PreprintPDF Available

Sauna and Mindfulness: How Sauna Bathing Affects Psychological States and Traits Related to Bodily Sensations and Emotions

Authors:
Preprints and early-stage research may not have been peer reviewed yet.

Abstract

Despite its popularity, little psychological and cognitive science research has investigated sauna bathing, especially among healthy individuals. This study empirically examines the connection between sauna bathing and psychological factors related to bodily sensations and emotional experiences. Two experiments were conducted with healthy subjects. In Study 1, we recruited 180 participants and categorized them into three groups (regular sauna, irregular sauna, and no sauna conditions). The participants were asked to complete an online questionnaire which asked about mindfulness and positive emotions. Study 2 was a field study conducted with 28 participants using a sauna at a spa facility. Before and after bathing, the participants answered a questionnaire similar to that used in Study 1. Twelve participants who took sauna baths and 16 of the participants who did not take sauna baths were included in the analysis. Study 1 showed that sauna bathers had a higher ability of bodily sensory imagery and a higher aesthetic evaluation of haiku poetry. Study 2 showed that sauna bathing increased the observing trait of mindfulness. Although the relationship between sauna bathing and bodily sensations or emotional experiences has been mentioned by sauna users and books, this is the first time that it has been empirically demonstrated. These results are considered to be caused by the increased sensitivity of bodily sensations and the heightened parasympathetic nervous system owing to the high and low temperatures. However, more detailed mechanisms are expected to be elucidated in the future.
Page 1/24
Sauna and Mindfulness: How Sauna Bathing Affects
Psychological States and Traits Related to Bodily
Sensations and Emotions
Jimpei Hitsuwari
Helmut Schmidt University https://orcid.org/0000-0002-0061-5318
Takechika Hayashi
Kyoto University https://orcid.org/0009-0003-4092-745X
Woodman Katarina
Kyoto University https://orcid.org/0009-0003-9661-0865
Meiyi Du
Kyoto University
Misaki Morita
Kyoto University
Shota Takiguchi
Kyoto University
Yutaka Yamashita
Kyoto University
Michio Nomura
Kyoto University https://orcid.org/0000-0003-1043-2811
Research Article
Keywords: sauna, mindfulness, bodily sensation, aesthetic experience, emotion
Posted Date: September 2nd, 2024
DOI: https://doi.org/10.21203/rs.3.rs-4991388/v1
License: This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License. 
Read Full License
Additional Declarations: The authors declare no competing interests.
Page 2/24
Abstract
Despite its popularity, little psychological and cognitive science research has investigated sauna bathing,
especially among healthy individuals. This study empirically examines the connection between sauna
bathing and psychological factors related to bodily sensations and emotional experiences. Two
experiments were conducted with healthy subjects. In Study 1, we recruited 180 participants and
categorized them into three groups (regular sauna, irregular sauna, and no sauna conditions). The
participants were asked to complete an online questionnaire which asked about mindfulness and
positive emotions. Study 2 was a eld study conducted with 28 participants using a sauna at a spa
facility. Before and after bathing, the participants answered a questionnaire similar to that used in Study
1. Twelve participants who took sauna baths and 16 of the participants who did not take sauna baths
were included in the analysis. Study 1 showed that sauna bathers had a higher ability of bodily sensory
imagery and a higher aesthetic evaluation of haiku poetry. Study 2 showed that sauna bathing increased
the observing trait of mindfulness. Although the relationship between sauna bathing and bodily
sensations or emotional experiences has been mentioned by sauna users and books, this is the rst time
that it has been empirically demonstrated. These results are considered to be caused by the increased
sensitivity of bodily sensations and the heightened parasympathetic nervous system owing to the high
and low temperatures. However, more detailed mechanisms are expected to be elucidated in the future.
Introduction
In recent years, saunas have become increasingly popular in Japan and worldwide, with their market size
expected to continue to grow over the next ve years (Mordor Intelligence, 2024). Correspondingly,
sauna research has gained momentum, with the number of studies increasing from a few in the 1950s to
more than 50 annually in recent years (PubMed; Figure 1). A sauna session consists of a few minutes in
the sauna room at a high temperature, usually between 70°C and 100°C, followed by a cold bath or
shower and a rest session in a well-ventilated room. During sauna bathing, the high temperature raises
the body temperature, dilates the blood vessels, and activates the sympathetic nervous system
(Laukkanen, Lipponen, et al., 2019). In the cold bath, the sympathetic nervous system is reactivated at
the same time as the blood vessels constrict; however, the parasympathetic nervous system is
signicantly activated by the nal rest, leading to a state of so-called “deep relaxation” (Saunar no Hikaru,
2023) or “
totonou
” (Chang et al., 2023, 2024). “
Totonou
” means “to be organized” in Japanese, although
here, it refers to a state of happiness in which the sauna automatically conditions the body and mind and
restores natural capacities (Chang et al., 2023). The sauna environment and the associated changes in
the autonomic nervous system are likely to be related to consciousness, emotions, mindfulness, and
mental imagery. Two systematic reviews on saunas have recently been carried out (Hussain & Cohen,
2018; Nagae, 2022). However, in the former, only six out of the 40 studies (Hussain & Cohen, 2018), and
in the latter, none of the studies (Nagae, 2022) focused on healthy individuals. As most sauna studies
have been conducted with clinical groups, they have focused predominantly on treating illness or injury
and sauna bathing’s association with health, meaning few psychological studies on sauna bathing have
been undertaken (Chang et al., 2023).
Page 3/24
[Figure 1 near here]
A limited number of studies on healthy individuals have recently begun to be conducted, including global
surveys involving multiple ethnic groups (Hussain et al., 2019) and cohort studies (T. Laukkanen,J. A.
Laukkanen,et al., 2019). For example, Laukkanen,J. A. Laukkanen,et al. (2019) associated 203
psychiatric disorders that developed during 24.9 years (median) of follow-up with sauna habits and
showed that individuals who took sauna baths 4–7 times a week had a 77% lower risk of developing
psychosis than those who took a sauna bath once a week. Several studies have also used neuroscience
methods (Cernych et al., 2018; Chang et al., 2023). Chang et al. (2023) conducted an
electroencephalogram before and after sauna and showed that alpha power and theta power, which
suggest a state of relaxation, increased and that the amount of theta power increase positively
correlated with the subjective rating of pleasure. However, considering the popularity of and attention
paid to sauna bathing, and the fact that it is a dynamic experience involving the mind and body, there are
many remaining questions to be addressed. In this study, we focus on two gaps in the existing research
and attempt to extend the existing ndings.
First, and overlapping with the above, is the bias in the samples used, such as those comprising disease
groups. In addition, other studies have focused on men in their 80s (Strandberg et al., 2018) and
homosexual individuals (Lee et al., 2012). In studies with the general population, most participants either
did not use a sauna (Podstawski et al., 2015), or, conversely, even the least frequent sauna users went
once a week (Kunutsor et al., 2024; Laukkanen et al., 2017). For example, Hussain et al. (2019) showed a
positive effect of sauna bathing on mental health, but on the basis that the group that engaged in sauna
bathing 5–15 times a month scored higher than the group that did so fewer than ve times a month.
Comparisons with non-sauna users need to be made as this frequency of sauna use (even ve times a
month) is likely to be uncommon in the overall population, except in countries where saunas are part of
everyday life.
Second, most related studies have examined well-being, anxiety, or stress as dependent variables
(Hayasaka et al., 2008; Podstawski et al., 2015; Podstawski et al., 2024), which contrasts the potentially
wide range of benets of the sauna experience. In the following sections, aspects that have been
suggested to be associated with sauna bathing in terms of bodily sensations and emotional experiences
will be highlighted and linked to the aims of the present study.
Bodily Sensations
As is to be expected given that they involve placing oneself in a space of hot and cold temperatures,
sauna experiences have the potential to induce physical transformations. The rst thing to consider is
the relationship between sauna bathing and mindfulness. The relationship between saunas and
mindfulness or meditation has often been noted at the descriptive level, suggesting positive effects
(Harju, 2023; Jaglarz, 2013). Such discourses frequently refer to the experience of emptying one’s mind
and being able to focus on what is in front of oneself. The term “saunafulness” has even been developed
Page 4/24
by combining the concept and practice of mindfulness with the characteristics of sauna bathing (Harju,
2023). Meditation practitioners have also described mindfulness experiences in saunas (Amaro, 2010).
