Available via license: CC BY 4.0
Content may be subject to copyright.
ARTICLE
When Group Appeals Backfire: Explaining the Asymmetric
Effects of Place-Based Appeals
Lukas Haffert1, Tabea Palmtag2and Dominik Schraff3
1
Department of Political Science and International Relations, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland,
2
Department of
Political Science, University of Zürich, Zürich, Switzerland and
3
Department of Politics and Society, Aalborg University,
Aalborg, Denmark
Corresponding author: Lukas Haffert; Email: lukashaffert@googlemail.com
(Received 26 June 2023; revised 6 January 2024; accepted 31 May 2024)
Abstract
Group-based identities are an important basis of political competition. Politicians consciously appeal to
specific social groups, and these group-based appeals often improve the evaluation of parties and candi-
dates. Studying place-based appeals, we advance the understanding of this strategy by distinguishing
between dominant and subordinate social groups. Using two survey experiments in Germany and
England, we show that group appeals improve candidate evaluation among subordinate (rural) voters.
By contrast, appeals to the dominant (urban) group trigger a negative reaction. While urban citizens’
weaker local identities and lower place-based resentment partly explain this asymmetry, they mainly dis-
like group-based appeals because of their antagonistic nature. If the same policies are framed as benefiting
urban and rural dwellers alike, candidate evaluation improves. Thus, people on the dominant side of a
group divide reject a framing of politics as antagonistically structured by this divide, even if they identify
with the dominant group.
Keywords: Group-based appeals; urban; rural; social identity; representation
Introduction
Political scientists have recently rediscovered the importance of social identities and group mem-
berships for political behaviour. Based on a revived interest in the importance of social cleavages
(Bornschier et al. 2021a; Ford and Jennings 2020; Guth and Nelsen 2021; Hooghe and Marks
2018; Hooghe and Marks 2022), these studies investigate the role of social groups on the demand
and supply side of politics. On the demand side, several studies have analyzed the importance of
social identities and group attachments for individual political behaviour (Bornschier et al. 2021b;
Evans, Stubager, and Langsæther 2022; Mason and Wronski 2018; Zollinger 2024). On the supply
side, a growing number of studies investigate how parties use group-based appeals to mobilize
different social groups (Dassonneville, Stubager, and Thau 2022; Dolinsky 2023; Horn et al.
2021; Huber 2022; Jacobs and Munis 2019; Robison et al. 2021; Stuckelberger and Tresch
2024; Thau 2019).
The literature on group-based appeals established the relevance of such appeals in party man-
ifestos and campaign communications (Dolinsky 2023; Horn et al. 2021; Huber 2022;
Stuckelberger and Tresch 2024; Thau 2019). Moreover, researchers have collected experimental
evidence that voters react positively to these types of appeals (Dassonneville, Stubager, and
Thau 2022; Jacobs and Munis 2019; Robison et al. 2021). These experimental studies generally
paint the picture of a highly effective political instrument: most appeals investigated elicit a
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press. This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative
Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction,
provided the original article is properly cited.
British Journal of Political Science (2024), page 1 of 22
doi:10.1017/S0007123424000218
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123424000218 Published online by Cambridge University Press
positive reaction from the targeted in-group. What is more, even out-groups often react neutrally
to such appeals. Thus, group-based appeals seem to have a considerable upside but little down-
side from the perspective of political parties.
This perception, however, might emerge from existing studies focusing on the disadvantaged
groups in major societal divides, such as lower-class or rural voters. Indeed, political appeals are
often argued to be especially successful among members of more deserving groups that hold a
marginalized position in society (Dassonneville, Stubager, and Thau 2022). So far, limited atten-
tion has been paid to how politicians can appeal to the opposing sides of these divides. First indi-
cative evidence from a study on class-based appeals suggests that such appeals may have strongly
asymmetric effects as upper-middle-class voters in the US react negatively to an
upper-middle-class appeal (Robison et al. 2021, supplementary material Figure OB1). This raises
a broader question on how group-based appeals work across both sides of structural societal
divides, and how we may explain such asymmetries.
In this paper, we demonstrate that group-based appeals can have systematically different
effects, depending on which side of the structural divide they address. We do so by reporting
the results of two survey experiments in Germany and the UK. They allow us to distinguish in
more detail for whom such group-based appeals have a positive impact, and when and why
they can elicit a negative reaction. The context in which we encounter these asymmetric effects
is in appeals to urban or rural voters. These appeals are of increasing substantive interest as
urban-rural divides characterize many recent elections in the United States and across Europe.
Indeed, ‘place’is increasingly seen as a crucial element of people’s social and political identities
(Bornschier et al. 2021a; Cramer 2016; Munis 2022). Accordingly, place-based identities are an
important potential target for group-based appeals. Rural (Dassonneville, Stubager, and Thau
2022; Jacobs and Munis 2019) and urban (Jacobs and Munis 2019) appeals have already been
studied in the experimental literature on group-based appeals.
Moreover, the urban-rural divide is of particular interest since it increasingly stratifies Western
societies, with urban people acquiring a socially dominant position while rural people are increas-
ingly in a (objectively and subjectively) subordinate position. This conflict allows for positive
in-group identification on both sides of the divide. While most rural citizens clearly have a posi-
tive identity as ‘rural’, a smaller but substantial number of urban citizens also hold a positive
‘urban’identity (Lin and Trujillo 2023; Lyons and Utych 2023). Still, group-based appeals are
typically studied in a context where only one group (the group with subordinate status; for
example, the working class, or ethnic minorities) holds an in-group identity and sees the dom-
inant, upper-status group as a clear out-group. By contrast, the members of the dominant
group often hold a weaker group identity, at least as long as this identity is not being threatened.
In the case of urban-rural divides, it is much more plausible that one person’s out-group is
another person’s in-group. Hence, studying the power of appeals on both sides of the urban-rural
divide provides a good context to study the scope conditions of group appeals.
1
Theoretically, we combine insights from the literature on cleavages and social positions with
the group-appeals literature to theorize the scope conditions for when group appeals work.
Specifically, we distinguish between dominant and subordinate groups based on their position
and trajectory in a social divide and propose that group-based appeals will have asymmetric
effects on these two groups. People on the dominant side of a social divide will typically have
no interest in the framing of politics revolving around this divide and will thus resist antagonistic
appeals. After all, such a framing necessarily emphasizes their dominant, privileged position,
which often triggers negative emotions of self-directed anger and guilt (Dover 2022; Harth,
Kessler, and Leach 2008; Leach, Iyer, and Pedersen 2006). Therefore, voters who perceive
themselves to be on the winning side of a social divide reject appeals that emphasize this divide.
1
Other divides in which the dominant group may hold more positive in-group identity include education, gender, and
centre-periphery-divides. This should be subject to empirical investigations in future research.
2 Lukas Haffert et al.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123424000218 Published online by Cambridge University Press
Even though they might value a policy that would favour their in-group, they prefer appeals that
depict this policy as serving a common interest of all groups and thereby downplay the divide. By
contrast, the group that perceives itself as subordinate is more receptive to conflictual appeals as
its position provides the moral grounds upon which to challenge the existing order and the out-
group dominance. The effectiveness of group-based appeals among subordinate groups mirrors
the existing literature’s focus on studying appeals to disadvantaged social groups such as the
working class, rural people, or people with lower education levels (for example, Dassonneville,
Stubager, and Thau 2022, Robison et al. 2021).
We test this theoretical argument with a set of survey experiments that expose dominant
(that is urban) or subordinate (that is rural) social groups to group-based appeals. We find
that appeals to rural voters indeed have the expected effect. In both Germany and the UK,
rural voters evaluate candidates who appeal to them as a group significantly better than generic
candidates who make a general appeal to all voters. By contrast, we do not find a positive effect
of appealing to urban voters. On the contrary, urban appeals reduce urban voters’evaluation of
a candidate.
While we anticipated an asymmetry in the reaction of urban and rural voters, we were sur-
prised by the intensity of urban voters’negative reaction to group-based appeals that have strong
positive effects among rural voters. Urban voters’antipathy towards an in-group appeal is partly
explained by the fact that urban citizens on average have weaker local identities and report lower
place-based resentment. Nevertheless, there remains a substantial difference between urban and
rural voters’reactions to the in-group appeal, even after controlling for these factors. Even people
with a strong urban identity at best react neutrally to an urban appeal.
