ArticlePublisher preview availableLiterature Review

Hidden social complexity behind vocal and acoustic communication in non-avian reptiles

The Royal Society
Philosophical Transactions B
Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

Social interactions are inevitable in the lives of most animals, since most essential behaviours require interaction with conspecifics, such as mating and competing for resources. Non-avian reptiles are typically viewed as solitary animals that predominantly use their vision and olfaction to communicate with conspecifics. Nevertheless, in recent years, evidence is mounting that some reptiles can produce sounds and have the potential for acoustic communication. Reptiles that can produce sound have an additional communicative channel (in addition to visual/olfactory channels), which could suggest they have a higher communicative complexity, the evolution of which is assumed to be driven by the need of social interactions. Thus, acoustic reptiles may provide an opportunity to unveil the true social complexity of reptiles that are usually thought of as solitary. This review aims to reveal the hidden social interactions behind the use of sounds in non-avian reptiles. Our review suggests that the potential of vocal and acoustic communication and the complexity of social interactions may be underestimated in non-avian reptiles, and that acoustic reptiles may provide a great opportunity to uncover the coevolution between sociality and communication in non-avian reptiles. This article is part of the theme issue ‘The power of sound: unravelling how acoustic communication shapes group dynamics’.
Hidden social complexity behind vocal
and acoustic communication in non-
avian reptiles
Feng-Chun Lin1, Si-Min Lin2 and Stephanie S. Godfrey1
1Department of Zoology, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand
2School of Life Science, National Taiwan Normal University, Taipei, Taiwan
F-CL,0000-0003-3762-3335; S-ML,0000-0001-7080-706X; SSG,0000-0003-1014-4684
Social interactions are inevitable in the lives of most animals, since
most essential behaviours require interaction with conspecifics, such as
mating and competing for resources. Non-avian reptiles are typically
viewed as solitary animals that predominantly use their vision and
olfaction to communicate with conspecifics. Nevertheless, in recent years,
evidence is mounting that some reptiles can produce sounds and have
the potential for acoustic communication. Reptiles that can produce
sound have an additional communicative channel (in addition to visual/
olfactory channels), which could suggest they have a higher communicative
complexity, the evolution of which is assumed to be driven by the need
of social interactions. Thus, acoustic reptiles may provide an opportunity
to unveil the true social complexity of reptiles that are usually thought
of as solitary. This review aims to reveal the hidden social interactions
behind the use of sounds in non-avian reptiles. Our review suggests that
the potential of vocal and acoustic communication and the complexity
of social interactions may be underestimated in non-avian reptiles, and
that acoustic reptiles may provide a great opportunity to uncover the
coevolution between sociality and communication in non-avian reptiles.
This article is part of the theme issue ‘The power of sound: unravelling
how acoustic communication shapes group dynamics’.
1. Introduction
Non-avian reptiles have traditionally been viewed as solitary or non-social
animals [1], and thus have simple communicative needs. However, sociality
need not be seen as dichotomy (i.e. classifying animals as either social or
non-social [1]), since interactions with conspecifics are inevitable in the lives
of most animals. For example, attracting individuals of the opposite sex for
reproductive opportunities or competing for resources or territories with
conspecifics forms the foundation of a social interaction. Communication in
some form must accompany these interactions.
Sound is not normally associated with communication in non-avian
reptiles; indeed visual (e.g. body colours, body movements [2]), and olfac-
tory signals (e.g. chemicals or pheromones [3,4]) are considered to be more
common methods of communication in this group. However, recent evidence
is mounting that non-avian reptiles are not as silent as we assume [5,6].
Non-avian reptiles are capable of producing sounds, both via vocal appara-
tus (vocalization) and mechanically via the movement of their body (e.g.
producing sounds, such as hissing or snapping, or tail rattling in snakes
[7]). However, the extent to which sound communication is used in social
contexts in non-avian reptiles is poorly understood, and largely limited to a
few, well-studied taxa.
© 2024 The Author(s). Published by the Royal Society. All rights reserved.
Review
Cite this article: Lin F-C, Lin S-M, Godfrey SS.
