ArticlePDF Available

A genome-wide association study of occupational creativity and its relations with well-being and career success

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Creativity is one defining characteristic of human species. There have been mixed findings on how creativity relates to well-being, and little is known about its relationship with career success. We conduct a large-scale genome-wide association study to examine the genetic architecture of occupational creativity, and its genetic correlations with well-being and career success. The SNP-h² estimates range from 0.08 (for managerial creativity) to 0.22 (for artistic creativity). We record positive genetic correlations between occupational creativity with autism, and positive traits and well-being variables (e.g., physical height, and low levels of neuroticism, BMI, and non-cancer illness). While creativity share positive genetic overlaps with indicators of high career success (i.e., income, occupational status, and job satisfaction), it also has a positive genetic correlation with age at first birth and a negative genetic correlation with number of children, indicating creativity-related genes may reduce reproductive success.
Content may be subject to copyright.
communications biology Article
https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-024-06686-5
A genome-wide association study of
occupational creativity and its relations
with well-being and career success
Check for updates
Wen-Dong Li 1, Xin Zhang 2,KailiYu
1,YimoZhu
3, Nianyao Du4,ZhaoliSong
3&QiaoFan 5
Creativity is one dening characteristic of human species. There have been mixed ndings on how
creativity relates to well-being, and little is known about its relationship with career success. We
conduct a large-scale genome-wide association study to examine the genetic architecture of
occupational creativity, and its genetic correlations with well-being and career success. The SNP-h2
estimates range from 0.08 (for managerial creativity) to 0.22 (for artistic creativity). We record positive
genetic correlations between occupational creativity with autism, and positive traits and well-being
variables (e.g., physical height, and low levels of neuroticism, BMI, and non-cancer illness). While
creativity share positive genetic overlaps with indicators of high career success (i.e., income,
occupational status, and job satisfaction), it also has a positive genetic correlation with age at rst birth
and a negative genetic correlation with number of children, indicating creativity-related genes may
reduce reproductive success.
Creativity plays a crucial role in shaping not only the optimal functioning of
individuals, but also the development and well-being of human societies and
civilizations13.Asignicant amount of research endeavors has thus been
devoted to identifying what factors contribute to creativity. Among these
endeavors, individual characteristics have received ample research attention
in the literature, which perhaps dates back to Galtons landmark research on
the heredity of creative genius4. Indeed, twin studies have shown sizable
genetic inuences on creativity57. Recent molecular genetics research has
further revealed specic genetic variants that may be responsible for the
heritability of creativity8,9.
Genetic research on creativity has shed light on the notion that crea-
tivity is perhaps one of the few dening characteristics of the human
species10, the importance of which looms large in the new era of articial
intelligence11. Yet, this line of inquiry is not without limitations. First,
affected by the proposition that creativity is domain specic12,13,theprior
research on adult creativity has primarily concentrated on either specic
occupations (e.g., artists or scientists) or a narrow group of people with
unique creative characteristics with small samples14. This has limited the
scope of creativity research, because creativity matters for a broader spec-
trum of work and occupations beyond artists and scientists13. Second, the
lack of a more inclusive and comprehensive approach to creativity has also
hampered the investigation of the genetic architecture of creativity using
large samples that allow us to tackle important questions in the eld, such as
whether and how the genetic architecture of creativity overlaps with that of
health and well-being variables. The stereotype that there may be a positive
correlation between creativity and mental disorderperhaps dating back to
Aristotle15suggests a possible negative phenotypic correlation between
creativity and well-being1619. A recent meta-analysis, however, imposes a
challenge to this stereotype by reporting a small, but signicant positive
phenotypic correlation (.14) between creativity and well-being20. Probing
genetic correlations between creativity and well-being may contribute to a
more nuanced understanding of the genetic etiology and the nature of such
relationships21. Third, another stereotype of creative people is that they often
struggle with their careers22,23. This has in fact been supported by census
data24,25 showing that artists often earn less incomean objective indicator
of career success26than counterparts in other occupations, suggesting a
negative phenotypic correlation between creativity and career success.
Research has also demonstrated that artists experienced higher levels of job
satisfactiona subjective indicator of career successthan incumbents in
other occupations27, which suggests a positive phenotypic relationship
between creativity and career success. Thus, a large-scale study on the nature
and genetic etiology of the relationship between creativity and a relatively
1Department of Management, CUHK Business School, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong, China. 2Department of Human Resource Management,
School of Business, Shanghai University of Finance and Economics, Shanghai, China. 3Department of Management and Organization, National University of
Singapore, Singapore, Singapore. 4Department of Statistics and Data Science, National University of Singapore, Singapore, Singapore. 5Centre for Quantitative
Medicine, Duke-NUS Medical School, National University of Singapore, Singapore, Singapore. e-mail: oceanbluepsy@gmail.com;xinzhang@sufe.edu.cn;
bizszl@nus.edu.sg;qiao.fan@duke-nus.edu.sg
Communications Biology | (2024) 7:1092 1
1234567890():,;
1234567890():,;
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
more comprehensive spectrum of career success variables (e.g., income,
occupation status, job satisfaction, and reproductive success) may shed light
on the inconsistent ndings on the phenotypic relationship between crea-
tivity and career success21.
We thus conducted a large-scale genome-wide association study using
data from the U.K. biobank (N= 219,473) and three independent replica-
tion samples (N= 26,975) to examine the above three questions. The
objective of this research is three-fold. First, we draw from the literatures on
creativity and organizational research28 and developed two more inclusive
and comprehensive creativity measures to capture creativity in a broader
fashion. Creativity is often dened as the generation of ideas, products,
problem solutions, approaches and practices that are both novel and
useful1,3,29. Creativity is a multifaceted construct and has been shown to be
very difcult to assess13,30. Prior research on adult creativity has primarily
concentrated on artistic and scientic creativity14 and thus neglected another
important form of creativity related to novel and useful management
practices and approaches in organizations, which we call managerial crea-
tivity (e.g., the introduction of modern assembly line)31. Indeed, the litera-
ture in organizational research has highlighted that managerial jobs,
particularly those at senior and leadership levels, require job incumbents to
come up with novel and appropriate ideas and practices to initiate changes
in the workplace and to deal with new challenges in the ever-changing
business world28,3234. Such novel challenges loom large in todaysbusiness
world wherein extant approaches to manage customersneeds, supply
chains, and employee motivation and turnover are constantly interrupted
and modied by broader political, economic, and technological changes34,35.
Accordingly, with our rst measure of occupational creativity,wedis-
tinguished the three types of creativity (artistic, scientic, and managerial) as
they are reected in the three categories of occupations based on the U.K.
Standard Occupational Classication (SOC) 2000 systems (we also com-
bined the three creative occupations to general a categorial variable of overall
occupational creativity). Furthermore, in order to provide a common metric
to assess creativity across various occupations (because job incumbents in
occupations beyond the three creative occupations may also exhibit high
levels of creativity), we capitalized on the literature on occupational classi-
cation/analyses and constructed an omnibus measure of creativity from
the U.S. Department of Labors Occupational Information Network
(O*NET)36 by a crosswalk linking the U.K. SOC system to the O*NET SOC
system37. We adopted three items from O*NET (e.g., tapping into partici-
pantsabilities to generate novel ideas and thinking creatively) to access
occupational creative achievement as a continuous variable. Employing the
two major measures of creativity enablesustofurtherexplorepossible
genetic polymorphisms at the whole genome level in our GWAS. To further
examine the distinction among the three different forms of creativity, we
also examine their different genetic underpinnings and how they are dis-
tinctively related to personal traits (e.g., intelligence, educational achieve-
ment, personality, and physical height) at the whole genome level (also for
well-being and career success variables).
Second, we investigated the genetic correlations between occupational
creativity and a number of health and well-being variables including bipolar
disorder and schizophrenia traditionally theorized and found as positive
correlates with creativity8,9,3841and others (e.g., subjective well-being,
BMI, autism, longevity, cannabis misuse, and alcohol use).
Third, we examined the genetic correlations between occupational
creativity with a wide range of important indicators of career success,
including income, occupation status, job satisfaction, and reproductive
success (number of children and age at rst birth). We also compared such
genetic correlations among the three different forms of creativity. We fur-
ther examined the genetic correlations between creativity and well-being
and career success variables after partialling out genetic inuences asso-
ciated with intelligence, an important cognitive predictor of creativity(3), as
well as educational achievement. Overall, our investigation contributes to
the scholarship on creativity by expanding its scope with including artistic,
scientic, managerial creativity and a more general metric of creative
achievement, examining the genetic architecture of occupational creativity
in a large-scale study, and probing the genetic correlations between occu-
pational creativity with well-being and career success.
Results
Phenotypical measures of occupational creativity
Due to its multifaceted nature, we assessed creativity with two measures (ve
variables) based on the major job responsibilities of participantsoccupa-
tions (Table S1). The rst was a categorial measure capturing three types of
occupational creativity: artistic, scientic, and managerial, as manifested in
occupations that require generating novel and useful ideas (in total four
binary variables). The literature has primarily focused on two forms of
creativity: artistic and scientic14. It has overlooked another form of crea-
tivity in managerial and leadership jobsmanagerial creativity, the
importance of which looms large when organizations face ever-changing
business environments to deal with novel challenges related tosupply-chain,
customer needs, and employee management34,35. We thus further included a
third category: managerial creativity. We captured the three types of
occupational creativity via categorizing participantsoccupations based on
the U.K. SOC 2000 system into creative occupations (artistic, scientic, and
managerial) according to the major responsibilities of the occupations that
require generating new and appropriate ideas (Method). In addition to
treating the three types of creative occupations (artistic, scientic, and
managerial occupations versus conventional/noncreative occupations) as
three binary variables, we also combined them to generate a variable of
overall occupational creativity (i.e., 1 =either of the three creative occupa-
tions, 0 = conventional occupations). Thus the rst categorical measure
yielded four binary creativity variables: artistic, scientic, managerial, and
overall. In order to assess occupational creativity from a broader scope
beyond the three types of occupational creativity with a common metric
across various types of occupations, our second measure of creativit y gauged
creative achievement using three items from O*NET by linking the U.K.
