Content uploaded by Matthieu Chidharom
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Matthieu Chidharom on Jul 16, 2024
Content may be subject to copyright.
RESEARCH ARTICLE
CORRESPONDING AUTHOR:
Matthieu Chidharom
Department of Psychology,
Lehigh University, 17 Memorial
Drive, Bethlehem, PA, USA
matthieuchidharom@gmail.
com
KEYWORDS:
Distractors; color-wheel;
attentional templates;
inter-individual differences;
fluctuations
TO CITE THIS ARTICLE:
Chidharom, M., Zafarmand, M.,
& Carlisle, N. B. (2024). Similar
Quality of Visual Working
Memory Representations
between Negative and Positive
Attentional Templates. Journal
of Cognition, 7(1): 55, pp. 1–13.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.5334/
joc.380
Similar Quality of
Visual Working Memory
Representations between
Negative and Positive
Attentional Templates
MATTHIEU CHIDHAROM**
MAHSA ZAFARMAND**
NANCY B. CARLISLE
ABSTRACT
Visual working memory (VWM) plays an important role during visual search, with some
theories suggesting an equivalence between working memory representations and
guidance from attentional templates. However, recent work has shown that participants
can also use ‘negative templates’, the foreknowledge of distractor-features stored in
VWM, to guide attention away from distractors during visual search. These negative
templates must also be represented in working memory, but the question remains
whether the quality of the working memory representations underlying negative and
positive templates are similar, in spite of their opposite impacts on attention. In this
study, participants (N = 33) engaged in a visual search task for a shape-defined target
after receiving a positive cue (target color), negative cue (distractor color) or neutral
cue (non-informative). In 20% of the trials, a color-wheel probe was presented instead
of a search array to measure the quality of the cue representation stored in VWM. Our
results revealed that participants were more likely to guess in response to neutral cues
than negative cues. Yet, the comparison between positive and negative cues showed
no significant differences. However, we found no difference in memory precision for
the three cue types. More interestingly, the more the VWM quality is boosted by the
negative cue, the greater the ability to guide attention away from distractors. Such a
pattern of results might map to recent evidence of between-individuals differences in
utilization of negative cues. These findings highlight the distinction between attentional
templates and simple maintenance in working memory.
*Author affiliations can be found in the back matter of this article
**Co-first authorship
2Chidharom et al.
Journal of Cognition
DOI: 10.5334/joc.380
1. INTRODUCTION
Many theories of attention suggest that the control of attention is dependent upon visual
working memory (Desimone and Duncan, 1995; Bundesen et al., 2005; Wolfe et al., 1989). For
example, when we are looking for tomato sauce at the grocery store, we are able to use our
knowledge of features of our target item (e.g., red) to generate internal positive templates that
guide our search toward likely target items. The Biased Competition Theory (Desimone & Duncan,
1995) makes the most direct linkage between working memory and attention by suggesting
that holding a representation in working memory is sufficient to guide attention to matching
items in the visual scene. According to this proposal, holding an item in working memory will
lead to continued activity in cells which are tuned toward the working memory representation.
When multiple stimuli are presented in a visual scene, objects that match working memory
receive a combination of activation driven from the working memory maintenance as well as
the incoming sensory activation. This increased activation creates a bias to attend memory-
matching items, and is proposed to be the neural instantiation of a positive target template. So,
according to Biased Competition, attentional templates are the working memory representations,
and working memory maintenance is inextricably linked to attentional template generated
attentional biases (Carlisle, 2019). This viewpoint has received much support in the literature
(Soto, et al., 2008; Olivers, et al., 2011), and it is still widely accepted that items in working
memory will automatically bias visual attention (Oberauer, 2019).
However, recent evidence has also shown that foreknowledge of distractor items, also called
negative templates, can allow us to guide attention away from distractor items to improve
visual search efficiency. The first study explicitly directed at assessing negative templates was
conducted by Arita and colleagues (2012; but see Carlisle & Woodman, 2011; Sawaki & Luck,
2010, and Woodman & Luck, 2007). In Arita’s study, participants searched for a shape-defined
target in a display containing two colors of Landolt Cs. Each search display was preceded by a
cue that could indicate the upcoming distractor color (negative cues), indicate the upcoming
target color (positive cues) or be non-informative (neutral cues). Note that the colors cued
changed on each trial, meaning that the colors would need to be maintained in working
memory to serve as templates. In this design, both negative and positive cues lead to faster
RTs compared to neutral cues, but with smaller benefits for negative compared to positive cues.
These benefits from negative templates have now been replicated multiple times (Carlisle &
Nitka, 2019; Conci et al., 2019; Reeder et al., 2017, 2018; Zhang et al., 2020; 2022) and are
derived from ignoring items that match working memory.
To reconcile these results with proposals of biased competition (Desimone & Duncan, 1995)
and viewpoints suggesting an automatic link between working memory and attention (Soto et
al., 2008; Olivers et al., 2011), some recent frameworks hypothesize that negative templates
will automatically guide attention towards distractors, then are rapidly suppressed in a reactive
manner, as proposed by the “search and destroy” hypothesis (Moher & Egeth, 2012). However,
other perspectives, like the “active suppression” hypothesis (Arita et al., 2012) consider these
findings on negative templates difficult to fully reconcile with biased competition theory, and
instead suggest negative templates can proactively guide attention away from distractor
features in a preventative manner (Geng & DiQuattro, 2009; Chidharom & Carlisle, 2023).