To gain a better understanding of this reaction, we ran a second survey targeted only at urban
voters in Germany. Since our interest in this survey was specifically about the backlash among
urban voters to in-group appeals, we recruited only urban respondents. Following our theoretical
considerations, we adjusted the policy content and the antagonistic nature of the urban appeals to
test whether this more advantaged group of voters prefers addressing their interests in a less con-
flictual way. In the second survey, we vary both the specific policy issues addressed in the appeal,
as well as how the relationship between urban and rural interests is presented. We find that the
specific issues do not make a difference: urban respondents dislike an innovation-focused appeal
and a housing-focused appeal just as much as a generic economic appeal. However, how the
appeal presents the relationship between urban and rural interests has a decisive impact; if the
appeal presents politics as an antagonistic conflict between urban and rural citizens, urban voters
reject the appeal. If the exact same policies are presented in a harmonious framework that empha-
sizes their benefits for all citizens, urban voters react positively.
These findings provide important scope conditions for the political effectiveness of group-
based appeals. Antagonistic appeals to dominant groups can reduce a politician’s support; this
is a finding that has been suggested for appeals to higher-class voters (Robison et al. 2021),
which we develop and explain systematically for urban residents. Conflictual appeals only mobil-
ize support among subordinate groups, for whom such appeals present societal order in line with
their own experiences. Our study suggests that successful group-based appeals require a consid-
eration of the type of social group targeted by the politician with respect to perceptions of dom-
inant vs. subordinate status.
This paper is organized as follows. In the next section, we develop our argument about the
limits of group-based appeals and the importance of considering which side of a structural divide
is the target of the appeal. We then explain why the urban-rural context is suitable for studying
the differential reaction to group-based appeals. Thereafter, we explain our survey design. In the
results section, we first present the results from our comparative survey, along with several sub-
group analyses. We then dig deeper into the behaviour of urban voters by analyzing the results of
the follow-up survey. In the conclusion, we discuss potential avenues for future research, with a
focus on the limits of group-based appeals.
British Journal of Political Science 3
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123424000218 Published online by Cambridge University Press
Argument
Group-Based Mobilization
The literature on group-based appeals starts from the theoretical premise that politics is funda-
mentally characterized by a conflict between social groups (Thau 2021). Therefore, these studies
argue that group-based appeals improve people’s evaluation of the appealing political candidate
or party if people perceive the group that is the target of the appeal as an important part of their
own social identity.
Some studies in this literature use observational data to study parties’actual appeals (Dolinsky
2023; Horn et al. 2021; Stuckelberger and Tresch 2024; Thau 2019). They show the continuing
importance of group-based appeals across countries and types of groups. While the most import-
ant group membership analyzed is class (Evans and Tilley 2017; Robison et al. 2021; Thau 2021),
other studies focused on gender (Kam, Archer, and Geer 2017), or a wider range of groups
(Dolinsky 2023; Huber 2022; Stuckelberger and Tresch 2024). Moreover, these studies show
that parties target these appeals strategically (Huber 2022).
In addition to these observational studies, a growing number of studies use survey experiments
to provide causal evidence for the effectiveness of group-based appeals. This literature typically
asks survey participants to evaluate fictitious candidates for political office based on short state-
ments or visual cues (Dassonneville, Stubager, and Thau 2022; Jacobs and Munis 2019; Robison
et al. 2021). Here, it is usually argued that signalling attention to and tuning of policies towards a
specific social group will be rewarded by group members as this enables a linking of political can-
didates to the group and its attributes (Robison et al. 2021). Indeed, these experimental studies
generally show that group-based appeals elicit a positive reaction from the targeted group.
Based on the theoretical understanding of politics as being structured by group conflict, we
thus derive our first hypothesis about the effectiveness of group-based appeals.
H1: Group-based appeals improve candidate assessment.
That said, this literature indicates two scope conditions for the success of group-based appeals.
2
Firstly, voters must identify with the group targeted by the appeal, and the appeal must make this
identity salient and imbue it with political meaning (Huddy 2013). Secondly, group members
need to see themselves in conflict with the out-group of the appeal and harbour resentment
against this out-group. Hence, as depicted in the framed part of Fig. 1, group-based appeals
only work when there is a certain identification with the in-group as well as a certain resentment
towards the out-group of the social divide.
These considerations lead to a second hypothesis about the scope conditions for the effectivity
of group appeals:
H2: The effect of group-based appeals is moderated by the strength of in-group identification
and out-group resentment.
However, the existing literature typically studies appeals to socially subordinate groups.
Robison et al. (2021), for example, focus on the effectiveness of working-class appeals, while
Dassonneville, Stubager, and Thau (2022) include appeals to four groups: the working class, peo-
ple with lower education levels, rural people, and the young. In all these cases (except, perhaps, for
the young), the in-group of the appeal is in a socially subordinate position while the out-group is
in a dominant position. An exception to the focus on the disadvantaged side of societal divides is
the study by Jacobs and Munis (2019), who studied both urban and rural appeals in the American
2
In addition, it has been shown that the appealing candidate needs to be perceived as an authentic representative of the
respective group and the appeal must not evoke stereotypes about a marginalized identity (Searle and Abrajano 2018).
4 Lukas Haffert et al.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123424000218 Published online by Cambridge University Press
context. Here, the authors find clear positive effects among rural voters but less effectiveness of
appeals among urban voters.
Asymmetry of Group-Based Conflict
Political cleavages typically emerge in reaction to the discontent of groups who perceive them-
selves as subordinate or threatened. Thus, the focus on subordinate groups in the political appeals
literature makes sense from a broader theoretical perspective. As Hooghe, Kamphorst, and Marks
(2022, 2) put it: ‘In the Lipset and Rokkan model, a major shock to the fabric of society motivates
opposition to the status quo on the part of groups whose lives are affected in ways that are dif-
ficult or impossible for them to escape.’Hence, parties representing a specific cleavage –be it
Socialists, Catholic parties, or Farmers’parties –typically emerged as representatives of those
who perceived themselves on the losing side of the industrial or national revolution, where losing
could mean either a generally low status or a relatively high but declining status.
3
By contrast, few parties actively seek to mobilize the other side of a cleavage. Whereas
working-class parties historically mobilized class-based divides, liberal or Christian-Democratic
parties often sought to deflect the importance of these divides, rather seeking to emphasize
the commonalities between bourgeois and working-class voters –for example, their common
nationality or faith.
Thus, the losing side typically drives the political mobilization of societal divides with clear
winners and losers. In 1959, Ralf Dahrendorf had already argued that groups of lower social status
were much more likely to see society in terms of conflict (Dahrendorf 1959). As he suggested, ‘the
dominant groups of society express their comparative gratification with existing conditions inter
alia by visualizing and describing these conditions as ordered and reasonable; subjected groups,
on the other hand, tend to emphasize the cleavages that, in their opinion, account for the depri-
vations they feel’(Dahrendorf 1959, 293). While Dahrendorf focused on the class divide, contem-
porary accounts emphasize that dominant groups such as white people or men are reluctant to
see society as structured by racial or gender divides, unless they start to see their position being
threatened (Friedman, O’Brien, and McDonald 2021; Jardina 2019; Phillips and Lowery 2018).
This suggests an important scope condition for the effectiveness of group-based appeals. As
the literature amply demonstrates, such appeals generally work for the subordinate side of a
political divide, as presented in the framed part of Fig. 1 (for example, Robison et al. 2021).
Figure 1. The general model of group-based appeals and its potential variation across social groups.
3
Accordingly, the literature on the radical right emphasizes that it has been most successful in mobilizing those who per-
ceive or foresee their status or relative position to decline (Burgoon et al. 2019; Häusermann, Kurer, and Zollinger 2023;
Kurer 2020).
British Journal of Political Science 5
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123424000218 Published online by Cambridge University Press
For groups who perceive their status as low and/or declining, these appeals ‘emphasize the clea-
vages that in their opinion account for the deprivations they feel’(Dahrendorf 1959, 293). Thus,
these appeals present a group’s self-interest as morally justified. Moreover, they may increase the
political entrepreneur’s credibility as an advocate of this group.