SOC 2000 occupation codes in the U.K. Biobank (UKB) data to the occu-
pation codes from O*NET37.
In our GWAS analyses with the UKB discovery data, we included
125,803 participants of European ancestry with 67,848 classied as holding
creative occupations that have high levels of occupational creativity, and
57,955 as holding conventional (less creative) occupations (Table 1,S2,S3&
Figure S1). Among participants with creative occupations, 6,625 were
artists, 18,225 scientists, and 42,998 managers. In addition, 219,473 parti-
cipants with available data on the creative achievement phenotype were
included in the discovery stage. The average creative achievement score was
3.24 ranging from 1.03 to 4.74.
In our replication analyses, we included three independent datasets: the
UKB follow-up dataset (N= 23,249), Add Health Wave IV dataset
(N= 1,461), and Wisconsin Longitudinal Study (WLS; N= 2,265). The
mean age (years) of participants was 59.8 in the UKB follow-up dataset
(32.72% male), 37.8 in the Add Health dataset (37.60% males), and 68.4 in
the WLS cohort (35.79% males; Table S5).
GWAS for occupational creativity
We conducted GWAS analyses on 9,804,641 variants that passed quality
control (QC) with a minor allele frequency (MAF) of more than 1% for the
ve creativity variables in UKB discovery data (see method and ref)42.The
linkage disequilibrium (LD) score regression intercept, which ranged from
1.004 to 1.049, indicated the expected polygenicity for these traits43 (see
Table S6 and Fig. 1and S2 for Manhattan plots).
For GWAS performed in this study, a locus was dened by an index
lead SNP (P<5×10
8)withitsanking 500-kb region in either direction.
Nineteen loci were identied to be signicantly linked to one of these ve
creativity variables at the P<1.0×10
8, with P-value accounted for multiple
testing correction for ve traits. Six were shared by at least two traits (see
Tables S71 and Figures S3 and S4 for QQ plots). Additionally, 12 more loci
displayed conventional genome-wide signicance for either of these ve
creativity variables, with a P-value of < 5.0 × 108(see Tables S71). Alto-
gether 31 loci were carried out for the replication.
https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-024-06686-5 Article
Communications Biology | (2024) 7:1092 2
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
We subsequently evaluated associations of these top loci with the
creativity variables in the three independent replication cohorts (see above).
All analyses were adjusted for age, sex and main principal components. For a
total of 43 variants at 31 top loci identied in UKB discovery, 28 variants at
19 loci showed genome-wide signicant for different traits at 1.0 × 108in
the meta-analysis (see Tables S71). Three proxy variants (r2> 0.9 with the
lead variant) were selected since the lead variant was not presented in the
meta-analysis results. Tables S72 summarizes ndings of our meta-
analyses based on both the discovery and replication samples. No hetero-
geneity was observed for the effect size across cohorts for these replicated
variants (P0.0737, see Tables S72).
Among the 19 top loci, ve were identied as signicant for overall
occupational creativity, one for artistic creativity at chromosome 18 gene
CELF4, one for managerial creativity and scientic creativity at
chromosome 6 MIR2113/PNKY, and three for scientic creativity (chro-
mosome 1 NEGR1, chromosome 3 GPX1/USP4/NICN1, chromosome 5
NDUFAF2/PART1/ZSWIM6). Several top loci identied for creativity traits
were overlapped with the genes previously reported for psychiatric traits
such as schizophrenia (ZSWIM6)40, bipolar disorder (micro RNA
MIR2113)41, neuroticism (CELF4)44, and autism (NEGR1)45.
The identied top loci harbor genes involved in various neuronal
functions. For example, gene NEGR1 encodes protein neuronal growth
regulator 1 (Negr1) of 46KDa. Negr1 is highly expressed in the cerebral
cortex, hippocampus, and cerebellum in the brain, and is a member of the
immunoglobulin LON family46. It accumulates in the neurotransmitter
GABAergic inhibitory synapses of neurons47. As neurotransmitters play a
crucial role in creativity48, the mutation of NEGR1 may have implications
for the trait of creativity. Another gene CELF4 (CUGBP,ELAV-likefamily
4) is related to a neural RNA-binding protein, predominantly expressed in
the central nervous system. CELF4 plays a critical role in various neuronal
functions and development, particularly in RNA processing49.
Sex-specic analyses were also conducted, which identied nine novel
loci for either males and females (see Table S8). Among these, one novel
signal was found for the creative achievement variable at gene SALL1 on
chromosome 16 in males, while two novel signals were identied at gene
NSUN3 on chromosome 3 and gene MAPKAP1 on chromosome 9 in
females (also see Figures S5 for Manhattan plots).
To test whether the aggregate estimates of genetic effects are associated
with creativity phenotypes, we constructed polygenic scores (PGS) based on
the GWAS summary statistics in the independent UKB follow-up dataset,
Addhealth,andWLSdatautilizingC+T (clumping +thresholding)50 and
PRS-SC approaches51 (Table S18). The results from both approaches are
Table 1 | Summary of the creativity phenotypes in the UKB
discovery sample
NCase Control Genomic
control λ
Artistic creativity 64,580 6625 57955 1.147
Scientic creativity 76,180 18225 57955 1.147
Managerial creativity 100,953 42998 57955 1.096
Overall occupational
creativity
125,803 67848 57955 1.147
N Mean SD Genomic control λ
Creative Achievement 219,473 3.24 0.79 1.310
Fig. 1 | Manhattan plot of GWAS analysis for creativity traits in the UKB dis-
covery sample. Results are shown for aoverall occupational creativity (n= 125,803),
and bcreative achievement (n= 219,473). The y axis represents -log
10
(p-value) for
association with each phenotype, and the x axis represents genomic position based
on human genome build 37. The cross in red represents independent genome-wide
signicant association signals, labelled by names of gene or nearest genes.
https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-024-06686-5 Article
Communications Biology | (2024) 7:1092 3
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
similar. The PGSs were signicantly associated with all 5 creativity phe-
notypes in the UK follow-up dataset (model tting P for PGS 6.53 × 105).
PGS accounted for a small amount of variance of leadership position, with
an incremental R2 up to 1.34%, on top of age, sex and top PCs. For
Addhealth and WLS, due to the small sample and heterogeneity of the
sample, the PGSs were not signicant, except for scientic creativity, and
creative achievement in WLS sample (p6.77 × 105). The ndings of small
predictive power were generally similar to those reported previously in
social sciences20.
Heritability and genetic correlations among the creativity vari-
ables across sex
We calculated common SNP heritability (SNP-h2) for the ve creativity
variables in the UKB discovery sample (Table S9 and Fig. 2). Using GWAS
summary statistics, we applied LD-score regression to estimate the pro-
portion of variance in liability to creativity traits that was explained by the
aggregated effect of the SNPs52. The SNP-h2estimates were 0.22 (95% CI
[0.18, 0.25]), 0.18 (95% CI [0.16, 0.21]). 0.08 (95% CI [0.06, 0.09]) and 0.12
(95% CI [0.11, 0.14]), for artistic, scientic, managerial and overall occu-
pational creativity. For creative achievement, the SNP-h2estimate was
0.0939 (95% CI [0.09, 0.10]). The SNP-h2estimates for artistic and scientic
creativity were signicantly higher than that for managerial creativity. The
SNP-h2estimatesobservedinthisstudyweresmallerthantheheritability
estimates reported in twin studies57. This is probably related to the fact that
creativity is a multifaceted construct and thus it is likely to be affected by
thousands of genetic variants (i.e., polygenity). Yet, the SNP-h2estimates are
similar to those reported for variables studied in social sciences, including
personality traits and subjective well-being21.
The ve creativity traits had moderate to strong genetic correlations
among them (Table 2). These ndings suggest a possibly similar signicant
genetic basis for the different types of creativity variables, and that certain
types of creativity may be more closely related than others. In addition, there
were high genetic correlations of creative phenotypes between males and
females (Table S10).
Genetic correlations between occupational creativity and
personal traits
We then examined the genetic overlaps between creativity variables and
personality traits, well-being, and success variables using summary statistics
from previous GWAS research (See Methods; Table S11). In order to correct
for multiple testing, we set the signicance level at a false discovery rate
(FDR) < 0.0553. With respect to personal traits, we included intelligence,
educational achievement, the big ve personality traits (openness, neuro-
ticism, extraversion, agreeableness, and conscientiousness), risk tolerance
and physical height. The creativity literature3,14,29,30,32 has theorized and
found signicant relationships between creativity with intelligence and
personality traits (e.g., openness, risk tolerance, and extraversion). Physical
height has been shown to affect ones career success and has also been
factored in important indicators of physical health54.
