ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

This article brings together research from the fields of pain science and Alexander Technique (AT) to investigate the mechanisms by which AT helps reduce pain. AT is a cognitive embodiment practice and a method for intentionally altering habitual postural behavior. Studies show that AT helps with various kinds of pain, although the mechanisms of pain reduction are currently not well understood. Advances in pain science may give insight into how this occurs. Modern interventions with efficacy for improving pain and function are consistent with active approaches within kinesiology. They also share similarities with AT and may have common mechanisms such as learning, mind–body engagement, normalization of sensorimotor function, improvement of psychological factors, and self-efficacy, as well as nonspecific treatment effects. AT likely has additional unique mechanisms, including normalization of muscle tone, neuronal excitability, and tissue loading, as well as alterations to body schema, attention redirection, and reduction in overall reactivity.
Modern Pain Science and Alexander Technique:
How Might Alexander Technique Reduce Pain?
Mari Hodges,
1
Rajal G. Cohen,
2
and Timothy W. Cacciatore
3
1
Medicine and Health, The University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, USA;
2
Department of Psychology & Communication, University of Idaho, Moscow, ID, USA;
3
University of College London, Cheltenham, United Kingdom
This article brings together research from the elds of pain science and Alexander Technique (AT) to investigate the mechanisms
by which AT helps reduce pain. AT is a cognitive embodiment practice and a method for intentionally altering habitual postural
behavior. Studies show that AT helps with various kinds of pain, although the mechanisms of pain reduction are currently
not well understood. Advances in pain science may give insight into how this occurs. Modern interventions with efcacy for
improving pain and function are consistent with active approaches within kinesiology. They also share similarities with AT and
may have common mechanisms such as learning, mindbody engagement, normalization of sensorimotor function, improvement
of psychological factors, and self-efcacy, as well as nonspecic treatment effects. AT likely has additional unique mechanisms,
including normalization of muscle tone, neuronal excitability, and tissue loading, as well as alterations to body schema, attention
redirection, and reduction in overall reactivity.
Keywords:embodiment, mindfulness, biopsychosocial, mindbody, mechanism, sensorimotor
Understanding the mechanisms of pain, how it affects motor
behavior, and how to manage or alleviate pain through exercise,
movement reeducation, and other interventions is a central focus of
the eld of kinesiology. Alexander Technique (AT) is one interven-
tion that has received attention recently in kinesiology (Cacciatore
et al., 2020;Woods et al., 2020), primarily for its potential to improve
health and performance (Cacciatore et al., 2014;Cohen et al., 2015;
Hameletal.,2016;Johnson & Cohen, 2023;Kinsey et al., 2021;
ONeilletal.,2015;Woodman & Moore, 2012) and, more recently,
in regard to pain. AT has been identied as one of the complementary
and alternative approaches to movement education and important
within the eld of kinesiology for more fully comprehending the
integrated nature of mind and body in human physical activity
(Anderson, 2020). This perspective is consistent with modern pain
researchersview that pain is a whole-person experience that dees
mentalphysical categorization and that the mindbody dualism
traditionally emphasized by Western medicine has limited utility for
understanding pain (Crowley-Matoka et al., 2009).
Studies show that the AT helps with various kinds of muscu-
loskeletal pain (Becker et al., 2021;Little et al., 2022;Little et al.,
2008;MacPherson et al., 2015;Preece et al., 2016). Many people
come to the AT to resolve a pain condition (Eldred et al., 2015).
Two major randomized clinical trials showed reductions in long-
term back and neck pain after a course of lessons, and one smaller
trial showed reductions in chronic knee pain. These positive results
occurred even though the AT does not typically target pain directly,
instead focusing on improving neuromuscular patterns involved in
activities of daily life.
But how and why does the AT reduce pain? This article aims to
explore these questions. Pain is complex, and modern pain science
has begun to shed light on various mechanisms behind pain and its
alleviation. Recent advances in pain science may help us to
understand ways that the AT can help to reduce pain.
Over the past few decades, there has been a substantial shift in
the eld of pain science that addresses many of the limitations of
past understanding. To clarify the signicance of this shift for the
AT, we will review what has previously been the mainstream
model of painreferred to as the biomedical modelalong with
its shortcomings. We will then describe a contemporary model of
pain that includes previously unrecognized factors and mechan-
isms of both acute and chronic pain.
1
Finally, we will theorize how
the AT ts into this new framework.
Pain is incredibly diverse, arising from a wide range of
conditions, from a paper cut to a broken bone to bromyalgia
to cancer, and there are multiple aspects of the pain experience,
including sensation, emotion, and cognition. While there are broad
ranges of both contributing factors and conditions, and each pain
experience is highly individual, pain emerges from a common
protective system. This system includes peripheral nerves, the
spinal cord, and the brainall within a person within an environ-
mentand all of which participate in the perception of pain.
AT
AT is a cognitive embodiment practice and a method for inten-
tionally self-regulating and altering habitual postural and motor
behavior (Alexander, 1932). It is a nonexercise approach that
involves nonjudgmental, nonreactive self-observation and monitor-
ing. One of the aims of AT is to reduce reactivity during activity to
promote efcient and healthy functioning and improved perfor-
mance of tasks from the mundane to the complex. Using hands-on
and/or verbal guidance combined with sophisticated observation
(Tinbergen, 1973), a teacher assists the learner in developing
proprioceptive, kinesthetic, and spatial awareness, in addition to
awareness of unhelpful habitual reactions and neuromuscular pat-
terns. Learners practice inhibitory and attentional skills to promote
an overall organization of the body in relation to surrounding space
Cohen https://orcid.org/0000-0001-6691-2561
Cacciatore https://orcid.org/0009-0005-6732-790X
Hodges (mari.hodges@gmail.com) is corresponding author, https://orcid.org/
0009-0007-1034-9184
1
Kinesiology Review, (Ahead of Print)
https://doi.org/10.1123/kr.2024-0035
© 2024 Human Kinetics, Inc. SCHOLARLY REVIEW
First Published Online: Oct. 8, 2024
that facilitates adaptability of muscle tone (Cohen et al., 2015).
These skills are practiced in quiet posture and during exploration of
the processes involved in attaining a simple goal, such as standing
from a chair, and then practiced in activities of daily life. Instruction
also typically includes basic anatomy.
AT has been shown to reduce pain (Becker et al., 2021;Little
et al., 2008,2022;MacPherson et al., 2015;Preece et al., 2016) and
improve dynamic modulation of postural tone (Becker et al., 2021;
Cacciatore et al., 2011;Cohen et al., 2015), postural sway (Cohen
& Hocketstaller, 2023;Gleeson et al., 2015), and movement
(Cacciatore et al., 2014;Cohen et al., 2015;Davies, 2020;
Glover et al., 2018;Hamel et al., 2016;Johnson & Cohen,
2023;ONeill et al., 2015;Preece et al., 2016). AT has also been
found to affect psychological outcomes such as anxiety, executive
inhibition, self-efcacy, self-management, psychological well-
being, and a sense of mindbody integration (Davies, 2020;
Gross et al., 2022;Hanefeld et al., 2021;Kinsey et al., 2021;
Klein et al., 2014;McClean et al., 2015;Woodman et al., 2018).
The Biomedical Model
and Its Shortcomings
The biomedical model posits that pain is a direct indication of tissue
damage and that the underlying pathology must be treated to reduce
pain. This idea appeals to our common sensepain feels like
something is damaged. In fact, mainstream Western medicine for
centuries has presumed that there is an underlying anatomical cause
to pain and that this relationship is proportional: more pain means
more damage. This presumption also implies that pain without
evidence of an anatomical source is not real,but rather imagined
or caused by psychological problems. Additionally, it ignores
evidence of the involvement of the nervous system, including the
brain, in the experience of pain. This perspective, while deeply
ingrained in todays approaches to pain, is limited in its utility for
understanding and addressing pain (Cohen & Quintner, 2012).
To understand these limitations, let us consider some familiar
examples. Many of us may have experienced a minoryet incredi-
bly painfulinjury, such as a stubbed toe or tension headache. In
these cases, the degree of damage is far out of proportion with the
severity of the pain. In other conditions such as chronic back pain
(Hartvigsen et al., 2018), bromyalgia (Pinto et al., 2023), or chronic
regional pain syndrome (Marinus et al., 2011), severe pain may
occur without any identiable structural pathology. On the other
hand, athletes injured on the eld may only notice the injury after the
game is over. Also, people with signicant spinal abnormalities such
as disc degeneration or hernia are often pain-free or show only weak
pain correlation with the degree of abnormality (Brinjikji et al.,
2015). Likewise with rotator cuff tears, a large percentage of people
with tears have no pain (Sher et al., 1995). In fact, recent research has
found that tissue damage is not predictive of pain severity. Pain is
such a poor indicator of the state of tissues that leading medical
bodies such as the American College of Radiology (American
College of Radiology, 2024;Hall et al., 2021) and National Institute
for Health and Care Excellence (National Institute for Health and
Care Excellence, 2016) now recommend against early scans for
people with back pain in the absence of red ags.
A Modern Understanding of Pain
There is now indisputable evidence that pain is not an unambiguous
consequence of tissue damage but rather a multifactorial and
multidimensional experience. In addition to physical sensation,
the experience of pain entails affective, cognitive, and behavioral
aspects. For example, pain involves unpleasant feelings, interpre-
tations, and protection of a body part. When studying pain,
researchers point not just to pain intensity but also to pain-related
fear and anxiety, mood disruptions, interference by pain in life
activities, and functioning. Research in recent decades has also
revealed previously unrecognized factors besides tissue damage
that inuence pain (Raja et al., 2020). Biological, psychological,
and social factors interact with lived experience to create a unique
pain experience for every individual and every incident
(Karunamuni et al., 2021). While information from the body is
of course important, the brain uses all the information it has
available to determine whether the person or body part is in danger
and in need of protection. Information from the body can also be
amplied or driven by nervous system processes that will be
discussed below.
Pain as Protection
Many researchers concur that pain can be better understood when
viewed as one of the bodys protective systems (Moseley, 2007;
Wallwork et al., 2016). Pain promotes a variety of protective
behaviors to address threats to bodily integrity and increase the
chances of survival, like withdrawing of a limb, guarding, resting,
and seeking help. Protective responses include varying degrees of
sensitization of the peripheral and central nervous systems, motor
changes, and psychosocial behavior, which themselves inuence
the experience of pain. The evolutionary advantage of pain is lost,
however, when pain endures for long periods of time. When this
happens, there is much more at play than tissue damage, and the
relationship between tissue damage and pain becomes more
tenuous.
