ArticlePDF Available

Structural equation modelling of the role of cognition in functional interference and treatment nonadherence among haemodialysis patients

PLOS
PLOS One
Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Background and objectives Cognitive impairment is common in haemodialysis patients and associated with adverse health outcomes. This may be due to cognitive impairments interfering with daily functioning and self-care, but evidence is limited. This cross-sectional study aims to explore the interrelationships between cognition and functional outcomes in haemodialysis patients. Methods Haemodialysis patients completed measures of objective cognitive function (Montreal Cognitive Assessment), everyday problem-solving skills (scenario-based task), and subjective cognitive complaints (self-report). Participants also self-reported sociodemographic information, functional interference, treatment nonadherence, and mood and fatigue symptoms. Patients’ clinical data including comorbidities and lab results were extracted from medical record. Structural equation modelling was performed. Results A total of 268 haemodialysis patients (mean age = 59.87 years; 42.5% female) participated. The final model showed satisfactory fit: CFI = 0.916, TLI = 0.905, RMSEA = 0.033 (90% confidence interval 0.024 to 0.041), SRMR = 0.066, χ²(493) = 618.573 (p < .001). There was a negative association between objective cognitive function and subjective cognitive complaints. Cognitive complaints were positively associated with both functional interference and treatment nonadherence, whereas objective performance was not. Everyday problem-solving skills emerged as a distinct aspect of cognition not associated with objective performance or subjective complaints, but had additive utility in predicting functional interference. Conclusions Subjective cognitive complaints and everyday problem-solving skills appear to be stronger predictors of functional variables compared to objective performance based on traditional tests. Routine screening of everyday cognitive difficulties may allow for early identification of dialysis patients at risk of cognitive impairment, functional interference, treatment nonadherence, and poor clinical outcomes.
Content may be subject to copyright.
RESEARCH ARTICLE
Structural equation modelling of the role of
cognition in functional interference and
treatment nonadherence among
haemodialysis patients
Frederick H. F. ChanID
1
, Pearl Sim
1
, Phoebe X. H. LimID
1
, Xiaoli Zhu
1,2
, Jimmy Lee
1,3
,
Sabrina Haroon
4
, Titus Wai Leong Lau
4
, Allen Yan Lun Liu
5
, Behram A. Khan
6,7,8
, Jason C.
J. Choo
9,10
, Konstadina GrivaID
1
*
1Lee Kong Chian School of Medicine, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore, Singapore, 2Nursing
Services, National Healthcare Group Polyclinics, Singapore, Singapore, 3Institute of Mental Health,
Singapore, Singapore, 4Division of Nephrology, Department of Medicine, National University Hospital,
Singapore, Singapore, 5Khoo Teck Puat Hospital, Singapore, Singapore, 6Renal Health Services,
Singapore, Singapore, 7National University Health System, Singapore, Singapore, 8Duke-NUS Medical
School, Singapore, Singapore, 9National Kidney Foundation, Singapore, Singapore, 10 Department of
Renal Medicine, Singapore General Hospital, Singapore, Singapore
*konstadina.griva@ntu.edu.sg
Abstract
Background and objectives
Cognitive impairment is common in haemodialysis patients and associated with adverse
health outcomes. This may be due to cognitive impairments interfering with daily functioning
and self-care, but evidence is limited. This cross-sectional study aims to explore the interre-
lationships between cognition and functional outcomes in haemodialysis patients.
Methods
Haemodialysis patients completed measures of objective cognitive function (Montreal Cog-
nitive Assessment), everyday problem-solving skills (scenario-based task), and subjective
cognitive complaints (self-report). Participants also self-reported sociodemographic infor-
mation, functional interference, treatment nonadherence, and mood and fatigue symptoms.
Patients’ clinical data including comorbidities and lab results were extracted from medical
record. Structural equation modelling was performed.
Results
A total of 268 haemodialysis patients (mean age = 59.87 years; 42.5% female) partici-
pated. The final model showed satisfactory fit: CFI = 0.916, TLI = 0.905, RMSEA = 0.033
(90% confidence interval 0.024 to 0.041), SRMR = 0.066, χ
2
(493) = 618.573 (p<.001).
There was a negative association between objective cognitive function and subjective cog-
nitive complaints. Cognitive complaints were positively associated with both functional
interference and treatment nonadherence, whereas objective performance was not.
PLOS ONE
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0312039 October 17, 2024 1 / 17
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
OPEN ACCESS
Citation: Chan FHF, Sim P, Lim PXH, Zhu X, Lee J,
Haroon S, et al. (2024) Structural equation
modelling of the role of cognition in functional
interference and treatment nonadherence among
haemodialysis patients. PLoS ONE 19(10):
e0312039. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.
pone.0312039
Editor: Henry H.L. Wu, Kolling Institute of Medical
Research, The University of Sydney, AUSTRALIA
Received: May 6, 2024
Accepted: September 30, 2024
Published: October 17, 2024
Peer Review History: PLOS recognizes the
benefits of transparency in the peer review
process; therefore, we enable the publication of
all of the content of peer review and author
responses alongside final, published articles. The
editorial history of this article is available here:
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0312039
Copyright: ©2024 Chan et al. This is an open
access article distributed under the terms of the
Creative Commons Attribution License, which
permits unrestricted use, distribution, and
reproduction in any medium, provided the original
author and source are credited.
Data Availability Statement: All relevant data are
within the paper and its Supporting information
files.
Everyday problem-solving skills emerged as a distinct aspect of cognition not associated
with objective performance or subjective complaints, but had additive utility in predicting
functional interference.
Conclusions
Subjective cognitive complaints and everyday problem-solving skills appear to be stronger
predictors of functional variables compared to objective performance based on traditional
tests. Routine screening of everyday cognitive difficulties may allow for early identification of
dialysis patients at risk of cognitive impairment, functional interference, treatment nonadher-
ence, and poor clinical outcomes.
Introduction
End-stage renal disease (ESRD) is the most advanced stage of chronic kidney disease where
kidney function is irreversibly lost, necessitating dialysis or transplantation [1,2]. Cognitive
impairments (CIs) are common in ESRD patients receiving haemodialysis (HD) treatment,
with more than 70% exhibiting at least mild impairments in one or more domains such as
attention, memory, and executive function [36]. “Brain fog” has been a common complaint
among dialysis patients [7] and a popular topic of discussion in online patient forums [8]. CIs
in ESRD are associated with adverse health outcomes including dialysis withdrawal, hospitali-
sation, and mortality [3,911]. These associations are assumed to be due to CIs interfering
with daily functioning, decision-making, and self-management capabilities, however empirical
evidence is scarce.
CIs are typically accompanied by functional interference because performance of everyday
activities (e.g., personal hygiene, managing finances, etc.) is dependent upon the integrity of
cognitive, motor, and sensory-perceptual skills [12]. In the context of ESRD, a specific aspect
of daily functioning that is of particular relevance to patients is self-care and treatment adher-
ence. HD patients are prescribed complex medical regimen that requires them to take daily
medications, follow strict dietary guidelines, control fluid intake, and attend thrice-weekly
dialysis sessions. These self-care activities are cognitively demanding and can be challenging
for those with CIs. Medication taking, for instance, requires multiple cognitive processes
including encoding and storage of health information (e.g., understanding the importance of
taking medicine), executive function (e.g., developing a plan to adhere), prospective memory
(e.g., remembering to take medicine on time), working memory (e.g., keeping the intention to
take medicines active while preparing to take it), and source monitoring (e.g., remembering
whether the medicine has been taken) [13].
Clearly cognition plays an essential role in HD patients’ daily functioning and ability to
maintain and optimise their health by following the prescribed medical regimen. However,
currently we lack a comprehensive picture of the interplay between aspects of cognition and
aspects of daily functioning in this population. Studies using neuropsychological tests, the gold
standard measure of cognition, have found positive associations between objective cognitive
ability and functional independence in dialysis patients [14]. Other studies have used self-
reported measures to assess subjective cognitive complaints (SCCs). More frequent SCCs in
HD patients have been found to be associated with greater functional impairment [15], and
worse treatment adherence indicated by self-reports and laboratory results [16,17]. Further-
more, scenario-based tasks that assess everyday problem-solving skills have been used. These
PLOS ONE
Cognition and patient outcomes in haemodialysis
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0312039 October 17, 2024 2 / 17
Funding: This work was supported by the
Venerable Yen Pei-National Kidney Foundation
Research Fund, Singapore [grant number NKFRC/
2021/01/02]. KG received research funding from
National Kidney Foundation Singapore. The funding
sources had no role in the study design,
recruitment of patients, data collection, analysis,
interpretation of the results, writing of the
manuscript, or decision to submit the manuscript
for publication.
Competing interests: The authors have declared
that no competing interests exist.
tasks require participants to generate solutions in response to problem scenarios, and are
thought to reflect executive ability in real-world contexts [18]. Two studies found that everyday
problem-solving skills positively predicted medication adherence in kidney transplant recipi-
ents [19,20].
Despite these promising findings, a study that comprehensively examines the complex asso-
ciations between cognition and real-world functioning in HD patients is lacking. As such, the
current cross-sectional study aims to adopt structural equation modelling (SEM) analysis to
disentangle the interrelationships among multiple aspects of cognition (i.e., objective perfor-
mance, subjective complaint, and everyday problem-solving) and key outcomes (i.e., func-
tional interference, treatment nonadherence, and clinical endpoints) in HD patients. SEM is a
powerful and flexible statistical technique that integrates factor analysis, path analysis, and
regression into a single framework [21]. It simultaneously accounts for multiple direct and
indirect associations among a range of variables, hence allowing for the validation of complex
theoretical models using a unified approach [21]. With this advanced statistical technique, this
study will map key cognitive indicators of functional interference and nonadherence so that
future interventions and support strategies could be developed to target the cognitive chal-
lenges that interfere with these key endpoints.