Our analyses (Fig. 3and Table S12) revealed signicant and relatively
large genetic correlations between the two major creativity variables (overall
occupational creativity combining all the three types of creativity and
creative achievement) and intelligence, as well as educational achievement.
The two creativity variables also shared signicant and moderate to large
genetic overlaps with openness. We also recorded ndings of moderate
genetic correlations of creativity with neuroticism and height.
We observed signicant differences in genetic correlations across the
three different types of occupational creativity (Tables S12 and S13; Fig. 4).
Scientic creativity (r
g
= 0.68, 95% CI = [0.62, 0.74]) had a larger genetic
correlation with intelligence than artistic creativity (r
g
= 0.46, 95% CI =
Fig. 2 | Summary of common SNP heritability estimations for creativity phe-
notypes from GWAS results in UKB data. Liability-scale h2for artistic creativity,
scientic creativity, managerial creativity, and overall occupational creativity,
Observed-scale h2for creative achievement. Vertical bars represent 95% CIs.
Asterisks denote the comparison of heritability between artistic creativity, scientic
creativity, and managerial creativity at Bonferroni-adjusted p-values < 0.01667.
https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-024-06686-5 Article
Communications Biology | (2024) 7:1092 4
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
[0.39, 0.53]; for comparison: P=3.72×10
6) and managerial creativity
(r
g
= 0.46, 95% CI = [0.38, 0.54]; P=1.43×10
5). Managerial creativity had
a larger genetic correlation with risk tolerance than artistic creativity and
scientic creativity. Artistic creativity had a lower genetic correlation (in
absolute value) with neuroticism than managerial creativity.
Genetic correlations between occupational creativity and
well-being
We selected well-being variables that are either phenotypically or
genetically related to creativity as reported previously in examining
their genetic correlations with occupational creativity (Fig. 3and
Table S12). We found positive genetic correlations of creativity with
autisman indicator of low levels of well-being, and with an indicator
of high levels of well-being: subjective well-being. Creativity also had
negative genetic correlations with indicators of low well-being: BMI
and non-cancer illness.
Similar to previous research, creativity was also genetically and posi-
tively correlated with bipolar disorder, schizophrenia, alcohol misuse,
cannabis use, and age at rst sextual intercourse. It also had a signicant and
negative genetic correlation with ADHD.
Our analyses also revealed signicant differences in such genetic cor-
relations across the three types of occupational creativity
(Tables S12 and S13; Fig. 4). For example, artistic creativity had a larger
positive genetic correlation with schizophrenia than managerial creativity.
Managerial creativity had a larger genetic correlation with subjective well-
being than artistic creativity. Scientic creativity had a larger genetic cor-
relation with autism than managerial creativity.
Genetic correlations between occupational creativity and career
success
The relationships of creativity with career success have received little
research attention in the literature. We found (Fig. 3and Table S12) sig-
nicant and positive genetic correlations between creativity and indicators
of high levels of career success including income, and occupational status.
The genetic correlation between overall occupational creativity and job
satisfaction was signicant.
With respect to reproductive success, both indicators of number of
children (number of children for men and for women) had negative genetic
correlations with creativity. Age of rst birth had a positive genetic corre-
lation with creativity.
Table 2 | Genetic correlations for creativity phenotypes in the UKB sample
Overall Genetic Correlation (S.E.) Artistic creativity Scientic creativity Managerial creativity Overall occupational creativity
Scientic creativity 0.7691 (0.0403)
Managerial creativity 0.6395 (0.0506) 0.6603 (0.0344)
Overall occupational creativity 0.8205 (0.031) 0.8809 (0.0148) 0.9271 (0.0081)
Creative achievement 0.8235 (0.028) 0.9235 (0.0156) 0.8347 (0.0223) 0.9608 (0.0108)
Fig. 3 | Genetic correlations of creativity phenotypes with personality traits,
health and well-being, and career success. A Overall occupational creativity and
creative achievement before partialling out genetic variance related to intelligence
and educational achievement. BOverall occupational creativity and creative
achievement after partialling out genetic variance related to intelligence. COverall
occupational creativity and creative achievement after partialling out genetic var-
iance related to educational achievement. Pvalues for the genetic correlations are
reported above each dot. Horizontal bars represent 95% CIs. Yellow as terisks denote
the genetic correlations at FDR < 0.05.
https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-024-06686-5 Article
Communications Biology | (2024) 7:1092 5
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Regarding the difference in genetic correlations across the three types
of occupational creativity, we found (Tables S12 and S13; Fig. 4)thatboth
managerial creativity and scientic creativity had greater positive genetic
correlations with income than artistic creativity. Scientic creativity had a
larger genetic correlation with age at rst birth than both artistic and
managerial creativity. The genetic correlations (in absolute value) between
number of children and scientic creativity were larger than those with
managerial creativity.
Genetic correlations of occupational creativity with well-being
and career success after partialling out genetic inuences
associated with intelligence and educational achievement
Thus, it is important to partial out genetic inuences associated with
intelligence or educational achievement when examining the above genetic
correlations. Such analyses shed light on the role of non-cognitive factors
underlying such genetic correlations. To estimate the genetic correlations
after partialling out the genetic variance of intelligence or educational
attainment, we used the Genomic Structure Equation Modeling (SEM)
(Genomic SEM)55. Our results (See Methods; Tables S14 and S15; Fig. 3)
show that partialling out genetic variance associated with intelligence did
not signicantly alter most of the genetic correlations with a few exceptions.
With intelligence controlled for, the signicant genetic correlations of
occupational creativity and creative achievement with autism became non-
signicant; the genetic correlation between creative achievement and
longevity also became non-signicant. Such ndings suggest that the
observed signicant genetic correlations between creativity and autism as
well as longevity may be shaped mostly by their genetic overlap with
intelligence.
After partialling out genetic inuences related to intelligence, the
previously observed signicant difference in genetic correlations across the
three types of occupational creativity with other variables generally did not
change signicantly; The changes mainly either involved genetic correla-
tions for scientic creativity or for well-being and success variables heavily
affected by intelligence.
Our results (See Methods; Tables S16 and S17; Fig. 3)showthatpar-
tialling out genetic variance associated with educational achievement, a
number of the signicant genetic correlations with other variables became
non-signicant, but not for other signicant genetic correlations, for
example, with risk tolerance, height, income, job satisfaction, and occupa-
tional status, for instance. In summary, the ndings show that the genetic
correlations of occupational creativity with the other variables cannot be
entirely attributed to the genetic overlap with intelligence or educational
achievement.
Discussion
Using a large-scale GWAS approach, we investigated the genetic archi-
tecture of occupational creativity and distinguished three important forms
of creativity: artistic, scientic, and managerial. We further probed the
genetic correlations of occupational creativity with psychological traits,
health and well-being variable, and career success. Our ndings suggest the
three types of occupational creativity are associated with distinct genetic
variants and shared different genetic overlaps with theoretically relevant
psychological traits. Furthermore, occupational creativity bore different
genetic overlaps with different well-being and career success variables,
suggesting some possible paradoxical inuences of genetic variants asso-
ciated with creativity on important career and life outcomes.
Our study identied some associated novel genes. We also found that
different genes were associated with different types of creativity (e.g., artistic,
scientic, and managerial), which one may use as further evidence that
creativity is domain specic. Yet, we caution against such a simplistic
explanation, because the different ndings might also be caused by chance
and the statistical power was limited so that one cannot expect the same loci
to be signicant in different GWASs.
In addition to corroborating prior ndings of twin studies that intel-
ligence, risk tolerance, and openness share the same genetic endowments
with creativity30,56, our research revealed positive genetic correlations
between occupational creativity and physical height and negative genetic
correlations with neuroticism. Physical height and neuroticism have
received little research attention in the creativity literature. Organizational
research suggests that taller employees are likely to be perceived as more
competent, intelligent and attractive, which in turn may bring about more
advantages to come up with and experiment novel ideas54. Less neurotic
Fig. 4 | Genetic correlations of artistic, scientic, and managerial creativity with outcomes. Vertical bars represent 95% CIs. The error bars are imposed on R2. Asterisks
denote the genetic correlations at FDR < 0.05 and the comparison of genetic correlations at Bonferroni-adjusted p-values <0.00152.
https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-024-06686-5 Article
Communications Biology | (2024) 7:1092 6
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
employees are emotionally more stable, which in turn may also be benecial
for employees to implement new ideas30.Notethatndings on signicant
genetic correlations do not imply causal relationships from one variable to
the other. Such signicant genetic correlations may also be caused by
assortative mating57. Future research should look into these phenotypic
correlations and their genetic linkages in greater depth.
Findings of the genetic correlations between occupational creativity
and well-being variables suggest that there are positive genetic overlaps
between creativity with both positive and negative well-being indicators.
Interestingly, occupational creativity had positive genetic correlations with
autism, longevity, subjective well-being, and age at rst sexual intercourse.
Given the mixed ndings on the phenotypic relationship between creativity
and autism58,ournding contributes to the literature by revealing that
occupational creativity and autism may share the same genetic make-up,
suggesting that environmental inuence may play an important role in
shaping their phenotypic correlation. We also found that occupational
creativity had negative genetic correlations with ADHD, and BMIindi-
cators of low levels of well-being. Together, our ndings suggest that the
genetic variants positively correlated with occupational creativity may also
enhance the chance for employees to have low levels (e.g., autism and
bipolar disorder) and high levels of well-being (e.g., longevity and subjective
well-being).
Perhaps the most intriguing results of this research lie in our revealing
genetic correlations between occupational creativity with career success,
which may be related to how creativity was measured in this research.
