Content uploaded by Sumedh Rao
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Sumedh Rao on Oct 15, 2024
Content may be subject to copyright.
European Journal of Social Psychology
RESEARCH ARTICLE
The Longitudinal Relationship Between Youth Intergroup
Contact and Social Cohesion Outcomes in Two Divided
Societies
Shelley McKeown1,2Christoph Daniel Schaefer1,2Shazza Ali1Pier-Luc Dupont3David Manley1
Sumedh Rao1,2Laura K. Taylor4Rose Meleady5
1School of Education, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK 2Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK 3Department of Politics,
Philosophy and International Relations, Swansea University, Swansea, UK 4School of Psychology, University College Dublin, Dublin, UK 5School of
Psychology, University of East Anglia, Norwich, UK
Correspondence: Shelley McKeown Jones (shelley.mckeownjones@psy.ox.ac.uk)
Received: 15 January 2024 Accepted: 18 September 2024
Keywords: intergroup contact | prejudice | prosocial behaviour | social cohesion | youth
ABSTRACT
Intergroup contact has long been established as a prejudice-reduction tool in divided societies, with contact being particularly
effective during adolescence. A large proportion of evidence, however, draws on cross-sectional surveys or analytical approaches
that do not distinguish between- and within-person effects. In the present research, we address this by exploring the potential of
intergroup contact longitudinally on social cohesion–related outcomes amongst youth (aged 14–19) in Belfast (Study 1, N=231) and
Bradford (Study 2, N=159). Measures included intergroup contact, outgroup attitudes, intergroup anxiety, outgroup empathy and
outgroup prosocial behaviour across three time points. Using random-intercept cross-lagged panel models, results demonstrate
between-person associations of contact with our outcomes, but limited within-person changes. Our findings demonstrate the
potential and limitations of intergroup contact for social cohesion–related outcomes for youth growing up in divided societies,
pointing to the need for developmental-focused future research.
1 Introduction
High levels of ethnic, religious and racial prejudice are a prevail-
ing feature in many societies across the globe, especially those
with a long history of political violence and ethnic tensions. One
approach to reducing such prejudice is to implement strategies
that promote positive and meaningful interactions between the
groups in conflict, known as intergroup contact (Allport 1954).
Substantial evidence shows that intergroup contact entails pos-
itive outcomes in different forms and amongst different groups
(e.g., Paluck, Green, and Green 2019; Pettigrew and Tropp 2006).
Perhaps unsurprisingly, therefore, intergroup contact principles
are often applied to promote social cohesion–related outcomes
in divided societies, including through education and other
community-focused interventions (Al Ramiah and Hewstone
2013; McKeown and Cairns 2012).
A large proportion of the evidence for the benefits of intergroup
contact, however, comes either from cross-sectional studies
that cannot establish causality or from longitudinal studies that
do not clearly separate between-person variance (differences
between individuals over time) from within-person variance
(changes in individual values over time). Recent analyses
using statistical techniques have found limited evidence of
within-person reduction of prejudice following within-person
increases in intergroup contact (Friehs et al. 2024; Hodson and
Meleady 2024). Commentators have posited several reasons
as to why this might be the case. One suggestion is that there
This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly
cited.
© 2024 The Author(s). European Journal of Social Psychology published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
European Journal of Social Psychology, 2024; 0:1–16
https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.3121
1of16
could be ‘third variable’ explanations such that those who
are more likely to hold negative attitudes due to individual
difference factors are less likely to engage in contact (Hodson
and Meleady 2024). Another is the idea that the impact of contact
experiences on prejudice diminishes as they become more
common for an individual over time (Friehs et al. 2024;Wölfer
et al. 2016). There may, however, be a related developmental
explanation whereby contact is most influential in reducing
prejudice in early-to-mid childhood, with a continued reduced
effect through adolescence and into adulthood due to the
stabilisation of intergroup attitudes. Evidence for this assertion is
supported by Merrilees et al. (2023) who found that the strength
of the intergroup contact quality–prejudice relationship drops
through adolescence, and by Wölfer et al. (2016) who found that
intergroup contact as measured via friendship was a stronger
predictor of adolescent compared to adult intergroup attitudes.
Few studies, however, have differentiated between-person and
within-person effects of contact on prejudice amongst youth. An
exception is Friehs et al. (2024) who examined contact effects
on attitudes over time with a sample of school children in
England (Mage =12.11 years) and found evidence for between-
person but not within-person effects of contact on prejudice.
It remains unclear, however, whether the lack of observed
effects applies to mid-late adolescence, and further, to outcomes
beyond prejudice reduction. We know, for example, that contact
has proven to be associated with lower levels of intergroup
anxiety (Pettigrew and Tropp 2008), higher levels of empathy
(Pettigrew and Tropp 2008) and to encourage prosocial actions
targeted towards outgroup members (Koschate et al. 2012;
McKeown and Taylor 2018), but whether this can be observed
when tested for within-person changes remains untested
to date.
The present research, therefore, aims to examine the between-
and within-person potential of the quantity and quality of
intergroup contact experiences in adolescence on outcomes
that relate to social cohesion, including outgroup attitudes,
outgroup empathy, intergroup anxiety and outgroup prosocial
behaviour. We assess these relationships amongst youth (aged 14–
19) growing up in Belfast and Bradford through a three-time-point
survey study. Youth in both settings are experiencing the effects
of past and present ethno-religious tensions, which highlights
the importance of further exploring the potential of intergroup
contact to promote social cohesion agendas.
1.1 Intergroup Contact Theory
For decades, intergroup contact has been considered one of
psychology’s most effective strategies for improving intergroup
relations. Inspired by early observations of the benefits of
desegregation on racial attitudes, Allport’s (1954) ‘contact hypoth-
esis’ holds that encouraging interactions across group lines is
key to reducing hostilities and improving intergroup relations.
According to the original formulation of the contact hypothesis,
contact works best under four conditions: cooperation, common
goals, equal status and social/institutional support. Reflecting
its intuitive appeal and applied potential, this hypothesis has
become one of the most extensively tested ideas in psychology.
Multiple meta-analytic integrations attest to the robust, positive
impact of intergroup contact on prejudice (e.g., Davies et al. 2011;
Lemmer and Wagner 2015; Pettigrew and Tropp 2006). Across
different implementations, participant populations and bases for
group membership, more contact is generally associated with less
prejudice, even when not all Allport’s contact conditions are met.
This is the case for both intergroup contact quantity (how often
interactions occur) and intergroup contact quality (how positive
interactions are).
The contact hypothesis has now evolved into a sophisticated
theoretical framework, more complex and complete than All-
port’s (1954) original formulation, specifying how, when and
why contact is associated with reduced prejudice (Hodson and
Hewstone 2013; Pettigrew and Tropp 2011). Intergroup contact has
been found, for example, to play an important role in reducing
intergroup anxiety, promoting empathy (Pettigrew and Tropp
2008) and, in turn, reducing prejudice. These emotive factors
can also have knock-on effects on indicators of social cohesion.
Reducing intergroup anxiety through imagined contact can, for
example, lead to more contact motivation and a lower tendency to
avoid outgroup members (Turner, West, and Christie 2013). There
is also evidence that empathy can lead to more positive attitudes
and, in turn, to actions that have benefits for outgroup members
and society at large (see the empathy-attitudes-action model;
Batson et al. 1997;2002). A study conducted amongst children
and young people in Northern Ireland, for example, found that
empathy can impact outgroup attitudes, and, in turn, not only
individual-helping actions, but also actions that are indicative of
structural and cultural change (Taylor and McKeown 2021)that
are arguably linked to social cohesion. Evidence can also be found
for the direct effects of contact on prosocial actions, including
those intended to benefit outgroup members (Koschate et al. 2012;
McKeown and Taylor 2018). Outcomes such as these are arguably
crucial for building more cohesive societies. Taken together,
there is significant evidence that contact can reduce prejudice
and can have wider outcomes beyond prejudice reduction that
have potential to impact individuals, groups and society at
large. There are, however, several methodological critiques of
contact research that must be acknowledged, which we discuss
now.
1.2 Methodological Critiques of Contact
Research
A significant challenge to the apparent effects of intergroup
contact arises through the cross-sectional nature of many of the
studies on which the evidence base is constructed, as they cannot
establish causality. Around 71% of the studies included in the
seminal meta-analysis of Pettigrew and Tropp (2006) were cross-
sectional (Christ and Wagner 2013), and only 5% of the studies
were experimental, varying widely in methodology and outgroups
(Paluck, Green, and Green 2019). Whilst experimental evidence
is an ideal way to test the causal effects of contact and has
shown reductions in prejudice, experiments are less frequently
conducted, with most studies being short term and involving
limited and infrequent contact (Paluck, Green, and Green 2019).
These observations point to a need to consider alternative designs
and approaches if we are to better understand contact effects over
time—whether they exist and whether they persist. Longitudinal
research holds considerable promise to measure such naturally
occurring psychological processes as they unfold.
