ArticlePDF Available

Taking into account the observers' uncertainty: A graduated approach to the credibility of the patient's pain evaluation

Authors:

Abstract

This article presents two experiments aiming to investigate the adoption of a graduated measure to describe credibility attribution by observers who evaluate patients' pain accounts. A total of 160 medical students were required to express a credibility judgment on the pain intensity level of hypothetical patients. We used 16 vignettes based on a factorial mixed-design. Within-participants factors were the reported pain, the presence of a physical sign, the patient's facial expression and the patient's gender, and between-groups factors were the patient's age and the geographical distribution of the patient's name. Results confirm the well-established tendency not to believe patients' self-reports and provide information regarding the evaluators' uncertainty. The findings suggest that a graduated measure is useful for assessing the degree of uncertainty of the observers and subtle effects of different factors upon the judgment of patient's pain.
The published article is available on SpringerLink:
http://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s10865-009-9232-5
The original publication is available at www.springerlink.com
Taking into account the observers’ uncertainty:
A graduated approach to the credibility of the patient’s pain evaluation
Patrice Rusconi, Paolo Riva, Paolo Cherubini, Lorenzo Montali
Department of Psychology, University of Milan-Bicocca, Italy
Author for correspondence is Patrice Rusconi:
Department of Psychology
Università Milano - Bicocca
1, Piazza Ateneo Nuovo
20126, Milano, Italy
e-mail: p.rusconi3@campus.unimib.it
fax: ++39 02 6448 3706
tel.: ++39 02 6448 3775
1
Acknowledgements
Authors wish to thank John Burns and three unknown referees for their pertinent comments
and suggestions. We are also grateful to Marco Marelli for his suggestions on the analyses of the
properties of the graduated approach used in Experiment 2.
1
Abstract
This article presents two experiments aiming to investigate the adoption of a graduated
measure to describe credibility attribution by observers who evaluate patients’ pain accounts. A
total of 160 medical students were required to express a credibility judgment on the pain intensity
level of hypothetical patients. We used 16 vignettes based on a factorial mixed-design. Within-
participants factors were the reported pain, the presence of a physical sign, the patient’s facial
expression and the patient’s gender, and between-groups factors were the patient’s age and the
geographical distribution of the patient’s name. Results confirm the well-established tendency not
to believe patients’ self-reports and provide information regarding the evaluators’ uncertainty. The
findings suggest that a graduated measure is useful for assessing the degree of uncertainty of the
observers and subtle effects of different factors upon the judgment of patient’s pain.
Key words: Credibility; Pain Assessment; Graduated Measure; Miscalibration.
2
1. Introduction
There is a wealth of evidence indicating that patients’ verbal reports regarding their pain are
disbelieved, or put into question through the assessment of other clues, by observers, including
healthcare professionals (e.g., Saxey, 1986; Scott, 1992; Thorn, 1997; Solomon, 2001; Clarke &
Iphofen, 2005; Clarke & Iphofen, 2008). In particular, a vast range of studies point to the fact that
there is a discrepancy between patients’ self evaluations and judgments expressed by observers,
often resulting in a phenomenon of “pain underestimation” (e.g., MacLeod et al., 2001; Marquié et
al., 2003; Kappesser et al., 2004; Kappesser et al., 2006).
This well-established tendency might bear major consequences. On the one hand, it might affect the
administration or the withholding of analgesics (Bell, 2000; Puntillo et al., 2003). On the other
hand, the disbelieved patient is more likely to experience an increased state of anxiety and negative
emotions (Jacques, 1992; Clarke & Iphofen, 2008) which may undermine effective care actions. For
this reason, it is worth clarifying the mechanisms underlying the phenomenon of disbelieving
patients’ accounts, as well as its boundaries, in order to improve pain control.
In the literature, the discrepancy between patients’ and observers’ judgments has often been
investigated by using unidimensional pain scales – such as the numeric rating scale (e.g., Chibnall
et al., 1997) which measures the severity of the described pain by requiring a discrete response. It is
made up of a fixed number of numerically tagged categories (usually eleven): the minimum value
of the scale is labeled ‘no pain’, and the maximum value is labeled ‘most intense pain imaginable’.
Patients are instructed to indicate the scale number corresponding to the intensity of pain they
actually feel.
In order to evaluate observers’ judgments, Iafrati’s criterion is commonly used: a score
outside the range of ±1 point compared to the patient’s self-rating is considered either an
overestimation or an underestimation (Iafrati, 1986; see also Kappesser et al., 2006). The finding of
the minimum clinically significant difference (MCSD) departs slightly from the Iafrati criterion
assumption: Strout & Burton (2004) found a MCSD of 1.45 (SD ± .84; 95% CI, 1.30-1.60), while
3
Kendrick and Strout (2005) reported a slightly different value equal to 1.39 (SD ± 1.05; 95% CI,
1.27-1.51). Even though this method is well suited for establishing whether the pain intensity
judged by the observer matches or not the level reported by the patient, it is not aimed at measuring
the observer’s degree of confidence, whereas this information might convey useful knowledge. For
example, given a patient reporting a pain intensity of 8, and two observers, A and B, both reporting
an estimated pain intensity of 6, the discrete measure classifies both observers as equally
miscalibrated. However, if the judgment includes some measures of subjective confidence, it might
result that observer A estimates the pain as ranging from 4 to 8, with an average of 6. By contrast,
observer B might be less uncertain about her guess, estimating a pain intensity ranging from 5 to 7,
with an average value of 6. This further piece of information helps understanding that A’s and B’s
degrees of disbelief of the patient’s rating are actually different: indeed A, but not B, includes the
patient’s rating within the interval that she deems plausible. Hence, our knowledge of the factors
affecting the credibility of patients’ self-reports might be fostered by considering it as a continuous
dimension, graduated by the range of variability that the observer associates to her own judgment,
instead of an all-or-nothing, discrete phenomenon. This approach seems suitable to judgments
characterized by uncertainty such as pain estimates, considering the evidence showing that people
can provide inconsistent estimates regarding the same pain stimulus, and that the same pain
stimulus can be perceived very differently by different persons (Mader et al., 2003).
In this study, we investigated whether using graduated estimates might actually improve our
understanding of the observers’ degree of belief in patients’ self-reports. Observers were asked to
indicate which pain intensity values they deemed credible, which were only partly credible, and
which were not at all credible. As a consequence, the observers were classified as calibrated or
miscalibrated, and a degree of confidence was associated to their calibration.
The aim was to devise a direct method for measuring the observer’s confidence towards the
patient’s pain rating that also elicited some information about the window of variability associated
to the judgment, and to compare the new method with the discrete one. As a secondary goal, we
4
investigated how different variables might affect the nature and quality of the confidence reported
by the observers.
Our perspective is not to question the validity and usefulness of discrete methods of pain evaluation,
but to seek for complementary measures that might enrich them, by providing supplementary
information.
2. Method
2.1. Materials, design and procedure
We devised two tasks involving the same procedure, materials and experimental design, the
only difference between them being the dependent variable and, consequently, part of the
instructions. Each of a series of 16 paper-and-pencil vignettes featured a patient who came to the
Emergency Department for a wart. During the visit, the patient reported having a headache.
Participants were told the patient’s name, gender, age, facial expression, the presence or absence of
physical signs (sensitivity to light or noise), and the patient’s pain self-rating. They were asked to
evaluate the patient’s pain.
For each of the 16 vignettes, participants were given a horizontal, 11-point, numeric rating
scale on which the patient’s rating was circled (see an example in the Appendix I). Then, they were
asked to express their degree of belief in the values of the scale as estimates of the patient’s pain
level by blackening (Experiment 1) or ranking (Experiment 2) each of the boxes above the eleven
units of the numeric rating scale. Booklets containing general instructions and the 16 vignettes were
handed out individually to each participant.