The hypothesised model to be tested in the current study is shown in Fig 1. We hypothe-
sised that objective cognitive function and everyday problem-solving skills would covary
(hence the bidirectional arrow) since they are both considered indicators of distinct aspects
of cognitive abilities [18]. While objective tests reflect individuals’ cognitive performance in
optimal conditions, problem-solving tasks reflect individuals’ problem-solving ability in
everyday contexts. We also hypothesised that these two variables would directly contribute
to SCCs since individuals with worse cognitive performance may experience more cogni-
tive difficulties in daily lives, hence reporting more frequent SCCs [7,22]. Furthermore, the
three cognitive indicators were each hypothesised to have a direct effect on functional inter-
ference and treatment nonadherence based on previous evidence [1417,19,20].
Fig 1. Hypothesised model of associations between cognition, functional interference, and treatment nonadherence.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0312039.g001
PLOS ONE
Cognition and patient outcomes in haemodialysis
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0312039 October 17, 2024 3 / 17
Materials and methods
Participants
A convenience sample of HD patients was recruited from the National Kidney Foundation
Singapore (NKF) between May 19 and November 4 2022. Ten NKF dialysis centres were
selected to ensure geographical representation of dialysis centres across Singapore. The inclu-
sion criteria were: (1) 21 years of age or older, (2) an estimated glomerular filtration rate lower
than 15mL/min/1.73m
2
, (3) having undergone HD treatment for at least 3 months, and (4) flu-
ent in either English or Mandarin. The exclusion criteria were: (1) only fluent in dialects, (2)
unable to give consent due to psychiatric diagnoses or established diagnosis of dementia, or
(3) unable to complete survey due to visual or hearing impairments.
Procedure
The study protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Nanyang Techno-
logical University (NTU-IRB-2021-025). A list of eligible patients was provided by the nurse
managers of each dialysis centre. Study team members fluent in the patients’ preferred lan-
guage approached each patient during their dialysis sessions. After obtaining written consent,
the following instruments were administered. Upon completion, patients were given a cash
compensation.
Measures
Objective cognitive function. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) was used to
assess objective cognitive function [23]. The MoCA is a cognitive screening test that assesses
visuospatial and executive functions (i.e., Trail-Making Test part B, cube copy, clock drawing,
abstraction), attention (i.e., digit span forward and backward, vigilance, serial-7 subtraction),
short-term memory (i.e., delayed recall), language (i.e., naming, sentence repetition, verbal flu-
ency), and orientation (i.e., awareness of time and place) [23].
Everyday problem-solving skills. The Everyday Problem-Solving (EPS) task consists of
real-world problem scenarios where participants were asked to generate solutions. Six scenar-
ios that have been used in previously studies were used in the current study [19,24]. Three sce-
narios described general daily problems and the other three described health-specific
problems. For each scenario participants were asked to generate as many solutions as possible.
The number of safe and effective solutions generated by each patient was used as an indicator
of problem-solving skills [20].
Subjective cognitive complaints. SCCs were measured using the 33-item Patient’s
Assessment of Own Functioning Inventory (PAOFI) [25]. This measure assesses SCCs in four
domains: memory (10 items), language (nine items), motor/sensory-perceptual ability (five
items), and higher-level cognitive functions (nine items). Participants rated on a six-point
Likert scale from “almost never” to “almost always” [25]. Mean scores of the four subscales
were calculated, with a higher score indicating more frequent SCCs. Cronbach’s alpha was
0.87 for the memory subscale, 0.89 for the language subscale, 0.73 for the motor/sensory-per-
ceptual subscale, and 0.87 for the higher-level cognitive function subscale.
Functional interference. We assessed functional interference as a key dependent variable
of the current study. The Work and Social Adjustment Scale (WSAS) is a measure of self-per-
ceived functional interference in five domains (i.e., work, home management, social leisure
activities, private leisure activities, and social relationships) attributable to an identified prob-
lem [26]. The original five items were used but preface was reworded to be cognition-specific.
PLOS ONE
Cognition and patient outcomes in haemodialysis
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0312039 October 17, 2024 4 / 17
Participants rated on a nine-point Likert scale ranging from 0 (i.e., “not at all impaired”) to 8
(i.e., “very severely impaired”). Cronbach’s alpha was 0.93 for WSAS.
Treatment nonadherence. Patients’ self-reported medication nonadherence was mea-
sured by the five-item Medication Adherence Report Scale (MARS-5 ©Professor Rob Horne),
which was rated on a five-point scale ranging from “never” to “always” [27]. A higher total
score indicates poorer medication adherence. Cronbach’s alpha was 0.77. The Dialysis Diet
and Fluid non-adherence Questionnaire (DDFQ) was also assessed. DDFQ is a four-item scale
that assesses frequency and degree of dietary and fluid nonadherence in dialysis patients [28].
Moreover, we collected patients’ interdialytic weight gain (IDWG) as a clinical indicator of
fluid adherence. Relative IDWG (i.e., the ratio of absolute IDWG to a patient’s dry weight) was
assessed prior to each dialysis session in the week of and the week before the survey date. The
IDWG values were averaged across the two weeks. Additionally, the latest lab assay results
(i.e., sodium [Na], potassium [K], calcium [Ca], phosphorus [PO4], and calcium-phosphorus
product [Ca×PO4]) were collected and used as indicators of dietary and medication
adherence.
Mood and fatigue symptoms. Patients’ mood symptoms were measured by the two-item
Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-2; α= 0.65) and the two-item Generalised Anxiety Disor-
der (GAD-2; α= 0.77). These two measures are brief screening tools of depression and anxiety
that have been used in dialysis patients [29,30]. Fatigue was measured using the one-item
vitality subscale from the Kidney Disease Quality of Life questionnaire [3133]. Higher scores
indicate greater mood or fatigue symptoms.
Sociodemographic and clinical information. Self-reported demographic information
was collected, including age, gender, ethnicity, education, relationship status, and employment
status. Clinical information including primary kidney disease diagnosis, comorbidities, dura-
tion on HD, dialysis dose (Kt/V), and medication count, were extracted from patients’ medical
record.
Statistical analyses
The steps of conducting the SEM analysis involved (1) testing of the baseline measurement
model with confirmatory factor analysis (CFA), (2) specification of the structural model con-
taining all hypothesised paths, and (3) modification of the structural model [3436]. These
steps were performed using the “lavaan” package with the WLSMV estimator in R 4.2.2 [37,
38]. To assess model fit, we used the Comparative Fit Index (CFI) [39], Tucker—Lewis Index
(TLI) [40], the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) [41], the Standardised
Root Mean Square (SRMR) [21], and the Chi-square [21]. For CFI and TLI, values above 0.90
were considered to indicate adequate model fit, and values above 0.95 were considered to indi-
cate excellent fit [39,4245]. RMSEA values lower than 0.06 and SRMR values lower than 0.08
were considered to indicate good fit [21].
Model development
Measurement model. The measurement model was first constructed, with a total of eight
latent variables, each measured by multiple indicator variables. Three latent variables were
constructed to reflect different aspects of cognition, namely “objective cognitive function”,
“everyday problem-solving skills”, and “subjective cognitive complaints”. These three latent
variables were indicated by the subscale/subdomain scores in the MoCA, EPS, and PAOFI,
respectively. Two latent variables were constructed for the dependent variables, including
“functional interference” measured by the five WSAS items, and “treatment nonadherence”
PLOS ONE
Cognition and patient outcomes in haemodialysis
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0312039 October 17, 2024 5 / 17
measured by self-report (i.e., MARS-5 and DDFQ), and physiological and biochemical param-
eters (i.e., IDWG, Na, K, Ca, PO4, and Ca×PO4).
Following previous SEM studies of cognitive function in patients with chronic disease [20,
34,46], we constructed three additional latent variables in order to account for the confound-
ing effects sociodemographic, clinical, and psychological factors in the hypothesised relation-
ships. Specifically, a “sociodemographic” latent variable (indicated by age and years of
education) and a “comorbidity” latent variable (indicated by presence of diabetes, hyperten-
sion, hyperlipidaemia, and cardiovascular disease) were constructed because these are estab-
lished risk or protective factors of CIs in HD patients [4749]. A “mood and fatigue
symptoms” latent variable (indicated by PHQ-2, GAD-2, and KDQOL vitality item) was also
specified because these symptoms can exacerbate SCCs [22] and are associated with beha-
vioural and clinical outcomes in ESRD patients [50].
A CFA was first conducted to verify the measurement quality of the eight latent variables.
The fit indices for the baseline measurement model were as follows: CFI = 0.839, TLI = 0.820,
RMSEA = 0.043 (90% confidence interval 0.036 to 0.049), SRMR = 0.078, χ
2
(566) = 810.166 (p
<.001), which suggested unsatisfactory model fit. All indicator variables had significant factor
loadings on their corresponding latent variables except for two adherence indicators (i.e., Na
and Ca). These two indicators were therefore removed from the model. However, fit indices
for the revised measurement model were still in the unacceptable range, CFI = 0.853,
TLI = 0.835, RMSEA = 0.043 (90% confidence interval 0.036 to 0.050), SRMR = 0.078, χ
2
(499)
= 719.322 (p<.001).