If negative templates in working memory lead to avoiding attention to memory-matching
items, while positive templates in working memory lead to attention toward these items, one
important outstanding question is how the working memory representations are similar or
different for positive and negative templates? This question is important for our understanding
of the mechanisms underlying attentional templates, but also in evaluating the theoretical
proposals suggesting automatic links between working memory and attention.
Previous work has utilized neurophysiological measures of working memory to examine the
relationship between attentional templates and working memory. One neurophysiological
index of the active maintenance of object representations in VWM is the contralateral delay
activity (CDA), an event-related potential observed on lateral occipital-temporal electrodes
(Vogel and Machizawa, 2004; Vogel et al., 2005). Carlisle and colleagues (2011) first used the
CDA to demonstrate that attentional templates are maintained in working memory when the
attentional template changes on each trial, providing physiological evidence in humans that
working memory is employed to maintain positive templates prior to visual search. Following
3Chidharom et al.
Journal of Cognition
DOI: 10.5334/joc.380
a similar logic, Rajsic and Woodman (2020) used the CDA to contrast the working memory
maintenance of positive and negative templates. They found both positive and negative cues
lead to similar amplitude CDA responses, suggesting that both positive and negative attentional
templates are stored in VWM. However, these results do not tell us about the quality of the
working memory representations of positive compared to negative templates.
In order to gain a more precise measure of the quality of the working memory representation,
one can use the color wheel method of testing working memory, as introduced by Zhang
and Luck in 2008. The color wheel task serves as a valuable tool for obtaining a detailed
assessment of WM capabilities. Within this task, participants are instructed to select the color
on the color wheel that closely corresponds to the given cue (see Figure 1). When participants
have forgotten the item in memory, they will report a random color on the wheel, leading to a
uniform pattern of guesses. When the participant maintains the item in memory, the reported
color should be close to the correct position on the wheel, although responses will likely fall in
a normal distribution centered on the correct color. By measuring the standard deviation of
this distribution of responses, we can determine the precision of the memory representation.
Small standard deviations indicate very precise working memory representations, while larger
standard deviations indicate less precise working memory representations.
Previous studies have used this method to examin the relationship between working memory
quality and the efficiency of visual search for positive templates. Rajsic and colleagues (2017)
had subjects report the color of a positive cue on a color wheel following a search array. Positive
templates were recalled with greater precision and were less likely to be forgotten compared to
a baseline condition, where the color in working memory was not to be used as a visual search
template. Even more interestingly, Rajsic and colleagues separated trials with “good” and “bad”
memory quality by using a median split on VWM performance. They revealed that search was
faster during “good” compared to “bad” VWM states, suggesting a critical role of VWM precision
in visual search efficiency. Although a similar pattern of results has been observed recently by
Dube and Al-Aidroos (2019) for positive templates, there are at present no studies examining
the working memory representations underlying negative (or distractor) templates.
One possibility is that both positive and negative templates must be maintained equally well
in working memory, in order to use this information to guide search. This would be in line with
recent evidence suggesting that working memory representations are distinct from attentional
templates (Yu, et al., 2022, Kerzel, 2019, Olivers and Eimer, 2011). Another possibility is that the
working memory representations of negative templates must be degraded, either by reducing
the likelihood that they are maintained or by reducing the precision of the memory, in order
to limit their impact on attention. This would be more compatible with Biased Competition
(Desimone & Duncan, 1995) and associated viewpoints (Soto, et al., 2008; Olivers, et al., 2011),
but still difficult to reconcile with the concept that what is maintained in working memory leads
to an automatic attentional bias towards items matching working memory.
Hollingworth (2022) highlights the inherent conflict between the concept of negative templates
(Carlisle, 2023) and the prevailing viewpoint that working memory leads to automatic
attentional biases towards memory matching items. It is therefore critical to understand more
about the working memory representations underlying negative templates and contrast them
with the representations of positive templates. The goal of this study is therefore to directly
contrast the working memory precision and likelihood of maintenance of positive and negative
templates. To do so, we engaged participants in a visual search task similar to that used by
Arita and colleagues (2012). In the majority of trials, participants engaged in a visual search
task. We randomly intermixed a memory probe on 20% of trials, where instead of a search
array participants were presented with a color wheel and asked to report their memory of
the color cue. If representations in working memory are automatically linked to attentional
biases, we would expect negative templates to show a lower precision or decreased likelihood
of maintenance compared to positive templates. However, if working memory representations
are fundamentally separated from attentional templates (Yu, et al., 2022; Carlisle, 2019), we
would expect to see a similar precision and likelihood of maintenance for positive and negative
templates. Finally, if a high-quality VWM representation is critical to generate an effective
attentional template, as suggested by the findings in positive cues from Rajsic and colleagues
(2017), we would expect to observe higher negative cue benefits when the VWM quality is
higher at the intra- and inter-individual levels.
4Chidharom et al.
Journal of Cognition
DOI: 10.5334/joc.380
2. METHODS
PARTICIPANTS
We analyzed a sample of 33 undergraduates from Lehigh University who gave informed
consent and participated in a search task for course credit (Mean Age = 19, SD = 0.84, 9
females). Procedures were approved by Lehigh University IRB. All participants reported normal
or corrected-to-normal vision and normal color perception.
STIMULI
Stimuli were presented on a gray background (90.0 cd/m2) at a viewing distance of ap-
proximately 60 cm. The search cue was a filled colored circle (1.3°) presented at the center
of the screen. Search items were outlined circles (1.3° in diameter with a 0.2° line thickness)
with a gap (0.5° long) that were presented 6.3° from fixation. The colors appearing for the cue
and during the search array were randomly selected on each trial from all colors used in the
continuous report color wheel created by Suchow et al., (2013; memtoolbox). Within a trial,
each color was at least 60° away from each other and no memory colors were repeated. The
target-color items and the distractor-color items were spatially intermixed.