The characteristics described in each vignette were orthogonally crossed in a factorial
experimental mixed design (see Table I). Within-participants factors were: intensity of the reported
pain (low, ‘3’ on the 0-10 scale vs. high, ‘7’ on the 0-10 scale), physical sign (sensitivity to light or
noise vs. no sensitivity), facial expression (tense vs. relaxed), and patient’s gender (female vs.
5
male). Between-groups factors were the patients’ age (young vs. old) and the geographical
distribution of the patients’ names (Northern Italian vs. Southern Italian).1
----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
Insert Table I about here
----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
We had two main reasons for choosing headache as a source of pain. Firstly, it is a “non-
obvious” disease, which should induce observers to provide high ratings (Marquié et al., 2003), and
high ratings should highlight any miscalibration that low ratings might conceal (Chibnall & Tait,
2004). Yet, crucially, the non-obviousness of the cause of pain per se should not affect the extent of
the miscalibration between patients’ and observers’ ratings (Marquié et al., 2003). Secondly,
headaches are a quite widespread type of pain whose main features are generally well-known not
only to experts but also to novices, such as medical students.
3. Experiment 1
3.1. Participants
A total of 80 Italian medical students attending courses at university (52 female, 28 male;
mean age 22.1 years, range 19-27 years) took part as volunteers in Experiment 1. The choice to use
a sample of medical students was motivated by the preliminary nature of the study, whose
generalizability may be tested in further researches. Moreover, some previous studies on similar
topics recruited students as participants (e.g., undergraduate psychology students in Tait &
Chibnall, 1994 and in Chibnall and Tait, 1995; first-year medical students in Chibnall et al., 1997;
undergraduate students in MacLeod et al., 2001). Yet they provided an insightful basis for
subsequent studies. Furthermore, Marquié et al. (2004) didn’t find any difference in the degree of
miscalibration between novices and experts.
1 The latter factor was introduced on the basis of a preliminary qualitative study in which it emerged that healthcare
professionals were influenced by the patients’ place of origin (Montali et al., 2009). Hence, in order to further test this
aspect we decided to take into account the geographical distribution of the patients’ names instead of tackling the better
known ethnic disparities (Edwards et al., 2001a; Edwards et al., 2001b; Cintron & Morrison, 2006).
6
3.2. Dependent variables
In the first experiment we examined whether the patient’s rating was included in a range of
credible pain level values or not. Specifically, participants were instructed to indicate to what
degree they believed each of the values of the scale; they did this by completely blackening the
boxes above the points of the scale which they deemed credible as ratings of the patient’s pain level,
partially blackening the boxes above the points of the scale that they deemed only partly credible,
and leaving the boxes blank for the values that they considered not credible.
3.3. Results and analyses
Inclusion of the patient’s rating in the credibility range
Table II shows the mean percentages of inclusion of the patient’s rating in each of the three
intervals that participants could use to judge the credibility of the pain rating values.
----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
Insert Table II about here
----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
Overall, patient ratings fell within the credibility interval in 45.23% of the participants’
judgments; they were within the partial credibility interval in 28.67% of cases and within the non-
credibility interval in 26.09%. In other words, more than half of the credibility intervals did not
include the patient’s rating. In the following we will concentrate on the credibility interval as a
measure of trust in the patients’ pain accounts.
----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
Insert Table III about here
----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
7
Table III shows the results of non-parametric tests2 on the rates of inclusion of the patient’s
rating in the credibility interval. Participants included the patient’s rating in the range more
frequently when the intensity of the reported pain was low than when it was high (p < .00001, r = -
.463), and when the patient’s face was tense more than when it was relaxed (p < .005, r = -.23).
There was a trend to trust the patient more when he/she was described as having a physical sign (p
= .088, r = -.14). No significant differences emerged regarding patient’s gender, age and
geographical distribution of the name.
Analysis of interactions
We performed an analysis of variance aimed to explore possible interactions. The robustness
of this analysis even when analyzing dichotomous data has been shown by Lunney, 1970. As shown
in Table III, the main effects were consistent with the results of the non-parametric analyses. The
severity of the reported pain significantly interacted with the patient’s facial expression (F(1,75)=
78; p < .00001; η2 = .112): when the pain level was low, the respondents judged the patient’s rating
more credible if the facial expression was relaxed (mean .69) rather than if it was tense (mean .47);
by contrast, when the pain level was high, credence was higher if the face was tense (.53) rather
than if it was relaxed (.14). All these differences were significant at p < .0001. Even though the
main effect of the physical sign was not significant, the interaction between the patient’s rating and
the presence of a physical sign was significant (F(1,75) = 30.77; p < .00001; η2 = .02). This shows
that when the physical sign was absent, participants believed more in a low patient’s rating (.63)
than in a high one (.25). The difference in credibility was smaller, and in the opposite direction,
when a physical sign was present (.42 for high levels of pain vs. .53 for low levels of pain). All
these differences were significant at least at p < .01. The intensity of the reported pain significantly
interacted with the patient’s age (F(1,75) = 10.99; p < .01; η2 = .014): when the reported pain was
2 In these analyses and in all the subsequent non-parametric analyses we performed a series of Wilcoxon tests when
considering the within-participants variables, while we used the Mann-Whitney test when examining the between-
groups factors.
3 The effect size r was computed, in these analyses and in all the subsequent non-parametric analyses, as follows:
r = Z / N, where N is the total number of observations (Field, 2005).
8
high participants believed more often to a young patient rather than to an old one (.39 vs. .28). The
physical sign interacted with the gender (F(1,75) = 8.64; p < .005; η2 = .004): when there was a
physical sign, males were believed more (.51) than females (.44). The geographical distribution of
the patients’ name significantly interacted with the patient’s age (F(1,75) = 13.38; p < .001; η2 =
.029). Old Northern patients were believed more than old Southern patients (.56 vs. .35).
Moreover, a three-way interaction was found to be significant between presence of physical
sign, facial expression and gender (F(1,75) = 5.93; p < .05; η2 = .002). When the face was tense and
the physical sign was present males were believed more than females (.59 vs. .45) (p < .001).
The interval width
We then examined the interval width of the pain values deemed credible, corresponding to the
number of scale points completely blackened. Table III shows the mean credibility interval widths
and its non-parametric comparisons to the mean of inclusions of the patient’s rating in the
credibility interval. When the intensity of reported pain was high, the interval was wider than when
it was low. The same occurred when a physical sign was present vs. absent, and when the face was
tense vs. relaxed. Finally, there was a slight tendency to provide wider intervals when the patients
were old rather than young.
The patient’s rating within the credibility range
We assume that the distance between the midpoint of the credibility interval and patient’s
rating (operationalized as midpoint minus patient’s rating, considering only the credibility intervals
which included the patient’s rating) can measure the participant’s “mistrust” of the patient. If this
distance is negative the observer underestimates the patient’s rating, whereas if it is positive she
overestimates it.
.
----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
9
Insert Table IV about here
----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
As shown in Table IV, this index was negative for each level of the variables we considered.
A set of non-parametric tests showed that the participants underestimated the patient’s rating more
when the pain was high vs. low and when there was no physical sign. Also, there was a trend that
old patients’ ratings were underestimated more than young patients’ ones.
3.4. Discussion
The aim of Experiment 1 was to test the plausibility of a new dependent variable – namely an
interval estimate of the credibility of patient ratings.
Participants included the patient ratings in a range of credible values less than half of the time
(45.23%), a result in keeping with previous studies on miscalibration. The inclusion of the patient
ratings within the credibility range was influenced by the reported pain severity, the patient’s facial
expression and the presence of a physical sign as a clue. Both the p-value and the effect size
provided converging evidence that the most important role was played by the patient’s rating,
followed by facial expression and by the physical sign. The interaction between physical sign and
reported pain level shows that participants trusted more the high intensity pain reports when there
was a physical sign, in line with the findings of previous studies (e.g., Loveman & Gale, 2000).