2024 Hidden social complexity behind vocal and
acoustic communication in non-avian reptiles.
Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 379: 20230200.
https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2023.0200
Received: 19 October 2023
Accepted: 8 January 2024
One contribution of 16 to a theme issue 'The
power of sound: unravelling how acoustic
communication shapes group dynamics’.
Subject Areas:
behaviour, evolution, ecology
Keywords:
signalling, social interactions, turtles, crocodiles,
lizards, the social complexity hypothesis for
communicative complexity
Author for correspondence:
Feng-Chun Lin
e-mail: kame224339@gmail.com,
olivia.lin@postgrad.otago.ac.nz
... Thus far, we have established that reptiles can produce and use a range of vocal and non-vocal acoustic signals. Following Lin et al. [110], we suggest that these signals may be used as mediators of complex social behaviours. Exploring how these signals may vary across populations could inform us about cultural behaviours in this group. ...
Article
Full-text available
It is becoming clear that the cognition of a species plays an important role in successful conservation, with cultural processes being a fundamental part of this. However, in contrast to mammals and birds, very little is known about cultural processes (and the social learning that underlies these) in reptiles. Here, we summarize the current state of knowledge, consider why this information is so limited and assess candidate behaviours observed in the wild, which warrant further investigation through the lens of cultural traditions. We then make suggestions for the fundamental next steps necessary to start to address this issue. This includes future experimental work and also consideration of how existing datasets, such as those capturing animal movement or acoustic activity, can be used to assess cultural questions. In addition, we emphasize the important role that engaging key conservation stakeholders, such as zoos, aquaria and ecotourism providers, could play in furthering our understanding of cultural behaviour in this group and the potential conservation implications of this knowledge. Whether there is cultural behaviour in reptiles and the relationship that this has with conservation remain unclear; however, the findings of this review suggest that these are areas worthy of further research. This article is part of the theme issue ‘Animal culture: conservation in a changing world’.
... Lin et al. [48] emphasize the underestimated potential for vocal and acoustic communication in non-avian reptiles, traditionally considered solitary with reliance on vision and olfaction for interactions. Recent findings suggest that acoustic communication in these reptiles may be more common than previously thought, indicating a higher communicative complexity and more social interactions than assumed. ...
Article
Full-text available
Acoustic signalling is a key mode of communication owing to its instantaneousness and rapid turnover, its saliency and flexibility and its ability to function strategically in both short- and long-range contexts. Acoustic communication is closely intertwined with both collective behaviour and social network structure, as it can facilitate the coordination of collective decisions and behaviour, and play an important role in establishing, maintaining and modifying social relationships. These research topics have each been studied separately and represent three well-established research areas. Yet, despite the close connection of acoustic communication with collective behaviour and social networks in natural systems, only few studies have focused on their interaction. The aim of this theme issue is therefore to build a foundation for understanding how acoustic communication is linked to collective behaviour, on the one hand, and social network structure on the other, in non-human animals. Through the building of such a foundation, our hope is that new questions in new avenues of research will arise. Understanding the links between acoustic communication and social behaviour seems crucial for gaining a comprehensive understanding of sociality and social evolution. This article is part of the theme issue ‘The power of sound: unravelling how acoustic communication shapes group dynamics’.
Article
Full-text available
Animal social systems are inherently dynamic, with individuals moderating how they associate with conspecifics according to spatiotemporal shifts in population demography and resource availability. Understanding such variation is important not only to further our knowledge of a species' ecology but also to gain insights into the factors influencing the evolution of animal social systems. Using a 10-year acoustic telemetry data set containing the movements and co-occurrences of 166 tagged individuals, we investigated how time of year, individual sex and maturity status affect the social organization and connectivity of a wild population of estuarine crocodiles, Crocodylus porosus. We found that our tagged population of crocodiles displayed social structure, where individuals segregated spatially into distinct communities along 120 km of river and estuary. The social organization and structure of these com- munities were temporally dynamic, with association rates and the connectedness of individuals peaking during the dry season before disintegrating prior to the onset of the wet season. The formation of communities was found to coincide with an increase in the frequency of co-occurrence events between mature and matureeimmature dyads prior to the onset of the mating season. Together these findings indicate that estuarine crocodiles have a structured social system, where the proximity to the mating season and an individual's maturity status dictate how they associate with conspecifics.