Nociception
Nociception refers to processing within the nervous system of input
from a stimulus that damages, or has the immediate potential to
damage, the body. Nociception is occurring all the time and is
neither necessary nor sufcient for pain, meaning that nociceptive
input is merely one factor that may contribute to the experience of
pain (Wall, 1979). That experience can be modied at multiple
levels of the nervous system (Grace et al., 2014).
Sensitization
Sensitization is one mechanism by which increased pain occurs in
the absence of tissue damage, inammation, or neural lesion
(Woolf, 2011). Sensitization refers to a reduced pain threshold
or a magnied response of the nervous system to stimuli and
heightened perception of pain. This type of plasticity involves
numerous mechanisms, including increased responsiveness of
peripheral nociceptors and spinal cord neurons, expanded receptive
eld of nociceptive neurons in the spinal cord, recruitment of
increasing numbers of spinal cord neurons that encode both
noxious and innocuous stimuli (Coghill, 2020), and altered facili-
tation and inhibition of synaptic transmission (Basbaum et al.,
2009).
Sensitization adds extra protection by changing the way
signals are processed in the nervous system. For example, a
sunburn increases the sensitivity of the nervous system such that
a light brushing of the skin or a shower can be painful despite lack
of harm. The heightened sensitivity contributes to preventing future
2HODGES, COHEN, AND CACCIATORE
(Ahead of Print)
potentially damaging behavior. While the increased tenderness
from a sunburn resolves in a matter of days, sustained changes
throughout the nervous system can occur and become an important
contributor to persistent pain.
Sensitivity can become problematic when it becomes overly
protective (Luo et al., 2014), as in the case of osteoarthritis or
chronic back pain. In chronic pain conditions in particular,
increased sensitivity can act to perpetuate pain independent of
the state of the tissues (Basbaum et al., 2009). Osteoarthritis is an
example of a condition in which a sensitized nervous system
contributes to joint pain. Evidence for this sensitization includes
greater sensitivity to pain in areas remote from the painful joint as
well as the weak correlation between structural damage and pain
(Gwilym et al., 2009;Horga et al., 2020;Lluch et al., 2014).
Sensitization may also contribute to, or become a driver of, other
pain conditions such as back pain, temporomandibular joint
disorder, headaches, and phantom limb pain even without corre-
sponding damage in the tissues (Giummarra et al., 2007;Woolf,
2011).
Sensorimotor Disruptions
A wide range of sensorimotor disruptions are also associated with
chronic pain. These include changes in the brains representations
of body parts and other widespread changes in the brain (Moseley
& Flor, 2012). Changes occur, for example, in relation to height-
ened attention (Clauwaert et al., 2018), inhibition (Staud, 2012),
and body schema (Bray & Moseley, 2011;Martinez et al., 2018;
Moseley & Flor, 2012). For example, a reorganized lumbar spine
representation in brain regions involved in sensory processing and
motor control has been observed in people with low back pain
(Goossens et al., 2018). Altered postural control and impaired
balance have also been observed in people with chronic neck pain
(Ruhe et al., 2011). People with persistent low back pain exhibit
reduced lumbar proprioception and have increased postural sway.
Altered position sense may contribute to development and perpet-
uation of back pain (Goossens et al., 2018). Likewise, people with
bromyalgia have been shown to have postural control decits
(Trevisan et al., 2017), which have been linked to impaired cervical
joint position sense (Reddy et al., 2022).
It is also clear that pain is associated with substantial and
diverse changes in the distribution of postural muscle activity and
movement coordination (Hodges & Tucker, 2011;van Dieen et al.,
2019). One such tendency is an overactivation of supercial
muscles with concurrent deactivation of deeper muscles in people
with chronic neck (Jull & Falla, 2016;Jull et al., 2008) and back
pain (Hodges & Danneels, 2019;van Dieen et al., 2019). However,
in general, the relationship between pain and motor control has
been tricky to study because of the highly individual nature of
motor changes to pain (Hodges & Danneels, 2019;van Dieen
et al., 2019).
While much remains to be learned about the relationship
between brain changes and pain, there is now growing evidence
that sensorimotor disruptions actually predict or even contribute to
chronic pain (Alshehri et al., 2024;Brumagne et al., 2019;Tanaka
et al., 2021). There is also some evidence that pain-related
plasticity can be reversed. For example, cognitive behavioral
therapy for chronic pain (Lazaridou et al., 2017;Seminowicz
et al., 2013) and hip arthroplasty for painful hip osteoarthritis
(Gwilym et al., 2010) have both been shown to reverse brain
changes. Moreover, these changes were correlated with improve-
ment in peoples pain. This supports the idea that plasticity plays a
role in perpetuating pain.
Psychosocial Factors
There is substantial evidence that psychological and social factors
are closely linked to pain (Linton & Shaw, 2011;Michaelides &
Zis, 2019). For example, people with posttraumatic stress disorder
or signicant adverse experiences in childhood are at greater risk of
increased pain sensitivity and chronic pain (Meints & Edwards,
2018). Likewise, distress, fear, expectations, and beliefs about back
pain strongly inuence pain intensity and the likelihood of devel-
oping back pain-related disability (Hartvigsen et al., 2018). In fact,
emotions inuence processing of noxious stimuli (such as injury or
temperature extremes), reex withdrawal from a noxious stimulus,
and pain perception (Rhudy et al., 2005). Also, the contextual
factors around an injury such as a hostile work environment, poor
sleep, or concurrent health issues may heighten a sense of threat
and increase sensitivity to pain (Moseley & Arntz, 2007;Timmers
et al., 2019). Contextual factors surrounding a treatment, including
practitionersnegative beliefs, can induce a nocebo effect (i.e.,
physiological changes brought about by expectations of negative
outcomes; Rossettini et al., 2018). The social context within which
an injury occurs and sociodemographic factors such as education
level and minority status have also been shown to inuence pain
and the transition to chronic pain (Booher, 2019;Borsook et al.,
2018). Even other peoples responses to the pain can inuence it
(Nees et al., 2022). Often, these threatening contextual factors are
broad and outside of conscious awareness. Essentially, anything
that can inuence the brains evaluation of threat can inuence pain
(Moseley & Flor, 2012).
Novel Interventions
Overwhelming evidence of nervous system changes and psycho-
social inuences on pain, as well as reconceptualization of pain as
protective rather than indicative of damage, has led to a shift in the
understanding and treatment of pain. Newer interventions for pain
and related disability based on this shift take an active approach
toward treatment, including behavior change and sensorimotor
retraining, a more effective approach that is consistent with AT
and other large-scale approaches in kinesiology to promote health,
such as the American College of Sports MedicinesExercise is
Medicinecampaign (Thompson et al., 2020).
One new intervention for chronic disabling low back pain is
cognitive functional therapy, which consists of rst identifying the
many factors contributing to each individuals pain and disability,
including movement patterns, posture, cognition, emotion, behav-
ior, social and lifestyle related aspects (Kent et al., 2023). Identi-
cation of individual factors is followed by tailored education about
pain. Finally, individuals learn relaxation techniques and active
strategies for gradually changing thoughts, posture, movement, and
lifestyle habits. This approach is designed to engage the individual
actively in management and recovery rather than promoting a
passive attitude toward treatment, as is typical of a biomedical
approach. A recent large trial (Kent et al., 2023) showed that
cognitive functional therapy led to strikingly large and sustained
reductions in pain and disability, in contrast with older, more
conventional interventions for chronic musculoskeletal pain, which
typically show short-term, but not long-term, reductions in pain.
These results are similar to those found by the large study on AT for
chronic back pain (Little et al., 2008), which also found a substan-
tial sustained reduction in pain and disability.
Another new intervention involves graded sensorimotor retrain-
ing that addresses the altered pain processing that disrupts sensori-
motor ability (Bagg et al., 2022). This psychophysical intervention
A MODERN PAIN SCIENCE LENS ON ALEXANDER TECHNIQUE 3
(Ahead of Print)
was designed to alter how people think about their body in pain,
how they process sensory information, and how they move. After
learning about pain, participants engaged in various activities
involving proprioception and active movement, including tactile
acuity tasks, observation and mental rehearsal of different body
congurations, and a gradual progression to physical movement.
These activities are intended to reestablish nonprotective patterns of
neural activity and movement (Wallwork et al., 2016). A recent large
trial found that graded sensorimotor training led to modest and
sustained improvements in low back pain (Bagg et al., 2022).
As a result of the new understanding of how closely linked
psychological experience is to pain, the mind is now considered a
central tool to address pain. This is in stark contrast to the
biomedical model and to simple stretching and strengthening based
approaches. It also paves the way for interventions that incorporate
a mental component, like the AT.
How Might Alexander Technique
Reduce Pain?
Advances in pain science may shed light on how the AT acts to
reduce pain. AT mechanisms may overlap with the mechanisms of
newer interventions that are supported by modern pain science, as
well as many of the nonspecic effects of older and newer
interventions. In addition, there may also be unique mechanisms
specic to the AT that reduce pain. Below, we provide descriptions
of some aspects of an AT lesson that may play a role in the pain
reduction observed with AT.
Specic Effects
Touch
In addition to nonspecic benets of touch, touch in the AT lesson
is also specically used both to assess and to invite change in the
postural state of the student. AT touch draws attention to a region
while encouraging nonreactivity, thereby providing feedback that
promotes changes in tension. For example, a teacher might touch a
students neck and back, helping to redistribute postural tone by
encouraging lengthening of the students spine or widening of the
back, depending on the students presentation. A teacher might also
gently move a students head to promote an adaptive state of tone.
Rather than imposing a particular position, AT touch invites
adaptive postural tone. This is important because of the highly
individual nature of posture and motor control in relation to pain
(van Dieen et al., 2019).
MindBody Engagement
A teacher will use verbal and other means to engage the students
mind in relation to their body and space. The concept of mindbody
unity behind this guidance is similar to the unied person perspec-
tive advocated by leading pain researchers (Moseley, 2019). The
teacher may ask the student to notice specic parts of their body, for
example their neck or feet, without judgment or attempt to change.
This kind of accepting attitude has been shown to be associated
with movement improvements and other pain-related outcomes
(Hughes et al., 2017;Vowles et al., 2007;Wetherell et al., 2011).