We then used the interval width as a measure of the uncertainty of the observer. This choice
is formally justified by Shannon’s (1948) information theory, that defines uncertainty (or entropy)
as
n
i = 1
E = pi log2 pi
10
where, given a set of n alternative and mutually exclusive hypotheses, pi is the probability that each
of them is the correct one.4 It follows that uncertainty is proportional to the number of plausible
hypotheses, that, in our study, is expressed by the number of plausible pain levels indicated by the
participants – that is, the interval width.5 The interval widths showed that participants were more
uncertain when reported pain was high. Interestingly, they were more uncertain when there was a
physical sign and a tensed face.
By integrating the two analyses, it emerges that, in line with the literature, participants were
more prone to believe to low ratings (e.g., Prkachin et al., 2007) and, at the same time, to exhibit
more uncertainty about the high ratings. A reverse pattern was found concerning the physical sign
and the facial expression. When the physical sign was present, the participants tended to believe
more to high values of reported pain (again, consistently with previous studies, e.g., Chibnall et al.,
1997), but the interval width shows their cautiousness. The same occurred when the face was tense:
high ratings of pain intensity were believed more (in line with previous researches, e.g., Igier et al.,
2007), but participants were more uncertain. This ambivalence is consistent with the literature,
which reports that observers could be doubtful about the facial expression of pain, given the
possible impression that patients may fake their expression (Craig et al., 1991; Kappesser et al.,
2006). With a single, graduated measure we were able to capture the miscalibration phenomenon
and the observer’s cautiousness toward some “objective” clues.
Our third analyses – concerning the degree of mistrust toward the patient, and its direction –
revealed that patients’ pain was systematically underestimated, the more so when the reported pain
was high, when there wasn’t a physical sign and when the patients were old. These findings imply
that, even when the patient’s rating was considered credible, different degrees of trust could be
detected – and they were affected by relevant variables.
4 When the n hypotheses are equiprobable the entropy reduces to the log2(n).
5 In keeping with this reading, the literature on interval estimates indicates that a wide interval is less informative than a
narrow interval (e.g., Yaniv & Foster, 1995; Yaniv & Foster, 1997; McKenzie et al., 2008).
11
Overall the results of the Experiment 1 show that the interval measure proved capable of
capturing the main known features of the miscalibration phenomenon, and, at the same time,
provided some additional information concerning the observers’ evaluation processes. A possible
limitation of this measure is that it does not allow to rank each pain level according to its relative
plausibility – thus, it does not allow to directly establish which level is the most plausible, as with
the discrete measures. In the second experiment we addressed this issue and we compared the
results of the use of a discrete vs. a graduated approach. We also explored further properties of a
graduated measure which a discrete value cannot reveal.
4. Experiment 2
4.1. Participants
A total of 80 medical students attending courses at university (49 female, 31 male; mean age
22.4 years, range 19-35 years) volunteered to participate in Experiment 2.
4.2 Dependent variables
Participants were instructed to indicate their degree of belief regarding each of the values of
the scale. They were asked to rank them by writing “1” in the box above the point of the scale they
deemed most credible as a rating of the patient’s pain level, “2” in the box above the next-most
credible value of the scale, and so on. Participants were told that they couldn’t assign the same rank
to two or more values.
This new dependent variable provided a measure of the credibility of each scale point and
thus allowed a comparison with the discrete measures. Specifically, we could pit the discrete
perspective against the graduated approach both in the analysis of the credibility judgments given
by the observers and in the description of pain miscalibration.
First, we analyzed the credibility judgments under the discrete vs. graduated perspectives.
We considered the scale point which was rated “1” by a participant as equivalent to the discrete
12
value, whereas the graduated index was calculated as a weighted mean, specifically as the sum of
the products of the scale points by the weights assigned to them divided by the sum of the weights
(the formula and two examples of its application are provided in Appendix II).
We then used two different measures of pain miscalibration. First, we computed pain
miscalibration as the value of the scale which was rated “1” by the participant, minus the patient’s
rating. This miscalibration value is equivalent to the discrete measures used to highlight the
discrepancy between patients and observers. The graduated measure that we devised encompassed –
in a single value – the estimations made by participants on each point of the numeric rating scale as
well as the uncertainty that was intrinsic to these estimations: it consisted in the weighted mean
(which was used as a graduated measure in the previous analysis on the credibility judgments)
minus the patient’s rating, divided by the standard deviation of the weighted mean itself (see
Appendix III). Namely, it was a miscalibration measure standardized by the degree of uncertainty
expressed by the participant: sign indicates underestimation vs. overestimation of the patient’s self-
report, while the absolute value is proportional to the evaluator’s confidence that her own judgment
is more appropriate than the patient’s self-report.
Finally, we analyzed some properties which are peculiar of the graduated measure and can
reveal subtle information about the credibility attribution process.
4.3 Results and analyses
Credibility judgments
A series of non-parametric comparisons between the levels of each variable shows that the
two measures of credibility agreed in highlighting the main effects of reported pain, physical sign
and facial expression on observers’ judgments (p < .00001, r -.45). However, the graduated index
also brought out a statistically significant gender-based effect, namely the observers provided lower
ratings when the patient was female than when the patient was male. This effect was not captured
by the discrete measure (see Table V).
13
----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
Insert Table V about here
----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
Pain miscalibration
Table VI summarizes the non-parametric comparisons between the two pain miscalibration
indices.
----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
Insert Table VI about here
----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
Both the discrete and the graduated measures indicated that the effects of reported pain
intensity, physical sign and patient’s facial expression were significant (p < .00001, r = -.45),
showing that when discrete miscalibration increased, the credibility of the patient’s self-report (that
is inversely proportional to the absolute value of the graduated measure) decreased. More
interestingly, the two indices differed regarding the patient gender: while there were no significant
differences in just how much the observer underestimated the patient’s report (as shown by the
discrete measure), the graduated measure showed a trend (p = .053, r = -.16) not to trust the
patient’s judgment more for female than for male patients.
An additional property of the graduated measure
By requiring the participants to rate each level of the pain scale we could also analyze a
property that a discrete value does not allow to explore. Specifically, we summed on the one side
the weights assigned to the values of the scale higher than the scale point ranked “1” and on the
other side the weights attributed to the scale points lower than the one rated “1”.6 We then
6 These two values are analog to the surface (i.e., the integral) to the right and left of the mode of a probability
distribution; in this context, they corresponds to the overall amount of trust allocated to the right and to the left of the
most trusted value.
14
computed the ratio between these two values, leaving at the numerator the one which included the
weight assigned to the patient’s rating. Accordingly, this measure yields values > 1 if the patient’s
rating is embedded in the most-trusted tail of judgments with respect to the observer’s first-rank
evaluation, and < 1 when the opposite is true. The mean ratio was equal to 2.19 and was 1.95 for
each level of the variables included in our experimental design. This shows that, once they deemed
a value as most credible, observers tended to attribute higher weights to the values on the side of the
scale which included the patient’s rating than to the pain levels on the opposite side, with respect to
the value ranked “1”. Psychologically, this means that evaluators adopted a mostly inclusive
attitude, as opposed to an exclusive one – the latter possibly more dramatic than the former, in terms
of pain underestimation (e.g., inclusive attitude: “the patient says 7; in my opinion she suffers 5, but
not less”; exclusive attitude: “the patient says 7, in my opinion she suffers 5, and no more). A series
of Wilcoxon tests showed that the inclusive attitude was significantly more accentuated when the
physical sign was absent than when it was present and when the patient’s facial expression was
relaxed rather than when it was tense (means: no physical sign 2.66, yes physical sign 2.32, Z = -
3.995, p < .0001, r = -.44; relaxed face 2.51, tensed face 2.39, Z = -4.348, p < .0001, r = -.48).
4.4 Discussion
The aim of Experiment 2 was to compare the analyses of the credibility judgments provided
by the participants and of pain miscalibration from a graduated vs. discrete perspectives.
Furthermore, we analyzed some parameters that can be derived only by means of a graduated
measure, and that can convey useful evaluations on the psychological properties of pain judgments.