Modification indices suggested inclusion of error covariance between PO4 and Ca×PO4 in
the model. These two variables were indeed highly correlated with each other, r= 0.95, p<
.001. There is evidence that calcium and phosphorus are controlled by similar regulatory
mechanisms [51], and high levels of serum phosphorus can combine with calcium to form cal-
cium-phosphorus product [52]. It may therefore be theoretically justifiable to allow the resid-
ual terms of these two variables to freely covary [21]. We also added error covariance among
DDFQ items due to the similar wordings of these questions, and we added error covariance
between relative IDWG and the two fluid items in DDFQ as they are both thought to reflect
fluid adherence. Following this modification, the final measurement model showed acceptable
fit with the exception of Chi-square, CFI = 0.917, TLI = 0.905, RMSEA = 0.033 (90% confi-
dence interval 0.024 to 0.041), SRMR = 0.065, χ
2
(492) = 616.607 (p<.001). However, Chi-
square test is sensitive to sample size with larger samples decreasing the pvalue; the significant
Chi-square was therefore not used as a basis for model rejection.
In the final measurement model, all factor loadings of the latent variables were at a signifi-
cance level of p<.05. Detailed description of the final measurement model, including the
latent and indicator variables, as well as their measurement, interpretation, mean values, per-
centage, and factor loadings, are presented in Table 1.
Structural model. A structural model was then constructed to examine the hypothesised
regression paths between the latent variables specified in Fig 1. Objective cognitive function
and EPS skills were hypothesised to covary with each other, and to have negative effects on
SCCs. These three cognitive indicators were also hypothesised to have direct effects on func-
tional interference and treatment nonadherence. However, the regression path from objective
cognitive function to treatment nonadherence was considered optional because, despite theo-
retical assumptions, extant empirical evidence in the context of kidney disease suggest no asso-
ciation between these two variables [19,20].
In addition, regression paths were added from sociodemographic factors, comorbidity, and
mood and fatigue symptoms, to each of the five latent variables in Fig 1 (i.e., objective cogni-
tive function, everyday problem-solving skills, subjective cognitive complaints, functional
PLOS ONE
Cognition and patient outcomes in haemodialysis
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0312039 October 17, 2024 6 / 17
Table 1. Description of latent variables and indicator variables of the final measurement model.
Latent Variable Indicator Variable Measurement/Example Item Interpretation of Higher
Values
Mean (SD)
/N(%)
Factor
Loading
Objective Cognitive
Function
Visuospatial/
Executive
Trail-making test part B; cube copy; clock drawing;
abstraction
Better visuospatial and
executive functions
4.07 (1.58) 0.65
Attention Digit span forward and backward; vigilance; serial-7
subtraction
Better attention ability 4.76 (1.47) 0.65
Memory Delayed recall Better memory 2.34 (1.89) 0.52
Language Naming; sentence repetition; verbal fluency Better language function 4.38 (1.03) 0.52
Orientation Awareness of time and place Better orientation 5.71 (0.54) 0.23
Everyday Problem-
Solving Skills
General Problem "Now let’s say that one evening you go to the refrigerator
and you notice that it is not cold inside, but rather, it’s
warm. What would you do?"
Better problem-solving skills for
general problems
7.59 (3.91) 0.75
Health Problem "You accidentally took the wrong combination of
medication. What do you do?"
Better problem-solving skills for
health-related problems
4.3 (2.27) 0.84
Subjective Cognitive
Complaints
Memory "How often do you lose things or have trouble
remembering where they are?"
More frequent memory
complaints
2.16 (0.81) 0.74
Language "How often do you have difficulty thinking of the names of
things?"
More frequent language
complaints
2.06 (0.86) 0.81
Motor/Sensory-
Perceptual
“How often do you have difficulty feeling things with your
right hand?”
More frequent complaints
about motor/sensory-
perceptual abilities
1.96 (0.9) 0.71
Higher-Level
Cognitive
“How often do you have difficulty finding your way about?” More frequent complaints
about higher-level cognitive
functions
1.74 (0.76) 0.93
Functional
Interference
Work "Because of my cognitive difficulties, my ability to work is
impaired."
Greater impact of cognitive
difficulties on work ability
1.39 (1.96) 0.80
Home
Management
"Because of my cognitive difficulties, my home
management (cleaning, tidying, shopping, cooking, looking
after home or children, paying bills) is impaired."
Greater impact of cognitive
difficulties on home
management
1.22 (1.96) 0.85
Social Activities "Because of my cognitive difficulties, my social leisure
activities (with other people, such as parties, bars, clubs,
outings, visits, dating, home entertainment) are impaired."
Greater impact of cognitive
difficulties on social leisure
activities
1.33 (2.09) 0.89
Leisure Activities "Because of my cognitive difficulties, my private leisure
activities (done alone, such as reading, gardening,
collecting, sewing, walking alone) are impaired."
Greater impact of cognitive
difficulties on private leisure
activities
1.23 (1.94) 0.87
Relationships "Because of my cognitive difficulties, my ability to form and
maintain close relationships with others, including those I
live with, is impaired."
Greater impact of cognitive
difficulties on social
relationships
1.00 (1.69) 0.81
Treatment
Nonadherence
MARS-5 "I forget to take them." Poorer medication adherence 7.29 (2.80) 0.60
DDFQ-1 "How many days during the past 14 days didn’t you follow
your diet guidelines?"
More frequent diet
nonadherence
2.65 (3.93) 0.33
DDFQ-2 "To what degree did you deviate from your diet guidelines?" Greater deviation from diet
guidelines
0.99 (0.93) 0.59
DDFQ-3 "How many days during the past 14 days didn’t you follow
your fluid guidelines?"
More frequent fluid
nonadherence
2.66 (3.99) 0.41
DDFQ-4 "To what degree did you deviate from your fluid
guidelines?"
Greater deviation from fluid
guidelines
1.03 (0.95) 0.64
K Latest available laboratory results Higher serum potassium 4.76 (0.64) 0.15
PO4 Higher serum phosphorus 4.66 (1.17) 0.20
Ca×PO4 Higher calcium-phosphorus
product
42.79
(11.36)
0.19
IDWG Relative interdialytic weight gain averaged across the week
of and the week before the survey date
Higher relative interdialytic
weight gain
3.34 (1.06) 0.13
(Continued)
PLOS ONE
Cognition and patient outcomes in haemodialysis
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0312039 October 17, 2024 7 / 17
interference, treatment nonadherence), in order to account for their potential confounding
effects [22,47,5355]. The structural model was tested and respecified until a final model was
determined by weighing model fit indices and model parsimony (i.e., a simpler model with
fewer parameters is favoured over more complex models provided the models fit the data simi-
larly well).
Results
Sample characteristics
We approached a total of 459 HD patients in NKF dialysis centres. Ninety patients were
excluded due to ineligibility. Within the remaining 369 eligible patients, 268 consented to par-
ticipate (response rate 72.6%). The main reasons for rejection were lack of interest and feeling
unwell. Therefore, 268 patients were included in final analyses. Table 2 reports the sociodemo-
graphic and clinical profiles of the sample. The mean age of the sample was 59.87 (SD = 11.72).
Patients on average had been on HD for 78.85 (SD = 62.80) months.
Final structural model
The fit indices for the full structural model were as follows: CFI = 0.917, TLI = 0.905,
RMSEA = 0.033 (90% confidence interval 0.024 to 0.041), SRMR = 0.065, χ
2
(492) = 616.607 (p
<.001). The path between objective cognitive function and treatment nonadherence was
indeed not statistically significant, consistent with prior work [19,20]. We removed this path
while retaining all other paths and reran the structural model, with fit indices as follows:
CFI = 0.916, TLI = 0.905, RMSEA = 0.033 (90% confidence interval 0.024 to 0.041),
SRMR = 0.066, χ
2
(493) = 618.573 (p<.001). A chi-squared difference test showed that this
Table 1. (Continued)
Latent Variable Indicator Variable Measurement/Example Item Interpretation of Higher
Values
Mean (SD)
/N(%)
Factor
Loading
Sociodemographic Age Years of age Older age 59.87
(11.72)
0.59
Education Years of full-time education Higher education level 9.59 (3.56) -0.61
Comorbidity Diabetes Medical record Presence of diabetes 145
(54.3%)
0.69
Hypertension Presence of hypertension 232
(86.9%)
0.58
Hyperlipidaemia Presence of hyperlipidaemia 143
(53.6%)
0.64
Cardiovascular
disease
Presence of cardiovascular
disease
141
(52.8%)
0.47
Mood & Fatigue
Symptoms
Depression "Over the last 2 weeks, how often have you been bothered
by the following problems? Little interest or pleasure in
doing things."
Higher depressive symptoms 1.10 (1.46) 0.82
Anxiety "Over the last 2 weeks, how often have you been bothered
by the following problems? Feeling nervous, anxious, or on
edge."
Higher anxious symptoms 1.06 (1.51) 0.84
Fatigue "How much of the time during the past 4 weeks did you
have a lot of energy?"
Higher fatigue symptoms 3.18 (1.45) 0.43
Notes. SD = Standard deviation. N= Sample size. MARS = Medication Adherence Report Scale. DDFQ = Dialysis Diet and Fluid non-adherence Questionnaire.
K = Serum potassium. PO4 = Serum phosphorus. Ca×PO4 = Calcium-phosphorus product. IDWG = Interdialytic weight gain.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0312039.t001
PLOS ONE
Cognition and patient outcomes in haemodialysis
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0312039 October 17, 2024 8 / 17
trimmed model, which was also the more parsimonious model, could explain the observed
data equally well compared to the full structural model without a significant loss in data-model
fit (p= .101).