2of16 European Journal of Social Psychology,2024
10990992, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/ejsp.3121 by Sumedh Rao - <Shibboleth>-member@open.ac.uk , Wiley Online Library on [14/10/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Longitudinal research on intergroup contact is growing. A
challenge, however, is that the majority of studies that have used
longitudinal designs to explore contact effects have relied on tra-
ditional methods of analysis such as the cross-lagged panel model
(CLPM) (e.g., Binder, Zagefka, and Brown 2009; Dhont, Van Hiel,
and Hewstone 2014;Swartetal.2011), which whilst valuable
are unable to clearly separate between-person variance from
within-person variance. This has implications for the conclusions
that can be drawn because, whilst we would expect to find that
in general people who engage in more contact report lower levels
of prejudice (between-person effects), we should also expect to
see individual variation such that someone who reports having
higher levels of contact also reports lower levels of prejudice
later (within-person effects). Here, the proportion of variance
that is stable across time can be understood as between-person
variance, whilst the proportion of variance that reflects individual
variability can be understood as within-person variance. While
between-person differences in the independent variable are not
necessarily problematic for causal inferences, between-person
differences reflected in the dependent variable can result in
inaccurate conclusions when using analytical methods such as
CLPMs. This is due to the influence of unobserved third variables
on the independent and the dependent variables. In such cases,
significant cross-lagged paths are assumed to represent evidence
that contact induces change that subsequently reduces prejudice,
but they may instead reflect stable differences (between people)
of third variables. Alternative statistical approaches are therefore
essential to differentiate within-effects from between-effects
more unequivocally, to move closer to a causal analysis in contact
research and to better understand the nature and effects of
intergroup contact on prejudice reduction and beyond.
1.3 Examining Within-Person Changes and
Between-Person Differences
Recent analyses using a new statistical technique capable
of separating within-person variance from between-person
variance—the ‘random intercept cross-lagged panel model’
(RI-CLPM)—have yielded significant between-person negative
associations between contact and prejudice, but no evidence
of within-person change of prejudice following within-person
change of contact (Friehs et al. 2024; Hodson and Meleady 2024).
These findings are incongruent with mainstream thinking about
contact as a process capable of producing changes within people
over time in the form of lower prejudice. Indeed, between-
person associations without within-person changes would
be consistent with third-variable explanations, according to
which the relation between contact and prejudice results from
differences in personality (i.e., the type of people that are involved
in contact also report lower prejudice) and not from a process
in which contact changes attitudes. Therefore, it is necessary
to ask why no associations of within-person changes are being
observed.
It is argued here that one reason evidence has not been found
that within-person change in intergroup contact predicts within-
person change in prejudice in these analyses is because we need
to consider if and how intergroup effects vary over time and
for different age groups. Evidence shows, for example, using
a five-wave study amongst youth in Northern Ireland that the
strength of the relationship between intergroup contact quality
and prejudice grows through adolescence, but then drops from
age 16 onwards (Merrilees et al. 2023). There is also evidence
that the relationship between contact, measured as friendship
through social networks, and prejudice reduction is stronger in
adolescence (aged 13–19) than in adulthood (aged 20–26, Wölfer
et al. 2016). It could be that these effects are due to the stabilisation
of attitudes during adolescence (Wölfer et al. 2016)andas
such, that contact may exert most of its within-person influence
in early-to-mid childhood, but then declines through adoles-
cence and into adulthood (leaving between-person differences
to drive more of the effects). That is, contact may exert mean-
ingful within-person effects mainly at a specific developmental
stage.
To our knowledge, few studies have considered whether there is
evidence for between- and within-person effects of contact during
adolescence as a critical period of development. An exception
is Friehs et al. (2024), who analysed social network data from a
five-wave social network sample of 11–12 year olds in England
to explore this and found no evidence of within-person change
in the contact–prejudice relationship. The authors note that this
finding may be due to a reduction of contact effects on prejudice
over time, i.e., the first series of intergroup interactions could be
particularly potent in reducing prejudice, but this effect could
reduce and stabilise with later repeated interactions. A connected
possibility, however, is that there are age-related effects. In
other words, it could be that between–within person dynamics
play out differently across stages of adolescence as indicated by
previous research exploring the contact–prejudice relationship
over time (Merrilees et al. 2023; Wölfer et al. 2016). It is the
nature of contact experiences in adolescence that we focus on
here.
2The Present Research
The present research pushes forward current knowledge by
exploring the potential impact of contact quantity and quality on
outcomes relevant to social cohesion for young people growing
up in socially divided societies. Previous research adopting an
RI-CLPM approach has explored the effects of contact measured
by quantity or friendship in the domain of harmony (i.e., liking;
Friehs et al. 2024; Hodson and Meleady 2024), and the domain
of equality (i.e., collective action, solidarity; Górska and Tausch
2023;Senguptaetal.2023), using data mostly from adult samples.
Here, we expand on these contributions in two ways by consider-
ing the longitudinal effects of both intergroup contact quantity
and intergroup contact quality: (1) amongst youth growing up in
divided contexts and (2) on outcomes related to social cohesion
that include outgroup attitudes, intergroup anxiety (measured as
comfort), outgroup empathy and outgroup prosocial behaviour.
Whilst we do not test developmental processes specifically, we
focus on exploring between- and within-person contact effects
during adolescence, as this may be one of the critical stages in
which to explore the nature of contact and its consequences, as
well as a time in which to intervene in promoting contact. If, for
example, we find that within-person effects of contact exist at a
specific developmental stage, then this suggests that contact is a
relevant intervention, but one that needs to be introduced earlier
than has been the focus in the field.
3of16
10990992, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/ejsp.3121 by Sumedh Rao - <Shibboleth>-member@open.ac.uk , Wiley Online Library on [14/10/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
RI:
Attitudes
Attitudes T1 Attitudes T3Attitudes T2
Attitudes
T2 (W)
Attitudes
T3 (W)
RI:
Contact
Quantity
Attitudes
T1 (W)
RI:
Contact
Quality
Contact
Quality T1
Contact
Quality T3
Contact
Quality T2
Contact
Quality
T2 (W)
Contact
Quality
T3 (W)
Contact
Quality
T1 (W)
Contact
Quantity
T2 (W)
Contact
Quantity
T3 (W)
Contact
Quantity
T1 (W)
Contact
Quantity T1
Contact
Quantity T3
Contact
Quantity T2
FIGURE 1 Representation of our conceptual model used in our RI-CLPM analyses. The figure depicts the model with the outcome variable
‘outgroup attitudes’ as an example. In our statistical analyses, one model is specified for each of the outcome variables, which were: outgroup attitudes,
outgroup empathy, intergroup anxiety and outgroup prosocial behaviour. The latent variables (i.e., random intercepts and centred variables) are derived
from the measured variables. The random intercepts are marked with ‘RI’. The centred variables (marked with ‘(W)’) reflect fluctuations around
individual average values (within-person variance). Of central interest in the RI-CLPM are the cross-lagged paths between the centred variables. (Not
shown in the figure are the errors of the centred variables at T2 and T3; neither shown are the permitted correlations between the random intercepts,
between the centred variables at T1 and between the errors of the centred variables within T2 and within T3.)
A simplified version of our conceptual and RI-CLPM model is
presented in Figure 1. Here, we include only one outcome variable
as an example, to ease readability.
To achieve our aims, we draw on three-time-point survey data
collected as part of a larger study on youth intergroup contact in
two settings: Belfast, Northern Ireland (Study 1), and Bradford,
England (Study 2). This multicontext analysis enables us to
examine contact effects in one historical and ongoing conflict
between ethno-religious groups and in another context of more
contemporary division between ethnic groups. Whilst previous
RI-CLPM studies have been conducted in societies with ethnic
tensions such as England (Friehs et al. 2024), Germany (Friehs
et al. 2024) and New Zealand (Sengupta et al. 2023), to our
knowledge, few have explored within-person effects of contact
in what might be best described as conflict or post-conflict
settings where contact tends to be less frequent and perhaps even
less positive due to the nature of segregated communities and
inequalities that continue to permeate society. We explain more
about each of these contexts below.
2.1 Research Context
Youth growing up in Belfast and Bradford are experiencing the
effects of past and present ethnic tensions. The Northern Ireland
context can be understood as a setting marked by protracted
ethno-religious conflict between those who wish Northern Ire-
land to remain part of the UK (Protestant) and those who wish
for reunification with Ireland (Catholic). Despite the 1998 Peace
Agreement, violence remains (Taylor et al. 2016) and community
4of16 European Journal of Social Psychology,2024
10990992, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/ejsp.3121 by Sumedh Rao - <Shibboleth>-member@open.ac.uk , Wiley Online Library on [14/10/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
relations are a government priority. For example, the Together:
Building a United Community Strategy (The Executive Office
[Northern Ireland] 2013) is a key focus of the Belfast City
Council’s Good Relations Programme.
Whilst Bradford does not have the same historical legacy of
conflict as Belfast, it is an ethnically, religiously and culturally
diverse context and thus a pertinent case of intergroup dynamics
to explore the effects of youth intergroup contact. According to
the 2021 census, most individuals living in Bradford belong to
the White/White British (56.7%) or Asian/Asian British (32.1%)
communities. Bradford is a site of integration focus in the
Government’s Integrated Communities Strategy Green Paper
(HM Government 2018) due to historical and continued ethnic
tensions. For example, in 2001, the Bradford riots, which occurred
due to increased tensions between the Asian and White British
communities following an antifascist rally in the city, resulted
in significant damage to property and to individuals, leading to
a large number of arrests (Waddington 2010). Due to instances
like this, promoting social cohesion is a priority in the Bradford
context, and policies such as ‘getting along’ (promoting greater
interaction) are a key pillar in the Bradford for Everyone Strategy
(Stronger Communities Partnership 2019). Given the socially
divided nature of both Belfast and Bradford, understanding the
potential effects of intergroup contact for prejudice reduction and
beyond is vital to inform policy and practice in both settings. In
the present research, we focus specifically on contact with and
attitudes towards the dominant ethno-religious groups in each
of the two contexts: Protestant and Catholic youth growing up
in Belfast (Study 1) and White/White British and Asian/Asian
British youth growing up in Bradford (Study 2).