The discrete and graduated measures were in full agreement as far as the effects of pain intensity,
physical sign and facial expression were concerned; yet, the graduated measures were more
sensitive than the discrete one in detecting the effect of the patient gender, as follows:
15
1) the continuous measure of pain judgment (computed as the mean of pain levels
weighted by their ranks) was significantly lower for female than for male patients
(an effect that did not reach statistical significance for the discrete measure);
2) the standardized measure of miscalibration (weighted miscalibration / weighted
standard deviation of pain judgments) showed that – even for similar
miscalibration levels in absolute value – participants trusted female self-reports
less than male self-reports.
Finally, the graduated measure caught the tendency of participants to be inclusive, as
opposed to exclusive, with respect to the patients’ ratings. They “softened” their judgments about
the value that they deemed most credible by rating with (descending) high values the scale points
going from their first-ranked judgment to the patient’s rating. The pain levels opposite to the
patients’ ratings, with respect to the level ranked 1, were given very low-weigh ranks. This
inclusive attitude, even though it was always present, actually increased when there was no physical
sign than when it was present and when the patient’s facial expression was relaxed rather than when
it was tense, possibly showing that – lacking strong clues of pain – the participants judgments were
more anchored to the patients’ ratings (even though they widely revised them).
5. Conclusions
Starting from the pioneer work of Marks and Sachar (1973), several studies over the last
thirty-five years have shown that pain control is judged considerably inaccurate by the patients (e.g.
Yates et al., 1998; Visentin et al., 2005; Breivik et al., 2006). The ineffective management of pain
has been ascribed to a range of factors, and among them a key role has been attributed to inaccurate
assessment (e.g. McCaffery et al., 2000; Bell, 2000; Puntillo et al., 2003). In order to achieve an
effective assessment process, the adoption of formal instruments (e.g. the numeric rating scale and
the visual analogue scale) represents a necessary but insufficient step, since patients’ self-reports,
after being recorded, undergo an evaluation by healthcare professionals who may just as well accept
16
as dismiss the patient’s rating (Jacques, 1992; Waterhouse, 1996; Thorn, 1997; Clarke & Iphofen,
2008).
Commonly, the judgment of credibility has been studied in terms of disbelief. This
phenomenon has been pointed out by requiring both patients and practitioners to estimate pain
intensity based on a scale. A discrepancy of ± 1 on the numeric rating scale has generally been
considered the threshold beyond which one talks about miscalibration (Iafrati, 1986; see also
Kappesser et al., 2006).
In the present study, we set out to critically analyze such an approach to the credibility
process. The Iafrati criterion assumption might fail to fit the process of credibility attribution if the
latter is conceived not as an all-or-nothing phenomenon, but rather as a continuum. In this
perspective, knowledge of the degrees of belief that the observers attribute to different pain levels
might highlight differences in terms of cautiousness and uncertainty more than a discrete measure,
considering that pain may be defined as a “continuous latent variable” (Pesudovs & Noble, 2005) .
In order to address these issues, we tested a new measure of credibility, by requiring
participants to provide an interval of values of the numeric rating scale deemed credible
(Experiment 1). We then performed a comparison between the analyses of credibility judgments
and between the descriptions of pain miscalibration provided by this graduated measure and the
discrete one (Experiment 2). Results of Experiment 1 confirmed the tendency not to believe
patients’ accounts. The graduated measure allowed estimating the observer’s uncertainty – by
means of the confidence interval width, that turned out to be affected by “objective” variables:
namely, the intensity of reported pain, the facial expression and the presence of a physical sign.
The findings of Experiment 2 showed that a graduated measure might reveal subtle
differences in both credibility judgments and pain miscalibration which would otherwise remain
concealed when using a discrete measure. This specifically concerned the detection of a gender
effect that was revealed only by the graduated measure. Finally, the graduated measure allowed to
highlight a property of pain judgments which a discrete measure cannot detect, namely the inclusive
17
attitude of the observer’s judgment: once they have selected their most plausible pain level,
observers apportion more confidence to the part of the scale that includes the patient’s rating, as
opposed to the other one.
The present study has some limitations. First, participants evaluated vignettes describing
fictitious patients, which cannot faithfully reproduce the complexity of real clinical settings.
However, it should be noted that the vignettes are considered a valid means to assess the quality of
care provided by physicians (Peabody et al., 2000) and have been usefully adopted in several
studies which focused on the impact of the patients’ self-reports on pain judgments (e.g., Tait &
Chibnall, 1994; Chibnall & Tait, 1995; Chibnall et al., 1997; Chibnall et al., 2000; MacLeod et al.,
2001; Elander et al., 2006; Igier et al., 2007; Marquié et al., 2007; Kappesser & Williams, 2008).
Our ability to replicate many previously known results concerning credibility judgments, pain
miscalibration and the factors that may affect them supports the use of paper-and-pencil vignettes.
Moreover, the limited ecological validity of the methodology we adopted did not prevent us from
finding differences between a graduated measure and a discrete index both in the detection of the
observers’ credibility and in the description of pain miscalibration.
A further limitation of our study lies in the focus of our materials on patients coming to the
Emergency Department. This means that our findings have yet to be generalized to inpatients and
chronic pain. Furthermore, our measures should be tested on a sample of healthcare professionals in
order to confirm their validity. Finally, the value of our graduated measures should be compared to
other commonly used instruments whose outcome is discrete, such as the visual analogue scale.
Despite these limitations, this study should be considered as a preliminary attempt to
provide evidence that the research on the credibility attribution to the patients’ pain self-reports
might take advantage of the adoption of a graduated measure, for it can provide information that
other measures cannot bring out.
18
References
Bell, F. (2000). A review of the literature on the attitudes of nurses to acute pain management.
Journal of Orthopaedic Nursing, 4: 64-70.
Breivik, H. H., Collett, B. B., Ventafridda, V. V., Cohen, R. R., & Gallacher, D. D. (2006). Survey
of chronic pain in Europe: prevalence, impact on daily life, and treatment. European journal
of pain, 10(4): 287-333.
Chibnall, J. T., and Tait, R. C (1995). Observer perceptions of low back pain: Effects of pain report
and other contextual factors. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 25(5): 418-439.
Chibnall, J. T., Tait, R. C., & Ross, L. R. (1997). The Effects of Medical Evidence and Pain
Intensity on Medical Student Judgments of Chronic Pain Patients. Journal of Behavioral
Medicine, 20(3): 257-271.
Chibnall, J. T., Dabney, A., & Tait, R. C. (2000). Internist judgments of chronic low back pain.
Pain Medicine, 1(3): 231-237.
Chibnall, J. T., & Tait, R. C. (2004). Comment on Marquie L. et al. Pain rating by patients and
physicians: evidence of systematic pain miscalibration (Pain 2003;102:289-96). Pain, 107:
192-193.
Cintron, A., & Morrison, R. S. (2006). Pain and ethnicity in the United States: a systematic review.
Journal of Palliative Medicine, 9(6): 1454-1473.
Clarke, K. A., & Iphofen, R. (2005). Believing the patient with chronic pain: a review of the
literature. British Journal of Nursing, 14(9): 490-493.
Clarke, K. A., & Iphofen, R. (2008). The effects of failing to believe patients’ experience of chronic
pain. Nursing Times, 104(8): 30-31.
Craig, K. D., Hyde S. A., & Patrick C. J. (1991). Genuine, suppressed and faked facial
behavior during exacerbation of chronic low back pain. Pain, 46: 161–171.
19
Edwards R. R., Doleys D. M., Fillingim R. B. , & Lowery D. (2001a). Ethnic differences in pain
tolerance: clinical implications in a chronic pain population. Psychosomatic Medicine, 63:
316–323.
Edwards C. L., Fillingim R. B., & Keefe F. (2001b). Race, ethnicity and pain. Pain, 94: 133–137.
Elander, J., Marczewska, M., Amos, R., Thomas, A., & Tangayi, S. (2006). Factors affecting
hospital staff judgments about sickle cell disease pain. Journal of Behavioral Medicine,
29(2): 203-214.
