ArticlePDF Available

Motivational "Spill-Over" During Weight Control: Increased Self-Determination and Exercise Intrinsic Motivation Predict Eating Self-Regulation

American Psychological Association
Health Psychology
Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Successful weight management relies on at least two health behaviors, eating and exercise. However, little is known about their interaction on a motivational and behavioral level. Based on the Hierarchical Model of Motivation the authors examined whether exercise-specific motivation can transfer to eating regulation during a lifestyle weight control program. The authors further investigated whether general, treatment-related, and exercise motivation underlie the relation between increased exercise and improved eating regulation. Overweight/obese women participated in a 1-year randomized controlled trial (N = 239). The intervention focused on promoting physical activity and internal motivation for exercise and weight loss, following Self-Determination Theory. The control group received general health education. General and exercise specific self-determination, eating self-regulation variables, and physical activity behavior. General self-determination and more autonomous exercise motivation predicted eating self-regulation over 12 months. Additionally, general and exercise self-determination fully mediated the relation between physical activity and eating self-regulation. Increased general self-determination and exercise motivation seem to facilitate improvements in eating self-regulation during weight control in women. These motivational mechanisms also underlie the relationship between improvements in exercise behavior and eating regulation.
Content may be subject to copyright.
APA PROOFS
Motivational “Spill-Over” During Weight Control:
Increased Self-Determination and Exercise Intrinsic
Motivation Predict Eating Self-Regulation
Jutta Mata, Marlene N. Silva, Paulo N. Vieira, Eliana V. Carrac¸a, Ana M. Andrade, Sı´lvia R. Coutinho,
Luis B. Sardinha, and Pedro J. Teixeira
Technical University of Lisbon
Objective: Successful weight management relies on at least two health behaviors, eating and exercise.
However, little is known about their interaction on a motivational and behavioral level. Based on the
Hierarchical Model of Motivation the authors examined whether exercise-specific motivation can
transfer to eating regulation during a lifestyle weight control program. The authors further investigated
whether general, treatment-related, and exercise motivation underlie the relation between increased
exercise and improved eating regulation. Design: Overweight/obese women participated in a 1-year
randomized controlled trial (N239). The intervention focused on promoting physical activity and
internal motivation for exercise and weight loss, following Self-Determination Theory. The control group
received general health education. Main Outcome Measures: General and exercise specific self-
determination, eating self-regulation variables, and physical activity behavior. Results: General self-
determination and more autonomous exercise motivation predicted eating self-regulation over 12 months.
Additionally, general and exercise self-determination fully mediated the relation between physical
activity and eating self-regulation. Conclusion: Increased general self-determination and exercise mo-
tivation seem to facilitate improvements in eating self-regulation during weight control in women. These
motivational mechanisms also underlie the relationship between improvements in exercise behavior and
eating regulation.
Keywords: Multibehavior change, autonomy, obesity, randomized controlled trial, physical activity
In contrast to many other health-enhancing treatments, weight
management programs almost always target changes in two dif-
ferent behaviors: eating and physical activity. These two behaviors
tend to cluster in cross-sectional studies (Pronk et al., 2004) and
may also display interactive effects in intervention studies (C. L.
Dunn et al., 2006; Jakicic, Wing, & Winters-Hart, 2002). Baker
and Brownell (2000) suggested that exercise may play a key role
in long-term weight management by influencing both physiologi-
cal processes such as energy metabolism and appetite (see also
Martins, Morgan, & Truby, 2008 for a review), as well as psycho-
logical aspects like self-efficacy, body image, or mood. Baker and
Brownell argued it was important that the latter mechanisms might
also result in stronger motivation and confidence, which would in
turn improve eating self-regulation leading to better dietary com-
pliance (as well as long-term exercise adherence). This model has
since been partially tested and supported (Annesi & Unruh, 2008).
Yet another pathway by which exercise might positively affect
the regulation of eating behavior is through its influence on vari-
ables such as motivation, commitment, and feelings of efficacy
(Baker & Brownell, 2000). These effects could involve both quan-
titative and qualitative dimensions. On the one hand, success in
adopting an exercise plan could increase confidence (self-
efficacy), internal locus of control, and the overall motivational
drive toward other behaviors involved in weight management,
such as restricting energy-dense foods, self-monitoring, and adopt-
ing stress management practices. At the same time, it is possible
that this motivational “spill-over effect” could also depend upon
the quality of the motivation involved, specifically whether the
exercise motivation is characterized by an internal locus of cau-
sality, more intrinsic motives to be active, and fueled by feelings
of autonomy and self-determination (high sense of volition), as
opposed to motivation being externally driven, such as to please
others, and subject to strong controlling influences (lower au-
tonomy and volition).
More autonomous and intrinsic motivation have been shown to
be powerful predictors of successful self-regulation in the domains
of exercise (Fortier, Sweet, O’Sullivan, & Williams, 2007), eating
(Pelletier & Dion, 2007; Pelletier, Dion, Slovinec-D’Angelo, &
Reid, 2004), weight loss, and weight loss maintenance (Teixeira et
al., 2006; Williams, Grow, Freedman, Ryan, & Deci, 1996). How-
Jutta Mata, Marlene N. Silva, Paulo N. Vieira, Eliana V. Carrac¸a, Ana
M. Andrade, Sı´lvia R. Coutinho, Luis B. Sardinha, and Pedro J. Teixeira,
Department of Exercise and Health, Faculty of Human Kinetics, Technical
University of Lisbon, Cruz Quebrada, Portugal.
This study was partially funded by the Portuguese Science and Tech-
nology Foundation grants POCI/DES/57705/2004 and SFRH/BPD/35953/
2007 (to Jutta Mata), and by the Calouste Gulbenkian Foundation grant
65565/2004. The authors thank Teresa Santos and Mariana Pessoa for their
help with this study.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Dr. Jutta
Mata, Department of Exercise and Health, Faculty of Human Kinetics,
Technical University of Lisbon, Estrada da Costa, 1495-688 Cruz Que-
brada, Portugal. E-mail: jmata@fmh.utl.pt
Health Psychology © 2009 American Psychological Association
2009, Vol. ●●, No. , 000– 000 0278-6133/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0016764
1
AQ: 1
tapraid5/zg1-hea/zg1-hea/zg100509/zg12352d09z
xppws S1 7/8/09 22:47 Art: 2009-1642
APA PROOFS
ever, it is unclear if affecting self-determination in one health
domain has repercussions on the motivational regulation for other
health behaviors. The hierarchical model of motivation (Vallerand,
1997; Vallerand & Ratelle, 2002) predicts that such a motivational
transference is possible. It suggests that motivation operates at
three hierarchically ordered levels, the situational, contextual, and
global level. Situational motivation relates to a specific scenario,
for example a run on a Saturday morning. Contextual motivation
refers to a specific life context or domain, such as physical activity.
Global motivation is the most general construct, akin to a person-
ality construct, such as whether a person’s motivation is generally
more internally or more extrinsically oriented. The three levels
dynamically influence each other through both top-down and
bottom-up processes. Top-down processing refers to the impact of
motivation of a higher level on a lower level. For example, if a
person is generally self-determined toward physical activity, she
will likely feel self-determined while engaging in a specific exer-
cise activity that is relevant to her. Bottom-up processes occur
when experiences on a lower level affect motivation at a higher
level; for example, repeated experiences of autonomy and strong
volition in specific exercise situations might affect contextual
motivation toward physical activities in general, which would
eventually contribute to a more self-determined motivation style
(Vallerand, 1997).
A number of observational studies have shown how different
levels of motivation affect each other and also how they affect
behavior. Both bottom-up and top-down processes in motivation
were shown in the domains of exercise between all three levels of
motivation (Blanchard, Mask, Vallerand, de la Sablionnie`re, &
Provencher, 2007). Pelletier and colleagues (Pelletier et al., 2004)
reported an association between global self-determined orientation
at baseline and eating-specific self-determination at follow-up 13
weeks later. Also, a positive relationship between a general level
of self-determination and autonomous regulation of eating behav-
ior was shown (Pelletier & Dion, 2007). In how far global self-
determination directly affects behavior was studied in a quasi-
experiment by Williams and colleagues (Williams et al., 1996):
They found that general and treatment autonomy orientation pre-
dicted both attendance to a weight loss program and actual weight
loss in a 6-month weight loss intervention, and also weight loss
maintenance and exercise behavior at 2-year follow up.
Another important aspect of motivation transfer between two or
more behaviors is the different contexts in which they might occur.
