ArticlePDF Available

An early-life challenge: becoming an older sibling in wild mandrills

The Royal Society
Royal Society Open Science
Authors:
  • Institut des Sciences de l'Evolution - Montpellier 2

Abstract and Figures

In monotocous mammals, most individuals experience the birth of a younger sibling. This period may induce losses in maternal care and can be physiologically, energetically and emotionally challenging for the older sibling, yet has rarely been studied in wild primates. We used behavioural data collected from a natural population of mandrills to investigate changes in maternal care and mother–juvenile relationship throughout the transition to siblinghood (TTS), by comparing juveniles who recently experienced the birth of a younger sibling, to juveniles who did not. We found that the TTS was associated with an abrupt cessation of the weaning process for the juvenile, and to a decrease in maternal affiliation. Juveniles’ reactions were sex-specific, as males associated less with their mother, while females tended to groom their mother more often after the birth of their sibling. Despite the substantial loss of maternal care, juveniles did not show an increase in conflict or anxiety-related behaviours. This study contributes to explain why short interbirth intervals often pose a risk to juveniles’ survival in monotocous primates. Our results contrast existing studies and further highlight the importance of examining the TTS in species and populations with various life histories and ecologies.
This content is subject to copyright.
An early-life challenge:
becoming an older sibling in
wildmandrills
Axelle Delaunay1, Océane Cossu-Doye1, Berta Roura-
Torres1,2,3, Loïc Sauvadet4, Barthélémy Ngoubangoye5,
Elise Huchard1,† and Marie J. E. Charpentier1,6,†
1Institute of Evolutionary Biology of Montpellier (ISEM), Université de Montpellier, CNRS, IRD,
EPHE, Montpellier, France
2Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology Unit, German Primate Center, Leibniz Institute of
Primate Research, Göttingen, Germany
3Department of Sociobiology/Anthropology, Institute of Zoology and Anthropology,
Johann-Friedrich-Blumenback, Georg-August University Göttingen, Göttingen, Germany
4Projet Mandrillus, SODEPAL, Bakoumba, Gabon
5Centre International de Recherches Médicales de Franceville, Franceville, Gabon
6Department for the Ecology of Animal Societies, Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior,
Bücklestrasse 5, Konstanz 78467, Germany
AD,0000-0003-4828-3989; BN,0000-0002-0868-3868;
MJEC,0000-0001-6530-5874
In monotocous mammals, most individuals experience the
birth of a younger sibling. This period may induce losses in
maternal care and can be physiologically, energetically and
emotionally challenging for the older sibling, yet has rarely
been studied in wild primates. We used behavioural data
collected from a natural population of mandrills to investigate
changes in maternal care and mother–juvenile relationship
throughout the transition to siblinghood (TTS), by comparing
juveniles who recently experienced the birth of a younger
sibling, to juveniles who did not. We found that the TTS was
associated with an abrupt cessation of the weaning process
for the juvenile, and to a decrease in maternal affiliation.
Juveniles’ reactions were sex-specific, as males associated
less with their mother, while females tended to groom their
mother more often after the birth of their sibling. Despite the
substantial loss of maternal care, juveniles did not show an
increase in conflict or anxiety-related behaviours. This study
contributes to explain why short interbirth intervals often
pose a risk to juveniles’ survival in monotocous primates.
Our results contrast existing studies and further highlight the
importance of examining the TTS in species and populations
with various life histories and ecologies.
© 2024 The Authors. Published by the Royal Society under the terms of the Creative
Commons Attribution License http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/, which permits
unrestricted use, provided the original author and source are credited.
Research
Cite this article: Delaunay A, Cossu-Doye O,
Roura-Torres B, Sauvadet L, Ngoubangoye B,
Huchard E, Charpentier MJE. 2024 An early-life
challenge: becoming an older sibling in wild
mandrills. R. Soc. Open Sci. 11: 240597.
https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.240597
Received: 11 April 2024
Accepted: 24 June 2024
Subject Category:
Organismal and evolutionary biology
Subject Areas:
behaviour, evolution
Keywords:
mother–offspring conflict, mother–offspring
relationship, sibling birth, sibling rivalry,
transition to siblinghood, weaning
Author for correspondence:
Axelle Delaunay
e-mail: axelle.delaunay@proton.me
These authors contributed equally to the study.
Electronic supplementary material is available
online at https://doi.org/10.6084/
m9.figshare.c.7315867.
1. Introduction
Parental investment is defined as any form of care provided to an offspring that increases the
offspring’s fitness at the cost of the parent’s future reproduction [1]. The amount of parental investment
that can be devoted to each offspring being limited, several conflicts are expected to arise between the
different family members. On the one hand, parents and offspring should conflict over the quantity
of parental investment, with offspring trying to get more investment than the parents are willing to
provide, and on the other hand, competition should arise between offspring, who should all try to
monopolize parental investment at the expense of their siblings [2,3].
Sibling competition to access and monopolize parental care [2] has been extensively studied across
taxa (for reviews, see [4,5]). Sibling competition can impact offspring’s development, with long-term
consequences on their morphology (e.g. insects [6]; birds [7]; mammals [8,9]), physiology (e.g. birds
[10–12]; mammals [13–15]) and behaviour (e.g. birds [16,17]; mammals [14,18]), and can ultimately
impact their fitness, for instance in species where siblicide is common (e.g. birds [19–21]; mammals
[9,22]). Most empirical studies to date have, however, focused on brood- or litter-rearing species
(especially in mammals; for reviews, see [5,23,24]), where same-age siblings compete over the same
maternal resources at the same time. Most of the theoretical models and evolutionary hypotheses
have thus been developed for intrabrood competition [24]. By contrast, interbrood competition, where
siblings are not at the same developmental stage [5], has been largely neglected so far, which is
surprising given that Trivers’ original model was developed for species producing singletons [2].
Monotocous mammals, which produce one offspring at a time, offer unique opportunities to study
interbrood competition between individuals who may not depend on the same maternal resources [24].
These species generally have long developmental periods, often with a juvenile phase, during which
offspring develop strong and enduring bonds with their mother that can extend way beyond infancy
(e.g. yellow baboons, Papio cynocephalus [25,26]; Asian elephants, Elephas maximus [27]; killer whales,
Orcinus orca [28]; red deer, Cervus elaphus [29]). Weaned offspring keep benefitting from post-weaning
forms of maternal care, such as protection against predators, facilitated access to food or social support
[30]. Several empirical studies have recently suggested that these post-weaning maternal resources
could improve offspring’s growth (e.g. chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes [31]), future reproductive success,
and longevity (chimpanzees 32,33]; red deer [34]). Monopolizing such maternal resources—if they can
be monopolized—could therefore be advantageous for offspring, meaning that sibling competition
could last long beyond lactation or nutritional dependency.
In line with this, recent empirical studies showed that sharing maternal resources between different-
age siblings could be costly for offspring. In Galapagos fur seals (Arctocephalus galapagoensis) and sea
lions (Zalophus wollebaeki) for instance, mothers can still be nursing their previous offspring by the
time they give birth to the next one, and the older sibling may attempt to prevent the newborn from
nursing or directly attack it, reducing its survival probability by 50% [35]. Similarly in false vampire
bats (Megaderma lyra), Leippert et al. [36] reported a case of a newborn being killed by its older,
weaned sibling. In several primate species, including in humans, short interbirth-intervals (IBIs) can
reduce survival for both the older (in rhesus macaques, Macaca mulatta [37]; in yellow baboons [38];
in humans, Homo sapiens [39–41]) and the younger sibling [37], and can even have intergenerational
effects [42]. Despite such evidence, behavioural studies in monotocous mammals have thus far focused
much more on mother–offspring conflict around weaning (for review, see [43]), where maternal milk is
the main resource at stake, than during later developmental milestones, such as the birth of a younger
sibling, or ‘transition to siblinghood’.
In primates, previous studies reported contrasting changes in mother–juvenile relationships
throughout the transition to siblinghood. In captive and free-ranging cercopithecines, the birth of
a younger sibling induces an abrupt decrease in time spent in close spatial proximity and in the
interaction rate between the mother and the juvenile, often driven by the juvenile itself [44–47].
Maternal aggression and rejection increases, often accompanied by signs of stress and ‘depression’
in the offspring [44–46,48], such as tantrums (a behaviour commonly considered as a behavioural
manifestation of mother–offspring conflict [43,49]) or self-directed behaviours (generally indicating
anxiety [50–52]). In wild chacma baboons (Papio ursinus), maternal behaviour does not change
following the birth of a new infant but juveniles nonetheless solicit their mother more often and
show more signs of distress (especially those that experience this transition at an older age) than
juveniles without a sibling [53]. In wild bonobos (Pan paniscus), juveniles face an abrupt fivefold
increase in cortisol level after the birth of their sibling, which endures for more than six months,
2
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rsos R. Soc. Open Sci. 11: 240597
although they show little behavioural changes [54]. In industrialized modern human societies, the
arrival of a younger sibling is often characterized by a decrease in maternal care and in the rate
of mother–child interactions, with a switch in who initiates interactions—from mother to children
becoming the primary initiator [55–58]. Confrontational behaviours with the mother increase, as well
as signs of distress and clinging/demanding behaviours in the older sibling—especially in children
experiencing this transition at a younger age [55,58,59]. Children’s behavioural adjustment shows high
interindividual variability in humans [55,58], however, and its quality (i.e. relatively positive versus
negative reactions to the birth of the younger sibling) is associated, potentially causally, with the
quality of siblings’ relationship later in life [60,61].
Importantly, species life-history traits might influence juveniles’ adjustment to the transition to
siblinghood. For instance, the birth of a younger sibling, when it triggers weaning or co-occurs
with it, might induce a harsher transition than in species where IBIs are longer. In line with this,
juvenile rhesus macaques can nurse up to 1 day before the birth of their sibling, following which
they experience an abrupt cessation of nursing, and those who experience steeper decreases in nursing
and maternal care show more signs of distress in the first month after the birth of their sibling
[44,62]. On the contrary, in chacma baboons, juveniles have already been weaned several months
before their mother gives birth, and the birth is not associated with any major change in mother–
offspring relationship [53]. Therefore, these two developmental milestones, weaning and transition to
siblinghood, do not always occur simultaneously across species or individuals. For instance, in some
populations, including mandrills, high-ranking females can conceive when they are still lactating,
while subordinate females display a slower reproductive pace [63], which could translate into a
harsher transition to siblinghood for offspring born to higher-ranking females. In addition, juveniles’
adjustment to the birth of a younger sibling could also be influenced by genetic relatedness between
siblings, as predicted by the kin selection theory [2,64]. For instance, in species characterized by a
high-reproductive skew and a long male tenure length, like in mountain gorillas (Gorilla beringei
beringei), full siblings may be common, which could induce a smoother transition (but see [65] for the
effect of relatedness on siblings’ relationships). In contrast, in species with lower relatedness between
siblings, as in mandrills, where males’ reproductive skew is high but tenure length is relatively short
[66], juveniles could experience a harsher transition after the birth of their sibling.