Amaro (2010) reported being able to stop thinking for the rst time inside the sauna room, and
additionally, that the slowing down of thoughts persisted even after leaving the sauna. Thus, sauna
bathing may increase the tendency toward mindfulness.
Second, saunas could be related to the ability to vividly evoke imagery, including bodily sensations.
Mental imagery has long been examined in psychology, with increasing attention being paid not only to
visual imagery but also to mental imagery for each sensory system, including auditory, tactile, and bodily
sensations (Andrade et al., 2014; Hitsuwari et al., 2023). It is possible that a quiet place or relaxing
environment may increase the clarity of mental imagery while also being associated with a state of
mindfulness. Preliminary results showed that a sauna bathing group scored higher than a control group
on the item “I could see images from my memory or imagination with extreme clarity” (Chang et al.,
2023).
Emotional Experience
Although saunas are often used for reasons of pleasure and comfort, further clarication is needed as to
which emotional experiences and tendencies of positive emotions are associated with sauna use. While
sauna bathing may be associated with positive emotions, such as improved well-being (Hussain et al.,
2019) and reduced anxiety (Hayasaka et al., 2008), it is important to capture the related emotions in
greater breadth and detail.
Shiota et al. (2006) attempted to distinguish seven positive emotions that are not global positive
emotions as measured by the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (Watson et al., 1988): joy,
contentment, pride, love, compassion, amusement, and awe. The differential associations of these
emotions with the Big Five and attachment styles suggest that they are distinct emotional tendencies
(Shiota et al., 2006). These positive emotions include what are called self-transcendent emotions, such
as awe and gratitude (Stellar et al., 2017). One denition is: “Under certain circumstances, the subjective
sense of one’s self as an isolated entity can temporarily fade into an experience of unity with other
people or one’s surroundings, involving the dissolution of boundaries between the sense of self and
‘other’” (Yaden et al., 2017, p. 143). Such self-transcendent emotions and sensations are also thought to
be related to the sauna experience; for example, according to Tokyo Sauna Japan (2023), sauna lovers
describe that the relaxed state after a sauna “feels connected to the universe” or “feels like becoming
one with that place and merging into a single entity.” In other words, sauna bathing may evoke self-
transcendent emotions. This is consistent with the idea that mindfulness leads to self-transcendence
(Hanley et al., 2023) and that saunas are sacred and sometimes religious spaces with a deep connection
to purication (Leipämaa-Leskinen et al., 2020; Wolff et al., 2024).
In association with self-transcendent emotions such as awe, sauna bathing may also inuence aesthetic
emotional experiences. Indeed, teamLab, a Japanese artist group, opened a popular temporary facility
where visitors could view artworks after sauna bathing, which became popular (Furutani, 2021).
Page 5/24
Describing the exhibition, TeamLab (2021) noted, “Visitors immerse in the art after the sauna, when their
senses are sharpened, their minds are clear and they are in a state of ‘perfect concentration,’ where
beautiful things seem even more beautiful. They get an experience that they do not realize with their
normal senses.
Aim
This research examines the relationship of sauna bathing habits (Study 1) and actual sauna bathing
(Study 2) with psychological tendencies related to bodily sensations and emotional experiences. In
addition to measures of well-being and anxiety tendency, which have been shown to be related to sauna
bathing in previous studies, we used measures related to bodily sensations, such as mindfulness and
multisensory mental imagery ability, and more precise measures of emotions, such as positive and self-
transcendent emotion. Sauna bathing was expected to show positive correlations with these scales. We
also considered socioeconomic status (SES) and health orientation, which have been associated with
sauna bathing in previous studies, as control variables. Regarding SES, sauna bathing frequency is
positively related to higher education (Hussain et al., 2019), place of permanent residence, parents’
education level, and students’ monthly budget (Podstawski et al., 2015). Regarding health orientation,
many people use saunas for health purposes, and more than 80% of all respondents cite “to improve
circulation,” “detoxication,” and “to improve sleep” as motivations for sauna bathing (Hussain et al.,
2019). In other words, to exclude the explanation that only people with high SES and health orientation
obtain positive psychological effects from sauna bathing, these factors were controlled for in the
analysis. The study was approved by the Ethical Review Committee of the Graduate School of Kyoto
University (CPE-584), and all procedures were carried out in accordance with their requirements and the
Declaration of Helsinki. All anonymized data and R-scripts for analysis from Studies 1 and 2 are openly
available for reference (https://osf.io/8ysmr/?view_only=25732ebcf3b14c83927183071016d596).
Study 1
Method
Participants
We recruited 60 people with a sauna habit (attending sauna at least once a week; regular sauna
condition), 60 with almost no sauna habits (not attending sauna but had the opportunity to attend in the
last 6 months; irregular sauna condition), and 60 with no sauna habit at all (had no opportunity to go to
saunas in the last 6 months; no sauna condition). The sample size was calculated assuming analysis of
covariance (ANCOVA) as the main analysis method (moderate effect size f = .25, power = .80,
signicance level = .05), with the three conditions among these participants as independent variables
and measures such as well-being as dependent variables, controlling for SES and health orientation.
Calculations using G*Power (3.1.9.7; Faul et al., 2009) yielded a total required sample size of 158
participants. Considering that a 10% missingness rate is possible in an online survey (Hoerger, 2010),
180 participants (60 per condition) were included in the study. After excluding participants who failed the
Page 6/24
attention check, 57 had a regular sauna habit (Mage = 41.72, SDage = 9.35, 36 men, 21 women), 60 had an
irregular sauna habit (Mage = 41.92, SDage = 8.89, 29 men, 31 women), and 60 had no sauna habit (Mage =
41.40, SDage = 8.98, 31 men, 29 women).
Materials
Twelvescales were utilized for measurements. The rst three variables measured were more general
variables, as examined in previous studies; variables four and ve were related to bodily sensation;
variables six to 10 were related to higher-order emotional experiences, including self-transcendent
emotion and spirituality; and variables 11 and 12 were controls.
Satisfaction with Life Scale. The Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS;Diener et al., 1985) is a ve-item
instrument that is answered using a7-point Likert scale and includes items such as “In most ways, my
life is close to my ideal.” We used the Japanese version of this scale (Sumino, 1994) as an indicator of
well-being.
State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. We used only the characteristic scales of the Japanese version of the
State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI;Nakazato & Mizuguchi, 1982; Spielberger et al., 1971). The
participants rated 20 items, such as “I feel good,” on a 4-point scale from “1 = Almost never” to “4 =
Often.
Ten-Item Personality Inventory. We used the Japanese version of thisBig Five A scale, which consists of
10 items on ve subscales that measure personality (Gosling et al., 2003; Oshio et al., 2012). For
example, extraversion is rated using a 7-point scale ranging from “1 = I think not at all” to “7 = I strongly
agree.
Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire, Short Version. The Japanese version of the Five Facet
Mindfulness Questionnaire, short version (FFMQ;Baer et al., 2006; Takahashi et al., 2022) comprises 24
items that measure trait mindfulness acrossve subscales (nonreactivity, observing,
actingwithawareness, describing, and nonjudging). For example, observation, measured using items like
“I pay attention to sensations such as the wind in my hair or the sun on my face,” is rated on a 5-point
scalefrom “1 = Never or very rarely true” to “5 = Very often or always true.”
Plymouth Sensory Imagery Questionnaire, Short Version. The Plymouth Sensory Imagery Questionnaire
(short version) (Psi-Q; Andrade et al., 2014) utilizesseven subscales to measure multisensory mental
imagery ability. The Japanese version, used in this study (Hitsuwari & Nomura, 2023), does not include
“emotional feeling” and has 21 items across six subscales:vision, sound, smell, taste, touch, and bodily
sensation. For example, for vision, items such as “Imagine the sound of …” followed by the word “bonre”
are rated using a 7-point scale ranging from “1 = No image at all” to “7 = As vivid as real life.”