For example, eating likely occurs in a family or work-related
setting, whereas physical activity within a weight loss program
could occur in exercise classes, group activities, or individual
leisure time. Hagger and colleagues (Hagger, Chatzisarantis, Cul-
verhouse, & Biddle, 2003) suggest that motivation underlying one
behavior can transfer from one context to the next (“trans-
contextual model”). Specifically, they showed that perceived au-
tonomy support and intrinsic motivation in the context of physical
education classes affects leisure time physical activity locus of
causality and identified regulation. These findings have been rep-
licated cross-culturally, showing that perceived autonomy support
and autonomous exercise motives in physical education class at
school transfer at least partially to exercise motivation in leisure
time activities (Hagger, Chatzisarantis, Barkoukis, Wang, & Bara-
nowski, 2005). To our knowledge, the dynamic interplay between
the different contexts and hierarchical levels of motivation across
different behaviors has not been tested in an experimental weight
control trial.
Goals
We sought to investigate how general, treatment, and exercise-
specific self-determined motivation relate to markers of eating
self-regulation in the context of a weight management program.
Specifically, we hypothesized that i) general, treatment, and
exercise-specific self-determination and motivation transfer to,
that is, are associated with important markers of eating self-
regulation, and that ii) self-reported physical activity is associated
with eating self-regulation through its effects on (i.e., mediated by)
general self-determination, treatment motivation, as well as
exercise-specific motivation.
Method
Design
The study was a randomized controlled trial in overweight and
moderately obese women, primarily focused on increasing exer-
cise self-motivation and exercise adherence, aiming at long-term
weight control. The intervention group participated in weekly or
biweekly sessions for approximately 1 year. Intervention targets
included increasing physical activity and energy expenditure,
adopting a diet consistent with a moderate energy deficit, and
ultimately establishing exercise and eating patterns that would
support weight maintenance. The program’s principles and style of
intervention were based on Self-Determination Theory (Deci &
Ryan, 1985; Ryan & Deci, 2000) and focusing on increasing
efficacy and self-determination toward exercise and weight con-
trol, while supporting participants’ autonomous decisions as to
which changes they wanted to implement and how. The control
group received a general health education program. The interven-
tion and its theoretical rationale have been described in detail
elsewhere (Silva et al., 2008). The Faculty of Human Kinetics
Ethics Committee reviewed and approved the study.
Participants
Participants were recruited from the community at large through
media advertisements. By design, only premenopausal women
(N258) were accepted into the study. Of these, 19 women were
subsequently excluded from all analyses because they started
taking medication (e.g., antidepressants, anxiolytics, antiepilep-
tics) susceptible to affect weight (n10), had a serious chronic
disease diagnosis or severe illness/injury (n4), became pregnant
(n2) or entered menopause (n3). These 19 women were of
similar age ( p.58) and Body Mass Index (BMI; p.42) as the
239 participants considered as the valid initial sample.
Participants were between 23 and 50 years old (38 6.8 years)
and were overweight or mildly obese, with an initial BMI of
31.3 4.1 kg/m
2
. They were relatively well educated: 67% had at
least some college education, 23% had between 10 and 12 years of
school and 10% had 9 years or less of school education. Regarding
marital status, 32% of the sample was unmarried, 56% was mar-
ried, and 12% was divorced or widowed.
Women in the intervention group did not differ from those in the
control group in terms of BMI, age, education, or marital status.
2MATA ET AL.
tapraid5/zg1-hea/zg1-hea/zg100509/zg12352d09z
xppws S1 7/8/09 22:47 Art: 2009-1642
APA PROOFS
There were also no differences between the 208 women who
completed the 12-month intervention and the 31 who quit the
program for any demographic or baseline psychosocial variable,
with the exception of age; women who stayed in the program were
on average 4 years older ( p.01).
Measurements
Psychosocial Measures
General self-determination was assessed with the Self-
Determination Scale (Sheldon, Ryan, & Reis, 1996) which evalu-
ates individual differences for functioning in a self-determined
way. That is, being aware of one’s sense of self and feeling a sense
of choice toward one’s behaviors. For each of 10 statement pairs
such as “I always feel like I choose the things I do” and “I
sometimes feel that it’s not really me choosing the things I do”
participants evaluated each pair on a 5-point scale from “only A
feels true” to “only B feels true” (Cronbach’s alpha .63).
Reasons for staying in treatment (autonomous vs. controlled)
were measured using the Treatment Self-Regulation Questionnaire
(Williams, Freedman, & Deci, 1998; Williams et al., 1996) which
consists of 13 items and assesses the degree to which a person’s
motivation for participating in treatment is autonomous. On a
5-point scale, participants are asked to evaluate how well each
statement represents their reasons for staying in the program (e.g.,
“I would have felt bad about myself if I didn’t.”). The question-
naire consists of two subscales, autonomous (␣⫽.86) and con-
trolled (␣⫽.80) treatment self-regulation.
Exercise autonomous versus controlled self-regulation was
assessed with the Self-Regulation Questionnaire for Exercise
(adapted from Ryan & Connell, 1989). Items such as “I exer-
cise...because I simply enjoy working out” were evaluated on
a 7-point scale, ranging from not at all true to absolutely true.
The scale can be divided into two subscales, autonomous (Cron-
bach’s alpha .91) and controlled exercise self-regulation
(Cronbach’s alpha .72).
Exercise intrinsic motivation was measured with the Intrinsic
Motivation Inventory (Ryan, 1982; Ryan & Connell, 1989). The
questionnaire consists of 16 items measuring enjoyment, compe-
tence, involvement, and (absence of) pressure toward exercise, and
yielding an overall score of intrinsic motivation, used in this study.
It includes items such as “I think I’m good at being physically
active compared to other people,” evaluated on a 5-point scale
from not totally agree to totally disagree (␣⫽.94).
Eating behavior was measured with the Three-Factor Eating
Questionnaire (Stunkard & Messick, 1985). The 51-item scale is
divided into three subscales: cognitive restraint (␣⫽.82), cogni-
tive disinhibiton (␣⫽.68), and perception of hunger (␣⫽.78).
Statements include “On social occasions, like parties, I generally
eat too much,” that are evaluated on a 4-point scale from agree to
disagree or “If I ate too much on one day I try to make up for it on
the next day,” with answer format “true” or “false.” Because in this
study we were primarily interested in measuring markers of the
cognitive control of eating behavior, we did not use the scale
perception of hunger, which is highly influenced by physiological
states.
The Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire (Van Strien, Frijters,
Bergers, & Defares, 1986) was applied to assess external eating
(Cronbach’s alpha .88) and emotional eating (␣⫽.95). It
consists of 31 questions such as “Do you have a desire to eat when
you are irritated?” Answers are given on a 5-point scale from
“never” to “very frequently.”
Eating self-efficacy, the belief in one’s capacity for changing
eating behavior, was assessed with the Weight Management Effi-
cacy Questionnaire (Clark, Abrams, Niaura, Eaton, & Rossi,
1991). Statements include “I can resist food when I’m nervous,” to
be evaluated on a 10-point scale from not at all confident to very
confident. A global score including all items was used (␣⫽.95).
Exercise/Physical Activity
Minutes per week of leisure-time moderate and vigorous phys-
ical activities were estimated with the 7-Day Physical Activity
Recall interview (Blair et al., 1998; A. Dunn et al., 1999). Habitual
activities with a MET value above 3.0 and performed during the
last 7 days (or on a typical week of the past month) were quantified
to produce this variable.
Statistical Analyses
Treatment motivation was included in the analyses to test for the
effects of exercise motivation on eating self-regulation indepen-
dent of more autonomous versus controlled reasons to remain in
the program, thus making the test of our hypotheses more conser-
vative. Given that exercise was an integral part of the treatment in
this weight loss program, there might have been some overlap
between treatment regulations (more internal or more externally
controlled reasons to participate in the program) and exercise
regulations. The present analyses help distinguish these processes.
Twelve-month scores were used for all analyses. This choice
was based on the fact that not all psychosocial variables were
assessed at baseline. Most participants did not engage in regular
exercise at the beginning of the intervention, which yielded exer-
cise self-regulation measures less valid (e.g., “I exercise because
I” . . .). Also, treatment self-regulation (i.e., reasons to stay in
treatment) could only be assessed after the start of the intervention.
For consistency, we decided to also use physical activity measures
at 12 months, instead of change in physical activity. Because this
sample was mostly sedentary at baseline, the outcome (12-month)
measure was considered to represent well the result of the inter-
vention for this variable.