Field, A. (2005). Discovering Statistics Using SPSS (2nd ed.). London: Sage.
Iafrati, N. S. (1986). Pain on the burn unit: Patient versus nurse perceptions. Journal of Burn Care
and Rehabilitation, 7: 413-416.
Igier, V., Mullet, E., & Sorum, P. C. (2007). How nursing personnel judge patients’ pain. European
Journal of Pain, 11(5): 542-550.
Jacques, A. (1992). Do you believe I’m in pain? Professional Nurse, 7(4): 249-251.
Loveman, E., & Gale, A. (2000). Factors influencing nurses’ inferences about patient pain. British
Journal of Nursing, 9(6): 334-337.
Lunney, G. H. (1970). Using analysis of variance with a dichotomous dependent variable: an
empirical study. Journal of Educational Measurement, 7(4): 263-269.
Kappesser, J., Williams, A. C. d. C., & Prkachin, K. (2004). What makes clinicians underestimate
pain? The Journal of Pain, 5(3, Supplement): S128.
Kappesser, J., Williams, A. C. d. C., & Prkachin, K. M. (2006). Testing two accounts of pain
underestimation. Pain, 124: 109-116.
Kappesser, J., & Williams, A. C. d. C. (2008). Pain judgements of patients’ relatives: examining the
use of social contract theory as theoretical framework. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 31:
309-317.
20
Kendrick, D. B., & Strout, T. D. (2005). The minimum clinically significant difference in patient-
assigned numeric scores for pain. The American Journal of Emergency Medicine, 23(7):
828-832.
MacLeod, F. K., LaChapelle, D. L., Hadjistavropoulos, T., & Pfeifer, J. E. (2001). The Effect of
Disability Claimants’ Coping Styles on Judgments of Pain, Disability, and Compensation: A
Vignette Study. Rehabilitation Psychology, 46(4): 417-435.
Mader, T. J., Blank, F. S. J., Smithline, H. A., & Wolfe, J. M. (2003). How Reliable Are Pain
Scores? A Pilot Study of 20 Healthy Volunteers. Journal of Emergency Nursing, 29(4): 322-
325.
Marks, R.M. & Sachar, E.J. (1973). Undertreatment of medical inpatients with narcotic analgesics.
Annals of internal medicine, 78, 173-181.
Marquié, L., Raufaste, E., Lauque, D., Mariné, C., Ecoiffier, M., & Sorum, P. (2003). Pain rating by
patients and physicians: evidence of systematic pain miscalibration. Pain, 102(3): 289-296.
Marquié, L., Raufaste, E., Lauque, D., Mariné, C., Ecoiffier, M., & Sorum, P. (2004). Further
results about pain rating by patients and physicians: reply to Chibnall and Tait. Pain 107:
194-195.
Marquié, L., Sorum, P. C., & Mullet, E. (2007). Emergency physicians’ pain judgments: cluster
analyses on scenarios of acute abdominal pain. Quality of Life Research, 16(7): 1267-1273.
McCaffery, M., Rolling Ferrell, B., & Pasero, C. (2000). Nurses’ Personal Opinions About Patients’
Pain and Their Effect on Recorded Assessments and Titration of Opioid Doses. Pain
Management Nursing, 1(3): 79-87.
McKenzie, C. R. M., Liersch, M. J., & Yaniv, I. (2008). Overconfidence in interval estimates: What
does expertise buy you? Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 107(2):
179-191.
Montali, L., Colombo, M., & Riva, P. (2009). Theories And Practices In Pain Management: A
Research On Doctors' Representations. Psicologia della Salute, 1, 33-56.
21
Peabody, J. W., Luck, J., Glassman, P., Dresselhaus, T. R., & Lee, M. (2000). Comparison of
Vignettes, Standardized Patients, and Chart Abstraction. A Prospective Validation Study of
3 Methods for Measuring Quality. The Journal of the American Medical Association,
283(13): 1715-1722.
Pesudovs, K., & Noble, B. A. (2005). Improving Subjective Scaling of Pain Using Rasch Analysis.
The Journal of Pain, 6(9): 630-636.
Prkachin, K. M., Solomon, P. E., & Ross, J. (2007). Underestimation of Pain by Health-Care
Providers: Towards a Model of the Process of Inferring Pain in Others. Canadian Journal of
Nursing Research, 39(2): 88-106.
Puntillo, K., Neighbor, M., & Nixon, R. (2003). Accuracy of Emergency Nurses in Assessment of
Patients’ Pain. Pain Management Nursing, 4(4): 171-175.
Saxey, S. (1986). The nurse’s response to postoperative pain. Nursing: The add on Journal of
Clinical Nursing, 3(10): 377-381.
Scott, I. (1992). Nurses’ attitudes to pain control and the use of pain assessment scales. British
Journal of Nursing, 2(1): 11-14.
Shannon, C. E. (1948). A mathematical theory of communication. The Bell System Technical
Journal, 27, 379-423, 623-656.
Solomon, P. (2001). Congruence between health professionals’ and patients’ pain ratings: a review
of the literature. Scandinavian Journal of Caring Sciences, 15(2): 174-180.
Strout, T. D., & Burton, J. H. (2004).Clinically significant change in physician-assigned numeric
pain rating scale scores. American Journal of Emergency Medicine, 22(3): 243-244.
Tait, R. C., and Chibnall, J. T. (1994). Observer perceptions of chronic low back pain. Journal of
Applied Social Psychology, 24(5): 415-431.
Thorn, M. (1997). A survey of nurses’ attitudes towards the assessment and control of postoperative
pain. Journal of Orthopaedic Nursing, 1: 30-38.
22
Visentin, M., Zanolin, E., Trentin, L., Sartori, S., & Marco, R. d. (2005). Prevalence and treatment
of pain in adults admitted to Italian hospitals. European Journal of Pain, 9: 61-67.
Waterhouse, M. (1996). Why pain assessment must start with believing the patient. Nursing Times,
92(38): 42-43.
Yaniv, I., & Foster, D. P. (1995). Graininess of judgment under uncertainty: an accuracy-
informativeness trade-off. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124(4): 424-432.
Yaniv, I, & Foster, D. P. (1997). Precision and accuracy of judgmental estimation. Journal of
Behavioral Decision Making, 10: 21-32.
Yates, P., Dewar, A., Edwards, H., Fetiman, B., Najman, J., Nash, R., et al. (1998). The Prevalence
and perception of pain amongst hospital in-patients. Journal of Clinical Nursing, 7: 521-
530.
23
Appendices
Appendix I
A sample vignette.
You are going to be presented with some vignettes about patients who come into the Emergency Department for a
problem on the back of the hand. Your diagnosis is that it is a wart.
During the visit, the patients also report having a headache (with this expression we do not mean a headache deriving
from injuries, cancer, ictus, etc., but a primary headache).
At the end of each vignette a pain intensity scale is reported (0 = no pain, 10 = maximum pain) on which the value
actually indicated by the patient is circled.
Experiment 1 version: Your task is to indicate, for each vignette, which pain intensity values you deem credible, which
in your view are only partly credible and which are not all credible.
In detail:
- completely blacken the boxes above the pain intensity values that you deem credible
- partially blacken the boxes above the pain intensity values that you deem only partly credible
- leave blank the boxes above the pain intensity values that you deem not credible
Experiment 2 version: Your task is to numerically rate the values of the scale, by writing “1” beside the value that you
deem most credible, “11” beside the value that you deem not credible at all, and using the intermediate numbers to
indicate your credibility towards the remaining values.
In detail, for each vignette you must:
- write “1” in the box above the value which according to you most likely coincides with the patient’s actual pain
intensity;
- write “2” in the box above the value that you judge to be second in terms of the patient’s actual pain intensity;
- write “3” in the box above the value that you judge to be third in terms of the patient’s actual pain intensity;
and so on until you get to number “11”.
Remember that you can only assign each number to each value once.