Dispositional Positive Emotion Scale. We used the Japanese version of the Dispositional Positive
Emotion Scale (DPES;Shiota et al., 2006; Sugawara et al., 2020),a 38-item instrument that measures the
Page 7/24
tendency to feel positive emotions across seven subscales (joy, contentment, pride, love, compassion,
amusement, and awe). For example, for joy, respondents answer the item “I often feel bursts of joy” on a
7-point scale ranging from “1 = I totally disagree” to “7 = I strongly agree.”
Gratitude Questionnaire-Six Item Form. The Gratitude Questionnaire-Six Item Form (McCullough et al.,
2002) isa six-item scale measuring gratitude propensity. The Japanese version, used in this study
(Shiraki & Igarashi, 2014), contains ve items because one item was dropped during the reliability and
validity validation stages. For example, items such as “I have so much in life to be thankful for” are
answered on a 7-point scale ranging from “1 = Strongly disagree” to “7 = Strongly agree.”
Aesthetic Evaluation ofArt (HaikuPoetry). The haiku, the world’s shortest poem, was used to measure
the aesthetic perception of art. A haiku can be appreciated in a short time, and evaluation tasks can be
easily carried out on a computer (Hitsuwari & Nomura, 2022).Haikus are associated with self-
transcendent emotions (Kato & Hitsuwari, 2024).Five haikus were evaluated using a 7-point scale
ranging from “1 = Not beautiful at all” to “7 = Very beautiful.
Japanese Youth Spirituality Rating Scale. The Japanese Youth Spirituality Rating Scale (Nigorikawa et al.,
2016) usesve subscales to measure Japanese spirituality (harmonizing with nature,
Ikigai
[the
Japanese word for one’s sense of purpose in life], awe toward the invisible, connecting with one’s
ancestors, and self-determination) on a 27-item scale. For example, for items such as “harmonizing with
nature,” the scale includes statements like “Being in nature gives me energy” and “I feel that I have a lot
of energy.” Items are rated on a 7-point scale ranging from “1 = Not at all true” to “7 = Very true.
Time Perception.A four-item scale measuring time perception was used (Rudd et al., 2012). Items such
as “I have lots of time in which I can get things done” were answered using a 7-point scale ranging from
“1 = Strongly disagree” to “7 = Strongly agree.” The higher the score, the more time the participant
perceived that they had available.
Socioeconomic Status. Several questions were used to measure SES (Shimizu et al., 2021; Yanagisawa
et al., 2013). First, participants reported their approximate annual household income using a 12-point
system, ranging from “less than 12 million yen” to “more than 12 million yen”(SES income). Second, the
participants answered the question, “If the whole of current Japanese society were to be divided into ve
strata, where would you place your household income?”, using a ve-factor system: “high,” “above
middle,” “below middle,” “above low,” and “below low”(SES ranking).Third, participants answered three
questions regarding their subjective currentand childhoodSES. For example, for subjective childhood
SES, the item “My family usually had enough money for things when I was growing up” was rated using a
7-pointscaleranging from “1 = Strongly disagree” to “7 = Strongly agree.”
Health Orientation.We useda 10-item scale measuring health awareness and health-related behavioral
practices (Takahashi & Muto, 1992). An example item is, “Do you think you are a person who takes care
of your health?” The other items were answered using a 4-point scale ranging from “1 = Not at all true” to
“4 = Very true.
Page 8/24
Procedure
Participants were recruited using CrowdWorks (https://crowdworks.jp/), a Japanese online
crowdsourcing company, and completed a questionnaire created on Qualtrics
(https://www.qualtrics.com/). After signing an informed consent form, they answered the
abovementioned questionnaire and questions related to sauna use (e.g., frequency of sauna attendance,
sauna history, motivation, sauna bathing methods, psychological and physical effects), their haiku and
art experiences and interests, and their demographic information.
Results and Discussion
Descriptive statistics for the three conditions and the results of the one-factor analysis of variance
between participants are presented in Table 1.The analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted using
the
anovakun
package (ver. 4.8.6.; Iseki, 2021) in R (ver. 4.2.3; R Core Team, 2023), and multiple
comparisons were conducted using the Shaffer method.
[Table 1 near here]
The results in Table 1 show that there were signicant differences in the three-group comparisons in the
Psi-Q’s somatosensory imagery ability (
F
(2, 174) = 3.38,
p
= .04,
η2
= .04), evaluations of the beauty of
the haikus (
F
(2, 174) = 3.88,
p
= .02,
η2
= .04), and subjective current SES (
F
(2, 174) = 3.87,
p
= .02,
η2
=
.04). Next, we conducted an ANCOVA, controlling for SES and health orientation. Several measures of
SES were employed in this research; however, in the ANCOVA, we used the participants’ subjective
current SES, which was signicantly different in the ANOVA.Using the
lmres
function within the R
pequod
package (Mirisola & Seta, 2016), bodily sensory imagery ability and theaestheticevaluation of
the haikus were entered as dependent variables, the experimental condition of sauna bathing frequency
was entered as the independent variable, and SES and health orientation were entered as control
variables. The effects of bodily sensationimagery ability(a subscale of the Psi-Q; Figure 2a)andthe
aestheticevaluationof haiku poetry (Figure 2b)remained signicant in an ANCOVA that controlled for
health orientation and subjective SES(Supplemental Material, Table S1).
[Figure 2 near here]
The irregular sauna and no sauna conditions were grouped together and compared post hoc according
to regular sauna attendance. Thet-testsshowed a signicant effect of the sauna condition on the
aesthetic evaluation of haiku poetry and subjective current SES. The t-test also showedsignicant trends
ofthe sauna conditionin nonreactivity,Psi-Q, taste imagery ability,
contentment,pride,amusement,awe,harmonizing withnature, and
Ikigai
(Figure 3; see also
Supplemental Material, Table S2 for statistical values).The aesthetic evaluation of haikus remained
signicant when controlling for subjective current SES and health orientation (
b
= -.31,
SE
= .13,
t
= -2.42,
β
= -.18,
p
= .02), whereas other variables were not signicant.
Page 9/24
[Figure 3 near here]
The results of Study 1 showed an association between the frequency of sauna bathing and some
psychological variables, particularly bodily sensation imagery ability and the aesthetic evaluation of
haikus. These variables have been suggested to be related at the observational level; however, using
statistical analysis, this study empirically revealed an association for the rst time. With its repetition of
hot and cold exposure, sauna can draw attention to the body and cause its imagery to become clearer,
even if an individual is not in the sauna room. The association with the aesthetic evaluation of haikus
was also consistent with the aim of the above-mentioned exhibition (teamLab, 2021), which sought to
enhance the art experience by clearing the mind in the sauna. However, Study 1 did not reveal causal
relationships, such as whether sauna bathing (a habit) affected these physical, emotional, and
psychological tendencies, or whether individuals with a higher propensity toward such tendencies were
more likely to take sauna baths. In Study 2, we examined the psychological effects of sauna bathing
through a eld experiment at a bathing facility with saunas.
Study 2
Method
Participants
A eld study was conducted at the
Sagano Onsen Tenzan no Yu
(https://www.ndg.jp/tenzan/), a bathing
facility in Kyoto, Japan. The main analysis was a one-sample t-test, with the pre–post scale used as the
dependent variable. A medium effect size (d = 0.5) was assumed using G*Power (3.1.9.7; Faul et al.,
2009) with a power of .80 and a signicance level of .05 (N = 27). Excluding those who did not take a
sauna bath or dropped out, 35 participants were recruited: Three did not respond post, and four did not
state whether they had taken a sauna bath or not, leaving 28 participants for analysis. Furthermore, more
participants than expected did not take sauna baths: 12 took sauna baths (Mage = 53.42, SDage = 15.79, 7
men, 4 women, and 1 who did not disclose their gender) and 16 did not (Mage = 50.25, SDage = 18.77, 5
men, 11 women).
Materials and Procedures
Facility users were approached at the entrance and asked to complete a questionnaire similar to that
used in Study 1 before and after bathing. The questionnaire consisted of questions on the SWLS, time
perception, the Self-Transcendence Scale (see below), the Observingsubscale ofthe FFMQ, the DPES,
the Psi-Q, theaestheticevaluation of ve haikus, health orientation, SES, sauna bathing, and
demographic information. The Self-Transcendence Scale (Castelo et al., 2021)consisted of four items,
including “I feel that on a higher level, all of us share a common bond,” which were answered using a 7-
point scale ranging from “1 = Strongly disagree” to “7 = Strongly agree.”