PROCEDURE
The trials start with the presentation of a fixation cross on a gray background. After 500 ms,
a cue was presented for 300 ms and characterized by a filled circle color cue. The cue was
followed by a 500 ms fixation screen. In 80% of the trials, a 12- item visual search array of
Landolt-Cs was presented on an imaginary circle centered on the fixation cross. Two colors
were selected for the search array at random on each trial from all colors used in the continuous
report color wheel. The subjects had to detect a target characterized by a gap opening facing
the top or bottom of the Landolt-C (see Figure 1). They were instructed to press the up arrow
of the keyboard when the gap was at the top, and the down arrow when the gap was at
the bottom. The search array remained on the screen until 200 ms after response, or for a
maximum of 3,500 ms. In 20% of the trials, the color wheel was presented for participants
to report the memorized color of the cue. Reports were made by selecting from a continuous
report color wheel (Suchow et al., 2013; Lively et al., 2021; Zhang & Luck, 2009). The wheel was
randomly rotated in each trial. Participants had no time limits to report the memorized color.
This task was divided into 3 blocks of 90 trials (18 color wheel probes/block), separated by cue type.
In the positive cue block, the cue indicated the color the target would appear in the upcoming
block. Using this cue would mean participants would only need to search through the 6 items in
this color to find the search target. Similarly, the negative cue indicated that the cued color would
not be the target, allowing participants the possibility of ignoring the 6 distractor items appearing
in the negative cue color. Finally, the neutral cue block contained a cue that would not appear
in the upcoming array, providing no information to help complete the search task. The order
Figure 1 Design of the Visual
Search Task. In separate
blocks, neutral, negative, or
positive cues were presented
with randomized color
selection per trial. Search trials
constituted 80% of the task,
requiring participants to locate
gap-oriented up or down
Landolt-C targets. Additionally,
20% of trials were probe trials,
prompting reporting of the
preceding cue’s color on a
color-wheel.
5Chidharom et al.
Journal of Cognition
DOI: 10.5334/joc.380
of the blocks were randomized across participants, and participants received instructions about
the meaning of the cue and practice trials for the cue before beginning each experimental block
(Figure 1). A feedback regarding memory accuracy was given during the practice trials only.
DATA AND STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
One participant was excluded for bad accuracy (lower than 2.5 standard deviations from the
mean accuracy). On the search-trials, trials with RT less than 300 ms, trials with no response
during the search window, or with incorrect responses were excluded from the analysis.
The percentage of correct responses is the number of correct responses divided by the total
number of trials per condition, multiplied by 100. On the probe-trials, visual working memory
performance was accessed using response error that is the angular deviation between the
selected and the original cue color. Performance was further quantified by fitting a simple
mixture model to the distribution of response errors for each participant using MemToolbox
(Suchow et al., 2013). We modeled the distribution of response errors as the mixture of a
von Mises distribution centered on the correct value and a uniform distribution. We obtained
maximum-likelihood estimates for two parameters: the dispersion of the von Mises distribution
(SD), which reflects the quality of the participant’s memory; and the height of the uniform
distribution (g), which reflects the probability of guessing (indicators of the VWM quality are
illustrated Figure 2). ANOVAs were performed including the within-subject factors Cue (Neutral,
Positive and Negative). In the case of statistically significant interactions, paired t-tests were
conducted. The hypothesis, task design and statistical analysis plan were pre registered on
Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/qse8y).
3. RESULTS
3.1 SEARCH TRIALS
The ANOVA performed on mean RT revealed a main effect of Cue, F(2,32) = 120.226, p <
0.001, ηp
2 = 0.79. Post-hoc paired t-tests revealed faster RT for positive (1317 ms) compared
to negative (1586 ms), t(32) = 9.24, p < 0.001, and neutral cues (1846 ms), t(32) = 15.11, p <
0.001. Moreover, the RT for the negative cue was faster compared to the neutral cue, t(32) =
6.86, p < 0.001 (Figure 3).
Figure 2 Measures of the
Visual working memory
quality: An example for the
negative cue (blue) condition
regarding Figure 1. If working
memory is linked to an
automatic attentional guidance
towards memory-matching
items, we would anticipate
that measures of working
memory quality (Zhang & Luck,
2008) would show evidence of
lower quality representations
for negative templates than
positive templates. This would
appear as a larger standard
deviation and/or higher guess
rate for negative templates
than positive templates.
Figure 3 Performance on
search trials. Error bars
represent between-subject
standard error from the mean
RTs. ***p < 0.001.
6Chidharom et al.
Journal of Cognition
DOI: 10.5334/joc.380
The ANOVA performed on the correct responses revealed a main effect of Cue, F(2,32) = 35.671,
p < 0.001, ηp
2 = 0.535. Post-hoc comparisons revealed a higher rate of correct responses to
negative cues compared to neutral cues, t(32) = 4.81, p < 0.001. Moreover, participants had
more correct responses to positive cues compared to negative t(32) = 4.33, p < 0.001 and
neutral cues t(32) = 7.9, p < 0.001 (Figure 3).