Carmen Battaglia is a 66-year-old woman. When she tells you that she has a headache you notice that her face appears
relaxed. The patient reports sensitivity to light.
When you ask her to indicate the intensity of her pain on a 0-10 scale, Carmen Battaglia rates her pain as a 7.
Experiment 1 version: Overall, which pain intensity levels would you deem credible (blacken corresponding boxes),
which would you judge only partly credible (partially blacken corresponding boxes) and which would you judge not
credible at all (leave corresponding boxes blank) for patient Carmen Battaglia?
Experiment 2 version: Numerically rate the values of the scale below, by writing “1” above the value that you deem
most credible, “11” above the value that you deem not credible at all, and using intermediate numbers to indicate your
credibility towards the remaining values. Remember that you can only assign each number to each value once.
24
Appendix II
The formula and two examples of the calculation of the graduated index used in Experiment 2 to analyze the credibility
judgments provided by the observers.
ni × ri / ri
where, ni are the scale points and ri are the weights assigned to them by the participants.
Example 1 – Index equal to 4.47
Example 2 – Index equal to 3.75
25
Appendix III
The formula of the standardized measure of pain miscalibration used in Experiment 2 and two examples of its
calculation.
[ ( ni × ri / ri ) - k ] / σ
where, ni are the scale points, ri are the weights assigned to them by the participants, k is the patient’s rating and σ is:
σ = { { [ni - ( ni × ri / ri )] 2 }× ri } / 54
Example 1 – Index equal to -1.1
Example 2 – Index equal to .34
26
Tables
Table I. Outline of the experimental design.
Variable Level 1 Level 2
patient’s rating 3 (low) 7 (high)
physical sign absent present
facial expression relaxed tensed
Within factors
patient’s gender female Male
patient’s age 25-40 (young) 65-80 (old)
Between factors geographical
distribution of
patient’s name
Southern
Northern
27
Table II. Experiment 1. Mean percentages of inclusion of the patient’s rating in each of the three credibility intervals
(in brackets the actual number of inclusions).
Non-credibility Interval Partial Credibility Interval Credibility Interval
low 16.09% (103) 26.56% (170) 57.34% (367) intensity of
reported pain high 36.09% (231) 30.78% (197) 33.13% (212)
no 28.13% (180) 28.28% (181) 43.59% (279)
physical sign yes 24.06% (154) 29.06% (186) 46.88% (300)
relaxed 31.25% (200) 27.81% (178) 40.94% (262) patient facial
expression tensed 20.94% (134) 29.53% (189) 49.53% (317)
female 25.31% (162) 30.16% (193) 44.53% (285)
patient gender male 26.88% (172) 27.19% (174) 45.94% (294)
old 28.13% (180) 27.19% (174) 44.69% (286)
patient age young 24.06% (154) 30.16% (193) 45.78% (293)
south 27.03% (173) 29.84% (191) 43.13% (276)
geographical
distribution of
patient name north 25.16% (161) 27.50% (176) 47.34% (303)
Total 26.09% (2004) 28.67% (2202) 45.23% (3474)
28
Table III. Experiment 1. Mean of inclusions of the patient’s rating in the credibility interval compared with mean
credibility interval width. We report non-parametric and parametric (ANOVA) comparisons.
Mean of
inclusion
s in the
credibilit
y interval
Z (P-
value) r F (P-
value)
Eta-
Square
(η2)
Interval
width Z (P-value) r
low 4.65* 2.72
intensity of
reported pain high 2.68*
-5.745
(<.00001) -.46 59.52
(<.00001) .077 3.02
-3.636
(< .001) -.29
no 3.53* 2.73
physical sign yes 3.80*
-1.705
(= .088) -.14 2.87
(= .095) .001 3.02
-4.501
(<.00001) -.36
relaxed 3.32* 2.68 patient facial
expression tensed 4.01*
-2.840
(< .005) -.23 8.88
(< .005) .009
3.07
-4.314
(< .0001) -.34
female 3.61* 2.89
patient gender male 3.72*
-.318
(= .751) -.03 .53
(= .469) .000 2.86
-.473
(= .636) -.04
old 7.33** 3.10
patient age young 7.33**
-.306
(= .760) -.03 .00
(= .964) .000 2.65
-1.694
(= .090) -.19
south 7.54** 2.97
geographical
distribution of
patient name north 7.14**
-.764
(= .445) -.09 .49
(= .485) .001
2.77
-.621
(= .535) -.07
* range 0-8
** range 0-16
29
Table IV. Experiment 1. The results of non-parametric analyses on the position of the patient’s rating within the
credibility interval.
Interval midpoint –
patient’s report Z (P-value) r
low - .02 intensity of
reported pain high - .74
-5.323 (< .00001) -.46
no - .37
physical sign yes - .13
-3.733 (< .001) -.31
relaxed - .26 patient facial
expression tensed - .25 -.210 (= .834) -.02
female - .25
patient gender male - .22 -.476 (= .634) -.04
old - .34
patient age young - .14 -1.902 (= .057) -.21
south - .30
geographical
distribution of
patient name north - .17 -.698 (= .485) -.08
30
Table V. Experiment 2. Comparison between the two indices of the credibility judgments according to the results of a
series of non-parametric tests.
discrete
measure* Z (P -value) r graduated
measure**
Z (P -
value) r
low 3.56 4.32 intensity of
reported pain high 4.74
-6.479
(< .00001) -.51 5.08
-7.075
(< .00001) -.56
no 3.7 4.43
physical sign yes 4.6
-5.723
(< .00001) -.45 4.97
-6.708
(< .00001) -.53
relaxed 3.44 4.25 patient facial
expression tensed 4.87
-6.817
(< .00001) -.54 5.16
-6.950
(< .00001) -.55
female 4.1 4.67
patient gender male 4.2
-1.654
(= .098) -.13 4.74
-2.161
(< .05) -.17
old 4.1 4.67
patient age young 4.2
-.525
(= .600) -.06 4.73
-.019
(= .985) .00
south 4.15 4.71 geographical
distribution of
patient name north 4.15
-.742
(= .458) -.08 4.69
-.390
(= .697) -.04
* Value of the scale which was rated “1” by the participant.
** Sum of the products of the scale points by the weights assigned to them divided by the sum of the weights.
31
32
Table VI. Experiment 2. Comparison between the two pain miscalibration indices according to the results of a series of
non-parametric tests.
discrete
measure* Z (P -value) r graduated
measure**
Z (P -
value) r
low .56 .55 intensity of
reported pain high -2.26
-7.704
(< .00001) -.61 -.84
-7.574
(< .00001) -.61
no -1.3 -.26
physical sign yes -.4
-5.723
(< .00001) -.45 -.02
-6.503
(< .00001) -.53
relaxed -1.56 -.33 patient facial
expression tensed -.13
-6.817
(< .00001) -.54 .05
-6.715
(< .00001) -.54
female -.9 -.16
patient gender male -.8
-1.654
(= .098) -.13 -.13
-1.936
(= .053) -.16
old -.9 -.15
patient age young -.8
-.525
(= .600) -.06 -.13
-.203
(= .839) -.02
south -.85 -.14 geographical
distribution of
patient name north -.85
-.742
(= .458) -.08 -.15
-.847
(= .397) -.1
* Value of the scale which was rated “1” by the participant, minus the patient’s rating.
** The weighted mean minus the patient’s rating, divided by the standard deviation of the weighted mean itself.
... This effect has also been found in a paediatric population (Bennett-Branson & Craig, 1993). In a quantitative study looking at medical student's ratings of the credibility of patient pain reports, Rusconi, Riva, Cherubini, and Montali (2010) found that women were attributed as experiencing less pain and their selfreports were not rated as being as credible as the male reports. Furthermore, qualitative accounts have shown that women's pain is more likely to be disregarded 24 or assessed as being 'not real' (Hoffmann & Tarzian, 2001;Werner & Malterud, 2003). ...