Results and Discussion
Page 10/24
The pre–post descriptive statistics for the 12 participants who did and 16 who did not take a sauna bath
are presented in Table2.
[Table 2 near here]
First, a pre–post one-sample t-test was conducted on all measures for which data were collected for the
12 participants who took sauna baths. The results showed that none of the variables were signicantly
different. Next, a post-hoc comparison was made between the participants who took sauna baths and
those who did not. Multiple regression analysis was conducted with post-scores as the dependent
variable; the presence or absence of sauna bathing as the independent variable; and pre-scores, health
orientation, andsubjective current SESas control variables. The results showed that only observing (
b
=
-.53,
SE
= .23,
t
= -2.30,β =-.34,
p
= .03) was affected by having a sauna bath (Figure4).
[Figure 4 near here]
The results of Study 2 showed that the scores for observing, a subscale of mindfulness, increased after
sauna bathing. This is consistent with the observation that sauna use increases mindfulness (Harju,
2023). Sauna bathing elevates attention to external events and internal experiences such as sensations,
feelings, and thoughts through bodily sensations. It should also be noted that the insucient sample
size of this study is a limitation, and future experiments with larger samples in a more controlled
environment are required.
General Discussion
This research examined how sauna bathing frequency (Study 1) and actual sauna bathing (Study 2) are
associated with psychological factors related to bodily sensory and emotional experiences. In Study 1,
bodily sensory imagery ability and the aesthetic evaluation of haikus were associated with frequency of
sauna use. In Study 2, observing (part of mindfulness) was positively associated with sauna bathing,
even after controlling for SES and health orientation. Though there has been an increase in the number of
studies on sauna bathing, in line with its growing popularity in real-life situations, the majority of the
research has focused on individuals with diseases (Hussain & Cohen, 2018; Nagae, 2022). We provide a
new perspective on sauna research by investigating healthy individuals and those who use saunas in
real-life situations, rather than in therapeutic settings. In particular, we were able to measure several
variables related to bodily sensations and emotional experiences, beyond those that have been
examined several times in previous studies, such as well-being and anxiety tendencies (Hayasaka et al.,
2008; Hussain et al., 2019; Podstawski et al., 2015).
First, as expected, sauna bathing improved bodily sensory imagery ability and mindfulness. The
environment of being exposed to high and low temperatures may have sharpened bodily sensations and,
at the same time, cleared the mind, leading to a state of mind that is not overconcentrated but rather
observing the inside and outside of the body. These changes may persist even after leaving the sauna
room, coinciding with what Amaro (2010) described in his recordings of meditation experiences in
Page 11/24
saunas. Studies examining the relationship between sauna bathing and heart rate variability suggest that
the sympathetic nervous system is heightened during sauna bathing, whereas the parasympathetic
nervous system becomes dominant during cooldown (Laukkanen, Lipponen, et al., 2019). Similarly, heart
rate variability is known to increase after mindfulness meditation practice (Azam et al., 2016), and it is
possible that sauna and mindfulness have similarities in terms of autonomic nervous system activity.
Regarding emotional experiences, the results suggest that sauna bathing may enhance aesthetic
experiences and emotions. One artist group’s (teamLab, 2021) assumption that a rich art experience
could be gained after sauna is likely to be correct. Given that the above-mentioned observing factor of
mindfulness is known to be associated with both affective and cognitive empathy (Himichi et al., 2021),
the increased sensitivity of haiku appreciation with sauna experiences can be coherently accounted for
by mindfulness and empathy. Neuroscientic evidence suggests that pleasant aesthetic experiences are
associated with default mode networks (DMN; Bel et al., 2019). To date, there have been several sauna
neuroscience studies (Cernych et al., 2018; Chang et al., 2023) but no magnetic resonance imaging
studies. As such, the relationship between sauna bathing and DMN remains unclear, but it is quite
possible that the relaxed state after sauna may enhance DMN. Though these are all important new
ndings, it is interesting that Study 1 and Study 2 produced different results. This suggests that a sauna
bathing habit (Study 1) and one-time sauna bathing (Study 2) may cause different psychological effects.
In other words, a single experience of sauna bathing may lead to a state of mindfulness, resulting in
sharpened bodily sensory imagery and enhanced aesthetic sensitivity as sauna bathing becomes a
habit. This difference can be an important topic for future research.
Some limitations should also be mentioned. First, the method of sauna bathing was not strictly
controlled. Sauna bathing is often combined with cold water and open-air bathing to reach relaxation, but
some may not engage in sauna bathing in this way. The time spent in the sauna and the temperature
also varied. Some previous studies have been conducted in controlled experimental settings (Chang et
al., 2023; Podstawski et al., 2024), and it is suggested that this psychological experiment should be
conducted in a similar way. Second, although we demonstrated a positive association between sauna
bathing habits and the aesthetic evaluation of haikus, it is challenging to generalize this association to
other art genres. As in teamLab’s (2021) attempt, immersive art experiences after sauna bathing may
generate additional aesthetic experiences, and there may be other forms in which such associations are
less likely to be found.
Conclusion
We examined the association between sauna bathing and psychological factors related to bodily
sensations and emotional experiences, nding positive relationships with bodily sensory imagery, the
aesthetic evaluation of haikus, and the observing tendency. It is expected that these results will be a
catalyst for increasingly active sauna research in the elds of psychology and cognitive science. The
results could also generate positive effects for the thriving sauna market and may provide insights into
how even healthy people can further enrich their lives through sauna use.
Page 12/24
Declarations
This work was supported by a Grant-in-aid for Scientic Research and JSPS KAKENHI (Grant number:
22H01103) and the 59th Incube Prize (Leave a Nest Co.). The authors thank the
Sagano Onsen Tenzan
no Yu
for allowing us to carry out Study 2. We also thank Editage (https://app.editage.jp/) for the English
proofreading.
All anonymized data and R-scripts for analysis from Studies 1 and 2 are openly available for reference
(https://osf.io/8ysmr/?view_only=25732ebcf3b14c83927183071016d596).
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Michio Nomura, Kyoto University,
Yoshida Honmachi, Sakyo-ku, Kyoto, 606-8501, Japan. Email: nomura.michio.8u@kyoto-u.ac.jp.
Telephone: +81 075-753-3004.
References
1. Amaro, A. (2010). Thinking: I. Understanding and Relating to Thought.
Mindfulness
,
1
(3), 189–192.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-010-0025-2
2. Andrade, J., May, J., Deeprose, C., Baugh, S. J., & Ganis, G. (2014). Assessing vividness of mental
imagery: The Plymouth Sensory Imagery Questionnaire.
British Journal of Psychology, 105
(4), 547–
563. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12050
3. Azam, M. A., Katz, J., Mohabir, V., & Ritvo, P. (2016). Individuals with tension and migraine headaches
exhibit increased heart rate variability during post-stress mindfulness meditation practice but a
decrease during a post-stress control condition—A randomized, controlled experiment.
International
Journal of Psychophysiology, 110
, 66–74. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2016.10.011
4. Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., & Toney, L. (2006). Using self-report assessment
methods to explore facets of mindfulness.
Assessment, 13
(1), 27–45.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191105283504
5. Bel, A. M., Vessel, E. A., Brielmann, A., Isik, A. I., Chatterjee, A., Leder, H., Pelli, D. G., & Starr, G. G.
(2019). Dynamics of aesthetic experience are reected in the default-mode network.
NeuroImage,
188
, 584–597. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.12.017
. Castelo, N., White, K., & Goode, M. R. (2021). Nature promotes self-transcendence and prosocial
behavior.
Journal of Environmental Psychology, 76
, 101639.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2021.101639
7. Cernych, M., Satas, A., & Brazaitis, M. (2018). Post-sauna recovery enhances brain neural network
relaxation and improves cognitive economy in oddball tasks.
International Journal of Hyperthermia
,
35
(1), 375–382. https://doi.org/10.1080/02656736.2018.1504992
. Chang, M., Ibaraki, T., Naruse, Y., & Imamura, Y. (2023). A study on neural changes induced by sauna
bathing: Neural basis of the “totonou” state.