By contrast, for dominant/ascending groups, these appeals clash with their preference for
‘visualizing and describing these conditions as ordered and reasonable’. On the contrary, the
antagonistic nature of these appeals has quite the opposite effect. It taps into distributive conflicts
between a person’s in-group and out-group, and thereby emphasizes the existing inequalities that
favour the dominant group. Group-based appeals to dominant social groups thus run the risk of
activating privilege and concerns for out-group disadvantage. Indeed, inequalities across social
groups induce feelings of guilt and unfairness that play a key role in producing out-group concern
among individuals belonging to dominant groups (Chudy, Piston, and Shipper 2019; Powell,
Branscombe, and Schmitt 2005). In line with that, dominant groups have been shown to bias pol-
itical party agendas by exempting divisive issues from political competition (Traber et al. 2022;
Weber 2020). We therefore expect that in-group appeals will be less effective on the dominant
side of a structural divide. Instead, we expect that groups who perceive their status as high
and ascending prefer harmonious appeals, which address the interests of a social group but
avoid opening up a conflict with other groups and, rather, emphasize that the group’s interests
are aligned with those of other groups. Based on these considerations, we thus hypothesize:
H3: Group-based appeals work better for subordinate than for dominant groups, even after con-
trolling for in-group identification and out-group resentment.
A priori, it is hard to predict how strong the difference between dominant and subordinate
groups will be. In the preregistration of our first survey experiment,
4
we pre-registered a strong
positive effect of rural appeals and a weaker, yet still positive, effect of urban appeals.
Theoretically, however, we can mainly predict an asymmetry but not its magnitude.
That said, within the dominant group, the reaction to group-based appeals will again depend
on the factors identified above: in-group identification and out-group resentment. Among group
members without in-group identification or out-group resentment, these appeals might even eli-
cit a negative reaction as these groups prefer to avoid and diffuse social conflicts over inequalities
from which they are benefitting. Trying to mobilize people as ‘wealthy’or ‘West German’can
backfire since few individuals strongly identify with these groups. This was probably also the
case for ‘whites’in the United States but this may be changing since whites increasingly perceive
their position as eroding (Jardina 2019). Similarly, if the dominant group does not harbour
resentment towards the out-group, group-based mobilization attempts could trigger negative
reactions. However, this becomes less clear if membership in the dominant group is an important
social identity. For example, highly-educated people are clearly privileged yet they hold a positive
in-group identity (Stubager 2009). In these situations, the reaction to in-group appeals among the
dominant group may be more neutral. Still, it will not have the same positive effect as for sub-
ordinate groups.
Research Design
Case Selection: The Urban-Rural Divide
To provide empirical evidence on the asymmetry of group-based appeals, we ideally would want
to study a societal divide in which dominance and subordination are clearly distributed yet allow
people on both sides of the divide to hold positive in-group identities. Many divides, such as
class, only fulfil the first criterion. While dominance and subordination are clearly assigned,
4
Preregistration available at: https://osf.io/m572d
6 Lukas Haffert et al.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123424000218 Published online by Cambridge University Press
those on the dominant side usually do not develop strong in-group identities. Other divides, such
as age, only fulfil the second criterion. While ‘young’and ‘old’may be relevant identities, it is less
clear which group is dominant and which is subordinate. We propose that the urban-rural divide
is well-suited to fulfilling both criteria. There is a widely shared perception of which group is win-
ning and losing, yet it still offers a positive identification to both groups.
We studied this divide in England and Germany, following a most-different case design. In
England, urban places (in particular, London) clearly dominate the country in economic and cul-
tural terms. Moreover, British (English) centralism reinforces centre-periphery dynamics. In
Germany, the economic model is centred on a strong industrial sector, whose ‘hidden champions’
are often based in rural areas. Moreover, strong federalism and the proportional electoral system
incentivize parties to represent voters from all parts of the country. We then ran the follow-up
survey only in Germany, given that our initial results were very similar in both countries.
Despite these objective country differences, urban dwellers are typically perceived as economic
and cultural winners in both countries. In economic terms, the countryside is often characterized as
being ‘left behind’, while urban areas benefit from the transformation to a knowledge economy
(Rodríguez-Pose 2017; Iversen and Soskice 2019). At the same time, there is a growing cultural
divergence between ‘cosmopolitan’cities and ‘nationalist’rural areas (Cramer 2016;Huijsmans
et al. 2021;Maxwell2019,2020). Indeed, in our survey, those who classified their place of
residence as ‘very urban’reported a significantly better economic and cultural trajectory of their
community over the last decade compared to all other respondents (Appendix B, Table B4–B7).
Finally, urban dwellers have better chances to make their political voices heard (Gimpel, Lee,
and Thorpe 2011; Haffert 2024). Accordingly, rural voters perceive systematically lower levels of
political efficacy across European countries (García del Horno, Rico, and Hernández 2023).
Most empirical analyses focus on rural voters and argue that their strong local attachments can
lead them to develop ‘rural resentment’(Cramer 2016; Munis 2022) because they feel that they do
not get their ‘fair share’and that their way of life is not being respected by urban voters. Both
types of grievances are much less common among urban voters. This line of research has estab-
lished that, especially in the US context, rural appeals are a very common and highly effective
feature of political mobilization (Munis 2020).
While this characterization of rural places is relatively uncontested, there are two potential
objections to the characterization of urban places as being advantaged. Firstly, not all urban
places are on the winning side. Rustbelt cities such as Sunderland or Newcastle in England or
Gelsenkirchen or Chemnitz in Germany often have higher unemployment and lower incomes
than rural counties. Secondly, not all inhabitants of booming cities are winners. Processes of gen-
trification often push out longstanding dwellers who cannot afford rising rents and costs of living.
The important point, however, is that these relative losers are unlikely to see their situation as
being related to an urban-rural divide. Even if they identify strongly with their place, they will not
see rural people as the out-group responsible for their plight. Rustbelt inhabitants are more likely
to blame globalization or immigration, whereas gentrification losers may blame wealthier urban
residents. At the same time, they are unlikely to feel strong commonalities with rural losers. For
example, they will often struggle with the consequences of population growth (in the form of ris-
ing rents) rather than population decline. Still, our empirical design includes appeals that tap into
both the strength of booming cities (‘innovation centres’) as well as the difficulties of some urban
dwellers (‘affordable housing’).
Domination and subordination in the urban-rural divide are thus clearly recognized.
Moreover, ‘place’is increasingly seen as an important element of people’s social and political
identities (Bornschier et al. 2021a; Bornschier et al. 2021b). Many studies emphasize the conflict
between identities based in rural communities (Cramer 2016) and cosmopolitan cities
(Cunningham and Savage 2015) when seeking to explain the rise of anti-establishment parties
(Lunz Trujillo 2022; Lyons and Utych 2023). Indeed, place-based identities are becoming more
important to people as other sources of identity such as membership in collective organizations
British Journal of Political Science 7
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123424000218 Published online by Cambridge University Press
become less salient (Fitzgerald 2018). In the European context, Hegewald and Schraff (2022)
demonstrate the existence of an identity-based affective polarization between urban and rural
voters while Schulte-Cloos and Bauer (2023) show that voters have an identity-based preference
for candidates from their own municipality.
As with the objective position of both groups, it is widely accepted that ‘rural’is an increas-
ingly important social identity. By contrast, it is less clear whether ‘urban’is a meaningful iden-
tity.
5
We argue that it is meaningful for an important subgroup of urban dwellers. One way to
demonstrate this is by asking about people’s perceived closeness to ‘urban’and ‘rural’people
(for a validation of this question, see Bornschier et al. 2021b). In Appendix A (Figures A7 and
A8), we show that perceived closeness to urban people varies just as much with local population
density as perceived closeness to rural people. However, identification is weaker for both urban
and rural voters who grew up in a different type of place (Hegewald and Schraff 2022; Lunz
Trujillo 2022).
While there seem to be distinct identities on both sides of the urban-rural divide, this is less clear
for the existence of place-based resentment. Indeed, the literature has a clear focus on ‘rural resent-
ment’(Cramer 2016). Using a place resentment scale, Munis (2022) finds that place-based resent-
ment is much higher among rural than among urban Americans. However, the growing salience of
urban-rural conflicts in politics may mean that urban dwellers start to develop anti-rural resent-
ment for political reasons. In the US context, this is epitomized in the sentence ‘land does not
vote, people vote’in response to choropleth maps of electoral results. More generally, rural voters
may be seen as blocking important reforms in areas from climate policies to social liberalization.
In terms of parties’actual behaviour, finally, existing observational studies show that place-
based appeals are an important element of parties’repertoire. This is particularly true of rural
appeals, which parties regularly employ in both Austria (Huber 2022) and the UK (Thau
2019). Urban appeals are much less common in Austria, at least between 2013 and 2019
(Huber 2022). In the UK, by contrast, they have been employed about as often as rural appeals
between 1964 and 2015 (Thau 2019).