The teacher may then cue the student to embody qualities such
as uidity, support, or freedom or to encourage a certain spatial
relationship within or between body segments, such as the length
and width of their back. Such embodied cues are referred to by AT
practitioners as directing.For example, a cue such as let the
back lengthen and widenmay encourage a student to reimagine
the conguration of the back, promoting changes in muscle tone
and overall postural state. The student is specically asked to
thinkthe cue and not to doit, consistent with the idea that AT
promotes remapping rather than repositioning. There is evidence
that promoting embodiment of particular physical characteristics
like size and strength is benecial for pain. For example, one pilot
study showed that embodiment of a strong, wide back led to a
reduction in back pain (Nishigami et al., 2019). In a study of
symptomatic knee osteoarthritis, illusory resizing of the knee led to
signicant reductions in knee pain (Stanton et al., 2018).
AT directing, guided by feedback from a teacher, may engage
and normalize body schema (Cacciatore et al., 2020). This proce-
dure may be similar to interventions involving tactile acuity
training and mental judgments of observed body positions, which
are also thought to address disturbed body schema (Bray &
Moseley, 2011;Moseley & Flor, 2012). Such tasks could act to
reengage and recruit disengaged body regions that have dropped
outof working body schema with the presence of pain (Moseley &
Flor, 2012). AT lessons often involve directing while performing a
functional task such as walking or bending. This way of performing
the task integrates multiple senses and thus may improve sensori-
motor disruptions, proprioception, and spatial acuityall of which
are relevant to pain (Moseley & Flor, 2012;Wallwork et al., 2016).
Changes in Postural Tone
Postural tone refers to background muscle activity across body
regions that enables both stability and movement in gravity. Some
studies suggest that AT inuences the distribution of postural tone.
For example, one study found that AT teachers have greater
adaptivity in their axial muscles than age-matched healthy controls
(Cacciatore et al., 2011). Another study showed that even a few
minutes of instruction in an AT-based approach to posture can
increase this adaptivity (Cohen et al., 2015).
AT lessons often involve activities or procedures that can be
viewed as experiments. These experimentsallow the teacher and
the student to observe how the students mental changes affect their
postural tone and the activity itself. For instance, it is common for a
teacher to guide a student from sitting to standing in a slow way that
highlights the difcult-to-prevent tendency to use momentum and
lurch forward at seat-off. The teacher helps the student alter their
postural state, for instance, while the student is seated, through
encouraging them to release tension and mentally engage with the
conguration of their body. Any resulting changes in effort and
smoothness can then be observed as the student rises from the chair.
One study found that AT teachers are able to do this task more
smoothly than age-matched controls; biomechanical modeling
indicated that the difference could be explained by the greater
adaptability of the AT teacherspostural tone (Cacciatore
et al., 2014).
Changes in postural state brought about by AT are likely to
include changes in the excitability of neural circuits that regulate
tone (Cacciatore et al., 2011;Gurnkel et al., 2006). These same
circuits have been hypothesized to underlie pain-related motor
disruption (Hodges & Tucker, 2011). Changes in postural state
may have various specic effects that relate to pain, including
(a) changing the loading on painful regions, (b) normalizing
sensorimotor function (Meier et al., 2019), (c) changes in excit-
ability (Hodges & Smeets, 2015), and (d) reducing protection from
pain (Hodges & Tucker, 2011). These changes in postural tone and
stiffness observed following AT lessons could also account for
reported reductions in knee and neck pain (Becker et al., 2021;
4HODGES, COHEN, AND CACCIATORE
(Ahead of Print)
Preece et al., 2016), perhaps via changes in excitability. In particu-
lar, the lower stiffness and higher adaptivity of muscle tone from
the AT (Cacciatore et al., 2011) could act to decrease prolonged
static or inappropriate tissue loading (Hodges & Smeets, 2015).
Self-Efcacy and Overcoming Fear Avoidance
ATsinuence on pain is likely mediated by improvements in self-
efcacy and reductions in fear avoidance. A large, randomized,
controlled trial on AT for back pain (Alexander technique lessons,
exercise, and massage) found signicant reductions in disability
and fear avoidance (Little et al., 2008). Development of certain
cognitive skills (Laura & Jones, 2020;Williams et al., 2020) can
reduce fear and anxiety (Kinsey et al., 2021;Klein et al., 2014) and
contribute to a greater sense of control over pain, all of which are
strongly correlated with pain reduction (Edwards et al., 2016).
Through AT lessons, the student learns not to fear previously
feared movements (Kinsey et al., 2021); disconrmation of the
expectation of pain or injury enhances learning that leads to long-
term pain reduction (Kube et al., 2022). For example, achieving a
slow, smooth sit-to-stand movement without lurching provides the
student with the opportunity to reappraise the degree of effort
required and to correct inaccurate predictions of harm when
performing the movement (Vlaeyen & Linton, 2000).
Improved movement performance and increased condence
(Kinsey et al., 2021) also lead to an increased sense of self-efcacy
(the belief in ones ability to engage in activities despite pain). Self-
efcacy is strongly correlated with pain reduction and reduced risk
of disability due to pain (Asghari & Nicholas, 2001). Studies on the
AT for neck pain found that increased self-efcacy after AT lessons
was linked to reduced neck pain (Becker et al., 2021;Woodman
et al., 2018).
Attention and Reactivity
There may be other ways in which the AT reduces pain, related to
attention and reactivity. For example, AT teachers often instruct a
student to attend more broadly than to the site of their pain. Aside
from distraction, this manner of intentionally redirecting attention
and expanding awareness is relevant to pain processing (Torta
et al., 2017). Movement coaching methods that direct attention
toward nonprovocative aspects of motion by prioritizing other
senses have been shown to reduce pain (Wand et al., 2023). Brain
imaging shows that diverting attention to cognitive tasks unrelated
to pain activates pain inhibitory systems (Torta et al., 2017). In
general, mindful awareness is associated with lower pain and
reactivity (Wand et al., 2023;Zeidan et al., 2012). Finally, the
AT could reduce heightened pain sensitivity by decreasing overall
reactivity. There is some evidence that the AT improves executive
inhibition (Gross et al., 2022), and this regulation of general
reactivity may act to decrease pain sensitivity (Bjekic et al., 2018).
Other Psychological Effects
Balance condence is negatively correlated with pain (Stubbs et al.,
2014), as is a sense of safety (Zillig et al., 2023). AT may promote
feelings of safety through improvements in balance (Batson &
Barker, 2008;Cacciatore et al., 2005;Cohen et al., 2020;Cohen &
Hocketstaller, 2023), as well as an increased sense of control
(Kinsey et al., 2021), empowerment, and self-care skills (Glover
et al., 2018;Kinsey et al., 2021;McClean et al., 2015). In essence,
these types of outcomes are hypothesized to reduce overprotection
(Caneiro et al., 2022). Other studies of the AT have found that it
increases psychological well-being, optimism, and condence.
Furthermore, a mixed methods study found that after AT lessons,
individuals changed their relationship with pain and their pain
management (McClean et al., 2015). All these psychological
factors are known to positively inuence pain.
Nonspecic Effects
One of the central features of this individualized interaction is the
use of touch by the teacher. Earlier, we addressed some specic
aspects of AT touch that may contribute to reducing pain. In
addition, touch has many nonspecic effects that are benecial
for pain. For example, it can suppress pain-related sensory input
(Mancini et al., 2015) while promoting reorganization of body
representations in the brain (Flor et al., 2001), feelings of safety and
relaxation, and a positive therapeutic relationship (Geri et al., 2019;
Jones & Glover, 2014).
There are other likely nonspecic contributors to ATs re-
ported benets. For instance, the fact that AT is taught by a highly
trained teacher creates expectations of improvement, which can
improve pain outcomes (Atlas & Wager, 2012;Maliet et al.,
2019). AT is taught in an individualized manner with importance
placed on an empathetic, caring relationship between teacher and
student. This kind of individualized care has been shown to be
important when addressing persistent pain (Lin et al., 2020;Wernli
et al., 2022), and a therapeutic relationship characterized by
empathy and positive communication enhances outcomes
(Doringer et al., 2013;Ferreira et al., 2013). While approach and
content vary widely, a teacher generally provides education on the
principles of AT, including awareness of habitual physical and
mental patterns, whole-body organization, and the psychomotor
processes involved in changing these. Education in and of itself can
be therapeutic (Cook, 2013), particularly when combined with
movement (Wallis & Taylor, 2011). The student is also actively
involved in AT lessons, initially through both the decision to take
lessons even though they are generally not covered by health
insurance and later through engagement in exploration of posture,
movement, and thought processes. While passive approaches in
which something is done to the individual tend to have poorer
outcomes (Covic et al., 2000), active participation that depends on
engagement by the individual has been demonstrated to be bene-
cial long-term for pain-related improvements (Blyth et al., 2005;
Nicholas et al., 2012).
Future Research
Further research to understand the multisystem mechanisms by
which AT reduces pain will help practitioners optimize pain care
and predict which individuals or subgroups are likely to benet
from AT. Research on AT is challenging due to heterogeneity of
AT teaching, the inherent difculty of studying postural mechan-
isms, and difculties in obtaining funding. Because trials of
participative mindbody therapies such as AT face unique chal-
lenges to avoid bias, measures must be taken to increase the rigor of
studies, including clear protocols, appropriate comparison inter-
ventions to distinguish nonspecic effects and limit confounding
effects, and standardization of reporting (Mehling et al., 2005). We
propose that postural tone and neural excitability are fruitful areas
of investigation into the relationship between AT and pain. We
encourage future researchers to investigate pain-related outcomes
using standardized, validated self-report tools including pressure
point threshold, self-efcacy, fear of movement, anxiety, and
depression; objective tests of proprioceptive acuity; function-
A MODERN PAIN SCIENCE LENS ON ALEXANDER TECHNIQUE 5
(Ahead of Print)
related outcomes such as sleep and sit to stand; and tests of central
nervous system-related mechanisms using conditioned pain mod-
ulation, brain imaging, and laterality judgments.
Conclusion
While there is evidence that Alexander Technique (AT) reduces
pain, the mechanisms by which this occurs are currently not well
understood. However, advances in pain science may shed light
on how AT reduces pain. We have seen that there is little direct
correlation between tissue damage and pain and that pain is the
action of a protective system that includes neural, biological, and
psychological mechanisms. Chronic pain is closely intertwined
with the plasticity of these systems. Modern interventions stem-
ming from the new understandings around pain show increased
efcacy for improving pain and function compared with older
interventions based on a simple biomechanical model. Many of
these new interventions share similarities with AT and are relevant
to the eld of kinesiology, with its aim of understanding the
mechanisms underlying movement in relation to pain and active
approach to treatment. They may also have common mechanisms,
such as learning, normalization of sensorimotor function, and
improvement of psychological factors. AT likely has other unique
mechanisms that relate to its pedagogy, including sensorimotor
changes related to normalizing muscle tone, neuronal excitability,
and tissue loading, as well as alterations to body schema and
reducing overall reactivity.