Contrary to the stereotype22,23, we found positive genetic correlations
between occupational creativity with income and occupational status. Genes
positively related to occupational creativity may also enhance the chance for
employees to earn high levels of income and have high status occupations
indicators of greater extrinsic career success. It is interesting that we
observed a signicant positive genetic correlation with only overall occu-
pational creativity, but not with creative achievement. Considering the likely
negative phenotypic correlation between creativity and income24,25 and the
phenotypic positive relationship between creativity and job satisfaction27,
the ndings suggest that environmental inuences may play a crucial role in
shaping such relationships at the phenotypic level. With respect to repro-
ductive success, occupational creativity had a positive genetic correlation
with age at rst birth and a negative genetic correlation with number of
children. Genetic variants positively related to occupational creativity may
reduce the chance for people to have high levels of reproductive success. This
offers some clues to answer the age-old question: would creative genius die
outover time17? Interestingly, a recent study59 found unique genetic var-
iants related to creativity may partially explain why modern Homo sapiens
survived and dominated over other hominids (e.g., Neanderthals and
chimpanzees) in human evolution. Such seemingly paradoxical genetic
inuences merits future research attention.
The different genetic correlations of the different types of occupational
creativity with other variables point to the importance of differentiating
creativity into different domains. We found that scientic creativity shared
more genetic overlap with intelligence than artistic and managerial
creativity38,39. Managerial creativity had a higher genetic correlation with risk
tolerance than artistic and scientic creativity, which is in consistent with
ndings of organizational research that risk tolerance is important for
management and leadership roles60. Managerial creativity bore a higher
positive genetic correlation with subjective well-being than artistic creativity.
Taken together, the ndings seem to suggest that compared to artistic and
scientic creativity, genetic variants associated with managerial creativity
appear to be related to better well-being outcomes.
We observed different genetic correlations with career success variables
across the three types of occupational creativity. Artistic creativity had a
lower positive genetic correlation with income than managerial and scien-
tic creativity. Scientic creativity had a higher positive genetic correlation
with age at rst birth than the other two forms of creativity, but its negative
genetic correlation with number of childrenwashigherthanmanagerial
creativity. Although genetic variants related to creativity tend to reduce the
chance for people to have children, this tendency seems more salient for
scientic creativity.
Our GWAS results indicate that creativity phenotypes are signicantly
heritable traits but are highly polygenic. This complexity presents sub-
stantial challenges in accurately estimating the true causaleffect sizes of
variants within the discovery dataset, thereby reducing the efciency of PGS
using these effect estimates. Our ndings underscore the polygenicity of the
genetic architecture underlying creativity phenotypes.
We were unable to ascertain the causal effects of genetic variants and
the direction of causality between occupational creativity with psychological
traits and well-being and success variables. Psychological and organizational
research on the phenotypic relationshipssuggeststhatitispossiblethat
psychological traits and well-being variables that share the same genetic
architecture with creativity may inuence occupational creativity3,16,32.It
appears also possible that creative achievement in one job may also affect job
incumbentswell-being and career success32,61.Ourndings may also be
affected by the way that occupational creativity was coded and measured.
For example, one person may exhibit multiple forms of creativity (e.g.,
artistic and scientic simultaneously), and there are other forms of creativity
beyond the three types. Therefore, future research may examine such
direction of causality issues and the neurobiological pathways linking the
genetic variants related to creativity measured with other approaches to
brain functions, psychological traits, and well-being and success.
Materials and Methods
Study samples
Our main analyses were based on the U.K. Biobank (UKB) data, which were
drawn from a population-based study of about 500,000 participants who
were 40 years old or older in the United Kingdom42,62,63. Participants
information on their jobs and occupations was obtained through interviews
during their visits to the study centers. UKB staffs coded such job and
occupation information (e.g., job titles and major responsibilities) into the
four-digit U.K. Standard Occupational Classication (SOC) system37 (ver-
sion 2000; Field identier: 132). Our analyses included participants of
European ancestry with available information on their occupation codes
and genotype data that passed quality control (Figure S7).
Replication samples included participants from the UKB follow-up
cohort (N= 23,249), the Add Health Wave IV cohort64,65 (N=1461), and
Wisconsin Longitudinal Study (WLS) cohort66 (N= 2265). Between June to
September 2015, about 102,000 UKB participants completed an online
follow-up assessment of their employment history. We selected those who
had past employment information but were not included in the discovery
phase because of lacking baseline job information. The Add Health study is a
U.S. longitudinal study of adolescents67. We used data from the Add Health
Wave IV survey that was conducted between 2007 to 2009, and included
unrelated Caucasian participants with valid occupation information and
genotype data in analyses. The Wisconsin Longitudinal Study (WLS) cohort
is a large U.S. longitudinal study of a random sample of people who grad-
uated from Wisconsin high schools in 1957 and of their randomly selected
siblings66. Six rounds of data collection were conducted from 1957 to 2011.
The occupation information was collected in a random sample in the last
four rounds. More detailed information about the data preparation of the
replication samples is presented in Supplementary notes and Figures S8-10.
Phenotype denitions and measures
Because multifaceted nature, creativity has been very challe ngingto measure
and thus it has been assessed in various fashions in the literature13,30.Inthis
research, we adopted the previous approach8,38,68 and captured creativity
primarily based on job tasks and responsibilities of participantsoccupations
with two measures (ve variables, Table S1). The rst measure was cate-
gorical, encompassing an overall measure of occupational creativity and its
three domain-specic types: artistic creativity, scientic creativity, and
managerial creativity. The second measure was a continuous variable,
creative achievement, which served as a common metric of creativity across
various occupations.
https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-024-06686-5 Article
Communications Biology | (2024) 7:1092 7
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Occupational creativity. The rst measure of occupational creativity
was derived from the major responsibilities of participantsoccupations
either from the U.K. SOC 2000 occupational codes (for the UKB data) or
from the U.S. Occupational Information Network (O*NET, https://
www.onetonline.org/) occupational codes (for the WLS and Add Health
data). Specically, based on the literature on creativity13,14, we identied
three groups of occupations from the U.K. SOC 2000 or the U.S. O*NET
systems that require high levels of creativity (i.e., artistic, scientic, and
managerial) and one control group (i.e., conventional occupations) that
require low levels of creativity based on the major tasks and responsi-
bilities of participantsoccupations. This generated four binary variables
with three reecting the three domain-specic creativity (e.g., for artistic
creativity, 1 = artist occupation and 0 = conventional occupation). For
the fourth variable reecting overall occupational creativity, we coded all
the artistic, scientic, and managerial occupations as creative occupa-
tions (i.e., as 1) and used less creative/conventional occupations as the
reference group (coded as 0). Below we provide a detailed description of
how each group of domain-specic creativity was operationalized. For
the control group, we drew from Hollands research on occupational
interest69 and coded conventional occupations as those with low levels of
creativity, because their primary tasks are more structured, orderly, and
routinized, and thus require less novel ideas69,70. Sample occupational
titles of the control group include counter clerks, plastics process
operatives, and ling assistants/clerks. Occupations that do not clearly
fall into the four categories of artisitic, scientic, manegrial, and con-
ventional jobs were coded as missing values.
Artistic creativity. Artists have long been treated as creative
professionals14. Following previous research on artistic occupations8,71,
we included the following occupations as those with high levels of artistic
creativity: actors, dancers, entertainers, musicians, visual artists, chor-
eographers and writers.
Scientic creativity. Prior research has also identied scientic occu-
pations as creative occupations8,71, because scientists are required to
generate innovative and useful ideas in conducting academic research.
Such scientic occupations are not limited to those in natural and bio-
logical sciences, but also include those in social sciences, engineering, and
mathematics. We thus operationalized scientic creativity as those
occupations whose primary job tasks and responsibilities involve
undertaking research in various sciences and conceiving engineering
designs. Sample occupation titles were chemists, biological scientists, civil
engineers, and scientic researchers.
Managerial creativity. Drawing on previous literature on creativity and
leadership28,29, we proposed a new category of creative professions:
managerial occupations. We treated senior managers and ofcials as
creative occupations because incumbents of such occupations are
increasingly required to generate novel ideas, practices, and solutions to
direct and coordinate the functioning of organizations and work teams,
particularly when faced with challenges and uncertainties brought about
by the changing business environment28. Sample occupation titles
include directors and chief executives of major organizations, marketing
and sales managers, and senior ofcers in re, ambulance, prison and
related services.
To ensure the reliability and validity of our coding, we developed a
coding scheme based on previous literature that dened four occupation
groups8,69,71. Two independent raters coded all the occupations by carefully
reviewing the structure of U.K. SOC 2000, the U.S. O*NET and SOC sys-
tems, and the detailed descriptions of the tasks and duties of each occupation
described in the occupation classication systems, as well as the cross-
country crosswalk on the basis of International Labor Organizations
International Standard Classication of Occupations (ISCO-88 and ISCO-
08) to ensure accuracy and reliability of the coding results. Furthermore, the
raters also referred to the list of creative industries and occupations released
by the U.K. Department for Digital, Culture, Media and Sport72,73 to ensure
that their coding was consistent with the major consensus in the creative
industry in the world. The initial interrater agreement was approximately
90%. Discrepancies were resolved through discussion among all the raters
and authors.
Creative achievement. Our second measure of creativitycreative
achievementwas assessed with three items obtained from the U.S.