Data and Operationalization
To study group appeals across the urban-rural divide, we fielded two online survey experiments
with the survey company Bilendi. The first experiment was conducted in Germany and England
in September 2021. The second experiment was conducted in Germany in June and July 2022,
serving as a follow-up study for questions arising from the first study. In all analyses, we exclude
speeders and those (roughly 10 per cent) respondents who failed to pass a simple attention check.
In the following, we describe the design of the first survey in detail. When introducing the
follow-up survey below, we only explain where the design deviated from the first survey.
The design of both experiments follows established approaches from the literature on group-
based appeals (Dassonneville, Stubager, and Thau 2022; Robison et al. 2021). In both surveys, we
asked respondents to evaluate a candidate for parliament at the next national election.
6
Within
each survey, we hold constant the overall candidate description. In the first survey, we presented
the candidate as follows, ‘David Williams is 46 years old. He is an office clerk and has two kids.
He has been living in your constituency for 20 years and volunteers as a youth coach in a local
sports club.’In the second survey, we changed the candidate’s gender, name, and occupation to
make her a more authentic representative of urban interests (see below).
5
Empirically, roughly one-third of our urban sample from Study 2 strongly identify as urban (see Appendix Table E3).
While they are more likely to live in prototypical urban places –inner-city districts with high-rise buildings –their individual
characteristics are similar to weak urban identifiers.
6
This is a natural task in the candidate-centred British electoral system but may appear less natural in the party-centred
German system. However, since we fielded the survey during the 2021 election campaign, local candidates were very salient at
the time of the survey.
8 Lukas Haffert et al.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123424000218 Published online by Cambridge University Press
The treatment conditions then attach a certain appeal to this candidate and test whether this
affects the respondents’candidate evaluation. Our main outcome is the question: ‘How would you
rate a candidate with political views like those of David Williams?’The outcome measure then
varies between ‘0–I think very poorly of him’to ‘10 –I think very highly of him’.
We measure subjective place of residence pre-treatment to establish whether people understand
themselves as residing in a rural or urban context. For this, we use a four-category scale, which we
then dichotomize into ‘urban’and ‘rural’. In Germany, 62 per cent of respondents describe their
residence as ‘very urban’or ‘somewhat urban’; this number is 70 per cent in the UK.
7
To maximize
statistical power, we then expose all participants to a treatment that fits their subjective place of resi-
dence. In other words, there is in principle no mistreatment (Hersh and Schaffner 2013).
We measure the strength of place-based identity by showing the respondents five pairs of photos,
one of an urban and one of a rural environment, and have them choose in which of the two places
people are more like themselves ‘in terms of their lifestyle and their opinions’. These photos seek to
illustrate both the advantages and disadvantages of an urban or rural lifestyle (see the photos in
Appendix B).
8
Respondents who chose four or five photos matching their perceived place of resi-
dence are coded as having a strong place-based identity. We prefer this measure to a survey ques-
tion about attachment to one’s place of residence since the reaction to the photos better captures an
abstract identification with urban or rural places, not the very concrete identification with one’sspe-
cific place of residence.
9
This is important since we are interested in the mobilizing power of appeals
to urban/rural places as such, not in the power of specific appeals to respondents’local places.
Furthermore, our visual-based measure helps to mitigate concerns that especially the dominant
group may hesitate to convey a strong identity through conventional language-based items.
In Germany, 62 per cent of those who described their place as ‘very urban’picked four or five
urban photos while 92 per cent of those who described their place as ‘very rural’picked four or
five rural photos. In the UK, the respective numbers were 53 per cent and 90 per cent, respect-
ively. While rural identities are more widespread than urban identities, more than half of urban
respondents seem to have some form of urban identity.
We measure people’s place-based resentment using a five-question battery based on Munis
(2022). These questions ask whether people feel that their place-based in-group is disadvantaged
compared to the out-group in terms of their economic, cultural, or political situation (see the full
list of questions in Appendix B). Answers for all items range from 1 to 5, and we code everyone
with a mean across all items above 3 as having place-based resentment.
This setup of the survey was basically identical in the first and the second surveys. However,
the surveys differed regarding the target population, the description of the candidate, and the
content of the presented appeals. We first present the specific design and the results of the com-
parative survey before we move on to the German survey of urban respondents.
Results from Comparative Surveys in Germany and the UK
The survey experiment follows a multi-arm vignette design with a control group and three treat-
ment groups. This leads to a set of seven groups receiving separate appeals, as presented in
Table 1. Following the appeals literature (Robison et al. 2021), we differentiate between symbolic
and policy appeals. Symbolic appeals portray the political elite as an ally of some social group but
7
Appendix A shows that people’s self-categorization in terms of subjective residency correlates strongly with an objective
measure of urbanity.
8
In Sociology, Social Geography, and Environmental Studies, it is common to use photos to elicit respondents’preferences.
For a recent application in political science, see Wood et al. (2023). Peng, Strijker and Wu (2020). discuss how photo-
elicitation has been used in the study of place identities. See Appendix B for further discussion.
9
Appendix Table B3 shows the correlation matrix with strong links between our photo-based identity measure, self-
reported residence type, and urbanity. While the measure effectively captures respondents’residence, results show that the
measure extends beyond mere residence recall, reflecting our interest in a broader identification with urban places.
British Journal of Political Science 9
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123424000218 Published online by Cambridge University Press
are void of any specific policy statements (Hersh and Schaffner 2013). Policy appeals, by contrast,
try to appeal to a social group by elaborating on a substantive political issue that might be of con-
cern to this group.
The control group received the following generic appeal that does not engage with the urban-
rural divide: ‘I want to be a voice for everyone in our country. It is time to put the people at the
centre of politics.’All other appeals start with an identical frame, which presents politics as a con-
flict between urban and rural people: ‘Too much attention has been given to big cities/rural areas
in recent political debates.’
10
After this, we add the appeals. The symbolic appeal is formulated in
broad terms, appealing to the urban or rural groups but not mentioning any substantive societal
issues: ‘I want to be a voice for rural areas/our cities. It is time for politicians to prioritize rural
areas/our cities.’
Our policy appeals are derived from the place-based resentment literature, as outlined in the
theory part. The economic appeal implies competition regarding investment between urban and
rural areas: ‘I want to support economic development in rural areas/our cities. The government
needs to invest more in infrastructure and the creation of jobs in rural areas/our cities.’The cul-
tural appeal, by contrast, forwards a competition in lifestyles across the urban-rural divide: ‘I want
to fight for the lifestyle of people in rural areas/our cities. The government needs to support social
and cultural activities in rural areas/our cities.’
We collected nationally representative samples (according to gender, age [18–74], education,
and region/state) from around 2500 respondents. Our study is well-powered to uncover effect
sizes of 0.6 or more, assuming a standard deviation of 2.5 on the standard normally distributed
outcome and splitting the sample into two equally sized groups (for example, urban and rural
residents).
11
We can capture even smaller effects if we pool across treatment conditions or peo-
ple’s places of residence. Appendix C presents descriptive statistics for the control and treatment
groups. The means indicate that samples are balanced and randomization was successful.
The empirical analysis presents the results from ordinary least squares (OLS) regressions of can-
didate evaluation on the treatments. Figure 2 presents predicted values of the candidate evaluation
with 95 per cent confidence intervals over the type of place and the experimental conditions. The
complete regression results are presented in Appendix D. Overall, we see similar patterns in both
countries. There is no statistical difference between urban and rural residents’candidate evaluation
in the control group, suggesting that our neutral description of the candidate did not trigger an
urban or rural frame. Looking at the treatments, place-based appeals tend to increase candidate
evaluation among rural voters but consistently decrease the rating among urban respondents.
Among respondents with subjective rural residency, we find that cultural appeal increases candidate
evaluations by 0.6 points in Germany and 0.5 points in the UK. These average treatment effects
(ATEs) are statistically significant at the one and five per cent level, respectively. Economic and
symbolic appeals do not affect candidate evaluations among rural voters in England. In
Germany, both appeals improve candidate evaluations among rural respondents. The symbolic
appeal increases evaluations by 0.5 points, statistically significant at the 5 per cent level. The eco-
nomic appeal has an ATE of 0.3 and is only marginally significant at the 10 per cent level.
Table 1. Structure of the vignette experiment
Control group Treatment 1 Treatment 2 Treatment 3
Urban residence Control statement Symbolic appeal (Urban) Economic appeal (Urban) Cultural appeal (Urban)
Rural residence Symbolic appeal (Rural) Economic appeal (Rural) Cultural appeal (Rural)
10
This framing exactly follows the design of Robison et al. (2021) and Dassonneville et al. (2022).