Stepwise hierarchical linear regressions with general self-
determination, treatment motivation, and exercise autonomous
motivation were used to test our first hypothesis. Because of
covariance in predictors (see Table 1), they were entered into the
model in a stepwise fashion; for models in which general self-
determination, treatment and exercise specific variables were en-
tered as predictors, general self-determination and treatment mea-
sures were entered first and exercise predictors in a second step.
This was done to determine the explanatory power of exercise
regulation variables above and beyond general self-determination
and treatment motivation.
Analyses were conducted for all participants (intervention and
controls) together. This was done to preserve statistical power and
increase variability in all measures under analysis, and also be-
cause the associations under scrutiny (self-determination as a pre-
dictor of eating self-regulation) were hypothesized to hold constant
3
EXERCISE SELF-DETERMINATION AND EATING SELF-REGULATION
AQ: 2
AQ: 3
T1, AQ:4
tapraid5/zg1-hea/zg1-hea/zg100509/zg12352d09z
xppws S1 7/8/09 22:47 Art: 2009-1642
APA PROOFS
regardless of group membership. Still, it is possible that the biva-
riate relationships between general self-determination, treatment
and exercise specific motivation, and eating variables were con-
founded by the intervention effect, which could have influenced
both. Thus, regression analyses were further adjusted by group
membership.
To examine whether general self-determination, treatment, and
exercise-specific motivation mediated the relationship between
physical activity and eating self-regulation, multiple mediation
with tests of indirect effects were conducted (Preacher & Hayes,
2008). This procedure tests the first two formal steps of mediation
(predictor to mediator, mediator to outcome) and then provides
total, direct (not mediated) and indirect effects of the predictor
(physical activity) on outcomes (eating variables). The latter ef-
fects are then tested for significance, providing a formal test of
indirect or mediated effects of the predictor on the outcomes.
Results
In the intervention group, 86% of participants attended more
than 75% of the intervention sessions. In the control group (for
whom attending health education sessions was not mandatory),
20% of participants attended more than 75% of the sessions. At 12
months (end of intervention), the intervention group had increased
weight loss (7.3% of initial body weight) and higher levels of
physical activity/exercise (M300 min/wk moderate plus vigor-
ous exercise; M9932 steps/day) than participants in the control
group (1.7% of initial body weight; M162 min/wk moderate
plus vigorous exercise; M7852 steps/day; all ps0.001).
Group differences in main intervention targets were medium to
large favoring the intervention group (all ps0.001), including
general self-determination (d0.40), and autonomous self-
regulation for treatment (d1.35) and exercise (d1.08). We
also found group differences in eating-related variables (all ps
.001); the intervention group had higher eating self-efficacy (d
0.64), higher cognitive restraint (d0.48), lower disinhibition
(d⫽⫺0.53), lower emotional eating (d⫽⫺0.28), and lower
external eating scores (d⫽⫺0.66). The effects of the intervention
trial are reported in detail elsewhere (Silva et al., 2009).
There were no baseline differences between intervention and
control group for all predictor and dependent variables used in this
study, except for exercise intrinsic motivation, t(205) ⫽⫺2.04,
p.04. However, effect size was small (d0.28) and there was
no baseline difference in autonomous exercise self-regulation;
therefore, this difference was not interpreted. Table 1 shows in-
tercorrelations among all variables in the study.
Table 2 shows the results for stepwise multiple regression models,
separately for three different models as predictors of eating self-
regulation: General and treatment self-determination (Model A),
exercise-specific self-determination (Model B), and general, treat-
ment, and exercise-specific self-determination (Model C). Measures
of general self-determination and treatment motivation consistently
predicted eating self-regulation variables with all relationships in the
expected direction: positive relationships between measures of auton-
omy and eating variables typically associated with successful weight
management (cognitive restraint and eating self-efficacy) and nega-
tive relationships for hindering eating variable (disinhibition, emo-
tional and external eating). With the exception of disinhibition and
restraint, eating measures were predicted by general self-
determination. Every eating measure was predicted by at least one
measure of treatment motivation (autonomous or controlled). Intrinsic
exercise motivation (or autonomous exercise self-regulation) also
predicted all eating variables; however, the percent variance ac-
counted for by exercise-specific measures was slightly lower than that
observed for general and treatment-related measures. Nevertheless,
the exercise-specific measures which entered the model (i.e., exercise
intrinsic motivation) generally predicted eating self-regulation even
after accounting for general self-determination and treatment motiva-
tion.
To test whether these relationships hold when adjusting for
group membership (i.e., controlling for the intervention effect),
Table 1
Correlation Matrix of the Variables in the Study
2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13
1. General self-determination .30
ⴱⴱ
.17
.24
ⴱⴱ
.11 .31
ⴱⴱ
.35
ⴱⴱ
.25
ⴱⴱ
.26
ⴱⴱ
.25
ⴱⴱ
.23
ⴱⴱ
.17
.20
ⴱⴱ
2. Autonomous treatment self-
regulation .10 .42
ⴱⴱ
.15
.59
ⴱⴱ
.37
ⴱⴱ
.35
ⴱⴱ
.37
ⴱⴱ
.29
ⴱⴱ
.18
.35
ⴱⴱ
.43
ⴱⴱ
3. Controlled treatment
self-regulation .04 .57
ⴱⴱ
.04 .17
.07 .15
.14
.19
ⴱⴱ
.11 .02
4. Intrinsic motivation inventory .07 .72
ⴱⴱ
.36
ⴱⴱ
.29
ⴱⴱ
.28
ⴱⴱ
.30
ⴱⴱ
.24
ⴱⴱ
.41
ⴱⴱ
.26
ⴱⴱ
5. Controlled exercise self-
regulation questionnaire .19
ⴱⴱ
.08 .11 .14
.08 .08 .03 .03
6. Autonomous exercise
self-regulation — .35
ⴱⴱ
.30
ⴱⴱ
.29
ⴱⴱ
.28
ⴱⴱ
.17
.36
ⴱⴱ
.32
ⴱⴱ
7. Eating self-efficacy .41
ⴱⴱ
.70
ⴱⴱ
.68
ⴱⴱ
.66
ⴱⴱ
.26
ⴱⴱ
.37
ⴱⴱ
8. TFEQ-restraint .25
ⴱⴱ
.43
ⴱⴱ
.19
ⴱⴱ
.30
ⴱⴱ
.36
ⴱⴱ
9. TFEQ-disinhibition .64
ⴱⴱ
.66
ⴱⴱ
.19
.28
ⴱⴱ
10. DEBQ-external eating .57
ⴱⴱ
.27
ⴱⴱ
.22
ⴱⴱ
11. DEBQ-emotional eating .15
.24
ⴱⴱ
12. Minutes of physical activity .36
ⴱⴱ
13. Weight change (%, 0–12 mo.)
Note. For weight change (%, 0 –12 months): negative numbers represent weight loss, positive numbers weight gain.
p.10 level (two-tailed).
p.05 (two-tailed).
ⴱⴱ
p.01.
4MATA ET AL.
T2
tapraid5/zg1-hea/zg1-hea/zg100509/zg12352d09z
xppws S1 7/8/09 22:47 Art: 2009-1642
APA PROOFS
the same regression models were run, this time with the group
membership always forced into the model as one predictor.
Results were comparable to the unadjusted models; in particu-
lar, all five eating regulation measures that were predicted by
intrinsic motivation for exercise without group adjustment were
still predicted by exercise motivation when controlling for
group (all s between .16 and .21 or .16 and .21, all
ps.05; results not shown).
To test our second hypothesis, that is, whether general,
treatment, and exercise-specific measures mediated the rela-
tionship between physical activity and eating self-regulation,
mediation analyses were conducted. Variables that most con-
sistently predicted eating self-regulation in the regression anal-
yses were chosen as putative mediators (see Table 3). Physical
activity (the predictor) was significantly correlated to all me-
diators, general self-determination, autonomous treatment reg-
ulation, and intrinsic exercise motivation (A). All mediators
were significantly related to all eating regulation variables (the
outcomes), except intrinsic exercise motivation was not related
to restraint and autonomous treatment regulation was not asso-
ciated with emotional eating (B). As hypothesized, physical
activity was associated with all eating regulation variables in
the expected directions (C). When the mediators were added to
the model, the relationship between physical activity and all
types of eating regulations became nonsignificant (D). Results
for indirect effects (i.e., the magnitude of the mediation effect)
for individual mediators were significant for most of the eating
regulation variables (E). General self-determination was not
significant for disinhibition and external eating, and autonomous
treatment motivation not significant for emotional eating. Collec-
tively, results showed that overall general self-determination, auton-
omous treatment motivation, and intrinsic exercise motivation
fully mediated the relationship between physical activity and eat-
ing regulation.