O*NET database with a crosswalk by linking the U.K. SOC 2000 occu-
pation codes in the UKB data to the occupation codes in the O*NET
system. For the Add Health and WLS data, given that participants
occupation codes were based on the U.S. SOC system, we extracted
information on creative achievement from O*NET directly by matching
participantsoccupation codes with the O*NET occupation codes. The
three items tap into core characteristics of creativity: uency of ideas,
originality, and thinking creatively. Sample items include What level of
thinking creatively is needed to perform your current job?and What
level of originality is needed to perform your current job?All the items
used a seven-point scale, ranging from the lowest (1) to the highest (7)
level of creative achievement. The internal consistency of this scale
(Cronbachsα) was 0.92.
Genotyping and imputation
We used imputed genotypes released by U.K. Biobank (bgen les; imputed
data v3 released in March 2018). The quality control and imputation were
done by U.K. Biobank42.Briey, genotyped variants were ltered based on
batch effects, plate effects, departures from Hardy Weinberg equilibrium
(HWE), genotype platform, and discordance across control replicates.
Participant samples were excluded based on missing rate larger than 5%,
inconsistencies in reported versus genetic sex, and excessive heterozygosity
based on a set of 605,876 high quality autosomal markers. Genotypes were
phased and the imputation was performed using IMPUTE4 with the
Haplotype Reference Consortium (HRC) data, UK10K and 1000 Genomes
Phase 3 dataset used as the reference set. A European subset was identied
by projecting the UKB participants onto the 1000 Genome Project principal
components coordination. For this study, we e xcluded genetic variants with
MAF < 1%, and poorly imputed markers (IMPUTE info < 0.3), resulting in
9,804,641 autosomal variants imputed or genotyped on individuals of
European ancestry. The genotyping, imputation, and ltering procedure
were similar across the UKB discovery sample and the follow-up sample.
For the Add Health cohort, the genotypes were imputed on the Hap-
lotype Reference Consortium, with quality controls detailed previously74.
Genetic variants were included with MAF > 1% and IMPUTE Info < 0.3.
Analyses were limited to individuals of European-ancestry and cryptically
related individuals were dropped from analyses. For the WLS cohort, the
genotypes were imputed using the 1000 Genomes Phase 3 dataset. Repli-
cation analyses included only unrelated European participants.
Genome-wide association analyses
We assumed an additive genetic model where the dosage of each SNP was a
continuous variable ranging from 0 to 2 for the effect allele. For UKB
GWAS, a linear mixed model accounting for genetic relatedness was con-
ducted to determine its association with the phenotypes. The analyses were
conducted with the software BOLT-LMM v.2.3.2 (https://data.
broadinstitute.org/alkesgroup/BOLT-LMM/downloads)75. The association
analyses were adjusted for age, sex, genotyping array, and top 20 principal
components (PCs). The GWASs were also conducted separately by sex.
Signicant independent variants and their surrounding genomic loci
were identied using LD-clumping in PLINK v.2.00 (https://www.cog-
genomics.org/plink/2.0/). The LD structure was estimated from the Eur-
opean panels in the 1000 Genome Project of phase 3 as the reference
population. For GWAS conducted for 5 traits in this study, we set
P<1×10
8as the genome-wide signicance. The index lead SNP was
identied (P<1×10
8), independent from other variants (r2< 0.01), at each
locus. Here a locus was dened by an index SNP with the region anking
https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-024-06686-5 Article
Communications Biology | (2024) 7:1092 8
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
500 kb on both sides. The coordinates and variant identiers were reported
on the NCBI B37 (hg19) genome build. The functional annotation and gene
mapping were performed using ANNOVAR (v.2018Apr16, https://doc-
openbio.readthedocs.io/projects/annovar/en/latest/user-guide/download/),
including types of intronic, exonic, intergenic, 5-UTR, and 3-UTR, etc.76.
Regional plots of each identied locus were made by LocusZoom (http://
locuszoom.org/), and the 1000 Genome of the European population was
used to estimate LD information.
Replication analyses
Genome-wide signicant SNPs at the top loci were evaluated in the UKB
follow-up dataset, the Add Health dataset, and the WLS cohort. We meta-
analyzed results with both discovery and replication samples using the
inverse-variance weighted x-effect model with METAL software (https://
genome.sph.umich.edu/wiki/METAL). For the four binary creative vari-
ables (artistic, scientic, and managerial, and overall creativity), the coef-
cients obtained from the linear mixed model in the UKB discovery sample
were on a standardized scale. Therefore, we transformed these coefcients to
make them comparable with the observations in all samples. We rescaled the
beta coefcients with the following formula: β
s
=β/k*(1 k),where kis the
prevalence of creative occupations in the UKB; the ORs were calculated
accordingly using the scaled beta coefcient β
s
.
Common SNP heritability
We employed the software LD-score regression (LDSC) v.1.0.1 (https://
github.com/bulik/ldsc) with GWAS summary statistics in our estimation of
common SNP-h2for the creativity phenotypes for the whole sample and
sub-samples by sex in the UKB discovery data52. We used LDSC to estimate
the proportion of variance in liability to creativity traits that was explained
by the aggregated effect of the SNPs. From GWAS summary-level data, we
included SNPs that were presented in the European panels in the 1000
Genome Project, with the exclusion of the major histocompatibility com-
plex (MHC) region on chromosome 6. SNPs with INFO < 0.8 were not
included in the LDSC regression analyses.
For the binary trait of creative occupations (artistic, scientic, and
managerial, and overall creativity), the estimated heritability was trans-
formed to the liability scale using the approach derived by Lee et al.77.Asthe
exact prevalence is unknown, we assumed the percentage of creative
occupations in the UKB sample under current analysis was equal to the
population prevalence, an approach adopted in calculating SNP-h2on the
liability scale for other dichotomous traits using the UKB data (http://www.
nealelab.is/uk-biobank).
Genetic correlations of occupational creativity with personal,
well-being, and success variables
We computed the genetic correlation among creativity variables (for the
overall sample and sub-samples by sex) using the GWAS summary statistics
from the UKB discovery sample. We adopted the bivariate LDSC method,
by regressing the product of testing statistics (z statistics) from each phe-
notype against the LD scores78.
We assessed the genetic correlations of creativity phenotypes with
personal traits, well-being, and success variables using summary statistics
from GWASs of European ancestry, with the detailed information of sample
sizes, phenotypes, and GWAS summary data resources presented in
Table S11. We used summary statistics from the previously published large-
scale GWAS or GWAS results based on the UKB data, including, for
example, subjective well-being79, job satisfaction (UKB data 4537), depres-
sive symptom79, neuroticism80,longevity
81, number of cancer illness (UKB
data 134), number of noncancer illness (UKB data 135), BMI82,height
82,and
intelligence83. Note that for the illustration purpose, we recoded job satis-
factionsothathigherscoresindicatehigherlevelsofjobsatisfaction.
To estimate the genetic correlations after partialling out the genetic
variance of intelligence or educational attainment, we used the Genomic
SEM55. For each genetic correlation between creativity and other pheno-
types, we tted the Genomic SEM model including three traits: creativity
trait (X), the other phenotype (Y), and intelligence (Z). In the path diagram,
there was a bidirectional arrow between two traits of X and Y, and uni-
directional arrows from Z to X and Z to Y. The genetic effect of Z was
regressed out from the variance of X and Y affecting the genetic correlation.
The genetic covariance matrix of X, Y, and Z was produced by the LDSC
method implemented in Genomic SEM. The process was repeated for each
of the personal traits, well-being and success variables.
Polygenic scores (PGS) analyses
Using the GWAS results from creativity traits, we generated PGSs in the
three replication samples. The polygenic scores were constructed in
PRSice (see URL: https://choishingwan.github.io/PRSice), a method
shown to have decent prediction accuracy involving LD pruning followed
by p-value thresholding50, and PRS-CS (see URL: https://github.com/
getian107/PRScs), a method which generates posterior SNP effect size
estimates using Bayesian regressions with continuous shrinkage priors51.
For the PRSice method, besides automatically optimizing p-value cutoff
as default, variants were also selected at p-value thresholds: p< 0.01 and
p<1×10
4. Independent lead variants associated with the phenotype
were identied by the clumping and thresholdingapproach, removing
those within 500 kb and in linkage disequilibrium r20.01 with the lead
variant in the region. An individuals polygenic score is a weighted sum
of the genotypes across all independent variants. The weighting factor is
the estimated additive effect size, beta coefcient, at each variant from the
GWAS summary statistics. Prediction accuracy was based on an ordinary
least squares (OLS) regression of the creativity phenotypes on the
polygenic score and a set of standard covariates, including age, sex, and
the top genetic PCs. The R2or McFadden pseudo-R2(for binary out-
come) for PGS was calculated as the incremental variance for creativity
variables, i.e., the R2of the model including polygenic scores and cov-
ariates minus the R2of the model including only covariates. For the PRS-
CS method, the 1000 Genomes Project Phase 3 European sample is used
as the external LD reference panel. The parameters in the gamma-
gamma prior are set as 1 and 0.5 respectively and the global shrinkage
parameter is automatically learned from GWAS summary statistics using
a fully Bayesian approach. The posterior SNP effect size estimates were
concatenated across all chromosomes, which was used as weight for
calculating individuals polygenic score in each cohort.