3Study 1
Study 1 explores the within- and between-person effects of contact
quantity and quality on outgroup attitudes, outgroup empathy,
intergroup anxiety and outgroup prosocial behaviour amongst
youth in Belfast, Northern Ireland.
3.1 Sample
A total of 488 participants, recruited from three participating
secondary schools in Belfast, filled in the survey at Time 1, 419
participants at Time 2 and 460 at Time 3. Of those participants,
we were able to match data for 282 across all the three time
points. As we were interested specifically in Catholic–Protestant
relations, we removed 51 participants who could not be assigned
to either the Catholic or Protestant community and/or who were
not born in Northern Ireland, Ireland or the wider UK context.
This resulted in a final sample of 231 participants (Mage,Time1 =14.7
years, SDage,Time1 =0.71; 36% female, 58% male, 6% other gender or
not stated; 34% Catholic, 66% Protestants). Of the total sample,
30% reported receiving free school meals (an indicator of low
socio-economic status), 2% described the financial situation of
their family as not very well off or as not at all very well off,
55% as average and 35% as well off or very well off (7% missing
values).
3.2 Recruitment
Due to our interest in exploring interactions between Catholic
and Protestant youth, we initially aimed to recruit ethno-
religiously mixed schools in Belfast to take part in the research.
Schools were selected using publicly available demographic
school data on the Northern Ireland Department of Education
website. All secondary-level schools in Belfast where the compo-
sition of the Catholic or Protestant community was no more than
60% of the pupil population (n=7) were contacted in July 2021,
and two agreed to take part in the research: one grammar school
(1438 enrolled, 44% Protestant) and one integrated school (628
enrolled, 44% Protestant). A second school recruitment round
was carried out in September 2021 with a further 11 schools
with no more than 85% of youth from either the Catholic or
Protestant community contacted to take part. This resulted in one
additional school agreeing to participate (controlled boys’ school,
1093 enrolled, 85% Protestant).
Of the participants in the final, matched, sample used for the
data analysis (n=231), 52% were from the grammar school (n=
120), of which 33% reported having a Catholic background and
67% reported having a Protestant background. In the integrated
school (n=99), which accounted for 32% of the final sample, 48%
of participants reported having a Catholic background and 52%
reported having a Protestant background. In the controlled school
(n=38, 16% of the final sample), 100% of participants reported
having a Protestant background.
3.3 Measures
In addition to a series of demographic questions (i.e., age, gender,
community background and free school meal as a proxy for socio-
economic status), youth participants completed the following
survey measures:
3.3.1 Contact Quantity
Adapted from Tam et al. (2009), contact quantity was measured
by asking participants to rate on a four-item scale ranging from
0(none)to3(alot) how much contact they have with each
of the Protestant and Catholic communities ‘at school’, ‘in your
neighbourhood’, ‘across all social situations’ and ‘online/on social
media’. Responses to this question for the outgroup commu-
nity only were considered (e.g., if Protestant, responses about
Catholics were selected). A higher score implied higher frequency
of outgroup contact (Time 1: α=0.76; Time 2: α=0.74;
Time 3: α=0.75). To assess measurement invariance of contact
quantity across the three measurement waves for this sample, we
specified confirmatory factor analysis models. All four contact
quantity items were used as indicators for one latent variable
per time point while allowing the residual variances of the same
items to correlate across waves. The configural models yielded
acceptable fits (CFI =0.971; TLI =0.951; RMSEA =0.062; SRMR
=0.043). The constraints testing for metric invariance did not
lead to a significant deterioration of the fit (p=0.359). The
additional constraints testing for scalar invariance neither led to a
significant deterioration of the fit compared to the previous model
(p=0.072).
5of16
10990992, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/ejsp.3121 by Sumedh Rao - <Shibboleth>-member@open.ac.uk , Wiley Online Library on [14/10/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
3.3.2 Contact Quality
To measure contact quality, youth responded to a series of bipolar
adjectives regarding how they find it when they meet people from
the Protestant and Catholic communities in general, adapted
fromTametal.(2009). This included: do you find the contact
‘pleasant or unpleasant?’ (0 =very unpleasant; 5=very pleasant),
‘competitive (trying to “win” or “beat” each other) or cooperative
(where you work together)?’ (0 =very competitive;5=very
cooperative), ‘casual (e.g., conversations that don’t really matter)
or meaningful (e.g., having deep or personal conversations)?’
(0 =very causal;5=very meaningful). An additional item was
developed to measure online contact quality: ‘When you interact
with people online from the following communities, do you
find the contact pleasant or unpleasant’ (0 =very unpleasant;5
=very pleasant). To examine outgroup contact specifically, we
used only the items that refer to the outgroup (e.g., for Catholic
participants we chose their responses about interactions with
Protestants).
To assess measurement invariance of outgroup contact quality
across the three measurement waves, we specified confirmatory
factor analysis models, using the four contact quality items as
indicators for a latent variable per time point and allowing the
residual variances of the same items to correlate across time
points. Since the configural models did not lead to an acceptable
fit, we omitted the third item above from the scale, which had the
lowest factor loadings (Time 1: α=0.72; Time 2: α=0.75; Time 3: α
=0.77). The resulting three-item measure resulted in good fits in
the configural models (CFI =0.995; TLI =0.988; RMSEA =0.034;
SRMR =0.033). The constraints testing for metric invariance did
not lead to a significant deterioration of the fit (p=0.566). The
additional constraints testing for scalar invariance neither led to a
significant deterioration of the fit compared to the previous model
(p=0.076).
3.3.3 Outgroup Attitudes
To assess outgroup attitudes, youth completed a feeling ther-
mometer (Cairns et al. 2006) asking them to rate how favourable
they feel towards the outgroup (Protestant or Catholic) commu-
nity (0 =unfavourable, 100 =favourable). Before entering this
variable into the statistical models, it was divided by a constant,
in this case by 20. This procedure is recommended by Muthén
and Muthén (1998–2017: 524) who specify observed variables with
variances greater than 10 should be divided by a constant.
3.3.4 Outgroup Empathy
To measure outgroup empathy, two items were adapted from
Hughes et al. (2013). Youth were asked to rate the following
statements: ‘I care about the problems faced by the [respective
outgroup]’ and ‘I find it easy to see things from the point of view
ofthe[respectiveoutgroup]’.Thescalesrangedfrom0(strongly
disagree)to5(strongly agree). The Spearman–Brown coefficient
was ρ=0.69 for Time 1, ρ=0.80 for Time 2 and ρ=0.82 for
Time 3 (Eisinga, Grotenhuis, and Pelzer 2013, demonstrating that
the Spearman–Brown coefficient is the most suitable reliability
measure for two-item scales).
3.3.5 Intergroup Anxiety
Youth were asked to respond to a single item statement on their
levels of comfort when interacting with members of the outgroup
as an indicator of intergroup anxiety, specifically: ‘How comfort-
able do you feel talking to people from the following communities
(more than just saying hello)?’. The scale ranged from 0 (very
uncomfortable)to5(very comfortable) and participants reported
feelings towards interacting with both the Catholic and Protestant
communities. Here, we focus on responses for outgroup contact
only. Responses to this item were reverse coded, so that a high
score indicates high levels of intergroup anxiety.
3.3.6 Outgroup Prosocial Behaviour
Youth were first asked to indicate how often they engaged in
concrete acts of prosocial behaviour (such as helping, cooperation
and concern) (adapted from Taylor et al. 2014). They were then
asked to use this list to report how often they implemented these
acts towards the Protestant or Catholic communities on a scale
ranging from 0 (never)to5(often). It is responses to this single
item that we used to measure outgroup prosocial behaviour in the
present research. Again, we focus only on responses to the item
referring to the outgroup community specifically.
3.4 Procedure
Prior to data collection in the three participating schools, ethical
approval was obtained from the University of Bristol and infor-
mation sheets were sent out to all potential participant’s parents
providing them with the opportunity to opt out their young
person as well as to participate in the research themselves. Survey
data were collected in each of the three Belfast secondary schools
during November 2021–January 2022 (Time 1), March 2022 (Time
2) and May 2022 (Time 3). Surveys took place during school hours
and in the presence of classroom teachers and researchers. In two
of the schools, surveys were completed in computer rooms via
Qualtrics whilst in the third school, they were completed using
paper and pen versions. Each classroom teacher was provided
with a survey information pack prior to data collection and
following this, all pupils were informed about the purpose of the
research, asked to read the information sheet provided and if they
wished to take part, to confirm written consent. The classroom
teachers and researchers were available to answer any questions
during the 30–40-minute data-collection period. All participants
who completed a survey were given a £10 Amazon voucher as
remuneration. Each school was also given a £500 payment at the
end of the project.
3.5 Data Analysis Plan
In our statistical analyses, we examined the effects of both contact
quality and contact quantity on our outcome variables using RI-
CLPMs in Mplus (version 8.5, Muthén and Muthén 1998–2017).
The RI-CLPMs were specified as proposed by Hamaker (2015)as
well as Mulder and Hamaker (2021), separating between-person
variances from within-person variances of both the independent
and the dependent variables. This was implemented by specifying
6of16 European Journal of Social Psychology,2024
10990992, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/ejsp.3121 by Sumedh Rao - <Shibboleth>-member@open.ac.uk , Wiley Online Library on [14/10/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
random intercepts as latent variables for all independent and
dependent variables. There is one random intercept as a latent
variable for each independent variable (contact quantity and
contact quality) and each dependent variable (outgroup attitudes,
outgroup empathy, intergroup anxiety and outgroup prosocial
behaviour), while each random intercept latent variable has
the respective measured variable of all waves as an indicator.