3.2 PROBE TRIALS
Performance on the probe trials is shown in Figure 4
The ANOVA revealed a main effect of Cue on mean absolute error deviation, F(2, 31) = 11.23, p <
0.001, ηp
2 = 0.266. The post-hoc tests revealed that the mean absolute error deviation was lower
after both negative and positive cues compared to neutral cues, t(31) = 3.43, p = 0.002, and
t(31) = 3.53, p = 0.001, respectively. However, no difference was observed between negative
and positive cues, t(31) = 0.727, p = 0.473. In order to better understand these differences in
working memory, we next utilized the mixture model (Zhang & Luck, 2009) to separate the
distribution into precision of items maintained in memory and likelihood of maintaining an
item in memory (Figure 4B).
The ANOVA revealed no main effect of Cues on SD (p = 0.128) (Figure 4C).
The ANOVA revealed a main effect of Cue on the guess rate, F(2, 32) = 5.47, p = 0.006, ηp
2 = .015.
Participants’ guess rates were higher in neutral cues compared to negative, t(32) = 2.60,
p < .001. However, no significant differences in guess rate were observed between neutral
and positive cues t(32) = 1.2, p = .103, and between negative and positive cues (p = 0.222)
(Figure 4D).
3.3 EXPLORATORY ANALYSIS: PEARSON’S CORRELATIONS
Interindividual differences in VWM in attentional template benefits
In order to examine whether there is a relationship between working memory precision and
visual search efficiency, we next examined the relationship between standard deviation
Figure 4 Performance on
probe trials. (A) Distributions
of recall errors with the
fits of Zhang and Luck’s
(2008) standard mixture
model across the three cue
conditions. (B) Absolute
error deviation of reported
value. (C) Standard Deviation
estimates from the mixture
model. (D) Guess rate
estimates from the mixture
model. Error bars represent
between-subject standard
error from the mean.
***p < 0.001.
7Chidharom et al.
Journal of Cognition
DOI: 10.5334/joc.380
estimates from the mixture model and benefits from the negative and positive cues. The
benefits were computed by subtracting the reaction times (RTs) for neutral cues from the RTs
for informative cues (negative or positive cues). The result of the Pearson Correlation showed a
significant negative relationship between the Benefits of RT for negative cues and the variability
of the error deviation of negative cues, r = –0.42, p = 0.017 (Figure 5). No effect was observed
between the RT benefits and the variability of the error deviation for positive cues (r = –.098, p =
0.593). This indicates individual differences in working memory precision can predict attentional
efficiency.
State-based differences in VWM in attentional template benefits
Since it has been shown that VWM quality fluctuates over time between good and bad states
(deBettencourt et al., 2019), we wanted to explore if search performance was related to
the VWM states. To do so, we used the same methods as Rajsic and colleagues (2017) and
performed a median split on the absolute memory error deviation. This approach allows us to
explore the search performance on the search trial (N–1) preceding the color wheel probe (N)
during periods of good and bad VWM. To explore the effect of VWM states on RT, we performed
repeated measures ANOVA including the within-subject factors Cue (Neutral, Positive and
Negative) and VWM states (Good and Bad VWM). In the case of statistically significant
interactions, paired t-tests were conducted. Additionally, we conducted t-tests against zero
to assess the significant RT benefits associated with both negative and positive cues. This
approach was taken because ANOVA may not be suitable for revealing significant costs and/or
benefits on performance associated with the VWM states.
As shown previously, the ANOVA performed on RT revealed the main effect of Cue, F(2,62) =
37.59, p < 0.001, ηp
2 = 0.55. However, no main effect of VWM states (p = 0.568), nor Cue × VWM
States interaction (p = 0.670) was observed.
The ANOVA performed on the RT benefits revealed a main effect of Cue, F(1,31) = 45.54,
p < 0.001, ηp
2 = 0.60, with higher benefits for positive compared to negative cues. However,
the ANOVA did not reveal a main effect of VWM states (p = 0.553), nor Cue × VWM States
interaction (p = 0.515).
To verify the benefits induced by positive and negative cues on RT, planned comparisons against
zero were performed (Figure 6). After positive cues, benefits were observed during both good
VWM periods, t(31) = –6.55, p < 0.001, d = –1.16, and bad VWM periods, t(31) = –8.80, p < 0.001,
d = –1.56. However, after negative cues, benefits were observed during good VWM periods,
t(31) = –2.21, p = 0.034, d = –0.39, but not during bad VWM periods (p = 0.101). These findings
indicate that state-level differences in working memory precision influence the attentional
impact of negative templates.
Figure 5 Correlation between
memory precision and
benefits of negative and
positive cues. (A) Individuals
with higher VWM precision
(lower s.d.) showed larger
negative cue benefits. (B) No
relationship between VWM
precision and positive cue
benefits. *p < 0.05.
8Chidharom et al.
Journal of Cognition
DOI: 10.5334/joc.380
4. DISCUSSION
Theories of attention propose a strong connection between working memory and attentional
templates (Wolfe, 1989; Bundesen, 1990; Bundesen et al., 2005), with Biased Competition Theory
(Desimone & Duncan, 1995) and the related “search and destroy” hypothesis (Moher & Egeth,
2012) suggesting an automatic link between maintaining an item in working memory and an
attentional bias towards memory matching items (Soto, et al., 2008; Olivers, et al., 2011). Recent
work on negative templates, where attention is directed away from items matching working
memory, presents a serious challenge to this prevailing viewpoint (Hollingworth, 2022). The
goal of this study was to better characterize the quality of the working memory representations
underlying negative templates, and contrast these representations to those used to create
positive templates. To do so, we used color-wheel probe trials intermixed with the typical visual
search task.