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Portfolio
Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Data availability
The UKB GWAS summary statistics for the ve creativity phenotypes will
be available on the NHGRI-EPI Catalog of human GWAS upon publica-
tion. The accession codes for occupational creativity, artistic creativity,
scientic creativity, managerial creativity, and creative achievement are
GCST90444391, GCST90444392, GCST90444393, GCST90444394,
GCST90444395, respectively. The summary statistics of the top index var-
iants were presented in the article or supporting information, along with the
data analyzed in this study.
Received: 10 December 2023; Accepted: 6 August 2024;
References
1. Paulus, P. B. & Nijstad, B. A. Group creativity: Innovation through
collaboration. (Oxford University Press, New York; 2003).
2. Runco, M. A. Creativity. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 55, 657687 (2004).
3. Sternberg, R. J. & Lubart, T. I. The concept of creativity: Prospects and
paradigms, in Handbook of creativity. (ed. R. J. Sternberg) (Cambridge
University Press, New York; 1999).
4. Galton, F. Hereditary genius: An inquiry into its laws and
consequences. (Macmillan, London; 1869).
https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-024-06686-5 Article
Communications Biology | (2024) 7:1092 9
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
5. Bouchard, T. J. Jr, Lykken, D. T., Tellegen, A., Blacker, D. M. & Waller,
N. G. Creativity, heritability, familiarity: which word does not belong?
Psychol. Inq. 4, 235237 (1993).
6. Nichols, R. C. Twin studies of ability, personality and interests. Homo
29, 158173 (1978).
7. Piffer, D. & Hur, Y.-M. Heritability of creative achievement. Creat. Res.
J. 26, 151157 (2014).
8. Power, R. A. et al. Polygenic risk scores for schizophrenia and bipolar
disorder predict creativity. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 953955 (2015).
9. Li, H. et al. Genome-wide association study of creativity reveals
genetic overlap with psychiatric disorders, risk tolerance, and risky
behaviors. Schizophr. Bull. 46, 13171326 (2020).
10. Simonton, D. K. Creativity: Cognitive, personal, developmental, and
social aspects. Am. Psychol. 55, 151 (2000).
11. Amabile, T. M. Creativity, articial intelligence, and a world of
surprises. Acad. Manag. Discov. 6, 351354 (2020).
12. Baer, J. & Kaufman, J. C. Bridging generality and specicity: The
amusement park theoretical (APT) model of creativity. Roep. Rev. 27,
158163 (2005).
13. Carson, S. H., Peterson, J. B. & Higgins, D. M. Reliability, validity, and
factor structure of the creative achievement questionnaire. Creat. Res.
J. 17,3750 (2005).
14. Feist, G. J. A meta-analysis of personality in scientic and artistic
creativity. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2, 290309 (1998).
15. Motto, A. L. & Clark, J. R. The paradox of genius and madness: Seneca
and his inuence. Cuad. de. Filol.ía cl.ásica: Estudios Lat. 2,
189200 (1992).
16. Weisberg, R. W. Genius and madness?: A Quasi-experimental test of
the hypothesis that manic-depression increases creativity. Psychol.
Sci. 5, 361367 (1994).
17. Juda, A. The relationship between highest mental capacity and
psychic abnormalities. Am. J. Psychiatry 106, 296307 (1949).
18. Simonton, D. K. The mad-genius paradox: Can creative people be
more mentally healthy but highly creative people more mentally ill?
Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 9, 470480 (2014).
19. Kendler, K. S., Ohlsson, H., Sundquist, J. & Sundquist, K. Is an
elevated family-genetic risk for major psychiatric disorders specicto
creative occupations? Psychol. Med. 53, 44744486 (2023).
20. Acar, S., Tadik, H., Myers, D., Van der Sman, C. & Uysal, R. Creativity
and Wellbeing: A Metaanalysis. J. Creat. Behav. 55, 738751 (2021).
21. Harden, K. P. & Koellinger, P. D. Using genetics for social science. Nat.
Hum. Behav. 4, 567576 (2020).
22. Menger, P.-M. Artistic labor markets and careers. Annu. Rev. Sociol.,
541-574 (1999).
23. Filer, R. K. TheStarving artist-Myth or reality? Earnings of artists in
the United States. J. Polit. Econ. 94,5675 (1986).
24. Alper, N. O. & Wassall, G. H. Artistscareers and their labor markets, in
Handbook of the Economics of Art and Culture, Vol. 1. (eds. V. A.
Ginsburgh & D. Thorsby) 813-864 (2006).
25. Menger, P.-M. The economics of creativity: Art and achievement
under uncertainty. (Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA; 2014).
26. Judge, T. A. & Kammeyer-Mueller, J. D. Personality and career
success, in Handbook of career studies, Vol. 57. (eds. H. P. Gunz & M.
A. Peiperl) 59-78 (Sage Publications, Thousand Oaks, CA; 2007).
27. Steiner, L. & Schneider, L. The happy artist: an empirical application of
the work-preference model. J. Cult. Econ. 37, 225246 (2013).
28. Mainemelis, C., Kark, R. & Epitropaki, O. Creative leadership: A multi-
context conceptualization. Acad. Manag. Ann. 9, 393482 (2015).
29. Amabile, T. M. The social psychology of creativity: A componential
conceptualization. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 45, 357376 (1983).
30. Kandler, C. et al. The nature of creativity: The roles of genetic factors,
personality traits, cognitive abilities, and environmental sources. J.
Pers. Soc. Psychol. 111, 230249 (2016).
31. Birkinshaw, J., Hamel, G. & Mol, M. J. Management innovation. Acad.
Manag. Rev. 33, 825845 (2008).
32. Zhou, J. & Hoever, I. J. Research on workplace creativity: A review and
redirection. Annu. Rev. Organ. Psych. 1, 333359 (2014).
33. Sternberg, R. J. A systems model of leadership: WICS. Am. Psychol.
62,3442 (2007).
34. Mumford, M. D., Zaccaro, S. J., Harding, F. D., Jacobs, T. O. &
Fleishman, E. A. Leadership skills for a changing world: Solving
complex social problems. Lead. Q. 11,1135 (2000).
35. Yukl, G. Effective leadership behavior: What we know and what
questions need more attention. Acad. Manag. Perspect. 26,
6685 (2012).
36. Peterson, N. G., Mumford, M. D., Borman, W. C., Jeanneret, P. R. &
Fleishman, E. A. An occupational information system for the 21st
century: The development of O*NET. (American Psychological
Association, Washington, DC; 1999).
37. Song, Z. et al. Genetics, leadership position, and well-being: An
investigation with a large-scale GWAS. PNAS (2022).
38. Kendler, K. S., Ohlsson, H., Sundquist, J. & Sundquist, K. Is an
elevated family-genetic risk for major psychiatric disorders specicto
creative occupations? Psychol. Med., 113 (2022).
39. Ourtani, T. The relationship between creativity and mental illness: A
systematic review. ScienceOpen Preprints (2021).
40. Lam, M. & Lencz, T. & Consortium, C.G. Identication of key SNPs and
pathways underlying differential genetic correlations between
education and cognition on schizophrenia. Eur.
Neuropsychopharmacol. 29, S943S944 (2019).
41. Hou, L. et al. Genome-wide association study of 40,000 individuals
identies two novel loci associated with bipolar disorder. Hum. Mol.
Genet. 25, 33833394 (2016).
42. Bycroft, C. et al. The UK Biobank resource with deep phenotyping and
genomic data. Nature 562, 203209 (2018).
43. Bulik-Sullivan, B. K., Loh, P.-R., Finucane, H. K., Ripke, S. & Yang,
J. Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics C,
et al. LD Score regression distinguishes confounding from
polygenicity in genome-wide association studies. Nat. Genet. 47,
291295 (2015).
44. Smith, D. J. et al. Genome-wide analysis of over 106 000 individuals
identies 9 neuroticism-associated loci. Mol. Psychiatry 21,
749757 (2016).
45. Meta-analysis of GWAS of over 16,000 individuals with autism
spectrum disorder highlights a novel locus at 10q24. 32 and
asignicant overlap with schizophrenia. Mol. Autism 8,117
(2017).
46. Miyata, S. et al. Biochemical and ultrastructural analyses of IgLON cell
adhesion molecules, Kilon and OBCAM in the rat brain. Neuroscience
117, 645658 (2003).
47. Amanda Tan, R. P. et al. Neuronal growth regulator 1 (NEGR1)
promotes synaptic targeting of glutamic acid decarboxylase 65
(GAD65). bioRxiv, 2022.2002. 2008.479601 (2022).
48. De Dreu, C. K. et al. Oxytonergic circuitry sustains and enables
creative cognition in humans. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9,
11591165 (2014).
49. Wagnon, J. L. et al. CELF4 regulates translation and local abundance
of a vast set of mRNAs, including genes associated with regulation of
synaptic function. PLoS Genet 8, e1003067 (2012).
50. Euesden, J., Lewis, C. M. & Oreilly, P. F. PRSice: polygenic risk score
software. Bioinformatics 31, 14661468 (2015).
51. Ge, T., Chen, C.-Y., Ni, Y., Feng, Y.-C. A. & Smoller, J. W. Polygenic
prediction via Bayesian regression and continuous shrinkage priors.
Nat. Com. 10, 1776 (2019).
52. Bulik-Sullivan, B. K. et al. LD Score regression distinguishes
confounding from polygenicity in genome-wide association studies.
Nat. Genet. 47, 291295 (2015).
53. Benjamini, Y. & Hochberg, Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a
practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. Roy. Stat. Soc.