Article
Full-text available
Simple Summary Vocalization is important for the survival of underwater animals. Pelodiscus sinensis spends most of its life in water, and its wild population is highly vulnerable. However, its underwater vocalization, which can serve as the basis for ecological and evolutionary research, has not been explored. We identified 10 call types of the species from underwater recordings and found differences in vocalizations between individuals of different ages and sexes. The Chinese soft-shelled turtle has a high diversity of vocalizations and shows a tendency for vocalization to become more diverse with age. These features help them to adapt to complex and dark underwater environments. Abstract Sound communication is important for underwater species. The wild population of the Chinese soft-shelled turtle (Pelodiscus sinensis) is listed as vulnerable. However, its vocalization, which can serve as the basis for ecological and evolutionary research, has not been studied. Here, we performed underwater recordings of 23 Chinese soft-shelled turtles of different ages and sexes and identified 720 underwater calls. The turtle calls were manually divided into 10 call types according to visual and aural inspection properties. The similarity test indicated that the manual division was reliable. We described the acoustic properties of the calls and the statistical analysis showed that the peak frequency of calls was significantly different between adult females and males, and also between subadults and adults. Similar to other aquatic turtles that prefer to live in deep water, Chinese soft-shelled turtles have a high vocal diversity and many harmonic calls, indicating that this highly aquatic species developed a variety of vocalizations to enhance their underwater communication, which helped them adapt to the complex and dim underwater environment. Furthermore, the turtles showed a tendency for vocalization to become more diverse with age.
Article
Full-text available
Escaping from predation saves life, but costs energy and time. The "threat-sensitive predator-avoidance" hypothesis proposes that prey may modulate their antipredator responses, and thus the associated costs, in accordance with the magnitude of predation risk. This process requires that prey accurately assess this risk by decoding available information from various sources. For example, distress calls are uttered by prey when a predator traps them and can serve as public information on predation risk. Such is the case for the weeping lizard whose distress calls trigger immobility in conspecifics. Here, we tested whether this antipredator response of the weeping lizard is modulated by witnessing predation. We exposed lizards to distress calls alone or paired with models of a prey (conspecific), a predator (snake), or a predatory event (a snake sub-jugating the conspecific). Data show that the sole presence of the predator or prey paired with distress calls seems not to modulate the antipredator responses. Contrarily, witnessing a predatory event associated with calls intensified antipredator responses; lizards reduced their activity for longer and avoided proximity to the stimuli, which may decrease predation risk by reducing the likelihood of being detected by the predator. We conclude that the weeping lizard can use multisensorial public information to assess predation risk and modulate its antipredator responses.
Article
Full-text available
Background Turtle vocalizations play an important role throughout their lives by expressing individual information (position, emotion, or physiological status), reflecting mating preferences, and synchronizing incubation. The Chinese striped-neck turtle ( Mauremys sinensis ) is one of the most widely distributed freshwater turtles in China, whose wild population is critically endangered. However, its vocalization has not been studied, which can be the basis for behavioral and ecological studies. Methods Five different sex–age groups of turtles were recorded underwater in a soundproof room. Cluster analysis and principal component analysis for classification of Chinese striped-neck turtle calls were unreasonable. The turtle calls were manually sought using visual and aural inspection of the recordings in Raven Pro 1.5 software and classified according to differences perceived through auditory inspection and the morphological characteristics of the spectrograms. The results of similarity analysis verified the reliability of manual classification. We compared the peak frequency of the calls among different age and sex groups. Results We identified ten M. sinensis call types, displayed their spectra and waveforms, and described their auditory characteristics. Most calls produced by the turtles were low-frequency. Some high-frequency call types, that are common in other turtle species were also produced. Similar to other turtles, the Chinese striped-neck turtle generates harmonic vocalizations. Courtship behaviors were observed when one of the call types occurred in the mixed-sex group. Adult females produced more high-frequency call types, and subadult males had higher vocalizations than other groups. These results provide a basis for future research on the function of vocalizations, field monitoring, and conservation of this species.