Notes
1. Chronic pain is usually dened as pain lasting longer than 3 months.
Acknowledgments
We are grateful to Dr. Patrick Johnson for his assistance in providing
feedback and helpful comments on drafts of this article. Conicts of
Interest: Hodges teaches Alexander Technique in private practice and has
received fees for presentations on Alexander Technique and on pain.
Cohen has received honoraria and travel-expense reimbursement for
presenting on pain. Cacciatore has received fees for presentations on the
Alexander Technique and on pain.
References
Alexander, F.M. (1932). Use of the self. Integral Press.
Alshehri, M.A., van den Hoorn, W., Klyne, D.M., van Dieen, J.H.,
Cholewicki, J., & Hodges, P.W. (2024). Poor lumbar spine coordi-
nation in acute low back pain predicts persistent long-term pain and
disability. European Spine Journal.https://doi.org/10.1007/s00586-
024-08205-w
American College of Radiology. (2024). Low back pain. Retrieved 5/11/24
from https://gravitas.acr.org/ACPortal/GetDataForOneScenario?
senarioId=5618
Anderson, D.I. (2020). Re-education: What can complementary and
alternative approaches to movement education teach kinesiology?
Kinesiology Review, 9(3), 181189. https://doi.org/10.1123/kr.2020-
0027
Asghari, A., & Nicholas, M.K. (2001). Pain self-efcacy beliefs and pain
behaviour. A prospective study. Pain, 94,85100. https://doi.org/10.
1016/S0304-3959(01)00344-X
Atlas, L.Y., & Wager, T.D. (2012). How expectations shape pain. Neuro-
science Letters, 520(2), 140148. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.
2012.03.039
Bagg, M.K., Wand, B.M., Cashin, A.G., Lee, H., Hubscher, M., Stanton,
T.R., OConnell, N.E., OHagan, E.T., Rizzo, R.R.N., Wewege,
M.A., Rabey, M., Goodall, S., Saing, S., Lo, S.N., Luomajoki, H.,
Herbert, R.D., Maher, C.G., Moseley, G.L., & McAuley, J.H. (2022).
Effect of graded sensorimotor retraining on pain intensity in patients
with chronic low back pain: A randomized clinical trial. Journal of
American Medical Association, 328(5), 430439. https://doi.org/10.
1001/jama.2022.9930
Basbaum, A.I., Bautista, D.M., Scherrer, G., & Julius, D. (2009). Cellular
and molecular mechanisms of pain. Cell, 139(2), 267284. https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2009.09.028
Batson, G., & Barker, S. (2008). Feasibility of group delivery of the
alexander technique on balance in the community-dwelling elderly:
Preliminary ndings. Activities, Adaptation & Aging, 32(2), 103119.
https://doi.org/10.1080/01924780802073005
Becker, J.J., McIsaac, T.L., Copeland, S.L., & Cohen, R.G. (2021).
Alexander Technique vs. targeted exercise for neck painA prelim-
inary comparison. Applied Sciences, 11(10), Article 640. https://doi.
org/10.3390/app11104640
Bjekic, J., Zivanovic, M., Puric, D., Oosterman, J.M., & Filipovic, S.R.
(2018). Pain and executive functions: A unique relationship be-
tween Stroop task and experimentally induced pain. Psychological
Research, 82(3), 580589. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-016-
0838-2
Blyth, F.M., March, L.M., Nicholas, M.K., & Cousins, M.J. (2005). Self-
management of chronic pain: A population-based study. Pain, 113(3),
285292. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2004.12.004
Booher, L. (2019). The impact of low socioeconomic status in adults with
chronic pain: An integrative review. Orthopaedic Nursing, 38(6),
381389. https://doi.org/10.1097/NOR.0000000000000620
Borsook, D., Youssef, A.M., Simons, L., Elman, I., & Eccleston, C.
(2018). When pain gets stuck: The evolution of pain chronication
and treatment resistance. Pain, 159(12), 24212436. https://doi.org/
10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001401
Bray, H., & Moseley, G.L. (2011). Disrupted working body schema of the
trunk in people with back pain. British Journal of Sports Medicine,
45(3), 168173. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsm.2009.061978
Brinjikji, W., Luetmer, P.H., Comstock, B., Bresnahan, B.W., Chen, L.E.,
Deyo, R.A., Halabi, S., Turner, J.A., Avins, A.L., James, K., Wald,
J.T., Kallmes, D.F., & Jarvik, J.G. (2015). Systematic literature
review of imaging features of spinal degeneration in asymptomatic
populations. American Journal of Neuroradiology, 36(4), 811816.
https://doi.org/10.3174/ajnr.A4173
Brumagne, S., Diers, M., Danneels, L., Moseley, G.L., & Hodges, P.W.
(2019). Neuroplasticity of sensorimotor control in low back pain.
Journal of Orthopaedic and Sports Physical Therapy, 49(6), 402
414. https://doi.org/10.2519/jospt.2019.8489
Cacciatore, T.W., Gurnkel, V.S., Horak, F.B., Cordo, P.J., & Ames, K.E.
(2011). Increased dynamic regulation of postural tone through Alex-
ander Technique training. Human Movement Science, 30(1), 7489.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2010.10.002
Cacciatore, T.W., Horak, F.B., & Henry, S.M. (2005). Improvement in
automatic postural coordination following Alexander Technique
lessons in a person with low back pain. Physical Therapy, 85(6),
565578.
Cacciatore, T.W., Johnson, P.M., & Cohen, R.G. (2020). Potential me-
chanisms of the alexander technique: Toward a comprehensive
neurophysiological model. Kinesiology Review, 9(3), 199213.
https://doi.org/10.1123/kr.2020-0026
6HODGES, COHEN, AND CACCIATORE
(Ahead of Print)
Cacciatore, T.W., Mian, O.S., Peters, A., & Day, B.L. (2014). Neuro-
mechanical interference of posture on movement: Evidence from
Alexander technique teachers rising from a chair. Journal of Neuro-
physiology, 112(3), 719729. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00617.2013
Caneiro, J.P., Smith, A., Bunzli, S., Linton, S., Moseley, G.L., &
OSullivan, P. (2022). From fear to safety: A roadmap to recovery
from musculoskeletal pain. Physical Therapy, 102(2), Article 271.
https://doi.org/10.1093/ptj/pzab271
Clauwaert, A., Torta, D.M., Danneels, L., & Van Damme, S. (2018).
Attentional modulation of somatosensory processing during the
anticipation of movements accompanying pain: An event-related
potential study. Journal of Pain, 19(2), 219227. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.jpain.2017.10.008
Coghill, R.C. (2020). The distributed nociceptive system: A framework
for understanding pain. Trends in Neurosciences, 43(10), 780794.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2020.07.004
Cohen, M., & Quintner, J. (2012). The lived experience of pain: A painful
journey for medicine. In H.M.J. Quintner & G. Bendelow (Eds.),
At the edge of being: The Aporia of pain (pp. 1935). Inter-
Disciplinary.Net.
Cohen, R.G., Baer, J.L., Ravichandra, R., Kral, D., McGowan, C., &
Cacciatore, T.W. (2020). Lighten up! Postural instructions affect
static and dynamic balance in healthy older adults. Innovation in
Aging, 4(2), Article igz056. https://doi.org/10.1093/geroni/igz056
Cohen, R.G., Gurnkel, V.S., Kwak, E., Warden, A.C., & Horak, F.B.
(2015). Lighten up: Specic postural instructions affect axial rigidity
and step initiation in patients with Parkinsons disease. Neuro-
rehabilitation and Neural Repair, 29(9), 878888. https://doi.org/
10.1177/1545968315570323
Cohen, R.G., & Hocketstaller, J. (2023). Postural instructions affect post-
ural sway in young adults. Gait Posture, 100, 193195. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2022.12.016
Cook, P. (2013). Any curriculum will do: Structure as a catalyst for
adult transformation (Doctoral dissertation). University of Exeter.
https://ore.exeter.ac.uk/repository/bitstream/handle/10871/13972/
cookp.pdf
Covic, T., Adamson, B., & Hough, M. (2000). The impact of passive
coping on rheumatoid arthritis pain. Rheumatology, 39, 10271030.
https://doi.org/10.1093/rheumatology/39.9.1027
Crowley-Matoka, M., Saha, S., Dobscha, S.K., & Burgess, D.J. (2009).
Problems of quality and equity in pain management: Exploring the
role of biomedical culture. Pain Medicine, 10(7), 13121324. https://
doi.org/10.1111/j.1526-4637.2009.00716.x
Davies, J. (2020). Alexander Technique classes improve pain and perfor-
mance factors in tertiary music students. Journal of Bodywork and
Movement Therapies, 24(1), Article 6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbmt.
2019.04.006
Doringer, L., Kerns, R.D., & Auerbach, S.M. (2013). Providersroles
in enhancing patientsadherence to pain self management. Transla-
tional Behavioral Medicine, 3(1), 3946. https://doi.org/10.1007/
s13142-012-0158-z
Edwards, R.R., Dworkin, R.H., Sullivan, M.D., Turk, D.C., & Wasan,
A.D. (2016). The role of psychosocial processes in the development
and maintenance of chronic pain. Journal of Pain, 17(Suppl. 9),
T70T92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2016.01.001
Eldred, J., Hopton, A., Donnison, E., Woodman, J., & MacPherson, H.
(2015). Teachers of the Alexander Technique in the UK and the
people who take their lessons: A national cross-sectional survey.
Complementary Therapies in Medicine, 23(3), 451461. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.ctim.2015.04.006
Ferreira, P.H., Ferreira, M.L., Maher, C.G., Refshauge, K.M., Latimer, J.,
& Adams, R.D. (2013). The therapeutic alliance between clinicians
and patients predicts outcome in chronic low back pain. Physical
Therapy, 93(4), 470478. https://doi.org/10.2522/ptj.20120137
Flor, H., Denke, C., Schaefer, M., & Grusser, S. (2001). Effect of sensory
discrimination training on cortical reorganisation and phantom limb
pain. Lancet, 357(9270), 17631764. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-
6736(00)04890-X
Geri, T., Viceconti, A., Minacci, M., Testa, M., & Rossettini, G. (2019).