11
Note that our actual sample does not have two equally sized groups along the urban-rural distinction. However, we could
not predict group shares in the subjective residence measure a priori, as respondents classified themselves as urban or rural. A
50/50 split was therefore our best guess for the power analysis.
10 Lukas Haffert et al.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123424000218 Published online by Cambridge University Press
We thus find clear, although not unequivocal, evidence for Hypothesis 1 in rural areas. Among
those with subjective urban residency, by contrast, all appeals strongly decrease candidate evalua-
tions. This negative response is significant at the 1 per cent level in all treatment conditions. The
cultural and economic appeals decrease candidate assessment by about 0.7 points compared to
the control group while the negative effect of the symbolic appeal is even bigger at around 1.1
points. These results are nearly identical across both countries. While rural voters seem to be
most susceptible to appeals that emphasize a cultural conflict, urban voters dislike all three
appeals to a similar extent.
In line with Hypothesis 3, these findings suggest that the effects of place-based appeals are
highly asymmetric: place-based appeals hurt candidates in an urban setting but they can benefit
their evaluation in a rural context. However, the strength of this asymmetry and the negative
reaction of urbanites to the urban appeal is remarkable and deserves further attention.
In the next step, we test whether different reactions of urban and rural voters can be explained
by a weaker place-based identity among urban voters (cf. Hypothesis 2). To do so, we collapse all
three treatments into a single category ‘treated’and compare all treated respondents to the non-
treated for each country. We interact the treatment variable with our measures of place-based
Figure 2. Main results –candidate evaluation: (a) Candidate evaluation Germany, (b) Candidate evaluation UK.
British Journal of Political Science 11
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123424000218 Published online by Cambridge University Press
identity. Figure 3 shows how respondents with weak and strong place-based identities react to the
treatment. Panel a) shows the results from Germany and panel b) the findings from the UK.
Respondents with a weak urban identity react negatively to the treatment in both countries. In
the UK, this effect disappears among high identifiers. That said, high identifiers still do not
rate the candidate better than the control candidate, meaning that the appeal may not hurt,
but neither does it benefit the candidate. In Germany, even strong urban identifiers react nega-
tively to the place-based appeal, although the effect is considerably smaller than among low
identifiers.
Unfortunately, in this survey, we only measured the respondents’place-based resentment after
the treatment since we had pre-registered a hypothesis on the effect of treatment on place-based
resentment. Hence, this measure might be confounded by the treatment. We thus only look into
the effect of resentment in the second survey. Moreover, we also pre-registered a range of hypoth-
eses about the different types of appeals (symbolic/economic/cultural) and subgroup effects (by
age or political ideology). These were meant to explore variation within the group of urban and
Figure 3. Strength of place-based identity and candidate evaluation. (a) Candidate evaluation Germany. (b) Candidate
evaluation UK.
12 Lukas Haffert et al.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123424000218 Published online by Cambridge University Press
rural respondents. However, our results indicate that the most important question is not the vari-
ation within these groups but the massive difference between these groups. While our results
indicate that place-based appeals are a promising strategy for rural candidates, we see no evidence
for positive reactions even among high-identifying urban voters to any of our appeals.
Hence, rather than studying within-group variation, we choose to focus on the big difference
between urban and rural residents. To understand the reactions of the dominant side of the
urban-rural divide to place-based appeals better and to discern why a majority of urbanites reject
urban appeals, we fielded a second survey in Germany that focused on urban voters, testing our
argument on the relevance of harmonious appeals for dominant groups (see Hypothesis 3). As
negative reactions among urban voters were very similar in Germany and England, we think
that just focusing on Germany still speaks to the broader puzzle.
Results From Urban Survey in Germany
While repeating the fundamental setup of the first survey among urban voters in Germany, we
introduce two major types of variation in the design of the follow-up survey. Firstly, we increase
the specificity of the policy proposal in the appeals, since one reason for the failure of urban treat-
ments in Study 1 could be that they do not pick up relevant policy concerns. Secondly, we vary
whether urban and rural interests are presented as antagonistic or harmonious, following our the-
oretical argument on the reluctance of dominant groups to endorse antagonistic appeals. In add-
ition, we also changed the demographic attributes of the fictitious candidate and presented the
candidate as follows: ‘Carolin Reuss is 42 years old. She is a graphic designer and a single mother
of a daughter. She has been living in your constituency for 20 years and volunteers in the admin
team of a local climbing hall.’This allows us to see whether urban voters may have considered the
original candidate as an inauthentic advocate of urban issues because of his gender, occupation,
or lifestyle.
For the follow-up survey, we recruited a total of 2850 inhabitants from the 150 biggest German
cities (that is, all cities with more than 60,000 inhabitants). Of these respondents, 54 per cent
described their place of residence as ‘very urban’,39percentas‘rather urban’, 6 per cent as ‘some-
what urban’, and 1.5 per cent as ‘not urban’(see the descriptive statistics in Appendix E). Our
results are robust to excluding the two latter groups. This sampling strategy allows us to focus
on the main puzzle emerging from Study 1, namely the strong negative reactions to political appeals
among urban respondents.
As a control condition, we used the same generic appeal as in Survey 1. As our first treatment,
we repeat the urban economic appeal from Study 1 to see whether we could replicate our initial
findings. As Fig. 4 demonstrates, urban voters again react negatively to this appeal (complete regres-
sion results can be found in Appendix F). Even if we describe the candidate in a way that more
closely fits the image of an urban lifestyle, respondents dislike the antagonistic group-based appeal
just as much. Thus, it seems that a lack of authenticity was not the problem with the original appeal.
We use the rural economic appeal from Study 1 as our second treatment to test whether urban
respondents may prefer a pro-rural (out-group) appeal. Exposing a subgroup of respondents to
the supposed out-group appeal is similar to the design of Robison et al. (2021), who administered
a working-class appeal not just to the working class but also to middle and upper-middle-class
respondents. Similarly, Dassonneville, Stubager, and Thau (2022) expose all respondents to the
same working class/rural/non-graduate/young appeal. Hence, they also administer the same
appeal as an in-group- and an out-group appeal.
Figure 4 shows that urban voters also dislike the rural appeal. That is, they do not just reject a
specific type of place-based appeal for targeting the wrong group but, rather, reject any type of
place-based appeal. This negative reaction demonstrates that the original finding is not due to
the fact that people perceive rural people as their in-group or have strong out-group concerns.
Even low-educated urbanites do not show any sign of class-based solidarity with ( potentially
British Journal of Political Science 13
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123424000218 Published online by Cambridge University Press
assumed, similarly lower-educated) rural voters (see Appendix Figure F4). Such a negative reac-
tion to appeals to a deserving out-group is a relatively uncommon finding. Dassonneville,
Stubager, and Thau (2022) find that people react neutrally to out-group appeals in most cases.
The exception is appeals to the young, which are actively disliked by old respondents. Yet
young people are significantly more positive towards this appeal. In our case, however, urban
respondents dislike pro-urban and pro-rural appeals just as much. This suggests that urban voters
have a more general preference for attenuating the urban-rural divide, no matter from which side
it is polarized. This is in line with our argument on the behaviour of dominant groups and with
broader insights on the political interest representation of advantaged groups (Weber 2020).
In addition to these replications and extensions, we introduce four new appeals. Here, the most
important change concerns how the appeal presents the relationship between urban and rural
citizens. In Study 1, all three appeals clearly depicted an antagonistic us-them dichotomy, a con-
flict between urban and rural areas. As demonstrated above, the lower level of place-based resent-
ment among urbanites may partly explain their negative reaction to this framing. However, even
among the resentful urbanites, the original treatments did not generate positive effects. We thus
vary whether respondents are confronted with a similarly antagonistic appeal or whether the
appeal emphasized the harmonious interests of all voters while still catering to urban interests.
Secondly, we also vary the specificity of the candidate’s proposal and seek to address it to dif-
ferent aspects of the urban-rural divide. On the one hand, we introduced an appeal that addresses
cities as beneficiaries of the transition to the knowledge economy.
The antagonistic version of this appeal read as follows:
Germany is falling behind with regard to innovations and technologies, since policymakers
have given too much attention to the economic interests of rural areas. The government
needs to invest more into the generation of future-proof jobs and the creation of innovation
centers in our cities.
By contrast, the harmonious appeal reads as follows:
Germany is falling behind with regard to innovations and technologies, since policymakers
have given too little attention to the future. The government needs to invest more into the gen-
eration of future-proof jobs and the creation of innovation centers in the whole country.