In this study, we investigated changes affecting mother–juvenile relationships following the birth of
a younger sibling in a long-lived social primate, the mandrill (Mandrillus sphinx). Mandrills live in huge
multi-male–multi-female groups, sometimes up to several hundred individuals, mainly composed of
females and their offspring [67]. Mandrills form matrilineal societies [68], where females are philopat-
ric and inherit their rank from their mother, while males disperse upon sexual maturity. Mandrills
are seasonal breeders, with most births occurring during the rainy season [63], meaning that most
offspring experience the birth of their younger sibling either during the next birth season, or two
seasons later, so that IBIs often present a bimodal distribution. Females give birth to one offspring
[69], which they nurse for eight months on average in captivity [70]. Importantly, female mandrills can
be lactating and pregnant simultaneously [63], which means that the birth of a younger sibling may
accelerate or terminate abruptly the weaning process, enhancing competition among siblings.
Here, we investigated the transition to siblinghood (hereafter, TTS) in the only natural population
of habituated mandrills, using behavioural data collected from 2015 to 2022. We hypothesized that TTS
would be associated with a loss of maternal care (H1), a weaker mother–offspring bond (H2), a greater
role of juveniles (relatively to mothers) in maintaining the mother–offspring bond (H3) and an increase
in juveniles’ anxiety-related behaviours (H4). Following a recent study in chacma baboons [53], we
compared mother–juvenile interactions among juveniles with no younger sibling and those of similar
age who recently experienced the birth of a younger sibling, and tested the following predictions:
(P1a) following the birth of a younger sibling, juveniles would no longer be nursed or carried by their
mother, independently of their age and sex, while juveniles without a younger sibling would show a
more gradual age-related decline for these two behaviours; (P1b) juveniles who underwent TTS at a
young age would show a more abrupt decrease in nursing and maternal carrying than juveniles who
experienced the birth of a younger sibling at an older age. In addition, (P2) grooming interactions
and spatial association between the mother and the juvenile would decrease, and (P3) juveniles would
initiate a greater proportion of grooming interactions, and be more proactive at maintaining spatial
proximity to their mother following the birth of their younger sibling. Finally, (P4) juveniles who
recently experienced the birth of a younger sibling, and especially those who experienced it at a young
age, would display more tantrums and self-directed behaviours than those who did not. For (P2−P4),
we explored potential sex-differences in juveniles’ reactions to TTS, as sex-differences in mother–
3
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rsos R. Soc. Open Sci. 11: 240597
offspring bond and social behaviour typically emerge early during development in cercopithecines
[71–73].
2. Material and methods
2.1. Study site and population
We studied a natural population of habituated mandrills who freely range in the Lékédi Park and its
surroundings, located in southern Gabon, daily monitored since 2012 by the Mandrillus Project. This
population originated from 65 individuals initially living in a semifree-ranging population housed at
Centre International de Recherches Médicales de Franceville, Gabon, who were released in two waves
in the Lékédi Park, in 2002 and 2006 [74]. Captive-born females started to breed with wild males
in 2003, and in late 2022, only four females out of 250 individuals in the group were captive-born.
All individuals, including infants and juveniles, are recognizable by trained observers using facial
and body features. During daily monitoring, observers on foot record data on individual life-history,
developmental trajectory and behaviour. In this study, we used data collected on 191 juveniles between
January 2015 and October 2022.
2.2. Demographic data
Individual birth dates were assessed with certainty when observed in the field (n = 88 infants),
or estimated within a time-window of 1 day to two months for the remaining 103 infants, based
on patterns of mother’s sexual swellings and infants’ physical appearance. Individual birth order
was inferred based on maternal reproductive history when known. We then divided birth order in
three different classes—first-, second- and later-born (birth order greater than or equal to three)—as
juveniles’ reaction to TTS may differ with maternal experience.
2.3. Behavioural data
Since 2015, trained observers collected daily ad libitum and 5 min focal behavioural observations
on all individually recognized infants and juveniles [75]. During focal observations, we recorded the
occurrence of suckling, ventral carrying and grooming bouts with the mother. For grooming bouts, we
recorded the direction of the interaction, and the identity of the individual who initiated the grooming
event. Mandrills typically initiate grooming either by starting to groom their partners (when they are
the groomer) or by approaching them and standing in a grooming position (e.g. head down, limbs
outstretched, presenting the body part they want to be groomed). We also collected the occurrence of
tantrums and anxiety-related behaviours (self-scratch and yawning) displayed by the focal individuals.
Tantrums are often considered a behavioural manifestation of mother–offspring conflict [43,49], and
are characterized in mandrills by a set of distinctive vocalizations such as moaning, gecking and
screaming [49]. Maintenance of spatial proximity was assessed by recording every close approach or
leave (to and from 1 m) between focal individuals and their mother. Finally, we collected scans every
2 min during focal observations, during which we recorded the identity of all groupmates—including
the mother—that were in body contact and within 5 m around the focal individual.
Mothers’ social rank was established using the outcomes of approach–avoidance interactions during
ad libitum and focal samplings performed on adult females. We computed yearly linear hierarchies
using normalized David’s score. Individuals’ social ranks were highly correlated between years [63],
therefore, we assigned each female a unique relative rank from 2012 to 2022. We then divided adult
females into three classes of rank of similar size (high-, medium- and low-ranking [76].
In this study, we aimed to investigate the short-term behavioural response to the birth of a younger
sibling. Following Delaunay et al. [53], we compared juveniles who recently experienced the birth of
a younger sibling—within the three months following the birth of the newborn—with juveniles who
did not yet. In our sample of focal observations, juveniles who had experienced the birth of a younger
sibling were 11−32 months old. Therefore, we restricted our dataset to this age range. For juveniles
without a younger sibling, we included any focal observations and scans collected on juveniles aged
4
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rsos R. Soc. Open Sci. 11: 240597
11−32 month old and until the birth of their younger sibling or until they stopped being followed
(when older than 32 months). For juveniles with a younger sibling, we included any focal observations
and scans collected during the first three months following the birth of their sibling. Data from
juveniles who experienced the birth of a younger sibling more than three months ago were excluded
from the study, as their TTS was considered as over. When a newborn died before three months of age,
we excluded all observations of the older sibling following this death. Individuals who were observed
both before and after the birth of their younger sibling were included in both groups, respectively
(with and without a younger sibling, n = 72). We used a total of 4866 focal observations (mean ± s.d.
= 25.48 ± 28.66 per individual) and 6989 scans (mean ± s.d. = 36.40 ± 41.88 per individual) from 191
individuals (n = 79 with a younger sibling, mean age ± s.d. = 21.1 ± 4.6 months; n = 184 without a
younger sibling, mean age ± s.d. = 16.3 ± 3.6 months) born to 76 mothers.
2.4. Statistical analyses
2.4.1. TTS effects on maternal care
To characterize the impact of TTS on maternal care, we used two measures: suckling frequency and
ventral carrying frequency. For each focal observation, we recorded whether the juvenile: (i) suckled
or (ii) was carried ventrally by its mother at least once (yes = 1, no = 0). Preliminary inspection of the
data revealed that juveniles with a younger sibling were no longer suckling or being carried by their
mother, so we provided below a descriptive analysis.
2.4.2. TTS effects on maternal grooming
To assess the effect of TTS on maternal grooming, we first recorded, for each focal observation, whether
the focal juvenile received grooming from its mother at least once (binary, yes/no—model 1), and
whether it groomed its mother (binary, yes/no—model 2). We modelled the probability to receive or
give grooming to the mother with two generalized linear mixed models (GLMMs) with a binomial
error structure.
Second, to investigate the effect of TTS on juveniles’ role in initiating grooming interactions with the
mother, we considered every grooming event for which the initiator was known, and recorded whether
it was initiated by the juvenile (1) or by its mother (0) (binary, initiated by the juvenile yes/no—model
3). We restricted this dataset to the first grooming event recorded during a focal observation, and
discarded any other grooming event that occurred after this first event, regardless of the direction,
because we considered these events as potentially non-independent. We modelled the probability that
a grooming bout was initiated by the juvenile (versus the mother) using a GLMM with a binomial error
structure (model 3) on 396 grooming bouts with a known initiator.
2.4.3. TTS effects on mother–juvenile spatial proximity
To investigate mother–juvenile proximity, we first used scan data to estimate how often a juvenile
was found in close proximity to the mother. For each scan observation, we recorded whether the
juvenile focal was found in proximity (1) or not (0) to its mother. We considered two distinct ranges
of proximity: body contact (binary, yes/no—model 4), and within 5 m from the mother (binary, yes/no
—model 5). We ran two GLMMs with a binomial error structure.
Second, we used focal data to characterize juveniles’ role in the maintenance of spatial proximity.
We considered every approach and leave between the mother and the focal juvenile, and recorded
whether it was initiated by the juvenile (1) or by the mother (0). Using 1199 approaches and 1350 leaves
between the mother and the focal juvenile, we ran two GLMMs with a binomial error structure to
investigate the probability that an approach or a leave were initiated by the juvenile (binary, yes/no—
models 6 and 7, respectively).
2.4.4. TTS effects on mother–juvenile conflict
To investigate the effect of TTS on mother–juvenile conflict, we considered two measures: juve-
nile tantrums and juvenile anxiety-related behaviours (yawning and self-scratches). For each focal
observation, we recorded whether: (i) a tantrum occurred (binary, 1/0—model 8) and (ii) the juvenile
5
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rsos R. Soc. Open Sci. 11: 240597
displayed at least one anxiety-related behaviour (binary, 1/0—model 9). We ran two GLMMs with a
binomial error structure.
2.4.5. Fixed and random effects
For each model, we tested the effect of having recently experienced the birth of a younger sibling
(yes/no), and included the following fixed effects: focal juvenile’s age (in months), sex, birth order
and maternal rank. We tested the interaction terms between the recent birth of a younger sibling
and the focal juvenile’s age and sex (except for model 8 because tantrums were rarely recorded). We
also included two additional fixed effects: (i) in model 3, grooming direction (i.e. grooming received
versus given) and its interaction term with the birth of a younger sibling to test whether juveniles
with a younger sibling initiate more often grooming interactions with their mother than juveniles
without a younger sibling, and especially initiate more often the grooming events where they are
groomed by their mother; (ii) in most models, duration of the focal observation to account for time
out-of-view (in seconds) (except in models 3, 6 and 7 because the probability that a behaviour is
initiated by the juvenile did not depend on the duration of the observation, and models 4 and
5 because based on scan data). In all models, we included the year of observation as a random
effect, and the focal juvenile’s identity nested in the mother identity to account for repeated observa-
tions. The structure of each model (fixed effects and random effects), along with sample sizes and
corresponding predictions are summarized in electronic supplementary material, table S1.