57, 289300 (1995).
https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-024-06686-5 Article
Communications Biology | (2024) 7:1092 10
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
54. Judge, T. A. & Cable, D. M. The effect of physical height on workplace
success and income: preliminary test of a theoretical model. J. Appl.
Psychol. 89, 428441 (2004).
55. Grotzinger, A. D. et al. Genomic structural equation modelling
provides insights into the multivariate genetic architecture of complex
traits. Nat. Hum. Behav. 3, 513525 (2019).
56. de Manzano, Ö. & Ullén, F. Genetic and environmental inuences on
the phenotypic associations between intelligence, personality, and
creative achievement in the arts and sciences. Intelligence 69,
123133 (2018).
57. Border, R. et al. Cross-trait assortative mating is widespread and
inates genetic correlation estimates. Science 378, 754761 (2022).
58. Pennisi, P., Giallongo, L., Milintenda, G. & Cannarozzo, M. Autism,
autistic traits and creativity: a systematic review and meta-analysis.
Cogn. Process. 22,136 (2021).
59. Zwir, I. et al. Evolution of genetic networks for human creativity. Mol.
Psychiatry 27, 354376 (2022).
60. Judge, T. A., Bono, J. E., Ilies, R. & Gerhardt, M. W. Personality and
leadership: A qualitative and quantitative review. J. Appl. Psychol. 87,
765779 (2002).
61. Li, W. D. et al. Can becoming a leader change your personality? An
investigation with two longitudinal studies from a role-based
perspective. J. Appl. Psychol. 106, 882901 (2021).
62. De Matteis, S. et al. Occupations associated with COPD risk in the
large population-based UK Biobank cohort study. Occup. Environ.
Med. 73, 378384 (2016).
63. Sudlow, C. et al. UK biobank: an open access resource for identifying
the causes of a wide range of complex diseases of middle and old age.
PLoS Med.12 (2015).
64. Harris, K. M. et al. Social, behavioral, and genetic linkages from
adolescence into adulthood. Am. J. Public Health 103,
S25S32 (2013).
65. Harris, K. M. et al. Cohort prole: The national longitudinal study of
adolescent to adult health (add health). Int. J. Epidemiol. 48,
14151415k (2019).
66. Herd, P., Carr, D. & Roan, C. Cohort prole: Wisconsin longitudinal
study (WLS). Int. J. Epidemiol. 43,34
41 (2014).
67. Harris, K. M. The add health study: Design and accomplishments.
Chapel Hill: Carolina Population Center,University of North Carolina at
Chapel Hill, 1-22 (2013).
68. Bachner-Melman, R. et al. AVPR1a and SLC6A4 gene polymorphisms
are associated with creative dance performance. PLoS Genet. 1,
e42 (2005).
69. Holland, J. L. Making vocational choices: A theory of vocational
personalities and work environments, Edn. 3rd. (Psychological
Assessment Resources, Odessa, FL; 1997).
70. Gottfredson, G. D. & Holland, J. L. Dictionary of Holland occupational
codes. (Psychological Assessment Resources Incorporated, 1996).
71. Kyaga, S. et al. Mental illness, suicide and creativity: 40-Year
prospective total population study. J. Psychiatr. Res. 47,
8390 (2013).
72. Department for Culture Media and Sports (DCMS) Creative industries:
Focus on employment. (Retreaved from https://assets.publishing.
service.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_
data/le/439714/Annex_C_-_Creative_Industries_Focus_on_
Employment_2015.pdf, 2015).
73. Bakhshi, H., Freeman, A. & Higgs, P. A dynamic mapping of the UKs
creative industries. (National endowment for science, technology and
the arts (NESTA), London; 2013).
74. Highland, H. M., Avery, C. L., Duan, Q., Li, Y. & Harris, K. M. Quality
control analysis of Add Health GWAS data. Carolina Population
Center,University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill,Chapel Hill,
NC (2018).
75. Loh, P.-R. et al. Efcient Bayesian mixed-model analysis increases
association power in large cohorts. Nat. Genet. 47, 284 (2015).
76. Wang, K., Li, M. & Hakonarson, H. ANNOVAR: functional annotation of
genetic variants from high-throughput sequencing data. Nucleic
Acids Res. 38, e164 (2010).
77. Lee, S. H., Wray, N. R., Goddard, M. E. & Visscher, P. M. Estimating
missing heritability for disease from genome-wide association
studies. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 88, 294305 (2011).
78. Bulik-Sullivan, B. et al. An atlas of genetic correlations across human
diseases and traits. Nat. Genet. 47, 1236 (2015).
79. Okbay, A. et al. Genetic variants associated with subjective well-
being, depressive symptoms, and neuroticism identied through
genome-wide analyses. Nat. Genet. 48, 624633 (2016).
80. Nagel, M. et al. Meta-analysis of genome-wide association studies for
neuroticism in 449,484 individuals identies novel genetic loci and
pathways. Nat. Genet. 50, 920927 (2018).
81. Deelen, J. et al. A meta-analysis of genome-wide association studies
identies multiple longevity genes. Nat. Com. 10,114 (2019).
82. Yengo, L. et al. Meta-analysis of genome-wide association studies for
height and body mass index in700000 individuals of European
ancestry. Hum. Mol. Genet. 27, 36413649 (2018).
83. Savage, J. E. et al. Genome-wide association meta-analysis in
269,867 individuals identies new genetic and functional links to
intelligence. Nat. Genet. 50, 912919 (2018).
Acknowledgements
This study was supported by the Singapore Ministry of Education (MOE)
grant R-317-000-138-115. Accessing data resources to conduct this study
was approved by the U.K. Biobank (application reference # 37334), Add
Health Study (application reference # 11030901), and Wisconsin
Longitudinal Study (application reference #98636). We are grateful to the
studys participantsand staff for data collection. The computational workfor
this study was performed on resources of the National Supercomputing
Centre, Singapore (https://www.nscc.sg).
Author contributions
W.-D.L., Z.S., and Q.F. designed research; W.-D.L., X.Z., Z.S., and Q.F.
performed research; Q.F and Z.S. contributed to source data collation and
maintenance; X.Z., K.Y., Y.Z., N.D., Z.S., and Q.F. analyzed data;and W.-
D.L., X.Z., K.Y. Y.Z., N.D., Z.S., and Q.F. wrote the paper.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests. Dr. Qiao Fan is an Editorial
Board Member for Communications Biology, but was not involved in the
editorial review of, nor the decision to publish this article.
Additional information
Supplementary information The online version contains
supplementary material available at
https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-024-06686-5.
Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to
Wen-Dong Li, Xin Zhang, Zhaoli Song or Qiao Fan.
Peer review information Communications Biology thanks the anonymous
reviewers for their contribution to the peer review of this work. Primary
Handling Editors: George Inglis and Benjamin Bessieres.
Reprints and permissions information is available at
http://www.nature.com/reprints
Publishers note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to
jurisdictional claims inpublished maps and institutional afliations.
https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-024-06686-5 Article
Communications Biology | (2024) 7:1092 11
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons
Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing,
adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as
long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the
source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if
changes were made. The images or other third party material in this
article are included in the articles Creative Commons licence, unless
indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not
included in the articles Creative Commons licence and your intended
use is not permitted by statutory regulat ion or exceeds the permitted use,
youwillneedtoobtainpermissiondirectlyfromthecopyrightholder.To
view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/
by/4.0/.
© The Author(s) 2024
https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-024-06686-5 Article
Communications Biology | (2024) 7:1092 12
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
Terms and Conditions
Springer Nature journal content, brought to you courtesy of Springer Nature Customer Service Center GmbH (“Springer Nature”).
Springer Nature supports a reasonable amount of sharing of research papers by authors, subscribers and authorised users (“Users”), for small-
scale personal, non-commercial use provided that all copyright, trade and service marks and other proprietary notices are maintained. By
accessing, sharing, receiving or otherwise using the Springer Nature journal content you agree to these terms of use (“Terms”). For these
purposes, Springer Nature considers academic use (by researchers and students) to be non-commercial.
These Terms are supplementary and will apply in addition to any applicable website terms and conditions, a relevant site licence or a personal
subscription. These Terms will prevail over any conflict or ambiguity with regards to the relevant terms, a site licence or a personal subscription
(to the extent of the conflict or ambiguity only). For Creative Commons-licensed articles, the terms of the Creative Commons license used will
apply.
We collect and use personal data to provide access to the Springer Nature journal content. We may also use these personal data internally within
ResearchGate and Springer Nature and as agreed share it, in an anonymised way, for purposes of tracking, analysis and reporting. We will not
otherwise disclose your personal data outside the ResearchGate or the Springer Nature group of companies unless we have your permission as
detailed in the Privacy Policy.
While Users may use the Springer Nature journal content for small scale, personal non-commercial use, it is important to note that Users may
not:
use such content for the purpose of providing other users with access on a regular or large scale basis or as a means to circumvent access
control;
use such content where to do so would be considered a criminal or statutory offence in any jurisdiction, or gives rise to civil liability, or is
otherwise unlawful;
falsely or misleadingly imply or suggest endorsement, approval , sponsorship, or association unless explicitly agreed to by Springer Nature in
writing;
use bots or other automated methods to access the content or redirect messages
override any security feature or exclusionary protocol; or
share the content in order to create substitute for Springer Nature products or services or a systematic database of Springer Nature journal
content.