Figure 4. Effect of treatment on candidate evaluation, Study 2.
14 Lukas Haffert et al.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123424000218 Published online by Cambridge University Press
Moreover, we introduce two appeals on the issue of housing, which addresses problems arising
from the growth of cities. Housing has been one of the most discussed political issues in
German cities in recent years. Hence, rent should be a topic of high salience to urban voters
and one where they see a shared group interest with other urbanites. In the antagonistic version,
the rise in costs of housing is attributed to the influx of rural people. Investment in housing in
this case is geared towards alleviating the worries of urban residents and their rent costs. The
appeal is formulated as follows:
More and more people in our cities suffer from rising rents. Because of the influx from the
countryside, people who have been living there for many years cannot afford the rent in
their district anymore. I want to invest in affordable housing for the people in our cities.
The government needs to stop the influx from the countryside and to improve the housing situ-
ation in the cities.
In the other condition, we refrain from pitting urban against rural constituencies and present the
suggested housing policies as benefitting people across the country. This of course hides the fact
that high rent prices are primarily an issue for urban residents. The appeal, therefore, pitches a
policy that is very much tuned towards the urban in-group but sells it as a beneficial policy for
everyone. This appeal reads as follows:
More and more people in our country suffer from rising rents. Increasingly, people who have
been renting for many years cannot afford their flats anymore. I want to invest in affordable
housing. The government needs to stop rent increases across the country.
We expect that, compared to the appeals presented to urban voters in the first survey, increasing
the specificity of the appeal and reducing how antagonistic it is should result in higher approval of
the candidate by urban voters. Especially, the non-antagonistic framing of pro-housing policies
should bolster the approval of the candidate amongst urban constituencies as it presents a
clear in-group interest in a way that hides the group’s dominance.
The mid-part of Fig. 4 shows that the specificity of the policy has, at most, minor effects. The
antagonistic innovation appeal is evaluated just as negatively as the original economic appeal
while the antagonistic housing appeal is evaluated slightly but is not significantly better. Thus,
all four appeals that present the urban-rural divide as an antagonistic, group-based conflict are
negatively evaluated by urban respondents –no matter whom politicians seek to benefit in
this conflict and which policy they propose to adopt.
Yet our findings show that urban voters react positively to appeals that use a more harmonious
language. As soon as the same policies are presented as benefitting the whole country, respondents
evaluate them positively. This is most clearly the case for housing. The promise to tackle an issue
that is of primary concern in urban areas receives a very positive reaction among urban voters when
it is presented as helping people ‘across the country’. However, even the somewhat less specific
innovation appeal is evaluated positively when presented in a harmonious framework.
To assess how urban respondents evaluate the candidate in more depth, we draw on post-
treatment questions about the candidate’s credibility, competence, and local understanding. We
find that all these more specific candidate evaluations exhibit similar patterns and align with the
overall assessment presented in the main analysis (see Appendix Figure F5). Urbanites find a can-
didate making harmonious appeals more credible, while antagonistic appeals lead to lower ratings
in terms of credibility, competence, and the candidate’s understanding of local issues.
This is in line with our argument on the asymmetric reaction of dominant and subordinate
groups to group-based appeals. While subordinate groups support the antagonistic framing of
appeals as they reference a legitimate fight against social inequalities, dominant groups prefer
de-emphasizing inequalities and conflicts. This finding is in line with insights from social
British Journal of Political Science 15
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123424000218 Published online by Cambridge University Press
psychology that demonstrate the strong role of guilt and fairness concerns in understanding pri-
vileged individuals’reactions to inequality (Chudy, Piston, and Shipper 2019; Powell,
Branscombe, and Schmitt 2005), as well as with Dahrendorfian conflict sociology. Our findings
indicate that dominant groups such as urban dwellers actively punish politicians for polarizing
societal conflict. This shows that individuals do not just adjust their own behaviour in reaction
to dominance, as the psychological literature indicates, but that dominant groups also adjust
their expectations towards the behaviour of political elites accordingly.
In this survey, we measured both identity and resentment pre-treatment. This allows us to
observe to what extent a combination of identity and resentment affects the results –although
the small number of respondents in this group leads to imprecise estimates. We therefore pool
the innovation and rent treatment.
12
As expected, those respondents who combine urban identity
and urban resentment are indeed considerably more positive towards the antagonistic appeals.
That said, they still do not rate the antagonistic appeals better than the control appeal. Hence,
the antagonistic appeal does not even work in the most likely condition (Fig. 5).
Alternative Explanations
An alternative interpretation of these results is that they are less about the implicit degree of conflict
but rather about the implicit justifications provided in the appeals. After all, an important difference
between antagonistic and harmonious appeals is that antagonistic appeals provide ‘pluralistic’jus-
tifications, aimed at advancing the interests of a group, while harmonious appeals provide ‘repub-
lican’justifications, appealing to the public good (Rehfeld 2009; Wolkenstein and Wratil 2021).
Urban citizens might have a specific self-conception of championing universal values and pol-
icies that are advancing the greater good of the entire society, not just their own places. Hence,
even if these respondents would not reject the ‘us-them’setting of the antagonistic appeals in gen-
eral, they might reject these appeals because of their group-centred justifications.
To test this interpretation, we asked respondents about their representation ideals; that is,
whether they preferred politicians to focus their work on improving the situation of their constitu-
ents or on the public good. While there was a quite substantial variation in respondents’represen-
tation ideals, they contributed relatively little to the explanation of candidate evaluations. As Fig. 6
shows, urban respondents with a universalist representation ideal always evaluated the candidate
slightly but insignificantly more positively. However, this is also the case for antagonistic appeals,
which universalists should dislike more strongly than particularists (full regression in Appendix G).
Our results could also be driven by urbanites who perceive the out-group (rural people) as
more deserving than the in-group.
13
Negative reactions to the antagonistic appeals would then
be due to out-group concern and not hinge on the framing of the societal divide as either antag-
onistic or harmonious.
Such an explanation is already put into question by the fact that urbanites actively dislike the
rural appeal as well. To further probe this explanation, we distinguish the reactions of urbanites
who rate rural dwellers as highly deserving from those who attribute less deservingness to the
out-group.
14
Figure 7 shows that both urbanites who perceive the out-group as deserving and
those who are less out-group oriented react similarly negatively to the antagonistic frame.
Moreover, the reactions of both respondent groups are similar for the harmonious appeals, to
which both react more positively (see the full regression in Appendix G).
12
Since very few urban respondents combine low identification and high resentment, we collapse the resentment categories
for the low identity group. Appendix G1 also shows the results for the interaction with the rent and innovation treatment
separately.
13
Deservingness perceptions are an important mediator investigated in Dassonneville, Stubager and Thau (2022).
14
We measure deservingness with a question about which groups deserve particularly strong support by politics.
Respondents who answered that people in rural places deserve more support than urban people were coded as attributing
a high deservingness to their rural out-group.
16 Lukas Haffert et al.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123424000218 Published online by Cambridge University Press
Moreover, the negative reaction to antagonistic appeals by urbanites could be driven by those
residing in objectively well-off places, where the proposed policies are less relevant. We thus
measure the objective economic conditions in the cities of our urban respondents and show
the different reactions of those who live in places with low/high welfare quotas.
15
As with the
other subgroup analyses, we find relatively little variation in terms of economic conditions or
hardship in the respective place of residence (see the full regression in Appendix G) (Fig. 8 ).
Moreover, in Appendix G we distinguish urbanites according to their length of residence in
the city in which they live. Overall, the results show that there is almost no difference between
urbanites who have lived in the respective city their entire life and those who have lived there
less than five years.
Last, we analyze whether the effect of our group appeals is driven by respondents’assumptions
about the candidates’partisanship. Whereas we did not present a party affiliation of our fictitious
candidate, we asked respondents to indicate how likely they thought that the candidate was
affiliated with each of the parties represented in the German parliament. Figure 9 shows the aver-
age association of each appeal (and appealing candidate) with one of the six parties (the scale
ranged from ‘not at all likely’[1] to ‘very likely’[4]).
Figure 5. Identity and resentment among
urbanites, Study 2.
Figure 6. Effect of treatment on candidate
evaluation by representation ideal, Study 2.
15
We use the population share receiving social assistance (SGBII quota). Cities with a share below/above the mean (11.65)
are coded as having a low/high welfare quota.