2.4.6. Statistical methods
All statistical analyses were performed using the R Studio software (v. 4.0.2). We ran mixed
models using the glmmTMB function from the glmmTMB package [77], and the bglmer
function from the blme package [78] to confirm the results with a Bayesian approach when-
ever we obtained a singular fit. We assessed fit singularity using the function check_singularity
from the performance package [79]. To control for the age of focal juveniles, we first visually
checked the relationship between age and our response variables. Whenever this relationship
was not linear, we fitted univariate models with a linear, a second- or third-degree polynomial
function to model the effect of age (random effects were also included) and compared model
fits, selecting the model with the lowest Akaike information criterion (AIC) [80]. All quantita-
tive variables were z-transformed (mean = 0, s.d. = 1) using the scale function from the car
package [81] to facilitate model convergence and to compare effect sizes across estimates [82].
To diagnose the presence of multi-collinearities, we computed the variance inflation factor (VIF)
for each predictor of each model using the function check_collinearity from the performance
package. VIFs were inferior to 2 in all cases, indicating that multi-collinearities did not impact
coefficients’ estimation in our models. To test the significance of our fixed factors, we calculated
the likelihood ratio test (LRT) and its associated p-values for each model using the drop1
function, and computed the 95% Wald confidence intervals. Non-significant interactions were
removed from the full model to limit the risk of overparametrization and facilitate the interpre-
tation of simple effects. Finally, we checked the distribution of the residuals using ‘simulateResi-
duals’ from the DHARMa package [83].
3. Results
3.1. TTS effects on maternal care
Juveniles with a younger sibling were no longer nursed or carried by their mother, regardless
of their age at the time of their younger sibling’s birth (figure 1a,b). In contrast, suckling
and carrying probabilities gradually decreased with age for juveniles without a younger sibling
and ceased around 25 months old for suckling (figure 1a) and 21 months old for carrying
(figure 1b). Juveniles without a younger sibling were still suckling in approximately 15% of the
observations at 11–12 months old (mean probability ± s.d. = 0.151 ± 0.360, i.e. once every 30
min), in approximately 7% at 17–18 months old (mean ± s.d. = 0.066 ± 0.248, i.e. once every
6
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rsos R. Soc. Open Sci. 11: 240597
75 min) and in approximately 3% at 21–22 months old (mean ± s.d. = 0.034 ± 0.181, i.e. once
every 145 min). Similarly, they were still being carried in approximately 3% of the observations
at 11–12 months old (mean ± s.d. = 0.029 ± 0.169, i.e. once every 170 min), and in 0.4% at
17–18 months old (mean ± s.d. = 0.004 ± 0.064, i.e. once every 1250 min).
3.2. TTS effects on maternal grooming
Juveniles in TTS were significantly less likely to be groomed by their mother than juveniles
without a younger sibling (odds ratio (OR) = 0.58, mean probability ± s.d. = 0.08 ± 0.27 for
juveniles with a younger sibling; 0.09 ± 0.29 for those without, table 1, figure 2a), although the
difference in raw grooming probability was small between the two categories of juveniles. The
probability to groom the mother tended to be influenced by the interaction between the sex of the
focal juvenile and its sibling status (p = 0.091): female juveniles with a younger sibling tended to
be more likely to groom their mother than females without a younger sibling (mean probability
± s.d. = 0.06 ± 0.24 for females with a younger sibling; 0.01 ± 0.11 for females without), while
male juveniles tended to be equally likely to groom their mother independently of their sibling
status (males with a younger sibling: 0.01 ± 0.10; without: 0.01 ± 0.08, table 1, figure 2b). Juveniles
born to medium- or low-ranking females were also significantly less likely to be groomed by their
mother than those born to high-ranking females (OR = 0.53 and 0.49, respectively, table 1), but
maternal rank did not influence the probability to groom the mother. Juveniles’ age and birth
(b)
0.03
Without
With
Without
With
0.02
0.01
0.00
(a)
0.15
0.10
0.05
0.00
11 13 15 17 19 2111 13 15 17 19 21 23 25
Age (in months)
Probability to suckle from the mother
Probability to be carried by the mother
Figure 1. Probability to (a) suckle and (b) be carried by the mother during a focal observation according to juvenile’s age and sibling
status, using raw data from 4866 focal observations on 191 juveniles. In all panels, “With” refers to juveniles who recently experienced
the birth of a younger sibling and “Without” those who did not. For graphical purposes, we pooled values per age class so that “11”
represent the mean probability to suckle/be carried for all juveniles aged 11–12 months old, “13”, juveniles 13 and 14 months old etc.
In (a), “25” represents the pooled values for all juveniles older than 25 months old as suckling was no longer observed after this age,
and in (b), “21” represents the pooled values for all juveniles older than 21 months old as carrying was no longer observed after this
age. Dots represent the average raw value for a given age class and vertical bars represent the standard errors.
7
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rsos R. Soc. Open Sci. 11: 240597
order did not influence the probability to receive or give grooming to the mother, and both sexes
were equally likely to be groomed by the mother (table 1).
Juveniles with and without a younger sibling were as likely to be the initiator of a grooming bout
with their mother (table 2). Juveniles were significantly more likely to be the ones initiating grooming
bouts with their mother as they grew older (OR = 1.47, table 2). Juveniles’ sex, birth order or maternal
rank did not influence juveniles’ probability to be the ones initiating a grooming with the mother (table
2).
3.3. TTS effects on mother–juvenile spatial proximity
Mother–juvenile proximity was partially influenced by the birth of a younger sibling: although the
probability to be in body contact with the mother was similar in both groups (table 3, figure 3a), the
probability to be within 5 m around the mother was significantly influenced by the interaction between
juveniles’ sex and sibling status (table 3, figure 3b). Male juveniles with a younger sibling were less
likely to be within 5 m of their mother than males without a younger sibling (mean probability ± s.e.
= 0.23 ± 0.042 and 0.46 ± 0.50, respectively), while female juveniles were as likely to be within 5 m as
those without (0.40 ± 0.49 for those with a sibling, 0.41 ± 0.49 for those without). Juveniles were less
likely to be in body contact and within 5 m around their mother as they grew older (OR = 0.80 and 0.77,
respectively), and first-born juveniles were more likely to be in body contact than later-born juveniles
Table 1. Results of the mixed models analysing the probability to receive (model 1) and give (model 2) grooming to the mother
during a focal observation. Estimates, 95% confidence intervals (CIs), LRT statistics and p-values of the predictors were estimated using
4866 focal observations on 191 juveniles (n = 79 with a younger sibling, n = 184 without). Juvenile identity nested in mother identity,
and year of observation were included as random effects. For categorical predictors, the reference category is indicated between
parentheses. Significant effects are indicated in bold.
fixed factor level estimate 2.5% 97.5% LRT p-value
model 1: probability to receive grooming from the mother
intercept −1.511 −2.278 −0.745
presence of a younger sibling (no) yes −0.546 −1.006 −0.086 5.641 0.018
juvenile’s age 0.096 −0.045 0.237 1.777 0.182
juvenile’s sex (female) male −0.249 −0.549 0.051 2.695 0.101
juvenile’s bir th order (first-born) second-born −0.410 −0.894 0.073 3.632 0.163
later-born −0.019 −0.381 0.343
maternal rank (high-ranking) medium-ranking −0.627 −1.021 −0.233 15.292 <0.001
low-ranking −0.720 −1.088 −0.353
focal duration 0.708 0.555 0.861 105.755 <0.001
model 2: probability to groom the mother
intercept −4.595 −5.778 −3.411
presence of a younger sibling (no) yes 1.379 0.337 2.421
juvenile’s age 0.157 −0.183 0.497 0.710 0.400
juvenile’s sex (female) male −0.904 −1.744 −0.064
juvenile’s bir th order (first-born) second-born −0.774 −1.923 0.376 3.014 0.222
later-born −0.669 −1.561 0.223
maternal rank (high-ranking) medium-ranking 0.133 −0.844 1.110 0.763 0.683
low-ranking −0.266 −1.249 0.717
focal duration 1.317 0.691 1.942 33.206 <0.001
presence of a younger sibling (no)*Juvenile’s
sex (female)
(yes, male) −1.337 −3.015 0.340 2.849 0.091
8
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rsos R. Soc. Open Sci. 11: 240597
(b)
***
(a)
0.7
0.6
0.5
0.4
0.3
0.2
0.1
0.0
0.40
0.35
0.30
0.25
0.20
0.15
0.10
0.05
0.00
Without With Female Male
Probability to groom the mother
Probability to receive grooming from the mother
Without
With
ns
Figure 2. Influence of the birth of the younger sibling on mother–juvenile grooming interactions. In all panels, ‘With’ refers to
juveniles who recently experienced the birth of a younger sibling and ‘Without’ those who did not. (a) Predicted probability to be
groomed by the mother depending on the sibling status during a focal observation. (b) Predicted probability to groom the mother
depending on the sibling status and focal juvenile’s sex (females on the left side of the graph, males on the right). Dots are fitted
values from the models (obtained with the function ‘fitted’ from the package stats), and boxplots show the median of the distribution
of the fitted values (black horizontal bar), the 25th and 75th quartiles (bottom and top of the boxes, respectively) and the whiskers
include a maximum of 1.5 times the interquartile range. The effect of the predictor ‘Sibling status’ and the associated p-values are
shown. ‘ns’: not significant (p > 0.05), *: p < 0.05, **: p < 0.01, ***: p < 0.001.