PLoS One
,
18
(11), e0294137.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0294137
Page 13/24
9. Chang, M., Tanaka, K., Naruse, Y., Imamura, Y., & Fujii, S. (2024). Inuence of monaural auditory
stimulation combined with music on brain activity.
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
,
17
, 1311602.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2023.1311602
10. Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Grin, S. (1985). The Satisfaction with Life Scale.
Journal
of Personality Assessment, 49
(1), 71–75. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13
11. Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A. G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G* Power
3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses.
Behavior Research Methods
,
41
(4), 1149–1160.
https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149
12. Furutani, K. (2021). teamLab now has a bathhouse and sauna with digital art in Roppongi.
TimeOut
.
https://www.timeout.com/tokyo/news/teamlab-now-has-a-bathhouse-and-sauna-with-digital-art-in-
roppongi-080221
13. Gosling, S. D., Rentfrow, P. J., & Swann Jr, W. B. (2003). A very brief measure of the Big-Five
personality domains.
Journal of Research in Personality, 37
(6), 504–528.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0092-6566(03)00046-1
14. Hanley, A. W., Dorjee, D., & Garland, E. L. (2023). Mindfulness training encourages self-transcendent
states via decentering.
Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 10
(4), 431–
440. https://doi.org/10.1037/cns0000262
15. Harju, C. (2023).
Saunafulness
(A. Kobayashi, Trans.; J. Laukkanen, Ed.). Toyo Keizai. (Original work
published 2021)
1. Hayasaka, S., Nakamura, Y., Kajii, E., Ide, M., Shibata, Y., Noda, T., Murata, C., Nagata, K., & Ojima, T.
(2008). Effects of charcoal kiln saunas (Jjimjilbang) on psychological states.
Complementary
Therapies in Clinical Practice, 14
(2), 143–148. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ctcp.2007.12.004
17. Himichi, T., Osanai, H., Goto, T., Fujita, H., Kawamura, Y., Smith, A., & Nomura, M. (2021). Exploring the
multidimensional links between trait mindfulness and trait empathy.
Frontiers in Psychiatry
,
12
,
498614. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.498614
1. Hitsuwari, J., Hayashi, T., Woodman, K., Liu, X., Takeura, K., Nishida, S., TO, M., & Nomura, M. (2023).
Scent of poetry: Inuence of olfactory imagery during Haiku appreciation on aesthetic evaluation. In
M. Goldwater, F. K. Anggoro, B. K. Hayes, & D. C. Ong (Eds.),
Proceedings of the 45th Annual
Conference of the Cognitive Science Society
(pp. 1777–1784). Cognitive Science Society.
https://escholarship.org/uc/item/4pk522h1
19. Hitsuwari, J., & Nomura, M. (2022). Ambiguity and beauty: Japanese-German cross-cultural
comparisons on aesthetic evaluation of haiku poetry: Japanese.
Psychology of Aesthetics,
Creativity, and the Arts
. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1037/aca0000497
20. Hitsuwari, J., & Nomura, M. (2023). Developing and validating a
Japanese version of the Plymouth
Sensory Imagery Questionnaire (Psi-Q).
Frontiers in Psychology
, 14.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1166543
21. Hoerger, M. (2010). Participant dropout as a function of survey length in Internet-mediated university
studies: Implications for study design and voluntary participation in psychological research.
Page 14/24
Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 13
(6), 697–700.
https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2009.0445
22. Hussain, J., & Cohen, M. (2018). Clinical effects of regular dry sauna bathing: A systematic review.
EvidenceBased Complementary and Alternative Medicine
,
2018
(1), 1857413.
https://doi.org/10.1155/2018/1857413
23. Hussain, J. N., Greaves, R. F., & Cohen, M. M. (2019). A hot topic for health: Results of the Global
Sauna Survey.
Complementary Therapies in Medicine, 44
, 223–234,
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ctim.2019.03.012
24. Iseki, R. (2021).
Anovakun (version 4.8.6.)
. https://riseki.cloudfree.jp/
25. Jaglarz, A. (2013). Religious and cultural aspects in shaping the public space of hygiene and
sanitation activities. In C. Stephanidis & M. Antona (Eds.),
Universal access in human-computer
interaction: Design methods, tools, and interaction techniques for eInclusion
.
7th International
Conference, UAHCI 2013, Proceedings, Part I
(pp. 304–312). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-
3-642-39188-0_33
2. Kato, J., & Hitsuwari, J. (2024). Association between the creative experience of haiku poetry and a
tendency toward self-transcendent emotions.
The Journal of Creative Behavior
.
https://doi.org/10.1002/jocb.657
27. Kunutsor, S. K., Kauhanen, J., & Laukkanen, J. A. (2024). Frequent sauna bathing and psychosis:
Interrelationship with cardiorespiratory tness.
Journal of Psychiatric Research, 175
, 75–80.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2024.04.044
2. Laukkanen, T., Kunutsor, S., Kauhanen, J., & Laukkanen, J. A. (2017). Sauna bathing is inversely
associated with dementia and Alzheimer’s disease in middle-aged Finnish men.
Age and Ageing,
46
(2), 245–249. https://doi.org/10.1093/ageing/afw212
29. Laukkanen, T., Laukkanen, J. A., & Kunutsor, S. K. (2019). Sauna bathing and risk of psychotic
disorders: A prospective cohort study.
Medical Principles and Practice
,
27
(6), 562–569.
https://doi.org/10.1159/000493392
30. Laukkanen, T., Lipponen, J., Kunutsor, S. K., Zaccardi, F., Araújo, C. G. S., Mäkikallio, T. H., Khan, H.,
Willeit, P., Lee, E., Poikonen, S., Tarvainen, M., & Laukkanen, J. A. (2019). Recovery from sauna
bathing favorably modulates cardiac autonomic nervous system.
Complementary Therapies in
Medicine, 45
, 190–197. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ctim.2019.06.011
31. Lee, S. S., Lam, A. N., Lee, C. K., & Wong, N. S. (2012). Virtual versus physical channel for sex
networking in men having sex with men of sauna customers in the City of Hong Kong.
PloS One
,
7
(2), e31072. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0031072
32. Leipämaa-Leskinen, H., Syrjälä, H., Kauppinen-Räisänen, H. & Jaskari, M.-M. (2020). Saunascape in
the nexus of socio-material sauna bathing practices. In G. Patsiaouras, J. Fitchett, & A. J. Earley
(Eds.),
Research in consumer culture theory, Vol. 3: Proceedings of the Consumer Culture Theory
Conference Leicester
,
2020
(pp. 147–149). Consumer Culture Theory Consortium.
https://urn./URN:NBN:-fe2022032324568
Page 15/24
33. McCullough, M. E., Emmons, R. A., & Tsang, J. A. (2002). The grateful disposition: A conceptual and
empirical topography.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82
(1), 112–127.
https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.82.1.112
34. Mirisola, A., & Seta, L. (2016).
Pequod: Moderated regression package. R package version 0.0.5
.
https://cran.r-project.org/src/contrib/Archive/pequod/
35. Mordor Intelligence. (2024).
Sauna spa market size & share analysis—Growth trends & forecasts
(2024–2029)
. https://www.mordorintelligence.com/industry-reports/sauna-and-spa-market
3. Nagae, N. (2022). Kokoro no kenko nitaisuru kanshiki sauna no kouka nitsuiteno bunken review
[Literature review on the effects of dry saunas on mental health].
Fukuoka University Journal of
Clinical Psychology, 21
, 29–36, https://fukuoka-u.repo.nii.ac.jp/records/6866
37. Nakazato, K., & Mizuguchi, T. (1982). Atarashii fuanshakudo STAI nihonban no sakusei: Josei wo
taishou to shita seiseiki [Development and validation of Japanese version of State-Trait Anxiety
Inventory].
Japanese Journal of Psychosomatic Medicine, 22
, 107–112.
https://doi.org/10.15064/jjpm.22.2_107
3. Nigorikawa, T., Mitsuishi, H., Endo, S., Hirono, M., & Kanou, H. (2016). Nihonjin seinen ni okeru
spirituality hyoutei shakudo no kaihatsu [Development of a spirituality rating scale for Japanese
Youth].