British Journal of Political Science 17
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123424000218 Published online by Cambridge University Press
The innovation appeal is more closely associated with the policy programme of centre-right
parties (CDU and FDP), while the rent appeal is perceived to come from a left party (SPD or
LINKE). Interestingly, there is very little variation for the Greens, and the AfD is most strongly
associated with antagonistic appeals. Since urban respondents dislike the rent and the innovation
appeals to a similar extent, it does not seem to play a major role whether the appeal is more
strongly perceived to come from the right or the left.
Conclusion
In this paper, we study the conditions under which the mobilization of social groups by politi-
cians appealing to them works or backfires. Using the case of place-based appeals across the
urban-rural divide, we argue that the success of group-based appeals hinges on the targeted
groups’relative position in a social divide. While subordinate social groups buy into antagonistic
appeals that mobilize against the dominant group, dominant groups react negatively to attempts
to appeal to them by polarizing existing inequalities.
We test this argument with two survey experiments on how urban and rural voters react to
political appeals that target people`s subjective places of residence. Our findings show that it is
indeed possible to garner support from rural voters with place-based appeals; candidates are eval-
uated significantly more positively when they address rural voters with such appeals, especially
Figure 7. Effect of treatment on candidate
evaluation by out-group deservingness,
Study 2.
Figure 8. Effect of treatment on candidate
evaluation by objective economic condi-
tions, Study 2.
18 Lukas Haffert et al.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123424000218 Published online by Cambridge University Press
when these appeals focus on cultural issues. By contrast, urban voters actively oppose appeals that
pitch urban and rural places against each other.
In a follow-up survey of urban voters, we find that the antagonistic framing of place-based
appeals produces a strong negative reaction among urban residents. However, more harmonious
appeals to urban interests that emphasize the common societal good are successful and substan-
tially increase candidate approval. This demonstrates that the success of conflictual appeals hinges
on the type of social group to which they are directed.
Compared to the existing survey-based literature on group-based appeals, we thus find an
important new pattern. While it is not uncommon to find that group-based appeals may have
null effects among certain groups of voters, it is rare that groups react negatively to such appeals
(the only example is Robison et al. [2021], who report a negative reaction among
upper-middle-class voters in their Appendix). Introducing new evidence on the urban-rural div-
ide, we uncover a broader puzzle around the negative reaction of advantaged groups to group-
based political appeals. Our findings show that urban respondents react decisively negatively
to appeals that cater to them as a group, be they economic, cultural, or symbolic appeals.
Importantly, these voters also react negatively to group-based appeals that emphasize the other
side of the divide, that is, rural voters. Thus, urban voters reject any appeal that presents an
urban-rural divide as a group-based conflict. At the same time, these voters are not critical of
the policies proposed in these appeals –they react positively to the same policy proposals
when they are presented in a harmonious framework.
A few tentative conclusions can be drawn from our study with respect to the conditions under
which group-based appeals might work or backfire. While our study is empirically restricted to the
case of urban-rural divides, the findings may hold broader importance for the analysis of group-
based appeals. After all, the fundamental setup of this divide is quite common –it is very clear
which of the two groups is seen as being on the winning side of the conflict and which of the
two groups is perceived as being on the losing side. This also holds for other group-based divides
such as class and education, which are often studied in the literature on group appeals. Hence, we
may speculate that group-based appeals would also have an asymmetric effect on those divides (as
suggested by Robison et al.’s[2021] finding). Indeed, it may be common that the dominant group
Figure 9. The average likelihood of candidate party affiliation by condition, Study 2
British Journal of Political Science 19
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123424000218 Published online by Cambridge University Press
actively rejects a framing that presents them as a group that is in conflict with another group.
However, the situation is less clear for divides based on race or gender, where the historically dom-
inant social groups may perceive their position as threatened, such as whites in the United States.
Here, antagonistic appeals may potentially be able to tap into some group members’perception of
status threat. At the same time, they may still trigger negative emotions of self-directed anger and
guilt among other group members, making the total effect indeterminate.
Potentially, another scope condition could concern the reaction of disadvantaged groups to
harmonious appeals. These groups could oppose a harmonious framing of a divide that is per-
ceived to exist at their cost. Since our analysis focused on dominant groups, we have no evidence
of this. However, because our results show similarly positive responses among rural voters across
a range of substantively different appeals (see also the findings of Dassonneville, Stubager, and
Thau 2022; Robison et al. 2021), we suspect that disadvantaged groups are likely to approve of
a wide range of appeals (including harmonious ones). Overall, this would suggest that harmoni-
ous appeals do not face similar scope conditions as antagonistic appeals.
For future research on the scope conditions under which group-based appeals work, we thus think
that itwould be most pertinent to focus onantagonistic appeals and to further disentangle the specific
contribution of status level and status threat to voters’perceptions of group dominance and their reac-
tions to group-based appeals. Do they have an additive or an interactive impact on the reaction to such
appeals? Moreover, such research should also investigate the formation of status and conflict percep-
tions across major societal dividesand should be open to the possibility of country-specific differences.
It might very well be possible that perceptions of group dominance differ systematically among group
members; for example, between different regions in biggercountries. This promises to generate a more
general model for the effectiveness of political appeals based on perceived group status.
Supplementary material. The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/
S0007123424000218.
Data availability statement. Replication data for this article can be found in Harvard Dataverse at: https://doi.org/10.7910/
DVN/OM7ZWL.
Acknowledgements. We would like to thank Diane Bolet, Björn Bremer, Katherine Cramer, Mahmoud Farag, Alexander
Horn, Rune Stubager, Mads Thau, Linus Westheuser, and Delia Zollinger, as well as all seminar participants at the Konstanz
Inequality Conference 2022, the DVPW Political Economy section conference 2022, the DPSA conference 2022, the SPSA
conference 2022, the CES conference 2023, the Universities of Aalborg, Bergen, Geneva, Mannheim and Zurich; the editor
Ruth Dassonneville, and three anonymous reviewers for helpful comments.
Financial support. This work was supported by the URPP Equality of Opportunity Programme, University of Zurich.
Competing interests. None.
Ethical standards. The research was reviewed and approved by the ETH Zurich Ethics Commission, application number
EK-2021-N-97.
References
Bornschier S et al. (2021a) Identity Formation between Structure and Agency –How ‘Us’and ‘Them’Relates to Voting
Behavior in Contexts of Electoral Realignment.
Bornschier S et al. (2021b) How “Us”and “them”relates to voting behavior –social structure, social identities, and electoral
choice. Comparative Political Studies 54(12), 2087–22. https://doi.org/10.1177/0010414021997504
Burgoon B et al. (2019) Positional deprivation and support for radical right and radical left parties. Economic Policy 34(97),
49–93. https://doi.org/10.1093/epolic/eiy017
Chudy J, Piston S and Shipper J (2019) Guilt by association: White collective guilt in American politics. Journal of Politics
81(3), 968–81.
Cramer KJ (2016) The Politics of Resentment –Rural Consciousness in Wisconsin and the Rise of Scott Walker. Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Cunningham N and Savage M (2015) The secret garden? Elite metropolitan geographies in the contemporary UK.
Sociological Review 63(2), 321–48.
20 Lukas Haffert et al.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123424000218 Published online by Cambridge University Press
Dahrendorf R (1959) Class and Class Conflict in Industrial Society. Stanford: Stanford University Press.
Dassonneville R, Stubager R and Thau M (2022) The Effectiveness of Group Appeals. Unpublished Manuscript.
Dolinsky AO (2023) Parties’group appeals across time, countries, and communication channels—examining appeals to
social groups via the parties’group appeals dataset. Party Politics 29(6), 1130–46.
Dover TL (2022) Not all inequalities are created equal: inequality framing and privilege threat for advantaged groups. Group
Processes & Intergroup Relations 25, 746–67.
Evans G and Tilley J (2017) The new Politics of Class in Britain: The Political Exclusion of the Working Class. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Evans G, Stubager R and Langsæther PE (2022) The conditional politics of class identity: Class origins, identity and political
attitudes in comparative perspective. West European Politics 45(6), 1178–205.
Fitzgerald J (2018) Close to Home: Local Ties and Voting Radical Right in Europe. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Ford R and Jennings W (2020) The changing cleavage politics of Western Europe. Annual Review of Political Science 23(1),
295–314. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-polisci-052217-104957
Friedman S, O’Brien D and McDonald I (2021) Deflecting privilege: class identity and the intergenerational self. Sociology
55(4), 716–33. https://doi.org/10.1177/0038038520982225
García del Horno R, Rico G and Hernández E (2023) Do they feel like they don’t matter? The rural-urban divide in external
political efficacy. West European Politics,1–26. https://doi.org/10.1080/01402382.2023.2261085
Gimpel JG, Lee FE and Thorpe RU (2011) The wellsprings of candidate emergence: Geographic origins of statewide can-
didacies in the United States. Political Geography 30(1), 25–37. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.polgeo.2010.12.005
Guth JL and Nelsen BF (2021) Party choice in Europe: Social cleavages and the rise of populist parties. Party Politics 27(3), 453–64.