Table 2. Results of the mixed model testing the probability that a grooming event is initiated by the juvenile (versus the mother,
model 3). Estimates, 95% CIs, LRT statistics and p-values of the predictors were estimated using 396 grooming events on 128 juveniles
(n = 23 with a younger sibling, n = 120 without). Grooming direction refers to grooming received from the mother (mother–juvenile,
n = 366) or given to the mother (juvenile–mother, n = 30). Juvenile identity nested in mother identity, and year of observation were
included as random effects. For categorical predictors, the reference category is indicated between parentheses. Significant effects are
indicated in bold.
fixed factor level estimate 2.5% 97.5% LRT p-value
model 3: probability that a grooming event is initiated by the juvenile
intercept 1.340 −0.214 2.894
presence of a younger sibling (no) yes 0.769 −0.270 1.808 2.371 0.124
juvenile’s age 0.386 0.058 0.714 5.371 0.021
juvenile’s sex (female) male −0.456 −1.224 0.313 1.626 0.202
grooming direction (juvenile–mother) mother–juvenile −3.662 −4.980 −2.344 47.152 <0.001
juvenile’s bir th order (first-born) second-born 0.092 −1.237 1.421 1.388 0.500
later-born 0.494 −0.472 1.461
maternal rank (high-ranking) medium-ranking 0.533 −0.487 1.553 1.271 0.530
low-ranking 0.157 −0.777 1.092
9
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rsos R. Soc. Open Sci. 11: 240597
(OR = 1.67, table 3). Maternal rank did not predict the probability to be in body contact or within 5 m
around the mother (table 3).
Juveniles were generally responsible for initiating an approach to their mother, independently from
their sibling status (table 4). The probability to leave one another was similar for juveniles and their
mother in general, and independent from the birth of a younger sibling (table 4). Juveniles born to
higher-ranking females were more likely to initiate an approach to their mother (OR = 2.10, table 4).
Juveniles’ age, sex and birth order did not influence the probability to approach or leave their mother,
and the probability to leave the mother was independent from maternal rank (table 4).
3.4. TTS effects on mother–offspring conflicts
Tantrums were uncommon, and juveniles’ probability to throw a tantrum was independent from their
sibling status, decreased as they grew older, and was lower for juveniles born to low-ranking females
than those born to high- and medium-ranking females (OR = 0.39 and 0.53, respectively, table 5).
Juveniles’ sex and birth order did not influence the probability to display a tantrum (table 5).
Finally, juveniles with a younger sibling were as likely to display anxiety-related behaviours
as juveniles without a younger sibling (table 5). Juveniles born to low-ranking females displayed
significantly more anxiety-related behaviours than juveniles born to high- or medium-ranking females
(OR = 1.28 and 1.32, respectively, table 5). Juveniles’ probability to display anxiety-related behaviours
was independent from their sex, age and birth order (table 5).
Table 3. Results of the mixed models analysing juveniles’ probability to be in body contact (model 4) or within 5 m around their
mother (model 5) during a scan observation. Estimates, 95% CIs, LRT statistics and p-values of the predictors were estimated using
6989 scans on 192 juveniles (n = 78 with a younger sibling, n = 184 without). Juvenile identity nested in mother identity, and year
of observation were included as random effects. For categorical predictors, the reference category is indicated between parentheses.
Significant effects are indicated in bold.
fixed factor level estimate 2.5% 97.5% LRT p-value
model 4: probability to be in body contact with the mother during a scan observation
intercept −1.024 −1.732 −0.316
presence of a younger sibling (no) yes −0.287 −0.689 0.115 1.991 0.158
juvenile’s age −0.228 −0.340 −0.116 16.237 <0.001
juvenile’s sex (female) male −0.147 −0.464 0.169 0.831 0.362
juvenile’s bir th order (first-born) second-born −0.619 −1.114 −0.124 6.123 0.047
later-born −0.328 −0.736 0.080
maternal rank (high-ranking) medium-ranking −0.196 −0.673 0.281 4.528 0.104
low-ranking −0.510 −0.974 −0.046
model 5: probability to be within 5 m around the mother during a scan observation
intercept 0.561 −0.214 1.337
presence of a younger sibling (no) yes 0.107 −0.303 0.517
juvenile’s age −0.258 −0.339 −0.177 39.442 <0.001
juvenile’s sex (female) male 0.053 −0.293 0.400
juvenile’s bir th order (first-born) second-born −0.061 −0.586 0.464 2.710 0.258
later-born −0.329 −0.767 0.109
maternal rank (high-ranking) medium-ranking −0.015 −0.483 0.453 0.005 0.998
low-ranking −0.003 −0.461 0.456
presence of a younger sibling (no)*juvenile’s
sex (female)
(yes, male) −1.011 −1.543 −0.479 13.931 <0.001
10
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rsos R. Soc. Open Sci. 11: 240597
4. Discussion
In this study, we investigated the behavioural changes in maternal care and mother–juvenile relation-
ship during the TTS in wild mandrills. First, following our prediction (P1), we found that nursing
and maternal carrying abruptly stopped after the birth of a younger sibling (P1a), thus juveniles who
entered TTS at a younger age experienced a greater loss of maternal care (P1b). Second, we showed
that juveniles with a younger sibling received less maternal grooming (P2), although the magnitude
of this loss was small (they lost on average 1/10th of time of maternal grooming compared to their
counterparts). Contrary to our prediction (P2), female juveniles with a younger sibling tended to
groom their mother more often than females without a sibling (six times more), while males tended to
groom their mother independently of their sibling status, and TTS did not impact juveniles’ probability
to be the ones initiating a grooming bout with the mother in both sexes (P3). Third, we found that
juvenile males spent less time close to their mother following the birth of their sibling, partially
supporting our prediction (P2), although spatial proximity remained stable for juvenile females.
However, juveniles from both sexes were mostly responsible for maintaining spatial proximity with
their mother, independently from the birth of their sibling (contrary to P3). Finally, we showed that
juveniles with a younger sibling neither displayed more tantrums nor signs of anxiety than those
without (contrary to P4). We discuss below the implications of our findings for the understanding of
parent–offspring conflict and sibling rivalry in primates.
First, mandrill mothers dramatically decreased their level of maternal investment toward their
juvenile when giving birth, as they stopped nursing and carrying them, and groomed them less often,
(b)
ns ***
(a)
0.7
0.6
0.5
0.4
0.3
0.2
0.1
0.0
0.2
0.4
0.6
0.8
1.0
0.0
Without With Female Male
Probability to be within 5 m around the mother
Probability to be in body contact with the mother
Without
With
ns
Figure 3. Influence of the birth of the younger sibling on mother–juvenile proximity. In all panels, ‘With’ refers to juveniles who
recently experienced the birth of a younger sibling and ‘Without’ those who did not. (a) Predicted probability to be in body contact
with the mother depending on the sibling status during a scan observation. (b) Predicted probability to be within 5 m around the
mother depending on the sibling status and focal juvenile’s sex (females on the left side of the graph, males on the right). Dots are
fitted values from the models (obtained with the function ‘fitted’ from the package stats), and boxplots show the median of the
distribution of the fitted values (black horizontal bar), the 25th and 75th quartiles (bottom and top of the boxes, respectively) and
the whiskers include a maximum of 1.5 times the interquartile range. The effect of the predictor ‘Sibling status’ and the associated
p-values are shown. ‘ns’: not significant (p > 0.05), *: p < 0.05, **: p < 0.01, ***: p < 0.001.
11
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rsos R. Soc. Open Sci. 11: 240597
regardless of the age of their juvenile offspring. Thus, in mandrills, the birth of the younger sibling
terminates the weaning process for the older sibling, as mothers are unable to nurse two offspring
at the same time. In free-ranging, provisioned rhesus macaques, juveniles also stopped suckling their
mother after the birth of their sibling [44], although mothers in this population can sometimes nurse
two consecutive offspring at the same time [37]. By contrast, in chacma baboons and bonobos, juveniles
were already weaned several months before their mother gave birth [53,54]. Mothers’ ability to nurse
while being pregnant might largely depend on their ecology and life history. For instance, mandrills
live in a rich environment where food is unlikely to be limiting, which, like provisioned macaques,
might enable them to face the cumulated costs of late lactation and early pregnancy at the same time
[63]. In contrast, chacma baboons live in an arid environment where food is drastically limited, which
might force females to space out births by weaning their previous offspring before starting a new
reproductive cycle. In bonobos and orang-utans, where the older juvenile is also weaned before the
birth of the younger sibling, mothers may start a new reproductive cycle only when the older juvenile
has reached nutritional independence and learnt most skills needed to extract food [54,84].
In addition to these two obvious forms of maternal care, mandrill mothers also groomed their
juvenile less often, and this decrease did not seem to be driven by the mother or the juvenile in
particular, as juveniles’ and mothers’ probability to be the one initiating grooming bouts or close
proximity did not change with the TTS. These results partially resemble previous findings in macaques
and industrialized human societies, where maternal grooming or maternal attention and responsive-
ness (in macaques and humans, respectively) decreased after the birth of a younger sibling [44,56,59].
However, juveniles’ reactions to these changes seem highly variable across studies and species: in the
Cayo Santiago macaques, juveniles seemed to be responsible for the decrease in maternal grooming
[44]. Maintaining a strong bond with the mother may be less critical in such free-ranging, provisioned
population, where energy and mortality costs are reduced (no predator, supplemental feeding). In
contrast, in humans, children initiated more interactions with their mother following the birth of a
Table 4. Results of the mixed models analysing the probability that an approach (model 6) or a leave (model 7) are initiated by
the juvenile (versus its mother). Estimates, 95% CIs, LRT statistics and p-values of the predictors were estimated using respectively,
1199 approaches between the mother and the juvenile on 134 juveniles (n = 21 with a younger sibling, n = 128 without), and 1350
leaves on 141 juveniles (n = 33 with a younger sibling, n = 132 without). Juvenile identity nested in mother identity, and year of
observation were included as random effects. For categorical predictors, the reference category is indicated between parentheses.
Significant effects are indicated in bold.
fixed factor level estimate 2.5% 97.5% LRT p-value
model 6: probability that an approach is initiated by the juvenile
intercept 2.645 1.764 3.526
presence of a younger sibling (no) yes −0.351 −1.206 0.503 0.758 0.384
juvenile’s age −0.116 −0.278 0.045 1.900 0.168
juvenile’s sex (female) male −0.084 −0.400 0.231 0.144 0.705
juvenile’s bir th order (first-born) second-born −0.263 −0.730 0.204 1.372 0.504
later-born −0.108 −0.505 0.289
maternal rank (high-ranking) medium-ranking −0.375 −0.874 0.125 12.111 0.002
low-ranking −0.742 −1.213 −0.270
model 7: probability that a leave is initiated by the juvenile
intercept −0.326 −0.910 0.257
presence of a younger sibling (no) yes 0.201 −0.500 0.901 0.373 0.542
juvenile’s age 0.052 −0.086 0.189 0.400 0.527
juvenile’s sex (female) male −0.193 −0.471 0.085 1.920 0.166
juvenile’s bir th order (first-born) second-born 0.047 −0.379 0.473 2.640 0.267
later-born 0.256 −0.101 0.614
maternal rank (high-ranking) medium-ranking −0.212 −0.625 0.201 3.220 0.200
low-ranking 0.094 −0.298 0.486
12
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rsos R. Soc. Open Sci. 11: 240597
sibling, as a likely attempt to compensate for the loss of maternal attention [55–58]. In chacma baboons,
although maternal behaviour did not change with TTS, juveniles nevertheless solicited their mother
more often [53].