Japanese Journal of Transpersonal Psychology/Psychiatry, 15
(1), 87–104.
https://doi.org/10.32218/transpersonal.15.1_87
39. Oshio, A., Abe, S., & Cutrone, P. (2012). Nihongoban Ten Item Personality Inventory (TIPI-J) sakusei
no kokoromi [Development, reliability, and validity of the Japanese version of Ten Item Personality
Inventory (TIPI-J)].
Japanese Journal of Personality, 21
(1), 40–52.
https://doi.org/10.2132/personality.21.40
40. Podstawski, R., Borysławski, K., & Biernat, E. (2024). The inuence of extreme thermal stress on the
physiological and psychological characteristics of young women who sporadically use the sauna:
practical implications for the safe use of the sauna.
Frontiers in Public Health
,
11
, 1303804.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2023.1303804
41. Podstawski, R., Honkanen, A., Tuohino, A., & Gizinska, R. (2015). The inuence of socioeconomic and
psychological factors on the popularity of sauna treatment among female university students.
Baltic
Journal of Health and Physical Activity, 7
(2), 67–82. https://doi.org/10.29359/BJHPA.07.2.06
42. R Core Team (2023).
R: A language and environment for statistical computing
. R Foundation for
Statistical Computing. https://www.R-project.org/
43. Rudd, M., Vohs, K. D., & Aaker, J. (2012). Awe expands people’s perception of time, alters decision
making, and enhances well-being.
Psychological Science, 23
(10), 1130–1136.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612438731
44. Sarris, J., de Manincor, M., Hargraves, F., & Tsonis, J. (2019). Harnessing the four elements for
mental health.
Frontiers in Psychiatry, 10
, 256. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00256
45. Saunar no Hikaru. (2023).
Kyoyo toshiteno Sauna
[Sauna as education]. Amazon Kindle Direct.
Page 16/24
4. Shimizu, H., Shimizu, H., & Kinosada, Y. (2021). Shakaikeizaiteki chii to ikarihyouji no mekanizumu:
shinriteki tokken ishiki to seitousei hyouka no baikai kouka ni chumoku shite [Th e mechanism of
socioeconomic status and anger expression: The mediation effect of psychological entitlement on
evaluation of justication].
Japanese Journal of Social Psychology, 36
(3), 76–87.
https://doi.org/10.14966/jssp.1909
47. Shiota, M. N., Keltner, D., & John, O. P. (2006). Positive emotion dispositions differentially associated
with Big Five personality and attachment style.
The Journal of Positive Psychology, 1
(2), 61–71.
https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760500510833
4. Shiraki, Y., & Igarashi, T. (2014). Kansha tokusei shakudo houyakuban no shinraisei oyobi datousei
no kentou [Development of the Japanese version of the trait gratitude scale].
Japanese Journal of
Interpersonal and Social Psychology, 14
, 27–33. https://doi.org/10.18910/36100
49. Spielberger, C. D., Gonzalez-Reigosa, F., Martinez-Urrutia, A., Natalicio, L. F., & Natalicio, D. S. (1971).
The State-Trait Anxiety Inventory.
Inter-American Journal of Psychology, 5
.
https://doi.org/10.30849/rip/ijp.v5i3&4.620
50. Strandberg, T. E., Strandberg, A., Pitkälä, K., & Benetos, A. (2018). Sauna bathing, health, and quality
of life among octogenarian men: The Helsinki Businessmen Study.
Aging Clinical and Experimental
Research
,
30
, 1053–1057. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40520-017-0855-z
51. Stellar, J. E., Gordon, A. M., Piff, P. K., Cordaro, D., Anderson, C. L., Bai, Y., Maruskin, L. A., & Keltner, D.
(2017). Self-transcendent emotions and their social functions: Compassion, gratitude, and awe bind
us to others through prosociality.
Emotion Review, 9
(3), 200–207.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073916684557
52. Sugawara, D., Arimitsu, K., & Sugie, M. (2020). Development of the Japanese version of Dispositional
Positive Emotion Scales.
Journal of Health Psychology Research, 33
(1), 57–65.
https://doi.org/10.11560/jhpr.190725130
53. Sumino, Z. (1994). Jinsei ni taisuru manzoku shakudo (the Satisfaction With Life Scale [SWLS])
nihonban sakusei no kokoromi [An attempt to create a 58 (Japanese version) of the Satisfaction
with Life Scale (SWLS)].
Proceedings of the 36th Annual Conference of Japanese Society of
Psychology
(p. 192). Japanese Society of Psychology. https://doi.org/10.20587/pamjaep.36.0_192
54. Takahashi, T. & Muto, Y. (1992). Kenkou shikoudo to kondate kouseiryoku no kanren [Relation
between how students intend to stay healthy and their ability to plan menus].
The Journal of the
Japan Association of Home Economics Education, 35
(1), 77–81.
https://doi.org/10.11549/jjahee.35.1_77
55. Takahashi, T., Saito, J., Fujino, M., Sato, M., & Kumano, H. (2022). The validity and reliability of the
short form of the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire in Japan.
Frontiers in Psychology
,
13
,
833381. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.833381
5. Tokyo Sauna Japan. (2023).
The most important word to understand Japan’s sauna culture is
“Totonou
.
Tokyo Sauna Japan. https://tokyosaunajapan.com/?p=392
Page 17/24
57. teamLab. (2021). teamLab Sauna “TikTok teamlabo Reconnect” Kaiki Encho. Sauna de Totonoi, Art
ni Botsunyu. Tokyo Keyakizakashita de, 11 Gatsu 23 Nichi Made [teamLab Sauna “TikTok teamLab
Reconnect” extended period: Refresh yourself in the sauna and immerse in art. At Keyakizaka in
Tokyo, Until Tuesday, November 23.].
PRTimes
.
https://prtimes.jp/main/html/rd/p/000000782.000007339.html
5. Wolff, LA., Heikkilä, E., Wallinheimo, K., Blomberg, W. (2024). When a phenomenon-based university
course went online: Students’ experiences and reections after sauna bathing. In S. Puiu & S. O.
Idowu (Eds.),
Online education during COVID-19 and beyond: CSR, sustainability, ethics &
governance
(pp. 31–64). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-031-49353-9_3
59. Yaden, D. B., Haidt, J., Hood, R. W., Vago, D. R., & Newberg, A. B. (2017). The varieties of self-
transcendent experience.
Review of General Psychology, 21
(2), 143–160.
https://doi.org/10.1037/gpr0000102
0. Yanagisawa, K., Masui, K., Furutani, K., Nomura, M., Yoshida, H., & Ura, M. (2013). Family
socioeconomic status modulates the coping-related neural response of offspring.
