorly understood. Maternal mood and fetal behavior were assessed between 34 and 37 gestational weeks. Using resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and diffusion MRI, we examined functional and structural connectivity within amygdala-prefrontal circuits in 64 infants (mean age=5.8±1.7 weeks) with (n=20) and without (n=44) in utero exposure to PMD. Resting fMRI and diffusion MRI both indicated atypical amygdala-prefrontal connectivity in PMD-exposed infants: Resting fMRI indicated increased inverse, or negative, functional connectivity between the amygdala and the dorsal prefrontal cortex (PFC), bilaterally, and diffusion MRI indicated decreased structural connectivity between the right amygdala and the right ventral PFC. Spectral dynamic causal modeling supported these findings suggesting altered amygdala-PFC effective (or directed) connectivity in PMD-exposed infants. Last, path analyses supported a mechanistic account relating PMD to a third-trimester fetal behavior: PMD alters amygdala-PFC connectivity, which in turn, is associated with an increase in fetal heart rate reactivity to in utero perturbation. These data suggest that the maturation and coordination of central and peripheral physiology are altered by prenatal exposure to maternal depression. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to directly associate infant MRI measures with a behavior - fetal heart rate response, and supports hypotheses that PMD-associated variations in the development of amygdala-PFC circuits are relevant for future neurobehavioral maturation.
Article
Full-text available
Synapses are essential components of neurons and allow information to travel coordinately throughout the nervous system to adjust behavior to environmental stimuli and to control body functions, memories, and emotions. Thus, optimal synaptic communication is required for proper brain physiology, and slight perturbations of synapse function can lead to brain disorders. In fact, increasing evidence has demonstrated the relevance of synapse dysfunction as a major determinant of many neurological diseases. This notion has led to the concept of synaptopathies as brain diseases with synapse defects as shared pathogenic features. In this review, which was initiated at the 13th International Society for Neurochemistry Advanced School, we discuss basic concepts of synapse structure and function, and provide a critical view of how aberrant synapse physiology may contribute to neurodevelopmental disorders (autism, Down syndrome, startle disease, and epilepsy) as well as neurodegenerative disorders (Alzheimer and Parkinson disease). We finally discuss the appropriateness and potential implications of gathering synapse diseases under a single term. Understanding common causes and intrinsic differences in disease‐associated synaptic dysfunction could offer novel clues toward synapse‐based therapeutic intervention for neurological and neuropsychiatric disorders. image In this Review, which was initiated at the 13th International Society for Neurochemistry ( ISN ) Advanced School, we discuss basic concepts of synapse structure and function, and provide a critical view of how aberrant synapse physiology may contribute to neurodevelopmental (autism, Down syndrome, startle disease, and epilepsy) as well as neurodegenerative disorders (Alzheimer's and Parkinson's diseases), gathered together under the term of synaptopathies. Read the Editorial Highlight for this article on page 783 .
Book
The science of human development informs our thinking about children and their development. The Brain Development Revolution asks how and why has brain development become the major lens for understanding child development, and its consequences. It describes the 1997 I Am Your Child campaign that engaged public attention through a sophisticated media communications effort, a White House conference, and other events. It explores the campaign's impact, including voter initiatives to fund early childhood programs and a national campaign for prekindergarten education, but also several missed opportunities. The study examines why brain development compels our attention, why we are – but shouldn't be – neurodeterminists, and the challenges of communicating developmental brain science. This book examines the framing of the brain development story, the selectivity of the messaging, and overpromising the results of early programs. Lastly, it discusses proposals for how science communication can be improved to better serve children and the public.
Article
There is a large epidemiological literature documenting inverse relations between socioeconomic status (SES) and morbidity as well as mortality. In this chapter we focus on biological mechanisms to explain how disadvantage gets under the skin. We adopt a life course perspective on this topic because it illuminates several issues: whether the timing and duration of exposure to disadvantage over the life course matter, and factors that may cause biological mechanisms, changed by deprivation in early life, to persist throughout the life course. This chapter is organized into 5 major sections. Sections 1 through 3 review evidence linking SES or one of its primary constituents to disease-relevant biological mechanisms during childhood, during adulthood, and prospectively from childhood to adulthood, respectively, and section 4 examines the durability of early life deprivation and altered trajectories in biological mechanisms over the life course. We conclude with section 5, which presents a research agenda and discusses intervention consequences of a life course perspective on the biology of disadvantage.