... Kappesser et al. (2006) found that when nurses watched videos of patients being treated for shoulder pain, their pain ratings were lower than patient ratings, however when medical information was provided, ratings of pain became more congruent with the patients rating. Similarly, Rusconi et al. (2010) found that the introduction of medical knowledge increased medical students pain and credibility ratings for patient-reports via video stimulus. It seems that medical evidence makes patient expression of pain more credible. ...
... This was found in both nursing and visitor conditions. Similarly, Rusconi et al., (2010) found that medical students were less certain about the credibility of patient-reports of pain, when presented with vignettes containing congruent high pain scenarios. This uncertainty about the credibility of patient-reports was not found for vignettes containing low pain cues. ...
... However, such overgeneralization could be particularly relevant in the clinical pain context. Indeed, the pain management literature indicates that pain reported by patients is often met with doubt and skepticism on the part of observers (Blomqvist & Edberg, 2002;Clarke & Iphofen, 2005;Montali et al., 2011), which might lead to an underestimation of the patient's pain (Riva, Rusconi, et al., 2011;Rusconi et al., 2010), especially when displayed by the elderly and women (Blomqvist & Edberg, 2002;Riva, Sacchi, et al., 2011), or ethnic minorities like black individuals (Banaji et al., 2021;Hoffman et al., 2016). In this context, different trust construal features will likely be at stake, ranging from a generalised trust to a more specific trust developed within the patient-carer relationship. ...
... Such a mechanism may have important implications for clinical pain practice, as previous research suggests that perceived trustworthiness does not reflect the actual trustworthiness of our interaction partners (Rule et al., 2013). Indeed, past research indicated that patients' pain reports are often met with doubt and skepticism by observers (Blomqvist & Edberg, 2002;Clarke & Iphofen, 2005;Montali et al., 2011) and that this can lead to an underestimation of pain (Riva, Rusconi, et al., 2011;Rusconi et al., 2010). ...
Article
Full-text available
Past research indicates that patients' reports of pain are often met with skepticism and that observers tend to underestimate patients' pain. The mechanisms behind these biases are not yet fully understood. One relevant domain of inquiry is the interaction between the emotional valence of a stranger's expression and the onlooker's trustworthiness judgment. The emotion overgeneralization hypothesis posits that when facial cues of valence are clear, individuals displaying negative expressions (e.g., disgust) are perceived as less trustworthy than those showing positive facial expressions (e.g., happiness). Accordingly, we hypothesized that facial expressions of pain (like disgust) would be judged more untrustworthy than facial expressions of happiness. In two separate studies, we measured trustworthiness judgments of four different facial expressions (i.e., neutral, happiness, pain, and disgust), displayed by both computer-generated and real faces, via both explicit self-reported ratings (Study 1) and implicit motor trajectories in a trustworthiness categorization task (Study 2). Ratings and categorization findings partly support our hypotheses. Our results reveal for the first time that when judging strangers' facial expressions, both negative expressions were perceived as more untrustworthy than happy expressions. They also indicate that facial expressions of pain are perceived as untrustworthy as disgust expressions, at least for computer-generated faces. These findings are relevant to the clinical setting because they highlight how overgeneralization of emotional facial expressions may subtend an early perceptual bias exerted by the patient's emotional facial cues onto the clinician's cognitive appraisal process.
... 51 This finding is similar to a study of medical students who believed the patient's pain report to be credible in only 45% of cases. 52 In another survey, 55% of pediatric nurses reported that at least 20% of children likely overreport their pain. 53 Potential reasons for discordant judgments include incongruent presentation, provider uncertainty, and provider bias. ...
... 54 Uncertainty in accepting or acting on individual self-reports is particularly increased in the presence of high pain scores and ambiguous behavioral or physical signs or symptoms. 52,54 These findings are not surprising, given that a large part of general interpersonal communication is nonverbal. ...
Article
Despite decades of research, national mandates, and widespread implementation of guidelines, recent reports suggest that the quality of pain assessment and management in hospitalized children remains suboptimal. The mismatch between what is advocated and what is done in practice has led experts to argue for a conceptual shift in thinking, where the pain assessment process is viewed from a complex social communication or transaction framework. This article examines the empirical evidence from the recent pediatric pain assessment and decision-making literature that supports adaptation of Schiavenato and Craig's "Pain Assessment as a Social Transaction" model in explaining pediatric acute pain management decisions. Multiple factors contributing to children's pain experiences and expressions are explored, and some of the difficulties interpreting their pain scores are exposed. Gaps in knowledge related to nurses' clinical pain management decisions are identified, and the importance of children's and parents' preferences and roles and the influence of risks and adverse events on decision making are identified. This review highlights the complexity of pediatric nurses' pain management decisions toward the clinical goal of improving comfort while minimizing risk. Further study evaluating the propositions related to nurses' decisions to intervene is needed in pediatric clinical settings to better synthesize this model for children.
... In addition, we explore the uncertainty inherent in evaluating these types of evidence in situ by varying the degree to which the evidence from the two sources are congruent with each other. We deploy vignettes, a methodology that has already been successfully used to study how novices and experts make judgements in conditions of uncertainty based on patients' self-reports as well as objective evidence, such as a physical symptom [49][50][51]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Despite the established evidence and theoretical advances explaining human judgments under uncertainty, developments of mobile health (mHealth) Clinical Decision Support Systems (CDSS) have not explicitly applied the psychology of decision making to the study of user needs. We report on a user needs approach to develop a prototype of a mHealth CDSS for Parkinson's disease (PD), which is theoretically grounded in the psychological literature about expert decision making and judgement under uncertainty. Methods: A suite of user needs studies was conducted in 4 European countries (Greece, Italy, Slovenia, the UK) prior to the development of PD_Manager, a mHealth-based CDSS designed for Parkinson's disease, using wireless technology. Study 1 undertook Hierarchical Task Analysis (HTA) including elicitation of user needs, cognitive demands and perceived risks/benefits (ethical considerations) associated with the proposed CDSS, through structured interviews of prescribing clinicians (N = 47). Study 2 carried out computational modelling of prescribing clinicians' (N = 12) decision strategies based on social judgment theory. Study 3 was a vignette study of prescribing clinicians' (N = 18) willingness to change treatment based on either self-reported symptoms data, devices-generated symptoms data or combinations of both. Results: Study 1 indicated that system development should move away from the traditional silos of 'motor' and 'non-motor' symptom evaluations and suggest that presenting data on symptoms according to goal-based domains would be the most beneficial approach, the most important being patients' overall Quality of Life (QoL). The computational modelling in Study 2 extrapolated different factor combinations when making judgements about different questions. Study 3 indicated that the clinicians were equally likely to change the care plan based on information about the change in the patient's condition from the patient's self-report and the wearable devices. Conclusions: Based on our approach, we could formulate the following principles of mHealth design: 1) enabling shared decision making between the clinician, patient and the carer; 2) flexibility that accounts for diagnostic and treatment variation among clinicians; 3) monitoring of information integration from multiple sources. Our approach highlighted the central importance of the patient-clinician relationship in clinical decision making and the relevance of theoretical as opposed to algorithm (technology)-based modelling of human judgment.
... In this regard, there remains a lack of empirical research on the cognitive underpinnings of pain miscalibration. 11e13, 49 The present study is an attempt to unveil a cognitive process that might intervene in pain assessment. We focused on the use of a readily accessible heuristic, namely anchoring and adjustment, whereby people rely excessively on an initial impression and interpret subsequent incoming evidence with reference to that impression. ...