Social Cognitive
and Affective Neuroscience, 8
(6), 617–622. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nss039
Tables
Table 1
Descriptive Statistics and Results of Analysis of Variance for Study 1
Page 18/24
Regular-sauna
(RS) Irregular-sauna
(IS) No-sauna
(NS) ANOVA
mean SD mean SD mean SD Multiple
Comparison
SWLS 3.54 1.47 3.34 1.60 3.16 1.43
STAI 2.28 .55 2.47 .67 2.39 .53
Big Five
Extraversion 3.61 1.50 3.64 1.42 3.22 1.48
Agreeable 5.16 1.10 5.03 1.18 5.14 1.25
Conscientious 4.35 1.39 4.04 1.42 3.90 1.78
Neuroticism 3.85 1.46 4.03 1.54 3.98 1.66
Openness 3.75 1.39 3.58 1.35 3.62 1.41
FFMQ 3.28 .45 3.22 .44 3.22 .47
Observing 3.20 .69 3.31 .80 3.42 .59
Describing 3.09 .78 2.99 .82 2.97 .91
Acting with
awareness 3.84 .79 3.79 .66 3.78 .71
Nonjudging 3.18 .71 3.11 .61 3.05 .66
Nonreacting 3.07 .63 2.89 .73 2.90 .67
Psi-Q 5.52 .80 5.28 .75 5.27 .72
Vision 5.82 .80 5.54 .93 5.74 .76
Sound 5.99 .82 5.84 .82 5.74 .89
Smell 4.53 1.36 4.22 1.15 4.27 1.15
Taste 5.50 1.04 5.18 1.11 5.24 1.01
Touch 5.73 1.07 5.54 .92 5.53 .92
Bodily sensation 5.54 .91 5.38 .88 5.12 .86 RS > NS
DPES
Joy 4.10 1.11 3.75 1.35 3.88 1.16
Contentment 4.07 1.35 3.70 1.53 3.69 1.42
Pride 4.01 1.13 3.59 1.27 3.78 1.13
Page 19/24
Love 4.06 1.19 3.58 1.37 3.88 1.22
Compassion 4.65 1.04 4.42 1.10 4.57 .99
Amusement 3.82 1.13 3.45 1.02 3.54 1.20
Awe 4.05 1.03 3.68 1.10 3.78 1.04
Gratitude 5.12 1.16 4.93 1.35 5.24 1.06
Aesthetic evaluation of
haikus 4.92 .82 4.61 .76 4.54 .77 RS > IS, RS > NS
Spirituality 4.33 .85 4.19 1.01 4.27 .87
Harmonizing with
nature 5.36 1.06 4.90 1.35 5.15 1.15
Ikigai 4.23 1.40 3.77 1.59 3.81 1.51
Awe towards the
invisible 4.20 1.22 4.19 1.32 4.22 1.13
Ancestors 3.79 1.35 3.82 1.42 3.70 1.46
Self-determination 4.05 1.37 4.29 1.26 4.47 1.37
Time perception 3.07 1.08 2.70 .81 2.97 1.04
SES
Income 4.61 2.10 5.17 2.53 4.57 2.47
Ranking 2.61 .56 2.57 .62 2.40 .72
Subjective current
SES 2.23 .52 2.10 .66 1.93 .56 RS > NS
Subjective
childhood SES 2.57 .62 2.56 .56 2.54 .73
Health orientation 2.98 .50 2.89 .50 2.96 .47
Note.
Multiple comparison shows only variables for which multiple comparisons in ANOVA resulted in an
adjusted
p
< 0.05.
Table2
Descriptive Statisticsfor Sauna Condition and Pre–post Condition
Page 20/24
Sauna No sauna
Pre Post Pre Post
Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD
SWLS 4.37 1.35 4.10 1.59 4.05 1.47 4.35 1.34
Observing (FFMQ) 3.33 .56 3.79 .77 3.84 .85 3.86 .91
Psi-Q 5.28 .74 5.20 .95 5.58 1.37 5.65 1.26
Vision 5.61 .58 5.47 .95 5.85 1.38 5.88 1.29
Audio 5.42 1.05 5.36 1.11 5.92 1.29 5.85 1.40
Smell 4.58 1.17 4.58 1.33 5.27 1.56 5.46 1.75
Taste 5.44 1.17 5.36 1.26 5.52 1.52 5.54 1.39
Touch 5.25 1.06 5.36 1.17 5.62 1.60 5.75 1.26
Bodily sensation 5.39 .91 5.06 1.23 5.31 1.63 5.44 1.62
DPES
Joy 4.49 1.43 4.47 1.33 4.61 1.24 4.89 .91
Contentment 4.68 1.38 4.72 1.49 4.62 1.39 4.76 1.11
Pride 4.03 1.33 4.05 1.35 3.99 .98 4.42 .84
Love 4.50 1.03 4.33 1.35 3.81 1.25 4.17 .96
Compassion 5.23 .95 5.25 .91 4.96 .90 5.04 1.31
Amusement 4.65 .96 4.70 1.12 4.40 1.09 4.65 .72
Awe 4.62 .83 4.71 1.23 4.62 1.28 5.01 .95
Aesthetic evaluation of haikus 4.47 .90 4.02 1.10 4.11 1.31 4.06 1.00
Time perception 3.10 1.07 3.29 1.27 3.33 .98 2.95 1.06
Self-transcendence 3.85 1.29 4.12 1.03 4.27 1.11 4.23 1.64
SES
Income - - 4.42 3.03 - - 3.94 2.32
Ranking - - 2.58 1.24 - - 2.75 .58
Subjective current SES - - 10.25 2.22 - - 9.56 2.06
Subjective childhood SES - - 8.83 4.09 - - 8.75 2.52
Health orientation 2.87 .71 - - 3.19 .51 - -
Page 21/24
Figures
Figure 1
Number of studies between 1950 and 2023 with the keyword “sauna” in PubMed
Note.
https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/.
Page 22/24
Figure 2
Plots of bodily sensationimagery ability subscale of Psi-Q (a) and aesthetic evaluation of haiku poetry (b)
for the three groups
Note.
RS = Regular-sauna condition, IS = Irregular-sauna condition, NS = No-sauna condition.
Page 23/24
Figure 3
Radar plots for comparison between the regular sauna condition and non-sauna condition regarding
general variables (a), bodily sensation variables (b), and emotional experience variables (c)
Note.
Several instruments were adjusted to a 7-point scale in order to display them on the same radar
chart. Current SES = Subjective current SES, Child SES = Subjective childhood SES, HwN = Harmonizing
Page 24/24
with nature, AtI = Awe toward the invisible, SD = Self-determination.
Figure 4
Observing pre–post scores with and without sauna bathing
Supplementary Files
This is a list of supplementary les associated with this preprint. Click to download.
supplementarymaterial1saunaandmindfulness.docx
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any citations for this publication.
Chapter
Full-text available
This chapter presents and discusses results from data in form of learning diaries that a group of Finnish students wrote on a distance university course arranged during the COVID-19 lockdown. Initially, the course was planned to take place at a World Heritage site, but because of the pandemic, it had to be released online, and in the students’ own outdoor surroundings with limited physical interaction between the students. The first version of the course was run before the COVID-19 pandemic, but it had to be totally modified first to an online version, and then to a hybrid version during the pandemic restrictions. For this reason, the World Heritage site as learning environment and focus had to quickly incorporate a current Finnish Cultural Heritage, the sauna bathing culture, in the course. The students were instructed to write or record their diaries in the form of stream of consciousness after their sauna bathing or other bathing, and these diaries were used as data in our study. The theoretical approach of the study is phenomenology, and we analysed the diaries employing hermeneutic/interpretative phenomenology. The result shows that the sauna diaries were a successful course element that made it possible for the students to express themselves and reflect on their daily lives in a critical situation, but also to reflect deeply on the course, and their own role in that context.
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Recently, the increasing attention to mental states and psychophysical health has fueled the research into methods that can aid in relaxation and recovery. Traditional methods like meditation and sauna, while effective, have their limitations; thus, the need for more accessible and convenient alternatives. Methods Our innovative approach combines monaural beats with music, attempting to replicate the relaxing effects of a sauna in the auditory domain. Results In comparison to normal music and silent condition, the power of the theta active band significantly increased when listening to our modified music. Furthermore, after listening to modified music, there was a significant increase in mismatch negativity (MMN) amplitude in the oddball task. Additionally, participants’ subjective responses to a questionnaire indicated significant changes in body relaxation and other metrics after listening to the processed music. Discussion This state is considered similar to the “totonou” state, which manifests in physical and mental feelings of relaxation, pleasure, and mental clarity in the sauna. Thus, the present research proposes a convenient method for achieving relaxation, opening an avenue for individuals to customize their “totonou” music based on personal preferences.
Article
Full-text available
Background Many individuals who use the sauna at a temperature of 120°C of higher are not aware of the negative consequences of extreme thermal stress. Despite extensive research into sauna use, the impact of extreme thermal stress on the physiological and psychological characteristics of sauna users have not been examined to date. Aim The aim was to determine the effect of 20 min sauna sessions with a temperature of 80°C and 120°C on the physiological and psychological characteristics of women who sporadically visit the sauna. Methods The study was conducted on 22 full-time female university students. Physical activity (PA) levels were evaluated with the Polish short version of the International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ). Anthropometric characteristics were measured before the first sauna session by the InBody270 body composition analyzer. Physiological parameters, including heart, energy expenditure, physical effort, and blood pressure (systolic blood pressure – SBP, and diastolic blood pressure – DBP), were assessed indirectly using Polar V800 heart rate monitors and the Omron M6 Comfort blood pressure monitor. The participants’ wellbeing was assessed with the Profile of Mood States (POMS) questionnaire. The presence of significant correlations between heat exhaustion and heat stress variables and syncope during the second sauna session was examined with the use of classification and regression trees (CRT) and the cross-validation technique. Results Twenty-minute sauna sessions with a temperature of 80°C and 120°C induced a significant (p < 0.001) decrease in the values of SBP (excluding the temperature of 120°C), DBP, and body mass, as well as a significant increase in HR and forehead temperature. Exposure to a temperature of 80°C led to a significant (p < 0.001) increase in vigor with a simultaneous decrease in tension, depression, anger, fatigue, and confusion. In turn, sauna bathing at a temperature of 120°C had an opposite effect on the above mood parameters. Vomiting and confusion were the main predictors of syncope that occurred in some of the surveyed women. Conclusion Excessive air temperature can induce symptoms characteristic of heat exhaustion and heat stress nausea, heavy sweating, fast weak or strong HR, high body temperature, and confusion. Therefore, sauna bathing at a temperature of 80°C can be recommended to women who sporadically use the sauna, whereas exposure to a temperature of 120°C is not advised in this group of sauna users. The present findings provide highly valuable inputs for managing wellness and SPA centers.