Manual therapy: Exploiting the role of human touch. Musculoskeletal
Science Practice, 44, Article 102044. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
msksp.2019.07.008
Giummarra, M.J., Gibson, S.J., Georgiou-Karistianis, N., & Bradshaw,
J.L. (2007). Central mechanisms in phantom limb perception: The
past, present and future. Brain Research Reviews, 54(1), 219232.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.01.009
Gleeson, M., Sherrington, C., Lo, S., & Keay, L. (2015). Can the
Alexander Technique improve balance and mobility in older adults
with visual impairments? A randomized controlled trial. Clinical
Rehabilitation, 29(3), 244260. https://doi.org/10.1177/026921551
4542636
Glover, L., Kinsey, D., Clappison, D.J., Gardiner, E., & Jomeen, J. (2018).
I never thought I could do that :::: Findings from an Alexander
Technique pilot group for older people with a fear of falling.
European Journal of Integrative Medicine, 17,7985. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.eujim.2017.11.008
Goossens, N., Rummens, S., Janssens, L., Caeyenberghs, K., &
Brumagne, S. (2018). Association between sensorimotor impairments
and functional brain changes in patients with low back pain: A critical
review. American Journal of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation,
97(3), 200211. https://doi.org/10.1097/PHM.0000000000000859
Grace, P.M., Hutchinson, M.R., Maier, S.F., & Watkins, L.R. (2014).
Pathological pain and the neuroimmune interface. Nature Reviews
Immunology, 14(4), 217231. https://doi.org/10.1038/nri3621
Gross, M., Bellingham, J.A.G., Brisset, P., Condie, C.L., Lazaro, S.,
Mello, B., & Cohen, R.G. (2022). Partnering with Poise: Alexander
technique online group classes are a promising intervention to
decrease lossofself and increase agency for care partners of people
living with dementia. Alzheimers & Dementia, 18(Suppl. 8), Article
292. https://doi.org/10.1002/alz.065292
Gurnkel, V., Cacciatore, T.W., Cordo, P., Horak, F., Nutt, J., & Skoss, R.
(2006). Postural muscle tone in the body axis of healthy humans.
Journal of Neurophysiology, 96(5), 26782687. https://doi.org/10.
1152/jn.00406.2006
Gwilym, S.E., Filippini, N., Douaud, G., Carr, A.J., & Tracey, I. (2010).
Thalamic atrophy associated with painful osteoarthritis of the hip
is reversible after arthroplasty: A longitudinal voxel-based morpho-
metric study. Arthritis and Rheumatism, 62(10), 29302940. https://
doi.org/10.1002/art.27585
Gwilym, S.E., Keltner, J.R., Warnaby, C.E., Carr, A.J., Chizh, B.,
Chessell, I., & Tracey, I. (2009). Psychophysical and functional
imaging evidence supporting the presence of central sensitization
in a cohort of osteoarthritis patients. Arthritis and Rheumatism, 61(9),
12261234. https://doi.org/10.1002/art.24837
Hall, A.M., Aubrey-Bassler, K., Thorne, B., & Maher, C.G. (2021). Do not
routinely offer imaging for uncomplicated low back pain. The BMJ,
372, Article 291. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.n291
Hamel, K.A., Ross, C., Schultz, B., ONeill, M., & Anderson, D.I.
(2016). Older adult Alexander Technique practitioners walk differ-
ently than healthy age-matched controls. Journal of Bodywork and
Movement Therapies, 20(4), 751760. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
jbmt.2016.04.009
Hanefeld, N., Glover, L., Jomeen, J., & Wadephul, F. (2021). Womens
experiences of using the Alexander Technique in the postpartum:
A MODERN PAIN SCIENCE LENS ON ALEXANDER TECHNIQUE 7
(Ahead of Print)
:::In a way, its just as benecial as sleep.Midwifery, 103, Article
103155. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.midw.2021.103155
Hartvigsen, J., Hancock, M.J., Kongsted, A., Louw, Q., Ferreira, M.L.,
Genevay, S., Hoy, D., Karppinen, J., Pransky, G., Sieper, J., Smeets,
R.J., Underwood, M., Buchbinder, R., Hartvigsen, J., Cherkin, D.,
Foster, N.E., Maher, C.G., Underwood, M., van Tulder, M., Lancet
Low Back Pain Series Working Group, & Woolf, A. (2018). What
low back pain is and why we need to pay attention. The Lancet,
391(10137), 23562367. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0140-6736(18)
30480-x
Hodges, P.W., & Danneels, L. (2019). Changes in structure and function
of the back muscles in low back pain: Different time points, ob-
servations, and mechanisms. Journal of Orthopaedic Sports Physical
Therapy, 49(6), 464476. https://doi.org/10.2519/jospt.2019.8827
Hodges, P.W., & Smeets, R.J. (2015). Interaction between pain, move-
ment, and physical activity: Short-term benets, long-term conse-
quences, and targets for treatment. Clinical Journal of Pain, 31(2),
97107. https://doi.org/10.1097/AJP.0000000000000098
Hodges, P.W., & Tucker, K. (2011). Moving differently in pain: A new
theory to explain the adaptation to pain. Pain, 152(Suppl. 3), S90
S98. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2010.10.020
Horga, L.M., Hirschmann, A.C., Henckel, J., Fotiadou, A., Di Laura, A.,
Torlasco, C., DSilva, A., Sharma, S., Moon, J.C., & Hart, A.J.
(2020). Prevalence of abnormal ndings in 230 knees of asymptom-
atic adults using 3.0 T MRI. Skeletal Radiology, 49(7), 10991107.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00256-020-03394-z
Hughes, L.S., Clark, J., Colclough, J.A., Dale, E., & McMillan, D. (2017).
Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT) for chronic pain: A
systematic review and meta-analyses. Clinical Journal of Pain, 33(6),
552568. https://doi.org/10.1097/AJP.0000000000000425
Johnson, M.B., & Cohen, R.G. (2023). Altered coordination strategies
during upright stance and gait in teachers of the Alexander Technique.
Front Aging, 4, Article 1090087. https://doi.org/10.3389/fragi.2023.
1090087
Jones, T., & Glover, L. (2014). Exploring the psychological processes
underlying touch: Lessons from the Alexander Technique. Clinical
Psychology Psychotherapy, 21(2), 140153. https://doi.org/10.1002/
cpp.1824
Jull, G., & Falla, D. (2016). Does increased supercial neck exor activity
in the craniocervical exion test reect reduced deep exor activity
in people with neck pain? Manual Therapy, 25,4347. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.math.2016.05.336
Jull, G.A., OLeary, S.P., & Falla, D.L. (2008). Clinical assessment of the
deep cervical exor muscles: The craniocervical exion test. Journal
of Manipulative Physiology Therapy, 31(7), 525533. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.jmpt.2008.08.003
Karunamuni, N., Imayama, I., & Goonetilleke, D. (2021). Pathways to
well-being: Untangling the causal relationships among biopsycho-
social variables. Social Science & Medicine, 272, Article 112846.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2020.112846
Kent, P., Haines, T., OSullivan, P., Smith, A., Campbell, A., Schutze, R.,
Attwell, S., Caneiro, J.P., Laird, R., OSullivan, K., McGregor, A.,
Hartvigsen, J., Lee, D.-C.A., Vickery, A., & Hancock, M. (2023).
Cognitive functional therapy with or without movement sensor
biofeedback versus usual care for chronic, disabling low back pain
(RESTORE): A randomised, controlled, three-arm, parallel group,
phase 3, clinical trial. The Lancet, 401(10391), 18661877. https://
doi.org/10.1016/s0140-6736(23)00441-5
Kinsey, D., Glover, L., & Wadephul, F. (2021). How does the Alexander
Technique lead to psychological and non-physical outcomes? A
realist review. European Journal of Integrative Medicine, 46, Article
1371. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eujim.2021.101371
Klein, S.D., Bayard, C., & Wolf, U. (2014). The Alexander Technique and
musicians: A systematic review of controlled trials. BMC Comple-
mentary and Alternative Medicine, 14, Article 414. https://doi.org/10.
1186/1472-6882-14-414
Kube, T., Korfer, K., Riecke, J., & Glombiewski, J.A. (2022). How
expectancy violations facilitate learning to cope with pain An
experimental approach. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 157,
Article 110807. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2022.110807
Laura, G., & Jones, G. (2020). Effectiveness of graded exercise & graded
exposure for chronic nonspecic low back pain: A rapid review.
Physiotherapy, 107, Article 145. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physio.
2020.03.145
Lazaridou, A., Kim, J., Cahalan, C.M., Loggia, M.L., Franceschelli, O.,
Berna, C., Schur, P., Napadow, V., & Edwards, R.R. (2017).
Effects of cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) on brain connectiv-
ity supporting catastrophizing in bromyalgia. Clinical of Journal
Pain, 33(3), 215221. https://doi.org/10.1097/AJP.00000000000
00422
Lin, I., Wiles, L., Waller, R., Goucke, R., Nagree, Y., Gibberd, M., Straker,
L., Maher, C.G., & OSullivan, P.P.B. (2020). What does best
practice care for musculoskeletal pain look like? Eleven consistent
recommendations from high-quality clinical practice guidelines: Sys-
tematic review. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 54(2), 7986.
https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsports-2018-099878
Linton, S., & Shaw, W. (2011). Impact of psychological factors in the
experience of pain. Physical Therapy, 91, 700711.
Little, J., Geraghty, A.W.A., Nicholls, C., & Little, P. (2022). Findings
from the development and implementation of a novel course con-
sisting of both group and individual Alexander Technique lessons for
low back pain. BMJ Open, 12(1), Article 39399. https://doi.org/10.
1136/bmjopen-2020-039399
Little, P., Lewith, G., Webley, F., Evans, M., Beattie, A., Middleton, K.,
Barnett, J., Ballard, K., Oxford, F., Smith, P., Yardley, L., Hollin-
ghurst, S., & Sharp, D. (2008). Randomised controlled trial of
Alexander technique lessons, exercise, and massage (ATEAM) for
chronic and recurrent back pain. The BMJ, 337, Article 884. https://
doi.org/10.1136/bmj.a884
Lluch, E., Torres, R., Nijs, J., & Van Oosterwijck, J. (2014). Evidence for
central sensitization in patients with osteoarthritis pain: A systematic
literature review. European Journal of Pain, 18(10), 13671375.
https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1532-2149.2014.499.x
Luo, C., Kuner, T., & Kuner, R. (2014). Synaptic plasticity in pathological
pain. Trends in Neurosciences, 37(6), 343355. https://doi.org/10.