For example, there is one latent variable reflecting the random
intercept of contact quality which has the measured contact
quality variables of Time 1, Time 2 and Time 3 as its three
indicators. The factor loadings are set as 1. These latent variables
are allowed to correlate, representing correlation of between-
person differences (similar to cross-sectional correlations). Group
membership, Catholic or Protestant, was used as a predictor for
the random intercepts.
Additionally, each measured variable is an indicator for another
latent variable which captures the deviation of the measured
variable from the personal average value (of each individual) at
each time point. These time-specific latent variables thus reflect
within-person variations of the respective construct. Structural
relations were specified between the within components anal-
ogously to the traditional cross-lagged panel models, specifying
auto-regressive and cross-lagged effects. The difference to the
traditional cross-lagged panel model is that within RI CLPMs,
within-person deviations from the respective individual average
are used when looking at cross-lagged and autoregressive effects.
For example, within-person deviations of attitudes at Time 2 are
regressed on within-person deviations of attitudes at Time 1 and
on within-person deviations of contact quality at Time 1.
We specified separate models for each outcome variable to
prevent estimation problems resulting from a high number
of parameters. While there is no universal direct relationship
between the number of parameters and the required sample size,
models with more parameters tend to require larger sample sizes
(Jackson 2003; Kline 2016). This approach led to four models, one
for each of the outcome variables: outgroup attitudes, outgroup
empathy, intergroup anxiety and outgroup prosocial behaviour.
Cross-lagged effects of independent variables on the respective
dependent variable were specified. In addition, cross-lagged
effects of the dependent variable on the independent variables
were specified. Autoregressive effects were also specified for all
variables. Full information maximum likelihood estimation was
used to deal with missing values in our dataset.
3.6 Results
Descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations between all
variables at each time point are presented in Table 1.Eachof
the four RI-CLPM models, one associated with each outcome
variable, yielded a good fit to the data (see Table 2for fit indices)
using the criteria specified by Hu and Bentler (1999)whereby
cut-off values close to 0.95 of the CFI and the TLI, values close
to 0.08 of the SRMR and values close to 0.06 of the RMSEA are
indications of a good fit.
At the between-person level, we found that contact quantity was
positively correlated with outgroup attitudes (B=0.30, SE =0.05,
p<0.001), outgroup empathy (B=0.30, SE =0.05, p<0.001) and
outgroup prosocial behaviour (B=0.22, SE =0.03, p<0.001),
and negatively correlated with intergroup anxiety (B=−0.25,
SE =0.04, p<0.001). Similarly, we found that contact quality
was positively correlated with outgroup attitudes (B=0.38,
SE =0.09, p<0.001), outgroup empathy (B=0.38, SE =0.07,
p<0.001) and outgroup prosocial behaviour (B=0.25, SE =0.05,
p<0.001), and negatively correlated with intergroup anxiety (B=
−0.37, SE =0.07, p<0.001). These between-person correlations
indicate the degree to which the participants’ average levels of
contact (across time points) are associated with participants’
average levels of the respective dependent variable (across time
points). The correlations are thus similar to traditional cross-
sectional correlations and the findings align with what would
be expected based on the contact literature, that is, the positive
effects of high quality and frequent contact on outcomes being
associated with stronger social cohesion.
The statistical within-person cross-lagged effects on the depen-
dent variables in the four models are reported in Table 2.A
within-person variable reflects, for each time point and person,
the deviation from an individual’s average value. Between these
within-person variables, cross-lagged and autoregressive paths
were specified, analogously to the paths in traditional cross-
lagged panel models. These paths thus represent associations
between deviations from individual average values, which are
separate from between-person differences. Of special interest in
the present context are the cross-lagged paths which are linking
the individual’s deviation from the individual’s average contact
score with the individual’s deviation from its average attitude
score at the following wave (see Hamaker, Kuiper, and Grasman
2015). Significant effects were observed for contact quantity at
Time 1 on more positive attitudes at Time 2 and contact quality at
Time 2 on all four dependent variables at Time 3. These statistical
within-person cross-lagged effects reflect associations between
time-shifted deviations from individual average values. These
within-person effects thus indicate that individual deviations
(from the individual average values) in contact predicted changes
in the dependent variable at the following time point (in the form
of individual deviations of the dependent variable when control-
ling for the deviations of the previous time point). These findings
demonstrate some, but at the same time, limited evidence for
within-person change of contact on social cohesion outcomes.
3.7 Discussion
The aim of Study 1 was to explore the between- and within-person
variances of contact quantity and quality in relation to outgroup
attitudes, outgroup empathy, intergroup anxiety and outgroup
prosocial behaviour amongst youth in Belfast. Similar to previous
RI-CLPM studies (Friehs et al. 2024; Hodson and Meleady
2024), we demonstrate evidence for between-person differences
of contact quality and quantity in relation to all the measured
outcomes with more frequent and better-quality contact being
associated with more positive outgroup attitudes, higher levels of
outgroup empathy, lower levels of intergroup anxiety and more
outgroup prosocial behaviour coupled with limited evidence for
within-person effects. Specifically, we find that contact quantity
at Time 1 entailed more positive outgroup attitudes at Time
2 and that contact quality at Time 2 entailed more positive
outgroup attitudes, higher outgroup empathy, higher engagement
7of16
10990992, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/ejsp.3121 by Sumedh Rao - <Shibboleth>-member@open.ac.uk , Wiley Online Library on [14/10/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
TABLE 1 Means, standard deviations and intercorrelations for the main variables of the Belfast sample.
Measure MSD 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18
1. Contact Quantity T1 1.67 0.70 0.62 0.66 0.48 0.44 0.40 0.48 0.46 0.46 0.48 0.39 0.40 −0.34 −0.37 −0.41 0.55 0.40 0.43
2. Contact Quantity T2 1.69 0.66 0.55 0.38 0.50 0.28 0.42 0.50 0.43 0.39 0.49 0.31 −0.30 −0.42 −0.34 0.44 0.45 0.35
3. Contact Quantity T3 1.65 0.65 0.44 0.39 0.47 0.42 0.35 0.45 0.41 0.34 0.40 −0.27 −0.33 −0.43 0.43 0.43 0.46
4. Contact Quality T1 3.47 0.87 0.53 0.54 0.61 0.43 0.43 0.47 0.43 0.43 −0.45 −0.39 −0.45 0.48 0.30 0.36
5. Contact Quality T2 3.37 0.98 0.63 0.53 0.63 0.57 0.42 0.61 0.44 −0.26 −0.67 −0.48 0.54 0.47 0.45
6. Contact Quality T3 3.49 0.92 0.43 0.40 0.51 0.34 0.31 0.41 −0.30 −0.41 −0.63 0.43 0.32 0.44
7. Intergroup Attitudes T1 3.56 1.20 0.62 0.62 0.51 0.51 0.51 −0.39 −0.46 −0.45 0.55 0.38 0.44
8. Intergroup Attitudes T2 3.45 1.25 0.70 0.37 0.51 0.38 −0.30 −0.52 −0.49 0.47 0.47 0.37
9. Intergroup Attitudes T3 3.55 1.21 0.40 0.38 0.42 −0.30 −0.48 −0.51 0.40 0.37 0.44
10. Outgroup Empathy T1 2.90 1.17 0.58 0.61 −0.33 −0.37 −0.34 0.49 0.39 0.42
11. Outgroup Empathy T2 2.95 1.20 0.60 −0.25 −0.43 −0.29 0.44 0.38 0.29
12. Outgroup Empathy T3 3.03 1.18 −0.17 −0.40 −0.35 0.42 0.30 0.44
13. Intergroup Anxiety T1 1.03 1.08 0.32 0.40 −0.34 −0.30 −0.23
14. Intergroup Anxiety T2 1.18 1.21 0.42 −0.47 −0.40 −0.46
15. Intergroup Anxiety T3 1.15 1.07 −0.37 −0.38 −0.41
16.Outgr.Prosoc.Beh.T1 2.06 0.92 0.47 0.53
17.Outgr.Prosoc.Beh.T2 2.18 0.82 0.47
18.Outgr.Prosoc.Beh.T3 2.05 0.82
Note: For all variables, a higher score indicates a higher level of the construct in question. For the intercorrelations, ns varied between 191 and 229, owing to missing values. All correlation coefficients (in bold) are significant
(ps<0.05, two-tailed).
8of16 European Journal of Social Psychology,2024
10990992, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/ejsp.3121 by Sumedh Rao - <Shibboleth>-member@open.ac.uk , Wiley Online Library on [14/10/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
TABLE 2 Statistical within-person cross-lagged effects of contact quantity and contact quality, with fit indicators of the respective model. There
was one model for each dependent model (i.e., four models in total).