Negative templates were not less likely to be maintained in working memory than the
positive templates, so any differences in attentional impact cannot be explained by a reduced
maintenance of negative templates vs. positive templates. We also found no significant difference
between the VWM precision of negative cues, positive cues, and neutral cues, suggesting that
differences in the quality of the working memory representations cannot explain the differential
attentional impact of the different template types. More interestingly, the more precise the
VWM for negative templates, the greater the ability to guide attention away from distractors.
Such a pattern of results was observed at the state-level, by contrasting periods of high and
low memory precision within the same individual, as well as at the between-subject level, by
contrasting individuals with higher and lower average working memory precision. These findings
reveal the critical importance of VWM quality in the use of negative templates and develop a
more accurate picture of the mechanisms underlying the use of negative templates.
RETHINKING THE THEORIES AND HYPOTHESES OF VISUAL SEARCH
These results also help to refine theories on visual search and do not support the idea of an
automatic link between maintenance in working memory and attentional biases, as proposed
by the biased competition theory (Desimone & Duncan, 1995). Indeed, according to the
Biased Competition (Desimone & Duncan, 1995) and associated viewpoints, working memory
representations of negative templates might have been degraded, either by reducing the
likelihood that they are maintained or by reducing the precision of the memory, in order to limit
their impact on attention. However, the present study reveals a quality of the working memory
representation similar between negative and positive templates, and that negative templates
are actively encoded in working memory, and not degraded. This work supports an additional
step in creating an attentional template beyond just maintenance in working memory (Carlisle
& Woodman, 2011; Carlisle, 2019; Kerzel, 2019; Olivers and Eimer, 2011; Yu, et al., 2022). While
high quality memory representations for negative cues would fit with the search and destroy
model (Moher & Egeth, 2012), which suggests attention is first directed towards negatively
cued items before they are suppressed, multiple studies examining the search and destroy
hypothesis have failed to find evidence for this early attention towards negatively cued items
Figure 6 t-tests compared
to a zero baseline of the RT
benefits of negative and
positive cues during periods
of good and bad visual
working memory. No benefits
of negative cues during
periods of lower VWM quality.
Error bars represent between-
subject standard errors from
the mean RT benefits. *p <
0.05, ***p < 0.001.
9Chidharom et al.
Journal of Cognition
DOI: 10.5334/joc.380
in terms of fMRI (Reeder, et al., 2018), EEGs (Carlisle & Nitka, 2019), attentional probes (Zhang,
et al., 2020), and eye tracking (Zhang, et al. 2022; Beck, et al., 2018). Therefore, our results are
much more in line with the “active suppression” hypothesis, suggesting that the suppression of
distractors occurs through a top-down engagement of attentional control actively guided by a
qualitative representation of the negative template (Carlisle, 2023).
NEGATIVE TEMPLATE REPRESENTATIONS ARE LESS LIKELY TO BE FORGOTTEN
The analysis of memory parameter estimates revealed that participants were less likely to forget
negative templates compared to neutral cues. Indeed, lower guess rates were observed after
negative cues compared to positive and neutral cues. This could suggest that negative templates
required greater activation and maintenance in working memory in order to be implemented.
This interpretation aligns with the additional cognitive effort required to implement these
negative templates (Rajsic, et al., 2020), as evidenced by the increase in fronto-medial theta
oscillatory activity observed in the EEG data (de Vries et al., 2019; Chidharom & Carlisle, 2023).
However, regarding the memory precision (SD), our mixture model did not reveal any boost in the
precision of the visual working memory for negative or positive cues compared to neutral cues.
Previous work from Rajsic and colleagues (2017) reported higher VWM precision after positive
cues than their baseline task, leading us to expect a similar pattern. One possible explanation
is the baseline (i.e., neutral) condition used in their task. Indeed, in the tasks used by Rajsic and
colleagues (2017), the participants always had to maintain two items in working memory, only
one of which served as an attentional template. In contrast, in our design the neutral (baseline)
condition only a single item was presented. These differences in task demands across the two
studies may tie into known differences in working memory based on the number of items
maintained. According to one perspective, the flexible resource theory (Bays & Husain, 2008;
Palmer, 1990; Wilken & Ma, 2004), allocating resources to several items will lower the quality
of the VWM representations. Similarly, research on attentional templates suggests placing
an item in a ‘prioritized’ state which can guide attention (Olivers, et al., 2011), and leads to
stronger working memory representations for that item compared to items maintained in an
accessory state which does not impact attention (Dube, et al., 2017; Dube & Al-Aidroos, 2019).
We can thus speculate that the baseline used by Rajsic and colleagues (2017), in which two
items have to be encoded, is more likely to reduce the visual working memory precision than
our single-item neutral condition baseline. These increased working memory demands in Rajsic
and colleagues’ work may have also led to a significant relationship between working memory
and search performance, which we did not observe in our study.
THE VISUAL WORKING MEMORY QUALITY IS ASSOCIATED WITH BETTER USE
OF NEGATIVE TEMPLATES
Our exploratory analysis revealed that individuals with higher VWM precision are more likely
to benefit from negative templates, as revealed by the significant correlation between the
memory SD and the RT benefits after negative cues. This is in line with a recent study, based on
a large sample size of 54 participants, in which we revealed that individuals with higher VWM
ability (measured through the k-score during a change localization task; Zhao et al. 2022) are
specifically faster at using negative cues (Chidharom & Carlisle, 2024a). These results could
suggest that interindividual differences in the quality of the VWM exist and play a critical role in
the ability to guide attention away from cued-distractors.