The final trimmed model is presented in Fig 2. For conciseness, only regression paths that
were statistically significant were presented in the figure. The model revealed that objective
cognitive performance negatively predicted SCCs, but was not associated with EPS skills, func-
tional interference, or treatment nonadherence. SCCs were positively associated with both
functional interference and treatment nonadherence, suggesting indirect effects of objective
performance on these dependent variables through SCCs. On the other hand, EPS skills were
found to be unrelated to either objective performance or SCCs, but had a negative association
with functional interference, suggesting that individuals with worse EPS skills had more severe
functional interference. Sociodemographic factors (i.e., age and years of education) were asso-
ciated with objective cognitive function, EPS skills, and treatment nonadherence, whereas
Table 2. Sample characteristics (N = 268).
Mean (SD) / N(%)
Sociodemographic
Gender
Male 154 (57.5%)
Female 114 (42.5%)
Age (years) 59.87 (11.72)
Range 26–84
Ethnicity
Chinese 151 (56.3%)
Malay 80 (29.9%)
Indian or others 37 (13.8%)
Years of education 9.59 (3.56)
Relationship status
In a relationship 182 (67.9%)
Not in a relationship 86 (32.1%)
Work status
Working 76 (28.5%)
Not working 191 (71.5%)
Clinical
Primary diagnosis
Diabetic nephropathy 122 (45.5%)
Glomerulonephritis 49 (18.3%)
Hypertension 36 (13.4%)
IgA nephropathy 12 (4.5%)
Others/uncertain aetiology 49 (18.3%)
Presence of diabetes 145 (54.3%)
Presence of hypertension 232 (86.9%)
Presence of hyperlipidaemia 143 (53.6%)
Presence of cardiovascular disease 141 (52.8%)
Duration on HD (months) 78.85 (62.80)
Medication count 12.76 (4.25)
Kt/V 1.60 (0.24)
Notes. SD = Standard deviation. N= Sample size. HD = Haemodialysis. Kt/V = Dialysis dose.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0312039.t002
PLOS ONE
Cognition and patient outcomes in haemodialysis
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0312039 October 17, 2024 9 / 17
mood and fatigue symptoms were predictive of SCCs and functional interference. Comorbid-
ity, however, was not associated with any other latent variables, and was therefore not pre-
sented in Fig 2. The final model explained 65.7% of the variance in functional interference
(indicated by an R
2
of 0.657), and 63.3% of the variance in treatment nonadherence (indicated
by an R
2
of 0.633).
Discussion
To the best of our knowledge, this is the first SEM study to explore the complex interrelation-
ships between multiple cognitive indicators and key functional and clinical variables in dialysis
patients. The final model revealed interesting pathways through which cognition was directly
and indirectly associated with functional interference and treatment nonadherence, highlight-
ing cognitive difficulties as an important barrier in this population to functional capacity, life
participation, treatment adherence, and disease management.
A key finding was that patients with more frequent SCCs also experienced more severe
daily interference and exhibited poorer treatment adherence. In contrast, objective cognitive
performance was only indirectly associated with these outcomes through SCCs. Indeed, it has
been suggested that objective CIs based on neuropsychological tests do not necessarily trans-
late to impairments in real-world functioning outside of test environment because some
patients may adopt strategies to compensate for everyday cognitive failure, which is typically
not allowed in standardised testing [56]. For some other patients, cognitive difficulties may be
too mild to be detected by objective tests, but are nevertheless problematic for everyday life
Fig 2. Final structural model. Standardised coefficients with standard errors are reported for regression paths. Standardised factor loadings are reported for paths
between indicator variables and latent variables. Nonsignificant paths are not presented in the figure.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0312039.g002
PLOS ONE
Cognition and patient outcomes in haemodialysis
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0312039 October 17, 2024 10 / 17
[57]. Consistent with our findings, Song et al. found that SCCs, but not objective performance,
predicted self-reported difficulties in performing activities of daily living in dialysis patients
[15].
Importantly, treatment nonadherence in the present study was measured by both self-
report and biochemical and physiological parameters. Inter-dialytic weight gain is typically
thought to be an indicator of fluid adherence and sodium intake, whereas potassium and phos-
phorus may indicate dietary adherence [55]. Serum phosphorus can also indicate patient
adherence to phosphate binders. Higher values of these clinical markers have been associated
with poor survival [55]. Our findings are consistent with a recent longitudinal study where a
reduction in SCCs over time was accompanied by a significant improvement in serum levels
of PO4 and Ca×PO4 in HD patients [17]. Notably, the observed association between SCCs and
nonadherence was significant even taking into account sociodemographic factors, comorbid-
ity, and mood and fatigue symptoms, suggesting that this effect cannot be explained by other
well-established determinants of nonadherence such as age and depression. Taken together,
SCCs appear to be a useful measure that can simultaneously red-flag functional interference,
nonadherence, and poor clinical outcomes in dialysis patients.
Another interesting finding was that patients with worse EPS skills were more likely to
experience functional interference. The EPS task used in this study required participants to
generate safe and effective solutions to the given scenarios. There is evidence in the cognitive
development literature that the ability to generate a number of alternative solutions is a good
indicator of problem-solving ability [58]. Patients who were unable to generate multiple solu-
tions in response to the EPS task scenarios may also be more likely to experience failure in
solving problems that arise in real-world contexts, which in the long term can impair indepen-
dence in various aspects of life.
Unexpectedly though, EPS task performance was not associated with treatment nonadher-
ence. This is inconsistent with two other studies where EPS skills were found to predict medi-
cation adherence in kidney transplantation recipients [19,20]. These two studies, however,
only assessed cognition using neuropsychological tests and the EPS task. It may be that SCCs
have a stronger association with adherence and therefore by accounting for its effect in our
model, the predictive value of EPS skills on adherence diminished. Indeed, the SCC measure
used in our study is comprehensive and covers multiple cognitive domains, whereas EPS skills
are dependent not just upon executive function, but also life experience, knowledge, and envi-
ronmental factors [59].
We found that patients with worse cognitive performance on objective tests also subjec-
tively reported more cognitive complaints. This observed association was modest, which is not
unexpected based on previous work in other populations [22,60]. Although objective tests and
subjective reports both intend to capture the same underlying construct (i.e., cognition), they
measure it using completely different methods and are influenced by different sets of predic-
tors. While objective tests assess performance at a single time point in distraction-free environ-
ments, self-reports are based on accumulative daily experience that may be more reflective of
longitudinal cognitive changes [60]. Objective and subjective cognition have therefore been
considered as distinct constructs that complement each other with different utility in clinical
and research settings. Assessing objective CIs can help establishing diagnosis of CIs and allow
for advance care planning, whereas assessing SCCs can help identifying aspects of CIs that
have the greatest impact on patients, hence allowing for a more patient-centred approach to
managing this debilitating complication [61].
The current study has several important clinical implications. First, cognitive indicators
that incorporate everyday scenarios (i.e., EPS and SCCs) appeared to be better predictors of
real-world functional and clinical outcomes in HD patients, compared to traditional cognitive
PLOS ONE
Cognition and patient outcomes in haemodialysis
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0312039 October 17, 2024 11 / 17
tests. The associations of SCCs with underlying cognitive deficits and worse outcomes high-
light the potential of SCCs as a stand-alone patient-reported outcome measure with clinical
utility in dialysis settings that simultaneously signifies risks in multiple aspects of patients’
health and well-being. Indeed, SCCs are increasingly considered as a prodromal marker of
progression to dementia in Alzheimer’s disease research [62,63], and as a core patient-
reported outcome in populations such as cancer [64] and HIV patients [65]. To date, research
and clinical practice in the field of nephrology have been predominantly focusing on objective
cognition, with SCCs being understudied and underrecognised. The emphasis on objective
cognition is essential for diagnostic purposes, but hinders a comprehensive understanding of
cognitive well-being in this population. Previous studies determined the utility of SCC mea-
sures solely based on their ability to distinguish patients with and without objective CIs [66].
We propose that SCCs should be treated as an outcome equally important as objective CIs,
and should be assessed in combination with neuropsychological tests where possible.
Second, everyday cognitive difficulties reflected by SCCs and the EPS task emerged as
potential modifiable risk factors for functional interference and treatment nonadherence.
There is evidence that functional impairment is present in 21 to 85% of ESRD patients [11],
and that optimal adherence is not achieved in as many as 80% of dialysis patients [55]. Our
study showed that these high rates may in part be explained by the cognitive burden experi-
enced by this population. It is therefore pivotal to develop and implement interventions that
improve everyday cognitive abilities and compensate for cognitive lapses in this population so
that the impacts of CIs on daily functioning and self-care can be mitigated. To date, there is a
lack of research on the feasibility and effectiveness of cognitive interventions for ERSD
patients. Future work in this area is needed to improve patient-centred care and to optimise
adaptation to dialysis initiation.
Several limitations warrant acknowledgement. First, it should be noted that this is a cross-
sectional observational study. Also, SEM is not inherently a causal method [21]. Caution is
therefore needed when interpreting the significant paths in our final model. Although the
directions of these associations have been hypothesised and tested, the study design and statis-
tical method do not permit the interpretation of a cause-and-effect sequence. Future longitudi-
nal studies and intervention research are needed to explore potential causal mechanisms and
further validate our observations. Second, the MoCA used in this study is only a brief screen-
ing tool for global cognition. No comprehensive neuropsychological battery or standard diag-
nostic test was conducted to determine objective cognitive function. This is because the tools
administered in this study were already very lengthy and required about 30–40 minutes for
each patient to complete. A more comprehensive neuropsychological assessment was therefore
not carried out considering patients’ response burden and potential fatigue. Third, SCCs and
functional interference were measured by self-report, which may be susceptible to recall bias.