Measures Model Fit Coefficients
DV IV CFI TLI RM SEA SRMR BSE p
Outgroup Attitudes T2 Contact Quantity T1 1.00 0.99 0.03 0.02 0.52 0.24 0.034
Contact Quality T1 0.14 0.20 0.471
Outgroup Empathy T2 Contact Quantity T1 1.00 1.00 0.01 0.02 0.22 0.23 0.336
Contact Quality T1 0.26 0.19 0.176
Intergroup Anxiety T2 Contact Quantity T1 1.00 1.00 0.00 0.02 −0.35 0.27 0.190
Contact Quality T1 −0.17 0.22 0.449
Outgroup Prosocial Behaviour T2 Contact Quantity T1 1.00 1.00 0.00 0.02 0.09 0.19 0.634
Contact Quality T1 −0.04 0.15 0.780
Outgroup Attitudes T3 Contact Quantity T2 −0.18 0.21 0.385
Contact Quality T2 0.36 0.12 0.004
Outgroup Empathy T3 Contact Quantity T2 −0.34 0.27 0.213
Contact Quality T2 0.36 0.17 0.038
Intergroup Anxiety T3 Contact Quantity T2 0.21 0.24 0.391
Contact Quality T2 −0.40 0.16 0.011
Outgroup Prosocial Behaviour T3 Contact Quantity T2 −0.24 0.19 0.217
Contact Quality T2 0.31 0.10 0.003
Note: For all variables, a higher score indicates a higher level of the construct in question. Outgroup attitudes had been divided by 20 to avoid large differences
between variances. (Autoregressive effects and cross-lagged effects of the respective dependent variable on the independent variables were also specified within
all models.) Full information maximum likelihood was used.
in prosocial outgroup behaviour and lower levels of intergroup
anxiety at Time 3. No effects of contact quantity were observed
on any of our outcomes from Time 2 to Time 3. Although these
findings suggest that there is some potential for within-person
change of contact, we are cautious about this due to the lack
of consistency across time and for our two contact measures.
It is worth noting here, however, that our findings may have
been impacted by the specific time lags in this study whereby
the Time 1–2 interval varied between 1.5 and 3.5 months (due
to school availability), while the Time 2–3 interval was constant
at 2 months and 1 week. This matters because the RI-CLPM
can only detect time-delayed associations between fluctuations
around personal average value, and so is sensitive towards the
choice of the examined time intervals (Hamaker, Kuiper, and
Grasman 2015). Nevertheless, our findings demonstrate the need
to continue to investigate the potential for intergroup contact
to evince within-person change on social cohesion outcomes
amongst adolescents.
4Study 2
Study 2 followed the design of Study 1 to replicate and explore
the within-person changes and between-person differences of
contact quantity and quality in relation to outgroup attitudes,
outgroup empathy, intergroup anxiety and outgroup prosocial
behaviour amongst youth growing up in a different divided
context: Bradford, England.
4.1 Sample
A total of 530 participants completed the survey at Time 1, 334
participants completed the survey at Time 2 and 221 completed
the survey at Time 3. Of those participants, we were able to match
data for 167 across the three time points. We focused specifically
on youth who identified as one of the two largest ethnic groups
in Bradford: White/White British and Asian/Asian British. Of
those participants, 102 specified their ethnicity as Pakistani, 18
as Bangladeshi, 7 as Indian, 1 as Punjabi and 2 as ‘other Asian’;
20 participants specified their ethnic group as White British
and 2 as White European; 9 participants did not specify their
ethnicity beyond White/White British or Asian/Asian British.
Eight participants who did not identify as either White/White
British or Asian/Asian British were removed, resulting in a final
sample of 159 participants (Mage,Time1 =16.5 years, SDage,Time1 =
0.75; 64% female, 31% male, 4% other or no stated gender; 84%
Asian, 16% White; 23% reported receiving free school meals, 72%
stated that they do not receive free school meals; 24% described
the financial situation of their family as not very well off or as
not at all very well off, 58% as average and 11% as well of or very
well off). Participants were recruited through local youth groups,
several grassroots organisations and a local college.1
4.2 Measures
Youth participants completed the same demographic and
survey measures as that of Study 1 with adaptations made
9of16
10990992, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/ejsp.3121 by Sumedh Rao - <Shibboleth>-member@open.ac.uk , Wiley Online Library on [14/10/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
for the Bradford context. The main variables of interest were
analogous to the items which have been used in the Belfast study.
Specifically, participants were asked to rate contact quantity,
contact quality, outgroup attitudes, outgroup empathy and
intergroup anxiety in relation to three ethnic groups: White,
Black and Asian. In the analysis that follows, we focus on
intergroup relations between White/White British and Asian
Asian/British participants, as the two largest ethnic groups in
Bradford. Tests for the scale characteristics of the multi-item
measured constructs are as follows (while intergroup anxiety and
outgroup prosocial behaviour were single item measures and not
tested for reliability but were still included in the survey):
4.2.1 Contact Quantity
The same four items used in Study 1 in Belfast were used for the
Bradford sample in Study 2, with the target groups White/White
British and Asian/Asian British (Time 1: α=0.68; Time 2: α=
0.62; Time 3: α=0.72). The residual variances of the same items
were again allowed to correlate across waves. The configural mod-
els which were used as a basis to assess measurement invariance
across the three waves yielded acceptable fits (CFI =0.988; TLI =
0.980; RMSEA =0.032; SRMR =0.046). The constraints testing
for metric invariance did not lead to a significant deterioration
of the fit (p=0.108). The additional constraints testing for scalar
invariance neither led to a significant deterioration of the fit
compared to the previous model (p=0.639).
4.2.2 Contact Quality
The three items which had been used in Study 1 were also used for
Study 2 (with adapted target groups) to measure contact quality
(Time 1: α=0.70; Time 2: α=0.64; Time 3: α=0.72). Allowing the
residual variances of the same items to correlate across waves, the
three-item measure resulted in good fits in the configural models
used to assess measurement invariance across the three waves
(CFI =0.986; TLI =0.967; RMSEA =0.048; SRMR =0.034). The
constraints testing for metric invariance did not lead to a signifi-
cant deterioration of the fit (p=0.260). The additional constraints
testing for scalar invariance neither led to a significant deteriora-
tion of the fit compared to the previous model (p=0.322).
4.2.3 Outgroup Empathy
The same two items which had been used in Study 1 were used for
the Bradford sample in Study 2 (with adapted target groups). The
Spearman–Brown coefficient was ρ=0.70 for Time 1, ρ=0.59 for
Time 2, and ρ=0.51 for Time 3.
4.3 Procedure
Ethical approval was obtained from the University of Bristol
prior to data collection. To develop the Bradford sample, a 12-
month period of recruitment was undertaken, supported by local
organisations. Emails were sent to ethnically diverse secondary
schools, colleges, and grassroots organisations to invite them to
take part in the research. The recruitment search resulted in a
number of youth groups and grassroots organisations as well as a
college agreeing to take part. No school participation was secured,
and although several expressed interest, they were already part
of other projects and were unable to find the time to participate.
Survey data were collected at three time points between 5.10.22
and 30.11.22 (Time 1), 20.02.22–24.04.22 (Time 2) and 05.06.23–
26.08.23 (Time 3) amongst youth participants across a range of
settings. In all cases, youth were asked to complete the survey
online in Qualtrics either in the presence of the researcher or
a youth leader (for those under 16) or on their own (if over
16). Where relevant, the researchers provided a summary video
about the research (using the same information given via the
information sheet to participants) that was shown to participants
in advance, and the researchers held video calls with youth groups
and grassroots organisations before sharing the survey link with
those interested in taking part. All youth participants were
asked to give informed consent prior to completing the survey.
The survey took approximately 30–40 minutes to complete, and
participants were sent a £10 Amazon voucher. At later data
collection points, all youth were contacted directly and invited
to take part. For those under 16 and recruited through youth
groups, youth leaders were asked to facilitate discussions with
the participants in advance of sending the survey link. Following
completion of the project, supporting organisations were thanked
for their time and facilitation with a £500 payment.
4.4 Data Analysis Plan
Descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations between all vari-
ables at each time point are presented in Table 3. We specified
RI-CLPMs with random intercepts as latent variables, repre-
senting individual average values to ensure the modelling was
analogous to Study 1. These latent variables were allowed to
correlate, indicating between-person associations. Group mem-
bership, White/White British or Asian/Asian British, was used
as a predictor for the random intercepts. Additionally, for each
construct and for each time point, a latent variable was specified
that reflected within-person differences. For all of these within-
person latent variables, autoregressive and cross-lagged effects
were specified. Full information maximum likelihood estimation
was used to deal with missing values.
4.5 Results
The four RI-CLPM models yielded good fit to the data. For
between-person associations, we found that contact quantity was
positively correlated with outgroup attitudes (B=0.15 SE =0.04,
p=0.001), with outgroup empathy (B=0.13, SE =0.04, p=
0.001) and with outgroup prosocial behaviour (B=0.19, SE =
0.04, p<0.001), while it was negatively correlated with intergroup
anxiety (B=−0.10, SE =0.04, p=0.015). We also found that
contact quality was positively correlated with outgroup attitudes
(B=0.32 SE =0.08, p<0.001) and with outgroup empathy (B=
0.29, SE =0.06, p<0.001), while it was negatively correlated with
intergroup anxiety (B=−0.29, SE =0.07, p<0.001), but not with
outgroup prosocial behaviour (B=0.08, SE =0.06, p=0.210).
These between-person correlations indicate the degree to which
the individual average level of contact is associated with the
individual average level of the respective dependent variable. The
10 of 16 European Journal of Social Psychology,2024
10990992, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/ejsp.3121 by Sumedh Rao - <Shibboleth>-member@open.ac.uk , Wiley Online Library on [14/10/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
TABLE 3 Means, standard deviations, and intercorrelations for the main variables of the Bradford sample.