In line with the restriction against commercial use, Springer Nature does not permit the creation of a product or service that creates revenue,
royalties, rent or income from our content or its inclusion as part of a paid for service or for other commercial gain. Springer Nature journal
content cannot be used for inter-library loans and librarians may not upload Springer Nature journal content on a large scale into their, or any
other, institutional repository.
These terms of use are reviewed regularly and may be amended at any time. Springer Nature is not obligated to publish any information or
content on this website and may remove it or features or functionality at our sole discretion, at any time with or without notice. Springer Nature
may revoke this licence to you at any time and remove access to any copies of the Springer Nature journal content which have been saved.
To the fullest extent permitted by law, Springer Nature makes no warranties, representations or guarantees to Users, either express or implied
with respect to the Springer nature journal content and all parties disclaim and waive any implied warranties or warranties imposed by law,
including merchantability or fitness for any particular purpose.
Please note that these rights do not automatically extend to content, data or other material published by Springer Nature that may be licensed
from third parties.
If you would like to use or distribute our Springer Nature journal content to a wider audience or on a regular basis or in any other manner not
expressly permitted by these Terms, please contact Springer Nature at
onlineservice@springernature.com
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any citations for this publication.
Article
Full-text available
Significance Our study presents the largest whole-genome investigation of leadership phenotypes to date. We identified genome-wide significant loci for leadership phenotypes, which are overlapped with top hits for bipolar disorder, schizophrenia, and intelligence. Our study demonstrated the polygenetic nature of leadership, the positive genetic correlations between leadership traits and a broad range of well-being indicators, and the unique association of leadership with well-being after accounting for genetic influences related to other socioeconomic status measures. Our findings offer insights into the biological underpinnings of leadership.
Preprint
Full-text available
Neuronal growth regulator 1 (NEGR1) is a glycosylphosphatidylinositol-anchored cell adhesion molecule encoded by an obesity susceptibility gene. We demonstrate that NEGR1 accumulates in GABAergic inhibitory synapses in hypothalamic neurons, a GABA-synthesizing enzyme GAD65 attaches to the plasma membrane, and NEGR1 promotes clustering of GAD65 at the synaptic plasma membrane. GAD65 is removed from the plasma membrane with newly formed vesicles. The association of GAD65 with vesicles results in increased GABA synthesis. In NEGR1 deficient mice, the synaptic targeting of GAD65 is decreased, the GABAergic synapse densities are reduced, and the reinforcing effects of food rewards are blunted. In mice fed a high fat diet, levels of NEGR1 are increased and GAD65 abnormally accumulates at the synaptic plasma membrane. Our results indicate that NEGR1 regulates a previously unknown step required for synaptic targeting and functioning of GAD65, which can be affected by bidirectional changes in NEGR1 levels causing disruptions in the GABAergic signaling controlling feeding behavior.
Article
Full-text available
In the original publication of the article, the first and last names of the authors are interchanged and published incorrectly. The correct author names are given below: Paola Pennisi, Laura Giallongo, Giusy Milintenda, Michela Cannarozzo.
Article
Full-text available
The genetic basis for the emergence of creativity in modern humans remains a mystery despite sequencing the genomes of chimpanzees and Neanderthals, our closest hominid relatives. Data-driven methods allowed us to uncover networks of genes distinguishing the three major systems of modern human personality and adaptability: emotional reactivity, self-control, and self-awareness. Now we have identified which of these genes are present in chimpanzees and Neanderthals. We replicated our findings in separate analyses of three high-coverage genomes of Neanderthals. We found that Neanderthals had nearly the same genes for emotional reactivity as chimpanzees, and they were intermediate between modern humans and chimpanzees in their numbers of genes for both self-control and self-awareness. 95% of the 267 genes we found only in modern humans were not protein-coding, including many long-non-coding RNAs in the self-awareness network. These genes may have arisen by positive selection for the characteristics of human well-being and behavioral modernity, including creativity, prosocial behavior, and healthy longevity. The genes that cluster in association with those found only in modern humans are over-expressed in brain regions involved in human self-awareness and creativity, including late-myelinating and phylogenetically recent regions of neocortex for autobiographical memory in frontal, parietal, and temporal regions, as well as related components of cortico-thalamo-ponto-cerebellar-cortical and cortico-striato-cortical loops. We conclude that modern humans have more than 200 unique non-protein-coding genes regulating co-expression of many more protein-coding genes in coordinated networks that underlie their capacities for self-awareness, creativity, prosocial behavior, and healthy longevity, which are not found in chimpanzees or Neanderthals.
Article
Full-text available
Creativity and well‐being are popular subjects in psychological and organizational studies. The recent literature presented mixed perspectives about the nature of the relationship between the two. Whereas the mad‐genius hypothesis, which was often explored among eminently creative individuals, seems to imply a negative relationship between the two, trends in the field of creativity, such as everyday creativity and general psychology (i.e., positive psychology) linked them positively. The present meta‐analysis study synthesized 189 effect sizes obtained from 32 samples in 26 different studies based on a total sample of 8,189. Analyses with multilevel modeling yielded a mean effect of r = .14. The moderator analysis tested the impact of age, gender, measure of creativity, measure of well‐being, stimulus type of creativity measure, and index of creativity measure. Only the creativity measure explained the variation in the study outcomes. The relationship between creativity and well‐being was significantly higher when creativity was measured by instruments focusing on creative activity and behavior (r = .22) than the divergent thinking tasks (r = .06). Those findings are discussed in terms of theoretical and practical implications.
Article
Creativity is an act of turning new and creative thoughts into fact. Creativity is characterized by means of the potential to understand the sector in new issue. Artificial Intelligence (AI) is an extensive-ranging branch of laptop science concerned with constructing clever machines able to performing duties. AI approaches can generate new ideas in three ways: by generating possible combinations of previously known ideas; by generating novel combinations of previously known ideas; and by generating unique combinations of previously known concepts. AI is solitary of those buzzwords that get about all the time. AI or specialized programming software is an area of studies that seeks to make the pc smarter. They work on their very own encryption. It is the one in ubiquitous Jargon AI utilized in marketing, buying and fashion. It could also be cast-off to come across credit score card fraud, driving. AI seems logical that day or every different computer will rule round.
Article
The observation of genetic correlations between disparate human traits has been interpreted as evidence of widespread pleiotropy. Here, we introduce cross-trait assortative mating (xAM) as an alternative explanation. We observe that xAM affects many phenotypes and that phenotypic cross-mate correlation estimates are strongly associated with genetic correlation estimates ( R ² = 74%). We demonstrate that existing xAM plausibly accounts for substantial fractions of genetic correlation estimates and that previously reported genetic correlation estimates between some pairs of psychiatric disorders are congruent with xAM alone. Finally, we provide evidence for a history of xAM at the genetic level using cross-trait even/odd chromosome polygenic score correlations. Together, our results demonstrate that previous reports have likely overestimated the true genetic similarity between many phenotypes.
Article
Background Despite a large descriptive literature linking creativity and risk for psychiatric illness, the magnitude and specificity of this relationship remain controversial. Methods We examined, in 1 137 354 native Swedes with one of 59 3-digit official and objective occupational codes in managerial and educated classes, their familial genetic risk score (FGRS) for ten major disorders, calculated from 1 st through 5 th degree relatives. Mean FGRS across disorders were calculated, in 3- and 4-digit occupational groups, and then controlled for those whose disorder onset preceded occupational choice. Using sequential analyses, p values were evaluated using Bonferroni correction. Results 3-digit professions considered to reflect creativity (e.g. ‘artists’ and ‘authors’) were among those with statistically significant elevations of FGRS. Among more specific 4-digit codes, visual artists, actors, and authors stood out with elevated genetic risks, highest for major depression (MD), anxiety disorders (AD) and OCD, more modest for bipolar disorders (BD) and schizophrenia and, for authors, for drug and alcohol use disorders. However, equal or greater elevations in FGRS across disorders were seen for religious (e.g. ministers), helping (e.g. psychologists, social workers), and teaching/academic occupations (e.g. professors). The potential pathway from FGRS → Disorder → Occupation accounts for a modest proportion of the signal, largely for MD and AD risk. Conclusions While traditional creative occupations were associated with elevated genetic risk for a range of psychiatric disorders, this association was not unique, as similar, or greater elevations were seen for religious, helping and teaching professions and was stronger for internalizing than psychotic disorders.
Article
[full-text available at https://rdcu.be/b8xyE]. Psychometric, historiometric and psychiatric studies are controversial on a hypothetical link between psychopathology and creativity. In this study, we will try to contribute to this debate by analysing the case of autism. Is there a relationship between autism and creativity? If so, can we find the same relationship in a watered-down form in subjects with autistic traits? In order to answer these questions, we carried out a systematic literature review of the studies on this topic published in the last 10 years. We followed the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses guidelines. We also conducted a meta-analysis of data. We found that in the clinical population there are fewer creative performances than in control groups; nonetheless, it is possible to delineate a medium creative profile of subjects with autism. The average creative profile of people with autism shows that they are inhibited in fluency and flexibility, but that they display a high level of detail and a particularly high level of originality in works either generated during tests or created in private time. In particular, the level of detail reached in the latter condition seems to be higher in the autistic population than in the control groups. Better linguistic skills appear to be linked to better creative performances. Linguistic tests, if compared with visual and performative tests, seem to favour the expression of originality in subjects with autism. Although our data on autistic traits are compatible with the hypothesis that a high level of autistic traits is a watered-down replica of the cognitive profile of subjects with autism, we have no sufficient data to support this hypothesis. To read the full text --> https://rdcu.be/b8xyE