Article
Full-text available
Research on decision making suggests that a wide range of spontaneous processes may influence medical judgment. We considered an easily accessible strategy, anchoring and insufficient adjustment, which might contribute to health care professionals' miscalibration of patients' pain. A sample (n=423) of physicians, nurses, medical students, and nursing students participated in a computerized task that showed 16 vignettes featuring fictitious patients reporting headache. In the experimental condition, participants were asked to evaluate the severity of the patient's pain before and after knowing the patient's rating. In the control condition, participants were shown all information about the patient at the same time and were required to make judgments in a single stage. When participants could express an initial impression before knowing the patient's rating, they fully anchored to their initial impressions in almost half of the responses. Moreover, even among those who revised their initial impression, the extent of the revision was often insufficient. Greater anchoring was associated with patients' ratings that were higher than the participants' initial impression. Finally, we provided evidence that anchoring increased pain miscalibration. We discuss our findings in terms of their contribution to the understanding of the cognitive processes involved in pain assessment. When estimating patients' pain intensity, observers are driven by anchoring, a rule of thumb that might have pernicious consequences in terms of unwarranted overreliance on initial impressions and insufficient revision in light of relevant disconfirming evidence. Taking this heuristic into account might foster accurate pain assessment and treatment.
Article
This study examined physician-generated pain dismissal experiences in adolescence between males and females. Young adults (ages 18–24, N = 178) with chronic or recurrent pain reported at least one pain dismissal experience in adolescence and answered a series of questions regarding the experience during this time period. Females were significantly more likely to report pain dismissal and a physician as the dismisser. Males were more likely to report that the dismisser expressed hostility toward them, feeling ambivalent regarding the dismissal experience, and a desire to avoid the dismisser. Females were more likely to report a desire to plead for understanding with the dismisser. Results suggest that female adolescents are more likely to report a pain dismissal experience with physicians, raising concerns that adolescent females may receive, or at least perceive, differential treatment for their chronic pain.
Article
Pain is a symptom pediatric nurses commonly encounter in the hospital setting. Untreated pain can lead to adverse physiologic and psychological effects. This study examines in-hospital pain assessment methods nurses report using and assesses challenges, difficulties, and barriers nurses report to assessing pain in hospitalized children. Cross-sectional study of 82 pediatric nurses from all pediatric departments of a tertiary hospital in Israel. A self-report questionnaire was developed to examine how nurses assess children's in-hospital pain and barriers to in-hospital pain assessment. Nearly all nurses (90%) reported having enough knowledge to assess children's pain in the hospital, relying on child's self-report (86%) and being familiar with commonly used validated pain scales (90%). However, a majority (75%) reported not using pain scales recently and only half (58%) reported using an alternative method involving the child. Most nurses (86%) reported relying on their own overall impression of the child's pain and only a third (34%) reported involving the parents in their pain assessments. Nurses included comments stressing the importance of pain assessments and their frustration with the current validated measures available. This study adds to a growing body of literature demonstrating a gap between recommended pediatric pain assessment guidelines and reported practice, with nurses showing a resistance to relying on single-item or unidimensional measures to assess and evaluate the rich and complex pain experience. A multidimensional approach involving child self-report, parent report, and nurses' own overall impression based on clinical assessment skills of pain is discussed.
Article
Full-text available
The effects of patients not being believed when they talk of their experiences of pain.
Article
Full-text available
- Pain is a symptom often associated with disease, but several studies indicate that hospital pain management continues to be inadequate. This fact seems to depend on representations of the problem and professional practices which are not likely to change. The purpose of this qualitative study, conducted in two hospitals in the north of Italy, is to analyze pain representations held by doctors, and their implications with respect to the practices adopted in assessment and treatment of pain. The results - expressed by the interviewees - indicate three meanings connected to different professional practices: pain is represented as a clinical cue, as a request for help and for attention and as a symptom to be treated. These different meanings are explained in relation to the bipolar nature of pain representation's nucleus which seems to be structured around the dialectic between subjective and objective pain. Key words: pain, pain-free hospital, doctors, social representations, qualitative research, content analysis Parole chiave: dolore, ospedale senza dolore, medici, rappresentazioni sociali, ricerca qualitativa, analisi del contenuto.
Article
Full-text available
This work concerns judgmental estimation of quantities under uncertainty. The authors suggest that the ''graininess'' or precision of uncertain judgments involves a trade-off between 2 competing objectives: accuracy and informativeness. Coarse (imprecise) judgments are less informative than finely grained judgments; however, they are likely to be more accurate. This trade-off was examined in 3 studies in which participants ranked judgmental estimates in order of preference. The patterns of preference ranking for judgments support an additive trade-off model of accuracy and informativeness. The authors suggest that this trade-off also characterizes other types of uncertain judgments, such as prediction, categorization, and diagnosis.
Article
Structured interviews of 37 medical inpatients being treated with narcotic analgesics for pain showed that 32% of the patients were continuing to experience severe distress, despite the analgesic regimen, and another 41% were in moderate distress. Chart review suggested significant undertreatment with narcotics: meperidine in doses of 50 mg every 3 to 4 hours or less (if needed) was prescribed for 63% of the 37 patients; a dose of more than 75 mg was prescribed for only 1 patient. The average amount actually received per day by the patients was 90 mg. A questionnaire survey of 102 house staff physicians in two New York teaching hospitals showed considerable misinformation about meperidine. Many physicians underestimated the effective dose range, overestimated the duration of action, and exaggerated the dangers of addiction for medical inpatients receiving meperidine in a therapeutic dosage range. Physicians who exaggerated the dangers of addiction were more likely to prescribe lower doses of drugs, even for patients with terminal malignancy. The authors suggest that such misconceptions probably lead to undertreatment with narcotic analgesics, causing much needless suffering in medical inpatients.
Article
Two important influences on pain underestimation by health care professionals were investigated by varying specific cues with reference to underestimation of patients' pain: when observers are not allowed to talk to patients and when observers expect social cheating. One hundred and twenty health care professionals watched videotaped facial expressions of pain patients and estimated their pain. The first group only saw the faces, the second group was given patients' self-reports in addition and the last group was given a context cue priming them to expect cheating in addition to faces and patients' ratings. Health care professionals generally underestimated patients' pain, but this varied depending on the cues given. Those viewing the face without patients' ratings underestimated pain to a greater extent than health care professionals provided with patients' ratings. Health care professionals primed to expect cheating underestimated pain as much as those seeing only patients' faces. Therefore, both accounts, verbal report as important but missing cue as well as an alerted cheating detection device, could account for underestimation.
Article
Facial activity was examined as 60 female and 60 male chronic low back pain patients responded to a painful range of motion exercise during a scheduled physical examination. Subsequently, they were asked to fake the facial response to the movement inducing the most pain or to attempt to suppress evidence that they were experiencing pain when this same movement was again repeated. Facial behavior was measured using the Facial Action Coding System. Self-reports of pain also were provided. The genuine expression was consistent with that observed in previous research, but minor differences indicated that the facial display of pain reflects differences between sources of pain, social context in which pain is induced and individual differences among patients. Considerable voluntary control over the facial expression of pain was observed, although the faked expression was more an intensified caricature of the genuine expression, and an attempt to suppress the facial grimace of pain was not entirely successful as residual facial activity persisted. Self-reports were only moderately correlated with facial behavior.
Article
Context: Because insurance adjusters' decisions about whether to grant compensation to disability claimants are often based on subjective criteria, such decisions may be susceptible to the influence of factors irrelevant to the claimant's disabling condition. Objective: To examine whether judgments about compensation claimants are affected by the claimants' way of coping with pain and by claimant sex. Participants: Two hundred undergraduate students. Study Design: Participants made judgments about vignettes describing fictitious compensation claimants with differing strategies of coping with pain. The main analytic approach involved a multivariate analysis of variance with 5 within-subjects factors (i.e., coping style described in each of 5 vignettes) and 2 between-subjects factors (i.e., sex of claimant described in the vignettes). Results: Coping style was significantly related to perceptions of pain severity and disability and to beliefs about whether compensation should be awarded. Claimants who were described as engaging in catastrophizing or in praying and hoping were more likely than other claimants to be perceived as disabled and as deserving compensation for their injuries. Conclusions: Decisions about disability compensation could be influenced by claimant coping styles. The subjective nature of such judgments, in combination with findings from this study, underscores the importance of creating objective guidelines for insurance adjusters.