Article
Full-text available
Saunas are becoming increasingly popular worldwide, being an activity that promotes relaxation and health. Intense feelings of happiness have been reported shortly after enjoying a hot sauna and cold water, what is known in Japan as the “totonou” state. However, no research has investigated what occurs in the brain during the “totonou” state. In the present study, participants underwent a sauna phase, consisting of three sets of alternating hot sauna, cold water, and rest. We elucidated changes in brain activity and mood in the “totonou” state by measuring and comparing brain activity and emotional scales before and after the sauna phase and during the rest phase in each set. We found significant increases in theta and alpha power during rest and after the sauna phase compared to before the sauna phase. Moreover, in an auditory oddball task, the p300 amplitude decreased significantly and MMN amplitude increased significantly after the sauna phase. The increase in MMN indicates higher activation of the pre-attentional auditory process, leading to a decrease in attention-related brain activity P300. Hence, the brain reaches in a more efficient state. Further, the response time in behavioral tasks decreased significantly. In addition, the participants’ subjective responses to the questionnaire showed significant changes in physical relaxation and other indicators after being in the sauna. Finally, we developed an artificial intelligence classifier, obtaining an average accuracy of brain state classification of 88.34%. The results have potential for future application.
Article
Full-text available
Mental imagery refers to the representation of stimuli that are not physically present and has long been a subject of interest in psychology. However, most research on mental imagery has been limited to visual images, with other types of imagery, such as sound and smell, receiving little attention. A possible reason for this is the lack of appropriate scales to measure the vividness of multisensory imagery. The Plymouth Sensory Imagery Scale (Psi-Q) has been developed to address this issue and has been used in several studies to measure the vividness of seven imageries: vision, sound, smell, taste, touch, body, and feeling. In this study of 400 participants in Japan, the Psi-Q was translated into Japanese and tested for reliability and validity. The results showed good internal reliability and retest reliability and moderate to high correlations with other measures of construct validity, including mindfulness, Big Five, and life satisfaction. Additionally, there is no significant difference in total Psi-Q scores between the Japanese and British samples, although some differences are found in individual sensory imagery abilities. This study provides valuable insights into multisensory mental imagery, and it is expected that research dealing simultaneously with the responses of multisensory modalities will further accumulate.
Article
Full-text available
In this study, ambiguity was classified into cognitive and emotional ambiguity, and their effects on the aesthetic evaluation of haiku were examined for native Japanese and German speakers. Because haiku is the shortest form of poetry in the world, it usually contains ambiguity and makes cultural comparison easier. Overall, 450 Japanese and 373 German speakers participated in an online experiment involving the evaluation of haiku poetry. They were also asked to fill out questionnaires based on personality traits. Then, participants were asked to rate a haiku based on 10 characteristics, including the degree to which they perceived beauty, cognitive ambiguity, awe, and nostalgia. Results showed that as cognitive ambiguity increased, the aesthetic evaluation of haiku decreased. This tendency was greater among German than among Japanese speakers; this can be explained by the differences in high- and low-context societies. Regarding emotional ambiguity, this study focused on the higher-order emotions of awe and nostalgia, which encompass both positive and negative emotions. It was found that the greater the participants felt these emotions, the higher was their aesthetic evaluation of haiku. This tendency was greater among German than Japanese speakers; this may be because Westerners tend to perceive awe and nostalgia highly positively. Ambiguity, also known as “the beauty of silence,” is one of the characteristics of haiku and was found to be an important factor in aesthetic evaluation; however, this relationship was also influenced by cultural differences.
Article
Full-text available
Background A brief measure of dispositional mindfulness is important for applied research on mindfulness. Although short forms of the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ), which measures the five aspects of mindfulness (i.e., observing, describing, acting with awareness, non-judging, and non-reactivity), have been developed worldwide, the validity and reliability of the Japanese version has not been examined. This study aims to examine the validity and reliability of the 24-item and 15-item versions of the FFMQ in Japan, which are the most widely used versions worldwide. Methods Online surveys were conducted for 889 adults in Japan through an online survey company using self-reported questionnaires including the FFMQ to confirm the factor structure and validity. To examine construct validity, we examined the relationship between the short form of FFMQ and mind wandering, interoceptive awareness, experiential avoidance, cognitive fusion, openness, neuroticism, self-compassion, depression, and anxiety, which have been theoretically or empirically shown to be related to mindfulness. In addition, 137 adults responded to the FFMQ again, after four weeks, for the test-retest reliability. Results The correlated five-factor and four-factor (excluding observing) models and the higher-order factor hierarchical model did not show sufficient goodness of fit, while the 24-item version showed acceptable fit when uncorrelated method factors loaded on by the positive and negative (reverse-scored) items were added. However, the 15-item version did not show acceptable fits for any of the models. Regarding reliability, the 24-item version showed acceptable values. In terms of the relationship between the original and the shortened version of the FFMQ, the 24-item version shared approximately 80% of the variance with the original one. In addition, although the wording effects of positive and negative items seemed to affect the correlations between the FFMQ and the other scales, the associations with related concepts were as predicted generally, supporting the construct validity of the short form of the FFMQ. Conclusion In Japan, the 24-item version of FFMQ showed acceptable validity and reliability similar to the original version, and we recommend that the 24-item version be used.
Article
Full-text available
Empathy and mindfulness are currently major topics of scientific interest. Although it is well-known that mindfulness—typically as an outcome related to meditation—generates empathy at the state level, only a small number of studies have documented the trait (i.e., personality) level association between mindfulness and empathy. Furthermore, the underlying mechanisms (subcomponents and mediator variables) that support this association remain unclear. Thus, here, with a focus on the trait level, we investigated relationships among multiple subcomponents of trait mindfulness and trait empathy (Study 1). Next, we reexamined the aforementioned relationships in an independent sample, with the further aim of investigating relevant mediation factors (Study 2). We found that two attention-related components of trait mindfulness—observing and acting with awareness—reliably and positively related to both affective and cognitive dimensions of trait empathy (i.e., empathic concern and perspective taking). Furthermore, we found that effortful control, reappraisal, and trait alexithymia mediated relationships between the aforementioned attention-related components of trait mindfulness and empathic concern. Taken together, our results suggest that the links between mindfulness and empathy are multidimensional and complex. These findings may ultimately contribute to an understanding of the mechanisms underlying the positive effects of meditation on empathy.
Article
Haiku is the world's shortest form of poetry, describing nature and ordinary everyday life. Previous studies and quotes from professional haiku poets suggest that haiku can foster self‐transcendent emotions, such as gratitude and awe. This study compares how those who did and did not create at least one haiku in the past month experience self‐transcendent emotions. A total of 192 haiku writers and 177 non‐writers responded to scales related to self‐transcendent emotions, such as gratitude for serenity, gratitude trait, and trait awe. The results of the Bayesian implementation of Generalized Linear Mixed models revealed that haiku writing increased the frequency of gratitude for serenity and awe, rather than general gratitude. These effects persisted even after controlling for interest in art and educational level, indicating that haiku writing has unique characteristics, including encouragement of attention to nature and a different perspective on daily life. Even in the absence of special events, a change in perspective toward everyday life occurs through creating haiku, and people appreciate and feel awe toward ordinary, everyday things. These novel findings contribute to the study of creativity and emotion.