Measure MSD 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18
1. Contact Quantity T1 1.49 0.63 0.59 0.56 0.26 0.09 0.14 0.28 0.22 0.16 0.39 0.31 0.25 −0.23 −0.14 −0.18 0.38 0.26 0.34
2. Contact Quantity T2 1.55 0.58 0.59 0.18 0.07 0.17 0.14 0.20 0.11 0.27 0.30 0.22 −0.13 −0.21 −0.26 0.28 0.24 0.24
3. Contact Quantity T3 1.56 0.62 0.27 0.20 0.28 0.27 0.24 0.18 0.28 0.31 0.32 −0.14 −0.23 −0.29 0.42 0.30 0.40
4. Contact Quality T1 3.42 0.85 0.49 0.48 0.51 0.39 0.37 0.47 0.33 0.41 −0.58 −0.23 −0.38 0.29 0.30 0.18
5. Contact Quality T2 3.39 0.80 0.64 0.42 0.51 0.52 0.28 0.31 0.25 −0.33 −0.46 −0.44 0.20 0.30 0.26
6. Contact Quality T3 3.40 0.83 0.36 0.39 0.60 0.29 0.35 0.49 −0.26 −0.35 −0.59 0.19 0.30 0.30
7. Intergroup Attitudes T1 3.55 1.15 0.53 0.47 0.46 0.28 0.27 −0.38 −0.37 −0.29 0.33 0.23 0.28
8. Intergroup Attitudes T2 3.41 1.06 0.49 0.37 0.25 0.28 −0.23 −0.41 −0.32 0.27 0.40 0.25
9. Intergroup Attitudes T3 3.38 1.11 0.25 0.22 0.38 −0.20 −0.46 −0.45 0.22 0.23 0.41
10. Outgroup Empathy T1 3.15 1.11 0.53 0.56 −0.45 −0.14 −0.23 0.30 0.25 0.19
11. Outgroup Empathy T2 3.09 1.13 0.54 −0.28 −0.27 −0.20 0.36 0.20 0.23
12. Outgroup Empathy T3 3.14 1.01 −0.27 −0.23 −0.38 0.27 0.35 0.38
13. Intergroup Anxiety T1 1.37 1.05 0.19 0.24 −0.25 −0.28 −0.26
14. Intergroup Anxiety T2 1.45 1.14 0.39 −0.15 −0.29 −0.29
15. Intergroup Anxiety T3 1.31 1.02 −0.18 −0.20 −0.20
16.Outgr.Prosoc.Beh.T1 2.11 0.83 0.46 0.43
17. Outgr. Prosoc. Beh. T2 2.06 0.81 0.46
18.Outgr.Prosoc.Beh.T3 2.05 0.82
Note: For all variables, a higher score indicates a higher level of the construct in question. For the intercorrelations, ns varied between 153 and 159, owing to missing values. Correlation coefficients in bold are significant (ps
<0.05, two-tailed).
11 of 16
10990992, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/ejsp.3121 by Sumedh Rao - <Shibboleth>-member@open.ac.uk , Wiley Online Library on [14/10/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
TABLE 4 Statistical within-person cross-lagged effects of contact quantity and contact quality, with fit indicator of the respective model. There
was one model for each dependent model (i.e., four models in total).
Measures Model Fit Coefficients
DV IV CFI TLI RM SEA SRMR BSEp
Outgroup Attitudes T2 Contact Quantity T1 0.99 0.97 0.05 0.03 −0.04 0.27 0.878
Contact Quality T1 0.03 0.19 0.860
Outgroup Empathy T2 Contact Quantity T1 1.00 1.00 0.00 0.03 0.32 0.26 0.222
Contact Quality T1 −0.05 0.17 0.777
Intergroup Anxiety T2 Contact Quantity T1 1.00 0.99 0.02 0.03 0.07 0.34 0.834
Contact Quality T1 0.25 0.27 0.358
Outgroup Prosocial Behaviour T2 Contact Quantity T1 1.00 1.00 0.01 0.03 −0.34 0.19 0.070
Contact Quality T1 0.30 0.13 0.025
Outgroup Attitudes T3 Contact Quantity T2 −0.40 0.34 0.241
Contact Quality T2 0.36 0.23 0.113
Outgroup Empathy T3 Contact Quantity T2 −0.30 0.40 0.449
Contact Quality T2 −0.40 0.27 0.136
Outgroup Anxiety T3 Contact Quantity T2 −0.29 0.31 0.344
Contact Quality T2 −0.19 0.21 0.360
Outgroup Prosocial Behaviour T3 Contact Quantity T2 −0.45 0.29 0.122
Contact Quality T2 0.15 0.19 0.426
Note: For all variables, a higher score indicates a higher level of the construct in question. Outgroup attitudes had been divided by 20 to avoid large differences
between variances. (Autoregressive effects and cross-lagged effects of the respective dependent variable on the independent variables were specified with the same
models.) Full information maximum likelihood was used.
statistical within-person cross-lagged effects on the dependent
variables in the four models are displayed in Table 4.There
were no significant within-person cross-lagged effects, except for
contact quality at Time 1 on outgroup prosocial behaviour at
Time 2. Thus, there was only one instance in which individual
deviations (from the individuals’ average values) in contact
statistically predicted deviations in the dependent variable at the
respective later time point. This contrasts with the Belfast sample,
as in the Belfast sample, contact quality at Time 2 was consistently
associated with all outcome variables at Time 3; this is considered
further in the general discussion.
4.6 Discussion
Study 2 aimed to provide a second test of the within- and between-
person effects of contact quantity and quality on outgroup
attitudes, outgroup empathy, intergroup anxiety and prosocial
behaviour amongst youth in a different context, Bradford. Find-
ings demonstrate evidence for between-person associations of
contact quality and quantity with most of the measured out-
comes, with more frequent and better-quality contact being
associated with more positive intergroup attitudes, higher levels
of outgroup empathy and lower levels of intergroup anxiety.
However, only contact quantity was associated with greater
outgroup prosocial behaviour. Unlike in Study 1, we did not
observe any effects of within-person changes of contact quality
and quantity at Time 1 on within-person changes of the out-
come measures at Time 2 or effects of within-person changes
of contact quality and quantity at Time 2 on within-person
changes of outcome measures at Time 3. This aligns with
previous research using the RI-CLPM which has failed to
find associations of within-person changes between contact
and attitudes and further related outcomes (e.g., Friehs et al.
2024; Hodson and Meleady 2024). In the general discussion,
we bring together the findings of our two studies and con-
sider implications for research and practice in socially divided
contexts.
5General Discussion
The present research explored the potential between- and within-
person impact of intergroup contact on social cohesion–related
outcomes amongst youth in two socially divided contexts, draw-
ing on three-time-point survey data amongst youth in Belfast
(Study 1) and Bradford (Study 2). Findings from across our
two studies align with previous RI-CLPM studies which have
demonstrated the presence of between-subjects effects of contact
on outcomes such as prejudice coupled with an absence of within-
person changes (e.g., Friehs et al. 2024; Hodson and Meleady
2024). We now discuss these collective findings in relation to the
wider literature.
Our finding of consistent between-person differences across our
two studies bolsters previous evidence that those who hold
12 of 16 European Journal of Social Psychology,2024
10990992, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/ejsp.3121 by Sumedh Rao - <Shibboleth>-member@open.ac.uk , Wiley Online Library on [14/10/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
outgroup friendships (Friehs et al. 2024) or have more frequent
intergroup contact (Hodson and Meleady 2024) also report more
positive outgroup attitudes, but pushes this forward by observing
that those who report frequent quantity and better quality contact
also report lower levels of intergroup anxiety (measured as
levels of comfort), higher levels of outgroup empathy and higher
likelihood of engaging in prosocial acts to support outgroup
members (for contact quality only in Study 1) as indicators related
to social cohesion. These between-person effects, however, were
observed in the partial absence of within-person changes. And
whilst we found that contact quantity at Time 1 entailed more
positive outgroup attitudes at Time 2 and that contact quality
at Time 2 entailed more positive outgroup attitudes, higher
outgroup empathy, higher engagement in prosocial outgroup
behaviour and lower levels of intergroup anxiety at Time 3 in
Study 1, we are unable to claim consistent within-person effects
over time or between our studies.
A possible explanation for the lack of within-person effects
observed in our studies, in line with the arguments put forward
by previous research (Friehs et al. 2024; Wölfer et al. 2016),
is that initial engagement in contact may be more powerful
than later engagement in contact in predicting our outcomes of
interest. As such, our participants may have already experienced
both frequent and good quality intergroup contact in the past
which means that we have not captured the potentially trans-
formative power of initial intergroup interactions. A further and
related explanation is that perhaps our participants were at an
age in which the strength of the relationship between contact
and prejudice had already declined. Our Study 2 participants
were older on average (Mage =16.5 years at Time 1) than our
Study 1 participants (Mage =14.7 years at Time 1) but due
to inconsistent findings across time points, we are unable to
determine whether the observation of some within-person effects
in Study 1 is due to developmental differences. Whilst Friehs
et al. (2024), who analysed social network data from a five-wave
social network sample of 11–12 year olds in England, found no
evidence of within-person change in the contact–prejudice rela-
tionship, questions still remain about potential developmental
pathways.
There may, however, be methodological reasons for our lack of
observed within-person effects. First, it is worth considering the
time lags that were used in the present research. Previous RI-
CLPM studies on contact effects have tended to have time lags
ranging from several months (Friehs et al. 2024: Study 1; Hodson
and Meleady 2024) to several years (Sengupta et al. 2023). Given
that longitudinal studies often have time lags that are too long to
detect within-person change (Dormann and Griffin 2015)andthat
one of the few studies that has detected significant within-person
contact effects on collective action (Górska and Tausch 2023)had
time lags of 2 weeks, our aim was to employ relatively short time
lags to increase the chances of observing within-person effects.
It could be that an even shorter time frame than in our studies
is required to find within-person effects. This is something that
should be explored in future research.