Discussion
As hypothesized, exercise motivation and self-regulation was
associated with several important markers of eating self-
regulation. Exercise intrinsic motivation predicts eating regulation
beyond general self-determination and treatment motivation. This
suggests that not only general self-determination and treatment
Table 2
Stepwise Regression Analyses. Self-Determined Motivation as Predictor for Eating Self-Regulation
Dependent variable Predictors
Model A
General treatment
& self-determination
Model B
Exercise-specific
self-determination
Model C
General treatment
& exercise-specific
self-determination
ppP
Eating self-efficacy Self-determination (SDS) 0.23 .001 not tested 0.23 .001
Autonomous treatment self-regulation (TSRQ) 0.31 .001 not tested 0.24 .002
Controlled treatment self-regulation (TSRQ) 0.15 .02 not tested not entered
Autonomous exercise self-regulation (ExSRQ) not tested 0.38 .001 not entered
Intrinsic motivation (IMI) not tested not entered 0.19 .01
R
2
(p) .23 (.001) .15 (.001) .27 (.001)
TFEQ-restraint Self-determination (SDS) not entered not tested 0.12 .10
Autonomous treatment self-regulation (TSRQ) 0.34 .001 not tested 0.24 .003
Intrinsic motivation (IMI) not tested 0.29 .001 0.16 .004
R
2
(p) .12 (.001) .10 (.001) .17 (.001)
TFEQ-disinhibition Autonomous treatment self-regulation (TSRQ) 0.38 .001 not tested 0.32 .001
Controlled treatment self-regulation (TSRQ) 0.17 .01 not tested 0.14 .05
Autonomous exercise self-regulation (ExSRQ) not entered 0.33 .001 not entered
Controlled exercise self-regulation (ExSRQ) not tested 0.17 .02 not entered
Intrinsic motivation (IMI) not tested not entered 0.16 .04
R
2
(p) .16 (.001) .12 (.001) .16 (.001)
DEBQ-external eating Self-determination (SDS) 0.24 .001 not tested 0.14 .06
Autonomous treatment self-regulation (TSRQ) 0.18 .01 not tested 0.19 .01
Intrinsic motivation (IMI) not tested 0.30 .001 0.19 .01
R
2
(p) .12 (.001) .09 (.001) .15 (.001)
DEBQ-emotional eating Self-determination (SDS) 0.15 .04 not tested 0.15 .05
Autonomous treatment self-regulation (TSRQ) 0.16 .03 not tested 0.09 .23
Controlled treatment self-regulation (TSRQ) 0.18 .01 not tested 0.17 .02
Intrinsic motivation (IMI) not tested 0.26 .001 0.19 .01
R
2
(p) .10 (.001) .06 (.001) .12 (.001)
Note. Model A: General and treatment self-determination as predictors of eating regulation. Model B: Exercise-specific self-determination as predictor
of eating regulation. Model C: General, treatment, and exercise-specific self-determination as predictor of eating regulation. SDS, TSRQ autonomous and
controlled were always tested as predictors of eating regulation for Model A and C; IMI, ExSRQ autonomous and controlled, were always tested as
predictors of eating regulation for Model B and C. If a predictor is not listed, it did not enter any of the three models. “not-tested” means that this variable
was not tested in the specific regression model. “not entered” means that this variable was tested in this specific stepwise regression model but did not enter
the model due to poor empirical fit.
5
EXERCISE SELF-DETERMINATION AND EATING SELF-REGULATION
T3
tapraid5/zg1-hea/zg1-hea/zg100509/zg12352d09z
xppws S1 7/8/09 22:47 Art: 2009-1642
APA PROOFS
Table 3
Summary of Mediation Analyses
A. Predictor (min moderate and vigorous physical activity) to mediators (motivation)
SE
General self-determination .19
.08
Autonomous treatment regulation .34
ⴱⴱ
.07
Intrinsic motivation to exercise .40
ⴱⴱ
.06
B. Direct effects of mediators (self-determined motivation) to outcomes (eating variables)
Eating self-efficacy TFEQ-restraint TFEQ-disinhibition DEBQ-external eating DEBQ-emotional eating
SE SE SE SE SE
General self-determination .22
ⴱⴱ
.07 .12
.07 .13
.08 .14
.07 .18
.07
Autonomous treatment regulation .24
ⴱⴱ
.08 .21
ⴱⴱ
.08 .29
ⴱⴱ
.08 .17
.08 .07 .08
Intrinsic motivation exercise .17
.08 .11 .08 .15
.09 .16
.09 .21
.09
C. Total effect of predictor (physical activity) on outcomes (eating variables)
Eating self-efficacy TFEQ-restraint TFEQ-disinhibition DEBQ-external eating DEBQ-emotional eating
SE SE SE SE SE
Min moderate and vigorous
physical activity .26
ⴱⴱ
.08 .27
ⴱⴱ
.07 .19
.08 .28
ⴱⴱ
.07 .15
.07
D. Direct effect of predictor (physical activity) on outcomes (eating variables)
Eating self-efficacy TFEQ-restraint TFEQ-disinhibition DEBQ-external eating DEBQ-emotional eating
SE SE SE SE SE
Min moderate and vigorous
physical activity .06 .08 .13
.07 .02 .08 .13 .08 .01 .08
E. Normal theory tests for indirect effects
Eating self-efficacy TFEQ-restraint TFEQ-disinhibition DEBQ-external eating DEBQ-emotional eating
SE SE SE SE SE
Total indirect effect .20
ⴱⴱ
.05 .14
ⴱⴱ
.04 .18
ⴱⴱ
.05 .15
ⴱⴱ
.04 .14
ⴱⴱ
.04
General self-determination .05
.02 .02
.02 .02 .02 .02 .02 .03
.02
Autonomous treatment regulation .08
.03 .07
.03 .10
ⴱⴱ
.04 .06
.03 .02 .03
Intrinsic motivation exercise .08
.04 .04
.03 .06
.03 .06
.04 .08
.04
Note.
p0.10 (all two-tailed).
p0.05 (two-tailed).
ⴱⴱ
p0.01.
6MATA ET AL.
tapraid5/zg1-hea/zg1-hea/zg100509/zg12352d09z
xppws S1 7/8/09 22:47 Art: 2009-1642
APA PROOFS
motivation “spilled-over” to eating regulation, but also that
exercise-specific motivation additionally contributed to improved
eating behavior. Furthermore, the relationship between self-
reported physical activity and eating regulation is mediated by
general self-determination, autonomous treatment motivation, and
(for most eating-related outcomes) intrinsic exercise motivation.
This suggests that, besides physiological effects of exercise which
may affect appetite regulation, motivational mechanisms may also
explain the association between physical activity and eating be-
haviors.
When controlling for intervention effects, the unique contribution
of exercise motivation for eating regulation persists, suggesting
that increase in exercise motivation is associated with eating
regulation independent of the intervention treatment. One inter-
esting difference in the group-adjusted analyses was that autono-
mous motivation for treatment was no longer predictive of emo-
tional eating and external eating. This suggests a strong
intervention effect on these particular variables, a fact consistent
with the intervention curriculum which covered these topics to a
considerable extent and in various sessions (Silva et al., 2008).
Our study’s results relate to findings from other weight manage-
ment intervention trials suggesting that changing both eating and
exercise behavior might have synergistic effects (C. L. Dunn et al.,
2006; Jakicic et al., 2002) or longer-lasting effects than diet or
exercise change alone (see Miller, Koceja, & Hamilton, 1997, for a
meta-analysis). However, few studies have included or reported on
the effect of psychological factors, such as motivation, on multiple
behavior change (J. O. Prochaska et al., 2008), thus identifying
potential mechanisms underlying this synergistic change. Taking mul-
tiple behavior intervention from a behavioral to a motivational level is
a promising step to discover underlying mechanisms, which in turn
can be powerful targets for more successful, long-term interventions.
Qualitative changes in motivation, from less to more autonomous,
may not happen in isolation but instead apply to various domains
simultaneously, even if to varying degrees. In this study, more auton-
omous general self-determination, treatment motivation, and exercise-
specific intrinsic motivation resulting from a theory-guided interven-
tion were related to several eating regulation variables. The predictive
power of general self-determination for eating regulation could sup-
port Vallerand’s (1997) assumption of motivational bottom-up pro-
cesses (e.g., an increase in exercise specific self-determination affect-
ing increased general self-determination), which would in turn
influence eating regulation (through top-down processes). However,
exercise-specific motivation was associated with eating regulation
beyond change in general self-determination (although explaining
fewer variance than general self-determination), thus suggesting also
a dynamic interplay between the two contextual levels (i.e., physical
activity and eating) of motivation, in line with Hagger and colleagues’
trans-contextual model (Hagger et al., 2003).