1016/j.tins.2014.04.002
MacPherson, H., Tilbrook, H., Richmond, S., Woodman, J., Ballard, K.,
Atkin, K., Bland, M., Eldred, J., Essex, H., Hewitt, C., Hopton, A.,
Keding, A., Lansdown, H., Parrott, S., Torgerson, D., Wenham, A., &
Watt, I. (2015). Alexander technique lessons or acupuncture sessions
for persons with chronic neck pain: A randomized trial. Ann Intern
Med, 163(9), 653662. https://doi.org/10.7326/M15-0667
Maliet, A., Ickmans, K., Huysmans, E., Coppieters, I., Willaert, W.,
Bogaert, W.V., Rheel, E., Bilterys, T., Wilgen, P.V., & Nijs, J.
(2019). Best evidence rehabilitation for chronic pain part 3: Low
back pain. Journal of Clinical Medicine, 8(7), Article 1063. https://
doi.org/10.3390/jcm8071063
Mancini, F., Beaumont, A.L., Hu, L., Haggard, P., & Iannetti, G.D.D.
(2015). Touch inhibits subcortical and cortical nociceptive responses.
Pain, 156(10), 19361944. https://doi.org/10.1097/j.pain.00000000
00000253
Marinus, J., Moseley, G.L., Birklein, F., Baron, R., Maihöfner, C.,
Kingery, W.S., & van Hilten, J.J. (2011). Clinical features and
pathophysiology of complex regional pain syndrome. The Lancet
8HODGES, COHEN, AND CACCIATORE
(Ahead of Print)
Neurology, 10(7), 637648. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1474-4422(11)
70106-5
Martinez, E., Aira, Z., Buesa, I., Aizpurua, I., Rada, D., & Azkue, J.J.
(2018). Embodied pain in bromyalgia: Disturbed somatorepre-
sentations and increased plasticity of the body schema. PLoS One,
13(4), Article 194534. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.
0194534
McClean, S., Brilleman, S., & Wye, L. (2015). What is the perceived
impact of Alexander technique lessons on health status, costs and pain
management in the real life setting of an English hospital? The results
of a mixed methods evaluation of an Alexander Technique service
for those with chronic back pain. BMC Health Services Research, 15,
Article 293. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12913-015-0966-1
Mehling, W.E., DiBlasi, Z., & Hecht, F. (2005). Bias control in trials of
bodywork a review of methodological issues. The Journal of Alter-
native and Complementary Medicine, 11(2), 333342. https://doi.
org/10.1089/acm.2005.11.333
Meier, M.L., Vrana, A., & Schweinhardt, P. (2019). Low back pain: The
potential contribution of supraspinal motor control and proprio-
ception. Neuroscientist, 25(6), 583596. https://doi.org/10.1177/
1073858418809074
Meints, S.M., & Edwards, R.R. (2018). Evaluating psychosocial contribu-
tions to chronic pain outcomes. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacol-
ogy & Biological Psychiatry, 87(Pt B), 168182. https://doi.org/10.
1016/j.pnpbp.2018.01.017
Michaelides, A., & Zis, P. (2019). Depression, anxiety and acute pain:
Links and management challenges. Postgraduate Medicine, 131(7),
438444. https://doi.org/10.1080/00325481.2019.1663705
Moseley, G.L. (2007). Reconceptualising pain according to modern pain
science. Physical Therapy Reviews, 12(3), 169178. https://doi.org/
10.1179/108331907x223010
Moseley, G.L. (2019). Why we need a pain revolution: From science to
practice. National Center for Complementary and Integrative Health.
https://videocast.nih.gov/watch=33411
Moseley, G.L., & Arntz, A. (2007). The context of a noxious stimulus
affects the pain it evokes. Pain, 133(13), 6471. https://doi.org/10.
1016/j.pain.2007.03.002
Moseley, G.L., & Flor, H. (2012). Targeting cortical representations in
the treatment of chronic pain: A review. Neurorehabilitation Neural
Repair, 26(6), 646652. https://doi.org/10.1177/1545968311433209
National Institute for Health and Care Excellence. (2016). Low back
pain and sciatica in over 16s: Assessment and management. NICE
guideline [NG59].https://www.nice.org.uk/guidance/ng59/chapter/
recommendations
Nees, F., Ditzen, B., & Flor, H. (2022). When shared pain is not half
the pain: Enhanced central nervous system processing and verbal
reports of pain in the presence of a solicitous spouse. Pain, 163(9),
e1006e1012. https://doi.org/10.1097/j.pain.0000000000002559
Nicholas, M.K., Asghari, A., Corbett, M., Smeets, R.J., Wood, B.M.,
Overton, S., Perry, C., Tonkin, L.E., & Beeston, L. (2012). Is
adherence to pain self-management strategies associated with im-
proved pain, depression and disability in those with disabling chronic
pain? European Journal of Pain, 16(1), 93104. https://doi.org/10.
1016/j.ejpain.2011.06.005
Nishigami, T., Wand, B.M., Newport, R., Ratcliffe, N., Themelis, K.,
Moen, D., Jones, C., Moseley, G.L., & Stanton, T.R. (2019).
Embodying the illusion of a strong, t back in people with chronic
low back pain. A pilot proof-of-concept study. Musculoskeletal
Science & Practice, 39, 178183. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.msksp.
2018.07.002
ONeill, M.M., Anderson, D.I., Allen, D.D., Ross, C., & Hamel, K.A.
(2015). Effects of Alexander Technique training experience on gait
behavior in older adults. Journal of Bodywork and Movement Ther-
apies, 19(3), 473481. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbmt.2014.12.006
Pinto, A.M., Luis, M., Geenen, R., Palavra, F., Lumley, M.A., Ablin, J.N.,
Amris, K., Branco, J., Buskila, D., Castelhano, J., Castelo-Branco,
M., Crofford, L.J., Fitzcharles, M.A., Hauser, W., Kosek, E., Mease,
P.J., Marques, T.R., Jacobs, J.W.G., Castilho, P., & da Silva, J.A.P.
(2023). Neurophysiological and psychosocial mechanisms of bro-
myalgia: A comprehensive review and call for an integrative model.
Neuroscience Biobehavioral Reviews, 151, Article 105235. https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2023.105235
Preece, S.J., Jones, R.K., Brown, C.A., Cacciatore, T.W., & Jones, A.K.
(2016). Reductions in co-contraction following neuromuscular re-
education in people with knee osteoarthritis. BMC Musculoskeletal
Disorders, 17(1), Article 372. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12891-016-
1209-2
Raja, S.N., Carr, D.B., Cohen, M., Finnerup, N.B., Flor, H., Gibson, S.,
Keefe, F.J., Mogil, J.S., Ringkamp, M., Sluka, K.A., Song, X.J.,
Stevens, B., Sullivan, M.D., Tutelman, P.R., Ushida, T., & Vader, K.
(2020). The revised International Association for the Study of Pain
denition of pain: Concepts, challenges, and compromises. Pain,
161(9), 19761982. https://doi.org/10.1097/j.pain.00000000000
01939
Reddy, R.S., Tedla, J.S., Dixit, S., Raizah, A., Al-Otaibi, M.L., Gular, K.,
Ahmad, I., & Sirajudeen, M.S. (2022). Cervical joint position sense
and its correlations with postural stability in subjects with bromyal-
gia syndrome. Life, 12(11), Article 1817. https://doi.org/10.3390/
life12111817
Rhudy, J.L., Williams, A.E., McCabe, K.M., Nguyen, M.A., & Rambo, P.
(2005). Affective modulation of nociception at spinal and supraspinal
levels. Psychophysiology, 42(5), 579587. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.
1469-8986.2005.00313.x
Rossettini, G., Carlino, E., & Testa, M. (2018). Clinical relevance of
contextual factors as triggers of placebo and nocebo effects in mus-
culoskeletal pain. BMC Musculoskeletal Disorders, 19(1), Article 27.
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12891-018-1943-8
Ruhe, A., Fejer, R., & Walker, B. (2011). Altered postural sway in
patients suffering from non-specic neck pain and whiplash associ-
ated disorder A systematic review of the literature. Chiropractic &
Manual Therapies, 19, Article 13. https://doi.org/10.1186/2045-
709X-19-13
Seminowicz, D.A., Shpaner, M., Keaser, M.L., Krauthamer, G.M.,
Mantegna, J., Dumas, J.A., Newhouse, P.A., Filippi, C.G., Keefe,
F.J., & Naylor, M.R. (2013). Cognitive-behavioral therapy increases
prefrontal cortex gray matter in patients with chronic pain. Journal
of Pain, 14(12), 15731584. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2013.
07.020
Sher, J., Uribe, J., Posada, A., Murphy, B., & Zlatkin, M. (1995).
Abnormal ndings on magnetic resonance images of asymptomatic
shoulders. The Journal of Bone and Joint Surgery, 77(1015), Article
2. https://doi.org/10.2106/00004623-199501000-00002
Stanton, T.R., Gilpin, H.R., Edwards, L., Moseley, G.L., & Newport, R.
(2018). Illusory resizing of the painful knee is analgesic in symptom-
atic knee osteoarthritis. PeerJ, 6, Article 5206. https://doi.org/10.
7717/peerj.5206
Staud, R. (2012). Abnormal endogenous pain modulation is a shared
characteristic of many chronic pain conditions. Expert Review of
Neurotherapeutics, 12(5), 577585. https://doi.org/10.1586/ern.12.41
Stubbs, B., West, E., Patchay, S., & Schoeld, P. (2014). Is there a
relationship between pain and psychological concerns related to
falling in community dwelling older adults? A systematic review.
Disability Rehabilitation, 36(23), 19311942. https://doi.org/10.
3109/09638288.2014.882419
A MODERN PAIN SCIENCE LENS ON ALEXANDER TECHNIQUE 9
(Ahead of Print)
Tanaka, S., Nishigami, T., Wand, B.M., Stanton, T.R., Mibu, A.,
Tokunaga, M., Yoshimoto, T., & Ushida, T. (2021). Identifying
participants with knee osteoarthritis likely to benet from physical
therapy education and exercise: A hypothesis-generating study.
European Journal of Pain, 25(2), 485496. https://doi.org/10.
1002/ejp.1687
Thompson, W.R., Sallis, R., Joy, E., Jaworski, C.A., Stuhr, R.M., & Trilk,
J.L. (2020). Exercise Is Medicine. American Journal of Lifestyle
Medicine, 14(5), 511523. https://doi.org/10.1177/15598276209
12192
Timmers, I., Quaedieg, C., Hsu, C., Heathcote, L.C., Rovnaghi, C.R., &
Simons, L.E. (2019). The interaction between stress and chronic pain
through the lens of threat learning. Neuroscience Biobehavioral
Reviews, 107, 641655. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.