Second, it is possible that our measurement of intergroup con-
tact, using self-reported items of contact quality and contact
quantity, does not fully capture the nature of participants’
interaction experiences and consequently we were unable to
detect consistent within-person change. This is plausible given
substantial empirical evidence that when primed, intergroup
contact has been found to reduce prejudice amongst those in
an experimental condition compared to a control condition
(Paluck, Green, and Green 2019). Future research, therefore,
may wish to explore intergroup contact experiences in more
depth.
5.1 Implications for Theory and Practice
Taken together, our two studies yield important theoretical
implications for the research literature on intergroup contact
as well as practical implications for social cohesion agendas.
In relation to theory, we provide further evidence for a lack of
consistent within-person changes in the relationship not only
between intergroup contact and prejudice but also between
intergroup contact and a wider range of social cohesion–related
outcomes. Our observation of consistent between-person effects
in the absence of within-person effects in line with previous
studies (e.g., Hodson and Meleady 2024;Senguptaetal.2023)
highlights the need to theoretically determine the conditions
under which and for whom within-person changes of contact
might be observed and on which outcomes. This requires further
theoretical development, building on the work of Merrilees
et al. (2023) and Wölfer et al. (2016) on the temporal nature
of intergroup contact, including, for example, if and when we
might expect within-person changes to occur across the lifespan
in relation to both prior contact experiences and developmental
trajectories. Our findings also suggest that the field may need
to consider how intergroup contact experiences are measured
in survey research, to truly determine whether within-person
changes are evinced following contact experiences.
Our findings also have practical implications for promoting
social cohesion in divided societies. We show, for example, that
youth who report having good quality and frequent contact
with outgroup members also report high scores on a range of
positive outcomes that can build stronger and more cohesive
societies, such as more positive outgroup attitudes, higher levels
of outgroup empathy, lower levels of outgroup anxiety and
higher levels of self-reported engagement in outgroup prosocial
behaviours. Whilst we are unable to provide evidence for within-
person changes of contact on our outcomes across time points
and samples, this does not, in our view, demonstrate that contact
is not effective in promoting social cohesion, rather that we
need to further explore how and when we might expect within-
person changes to occur. Indeed, we know from experimental
research that contact reduces prejudice (Paluck, Green, and
Green 2019) and as such, it remains an important tool for pro-
moting more positive group relations in divided societies. What
we would suggest, however, is that contact should not be seen
as a panacea and other approaches towards social cohesion are
needed.
5.2 Limitations and Future Directions
Whilst our research has strengths in exploring contact effects
longitudinally amongst youth in two different contexts, there
are some important limitations. First, our sample sizes are
13 of 16
10990992, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/ejsp.3121 by Sumedh Rao - <Shibboleth>-member@open.ac.uk , Wiley Online Library on [14/10/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
relatively small and not fully representative of the ethnic groups
present within each context, especially in Study 2. While previous
research with larger samples (e.g., Hodson and Meleady 2024;
Sengupta et al. 2023) has failed to observe within-person effects of
contact, we cannot rule out that our failure to observe consistent
results is due to our underpowered samples. Future work should,
therefore, seek to recruit a higher number of participants to
allow a more robust analysis of within-person effects across time
amongst adolescents and for different ethnic groups. The need
for larger sample sizes is underlined by the results of our post
hoc power analyses. In the Monte Carlo simulations based on
the Belfast data, a simulated sample size of 440 was required
to ensure that all replications could be computed without any
estimation problems (while entailing power values of over 80%
for the significant within-person effects), as reported above. For
two outcome variables in the Bradford sample, even a simulated
sample size of 10,000 was not sufficient to yield replications that
were consistently without estimation problems. This suggests
that our empirical sample sizes should have ideally been larger
to provide robust parameter estimates. Additionally, needing a
simulated sample size of 600 to reach power estimates of over 80%
in the Bradford sample suggests that the studies might have been
underpowered.
Second, whilst our sample included youth participants across
mid-late adolescence, we did not have a sample over a long
enough period of time to be able to examine developmental
trajectories. A crucial next step will be to conduct research
that enables an analysis of the effects of contact on outcomes
across different developmental ages to determine an under-
standing of whether, and if so, under which context conditions
there is a point in which within contact effects weaken with
age.
Third, several practical considerations meant that participants
did not always complete the survey in the same format (online
vs. on paper), not always across the same time intervals, and
not always in the same setting (some in school, some in college,
some in youth groups). A more controlled approach would
enable stronger conclusions to be made regarding the relation-
ship between intergroup contact and social cohesion–related
outcomes across the two research contexts.
Fourth, we relied on single-item measures of intergroup anxiety
and outgroup prosocial behaviour. This enabled us to have a
more comprehensive survey with our young people by reducing
item response burden, but there may be consequences for overall
validity. For example, our measure of intergroup anxiety may
be related more to feelings of comfort specifically, although
it is worth noting that comfort is one of the most measured
affective components of intergroup anxiety alongside others such
as feeling anxious, at ease, etc. (Stephan 2014). Future research,
however, should aim to measure outcomes using a wider range of
items.
Finally, whilst our research expands the range of outcomes of
intergroup contact that have been previously explored through
RI-CLPM studies, it would be advantageous to assess tertiary-
related outcomes. This is because within-person change might
be observed for outcomes that transcend beyond prejudice
reduction—such as cognitive flexibility or creativity.
6 Conclusion
Intergroup contact has long been established as a prejudice-
reduction tool in divided societies. A large proportion of the
evidence for the benefits of intergroup contact, however, comes
either from cross-sectional studies that cannot establish causality
or from longitudinal studies, many of which do not clearly
separate between-person variance from within-person variance.
The present research aimed to address this limitation by exploring
between-person associations and within-person changes regard-
ing the relationship of intergroup contact with social cohesion
outcomes amongst youth in two divided cities: Belfast and
Bradford. Drawing on three-time-point survey data, our findings
extend previous research demonstrating between-subject effects
of contact quality and quantity on outcomes including outgroup
attitudes, empathy, intergroup anxiety and prosocial behaviour,
but limited evidence of within-person effects on these outcomes.
By focusing on contact effects amongst youth in socially divided
contexts and by exploring a wide range of social cohesion–related
outcomes, our research offers a grounded assessment of the
potential and limitations of intergroup contact to promote social
cohesion. And, whilst failing to observe consistent within-person
effects of contact in our research, we recognise the importance
of continuing to recognise the potential of intergroup contact as
a prejudice-reduction tool, as evidenced in experimental studies
in the field, but point to the need to better understand potential
within-person effects through robust longitudinal studies that
consider the development of intergroup contact experiences over
time and across the lifespan.
Author Contributions
McKeown, Taylor and Manley acquired funding for this research and
led the research design. Data were collected by McKeown and Ali, input
by Ali, analysed by Schaefer and supported by Meleady and Taylor.
All authors contributed to the conceptualisation and write up of the
manuscript.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by funding obtained from the Economic and
Social Research Council [ES/T014709/1]. Our deep appreciation to the
organisations, administrators, teachers, youth leaders and young people
for supporting and participating in this project.
Conflicts of Interest
The authors declare no conflicts of interest.
Data Availability Statement
The data that support the findings of the study will be made available on
the UK Data Archive following the completion of the research project.
Endnotes
1The original aim was to recruit participants aged 14–16 through schools
as per Study 1, but the team was unable to secure access to local schools
and so proceeded with alternative recruitment approaches, supported by
local organisations and the project advisory group, and included a higher
upper age boundary to enable a larger sample to be collected.
14 of 16 European Journal of Social Psychology,2024
10990992, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/ejsp.3121 by Sumedh Rao - <Shibboleth>-member@open.ac.uk , Wiley Online Library on [14/10/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
References
Allport, G. W. 1954. The Nature of Prejudice. Cambridge, MA: Addison
Wes ley.
Al Ramiah, A., and M. Hew stone. 2013. “Intergroup Contact as a Tool
for Reducing, Resolving, and Preventing Intergroup Conflict: Evidence,
Limitations, and Potential.” American Psychologist 68, no. 7: 527–542.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032603.
Batson, C. D., M. P. Polycarpou, E. Harmon-Jones, et al. 1997. “Empathy
and Attitudes: Can Feeling for a Member of a Stigmatized Group Improve
Feelings Toward the Group?.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
72, no. 1: 105–118. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.72.1.105.
Batson, C. D., J.Chang, R. Orr, and J. Rowland. 2002. “Empathy,Attitudes,
and Action: Can Feeling for a Member of a Stigmatized Group Motivate
one to Help the Group?.” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 28, no.
12: 1656–1666.
Binder, J., H. Zagefka, R. Brown, et al. 2009. “Does Contact Reduce
Prejudice or Does Prejudice Reduce Contact? A Longitudinal Test of
the Contact Hypothesis Among Majority and Minority Groups in Three
European Countries.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 96, no.
4: 843–856. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013470.
Cairns, E., J. Kenworthy, A. Campbell, and M. Hewstone. 2006. “The
Role of In-Group Identification, Religious Group Membership and Inter-
group Conflict in Moderating In-Group and Out-Group Affect.” British
Journal of Social Psychology 45, no. 4: 701–716. https://doi.org/10.1348/
014466605X69850.
Christ, O., and U. Wagner. 2013. “Methodological Issues in the Study of
Contact: Towards a New Wave of Research.” In Advances in Intergroup
Contact, edited by G. Hodson and M. Hewstone, 233–261. London:
Psychology Press.
Davies, K., L. R. Tropp, A. Aron, T. F. Pettigrew, and S. C. Wright. 2011.
“Cross-Group Friendships and Intergroup Attitudes: A Meta-analytic
Review.” Personality and Social Psychology Review 15, no. 4: 332–351.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868311411103.