General and specific motivational change resulting from interven-
tions is one plausible mechanism underlying the association between
increased physical activity and improved eating regulation. In future
studies, it could be promising to measure psychological factors simul-
taneously with other factors that may be involved in the relationship
between physical activity and eating regulation, such as physiological
(e.g., appetite regulation) or behavioral (e.g., stress management strat-
egies), to compare their relative influences on eating behavior. It
should be noted that although we specifically wanted to explore the
hypothesis that led from exercise behavior and exercise motivation to
eating behavior, one cannot exclude reciprocal effects at the motiva-
tional level, where, for instance, success at eating self-regulation
would also positively influence motivation and/or confidence for
exercising.
Our results concerning self-reported physical activity echo previous
observational research suggesting physical activity as a gateway behavior
for motivational changes in eating regulation (Blakely, Dunnagan,
Haynes, Moore, & Pelican, 2004; Nigg et al., 1999; Tucker & Reicks,
2002). However, intervention studies have not generally found physical
activity to have such a gateway function (Dutton, Napolitano, Whiteley,
& Marcus, 2008; Wilcox, King, Castro, & Bortz, 2000). One possible
explanation for these inconsistent findings is that motivational change
toward health behaviors was not the target of the intervention trials but
rather implementation of behavioral programs. For example, Wilcox and
colleagues (2005) reported that both physical activity and eating behavior
changed after a behavioral intervention targeting physical activity. How-
ever, change in physical activity did not explain change in eating behav-
ior, suggesting the existence of a third factor that would underlie the
change in eating through physical activity change. Such a factor could be
motivational in nature, namely the extent to which motivation regulation
is more self-directed and less contingent on external demands. A strictly
behavior-focused intervention, not directed at creating an autonomy-
promoting climate may insufficiently influence internal self-regulation
and intrinsic motivation.
In conclusion, this study shows that spill-over effects may occur
between treatment and exercise motivation and eating self-regulation, in
the course of a weight control intervention. Furthermore, the qualitative
nature of motivational regulation (i.e., intrinsic and autonomous vs. ex-
ternally controlled) seem to be underlying mechanisms for the relation-
ship between actual physical activity and eating regulation. Research
concerning multiple behavior change is the future of preventive medicine
(J. O. Prochaska, 2008). However, so far there has been little effort to
develop theories of health behavior that directly address intervention in
more than one behavior simultaneously (J. J. Prochaska, Spring, & Nigg,
2008) or programmatic research showing the effectiveness of interven-
tions targeting two or more health behaviors (J. O. Prochaska, 2008).
Investigating motivation in more detail in the context of behavioral
weight management programs holds promise for the development of
psychological models of multiple behavior change. It also has direct
applied value, informing possible effective strategies for weight manage-
ment in clinical practice.
References
Annesi, J. J., & Unruh, J. L. (2008). Relations of exercise, self-appraisal,
mood changes and weight loss in obese women: Testing propositions
based on Baker and Brownell’s 2000 model. The American Journal of
Medical Sciences, 335, 198 –204.
Baker, C. W., & Brownell, K. D. (2000). Physical activity and maintenance
of weight loss: Physiological and psychological mechanisms. In B.
Christopher (Ed.), Physical Activity and Obesity (pp. 311–328). Cham-
paign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Blair, S., Applegate, W., Dunn, A., Ettinger, W., Haskell, W., King, A.,
et al. (1998). Activity Counseling Trial (ACT): Rationale, design, and
methods. Medicine & Science in Sports and Exercise, 30, 1097–1106.
Blakely, F., Dunnagan, T., Haynes, G., Moore, S., & Pelican, S. (2004).
Moderate physical activity and its relationship to select measures of a
healthy diet. Journal of Rural Health, 20, 160 –165.
Blanchard, C., Mask, L., Vallerand, R. J., de la Sablionnie`re, R., &
Provencher, P. (2007). Reciprocal relationships between contextual and
7
EXERCISE SELF-DETERMINATION AND EATING SELF-REGULATION
AQ: 5
tapraid5/zg1-hea/zg1-hea/zg100509/zg12352d09z
xppws S1 7/8/09 22:47 Art: 2009-1642
APA PROOFS
situational motivation in a sports setting. Psychology of Sport & Exer-
cise, 8, 854 – 873.
Clark, M. M., Abrams, D. B., Niaura, R. S., Eaton, C. A., & Rossi, J. S.
(1991). Self-efficacy in weight management. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 59, 739 –744.
Deci, E., & Ryan, R. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in
human behavior. New York: Plenum Press.
Dunn, A., Marcus, B., Kampert, J., Garcia, M., Kohl, H., & Blair, S.
(1999). Comparison of lifestyle and structured interventions to increase
physical activity and cardiorespiratory fitness. Journal of the American
Medical Association, 281, 327–334.
Dunn, C. L., Hannan, P. J., Jeffery, R. W., Sherwood, N. E., Pronk, N. P.,
& Boyle, R. (2006). The comparative and cumulative effects of a dietary
restriction and exercise on weight loss. International Journal of Obesity,
30, 112–121.
Dutton, G. R., Napolitano, M. A., Whiteley, J. A., & Marcus, B. H. (2008).
Is physical activity a gateway behavior for diet? Findings from a phys-
ical activity trial. Preventive Medicine, 46, 216 –221.
Fortier, M. S., Sweet, S. N., O’Sullivan, T. L., & Williams, G. C. (2007).
A self-determination process model of physical activity adoption in the
context of a randomized controlled trial. Psychology of Sport and Ex-
ercise, 8, 741–757.
Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N. L., Culverhouse, T., & Biddle, S. J. H.
(2003). The process by which perceived autonomy support in physical
education promotes leisure-time activity intentions and behavior: A trans-
contextual model. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 784 –795.
Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., Barkoukis, V., Wang, C. K. J., &
Baranowski, J. (2005). Perceived autonomy support in physical education
and leisure-time physical activity: A cross-cultural evaluation of the trans-
contextual model. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 376 –390.
Jakicic, J. M., Wing, R. R., & Winters-Hart, C. (2002). Relationship of
physical activity to eating behaviors and weight loss in women. Medi-
cine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 34, 1653–1659.
Martins, C., Morgan, L., & Truby, H. (2008). A review of the effects of
exercise on appetite regulation: An obesity perspective. International
Journal of Obesity, 32, 1337–1347.
Miller, W. C., Koceja, D. M., & Hamilton, E. J. (1997). A meta-analysis of
the past 25 years of weight loss research using diet, exercise or diet plus
exercise intervention. International Journal of Obesity and Related
Metabolic Disorders, 21, 941–947.
Nigg, C. R., Burbank, P. M., Padula, C., Dufresne, R., Rossi, J. S., Velicer,
W. F., et al. (1999). Stages of change across ten health risk behaviors for
older adults. Gerontologist, 39, 473– 482.
Pelletier, L. G., & Dion, S. C. (2007). An examination of general and
specific motivational mechanisms for the relation between body dissat-
isfaction and eating behaviors. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychol-
ogy, 26, 303–333.
Pelletier, L. G., Dion, S. C., Slovinec-D’Angelo, M., & Reid, R. (2004).
Why do you regulate what you eat? Relationships between forms of
regulation, eating behaviors, sustained dietary behavior change, and
psychological adjustment. Motivation and Emotion, 28, 245–277.
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling
strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple me-
diator models. Behavioral Research Methods, 40, 879 – 891.
Prochaska, J. J., Spring, B., & Nigg, C. R. (2008). Multiple health behavior
change research: An introduction and overview. Preventive Medicine,
46, 181–188.
Prochaska, J. O. (2008). Multiple Health Behavior Research represents the
future of preventive medicine. Preventive Medicine, 46, 281–285.
Prochaska, J. O., Butterworth, S., Redding, C. A., Burden, V., Perrin, N.,
Leo, M., et al. (2008). Initial efficacy of MI, TTM tailoring and HRI’s
with multiple behaviors for employee health promotion. Preventive
Medicine, 46, 226 –231.
Pronk, N. P., Anderson, L. H., Crain, A. L., Martinson, B. C., O’Connor,
P. J., Sherwood, N. E., et al. (2004). Meeting recommendations for
multiple healthy lifestyle factors. Prevalence, clustering, and predictors
among adolescent, adult, and senior health plan members. American
Journal of Preventive Medicine, 27, 25–33.