Future studies that incorporate informant-reports of SCCs and more objective measures of
functional capacity may be needed. Finally, the study was conducted in a sample of HD
patients, which limited the generalisability to other kidney disease subgroups such as perito-
neal dialysis and kidney transplantation.
Conclusion
In summary, this study adopted the SEM technique to disentangle the complex associations
between cognition and key functional and clinical parameters in HD patients. Results revealed
the central role of SCCs in indicating not just underlying cognitive deficits but also functional
interference, treatment nonadherence, and suboptimal clinical outcomes. In contrast, EPS
skills were found to be associated with only functional interference but not nonadherence,
PLOS ONE
Cognition and patient outcomes in haemodialysis
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0312039 October 17, 2024 12 / 17
whereas performance on traditional tests did not associate with any of these variables. It may
be important to screen for SCCs in dialysis patients which would allow for early identification
of the high-risk population, and subsequently early prevention or intervention strategies to
mitigate the consequences of CIs.
Supporting information
S1 Data.
(CSV)
Acknowledgments
We thank the nurse managers and staff of the dialysis centres of National Kidney Foundation
Singapore for facilitating the patient recruitment procedures and providing patients’ medical
data. We would also like to thank Kevin Tan, Shane Tan, Sze Ing Tan, and Nicole Tan for their
assistance in patient recruitment and data entry.
Author Contributions
Conceptualization: Konstadina Griva.
Data curation: Frederick H. F. Chan, Pearl Sim, Phoebe X. H. Lim, Behram A. Khan.
Formal analysis: Frederick H. F. Chan, Konstadina Griva.
Funding acquisition: Jimmy Lee, Sabrina Haroon, Titus Wai Leong Lau, Behram A. Khan,
Konstadina Griva.
Investigation: Frederick H. F. Chan.
Methodology: Frederick H. F. Chan, Jimmy Lee, Sabrina Haroon, Titus Wai Leong Lau, Beh-
ram A. Khan, Konstadina Griva.
Project administration: Frederick H. F. Chan, Pearl Sim, Phoebe X. H. Lim, Konstadina
Griva.
Supervision: Konstadina Griva.
Validation: Frederick H. F. Chan.
Visualization: Frederick H. F. Chan.
Writing original draft: Frederick H. F. Chan.
Writing review & editing: Frederick H. F. Chan, Pearl Sim, Phoebe X. H. Lim, Xiaoli Zhu,
Jimmy Lee, Sabrina Haroon, Titus Wai Leong Lau, Allen Yan Lun Liu, Behram A. Khan,
Jason C. J. Choo, Konstadina Griva.
References
1. Levey AS, Coresh J. Chronic kidney disease. Lancet. 2012; 379: 165–180. https://doi.org/10.1016/
S0140-6736(11)60178-5
2. Himmelfarb J, Vanholder R, Mehrotra R, Tonelli M. The current and future landscape of dialysis. Nat
Rev Nephrol. 2020; 16: 573–585. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41581-020-0315-4 PMID: 32733095
3. van Zwieten A, Wong G, Ruospo M, Palmer SC, Teixeira-Pinto A, Barulli MR, et al. Associations of Cog-
nitive Function and Education Level With All-Cause Mortality in Adults on Hemodialysis: Findings From
the COGNITIVE-HD Study. Am J Kidney Dis. 2019; 74: 452–462. https://doi.org/10.1053/j.ajkd.2019.
03.424 PMID: 31160141
PLOS ONE
Cognition and patient outcomes in haemodialysis
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0312039 October 17, 2024 13 / 17
4. Drew DA, Tighiouart H, Rollins J, Duncan S, Babroudi S, Scott T, et al. Evaluation of screening tests for
cognitive impairment in patients receiving maintenance hemodialysis. J Am Soc Nephrol. 2020; 31:
855–864. https://doi.org/10.1681/ASN.2019100988 PMID: 32132197
5. Erken E, Altunoren O, Senel ME, Tuncel D, Yilmaz T, Ganidagli SE, et al. Impaired cognition in hemodi-
alysis patients: The Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) and important clues for testing. Clin
Nephrol. 2019; 91: 275–283. https://doi.org/10.5414/CN109506 PMID: 30686287
6. Murray AM, Tupper DE, Knopman DS, Gilbertson DT, Pederson SL, Li S, et al. Cognitive impairment in
hemodialysis patients is common. Neurology. 2006; 67: 216–223.
7. Chan FHF, Goh ZZS, Zhu X, Tudor Car L, Newman S, Khan BA, et al. Subjective cognitive complaints
in end-stage renal disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Health Psychol Rev. 2023; 17:
614–640. https://doi.org/10.1080/17437199.2022.2132980
8. Crowe K, Quinn TJ, Mark PB, Findlay MD. “Is It Removed During Dialysis?”—Cognitive Dysfunction in
Advanced Kidney Failure—A Review Article. Front Neurol. 2021; 12: 787370. https://doi.org/10.3389/
fneur.2021.787370 PMID: 34925220
9. Kurella M, Mapes DL, Port FK, Chertow GM. Correlates and outcomes of dementia among dialysis
patients: The dialysis outcomes and practice patterns study. Nephrol Dial Transplant. 2006; 21: 2543–
2548. https://doi.org/10.1093/ndt/gfl275 PMID: 16751655
10. Griva K, Stygall J, Hankins M, Davenport A, Harrison M, Newman SP. Cognitive Impairment and 7-Year
Mortality in Dialysis Patients. Am J Kidney Dis. 2010; 56: 693–703. https://doi.org/10.1053/j.ajkd.2010.
07.003 PMID: 20800327
11. Kallenberg MH, Kleinveld HA, Dekker FW, van Munster BC, Rabelink TJ, van Buren M, et al. Functional
and cognitive impairment, frailty, and adverse health outcomes in older patients reaching ESRD-a sys-
tematic review. Clin J Am Soc Nephrol. 2016; 11: 1624–1639. https://doi.org/10.2215/CJN.13611215
PMID: 27342598
12. Cipriani G, Danti S, Picchi L, Nuti A, Di Fiorino M. Daily functioning and dementia. Dement e Neuropsy-
chol. 2020; 14: 93–102. https://doi.org/10.1590/1980-57642020dn14-020001 PMID: 32595877
13. Insel K, Morrow D, Brewer B, Figueredo A. Executive function, working memory, and medication adher-
ence among older adults. Journals Gerontol—Ser B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2006; 61B: 102–107. https://
doi.org/10.1093/geronb/61.2.p102 PMID: 16497953
14. Kavanagh NT, Schiller B, Saxena AB, Thomas IC, Kurella Tamura M. Prevalence and correlates of
functional dependence among maintenance dialysis patients. Hemodial Int. 2015; 19: 593–600. https://
doi.org/10.1111/hdi.12286 PMID: 25731070
15. Song M-K, Ward SE, Bair E, Weiner LJ, Bridgman JC, Hladik GA, et al. Patient-reported cognitive func-
tioning and daily functioning in chronic dialysis patients. Hemodial Int. 2015; 19: 90–99. https://doi.org/
10.1111/hdi.12202 PMID: 25110172
16. Chan FHF, Newman S, Khan BA, Griva K. The role of subjective cognitive complaints in self-manage-
ment among haemodialysis patients: a cross-sectional study. BMC Nephrol. 2022; 23: 363. https://doi.
org/10.1186/s12882-022-02994-2 PMID: 36376848
17. Chan FHF, Newman S, Khan BA, Griva K. Prevalence and trajectories of subjective cognitive com-
plaints and implications for patient outcomes: a prospective study of haemodialysis patients. Br J Health
Psychol. 2023; 28: 651–671. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjhp.12645 PMID: 36720474
18. Thornton WL, Dumke HA. Age differences in everyday problem-solving and decision-making effective-
ness: A meta-analytic review. Psychol Aging. 2005; 20: 85–99. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.20.1.
85 PMID: 15769216
19. Gelb SR, Shapiro RJ, Thornton WL. Predicting Medication Adherence and Employment Status Follow-
ing Kidney Transplant: The Relative Utility of Traditional and Everyday Cognitive Approaches. Neuro-
psychology. 2010; 24: 514–526. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018670 PMID: 20604625
20. Paterson TSE, O’Rourke N, Jean Shapiro R, Thornton WL. Medication adherence in renal transplant
recipients: A latent variable model of psychosocial and neurocognitive predictors. PLoS One. 2018; 13:
e0204219. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0204219 PMID: 30265697
21. Mueller RO, Hancock GR. Best Practices in Structural Equation Modeling. In: Osborne J, editor. Best
Practices in Quantitative Methods. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications, Inc.; 2008. pp. 488–508.
22. Rabin LA, Smart CM, Amariglio RE. Subjective Cognitive Decline in Preclinical Alzheimer’s Disease.
Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2017; 13: 369–396. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032816-045136
PMID: 28482688
23. Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bedirian V, Charbonneau S, Whitehead V, Collin I, et al. The Montreal Cog-
nitive Assessment, MoCA: A Brief Screening Tool For Mild Cognitive Impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc.
2005; 53: 695–699. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x PMID: 15817019
PLOS ONE
Cognition and patient outcomes in haemodialysis
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0312039 October 17, 2024 14 / 17
24. Allaire JC, Marsiske M. Well- and ill-defined measures of everyday cognition: relationship to older
adults’ intellectual ability and functional status. Psychol Aging. 2002; 17: 101–115. https://doi.org/10.