Dhont, K., A. Van Hiel, and M. Hewstone. 2014.“Changing the Ideological
Roots of Prejudice: Longitudinal Effects of Ethnic Intergroup Contact on
Social Dominance Orientation.” Group Processes & Intergroup Relations
17, no. 1: 27–44. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430213497064.
Dormann, C., and M. A. Griffin. 2015. “Optimal Time Lags in Panel
Studies.” Psychological Methods 20, no. 4: 489–505. https://doi.org/10.
1037/met0000041.
Eisinga, R., M. Grotenhuis, and B. Pelzer. 2013. “The Reliability of a
Two-Item Scale: Pearson, Cronbach,or Spearman-Brown?.” International
Journal of Public Health 58: 637–642. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00038-012-
0416-3.
Friehs, M.-T., C. Bracegirdle, N. K. Reimer, et al. 2024. “The Between-
Person and Within-Person Effects of Intergroup Contact on Out-
group Attitudes: A Multi-Context Examination.” Social Psychologi-
cal and Personality Science 15, no. 2: 125–141. https://doi.org/10.1177/
19485506231153017.
Górska, P., and N. Tausch. 2023. “Dynamic, Yet Stable: Separating
Within- and Between-Person Components of Collective Action in Support
of a Disadvantaged Outgroup and Its Antecedents.” Social Psycholog-
ical and Personality Science 14, no. 7: 875–887. https://doi.org/10.1177/
19485506221133882.
Hamaker, E. L., R. M. Kuiper, and R. P. P. P. Grasman. 2015. “A Critique of
the Cross-Lagged Panel Model.” Psychological Methods 20, no. 1: 102–116.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038889.
HM Government. 2018. Integrated Communities Strategy Green Paper.
London: HM Government. https://assets.publishing.service.gov.uk/
media/5ac490f4e5274a0b1849f8c2/Integrated_Communities_Strategy.
pdf.
Hodson, G., and M. Hewstone. 2013. Advances in Intergroup Contact.
London: Psychology Press. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203095461.
Hodson, G., and R. Meleady. 2024. “Replicating and Extending Sengupta,
et al. (2023): Contact Predicts No Within-Person Longitudinal Outgroup-
Bias Change.” American Psychologist 79, no. 3: 451–462. https://doi.org/
10.1037/amp0001210.
Hu, L., and P. M. Bentler. 1999. “Cutoff Criteria for Fit Indexes in Covari-
ance Structure Analysis: Conventional Criteria Versus New Alternatives.”
Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal 6, no. 1: 1–55.
https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118.
Hughes, J., A. Campbell, S. Lolliot, M. Hewstone, and T. Gallagher. 2013.
“Inter-Group Contact at School and Social Attitudes: Evidence From
Northern Ireland.” Oxford Review of Education 39, no. 6: 761–779. https://
doi.org/10.1080/03054985.2013.857595.
Jackson, D. L. 2003. “Revisiting Sample Size and Number of Parameter
Estimates: Some Support for the N:q Hypothesis.” Structural Equation
Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal 10, no. 1: 128–141. https://doi.org/
10.1207/S15328007SEM1001_6.
Kline, R. B. 2016. Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling,
4th ed. New York: Guilford Press.
Koschate, M., S. Oethinger, D. Kuchenbrandt, and R. Van Dick. 2012.
“Is an Outgroup Member in Need a Friend Indeed? Personal and
Task-Oriented Contact as Predictors of Intergroup Prosocial Behavior.”
European Journal of Social Psychology 42, no. 6: 717–728. https://doi.org/
10.1002/ejsp.1879.
Lemmer, G., and U. Wagner. 2015. “Can We Really Reduce Ethnic
Prejudice Outside the Lab? A Meta-analysis of Direct and Indirect Contact
Interventions.” European Journal of Social Psychology 45, no. 2: 152–168.
https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2079.
McKeown, S., and E. Cairns. 2012. “Peacemaking Youth Programmes in
Northern Ireland.” Journal of Aggression, Conflict and Peace Research 4,
no. 2: 69–75. https://doi.org/10.1108/17596591211208274.
McKeown, S., and L. K. Taylor. 2018. “Perceived Peer and School Norm
Effects on YouthAntisocial and Prosocial Behaviours Through Intergroup
Contact in Northern Ireland.” British Journal of Social Psychology 57, no.
3: 652–665. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12257.
Merrilees,C.E.,L.K.Taylor,M.Klotz,M.C.Goeke-Morey,P.Shirlow,and
E. M. Cummings. 2023. “Timing is Everything: Developmental Changes
in the Associations Between Intergroup Contact and Bias.” International
Journal of Behavioral Development 47, no. 3: 243–252. https://doi.org/10.
1177/01650254221146409.
Mulder, J. D., and E. L. Hamaker. 2021. “Three Extensions of the Random
Intercept Cross-Lagged Panel Model.” Structural Equation Modeling:
A Multidisciplinary Journal 28, no. 4: 638–648. https://doi.org/10.1080/
10705511.2020.1784738.
Muthén, L. K., and B. O. Muthén. 1998–2017. Mplus User’s Guide, 8th ed.
Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.
Paluck, E. L., S. A. Green, and D. P. Green. 2019. “The Contact Hypothesis
Re-Evaluated.” Behavioural Public Policy 3, no. 2: 129–158. https://doi.org/
10.1017/bpp.2018.25.
Pettigrew, T. F., and L. R. Tropp. 2006. “A Meta-analytic Test of Intergroup
Contact Theory.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 90, no. 5:
751–783. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.90.5.751.
Pettigrew, T. F., and L. R. Tropp. 2008. “How Does Intergroup Contact
Reduce Prejudice? Meta-analytic Tests of Three Mediators.” European
Journal of Social Psychology 38, no. 6: 922–934. https://doi.org/10.1002/
ejsp.504.
Pettigrew, T. F., and L. R. Tropp. 2011. When Groups Meet: The Dynamics
of Intergroup Contact. New York: Psychology Press.
Sengupta, N. K., N. K. Reimer, C. G. Sibley, and F. K. Barlow. 2023.
“Does Intergroup Contact Foster Solidarity With the Disadvantaged? A
Longitudinal Analysis Across 7 Years.” American Psychologist 78, no. 6:
750–760. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0001079.
Stephan, W. G. 2014. “Intergroup Anxiety: Theory, Research, and Prac-
tice.” Personality and Social Psychology Review 18, no. 3: 239–255. https://
doi.org/10.1177/1088868314530518.
15 of 16
10990992, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/ejsp.3121 by Sumedh Rao - <Shibboleth>-member@open.ac.uk , Wiley Online Library on [14/10/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Stronger Communities Partnership. 2019. Bradford for Everyone
Strategy: Building Stronger Communities Together: 2018–2023.Bradford
Council. https://bradfordforeveryone.co.uk/wp-content/uploads/2021/
11/Bradford-for-Everyone-Strategy-2018-2023.pdf.
Swart, H., M. Hewstone, O. Christ, and A. Voci. 2011. “Affective Mediators
of Intergroup Contact: A Three-Wave Longitudinal Study in South
Africa.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 101, no. 6: 1221–1238.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024450.
Tam, T., M. Hewstone, J. Kenworthy, and E. Cairns. 2009. “Intergroup
Trust in Northern Ireland.” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 35,
no. 1: 45–59. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167208325004.
Taylor, L. K., and S. McKeown. 2021. “Adolescent Outgroup Helping, Col-
lective Action, and Political Activism in a Setting of Protracted Conflict.”
International Journal of Intercultural Relations 85: 37–46. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.ijintrel.2021.09.001.
Taylor, L. K., C. E. Merrilees, M. C. Goeke-Morey, P. Shirlow, E.
Cairns, and E. M. Cummings. 2014. “Political Violence and Adolescent
Out-Group Attitudes and Prosocial Behaviors: Implications for Positive
Inter-Group Relations.” Social Development 23, no. 4: 840–859. https://doi.
org/10.1111/sode.12074.
Taylor, L. K., C. E. Merrilees, M. C. Goeke-Morey, P. Shirlow, and E.
M. Cummings. 2016. “Trajectories of Adolescent Aggression and Family
Cohesion: The Potential to Perpetuate or Ameliorate Political Conflict.”
Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology 45, no. 2: 114–128. https://
doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2014.945213.
The Executive Office. 2013. Together: Building a United Community
Strategy (TBUC). Belfast: The Execute Office. https://www.
executiveoffice-ni.gov.uk/sites/default/files/publications/ofmdfm_dev/
together-building-a- united-community-strategy.pdf.
Turner, R. N., K. West, and Z. Christie. 2013. “Out-Group Trust, Inter-
group Anxiety, and Out-Group Attitude as Mediators of the Effect of
Imagined Intergroup Contact on Intergroup Behavioral Tendencies.”
Journal of Applied Social Psychology 43, no. Suppl no. 2: E196–E205.
https://doi.org/10.1111/jasp.12019.
Waddington, D. P. 2010. “Applying the Flashpoints Model of Public
Disorder to the 2001 Bradford Riot.” The British Journal of Criminology
50, no. 2: 342–359. https://doi.org/10.1093/bjc/azp082.
Wölfer, R., K. Schmid, M. Hewstone, and M. Van Zalk. 2016. “Develop-
mental Dynamics of Intergroup Contact and Intergroup Attitudes: Long-
Term Effects in Adolescence and Early Adulthood.” Child Development
87, no. 5: 1466–1478. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12598.
16 of 16 European Journal of Social Psychology,2024
10990992, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/ejsp.3121 by Sumedh Rao - <Shibboleth>-member@open.ac.uk , Wiley Online Library on [14/10/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License