Ryan, R. M. (1982). Control and information in the intrapersonal sphere:
An extension of cognitive evaluation theory. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 43, 450 – 461.
Ryan, R. M., & Connell, J. (1989). Perceived locus of causality and
internalization: Examining reasons for acting in two domains. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 749 –761.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivations:
Classic definitions and new directions. Contemporary Educational Psy-
chology, 25, 54 – 67.
Silva, M. N., Markland, D. A., Minderico, C. S., Vieira, P. N., Castro,
M. M., Coutinho, S. R., et al. (2008). A randomized controlled trial to
evaluate Self-Determination Theory for exercise adherence and weight
control: Rationale and intervention description. BMC Public Health, 8,
234.
Silva, M. N., Vieira, P. N., Coutinho, S. R., Matos, M. G., Sardinha, L. B.,
& Teixeira, P. J. (2009). Using self-determination theory to promote
physical activity and weight control: A randomized controlled trial in
women. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Stunkard, A., & Messick, S. (1985). The three-factor eating questionnaire
to measure dietary restraint, disinhibition and hunger. Journal of Psy-
chosomatic Research, 29, 71– 83.
Teixeira, P. J., Going, S. B., Houtkooper, L. B., Cussler, E. C., Metcalfe,
L. L., Blew, R. M., et al. (2006). Exercise motivation, eating, and body
image variables as predictors of weight control. Medicine & Science in
Sports & Exercise, 38, 179 –188.
Tucker, M., & Reicks, M. (2002). Exercise as a gateway behavior for
healthful eating among older adults: An exploratory study. Journal of
Nutrition Education & Behavior, 34, S14 –S19.
Vallerand, R. J. (1997). Toward a hierarchical model of intrinsic and
extrinsic motivation. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental
social psychology (pp. 271–360). New York: Academic Press.
Vallerand, R. J., & Ratelle, C. F. (2002). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation:
A hierarchical model. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), The motivation
and self-determination of behavior: Theoretical and applied issues.
Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.
Van Strien, I., Frijters, J., Bergers, G., & Defares, P. (1986). The Dutch
Eating Behavior Questionnaire (DEBQ) for assessment of restrained,
emotional and external eating behavior. International Journal Eating
Disorders, 5, 295–315.
Wilcox, S., King, A. C., Castro, C., & Bortz, W. (2000). Do changes in
physical activity lead to dietary changes in middle and old age? Amer-
ican Journal of Preventive Medicine, 18, 276 –283.
Williams, G. C., Freedman, Z. R., & Deci, E. L. (1998). Supporting
autonomy to motivate patients with diabetes for glucose control. Dia-
betes Care, 21, 1644 –1651.
Williams, G. C., Grow, V. M., Freedman, Z. R., Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L.
(1996). Motivational predictors of weight loss and weight-loss mainte-
nance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 115–126.
8MATA ET AL.
tapraid5/zg1-hea/zg1-hea/zg100509/zg12352d09z
xppws S1 7/8/09 22:47 Art: 2009-1642
... Besides influencing the target behavior, psychosocial interventions aiming at an increase of MVPA may either increase (spillover effect) or reduce (compensatory effect) the likelihood of initiation or maintenance of another health behavior (Geller et al., 2017). According to the spillover hypothesis, individuals may learn self-regulatory skills while attempting to change one behavior, such as MVPA, and use those skills later to attempt to adopt another behavior, such as healthy nutrition (Mata et al., 2009). The compensatory hypothesis may be further explained by mental fatigue models, which assume that exerting regular cognitive effort to adopt one behavior may reduce the regulatory capacity, resulting in difficulties in the continuous selfregulation of other behaviors (Gergelyfi et al., 2015). ...
... The evidence for spillover or compensatory effects obtained in prospective or experimental research is mixed (Geller et al., 2017;Kulis et al., 2023;Mata et al., 2009). For example, PA training resulted in a decrease in fat intake in adults in the control and KULIS ET AL. 2 experimental conditions, but no Time × Group interactions were observed (Dutton et al., 2008). ...
Article
Full-text available
Objetivo: Este estudio fue diseñado para investigar los efectos de la planificación individual, diádica y colaborativa sobre la actividad física de moderada a intensa (MVPA, por sus siglas en inglés; resultado primario) y la ingesta de alimentos ricos en energía (resultado secundario) en diadas de padres y sus niños de 9 a 15 años. La planificación individual refleja una planificación “yo para mí” del comportamiento de una persona. La planificación colaborativa (“nosotros para nosotros”) se refiere a la planificación conjunta del comportamiento de ambos miembros de la diada, mientras que la planificación diádica (“nosotros para mí”) implica la planificación conjunta únicamente del comportamiento de la persona objetivo. Métodos:N = 247 díadas participaron en un ensayo controlado aleatorio con condiciones de planificación y control de actividad física (PA, por sus siglas en inglés) individual, diádica o colaborativa (educación sobre PA, comportamiento sedentario, nutrición, equilibrio entre consumo y gasto de energía). La MVPA se midió con acelerómetros ActiGraph wGT3X-BT al inicio, a la semana y a las 36 semanas de seguimiento. La ingesta de alimentos ricos en energía fue autoinformada al inicio, a las 9 semanas y a las 36 semanas de seguimiento. Los modelos lineales mixtos se ajustaron para padres e hijos por separado. Resultados: En el seguimiento de 36 semanas, los niños en la condición de planificación diádica “nosotros para mí” redujeron su MVPA en comparación con la condición de control. Al mismo tiempo, los niños en la condición de planificación diádica también redujeron la ingesta de alimentos ricos en energía en el seguimiento de 36 semanas. No se encontraron efectos entre los niños en condiciones de planificación de PA individual y colaborativa. Los padres en cualquier condición experimental redujeron la ingesta de alimentos ricos en energía. Conclusiones: La disminución de la MVPA en los niños en la condición de planificación diádica de la PA se vio compensada por una reducción de la ingesta de energía, lo que puede representar un mecanismo compensatorio. La eficacia limitada de las intervenciones diádicas de “nosotros para mí” en las diadas de padres e hijos puede deberse a las necesidades de individuación de los jóvenes y a su reacción al apoyo de sus padres.
... Each row presents a parameter (solid circle) and confidence interval (horizontal bar) from a meta-analysis of identically-specified stochastic actororiented models fitted in each village; effect groups A-F are identical to those shown in Fig. 3; estimates are interpreted as log-odds ratios of a discussion tie being sent to a given other villager; these parameters were simultaneously estimated with those presented in Fig. 4; for further explanatory variables in the models as well as heterogeneity statistics for the meta-analyses, see section 4 of the Supplementary Information. prevention behaviors (16). The full models include further control variables explaining discussion ties, which are listed in section 4 of the Supplementary Information. ...
Article
Full-text available
The effective prevention of many infectious and non-infectious diseases relies on people concurrently adopting multiple prevention behaviors. Individual characteristics, opinion leaders, and social networks have been found to explain why people take up specific prevention behaviors. However, it remains challenging to understand how these factors shape multiple interdependent behaviors. We propose a multilevel social network framework that allows us to study the effects of individual and social factors on multiple disease prevention behaviors simultaneously. We apply this approach to examine the factors explaining eight malaria prevention behaviors, using unique interview data collected from 1529 individuals in 10 hard-to-reach, malaria-endemic villages in Meghalaya, India in 2020–2022. Statistical network modelling reveals exposure to similar behaviors in one’s social network as the most important factor explaining prevention behaviors. Further, we find that households indirectly shape behaviors as key contexts for social ties. Together, these two factors are crucial for explaining the observed patterns of behaviors and social networks in the data, outweighing individual characteristics, opinion leaders, and social network size. The results highlight that social network processes may facilitate or hamper disease prevention efforts that rely on a combination of behaviors. Our approach is well suited to study these processes in the context of various diseases.