Article
Full-text available
The aim of this study was to identify underwater vocalizations in red-eared turtles (Trachemys scripta elegans) and assess differences between sexes and ages. We recorded the underwater vocalizations of the red-eared sliders and identified 12 call types through manual visual and aural inspection of the recordings. Similarity analysis verified that manual classification was relatively reliable. The call types of the turtle were described and displayed as spectrograms and waveforms. The turtles produced fewer high-frequency call types than low-frequency types in all recordings. Statistical analysis revealed significant differences in the frequencies and duration of the calls of red-eared turtles between different sexes and ages. Males vocalized pulse calls very frequently, whereas a high proportion of high-frequency call types was emitted by the female adult group. The male subadult group emitted higher frequencies of Type A, B, and C calls, which is in accordance with the phenomenon that vocal frequency is often inversely proportional to the turtle size. Some call types produced by red-eared turtles were above the frequency range of their known hearing range. This may have been a by-product of the sound production mechanism or it may have adaptive value in mitigating interference to communication from low-frequency noise common in natural waters in communication The behavioral implications of these vocalizations and whether turtles can hear such high sounds warrant further study.
Article
Full-text available
Acoustic communication, broadly distributed along the vertebrate phylogeny, plays a fundamental role in parental care, mate attraction and various other behaviours. Despite its importance, comparatively less is known about the evolutionary roots of acoustic communication. Phylogenetic comparative analyses can provide insights into the deep time evolutionary origin of acoustic communication, but they are often plagued by missing data from key species. Here we present evidence for 53 species of four major clades (turtles, tuatara, caecilian and lungfish) in the form of vocal recordings and contextual behavioural information accompanying sound production. This and a broad literature-based dataset evidence acoustic abilities in several groups previously considered non-vocal. Critically, phylogenetic analyses encompassing 1800 species of choanate vertebrates reconstructs acoustic communication as a homologous trait, and suggests that it is at least as old as the last common ancestor of all choanate vertebrates, that lived approx. 407 million years before present.
Article
Full-text available
Ambient noise is a major constraint on acoustic communication in both animals and humans. One mechanism to overcome this problem is Spatial Release from Masking (SRM), the ability to distinguish a target sound signal from masking noise when both sources are spatially separated. SRM is well described in humans but has been poorly explored in animals. Although laboratory tests with trained individuals have suggested that SRM may be a widespread ability in vertebrates, it may play a limited role in natural environments. Here we combine field experiments with investigations in captivity to test whether crocodilians experience SRM. We show that 2 species of crocodilians are able to use SRM in their natural habitat and that it quickly becomes effective for small angles between the target signal source and the noise source, becoming maximal when the angle exceeds 15 ∘ . Crocodiles can therefore take advantage of SRM to improve sound scene analysis and the detection of biologically relevant signals.
Article
Acoustic techniques are rapidly becoming powerful tools for species monitoring and biodiversity assessment. These methods can be particularly appropriate for forest-dwelling crocodiles which are difficult to survey visually. However, basic vocal-repertoire data is lacking for many of the poorly known species. Here, we used passive acoustic recorders to capture 97 spontaneous vocal signals from a pair of captive adult African dwarf crocodiles (Osteolaemus tetraspis). We catalogued their acoustic repertoire and compared the calls recorded in captivity with 201 suspected wild O. tetraspis calls recorded in Gabon to determine whether the wild calls belonged to the same species. Captive and wild crocodiles produced the same four types of calls, not previously identified in other crocodylids. Short, low-frequency “drums” (31±12 Hz), longer, low-frequency “rumbles” (40 ± 14 Hz), as well as higher frequency “moos” (299 ± 133 Hz) and “gusts” (219 ± 108 Hz). Our results provide reference for species identification and support implementation of acoustic-based methods for African dwarf crocodile monitoring and conservation assessment. The data can further contribute to landscape-wide biodiversity monitoring and counter-poaching activities, as well as improving our understanding of crocodilian ecology and behaviour.