While inter-individual differences in VWM seem associated with better suppression of distractors,
a similar pattern of results has been observed within-individuals, at the state-level. Indeed,
by separating periods of “good” and “bad” states of memory (Rajsic et al. 2019), our analysis
revealed that participants benefit from negative templates during periods of “good” visual
working memory but not during periods of “bad” visual working memory. This could suggest
that fluctuations in VWM quality (deBettencourt et al., 2019) impair the ability to use negative
templates. Interestingly, such fluctuations are not associated with positive template use since
benefits of positive cues have been observed during both “good” and “bad” states of memory.
Taken together, those results based on interindividual differences and state-based differences
highlight the critical role of the VWM quality to efficiently guide attention away from irrelevant
items. Indeed, it has been previously shown that negative templates rely on higher proactive
10Chidharom et al.
Journal of Cognition
DOI: 10.5334/joc.380
control mechanisms. For example, higher fronto-parietal theta power has been observed after
negative cues compared to both positive and neutral cues (de Vries et al., 2019; Chidharom &
Carlisle, 2023). We also recently showed that individuals with higher proactive control efficiency
are better at using negative templates (Chidharom & Carlisle, 2024b).1 Taken together, this work
suggests a coherent tripartite system to optimally suppress distractors: (1) negative templates
– (2) visual working memory – and (3) proactive control. The current results are thus in line
with previous research showing the critical role of working memory in efficient engagement
of proactive control (Redick, 2014; Gonthier et al. 2019; Lin et al. 2022; Wiemers & Redick,
2018). To go further, we can speculate that negative templates relate to the quality of VWM
representation allowing a higher efficiency in individuals’ ability to proactively avoid distractor-
features and guide attention away from irrelevant items.
Most importantly, our findings draw a clear separation between working memory and attentional
guidance, providing a stark contrast to a predominant view in the literature that working
memory leads to an automatic attentional bias towards memory-matching items (Desimone
& Duncan, 1995; Soto, et al., 2008; Olivers, 2011; Oberauer, 2019). Our study shows that
similar quality working memory representations can either lead to attentional enhancement or
attentional avoidance. These provide further support for the idea that attentional templates are
distinct from working memory representations, an idea that has been previously suggested in
the literature (Carlisle, 2019; Hollingworth, 2022; Dube, et al., 2016; Carlisle & Woodman, 2011).
5. LIMITATIONS
Several limitations can be drawn in the current study. First, the exploratory results on the state-based
difference in visual search efficiency need to be replicated in future studies. While our ANOVA did not
reveal any significant interaction between cues RT benefits and VWM states, our follow-up t-tests
against zeros revealed preserved benefits for negative cues only during periods of good VWM. While
we contend the against-baseline tests provide complementary information about the presence or
absence of benefits within a condition, the lack of an interaction effect indicates limited differences
between states when considering our experimental manipulations. A second limitation is related
to the reliability of our estimates. The estimation performed by the mixed model was based on a
limited number of trials (18 trials per cue conditions), and a replication with a larger number of trials
is necessary to confirm these results and to enhance the robustness and reliability of findings.
6. CONCLUSIONS
In conclusion, our study revealed the clear role of the VWM quality in using and implementing
negative attentional templates efficiently. However, there was no evidence that the working
memory representations of negative templates were reduced in precision or likelihood of
maintenance compared to positive templates. Future studies should better characterize the
neural mechanisms underlying the quality of the VWM and how it interacts with top-down
processes, especially proactive control mechanisms, to optimally avoid distractor items.
DATA ACCESSIBILITY STATEMENT
The datasets are available on Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/mr85k/?view_only=None).
ETHICS AND CONSENT
This study was approved by Lehigh’s IRB (1544739-5). Participants provided witten informed
consent. This study was perfromed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinky.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
This paper was supported by a grant from the National Eye Institute of the National Institutes
of Health: R15EY030247 to Nancy B. Carlisle.
1 The correlation between SD-RT and the benefits of negative cues and SD-RT and the benefits of positive cues
differed significantly (z = −2.60, p < .01).
11Chidharom et al.
Journal of Cognition
DOI: 10.5334/joc.380
COMPETING INTERESTS
The authors have no competing interests to declare.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
Matthieu Chidharom and Mahsa Zafarmand share Co-first authorship.
AUTHOR AFFILIATIONS
Matthieu Chidharom orcid.org/0000-0003-4611-3126
Department of Psychology, Lehigh University, Bethlehem, Pennsylvania, 18015, USA
Mahsa Zafarmand orcid.org/0000-0003-4653-8108
Department of Psychology, Lehigh University, Bethlehem, Pennsylvania, 18015, USA
Nancy B. Carlisle orcid.org/0000-0002-9161-8556
Department of Psychology, Lehigh University, Bethlehem, Pennsylvania, 18015, USA
REFERENCES
Arita, J. T., Carlisle, N. B., & Woodman, G. F. (2012). Templates for rejection: Configuring attention
to ignore task-irrelevant features. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and
Performance, 38(3), 580–584. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027885
Bays, P. M., & Husain, M. (2008). Dynamic Shifts of Limited Working Memory Resources in Human Vision.