... The meta-analysis results indicate that autonomous motivation is the primary factor influencing women's sports participation. 50,52 The stronger the autonomous motivation, the more it encourages women to engage in physical exercise. This conclusion matches most empirical studies and is consistent with the perspectives of SDT, suggesting that autonomy in the motivational process plays an active role in better motivating and maintaining individual behavior. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction The lack of physical exercise is a global health concern, particularly affecting women. However, there is limited research on factors influencing women’s sports participation. Recent studies on increasing women’s physical activity levels differ in methodologies and conclusions. Motivation, as the cornerstone of most human behaviors, has important effects on female motor participation. Self-Determination Theory (SDT) is an important method to study human behavioral motivation and supported by empirical evidence. In the field of women’s sports, the SDT is also widely used. This review explores the impact of SDT-related factors on women’s sports participation, considering age variations. It aims to guide future empirical research and promote PA across demographics. Methods This review, by searching the existing empirical literature in Web of Science, Google Scholar, Elsevier ScienceDirect, CNKI, obtained 32 independent studies, conducted a meta-analysis after coding them, considering 11 influencing factors from the integration of SDT and Basic Psychological Needs Theory. Results/Conclusion The study found a significant positive correlation between autonomous motivation and women’s sports participation, with identified regulation having the strongest influence. Controlled motivation showed no significant impact on women’s exercise, while amotivation had an inhibitory effect. Enhancing women’s perception of autonomy, competence, and relatedness significantly promotes sports participation. Age differences were observed in the relationship between autonomous motivation, basic psychological needs, and sports participation, with the strongest effects on women aged 25–40, while the impact was relatively weaker in older women. The correlation between basic psychological needs and female sports participation also has a significant age difference. Thus, different measures should be taken to improve exercise participation in women of different age groups.
... Al respecto, como explicación de la influencia de la actividad física sobre la conducta alimentaria, otra investigación mostró que podría haber una interacción a nivel motivacional y conductual al realizar actividad física. En este sentido, los mecanismos motivacionales pueden influir en la regulación alimentaria y selección de alimentos (Mata et al., 2009). ...
Article
Full-text available
El bajo consumo de frutas y de verduras es una preocupación mundial. Varios estudios han realizado intervenciones conductuales para mejorarlo. A su vez, de acuerdo con el modelo de determinantes de la salud, las conductas son consideradas determinantes intermedios, debido a que su expresión incide sobre la prevención o aparición de enfermedades. La importancia de estos determinantes justificó la realización de un análisis de perspectiva acerca de un conjunto de conductas que pueden favorecer el consumo de frutas y verduras en escolares. En este análisis, se identificaron como determinantes conductuales: conductas que corresponden al individuo, tales como cocinar, cultivar o sembrar alimentos, realizar actividad física; y conductas que dependen del entorno, tales como algunas prácticas de los padres o cuidadores al ofrecer alimentos a los escolares, la práctica de la lactancia materna y la selección de alimentos en el restaurante escolar. Dentro de los resultados cabe resaltar algunos elementos que pudieron incidir en el éxito de intervenciones que usaron determinantes conductuales para incrementar la ingesta de frutas y verduras: todas las investigaciones que incluyeron actividad física más educación alimentaria y nutricional lograron incrementos en la ingesta de frutas y verduras de los escolares. Las estrategias de comportamiento planificado pueden ser más efectivas cuando estas son planeadas y ejecutadas directamente por los escolares; la lactancia materna favorece la ingesta de verduras y frutas durante la infancia; la exposición repetida de alimentos puede favorecer la preferencia hacia los alimentos presentados, pero no necesariamente su ingesta. Un aspecto crucial a tener en cuenta es el número de exposiciones. Las actividades de siembra y jardinería pueden ser más efectivas si se acompañan de actividades de educación alimentaria y nutricional. Son necesarias más investigaciones que amplíen la información sobre el uso de estos determinantes para modificar la conducta alimentaria.
... While this review examined PA as the outcome, other lifestyle and behavioral factors affect longterm conditions and are important to consider in designing interventions. Research has shown that success in initiating and maintaining PA may be a gateway behavior that can promote other lifestyle behaviors important for specific longterm conditions [58]. Thus, isolating PA as an outcome for interventions may not achieve a broader impact on overall lifestyle for people living with long-term conditions. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Interventions that use the Health Action Process Approach (HAPA) model show promise for increasing PA frequency, duration, and intensity. However, there is limited understanding of how HAPA model variables have been operationalized for PA interventions in chronic disease to promote behavior change and sustained PA or whether the phase or continuous form of the HAPA model was used. The aim of this scoping review is to describe how the HAPA model variables for PA interventions were operationalized and provide details of implementation. Method We searched five databases to identify studies published between January 1992 and March 2024. We aimed to describe (1) the characteristics of interventions including setting, delivery mode, duration, and content; (2) which HAPA variables were operationalized and the strategies used; and (3) the physical activity measures and outcome effects. Results The search identified 23 interventions in 30 papers (12 protocols, 3 quasi-experimental studies, and 15 randomized controlled trials (RCTs)). Seven of the 15 RCTs reported significant positive effects of the HAPA model on PA behavior outcomes. Interventions operationalized between three and nine HAPA constructs showed significant variability in how the HAPA model is used in intervention research. PA measures varied from self-report to validated objective instruments. Conclusion We found a lack of clarity in decisions about which HAPA constructs were included in interventions. The wide variability in operationalized HAPA constructs made it challenging to compare interventions. Researchers should provide more detail about intervention design and implementation procedures to enhance transparency.
Thesis
Full-text available
In domains such as the environment or health, a combination of behaviors is often required to achieve a significant impact. The "spillover effect" is observed when an intervention has effects that go beyond the behavior targeted by the intervention. Taking the perspective of self-determination theory, we tested the extent to which a message based on autonomysupportive and descriptive norm arguments (i.e., autonomy-normative) could have an impact not only on the targeted behavior, but also on other non-targeted behaviors (positive spillover effect). A message based on controlling arguments was unlikely to produce such effects. Autonomous motivation was thought to mediate the spillover effect. Three studies focused on the impact of communications designed to motivate students to adopt pro-environmental behaviors on their campus. An autonomous-normative communication on waste separation generated a positive spillover effect on other pro-environmental behaviors through the promotion of autonomous motivation. A controlling communication only had an effect on the targeted behaviors. A fourth study was carried out on domestic energy consumption in the context of a national energy saving plan (general population). Only autonomous-normative communication favored both targeted and non-targeted behaviors, with a mediating effect of autonomous motivation. A fifth study in the health field tested communications aimed at encouraging the adoption of preventive behaviors towards COVID-19 (general population). Autonomous-normative communication targeting vaccination behavior had no impact, but controlling communication negatively affected both targeted and non-targeted behaviors (negative spillover effect). The implications and perspectives of this research program are discussed with regard to the implementation of communication campaigns with potential spillover effects.
Article
The predominant method for treating obesity has been suggesting and providing information on a controlled diet and, to a lesser extent, increased exercise. That approach has largely failed beyond the short term for many decades as obesity rates continue to rise. Therefore, leveraging improvements in psychosocial correlates of weight-loss behaviors has sometimes been suggested instead. The aim of this study was evaluation of targeted improvements in self-regulation and self-efficacy within a theoretically derived weight-loss program. Women with obesity ( N = 103) participated in a year-long community-based program emphasizing self-regulatory skills development to counter lifestyle barriers/challenges to first exercise, then controlled eating. Within a structured treatment protocol administered by community facility employees–and based on tenets of social cognitive theory, self-regulation theory, self-efficacy theory, and coaction theory–self-regulatory skills were initially developed to foster adherence to exercise. Those skills were then adapted to promote eating-behavior changes, emphasizing fruit and vegetable intake. Improvements in measures of exercise self-regulation, eating self-regulation, and self-efficacy for controlling eating, their corresponding behaviors, and weight and waist circumference were significant. Greater within-participant carry-over of changes in exercise self-regulation to eating self-regulation was significantly associated with more weight and waist circumference reductions over both 6 and 12 months. Change in eating-related self-efficacy significantly mediated those relationships. The mean weight reduction of approximately 6% indicated positive effects on obesity-related health risks. The community-based setting indicated potentials for large-scale dissemination of theory- and evidence-driven behavioral obesity treatments focused primarily on self-regulatory skills development.
Article
Increasing understanding of self-regulation processes within behavioral obesity treatments is required to improve consistently inadequate outcomes. The mood-behavior model proposes that, under conditions of goal striving and mood awareness, enhanced mood will mobilize self-regulation resources. That was tested within community-based obesity treatments where increased exercise might enhance mood, and increased self-regulation could improve weight-loss behaviors. Women with obesity participating in treatments focusing on either moderate exercise and self-regulation (Exercise/Self-Regulation [Exercise/SR] group, n = 42) or education on healthy eating and (to a lesser extent) exercise (Usual Care group, n = 48) were assessed on psychological and behavioral changes over 6 months. Significant overall improvements in negative mood, self-regulation for exercise and controlling eating, exercise, and fruit/vegetable intake were significantly greater in the Exercise/SR group. Using aggregated data ( N = 90), at baseline, negative mood did not significantly predict self-regulation. However, at months 3 and 6, negative mood significantly predicted both self-regulation for exercise and self-regulation for controlling eating. The addition of the group as a predictor significantly increased the explained variances. Increase in self-regulation for exercise significantly mediated the prediction of increased exercise via earlier reduction in negative mood. When group was entered into that model, it significantly moderated the mood-self-regulation and mood-exercise relationships. Increase in self-regulation for controlling eating significantly mediated the prediction of increase in fruit/vegetable intake via reduction in negative mood. Group marginally moderated the mood-self-regulation relationship only. Findings supported viability of the mood-behavior model in a field-based setting and suggested a need to facilitate improved mood to increase participants’ self-regulatory skill usage within obesity treatments.