1037/0882-7974.17.1.101 PMID: 11931279
25. Chelune GJ, Heaton RK, Lehman RAW. Neuropsychological and Personality Correlates of Patients’
Complaints of Disability. In: Goldstein G, Tarter RE, editors. Advances in Clinical Neuropsychology.
Boston, MA.: Springer; 1986. pp. 95–126.
26. Mundt JC, Marks IM, Shear MK, Greist JH. The Work and Social Adjustment Scale: A simple measure
of impairment in functioning. Br J Psychiatry. 2002; 180: 461–464. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.180.5.
461 PMID: 11983645
27. Horne R, Weinman J. Self-regulation and self-management in asthma: Exploring the role of illness per-
ceptions and treatment beliefs in explaining non-adherence to preventer medication. Psychol Heal.
2002; 17: 17–32. https://doi.org/10.1080/08870440290001502
28. Vlaminck H, Maes B, Jacobs A, Reyntjens S, Evers G. The dialysis diet and fluid non-adherence ques-
tionnaire: Validity testing of a self-report instrument for clinical practice. J Clin Nurs. 2001; 10: 707–715.
https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2702.2001.00537.x PMID: 11822521
29. Arroll B, Goodyear-Smith F, Crengle S, Gunn J, Kerse N, Fishman T, et al. Validation of PHQ-2 and
PHQ-9 to screen for major depression in the primary care population. Ann Fam Med. 2010; 8: 348–353.
https://doi.org/10.1370/afm.1139 PMID: 20644190
30. Plummer F, Manea L, Trepel D, McMillan D. Screening for anxiety disorders with the GAD-7 and GAD-
2: A systematic review and diagnostic metaanalysis. Gen Hosp Psychiatry. 2016; 39: 24–31. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2015.11.005 PMID: 26719105
31. Yang F, Wang VW, Joshi VD, Lau TWL, Luo N. Validation of the english version of the kidney disease
quality of life questionnaire (KDQOL-36) in haemodialysis patients in Singapore. Patient. 2013; 6: 135–
141. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40271-013-0015-2 PMID: 23613285
32. Rao S, Carter WB, Mapes DL, Kallich JD, Kamberg CJ, Spritzer KL, et al. Development of subscales
from the symptoms/problems and effects of kidney disease scales of the Kidney Disease Quality of Life
Instrument. Clin Ther. 2000; 22: 1099–1111. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0149-2918(00)80087-9 PMID:
11048907
33. Hays RD, Kallich JD, Mapes DL, Coons SJ, Carter WB. Development of the Kidney Disease Quality of
Life (KDQOL) Instrument. Qual Life Res. 1994; 3: 329–338. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00451725
PMID: 7841967
34. Vance DE, Okonkwo O, Antia L, Smith BA, Blanshan SA, Hiers KM, et al. Factors of cognitive com-
plaints in adults with HIV: A structural equation model analysis. Occup Ther Ment Heal. 2009; 25: 4–25.
https://doi.org/10.1080/01642120802644896
35. Ramlall I. Model Modification. Applied Structural Equation Modelling for Researchers and Practitioners.
Bingley: Emerald Group Publishing Limited; 2016. pp. 75–77.
36. Kremelberg D. Structural Equation Modeling. Practical Statistics: A Quick and Easy Guide to IBM
®
SPSS
®
Statistics, STATA, and Other Statistical Software. SAGE Publications, Inc.; 2014.
37. Rosseel Y. lavaan: An R Package for Structural Equation Modeling. J Stat Softw. 2012; 48: 1–36.
38. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation
for Statistical Computing; 2018. https://www.r-project.org/
39. Hu L, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria
versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Model. 1999; 6: 1–55. https://doi.org/10.1080/
10705519909540118
40. McDonald RP, Marsh HW. Choosing a multivariate model: Noncentrality and goodness of fit. Psychol
Bull. 1990; 107: 247.
41. Browne MW, Cudeck R. Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In: Bollen KA, Long JS, editors. Testing
structural equation models. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage; 1993. pp. 136–162.
42. Hopwood CJ, Donnellan MB. How should the internal structure of personality inventories be evaluated?
Personal Soc Psychol Rev. 2010; 14: 332–346. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868310361240 PMID:
20435808
43. Marsh HW, Hau KT, Wen Z. In search of golden rules: Comment on hypothesis-testing approaches to
setting cutoff values for fit indexes and dangers in overgeneralizing Hu and Bentler’s (1999) findings.
Struct Equ Model. 2004; 11: 320–341. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15328007sem1103_2
44. Clark DA, Bowles RP. Model Fit and Item Factor Analysis: Overfactoring, Underfactoring, and a Pro-
gram to Guide Interpretation. Multivariate Behav Res. 2018; 53: 544–558. https://doi.org/10.1080/
00273171.2018.1461058 PMID: 29683723
45. Brown TA. Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research. New York: Guilford Press; 2006.
PLOS ONE
Cognition and patient outcomes in haemodialysis
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0312039 October 17, 2024 15 / 17
46. Carter SL, Murji S, Rourke SB, Shore D, Rourke BP. Cognitive complaints, depression, medical symp-
toms, and their association with neuropsychological functioning in HIV infection: A structural equation
model analysis. Neuropsychology. 2003; 17: 410–419. https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.17.3.410
PMID: 12959507
47. Olczyk P, Kusztal M, Gołębiowski T, Letachowicz K, Krajewska M. Cognitive Impairment in End Stage
Renal Disease Patients Undergoing Hemodialysis: Markers and Risk Factors. Int J Environ Res Public
Health. 2022; 19: 2389. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19042389 PMID: 35206577
48. Drew DA, Weiner DE, Sarnak MJ. Cognitive Impairment in CKD: Pathophysiology, Management, and
Prevention. Am J Kidney Dis. 2019; 74: 782–790. https://doi.org/10.1053/j.ajkd.2019.05.017 PMID:
31378643
49. Viggiano D, Wagner CA, Martino G, Nedergaard M, Zoccali C, Unwin R, et al. Mechanisms of cognitive
dysfunction in CKD. Nat Rev Nephrol. 2020; 16: 452–469. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41581-020-0266-9
PMID: 32235904
50. Goh ZS, Griva K. Anxiety and depression in patients with end-stage renal disease: impact and manage-
ment challenges—a narrative review. Int J Nephrol Renov Diseajse. 2018; 11: 93–102. https://doi.org/
10.2147/IJNRD.S126615 PMID: 29559806
51. Lamberg-Allardt C, Kemi V. Interaction Between Calcium and Phosphorus and the Relationship to
Bone Health. 1st ed. In: Gutie
´rrez OM, Kalantar-Zadeh K, Mehrotra R, editors. Clinical Aspects of Natu-
ral and Added Phosphorus in Foods. 1st ed. New York, NY: Springer; 2017. pp. 145–157.
52. National Kidney Foundation. K/DOQI Clinical Practice Guidelines for Bone Metabolism and Disease in
Chronic Kidney Disease. American Journal of Kidney Diseases. 2003.
53. Kardas P, Lewek P, Matyjaszczyk M. Determinants of patient adherence: A review of systematic
reviews. Front Pharmacol. 2013; 4: 91. https://doi.org/10.3389/fphar.2013.00091 PMID: 23898295
54. Molinuevo JL, Rabin LA, Amariglio R, Buckley R, Dubois B, Ellis KA, et al. Implementation of subjective
cognitive decline criteria in research studies. Alzheimer’s Dement. 2017; 13: 296–311. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.jalz.2016.09.012 PMID: 27825022
55. Clark S, Farrington K, Chilcot J. Nonadherence in dialysis patients: Prevalence, measurement, out-
come, and psychological determinants. Semin Dial. 2014; 27: 42–49. https://doi.org/10.1111/sdi.12159
PMID: 24164416
56. Domensino AF, Evans J, van Heugten C. From word list learning to successful shopping: The neuropsy-
chological assessment continuum from cognitive tests to cognition in everyday life. Appl Neuropsychol.
2022. https://doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2022.2079087 PMID: 35654744
57. Horowitz TS, Suls J, Treviño M. A Call for a Neuroscience Approach to Cancer-Related Cognitive
Impairment. Trends Neurosci. 2018; 41: 493–496. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2018.05.001 PMID:
29907436
58. Denney NW, Pearce KA. A developmental study of practical problem solving in adults. Psychol Aging.
1989; 4: 438–442. https://doi.org/10.1037//0882-7974.4.4.438 PMID: 2619950
59. Crawford S, Channon S. Dissociation between performance on abstract tests of executive function and
problem solving in real-life-type situations in normal aging. Aging Ment Heal. 2002; 6: 12–21. https://doi.
org/10.1080/13607860120101130 PMID: 11827618
60. Costa DSJ, Fardell JE. Why are objective and perceived cognitive function weakly correlated in patients
with cancer? J Clin Oncol. 2019; 37: 1154–1158. https://doi.org/10.1200/JCO.18.02363 PMID:
30920881
61. Van Dyk K, Ganz PA, Ercoli L, Petersen L, Crespi CM. Measuring cognitive complaints in breast cancer
survivors: psychometric properties of the patient’s assessment of own functioning inventory. Support
Care Cancer. 2016; 24: 4939–4949. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00520-016-3352-6 PMID: 27432402
62. Li H, Tan C-C, Tan L, Xu W. Predictors of cognitive deterioration in subjective cognitive decline: evi-
dence from longitudinal studies and implications for SCD-plus criteria. J Neurol NeurosurgPsychiatry.