Surprisingly, age did not mediate the effect of TTS on mother–juvenile relationships. As the birth
of the younger sibling terminated the weaning process for the juvenile and the loss incurred in terms
of suckling was larger for young juveniles, we were expecting juveniles who entered TTS at a younger
age to experience greater changes in their relationship with the mother. Indeed, in rhesus macaques,
juveniles who experienced a greater decrease in nursing after their sibling’s birth showed more signs
of distress [62]. In humans, as well, the increase in anxiety, clinginess and confrontational behaviours
toward the mother after the birth of the newborn was more pronounced in younger children [55,59,85].
Recent studies in macaques and baboons also showed that having a too close in age younger sibling
increased mortality risks at all ages for the older sibling [37,38], most likely because it induced a
decrease in maternal investment. In mandrills, however, the birth of a younger sibling was perceived
similarly and independently of age (and thus, of weaning). A potential explanation could be that
juvenile mandrills adjust to their mother s reproductive pace. Juveniles experiencing TTS at a younger
age could speed up in the acquisition of autonomy when their mother becomes pregnant, so that they
are ‘ready’ and independent enough for the birth of their younger sibling. This could explain why they
do not express more anxiety-related behaviours nor initiate a larger proportion of the interactions with
their mother when experiencing TTS earlier in life.
In contrast to two recent studies in wild baboons and bonobos [53,54], juvenile mandrills’ reactions
to TTS appeared to be sex-specific. Indeed, juvenile males associated less often with their mother,
while juvenile females tended to increase their grooming rate towards them. These differences may
be triggered by several, non-mutually exclusive factors. First, the tendency of females to increase
grooming could result from an attraction to the newborn, which is a common behaviour in primates,
Table 5. Results of the mixed models analysing the probability to display a tantrum (model 8) and anxiety-related behaviours (model
9) during a focal observation. Estimates, 95% CIs, LRT statistics and p-values of the predictors were estimated using 4866 focal
observations on 191 juveniles (n = 79 with a younger sibling, n = 184 without). Juvenile identity nested in mother identity, and year
of observation were included as random effects. For categorical predictors, the reference category is indicated between parentheses.
Significant effects are indicated in bold.
fixed factor level estimate 2.5% 97.5% LRT p-value
model 8: probability of tantrum
intercept −4.109 −5.098 −3.119
presence of a younger sibling (no) yes −0.373 −1.531 0.784 0.500 0.480
juvenile’s age −0.325 −0.609 −0.041 5.384 0.020
juvenile’s sex (female) male 0.489 −0.082 1.060 2.680 0.102
juvenile’s bir th order (first-born) second-born −0.197 −1.049 0.655 0.749 0.688
later-born −0.299 −0.995 0.397
maternal rank (high-ranking) medium-ranking −0.383 −1.089 0.322 8.330 0.016
low-ranking −1.024 −1.754 −0.293
focal duration 0.445 0.199 0.692 13.946 <0.001
model 9: probability of anxiety-related behaviours
intercept 0.202 −0.281 0.684
presence of a younger sibling (no) yes 0.112 −0.161 0.385 0.700 0.403
juvenile’s age −0.073 −0.154 0.008 3.280 0.070
juvenile’s sex (female) male 0.061 −0.124 0.246 0.450 0.502
juvenile’s bir th order (first-born) second-born −0.095 −0.384 0.194 2.006 0.367
later-born −0.164 −0.398 0.071
maternal rank (high-ranking) medium-ranking −0.027 −0.277 0.222 7.097 0.029
low-ranking 0.247 0.009 0.484
focal duration 0.669 0.603 0.734 440.446 <0.001
13
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rsos R. Soc. Open Sci. 11: 240597
where juvenile and adult females often try to touch or handle infants of others [86–88]. A common
way to gain access to the infant is to initiate a grooming with the mother [89–91]. Second, mandrills’
sex-specific reactions might reflect life history differences between sexes [72]. Indeed, in such matrilin-
eal society, maintaining and/or reinforcing close social bonds with the mother could be more advanta-
geous for juvenile females than for males, as strong social bonds with close maternal kin in adulthood
translate into fitness advantages in female cercopithecines [92,93]. In these species, immature males
typically form weaker bonds with their mother and are less integrated in their matriline, and such sex
differences may emerge as early as the first year of life (e.g. [71–73,94]). Hence, the birth of a younger
sibling may foster the independence of juvenile males, as in free-ranging rhesus macaques [44], while
juvenile females may instead react by strengthening their bond with their mother. More generally, TTS
may mark a turning point where juveniles start to balance the benefits of remaining attached with the
mother (e.g. decreased predation risk, access to social learning and foraging skills) with the benefits of
associating with peers (e.g. development of their own social network outside of their mother’s), which
could lead to different trade-offs between sexes.
Despite facing a drastic decrease in maternal care, juvenile mandrills showed little indication that
this transition was a source of conflict with their mother, or of anxiety. Indeed, they did not throw more
tantrums or display more signs of anxiety, nor did they initiate a greater proportion of the interactions
with their mother. Tantrums have often been considered as a conspicuous form of behavioural conflict
over parental investment in primates, expressed during developmental milestones where mother and
juvenile’s evolutionary interests diverge, such as weaning [43]. In mandrills, tantrums are often less
conspicuous than in baboons (personal observation), and are already rare by the age juveniles enter
TTS. Although the youngest juveniles were still able to throw tantrums, conflicts with the mother could
be expressed through more subtle behaviours. In chacma baboons for instance, juveniles solicited their
mother more often after the birth of their younger sibling, which suggests a conflict over the amount
of maternal investment received [53]. Surprisingly, juvenile mandrills did not seem more anxious
after the birth of their sibling. Studies in several cercopithecine species reported increased signs of
distress or ‘depression’ during TTS [44,47,53,62]. In bonobos, a recent study reported a strong increase
in cortisol level following the birth of a younger sibling, but they did not investigate whether this
physiological change was associated with observable distress behaviours [54]. In mandrills, the birth
of a younger sibling could also induce physiological changes that are not associated with observable
distress behaviour. Alternatively, the lack of conflict and anxiety behaviours in mandrills could also
reflect the absence of overt conflict with the mother, which could be the case if juveniles are already
independent enough by the time their mother gives birth.
Finally, the decrease in maternal care induced by the birth of the younger sibling could translate
into sibling rivalry [2]. As mothers can nurse and be pregnant at the same time in mandrills, pregnancy
could represent a perfect case study of interbrood sibling competition, where the monopolization of
maternal care through suckling by the older sibling could directly reduce the viability of the foetus.
After the birth of a younger sibling, competition for milk could also occur, as in Galapagos fur seals
and sea lions [35], if the older sibling disagrees with the abrupt end of weaning that it experiences.
In mandrills, juveniles’ aggressive behaviours toward their infant sibling have never been observed,
but older siblings sometimes attempt—and even more rarely succeed—to suckle their mother at the
same time as their younger sibling (B.R.T. and M.J.E.C., personal observation). Similar observations
were reported from the free-ranging rhesus macaque population in Cayo Santiago, with mothers
occasionally nursing two consecutive offspring at the same time [37]. In Cayo Santiago, despite living
in a relatively rich environment, short IBIs between two siblings reduced survival chances for both
the older and younger sibling, most likely due to the dilution of maternal resources [37]. Recent
findings from the same mandrill population showed that the duration of IBIs was highly correlated
to female’s rank, with higher-ranking females giving birth to a new offspring every year on average,
while lower-ranking females do so once every 2 years [63], a difference that was not observed in
provisioned captive mandrills [69]. Therefore, juveniles who entered TTS at the youngest ages might be
born to females who can afford the combined energetic costs of gestation and lactation, with little costs
for the older sibling, explaining the absence of overt mother–offspring conflict during TTS. Whether
mandrill juveniles could pay other costs, similar to those of rhesus macaques, such as decreased
survival or slower development for instance, when having a close-in-age younger sibling, remains to
be studied.
For young mandrills, the birth of a younger sibling induces an abrupt decrease in maternal care,
terminating the weaning process for those who enter TTS at a young age. Juveniles’ reactions to TTS
appeared to be sex-specific, with females tending to increase affiliation with their mother while males
14
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rsos R. Soc. Open Sci. 11: 240597
decreased their association with the mother, but both sexes did not show more conflicts with the
mother or more anxiety-related behaviours. These results suggest that, although TTS could induce
mother–offspring conflict and sibling competition over maternal investment, young mandrills adjust
their developmental pace to their mothers reproductive pace. Overall, our study adds to recent studies
in primates investigating this intriguing developmental milestone, and highlights a variety of reactions
across species, probably mirroring contrasts in life histories and ecologies.
Ethics. This study was approved by an authorization from the CENAREST institute (permit no. AR017/22/
MESRSTTCA/CENAREST/CG/CST/CSAR).
Data accessibility. All datasets and codes used in this study are available at Dryad [95].
Supplementary material is available online [96].
Declaration of AI use. We have not used AI-assisted technologies in creating this article.
Authors’ contributions. A.D.: conceptualization, data curation, formal analysis, investigation, methodology, writing—
original draft, writing—review and editing; O.C.-D.: data curation, formal analysis, writing—review and editing;
B.R.-T.: investigation, writing—review and editing; L.S.: investigation, writing—review and editing; B.N.: project
administration, resources, writing—review and editing; E.H.: conceptualization, funding acquisition, methodol-
ogy, supervision, writing—review and editing; M.J.E.C.: conceptualization, data curation, funding acquisition,
methodology, project administration, resources, supervision, writing—review and editing.
All authors gave final approval for publication and agreed to be held accountable for the work performed
therein.
Conflict of interest declaration. We declare we have no competing interests.
Funding. This study was funded by several grants that allowed long-term data collection: SEEG Lekedi (CNRS
Ecologie & Environnement) and a MPI Sabbatical Award to M.J.E.C., and Agence Nationale de la Recherche (ANR
SLEEP 17-CE02- 0002 to M.J.E.C. and ANR ERS-17-CE02-0008, 2018–2021 to E.H.).
Acknowledgements. We are grateful to past and present field assistants of the Mandrills Project who collect daily
behavioural data on the study population, and in particular to Mélanie Harté, Jade Meric de Bellefon, Steven Abaga,
Paul Amblard-Rambert, Anna Cryer and George Havill who tackled the challenge of recognizing juvenile mandrills
who all look the same to collect behavioural data on them. We further thank the SODEPAL-COMILOG society
(ERAMET group) for their long-term logistical support and contribution to the Mandrillus Project. This is a Project
Mandrillus publication number 35 and ISEM 2024-154 SUD.