10.007
Tinbergen, N. (1973). Nobel lecture. NobelPrize.org. Nobel Prize Out-
reach AB. June 1, 2024. https://www.nobelprize.org/prizes/medicine/
1973/tinbergen/lecture/
Torta, D.M., Legrain, V., Mouraux, A., & Valentini, E. (2017). Attention
to pain! A neurocognitive perspective on attentional modulation of
pain in neuroimaging studies. Cortex, 89, 120134. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.cortex.2017.01.010
Trevisan, D.C., Driusso, P., Avila, M.A., Gramani-Say, K., Moreira,
F.M.A., & Parizotto, N.A. (2017). Static postural sway of women
with and without bromyalgia syndrome: A cross-sectional study.
Clinical Biomechanics, 44,8389. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
clinbiomech.2017.03.011
van Dieen, J.H., Reeves, N.P., Kawchuk, G., van Dillen, L.R., & Hodges,
P.W. (2019). Motor control changes in low back pain: Divergence in
presentations and mechanisms. Journal of Orthopaedic Sports Phys-
ical Therapy, 49(6), 370379. https://doi.org/10.2519/jospt.2019.
7917
Vlaeyen, J.W.S., & Linton, S.J. (2000). Fear-avoidance and its conse-
quences in chronic musculoskeletal pain: A state of the art. Pain, 85,
317332. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3959(99)00242-0
Vowles, K.E., McNeil, D.W., Gross, R.T., McDaniel, M.L., Mouse, A.,
Bates, M., Gallimore, P., & McCall, C. (2007). Effects of pain
acceptance and pain control strategies on physical impairment in
individuals with chronic low back pain. Behavior Therapy, 38(4),
412425. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2007.02.001
Wall, P. (1979). On the relation of injury to pain. Pain, 6, 253264.
Wallis, J.A., & Taylor, N.F. (2011). Pre-operative interventions (non-
surgical and non-pharmacological) for patients with hip or knee
osteoarthritis awaiting joint replacement surgerya systematic
review and meta-analysis. Osteoarthritis Cartilage, 19(12), 1381
1395. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joca.2011.09.001
Wallwork, S.B., Bellan, V., Catley, M.J., & Moseley, G.L. (2016). Neural
representations and the cortical body matrix: Implications for sports
medicine and future directions. British Journal of Sports Medicine,
50(16), 990996. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsports-2015-095356
Wand, B.M., Cashin, A.G., McAuley, J.H., Bagg, M.K., Orange, G.M., &
Moseley, G.L. (2023). The t-for-purpose model: Conceptualizing
and managing chronic nonspecic low back pain as an information
problem. Physical Therapy, 103(2), Article 151. https://doi.org/10.
1093/ptj/pzac151
Wernli, K., Smith, A., Coll, F., Campbell, A., Kent, P., & OSullivan, P.
(2022). From protection to non-protection: A mixed methods study
investigating movement, posture, and recovery from disabling low
back pain. European Journal of Pain, 26(10), 20972119. https://doi.
org/10.1002/ejp.2022
Wetherell, J.L., Afari, N., Rutledge, T., Sorrell, J.T., Stoddard, J.A.,
Petkus, A.J., Solomon, B.C., Lehman, D.H., Liu, L., Lang, A.J.,
& Atkinson, H.J. (2011). A randomized, controlled trial of acceptance
and commitment therapy and cognitive-behavioral therapy for
chronic pain. Pain, 152(9), 20982107. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
pain.2011.05.016
Williams, A.C.C., Fisher, E., Hearn, L., & Eccleston, C. (2020). Psycho-
logical therapies for the management of chronic pain (excluding
headache) in adults. The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews,
8, Article CD007407. https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD007407.
pub4
Woodman, J., Ballard, K., Hewitt, C., & MacPherson, H. (2018).
Self-efcacy and self-care-related outcomes following Alexander
Technique lessons for people with chronic neck pain in the ATLAS
randomised, controlled trial. European Journal of Integrative Medi-
cine, 17,6471. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eujim.2017.11.006
Woodman, J.P., & Moore, N.R. (2012). Evidence for the effectiveness
of Alexander Technique lessons in medical and health-related con-
ditions: A systematic review. International Journal of Clinical
Practice, 66(1), 98112. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1742-1241.2011.
02817.x
Woods, C., Glover, L., & Woodman, J. (2020). An education for life:
The process of learning the Alexander technique. Kinesiology
Review, 9(3), 190198. https://doi.org/10.1123/kr.2020-0020
Woolf, C.J. (2011). Central sensitization: Implications for the diagnosis
and treatment of pain. Pain, 152(Suppl. 3), S2S15. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.pain.2010.09.030
Zeidan, F., Grant, J.A., Brown, C.A., McHafe, J.G., & Coghill, R.C.
(2012). Mindfulness meditation-related pain relief: Evidence for
unique brain mechanisms in the regulation of pain. Neuroscience
Letters, 520(2), 165173. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2012.03.
082
Zillig, A.L., Pauli, P., Wieser, M., & Reicherts, P. (2023). Better safe than
sorry?On the inuence of learned safety on pain perception. PLoS
One, 18(11), Article 289047. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.
0289047
10 HODGES, COHEN, AND CACCIATORE
(Ahead of Print)
... Of these, muscle tone has been studied most in the Alexander Technique, which has been found to increase adaptivity of axial tone (Cacciatore et al., 2011a;Cohen et al., 2015), but alterations in tone are also implicated in the Feldenkrais Method (Stephens et al., 2006). Changes in muscle tone may explain improvements in performance and pain resulting from these practices (Stephens et al., 2006;Little et al., 2008;MacPherson et al., 2013;Preece et al., 2016;Becker et al., 2021;Berland et al., 2022;Little et al., 2022;Hodges et al., 2024). While contextual factors such as expectations of improvement generally contribute to clinical benefit (Rossettini et al., 2018), pain reduction and alterations to tone and coordination from the Alexander Technique and Feldenkrais Method have exceeded that for control interventions (Little et al., 2008;Cacciatore et al., 2011a;MacPherson et al., 2013;Berland et al., 2022), suggesting a non-placebo mechanism. ...
Article
Full-text available
Muscle tone represents a foundational property of the motor system with the potential to impact musculoskeletal pain and motor performance. Muscle tone is involuntary, dynamically adaptive, interconnected across the body, sensitive to postural demands, and distinct from voluntary control. Research has historically focused on pathological tone, peripheral regulation, and contributions from passive tissues, without consideration of the neural regulation of active tone and its consequences, particularly for neurologically healthy individuals. Indeed, simplistic models based on the stretch reflex, which neglect the central regulation of tone, are still perpetuated today. Recent advances regarding tone are dispersed across different literatures, including animal physiology, pain science, motor control, neurology, and child development. This paper brings together diverse areas of research to construct a conceptual model of the neuroscience underlying active muscle tone. It highlights how multiple tonic drive networks tune the excitability of complex spinal feedback circuits in concert with various sources of sensory feedback and in relation to postural demands, gravity, and arousal levels. The paper also reveals how tonic muscle activity and excitability are disrupted in people with musculoskeletal pain and how tone disorders can lead to marked pain and motor impairment. The paper presents evidence that integrative somatic methods address the central regulation of tone and discusses potential mechanisms and implications for tone rehabilitation to improve pain and performance.
Article
Full-text available
Purpose Sitting balance on an unstable surface requires coordinated out-of-phase lumbar spine and provides sufficient challenge to expose quality of spine control. We investigated whether the quality of spine coordination to maintain balance in acute low back pain (LBP) predicts recovery at 6 months. Methods Participants in an acute LBP episode (n = 94) underwent assessment of sitting balance on an unstable surface. Seat, hip and spine (lower lumbar, lumbar, upper lumbar, thoracic) angular motion and force plate data were recorded. Coordination between the seat and hip/spine segments to maintain balance was quantified in the frequency domain to evaluate coordination (coherence) and relative timing (phase angle: in-phase [segments move together]; out-of-phase [segments move opposite]). Center of pressure (CoP) and upper thorax motion assessed overall balance performance. Hip and spine coordination with the seat were compared between those who did not recover (increased/unchanged pain/disability), partially recovered (reduced pain/disability) or recovered (no pain and disability) at 6 months. Results In both planes, coherence between the seat and lower lumbar spine was lower (and in-phase—unhelpful for balance) at baseline in those who did not recover than those who recovered. Coherence between the seat and hip was higher in partially recovered in both planes, suggesting compensation by the hip. LBP groups had equal overall balance performance (CoP, upper thorax motion), but non-recovery groups used a less optimal strategy that might have consequences for long-term spine health. Conclusion These longitudinal data revealed that individuals with compromised contribution of the lumbar spine to the balance during unstable sitting during acute LBP are less likely to recover.
Article
Full-text available
The experience of threat was found to result—mostly—in increased pain, however it is still unclear whether the exact opposite, namely the feeling of safety may lead to a reduction of pain. To test this hypothesis, we conducted two between-subject experiments (N = 94; N = 87), investigating whether learned safety relative to a neutral control condition can reduce pain, while threat should lead to increased pain compared to a neutral condition. Therefore, participants first underwent either threat or safety conditioning, before entering an identical test phase, where the previously conditioned threat or safety cue and a newly introduced visual cue were presented simultaneously with heat pain stimuli. Methodological changes were performed in experiment 2 to prevent safety extinction and to facilitate conditioning in the first place: We included additional verbal instructions, increased the maximum length of the ISI and raised CS-US contingency in the threat group from 50% to 75%. In addition to pain ratings and ratings of the visual cues (threat, safety, arousal, valence, and contingency), in both experiments, we collected heart rate and skin conductance. Analysis of the cue ratings during acquisition indicate successful threat and safety induction, however results of the test phase, when also heat pain was administered, demonstrate rapid safety extinction in both experiments. Results suggest rather small modulation of subjective and physiological pain responses following threat or safety cues relative to the neutral condition. However, exploratory analysis revealed reduced pain ratings in later trials of the experiment in the safety group compared to the threat group in both studies, suggesting different temporal dynamics for threat and safety learning and extinction, respectively. Perspective: The present results demonstrate the challenge to maintain safety in the presence of acute pain and suggest more research on the interaction of affective learning mechanism and pain processing.