Article
Full-text available
The chapter outlines a general model of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation, the Hierarchical model. This model serves two objectives. First, the model provides a framework to organize the literature on intrinsic and extrinsic motivation, as well as to identify the psychological mechanisms underlying motivational changes. Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation represents a substantial portion of people's experiences when involved in activities. Furthermore, conceptual and methodological advances are presented supporting a multidimensional approach to the study of motivational phenomena. Such an approach has been found useful—for instance, for distinguishing nonintrinsic but internalized motivational forces that promote adaptive consequences such as persisting at difficult tasks from other internalized forces that compromise adaptive adjustment. A second objective of the hierarchical model is to lead to novel and testable hypotheses. Moreover, other aspects of the model also appear as prime candidates for future research. Such a model serves to integrate the literature and points toward new research. It is within such a contextually situated perspective that the model shows its greatest promise of usefulness.
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this study was to test a model of the regulation of eating behaviors that could help to better understand the processes by which body dissatisfaction could be either associated with dysfunctional eating behaviors or with healthy eat- ing behaviors. Based on Self-Determination Theory (Deci & Ryan, 1985; Ryan & Deci, 2000, it appears that women's general level of self-determination in life may help protect against pressures about body image and endorsement of society's be- liefs about thinness and obesity. Findings also suggest that women's general level of self-determination is positively associated with an autonomous regulation of eating behaviors (AREB) and negatively associated with a controlled regulation of eating behaviors (CREB). In turn, the AREB is positively associated with healthy eat- ing while CREB is positively associated with dysfunctional eating. Overall, it ap- pears that body dissatisfaction resulting from pressures about body image and endorsement of society's beliefs about thinness and obesity may be more closely associated with a controlled regulation of eating behaviors, which may explain its relation with eating pathology.
Article
Full-text available
In 3 studies, the authors examined how autonomous and controlled forms of motivation for the regulation of eating behaviors were related to self-reported eating behaviors, and sustained dietary behavior change. Studies 1 and 2 supported the factorial structure and the psychometric properties of a scale designed to measure different forms of regulation as defined by Self-Determination Theory. A motivational model of the regulation of eating behaviors suggested that an autonomous regulation was positively associated with healthy eating behaviors whereas a controlled regulation was positively associated with dysfunctional eating behaviors and negatively associated with healthy eating behaviors. In Study 3, long-term adherence to healthier dietary behaviors in a population at risk for coronary artery disease was examined over a 26-week period. A general measure of self-determined motivation assessed at week 1 was found to be a reliable predictor of the level of self-determination for eating behaviors 13 weeks later. In turn, self-determination for eating behaviors was a significant predictor of dietary behavior changes at 26 weeks. Finally, the dietary behavior measures were related to improvements in weight and blood lipid parameters (LDL-cholesterol, HDL-cholesterol, triglycerides). Results are discussed in terms of their implication for the integration and maintenance of a successful healthy regulation.
Article
Full-text available
Behavior change interventions are effective to the extent that they affect appropriately-measured outcomes, especially in experimental controlled trials. The primary goal of this study was to analyze the impact of a 1-year weight management intervention based on self-determination theory (SDT) on theory-based psychosocial mediators, physical activity/exercise, and body weight and composition. Participants were 239 women (37.6 +/- 7.1 years; 31.5 +/- 4.1 kg/m(2)) who received either an intervention focused on promoting autonomous forms of exercise regulation and intrinsic motivation, or a general health education program (controls). At 12 months, the intervention group showed increased weight loss (-7.29%,) and higher levels of physical activity/exercise (+138 +/- 26 min/day of moderate plus vigorous exercise; +2,049 +/- 571 steps/day), compared to controls (P < 0.001). Main intervention targets such as more autonomous self-regulation (for treatment and for exercise) and a more autonomous perceived treatment climate revealed large effect sizes (between 0.80 and .96), favoring intervention (P < 0.001). Results suggest that interventions grounded in SDT can be successfully implemented in the context of weight management, enhancing the internalization of more autonomous forms of behavioral regulation, and facilitating exercise adherence, while producing clinically-significant weight reduction, when compared to a control condition. Findings are fully consistent with previous studies conducted within this theoretical framework in other areas of health behavior change.
Article
Full-text available
Extended findings that support cognitive evaluation theory to intrapersonal processes by exploring the effects of informational vs controlling feedback, when self-selected and administered vs other-administered, and in conditions of task-involvement (intended to create an informational orientation in relation to the activity) vs ego-involvement (intended to create a controlling orientation in relation to the activity). 128 undergraduates working on a hidden figures task received either an ego- or task-involving induction and then a series of 3 puzzle problems for which half of the Ss received informational feedback and the other half controlling feedback. Half the Ss had the feedback self-administered, and half had it administered by the experimenter. After puzzle-solving, Ss were left alone with additional puzzles and magazines and were observed to see if they worked on the puzzles. Finally, Ss completed a questionnaire assessing their interest and attitudes toward the target activity. Results confirm that controlling feedback, whether self- or other administered, undermined intrinsic motivation relative to task-involvement. Results are discussed in terms of the application of cognitive evaluation theory to intrapersonal processes and self-control theories. (34 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Self determination theory proposes that behavior change will occur and persist if it is autonomously motivated. Autonomous motivation for a behavior is theorized to be a function both of individual differences in the autonomy orientation from the General Causality Orientations Scale and of the degree of autonomy supportiveness of relevant social contexts. We tested the theory with 128 patients in a 6-month, very-low-calorie weight-loss program with a 23-month follow-up. Analyses confirmed the predictions that (a) participants whose motivation for weight loss was more autonomous would attend the program more regularly, lose more weight during the program, and evidence greater maintained weight loss at follow-up, and (b) participants' autonomous motivation for weight loss would be predicted both by their autonomy orientation and by the perceived autonomy supportiveness of the interpersonal climate created by the health-care staff. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Intrinsic and extrinsic types of motivation have been widely studied, and the distinction between them has shed important light on both developmental and educational practices. In this review we revisit the classic definitions of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation in light of contemporary research and theory. Intrinsic motivation remains an important construct, reflecting the natural human propensity to learn and assimilate. However, extrinsic motivation is argued to vary considerably in its relative autonomy and thus can either reflect external control or true self-regulation. The relations of both classes of motives to basic human needs for autonomy, competence and relatedness are discussed.
Article
Activity Counseling Trial: rationale, design, and methods. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc., Vol. 30, No. 7, pp. 1097-1106, 1998. The Activity Counseling Trial (ACT) is a multicenter, randomized controlled trial to evaluate the effectiveness of interventions to promote physical activity in the primary health care setting. ACT has recruited, evaluated, and randomized 874 men and women 35-75 yr of age who are patients of primary care physicians. Participants were assigned to one of three educational interventions that differ in amount of interpersonal contact and resources required: standard care control, staff-assisted intervention, or staff-counseling intervention. The study is designed to provide 90% power in both men and women to detect a 1.1 kcal·kg-1·day-1 difference in total daily energy expenditure between any two treatment groups, and over 90% power to detect a 7% increase in maximal oxygen uptake, the two primary outcomes. Primary analyses will compare study groups on mean outcome measures at 24 months post-randomization, be adjusted for the baseline value of the outcome measure and for multiple comparisons, and be conducted separately for men and women. Secondary outcomes include comparisons between interventions at 24 months of factors related to cardiovascular disease (blood lipids/lipoproteins, blood pressure, body composition, plasma insulin, fibrinogen, dietary intake, smoking, heart rate variability), psychosocial effects, and cost-effectiveness, and at 6 months for primary outcome measures. ACT is the first large-scale behavioral intervention study of physical activity counseling in a clinical setting, includes a generalizable sample of adult men and women and of clinical settings, and examines long-term (24 months) effects. ACT has the potential to make substantial contributions to the understanding of how to promote physical activity in the primary health care setting.