References
1. Trivers RL. 1972 Parental investment and sexual selection. See http://joelvelasco.net/teaching/3330/trivers72-parentalinvestment.pdf (accessed
31 August 2020).
2. Trivers RL. 1974 Parent-offspring conflict. Am. Zool. 14, 249–264. (doi:10.1093/icb/14.1.249)
3. Stearns SC. 1992 The evolution of life histories. vol. 249. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
4. Mock DW, Parker GA. 1997 The evolution of sibling rivalry. vol. 464. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
5. Roulin A, Dreiss AN. 2012 Sibling competition and cooperation over parental care. In The evolution of parental care, pp. 133–149. Oxford, UK:
Oxford University Press.
6. Schrader M, Jarrett BJM, Kilner RM. 2018 Parental care and sibling competition independently increase phenotypic variation among burying
beetle siblings. Evolution 72, 2546–2552. (doi:10.1111/evo.13607)
7. De Kogel CH, Prijs HJ. 1996 Effects of brood size manipulations on sexual attractiveness of offspring in the zebra finch. Anim. Behav. 51, 699–
708. (doi:10.1006/anbe.1996.0073)
8. Fisher HS, Hook KA, Weber WD, Hoekstra HE. 2018 Sibling rivalry: males with more brothers develop larger Testes. Ecol. Evol. 8, 8197–8203. (doi:
10.1002/ece3.4337)
9. Hofer H, East ML. 2008 Siblicide in Serengeti spotted hyenas: a long-term study of maternal input and cub survival. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 62,
341–351. (doi:10.1007/s00265-007-0421-3)
10. Drummond H, Rodríguez C. 2013 Costs of growing up as a subordinate sibling are passed to the next generation in blue-footed boobies. J. Evol.
Biol. 26, 625–634. (doi:10.1111/jeb.12087)
11. Nettle D, Monaghan P, Gillespie R, Brilot B, Bedford T, Bateson M. 2015 An experimental demonstration that early-life competitive
disadvantage accelerates telomere loss. Proc. R. Soc. B 282, 20141610. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2014.1610)
12. Verhulst S, Holveck MJ, Riebel K. 2006 Long-term effects of manipulated natal brood size on metabolic rate in zebra finches. Biol. Lett. 2, 478–
480. (doi:10.1098/rsbl.2006.0496)
13. Fey K, Trillmich F. 2008 Sibling competition in guinea pigs (Cavia aperea f. porcellus): scrambling for mother’s teats is stressful. Behav. Ecol.
Sociobiol. 62, 321–329. (doi:10.1007/s00265-007-0419-x)
14. Guenther A, Trillmich F. 2015 Within-litter differences in personality and physiology relate to size differences among siblings in cavies. Physiol.
Behav. 145, 22–28. (doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.03.026)
15
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rsos R. Soc. Open Sci. 11: 240597
15. Stauffer J, Panda B, Ilmonen P. 2018 Telomere length, sibling competition and development of antioxidant defense in wild house mice. Mech.
Ageing Dev. 169, 45–52. (doi:10.1016/j.mad.2017.10.002)
16. Bebbington K, Kingma SA, Fairfield EA, Spurgin LG, Komdeur J, Richardson DS. 2017 Consequences of sibling rivalry vary across life in a
passerine bird. Behav. Ecol. 28, 407–418. (doi:10.1093/beheco/arw167)
17. Ekman J, Eggers S, Griesser M. 2002 Fighting to stay: the role of sibling rivalry for delayed dispersal. Anim. Behav. 64, 453–459. (doi:10.1006/
anbe.2002.3075)
18. Hudson R, Bautista A, Reyes-Meza V, Montor JM, Rödel HG. 2011 The effect of siblings on early development: a potential contributor to
personality differences in mammals. Dev. Psychobiol. 53, 564–574. (doi:10.1002/dev.20535)
19. Braun BM, Hunt GL. 1983 Brood reduction in black-legged kittiwakes. Auk 100, 469–476. (doi:10.1093/auk/100.2.469)
20. Fujioka M. 1985 Sibling competition and siblicide in asynchronously-hatching broods of the cattle egret Bubulcus ibis. Anim. Behav. 33, 1228–
1242. (doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(85)80183-4)
21. Lougheed LW, Anderson DJ. 1999 Parent blue-footed boobies suppress siblicidal behavior of offspring. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 45, 11–18. (doi:10.
1007/s002650050535)
22. Andersen IL, Nævdal E, Bøe KE. 2011 Maternal investment, sibling competition, and offspring survival with increasing litter size and parity in
pigs (Sus scrofa). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 65, 1159–1167. (doi:10.1007/s00265-010-1128-4)
23. Drummond H. 2006 Dominance in vertebrate broods and litters. Q. Rev. Biol. 81, 3–32. (doi:10.1086/503922)
24. Hudson R, Trillmich F. 2008 Sibling competition and cooperation in mammals: challenges, developments and prospects. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.
62, 299–307. (doi:10.1007/s00265-007-0417-z)
25. Silk JB, Altmann J, Alberts SC . 2006 Social relationships among adult female baboons (Papio cynocephalus) I. Variation in the strength of social
bonds. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 61, 183–195. (doi:10.1007/s00265-006-0249-2)
26. Silk JB, Alberts SC, Altmann J. 2006 Social relationships among adult female baboons (Papio cynocephalus) II. Variation in the quality and
stability of social bonds. Behav. Ecol. S ociobiol. 61, 197–204. (doi:10.1007/s00265-006-0250-9)
27. Lynch EC, Lummaa V, Htut W, Lahdenperä M. 2019 Evolutionary significance of maternal kinship in a long-lived mammal. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B
374, 20180067. (doi:10.1098/rstb.2018.0067)
28. Weiss MN, Ellis S, Franks DW, Nielsen MLK, Cant MA, Johnstone RA, Ellifrit DK, Balcomb KC, Croft DP. 2023 Costly lifetime maternal investment in
killer whales. Curr. Biol. 33, 744–748. (doi:10.1016/j.cub.2022.12.057)
29. Clutton-Brock TH, Guinness FE, Albon SD. 1982 Red deer: behavior and ecology of two sexes. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
30. Clutton-Brock TH. 1991 The evolution of parental care. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. See https://www.degruyter.com/document/doi/
10.1515/9780691206981/html (accessed 26 April 2021).
31. Samuni L, Tkaczynski P, Deschner T, Löhrrich T, Wittig RM, Crockford C. 2020 Maternal effects on offspring growth indicate post-weaning
juvenile dependence in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus). Front. Zool. 17, 1. (doi:10.1186/s12983-019-0343-8)
32. Crockford C, Samuni L, Vigilant L, Wittig RM. 2020 Postweaning maternal care increases male chimpanzee reproductive success. Sci. Adv. 6,
eaaz5746. (doi:10.1126/sciadv.aaz5746)
33. Stanton MA, Lonsdorf EV, Murray CM, Pusey AE. 2020 Consequences of maternal loss before and after weaning in male and female wild
chimpanzees. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 74, 22. (doi:10.1007/s00265-020-2804-7)
34. Andres D, Clutton-Brock TH, Kruuk LEB, Pemberton JM, Stopher KV, Ruckstuhl KE. 2013 Sex differences in the consequences of maternal loss in a
long-lived mammal, the red deer (Cervus elaphus). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 67, 1249–1258. (doi:10.1007/s00265-013-1552-3)
35. Trillmich F , Wolf JBW. 2008 Parent–offspring and sibling conflict in Galápagos fur seals and sea lions. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 62, 363–375. (doi:
10.1007/s00265-007-0423-1)
36. Leippert D, Goymann W, Hofer H. 2000 Between-litter siblicide in captive Indian false vampire bats (Megaderma lyra). J. Zool. 251, 535–547.
(doi:10.1017/S0952836900228136)
37. Lee DS, Ruiz-Lambides AV, Higham JP. 2019 Higher offspring mortality with short interbirth intervals in free-ranging rhesus macaques. Proc.
Natl Acad. Sci. USA 116, 6057–6062. (doi:10.1073/pnas.1817148116)
38. Tung J, Archie EA, Altmann J, Alberts SC. 2016 Cumulative early life adversity predicts longevity in wild baboons. Nat. Commun. 7, 11181. (doi:
10.1038/ncomms11181)
39. Conde-Agudelo A, Rosas-Bermúdez A, Kafur y-Goeta AC. 2006 Birth spacing and risk of adverse perinatal outcomes: a meta-analysis. JAMA 295,
1809–1823. (doi:10.1001/jama.295.15.1809)
40. Rutstein SO. 2005 Effects of preceding birth intervals on neonatal, infant and under-five years mortality and nutritional status in developing
countries: evidence from the demographic and health surveys. Intl. J. Gynecol. Obstet. 89, S7–24. (doi:10.1016/j.ijgo.2004.11.012)
41. Wendt A, Gibbs CM, Peters S, Hogue CJ. 2012 Impact of increasing inter-pregnancy interval on maternal and infant health. Paediatr. Perinat.
Epidemiol. 26, 239–258. (doi:10.1111/j.1365-3016.2012.01285.x)
42. Zipple MN, Archie EA, Tung J, Altmann J, Alberts SC. 2019 Intergenerational effects of early adversity on survival in wild baboons. eLife (eds M
Przeworski, E Franco, Z Thayer, D Blumstein, C Kuzawa), 8, e47433. (doi:10.7554/eLife.47433)
43. Maestripieri D. 2002 Parent–offspring conflict in primates. Int. J. Primatol. 23, 923–951. (doi:10.1023/A:1015537201184)
44. Devinney BJ, Berman CM, Rasmussen KLR. 2001 Changes in yearling rhesus monkeys’ relationships with their mothers after sibling birth. Am. J.
Primatol. 54, 193–210. (doi:10.1002/ajp.1030)
45. DiGregorio G, Suomi SJ, Eisele CE, Chapman SA. 1987 Reactions of nuclear-family–reared rhesus macaques to the births of younger siblings. Am.
J. Primatol. 13, 231–253. (doi:10.1002/ajp.1350130303)
16
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rsos R. Soc. Open Sci. 11: 240597
46. Holman SD, Goy RW. 1988 Sexually dimorphic transitions revealed in the relationships of yearling rhesus monkeys following the birth of
siblings. Int. J. Primatol. 9, 113–133. (doi:10.1007/BF02735732)
47. Schino G, Troisi A. 2001 Relationship with the mother modulates the response of yearling Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata) to the birth of a
sibling. J. Comp. Psychol. 115, 392–396.