Haffert L (2024) Unequal geographic representation in a mixed-member electoral system: Evidence from the German
Bundestag. German Politics 33(1), 86–109. https://doi.org/10.1080/09644008.2021.1982901
Haffert L, Palmtag T and Schraff D (2024) Replication Data for: When group appeals backfire: explaining the asymmetric
effects of place-based appeals. Available from https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/OM7ZWL, Harvard Dataverse, V1.
Harth NS, Kessler T and Leach CW (2008) Advantaged group’s emotional reactions to intergroup inequality: the dynamics
of pride, guilt, and sympathy. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 34(1), 115–29. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.
1177/0146167207309193
Häusermann S, Kurer T and Zollinger D (2023) Aspiration versus apprehension: Economic opportunities and electoral pre-
ferences. British Journal of Political Science 53(4), 1230–51. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123423000145
Hegewald S and Schraff D (2022) Is the urban-rural divide affectively polarised? Comparative evidence from nine European
countries. OSF Preprints, July 8. https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.io/ab3dg
Hersh ED and Schaffner BF (2013) Targeted campaign appeals and the value of ambiguity. The Journal of Politics 75(2),
520–34. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0022381613000182
Hooghe L and Marks G (2018) Cleavage theory meets Europe’s crises: Lipset, Rokkan, and the transnational cleavage.
Journal of European Public Policy 25(1), 109–35.
Hooghe L and Marks G (2022) The social roots of the transnational cleavage: education, occupation, and sex. Robert
Schuman Centre for Advanced Studies Research Paper No. 2022_53.
Hooghe L, Marks G and Kamphorst J (2022) Field of education and the transnational cleavage. Paper Presented at the
Conference “Cleavage Formation in the 21st Century,”University of Zurich, July 18/19th 2022.
Horn A et al. (2021) Political parties and social groups: New perspectives and data on group and policy appeals. Party Politics
27(5), 983–95. https://doi.org/10.1177/1354068820907998
Huber LM (2022) Beyond policy: The use of social group appeals in party communication. Political Communication 39(3),
293–310. https://doi.org/10.1080/10584609.2021.1998264
Huddy L (2013) From group identity to political cohesion and commitment. In Huddy L, Sears DO and Levy JS (eds), The
Oxford Handbook of Political Psychology. Oxford, Oxford University Press, 737–73.
Huijsmans T, Harteveld E, van der Brug W and Lancee B (2021) Are cities ever more cosmopolitan? Studying trends in
urban-rural divergence of cultural attitudes. Political Geography 86, 102353. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.polgeo.2021.102353
Iversen T and Soskice D (2019) Democracy and Prosperity: Reinventing Capitalism through a Turbulent Century. Princeton:
Princeton University Press.
Jacobs NF and Munis BK (2019) Place-based imagery and voter evaluations: Experimental evidence on the politics of place.
Political Research Quarterly 72(2), 263–77. https://doi.org/10.1177/1065912918781035
Jardina A (2019) White Identity Politics. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Kam CD, Archer AMN and Geer JG (2017) Courting the women’s vote: the emotional, cognitive, and persuasive effects of
gender-based appeals in campaign advertisements. Political Behavior 39(1), 51–75. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-016-9347-7
Kurer T (2020) The declining middle: Occupational change, social Status, and the populist right. Comparative Political
Studies 53(10–11), 1798–835. https://doi.org/10.1177/0010414020912283
Leach CW, Iyer A and Pedersen A (2006) Anger and guilt about ingroup advantage explain the willingness for political action.
Personality and Social Psyc hology Bulletin 32(9), 1232–45. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167206289729
Lin J and Trujillo KL (2023) Urban-Rural Residency, Place Identity, and Affective Polarization in the United States. https://
doi.org/10.31219/osf.io/97a3x
British Journal of Political Science 21
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123424000218 Published online by Cambridge University Press
Lunz Trujillo K (2022) Rural identity as a contributing factor to anti-intellectualism in the U.S. Political Behavior 44,1509–32.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-022-09770-w
Lyons J and Utych SM (2023) You’re not from here!: The consequences of urban and rural identities. Political Behavior 45,
75–101. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-021-09680-3
Mason L and Wronski J (2018) One tribe to bind them all: How our social group attachments strengthen partisanship.
Political Psychology 39, 257–77. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12485
Maxwell R (2019) Cosmopolitan immigration attitudes in large European cities: contextual or compositional effects?
American Political Science Review 672,1–19. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0003055418000898
Maxwell R (2020) Geographic divides and cosmopolitanism: evidence from Switzerland. Comparative Political Studies
53(13), 2061–90. https://doi.org/10.1177/0010414020912289
Munis B (2020) Divided by place: the enduring geographical fault lines of American politics. A Dissertation presented to
the Graduate Faculty of the University of Virginia for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy. Department of Politics,
University of Virginia.
Munis BK (2022) Us over here versus them over there …literally: Measuring place resentment in American politics. Political
Behavior 44, 1057–78. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-020-09641-2
Peng J, Strijker D and Wu Q (2020) Place identity: How far have we come in exploring its meanings? Frontiers in Psychology
11, 294. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00294
Phillips LT and Lowery BS (2018) Herd invisibility: The psychology of racial privilege. Current Directions in Psychological
Science 27(3), 156–62. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721417753600
Powell AA, Branscombe NR and Schmitt MT (2005) Inequality as ingroup privilege or outgroup disadvantage: the impact
of group focus on collective guilt and interracial attitudes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 31(4), 508–21. https://
doi.org/10.1177/0146167204271713
Rehfeld A (2009) Representation rethought: On trustees, delegates, and gyroscopes in the study of political representation
and democracy. American Political Science Review 103(2), 214–30. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0003055409090261
Robison J et al. (2021) Does class-based campaigning work? How working class appeals attract and polarize voters.
Comparative Political Studies 54(5), 723–52. https://doi.org/10.1177/0010414020957684
Rodríguez-Pose A (2018) The revenge of the places that don’t matter (and what to do about it). Cambridge Journal of
Regions, Economy and Society 11(1), 189–209. https://doi.org/10.1093/cjres/rsx024
Schulte-Cloos J and Bauer PC (2023) Local candidates, place-based identities, and electoral success. Political Behavior 45,
679–98.
Searle DM and Abrajano M (2018) Appealing to diverse electorates in the United States. In Suhay E, Grofman B and
Trechsel AH (eds), The Oxford Handbook of Electoral Persuasion. Oxford, Oxford University Press, 258–77.
Stubager R (2009) Education-based group identity and consciousness in the authoritarian-libertarian value conflict.
European Journal of Political Research 48, 204–33.
Stuckelberger S and Tresch A (2024) Group appeals of parties in times of economic and identity conflicts and realignment.
Political Studies 72(2), 463–85. https://doi.org/10.1177/00323217221123147
Thau M (2019) How political parties use group-based appeals: Evidence from Britain 1964–2015. Political Studies 67(1),
63–82. https://doi.org/10.1177/0032321717744495
Thau M (2021) The social divisions of politics: How parties’group-based appeals influence social group differences in vote
choice. The Journal of Politics 83(2), 675–88. https://doi.org/10.1086/710018
Traber D et al. (2022) Social status, political priorities and unequal representation. European Journal of Political Research
61(2), 351–73. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1111/1475-6765.12456
Weber T (2020) Discreet inequality: How party agendas embrace privileged interests. Comparative Political Studies
53(10–11), 1767–97.
Wolkenstein F and Wratil C (2021) Multidimensional representation. American Journal of Political Science 65(4), 862–76.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1111/ajps.12563
Wood M et al. (2023) What do “left behind communities”want? A qualitative study in the United Kingdom using photo
elicitation. American Political Science Review 117(4), 1173–87.
Zollinger D (2024) Cleavage Identities in voters’own words: Harnessing open-ended survey responses. American Journal of
Political Science 68(1), 139–59.
Cite this article: Haffert L, Palmtag T, Schraff D (2024). When Group Appeals Backfire: Explaining the Asymmetric Effects
of Place-Based Appeals. British Journal of Political Science 1–22. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123424000218
22 Lukas Haffert et al.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123424000218 Published online by Cambridge University Press