Science, 321(5890), 851–854. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1158023
Beck, V. M., Luck, S. J., & Hollingworth, A. (2018). Whatever you do, don’t look at the …: Evaluating
guidance by an exclusionary attentional template. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human
Perception and Performance, 44(4), 645–662. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000485
Bundesen, C. (1990). A theory of visual attention. Psychological review, 97(4), 523. DOI: https://doi.
org/10.1037/0033-295X.97.4.523
Bundesen, C., Habekost, T., & Kyllingsbæk, S. (2005). A Neural Theory of Visual Attention: Bridging
Cognition and Neurophysiology. Psychological Review, 112(2), 291–328. DOI: https://doi.
org/10.1037/0033-295X.112.2.291
Carlisle, N. B. (2019). Focus: Attention science: Flexibility in attentional control: Multiple sources and
suppression. The Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine, 92(1), 103–113.
Carlisle, N. B. (2023). Negative and positive templates: Two forms of cued attentional control. Attention,
Perception, & Psychophysics, 85(3), 585–595. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-022-02590-4
Carlisle, N. B., Arita, J. T., Pardo, D., & Woodman, G. F. (2011). Attentional Templates in Visual
Working Memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(25), 9315–9322. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/
JNEUROSCI.1097-11.2011
Carlisle, N. B., & Nitka, A. W. (2019). Location-based explanations do not account for active attentional
suppression. Visual Cognition, 27(3–4), 305–316. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/13506285.2018.1553222
Carlisle, N. B., & Woodman, G. F. (2011). When memory is not enough: Electrophysiological evidence
for goal-dependent use of working memory representations in guiding visual attention. Journal of
Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(10), 2650–2664. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2011.21602
Chidharom, M., & Carlisle, N. B. (2023). Neurophysiological Measures of Proactive and Reactive Control
in Negative Template Use. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1–12. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1162/
jocn_a_01996
Chidharom, M., & Carlisle, N. B. (2024a). Distinct Mechanisms of Attentional Suppression: Exploration
of Trait Factors Underlying Cued- and Learned-Suppression. Cognitive Research: Principles and
Implications, 9(26). DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s41235-024-00554-w
Chidharom, M., & Carlisle, N. B. (2024b). Why Are Some Individuals Better at Using Negative Attentional
Templates to Suppress Distractors? Exploration of Interindividual Differences in Cognitive Control
Efficiency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, in press. DOI:
https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0001214
Conci, M., Deichsel, C., Müller, H. J., & Töllner, T. (2019). Feature guidance by negative attentional
templates depends on search difficulty. Visual Cognition, 27(3–4), 317–326. DOI: https://doi.org/10.10
80/13506285.2019.1581316
deBettencourt, M. T., Keene, P. A., Awh, E., & Vogel, E. K. (2019). Real-time triggering reveals concurrent
lapses of attention and working memory. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(8), 808–816. DOI: https://doi.
org/10.1038/s41562-019-0606-6
Desimone, R., & Duncan, J. (1995). Neural Mechanisms of Selective Visual Attention. DOI: https://doi.
org/10.1146/annurev.ne.18.030195.001205
12Chidharom et al.
Journal of Cognition
DOI: 10.5334/joc.380
de Vries, I. E., Savran, E., van Driel, J., & Olivers, C. N. (2019). Oscillatory mechanisms of preparing for
visual distraction. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 31(12), 1873–1894. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1162/
jocn_a_01460
Dube, B., & Al-Aidroos, N. (2019). Distinct prioritization of visual working memory representations
for search and for recall. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 81, 1253–1261. DOI: https://doi.
org/10.3758/s13414-018-01664-6
Dube, B., Basciano, A., Emrich, S. M., & Al-Aidroos, N. (2016). Visual working memory simultaneously
guides facilitation and inhibition during visual search. Attention, Perception & psychophysics, 78,
1232–1244. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-016-1105-8
Dube, B., Emrich, S. M., & Al-Aidroos, N. (2017). More than a filter: Feature-based attention regulates
the distribution of visual working memory resources. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human
Perception and Performance, 43(10), 1843–1854. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000428
Geng, J., & DiQuattro, N. (2009). Attentional capture by a salient non-target improves target selection.
Journal of Vision, 9(8), 109-109. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1167/9.8.109
Gonthier, C., Zira, M., Colé, P., & Blaye, A. (2019). Evidencing the developmental shift from reactive to
proactive control in early childhood and its relationship to working memory. Journal of experimental
child psychology, 177, 1–16. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2018.07.001
Hollingworth, A. (2022). The architecture of interaction between visual working memory and visual
attention. In Visual memory (pp. 26–48). Routledge. DOI: https://doi.org/10.4324/9781003158134-3
Kerzel, D. (2019). The precision of attentional selection is far worse than the precision of the underlying
memory representation. Cognition, 186, 20–31. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2019.02.001
Lin, Y., Brough, R. E., Tay, A., Jackson, J. J., & Braver, T. S. (2022). Working memory capacity preferentially
enhances implementation of proactive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
Memory, and Cognition. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001195
Lively, Z., Robinson, M. M., & Benjamin, A. S. (2021). Memory Fidelity Reveals Qualitative Changes in
Interactions Between Items in Visual Working Memory. Psychological Science, 32(9), 1426–1441. DOI:
https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797621997367
Moher, J., & Egeth, H. E. (2012). The ignoring paradox: Cueing distractor features leads first to selection,
then to inhibition of to-be-ignored items. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74, 1590–1605. DOI:
https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-012-0358-0
Oberauer, K. (2019). Working Memory and Attention – A Conceptual Analysis and Review. Journal of
Cognition, 2(1), 298–23. DOI: https://doi.org/10.5334/joc.58
Olivers, C. N. L. (2011). Long-term visual associations affect attentional guidance. Acta Psychologica,
137(2), 243–247. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.07.001
Olivers, C. N., & Eimer, M. (2011). On the difference between working memory and attentional set.