48. Bolwig N. 1980 Early social development and emancipation of Macaca nemestrina and species of Papio. Primates 21, 357–375. (doi:10.1007/
BF02390466)
49. Barrett L, Henzi SP. 2000 Are baboon infants Sir Phillip Sydney’s offspring Ethology 106, 645–658. (doi:10.1046/j.1439-0310.2000.00577.x)
50. Castles DL, Whiten A, Aureli F. 1999 Social anxiety, relationships and self-directed behaviour among wild female olive baboons. Anim. Behav.
58, 1207–1215. (doi:10.1006/anbe.1999.1250)
51. Maestripieri D, Schino G, Aureli F, Troisi A. 1992 A modest proposal: displacement activities as an indicator of emotions in primates. Anim.
Behav. 44, 967–979. (doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80592-5)
52. Palagi E, Norscia I. 2011 Scratching around stress: hierarchy and reconciliation make the difference in wild brown lemurs (Eulemur fulvus). Stress
14, 93–97. (doi:10.3109/10253890.2010.505272)
53. Delaunay A, Baniel A, Dezeure J, Carter AJ, Cowlishaw G, Charpentier MJE, Huchard E. 2023 Transition to siblinghood in a wild chacma baboon
population. Anim. Behav. 199, 123–139. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2023.02.011)
54. Behringer V, Berghänel A, Deschner T, Lee SM, Fruth B, Hohmann G. 2022 Transition to siblinghood causes a substantial and long-lasting
increase in urinary cortisol levels in wild bonobos. eLife (ed. S Rosenbaum), 11, e77227. (doi:10.7554/eLife.77227)
55. Dunn J, Kendrick C, MacNamee R. 1981 The reaction of first-born children to the birth of a sibling: mothers’ reports. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatr.
22, 1–18. (doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1981.tb00527.x)
56. Dunn J, Kendrick C. 1980 The arrival of a sibling: changes in patterns of interaction between mother and first-born child. J. Child Psychol.
Psychiatr. 21, 119–132. (doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1980.tb00024.x)
57. Stewart RB, Mobley LA, Van Tuyl SS, Salvador MA. 1987 The firstborn’s adjustment to the birth of a sibling: a longitudinal assessment. Child Dev.
58, 341–355. (doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1987.tb01382.x)
58. Volling BL. 2012 Family transitions following the birth of a sibling: an empirical review of changes in the firstborn’s adjustment. Psychol. Bull.
138, 497–528. (doi:10.1037/a0026921)
59. Kendrick C, Dunn J. 1980 Caring for a second baby: effects on interaction between mother and firstborn. Dev. Psychol. 16, 303–311. (doi:10.
1037/0012-1649.16.4.303)
60. Brody GH. 1998 Sibling relationship quality: its causes and consequences. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 49, 1–24. (doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.49.1.1)
61. Pike A, Coldwell J, Dunn JF. 2005 Sibling relationships in early/middle childhood: links with individual adjustment. J. Fam. Psychol. 19, 523–
532. (doi:10.1037/0893-3200.19.4.523)
62. Devinney, Berman, Rasmussen. 2003 Individual differences in response to sibling birth among free-ranging yearling rhesus monkeys (Macaca
mulatta) on Cayo Santiago. Behaviour 140, 899–924. (doi:10.1163/156853903770238373)
63. Dezeure J, Charpentier MJE, Huchard E. 2022 Fitness effects of seasonal birth timing in a long-lived social primate living in the equatorial forest.
Anim. Behav. 185, 113–126. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2022.01.002)
64. Hamilton WD. 1964 The genetical evolution of social behaviour. II. J. Theor. Biol. 7, 1–16. (doi:10.1016/0022-5193(64)90038-4)
65. Grebe N, Hirwa JP, Stoinski TS, Vigilant L, Rosenbaum S. 2022 Mountain gorillas maintain strong maternal affiliative biases despite high male
reproductive skew and extensive exposure to paternal kin. eLife (ed. AK Kalan), 11, e80820. (doi:10.7554/eLife.80820)
66. Charpentier M, Peignot P, Hossaert-McKey M, Gimenez O, Setchell JM, Wickings EJ. 2005 Constraints on control: factors influencing reproductive
success in male mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx). Behav. Ecol. 16, 614–623. (doi:10.1093/beheco/ari034)
67. Abernethy KA, White LJT, Wickings EJ. 2002 Hordes of mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx): extreme group size and seasonal male presence. J. Zool.
258, 131–137. (doi:10.1017/S0952836902001267)
68. Charpentier MJE, Peignot P , Hossaert-McKey M, Wickings EJ. 2007 Kin discrimination in juvenile mandrills, Mandrillus sphinx. Anim. Behav. 73,
37–45. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.02.026)
69. Setchell JM, Lee PC, Wickings EJ, Dixson AF. 2002 Reproductive parameters and maternal investment in mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx). Int. J.
Primatol. 23, 51–68. (doi:10.1023/A:1013245707228)
70. Setchell JM, Wickings EJ. 2004 Social and seasonal influences on the reproductive cycle in female mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx). Am. J. Phys.
Anthropol. 125, 73–84. (doi:10.1002/ajpa.10375)
71. Amici F, Kulik L, Langos D, Widdig A. 2019 Growing into adulthood—a review on sex differences in the development of sociality across
macaques. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 73, 18. (doi:10.1007/s00265-018-2623-2)
72. Lange EC, Griffin M, Fogel AS, Archie EA, Tung J, Alberts SC. 2023 Environmental, sex-specific and genetic determinants of infant social
behaviour in a wild primate. Proc. R. Soc. B 290, 20231597. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2023.1597)
73. Roatti V, Cowlishaw G, Huchard E, Carter A. 2023 Social network inheritance and differentiation in wild baboons. R. Soc. Open Sci. 10, 230219.
(doi:10.1098/rsos.230219)
74. Peignot P, Charpentier MJE, Bout N, Bourr y O, Massima U, Dosimont O, Terramorsi R, Wickings EJ. 2008 Learning from the first release project of
captive-bred mandrills Mandrillus sphinx in Gabon. Oryx 42, 122–131. (doi:10.1017/S0030605308000136)
75. Altmann J. 1974 Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. Behaviour 49, 227–267. (doi:10.1163/156853974x00534)
76. Charpentier MJE, Harté M, Poirotte C, de Bellefon JM, Laubi B, Kappeler PM, Renoult JP. 2020 Same father, same face: deep learning reveals
selection for signaling kinship in a wild primate. Sci. Adv. 6, eaba3274. (doi:10.1126/sciadv.aba3274)
17
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rsos R. Soc. Open Sci. 11: 240597
77. Brooks ME, Kristensen K, Benthem K van, Magnusson A, Berg CW, Nielsen A, Skaug HJ, Mächler M, Bolker B. 2017 glmmTMB balances speed and
flexibility among packages for zero-inflated generalized linear mixed modeling. R. J. 9, 378. (doi:10.32614/RJ-2017-066)
78. Dorie V. 2021 blme: Bayesian linear mixed-effects models. See https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=blme (accessed 25 March 2021).
79. Lüdecke D, Ben-Shachar M, Patil I, Waggoner P, Makowski D. 2021 performance: an R package for assessment, comparison and testing of
statistical models. J. Open Source Softw. 6, 3139. (doi:10.21105/joss.03139)
80. Zuur AF, Ieno EN, Walker NJ, Saveliev AA, Smith GM. 2009 Mixed effects models and extensions in ecology with R. vol. 574. New York, NY:
Springer.
81. Fox J, Weisberg S. 2019 An R companion to applied regression, 3rd edn. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. See https://socialsciences.mcmaster.ca/jfox/
Books/Companion/.
82. Harrison XA, Donaldson L, Correa-Cano ME, Evans J, Fisher DN, Goodwin CED, Robinson BS, Hodgson DJ, Inger R. 2018 A brief introduction to
mixed effects modelling and multi-model inference in ecology. PeerJ 6, e4794. (doi:10.7717/peerj.4794)
83. Hartig F, Lohse L. DHARMa: residual diagnostics for hierarchical (multi-level / mixed) regression models [Internet]. See https://CRAN.R-project.org/
package=DHARMa (accessed 25 March 2021).
84. van Noordwijk MA, van Schaik CP. 2005 Development of ecological competence in Sumatran orangutans. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 127, 79–94.
(doi:10.1002/ajpa.10426)
85. Volling BL , Yu T, Gonzalez R , Kennedy DE, Rosenberg L, Oh W. 2014 Children’s responses to mother–infant and father–infant interaction with a
baby sibling: jealousy or joy J. Fam. Psychol. 28, 634–644. (doi:10.1037/a0037811)
86. Dunayer ES, Berman CM. Infant handling among primates. Int. J. Comp. Psychol. 31. (doi:10.46867/ijcp.2018.31.02.06)
87. Hrdy SB. 1976 Care and exploitation of nonhuman primate infants by conspecifics other than the mother. In Advances in the study of behavior
(eds JS Rosenblatt, RA Hinde, E Shaw, C Beer), pp. 101–158. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. See http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/
pii/S0065345408600832.
88. Meredith SL. 2015 Anchoring the clade: primate-wide comparative analysis supports the relationship between juvenile interest in infants and
adult patterns of infant care. Folia Primatol. 86, 117–123. (doi:10.1159/000368356)
89. Caselli M, Zanoli A, Palagi E, Norscia I . 2021 Infant handling increases grooming towards mothers in wild geladas (Theropithecus gelada). Behav.
Processes 192, 104501. (doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2021.104501)
90. Frank RE, Silk JB. 2009 Grooming exchange between mothers and non-mothers: the price of natal attraction in wild baboons (Papio anubis).
Behaviour 146, 889–906. (doi:10.1163/156853908X396656)
91. Henzi SP, Barrett L. 2002 Infants as a commodity in a baboon market. Anim. Behav. 63, 915–921. (doi:10.1006/anbe.2001.1986)
92. Silk JB, Beehner JC, Bergman TJ, Crockford C, Engh AL, Moscovice LR, Wittig RM, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL. 2010 Strong and consistent social
bonds enhance the longevity of female baboons. Curr. Biol. 20, 1359–1361. (doi:10.1016/j.cub.2010.05.067)
93. Silk JB, Alberts SC, Altmann J. 2003 Social bonds of female baboons enhance infant survival. Science 302, 1231–1234. (doi:10.1126/science.