2023. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp-2022-330246 PMID: 36868847
63. Chapman S, Renterı
´a MA, Dworkin JD, Garriga SM, Barker MS, Avila-Rieger J, et al. Association of
Subjective Cognitive Decline With Progression to Dementia in a Cognitively Unimpaired Multiracial
Community Sample. Neurology. 2023; 100: e1020–e1027. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.
0000000000201658 PMID: 36450605
64. Henneghan AM, Van Dyk K, Kaufmann T, Harrison R, Gibbons C, Kesler SR. Measuring Self-Reported
Cancer-Related Cognitive Impairment: Recommendations From the Cancer Neuroscience Initiative
Working Group. J Natl Cancer Inst. 2021; 113: djab027. https://doi.org/10.1093/jnci/djab027 PMID:
33638633
PLOS ONE
Cognition and patient outcomes in haemodialysis
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0312039 October 17, 2024 16 / 17
65. Waldrop D, Irwin C, Nicholson C, Lee CA, Webel A, Fazeli PL, et al. The Intersection of Cognitive Ability
and HIV: A Review of the State of the Nursing Science. J Assoc Nurses AIDS Care. 2021; 32: 306–321.
https://doi.org/10.1097/JNC.0000000000000232 PMID: 33449578
66. Kurella M, Luan J, Yaffe K, Chertow GM. Validation of the Kidney Disease Quality of Life (KDQOL) Cog-
nitive Function subscale. Kidney Int. 2004; 66: 2361–2367. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1523-1755.2004.
66024.x PMID: 15569327
PLOS ONE
Cognition and patient outcomes in haemodialysis
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0312039 October 17, 2024 17 / 17
... However, many studies on symptom management for hemodialysis patients lack a suitable theory, complicating the understanding of intervention choices and limiting research replicability and scalability. Existing theoretical models have several limitations: (1) They focus on antecedent variables rather than symptom burden itself [32,[37][38][39][40][41][42][43]; (2) They address only one aspect of symptom management, such as a single symptom or outcome [44,45]; (3) They fail to explain how external factors affect intermediary variables, reducing explanatory power [32,46,47]; (4) They focus on relationships between symptoms and outcomes rather than pathways to reduce symptom burden [32,37,38,48]; (5) Their complexity limits practical clinical use [32,37,38]; and (6) They do not adjust for covariables [38,39,41,44,[47][48][49]. This study addresses these gaps by developing a personcentered symptom management model based on empirical evidence, initially validated using Partial Least Squares Structural Equation Modeling (PLS-SEM), which is ideal for early theory development as it does not require a strong theoretical foundation [50]. ...
... However, many studies on symptom management for hemodialysis patients lack a suitable theory, complicating the understanding of intervention choices and limiting research replicability and scalability. Existing theoretical models have several limitations: (1) They focus on antecedent variables rather than symptom burden itself [32,[37][38][39][40][41][42][43]; (2) They address only one aspect of symptom management, such as a single symptom or outcome [44,45]; (3) They fail to explain how external factors affect intermediary variables, reducing explanatory power [32,46,47]; (4) They focus on relationships between symptoms and outcomes rather than pathways to reduce symptom burden [32,37,38,48]; (5) Their complexity limits practical clinical use [32,37,38]; and (6) They do not adjust for covariables [38,39,41,44,[47][48][49]. This study addresses these gaps by developing a personcentered symptom management model based on empirical evidence, initially validated using Partial Least Squares Structural Equation Modeling (PLS-SEM), which is ideal for early theory development as it does not require a strong theoretical foundation [50]. ...
... However, many studies on symptom management for hemodialysis patients lack a suitable theory, complicating the understanding of intervention choices and limiting research replicability and scalability. Existing theoretical models have several limitations: (1) They focus on antecedent variables rather than symptom burden itself [32,[37][38][39][40][41][42][43]; (2) They address only one aspect of symptom management, such as a single symptom or outcome [44,45]; (3) They fail to explain how external factors affect intermediary variables, reducing explanatory power [32,46,47]; (4) They focus on relationships between symptoms and outcomes rather than pathways to reduce symptom burden [32,37,38,48]; (5) Their complexity limits practical clinical use [32,37,38]; and (6) They do not adjust for covariables [38,39,41,44,[47][48][49]. This study addresses these gaps by developing a personcentered symptom management model based on empirical evidence, initially validated using Partial Least Squares Structural Equation Modeling (PLS-SEM), which is ideal for early theory development as it does not require a strong theoretical foundation [50]. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Background Symptom burden among hemodialysis patients significantly impacts their quality of life. Effective symptom management, supported by social support and coping strategies, is critical to improve patient outcomes. However, the role of social support, self-regulatory fatigue, and different coping mechanisms in patient-centered symptom management is not well understood. Methods A cross-sectional study using Partial Least Squares Structural Equation Modeling (PLS-SEM). Data were collected from multiple hemodialysis centers in various regions across China, ensuring a representative sample of diverse geographical areas. Participants were recruited through convenience sampling across different regions, ensuring broad demographic representation. This study used PLS-SEM to develop and validate a theoretical model that describes the relationships among social support, self-regulatory fatigue, adaptation, patient activation, and symptom burden. Results A total of 1,120 patients participated, with a mean age of 51.6 years (SD = 13.8), and 59.1% were male. The Partial Least Squares Structural Equation Modeling (PLS-SEM) results showed that social support had a significant positive effect on both patient activation (β = 0.209, p < 0.001) and adaptation (β = 0.472, p < 0.001), indicating higher levels of social support were associated with increased patient activation and adaptation. Self-regulatory fatigue had a significant negative effect on adaptation (β = -0.131, p < 0.001) but no significant effect on patient activation (β = -0.026, p = 0.455). Patient activation (β = -0.024, p = 0.019) and adaptation (β = -0.023, p = 0.011) both had significant negative effects on symptom burden, indicating that higher levels of activation and adaptation were linked to lower symptom burden. Mediation analysis revealed that social support indirectly reduced symptom burden through both adaptation (β = -0.011, p = 0.011) and patient activation (β = -0.005, p = 0,032). Patient activation demonstrated greater importance in symptom management compared to adaptation based on the importance-performance analysis. Conclusions This study reveals that social support significantly enhances both patient activation and adaptation, leading to a reduction in symptom burden among hemodialysis patients. Self-regulatory fatigue, however, negatively affects adaptation but does not have a significant impact on patient activation. The dual coping strategies—adaptation (passive) and patient activation (proactive)—mediate the relationship between social support and symptom burden, with patient activation showing greater importance in symptom management. These findings emphasize the importance of enhancing social support, reducing self-regulatory fatigue, and fostering duel coping strategies to effectively alleviate the symptom burden in hemodialysis patients.
Article
Full-text available
Objectives: Cognitive impairment is common in haemodialysis patients and is associated with increased hospitalization and mortality. However, subjective cognitive complaints (SCCs), the self-experienced difficulties in everyday cognitive activities, remain poorly understood. This study examined the prevalence and course of SCCs in haemodialysis patients and its longitudinal associations with sociodemographic, clinical and patient-reported variables. Design: Observational prospective study with baseline and 12-month follow-up assessment. Methods: Based on a validated cut-off point on the Kidney Disease Quality of Life Cognitive Function subscale, haemodialysis patients (N = 159; 40.3% female, mean age 53.62) were classified into cognitive complaint trajectories: (1) resilient (60.4%; no/low SCCs throughout); (2) persistent (8.8%; stable high SCCs); (3) deterioration (17.6%; from no/low to high SCCs); and (4) recovery (13.2%; from high to no/low SCCs). Sociodemographic/clinical characteristics, self-efficacy, self-management skills, adherence, mood and biochemical assays were measured at both assessments and compared among trajectories using mixed ANOVAs. Results: Interaction effects indicated significant improvements in the recovery group in clinical outcomes (i.e., decreased phosphorus and calcium-phosphorus product), self-efficacy and mood over time. Group effects indicated significantly poorer self-efficacy, self-management skills and adherence in the persistent group than other trajectories across both assessments. None of the sociodemographic/clinical characteristics was associated with SCC trajectories. Conclusions: The extent of SCCs vary over time across haemodialysis patients. Routine screening of SCCs in dialysis settings may help identifying patients at risk of poor self-management and worse prognosis. Strategies that compensate for cognitive lapses may mitigate the perceived cognitive burden of this population.
Article
Full-text available
Background and objectives This prospective study seeks to examine the utility of SCD as a marker of future progression to dementia in a community-based cohort of non-Latinx White, non-Latinx Black and Latinx individuals. Debate surrounds the utility of Subjective Cognitive Decline (SCD), the subjective perception of decline in one’s cognition before such impairment is evident in traditional neuropsychological assessments, as an early indicator of impending Alzheimer’s disease. Unfortunately, most studies examining SCD have been conducted in non-Latinx White samples and commonly exclude groups of individuals shown to be most vulnerable to dementia. Methods Participants were enrolled into this cohort study from the Washington Heights–Inwood Columbia Aging Project (WHICAP) if they were cognitively unimpaired, had baseline measurement of SCD and self-identified as non-Latinx White, non-Latinx Black or Latinx. SCD was measured as a continuous sum of 10-items assessing cognitive complaints. Competing risk models tested main effects of baseline SCD on progression to dementia. Models were adjusted for age, sex/gender, years of education, medical comorbidity burden, enrollment cohort and baseline memory test performance with death jointly modelled as a function of race/ethnicity. Results A total of 4,043 (1,063 non-Latinx White, 1,267 non-Latinx Black and 1,713 Latinx) participants were selected for this study with mean age of 75 years, 67% women and with a mean follow up of 5 years. Higher baseline SCD was associated with increased rates of incident dementia over time in the full sample (HR=1.085, CI=1.047, 1.125, p<.001) as well as within Latinx (HR=1.084, CI=1.039, 1.130, p<.001) and Black individuals (HR=1.099, CI=1.012, 1.194, p=.024). Discussion Overall results of this study support SCD as a prodromal marker of dementia in a multiracial community sample, and in Latinx and non-Latinx Black individuals in particular. As models examining the risk of dementia were adjusted for baseline memory test performance, results support the idea that SCD, a subjective reflection of one’s own current cognitive functioning, contributes information above and beyond standard memory testing. Current findings highlight the importance of carefully evaluating any memory concerns raised by older adults during routine visits and underscore the potential utility of screening older adults for SCD.