Neuropsychologia, 49(6), 1553–1558. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.11.033
Palmer, J. (1990). Attentional Limits on the Perception and Memory of Visual Information. DOI: https://doi.
org/10.1037//0096-1523.16.2.332
Rajsic, J., Burton, J. A., & Woodman, G. F. (2019). Contralateral delay activity tracks the storage of visually
presented letters and words. Psychophysiology, 56(1), e13282. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13282
Rajsic, J., Carlisle, N. B., & Woodman, G. F. (2020). What not to look for: Electrophysiological evidence
that searchers prefer positive templates. Neuropsychologia, 140, 107376. DOI: https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107376
Rajsic, J., Ouslis, N. E., Wilson, D. E., & Pratt, J. (2017). Looking sharp: Becoming a search template
boosts precision and stability in visual working memory. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 79(6),
1643–1651. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-017-1342-5
Rajsic, J., & Woodman, G. F. (2020). Do we remember templates better so that we can reject distractors
better? Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 82(1), 269–279. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-
019-01721-8
Redick, T. S. (2014). Cognitive control in context: Working memory capacity and proactive control. Acta
Psychologica, 145, 1–9. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.10.010
Reeder, R. R., Olivers, C. N. L., Hanke, M., & Pollmann, S. (2018). No evidence for enhanced distractor
template representation in early visual cortex. Cortex, 108, 279–282. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
cortex.2018.08.005
Reeder, R. R., Olivers, C. N. L., & Pollmann, S. (2017). Cortical evidence for negative search templates.
Visual Cognition, 25(1–3), 278–290. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/13506285.2017.1339755
Sawaki, R., & Luck, S. J. (2010). Capture versus suppression of attention by salient singletons:
Electrophysiological evidence for an automatic attend-to-me signal. Attention, Perception, &
Psychophysics, 72(6), 1455–1470. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/APP.72.6.1455
Soto, D., Hodsoll, J., Rotshtein, P., & Humphreys, G. W. (2008). Automatic guidance of attention from
working memory. Trends in cognitive sciences, 12(9), 342–348. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
tics.2008.05.007
Suchow, J. W., Brady, T. F., Fougnie, D., & Alvarez, G. A. (2013). Modeling visual working memory with the
MemToolbox. Journal of Vision, 13(10), 9–9. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1167/13.10.9
13Chidharom et al.
Journal of Cognition
DOI: 10.5334/joc.380
TO CITE THIS ARTICLE:
Chidharom, M., Zafarmand, M.,
& Carlisle, N. B. (2024). Similar
Quality of Visual Working
Memory Representations
between Negative and Positive
Attentional Templates.
Journal
of Cognition,
7(1): 55, pp. 1–13.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.5334/
joc.380
Submitted: 02 November 2023
Accepted: 22 June 2024
Published: 16 July 2024
COPYRIGHT:
© 2024 The Author(s). This
is an open-access article
distributed under the terms
of the Creative Commons
Attribution 4.0 International
License (CC-BY 4.0), which
permits unrestricted use,
distribution, and reproduction
in any medium, provided the
original author and source
are credited. See http://
creativecommons.org/
licenses/by/4.0/.
Journal of Cognition is a peer-
reviewed open access journal
published by Ubiquity Press.
Vogel, E. K., & Machizawa, M. G. (2004). Neural activity predicts individual differences in visual working
memory capacity. Nature, 428(6984), 748–751. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nature02447
Vogel, E. K., McCollough, A. W., & Machizawa, M. G. (2005). Neural measures reveal individual differences
in controlling access to working memory. Nature, 438(7067), 500–503. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/
nature04171
Wiemers, E. A., & Redick, T. S. (2018). Working memory capacity and intra-individual variability of
proactive control. Acta Psychologica, 182, 21–31. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2017.11.002
Wilken, P., & Ma, W. J. (2004). A detection theory account of change detection. Journal of Vision, 4(12),
11. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1167/4.12.11
Wolfe, J. M., Cave, K. R., & Franzel, S. L. (1989). Guided Search: An Alternative to the Feature Integration
Model for Visual Search. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037//0096-1523.15.3.419
Woodman, G. F., & Luck, S. J. (2007). Do the contents of visual working memory automatically influence
attentional selection during visual search? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and
Performance, 33(2), 363. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.33.2.363
Yu, X., Hanks, T. D., & Geng, J. J. (2022). Attentional guidance and match decisions rely on different
template information during visual search. Psychological science, 33(1), 105–120. DOI: https://doi.
org/10.1177/09567976211032225
Zhang, W., & Luck, S. J. (2008). Discrete fixed-resolution representations in visual working memory.
Nature, 453(7192), 233–235. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nature06860
Zhang, W., & Luck, S. J. (2009). Sudden death and gradual decay in visual working memory. Psychological
science, 20(4), 423–428. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02322.x
Zhang, Z., Gaspelin, N., & Carlisle, N. B. (2020). Probing early attention following negative and positive
templates. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 82(3), 1166–1175. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/
s13414-019-01864-8
Zhang, Z., Sahatdjian, R., & Carlisle, N. B. (2022). Benefits from negative templates in easy and difficult
search depend on rapid distractor rejection and enhanced guidance. Vision Research, 197, 108031.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2022.108031
Zhao, C., Vogel, E., & Awh, E. (2022). Change localization: A highly reliable and sensitive measure
of capacity in visual working memory. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics. DOI: https://doi.
org/10.3758/s13414-022-02586-0