1088580)
94. Kulik L, Langos D, Widdig A. 2016 Mothers make a difference: mothers develop weaker bonds with immature sons than daughters. PLoS One 11,
e0154845. (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0154845)
95. Delaunay A etal. 2024 Data from: an early-life challenge: becoming an older sibling in wild mandrills. Dryad (doi:10.5061/dryad.7wm37pw25)
96. Delaunay A, Cossu-Doye, O, Roura Torres B, Sauvadet L, Ngoubangoye B, Huchard E. 2024 Data from: An early-life challenge: becoming an older
sibling in wild mandrills. Figshare. (doi:10.6084/m9.figshare.c.7315867)
18
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rsos R. Soc. Open Sci. 11: 240597
Article
Full-text available
Mammalian species with slow life histories invest heavily in offspring care to meet offspring nutritional and developmental requirements, typically at significant costs to mothers. While maternal care has been extensively studied, understanding the mechanisms driving variation in mother‒offspring relationships during key offspring developmental periods require more comparative data from natural populations. Using eight years of behavioral data, we analyzed mother-offspring interactions in 68 infants born to 46 mothers in wild mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx), spanning multiple birth cohorts and the entire first year of infant life, a critical period for individual social and physical development. We found that mothers dynamically adjusted caregiving behavior as infants aged, reducing physical contact while promoting spatial independence and social integration. Maternal traits, such as age, social rank, and reproductive history, shaped maternal phenotypes: high-ranking mothers promoted infant socialization while reducing carrying, and older, multiparous mothers invested more in grooming and physical contact than younger, inexperienced females. Previous infant loss predicted reduced maternal aggression, potentially due to fewer immatures to care for or behavioral adjustments aimed at improving offspring survival. Finally, mothers fostered closer bonds with their daughters while encouraging their sons’ independence, possibly resulting in more frequent tantrums observed in males. This sex-biased pattern likely reflects preparation for contrasting life histories between the sexes in this species. By combining extensive longitudinal observations with fine-scale, individual analyses, our study emphasizes the dynamic and multifaceted nature of early mother-offspring interactions and their evolutionary implications in long-lived mammals.
Article
Full-text available
Affiliative social bonds are linked to fitness components in many social mammals. However, despite their importance, little is known about how the tendency to form social bonds develops in young animals, or if the timing of development is heritable and thus can evolve. Using four decades of longitudinal observational data from a wild baboon population, we assessed the environmental determinants of an important social developmental milestone in baboons—the age at which a young animal first grooms a conspecific—and we assessed how the rates at which offspring groom their mothers develops during the juvenile period. We found that grooming development differs between the sexes: female infants groom at an earlier age and reach equal rates of grooming with their mother earlier than males. We also found that age at first grooming for both sexes is weakly heritable (h² = 0.043, 95% CI: 0.002–0.110). These results show that sex differences in grooming emerge at a young age; that strong, equitable social relationships between mothers and daughters begin very early in life; and that age at first grooming is heritable and therefore can be shaped by natural selection.
Article
Full-text available
Immatures' social development may be fundamental to understand important biological processes, such as social information transmission through groups, that can vary with age and sex. Our aim was to determine how social networks change with age and differ between sexes in wild immature baboons, group-living primates that readily learn socially. Our results show that immature baboons inherited their mothers' networks and differentiated from them as they aged, increasing their association with partners of similar age and the same sex. Males were less bonded to their matriline and became more peripheral with age compared to females. Our results may pave the way to further studies testing a new hypothetical framework: in female-philopatric societies, social information transmission may be constrained at the matrilineal level by age- and sex-driven social clustering.
Article
Full-text available
Evolutionary theories predict that sibling relationships will reflect a complex balance of cooperative and competitive dynamics. In most mammals, dispersal and death patterns mean that sibling relationships occur in a relatively narrow window during development, and/or only with same-sex individuals. Besides humans, one notable exception are mountain gorillas, in which non-sex biased dispersal, relatively stable group composition, and the long reproductive tenures of alpha males mean that animals routinely reside with both maternally and paternally related siblings, of the same and opposite sex, throughout their lives. Using nearly 40,000 hours of behavioral data collected over 14 years on 699 sibling and 1235 non-sibling pairs of wild mountain gorillas, we demonstrate that individuals have strong affiliative preferences for full and maternal siblings over paternal siblings or unrelated animals, consistent with an inability to discriminate paternal kin. Intriguingly, however, aggression data imply the opposite. Aggression rates were statistically indistinguishable among all types of dyads except one: in mixed-sex dyads, non-siblings engaged in substantially more aggression than siblings of any type. This pattern suggests mountain gorillas may be capable of distinguishing paternal kin, but nonetheless choose not to affiliate with them over non-kin. We observe a preference for maternal kin in a species with high reproductive skew (i.e., high relatedness certainty), even though low reproductive skew (i.e., low relatedness certainty) is believed to underlie such biases in other non-human primates. Our results call into question reasons for strong maternal kin biases when paternal kin are identifiable, familiar, and similarly likely to be long-term groupmates, and they may also suggest behavioral mismatches at play during a transitional period in mountain gorilla society.
Article
Full-text available
In animals with slow ontogeny and long-term maternal investment, immatures are likely to experience the birth of a younger sibling before reaching maturity. In these species, the birth of a sibling marks a major event in an offspring's early life, as the older siblings experience a decrease in maternal support. The transition to siblinghood (TTS) is often considered to be stressful for the older offspring, but physiological evidence is lacking. To explore the TTS in wild bonobos, we investigated physiological changes in urinary cortisol (stress response), neopterin (cell-mediated immunity), and total triiodothyronine (T3, metabolic rate), as well as changes in behaviors that reflect the mother-offspring relationship. Following a sibling's birth, urinary cortisol levels of the older offspring increased fivefold, independent of their age, and remained elevated for seven months. The cortisol level increase was associated with declining neopterin levels, however T3 levels and behavioral measures did not change. Our results indicate that the TTS is accompanied by elevated cortisol levels and that this change does not coincide with nutritional weaning and attainment of physical independence. Our results suggest that bonobos and humans experience TTS in similar ways and that this developmental event may have emerged in the last common ancestor.
Book
This book introduces life history evolution to postgraduate students just beginning their research in population biology, ecology, or evolutionary biology. It discusses major analytical tools, gives examples of their applications, and provides problems for discussion at the end of each chapter. It will interest all biologists wishing to understand the evolution of the life cycle and the causes of phenotypic variation in fitness, and it contains the seeds of applications of life history theory to population dynamics, behaviour, and community ecology. Care is taken in Part I to build up the tools needed for a well-rounded evolutionary explanation: demography, quantitative genetics, reaction norms, trade offs and phylogenetic/comparative analysis. Part II discusses the evolution of major life history traits. This is a comprehensive, up-to-date text in a field that holds a central position in modern ecology and evolution.
Book
One of the main tenets of evolutionary biology is that organisms behave so as to maximize the number of their genes that will be passed on to future generations. Parents often produce more offspring than they can rear in case special opportunities or calamities occur. This frequently leads to deprivations and even death of some offspring. This book is about the evolutionary diversity, importance, and consequences of such squeezes. In it the authors, top in their field, review the theory, field experiments, and natural history of sibling rivalry across a broad sweep of organisms, in a clear and accessible style that should appeal to both academics and natural historians.
Article
Parents often sacrifice their own future reproductive success to boost the survival of their offspring, a phenomenon referred to as parental investment. In several social mammals, mothers continue to improve the survival of their offspring well into adulthood;1,2,3,4,5 however, whether this extended care comes at a reproductive costs to mothers, and therefore represents maternal investment, is not well understood. We tested whether lifetime maternal care is a form of parental investment in fish-eating "resident" killer whales. Adult killer whales, particularly males, are known to receive survival benefits from their mothers;3 however, whether this comes at a cost to mothers' reproductive success is not known. Using multiple decades of complete census data from the "southern resident" population, we found a strong negative correlation between females' number of surviving weaned sons and their annual probability of producing a viable calf. This negative effect did not attenuate as sons grew older, and the cost of sons could not be explained by long-term costs of lactation or group composition effects, supporting the hypothesis that caring for adult sons is reproductively costly. This is the first direct evidence of lifetime maternal investment in an iteroparous animal, revealing a previously unknown life history strategy.
Article
Reproductive seasonality is the norm in mammals from temperate regions but less common at lower latitudes, where a broad diversity of reproductive phenology strategies is observed. Our knowledge of the evolutionary determinants shaping this diversity remains fragmentary and may reflect high phenotypic plasticity in individual strategies. Here we investigated the ecological determinants and fitness consequences of variation in birth timing across the annual cycle in a social primate endemic to the Congo basin, the mandrill, Mandrillus sphinx, which breeds seasonally. We further examined traits that modulate this variation within and across individuals. We used 9 years of ecological, life history and behavioural data from a natural population to characterize patterns of environmental and reproductive seasonality. We then investigated the consequences of variation in birth timing for pre- and postnatal offspring survival and maternal interbirth intervals. Finally, we studied the influence of within- (reproductive history and age) and between-individual (social rank) traits on variation in birth timing. We found that mandrills’ daily foraging time varied seasonally, with greater fluctuations for subordinate than dominant females. Birth timing was plastic, as females gave birth year round without detectable consequences for postnatal offspring survival. Giving birth within the birth peak, however, decreased interbirth intervals and probability of miscarriage. Finally, reproductive history and social rank mediated within- and between-individual variation in birth timing, respectively. Specifically, females that experienced a previous reproductive failure gave birth early in the next birthing season and dominant females bred less seasonally than subordinates, which may reflect their more even access to resources across the year. Overall, the selective pressures shaping mandrill reproductive seasonality differed from a classical scenario of seasonal fluctuations in resources limiting offspring survival. A complex interplay between social and ecological factors may thus determine within- and between-individual variation in phenology strategies of tropical and gregarious mammals.
Article
Infant handling - involving affiliative behavior from non-mothers to infants – is a phenomenon that is variably present in Old World monkeys and can be granted by mothers to obtain social services, such as grooming. Here we investigated for the first time whether infant handling could influence grooming exchange in wild geladas. We gathered data on the population of Kundi highland (Ethiopia) in 2019/2020. Via sampling on 15 focal mothers from eight different One-Male Units, we video-recorded 55 grooming sessions between focal mothers and non-focal females (mothers or non-mothers). We also recorded the possible occurrence of infant handling performed by non-focal females. We found that grooming sessions were longest between mother and non-mothers and in the presence than in the absence of infant handling. Hence, our results show that infant handling can influence the grooming exchange between wild gelada females. Because grooming is used to establish and reinforce social bonds in primates, infant handling may act as a ‘social bridge’ in a female bonded society. From an evolutionary perspective, infant handling strategies might represent the stepping stone to more complex forms of infant care, such as allomaternal care and cooperative breeding. Data Availability Statement The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.