Article
Full-text available
Deterioration in movement and posture often occurs with aging. Yet there may be approaches to movement training that can maintain posture and movement coordination patterns as we age. The Alexander Technique is a non-exercise-based approach that aims to improve everyday movement and posture by increasing awareness and modulating whole-body postural muscle activity. This study assessed whether nineteen 55–72-year-old Alexander Technique teachers showed different posture and movement coordination patterns than twenty age-matched controls during a standing and walking protocol using 3D inertial sensors. During upright stance, Alexander Technique teachers showed lower centroidal sway frequency at the ankle (p = .04) and lower normalized jerk at the sternum (p = .05) than controls. During gait, Alexander Technique teachers had more symmetrical gait cycles (p = .04), more symmetrical arm swing velocity (p = .01), greater arm swing velocity (p < .01), greater arm swing range of motion (p = .02), and lower range of acceleration of the torso in the frontal plane (p = .03) than controls. Smoother control of upright posture, more stable torso motion, and less restrained arm mobility suggest that Alexander Technique training may counter movement degradation that is found with aging. Results highlight the important balance between mobility and stability within the torso and limbs.
Article
Full-text available
Cervical joint position sense (JPS) and postural stability are vital to maintaining balance and preventing falls in fibromyalgia syndrome (FMS). Impaired cervical JPS may influence postural stability, and understanding the relationship between them can aid in formulating treatment strategies in individuals with FMS. This study aims to (1) assess cervical JPS and postural stability between FMS and control group and (2) determine the correlation between cervical JPS and postural stability in the FMS group. This cross-sectional study recruited 92 FMS patients (mean age: 51.52 ± 7.7 yrs.) and 92 healthy controls (mean age: 49.36 ± 6.9 yrs.). A cervical range of motion (CROM) unit was utilized to assess cervical JPS. The postural stability was assessed using an IsoFree force platform, and anterior-posterior (A/P) and medial-lateral (M/L) directions of sway and ellipse area were measured. Cervical JPS and postural stability tests were assessed and compared between FMS and control groups. Cervical JPS was significantly impaired in FMS compared to the control group (p < 0.001). The JPS errors in FMS group were larger in flexion (FMS = 5.5°, control = 2.4°), extension (FMS = 6.4°, control = 3.0°), and rotation in left (FMS = 5.4°, control = 2.2°) and right directions (FMS = 5.1°, control = 2.8°). FMS individuals demonstrated statistically significant impaired postural stability compared to control in both the dominant and non-dominant legs tested (p < 0.001). The cervical JPS test showed moderate to strong positive correlations with postural stability variables. Statistically significant correlations were observed in all the JPS directions tested with all the postural stability variables (A/P and M/L sway and ellipse area). The correlation coefficients ranged between r = 0.37 (moderate) to 0.75 (strong). Cervical JPS and postural stability are impaired in FMS individuals. A moderate to strong relationship existed between JPS and postural stability. Individuals with FMS who had a greater magnitude of cervical JPS errors exhibited more severe postural control deficits. Therefore, cervical JPS and postural stability tests should be incorporated into routine clinical practice when assessing or formulating treatment strategies for patients with FMS.
Article
Research into the neurobiological and psychosocial mechanisms involved in fibromyalgia has progressed remarkably in recent years. Despite this, current accounts of fibromyalgia fail to capture the complex, dynamic, and mutual crosstalk between neurophysiological and psychosocial domains. We conducted a comprehensive review of the existing literature in order to: a) synthesize current knowledge on fibromyalgia; b) explore and highlight multi-level links and pathways between different systems; and c) build bridges connecting disparate perspectives. An extensive panel of international experts in neurophysiological and psychosocial aspects of fibromyalgia discussed the collected evidence and progressively refined and conceptualized its interpretation. This work constitutes an essential step towards the development of a model capable of integrating the main factors implicated in fibromyalgia into a single, unified construct which appears indispensable to foster the understanding, assessment, and intervention for fibromyalgia.
Article
Background: Low back pain is the leading cause of years lived with disability globally, but most interventions have only short-lasting, small to moderate effects. Cognitive functional therapy (CFT) is an individualised approach that targets unhelpful pain-related cognitions, emotions, and behaviours that contribute to pain and disability. Movement sensor biofeedback might enhance treatment effects. We aimed to compare the effectiveness and economic efficiency of CFT, delivered with or without movement sensor biofeedback, with usual care for patients with chronic, disabling low back pain. Methods: RESTORE was a randomised, controlled, three-arm, parallel group, phase 3 trial, done in 20 primary care physiotherapy clinics in Australia. We recruited adults (aged ≥18 years) with low back pain lasting more than 3 months with at least moderate pain-related physical activity limitation. Exclusion criteria were serious spinal pathology (eg, fracture, infection, or cancer), any medical condition that prevented being physically active, being pregnant or having given birth within the previous 3 months, inadequate English literacy for the study's questionnaires and instructions, a skin allergy to hypoallergenic tape adhesives, surgery scheduled within 3 months, or an unwillingness to travel to trial sites. Participants were randomly assigned (1:1:1) via a centralised adaptive schedule to usual care, CFT only, or CFT plus biofeedback. The primary clinical outcome was activity limitation at 13 weeks, self-reported by participants using the 24-point Roland Morris Disability Questionnaire. The primary economic outcome was quality-adjusted life-years (QALYs). Participants in both interventions received up to seven treatment sessions over 12 weeks plus a booster session at 26 weeks. Physiotherapists and patients were not masked. This trial is registered with the Australian New Zealand Clinical Trials Registry, ACTRN12618001396213. Findings: Between Oct 23, 2018 and Aug 3, 2020, we assessed 1011 patients for eligibility. After excluding 519 (51·3%) ineligible patients, we randomly assigned 492 (48·7%) participants; 164 (33%) to CFT only, 163 (33%) to CFT plus biofeedback, and 165 (34%) to usual care. Both interventions were more effective than usual care (CFT only mean difference -4·6 [95% CI -5·9 to -3·4] and CFT plus biofeedback mean difference -4·6 [-5·8 to -3·3]) for activity limitation at 13 weeks (primary endpoint). Effect sizes were similar at 52 weeks. Both interventions were also more effective than usual care for QALYs, and much less costly in terms of societal costs (direct and indirect costs and productivity losses; -AU$5276 [-10 529 to -24) and -8211 (-12 923 to -3500). Interpretation: CFT can produce large and sustained improvements for people with chronic disabling low back pain at considerably lower societal cost than that of usual care. Funding: Australian National Health and Medical Research Council and Curtin University.
Article
Background: Instructions to exert effort to correct one's posture are ubiquitous, but previous work indicates that effort-based postural instructions can impair balance control in older adults with and without neurodegenerative disease. Although less-studied, young adults are at high risk of injurious falls. Research question: How do different postural instructions influence static balance in young adults? Methods: Single-session, counterbalanced, within-subjects design. Twenty young adults briefly practiced three different ways of thinking about their posture, then attempted to employ each way of thinking while standing on springy foam for 30 s with eyes open. Relax instructions were used as a baseline between experimental conditions. Effort-based instructions emulated popular concepts of posture correction using muscular exertion. Light instructions aimed at encouraging length and width while reducing excess tension. Postural sway was assessed with an inertial sensor at the low back. Results: Effort-based postural instructions increased path length and jerk of postural sway during quiet stance, relative to Light and Relaxed instructions. Significance: These results are consistent with previous work in older adults indicating that thinking of upright posture as inherently effortful impairs balance. Therefore, the common practice of instructing young adults to use effortful posture may impair their balance performance.
Article
Studies show 1:1 in‐person Alexander technique (AT) sessions can reduce anxiety and musculoskeletal pain with long‐term benefits [1‐4]. Care partners (CP) often experience role engulfment and loss of self [5]. AT is an embodied approach to choose functional patterns that transform disruptive stress reactions to adaptive responses, increasing confidence, self‐control, and self‐awareness. Previous in‐person group course results from 71 Parkinson’s CP showed promise with benefits retained at 12 months. COVID‐19 provided an opportunity to test synchronous online AT‐based courses for CP of people living with dementia. Design: Controlled feasibility trial. AT group: 14 participants (12F/2M) from 9 states; mean age 68; 72% provided 41+ hrs/wk care for family member with dementia; 13 lived with care‐receiver. Control group: 9 participants (8F/1M) from 4 states; mean age 63; 67% provided 41+ hrs/wk care; 6 lived with family care‐receiver. AT groups met 90 min/week x 10wks. Intervention: Delivered in‐home via Zoom. Embodied self‐management strategies included intentional strategies to interrupt automatic reactions and affirm centrality of the self, taught via verbal and visual instruction, activities, anatomical models/images, and demonstration videos. AT principles were embedded in everyday activities (gait, sit‐to‐stand, IADLs, etc.). Outcome Measures: Self‐report, executive function, anonymous evaluations, interviews. 70% retention, 87% attendance. AT group improved executive function (Digit Span p = .003; Stroop p = .05); control group did not. AT group reported increased self‐efficacy (caregiver scale p = .08); and reduced fear (p = .004); control group did not. On 0‐10 scale, mean rating was >9 for novelty of ideas, practical tools for physical self‐management, and would recommend to a friend; and >8 for enjoyment of group interaction, practical tools for self‐management of emotions and reactions, and prevention of pain; 7.9 for feeling better prepared for CP daily challenges. Six‐month follow‐up is underway. Alexander technique shows promise as a long‐term self‐management approach to reduce loss‐of‐self in CP of people living with cognitive decline. Group classes can provide cost‐effective delivery with social benefits. Online classes can increase accessibility. A full‐scale study is merited. 1.Little (2008) 2.MacPherson (2015) 3.Klein (2014) 4.Stallibrass (2002) 5.Skaff, Pearlin (1992)
Article
Chronic nonspecific low back pain (CNSLBP) is a complex and multifaceted problem. The following Perspective Piece tries to help make sense of this complexity by describing a model for the development and maintenance of persistent low back pain that integrates modifiable factors across the biopsychosocial spectrum. The Fit-for-Purpose model (FFPM) posits the view that chronic nonspecific low back pain represents a state in which the person in pain holds strong and relatively intransient internal models of an immutably damaged, fragile, and unhealthy back, and information that supports these models is more available and trustworthy than information that counters them. This Perspective proposes a corresponding treatment framework for persistent pain that aims to shift internal models of a fragile, damaged, unhealthy, and unchangeable self toward the formulation of the back as healthy, strong, adaptable and fit-for-purpose and to provide the system with precise and trustworthy evidence that supports this supposition while minimizing information that works against it.