Article
Full-text available
Background Subjective cognitive complaints refer to self-experienced difficulties with everyday cognitive tasks. Although there has been a fair amount of research on cognitive impairments and cognitive complaints in end-stage renal disease, the practical implications of these complaints remain unclear. The current study aims to examine the associations of cognitive complaints with sociodemographic and clinical variables, mood, as well as key patient-reported outcomes, i.e., self-efficacy, self-management skills, and treatment adherence. Methods A total of 305 haemodialysis patients (mean age = 53.97 years, 42.6% female) completed the Kidney Disease Quality of Life Cognitive Function subscale, a brief measure of cognitive complaints. The recommended cut-off point of 60 was used to identify probable cognitive impairment. Measures of self-efficacy, self-management skills (i.e., symptom coping, health monitoring, health service navigation), treatment adherence, and mood symptoms were also administered. Between-group comparisons and correlational analyses were performed to examine associations of cognitive complaints with sociodemographic, clinical, and health behaviour variables. Mediation analyses were also conducted to investigate the mediating role of self-efficacy on the relationship between cognitive complaints and treatment adherence. Results Nearly a quarter (23.0%) of haemodialysis patients reported cognitive complaints indicative of clinical impairments. Risk of probable impairments was higher for patients with hypertension, diabetes, those diagnosed with end-stage renal disease at an older age, and those with shorter time on dialysis. Subjective cognitive complaints (both rates of probable impairments as per cut-off and continuous scores) were significantly associated with lower disease and treatment self-efficacy, poorer self-management skills, lower treatment adherence, as well as higher symptoms of distress. Mediation analysis indicated that treatment self-efficacy mediated the relationship between cognitive complaints and treatment adherence. Conclusions The current study demonstrated the clinical characteristics of haemodialysis patients who report cognitive complaints indicative of probable cognitive impairments and showed the associations of these complaints with self-management outcomes. Future studies should adopt more comprehensive measures of cognitive complaints and longitudinal designs to confirm the current findings.
Article
Full-text available
Cognitive impairment is common in patients with end-stage renal disease (ESRD) and is associated with compromised quality of life and functional capacity, as well as worse clinical outcomes. Most previous research and reviews in this area were focused on objective cognitive impairment, whereas patients’ subjective cognitive complaints (SCCs) have been less well-understood. This systematic review aimed to provide a broad overview of what is known about SCCs in adult ESRD patients. Electronic databases were searched from inception to January 2022, which identified 221 relevant studies. SCCs appear to be highly prevalent in dialysis patients and less so in those who received kidney transplantation. A random-effects meta-analysis also shows that haemodialysis patients reported significantly more SCCs than peritoneal dialysis patients (standardised mean difference -0.20, 95% confidence interval -0.38 to -0.03). Synthesis of longitudinal studies suggests that SCCs remain stable on maintenance dialysis treatment but may reduce upon receipt of kidney transplant. Furthermore, SCCs in ESRD patients have been consistently associated with hospitalisation, depression, anxiety, fatigue, and poorer quality of life. There is limited data supporting a strong relation between objective and subjective cognition but preliminary evidence suggests that this association may be domain-specific. Methodological limitations and future research directions are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Cognitive deficits are common after brain injury and can be measured in various ways. Many neuropsychological tests are designed to measure specific cognitive deficits, and self-report questionnaires capture cognitive complaints. Measuring cognition in daily life is important in rehabilitating the abilities required to undertake daily life activities and participate in society. However, assessment of cognition in daily life is often performed in a non-standardized manner. In this opinion paper we discuss the various types of assessment of cognitive functioning and their associated instruments. Drawing on existing literature and evidence from experts in the field, we propose a framework that includes seven dimensions of cognition measurement, reflecting a continuum ranging from controlled test situations through to measurement of cognition in daily life environments. We recommend multidimensional measurement of cognitive functioning in different categories of the continuum for the purpose of diagnostics, evaluation of cognitive rehabilitation treatment, and assessing capacity after brain injury.
Article
Full-text available
(1) Background: Cognitive impairment (CI) is common in chronic kidney disease (CKD) and patients treated with hemodialysis. (2) Methods: The systematic review was prepared following the PRISMA statement (2013). The biomedical electronic databases MEDLINE and SCOPUS were searched. (3) Results: out of 1093 studies, only 30, which met problem and population criteria, were included in this review. The risk factors for CI can be divided into three groups: traditional risk factors (present in the general population), factors related to dialysis sessions, and nontraditional risk factors occurring more frequently in the HD group. (4) Conclusions: the methods of counteracting CI effective in the general population should also be effective in HD patients. However, there is a need to develop unique anti-CI approaches targeting specific HD risk factors, i.e., modified hemodialysis parameters stabilizing cerebral saturation and blood flow.
Article
Full-text available
Cognitive impairment is independently associated with kidney disease and increases in prevalence with declining kidney function. At the stage where kidney replacement therapy is required, with dialysis or transplantation, cognitive impairment is up to three times more common, and can present at a younger age. This is not a new phenomenon. The cognitive interactions of kidney disease are long recognized from historical accounts of uremic encephalopathy and so-called “dialysis dementia” to the more recent recognition of cognitive impairment in those undergoing kidney replacement therapy (KRT). The understanding of cognitive impairment as an extra-renal complication of kidney failure and effect of its treatments is a rapidly developing area of renal medicine. Multiple proposed mechanisms contribute to this burden. Advanced vascular aging, significant multi-morbidity, mood disorders, and sleep dysregulation are common in addition to the disease-specific effects of uremic toxins, chronic inflammation, and the effect of dialysis itself. The impact of cognitive impairment on people living with kidney disease is vast ranging from increased hospitalization and mortality to decreased quality of life and altered decision making. Assessment of cognition in patients attending for renal care could have benefits. However, in the context of a busy clinical service, a pragmatic approach to assessing cognitive function is necessary and requires consideration of the purpose of testing and resources available. Limited evidence exists to support treatments to mitigate the degree of cognitive impairment observed, but promising interventions include physical or cognitive exercise, alteration to the dialysis treatment and kidney transplantation. In this review we present the history of cognitive impairment in those with kidney failure, and the current understanding of the mechanisms, effects, and implications of impaired cognition. We provide a practical approach to clinical assessment and discuss evidence-supported treatments and future directions in this ever-expanding area which is pivotal to our patients' quality and quantity of life.
Article
Background Subjective cognitive decline (SCD) is an early manifestation of cognitive deterioration (CD) in some individuals. Therefore, it is worthwhile to conduct a systematic review and meta-analysis to summarise predictors of CD among individuals with SCD. Method PubMed, Embase, and Cochrane Library were searched until May 2022. Longitudinal studies that assessed factors associated with CD in SCD population were included. Multivariable-adjusted effect estimates were pooled using random-effects models. The credibility of evidence was assessed. The study protocol was registered with PROSPERO. Results A total of 69 longitudinal studies were identified for systematic review, of which 37 were included for the meta-analysis. The mean conversion rate of SCD to any CD was 19.8%, including all-cause dementia (7.3%) and Alzheimer’s disease (4.9%). Sixteen factors (66.67%) were found as predictors, including 5 SCD features (older age at onset, stable SCD, both self- and informant-reported SCD, worry and SCD in the memory clinic), 4 biomarkers (cerebral amyloid β-protein deposition, lower scores of Hulstaert formula, higher total tau in the cerebrospinal fluid and hippocampus atrophy), 4 modifiable factors (lower education, depression, anxiety and current smoking), 2 unmodifiable factors (apolipoprotein E4 and older age) and worse performance on Trail Making Test B. The robustness of overall evidence was impaired by risk of bias and heterogeneity. Conclusion This study constructed a risk factor profile for SCD to CD conversion, supporting and supplementing the existing list of features for identifying SCD populations at high risk of objective cognitive decline or dementia. These findings could promote early identification and management of high-risk populations to delay dementia onset. PROSPERO registration number CRD42021281757.
Article
Cancer and its treatments are associated with increased risk for cancer-related cognitive impairment (CRCI). Methods and measures used to study and assess self-reported CRCI (sr-CRCI), however, remain diverse, resulting in heterogeneity across studies. The Patient-Reported Outcomes Working Group has been formed to promote homogeneity in the methods used to study sr-CRCI. In this report, using a psychometric taxonomy, we inventory and appraise instruments used in research to measure sr-CRCI, and we consider advances in patient-reported outcome methodology. Given its psychometric properties, we recommend the Patient-Reported Outcome Measurement Information System Cognitive Function Short Form 8a for measurement of sr-CRCI in cancer patients and survivors, at a minimum, in order to increase scientific rigor and progress in addressing CRCI.