Content uploaded by Waqas Ejaz
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Waqas Ejaz on May 21, 2024
Content may be subject to copyright.
https://doi.org/10.1177/14614448241250302
new media & society
1 –20
© The Author(s) 2024
Article reuse guidelines:
sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/14614448241250302
journals.sagepub.com/home/nms
Trust is key: Determinants
of false beliefs about climate
change in eight countries
Waqas Ejaz
University of Oxford, UK
Sacha Altay
University of Oxford, UK; University of Zurich, Switzerland
Richard Fletcher
University of Oxford, UK
Rasmus Kleis Nielsen
University of Oxford, UK
Abstract
Science has established the human-caused nature of climate change, yet the prevalence of
climate-related misinformation persists, undermining public understanding and impeding
collective action. Strikingly, existing research on belief in misinformation about climate
change has disproportionately focused on WEIRD (Western, Educated, Industrialized,
Rich, and Democratic) countries. To move beyond this, our online survey (N = 8541)
includes high-income countries in North America (US), Western Europe (France,
Germany, UK) and East Asia (Japan), as well as an upper-middle income country in South
America (Brazil) and lower-middle income countries in South Asia (India and Pakistan).
By examining the interplay of news media usage, information sources, and trust in these
sources, we advance our understanding of how these factors influence belief in climate
change-related misinformation in diverse socio-cultural contexts. Across countries,
we found that the strongest determinants of belief in misinformation about climate
change were identifying as right-wing (compared with left-wing), consuming less offline
news, having less trust in scientists, environmental activists, as well as international
Corresponding author:
Waqas Ejaz, Reuters Institute for the Study of Journalism, University of Oxford, 13 Norham Gardens
OX2 6PS, Oxford, UK.
Email: waqas.ejaz@politics.ox.ac.uk
1250302NMS0010.1177/14614448241250302new media & society</italic>Ejaz et al.
research-article2024
Article
2 new media & society 00(0)
organizations, and having more trust in politicians, celebrities, and energy companies.
Overall, trust in sources of information about climate change and demographic variables
were much stronger predictors of belief in misinformation about climate change than
reported news consumption (online, offline or on social media). These findings suggest
that trust is key to understanding belief in false information about climate change.
Keywords
Climate change, global south, media trust, misinformation, news media
Introduction
Despite the long-established scientific consensus that human activities are the primary
drivers of climate change, conspiracy theories, contrarians’ viewpoints, and misinforma-
tion about its origins, impacts, and solutions continue to propagate in the contemporary
information environment (Biddlestone et al., 2022; Cook, 2020; van der Linden et al.,
2017). Exposure to and belief in such misinformation, especially in Anglophone countries,
continues to confuse the public, limiting support for mitigation policies (Cook et al., 2018;
Zhou and Shen, 2021), preventing the required collective action (van der Linden et al.,
2017), and reducing public trust in scientific consensus (Painter et al., 2023).
Past research highlights several factors, such as political identities (Walter and Murphy,
2018), ideological biases (Newman et al., 2018), and cultural worldviews (Kahan et al.,
2011), that contribute to false beliefs about climate change. However, besides these indi-
vidual factors, while at their best, news media can help provide accurate and reliable infor-
mation, news media have also been identified as a key external factor which – sometimes
intentionally, sometimes unintentionally – helps amplify and propagate non-scientific
claims by climate denialists as well as vested interest groups (Cook, 2020; Petersen et al.,
2019; Supran and Oreskes, 2017; van der Linden et al., 2017).
Coan et al. (2021) classified the arguments frequently used by contrarians as follows:
(1) global warming does not occur, (2) humans are not the cause, (3) the impacts are not
bad, (4) solutions will not work and (5) climate science is unreliable. Since these claims
contradict mainstream climate science (Cook, 2020), they fall under the definition of mis-
information proposed by Vraga and Bode (2020), which refers to ‘information considered
incorrect based on the best available evidence from relevant experts at the time’ (p. 138).
Accordingly, we use average agreement with these claims as a scale to measure false
beliefs about climate change, understood here as belief in climate misinformation.
Research has highlighted that the news media serves as a primary source of informa-
tion for the public on climate change (Newman et al., 2020; Schäfer and Painter, 2020).
Across the countries where data are available, television remains the single most impor-
tant medium, whereas online media including news websites, social media platforms,
and messaging apps hold equal prominence (Ejaz et al., 2022). Mainstream news media
can influence public policies on climate change, enhance people’s understanding of its
impacts, and often seeks to inform, educate, and engage the public on climate change
(Boykoff, 2011; Cheng and Gonzalez-Ramirez, 2020; Comfort et al., 2020; Ejaz et al.,
2022).
Ejaz et al. 3
Although the positive effects of news use on political knowledge and climate change-
related attitudes are well documented, albeit mostly in the United States and Western
European countries, the relationship between news use and climate change misinforma-
tion is less clear, particularly beyond Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, and
Democratic (WEIRD) countries. For instance, fossil fuel companies have used the news
media to raise doubts about the adverse effects of climate change by undermining the
scientific consensus and spreading misinformation (Supran and Oreskes, 2017).
Likewise, content analysis of conservative media in the United States and Europe con-
firms that contrarians feature more prominently in media coverage of climate change
relative to expert scientists (Bohr, 2020; Dunlap and Brulle, 2020; Krange et al., 2021;
Petersen et al., 2019; Vowles and Hultman, 2021). In addition, it has been argued that
people may be primarily exposed to misinformation by the news media (Tsfati et al.,
2020), and that by reporting or debunking misinformation, news media risks amplifying
it. However, research has found that exposure to fact checks reduces people’s belief in
misinformation (Porter and Wood, 2021), and there is some evidence to suggest that
news use lowers the acquisition of false beliefs over time (Altay et al., 2023).
The scientific literature on climate news consumption and misinformation about cli-
mate change is sparse and has focused on the unusual case of the United States, leaving
many of the countries most affected by climate change underrepresented. Therefore, to
fill this gap and respond to growing calls to include more non-WEIRD countries (see, for
example, Cologna and Siegrist, 2020; Schäfer and Painter, 2020), we conducted an
online survey between August and September 2022 in eight countries: Brazil, France,
Germany, India, Japan, Pakistan, the United Kingdom and the United States. We col-
lected data on people’s beliefs in five different examples of climate change-related mis-
information: global warming is not happening, greenhouse gases are not causing global
warming, actions being taken to prevent climate change won’t work, climate change will
not have a negative impact on my country, and climate change will not have a negative
impact on the world (Coan et al., 2021). This scale takes into account the most recent
evolution of climate change denialism, and includes new elements that have been added
over time, such as the scepticism over solutions to mitigate climate change (Painter et al.,
2023) and the notion that the impact of climate change is exaggerated (Capstick and
Pidgeon, 2014; Whitmarsh, 2011). We also measured respondent’s use of offline and
online platforms, trust in climate news, and recalling their respective sources.
The countries were selected because we were interested in comparing countries – like
France, Germany, Japan, the United Kingdom and the United States – where patterns of
news use and misperceptions about climate change are relatively well mapped out com-
pared with those – like Brazil, India and Pakistan – where we know very little about news
media use and misperceptions about climate change. Furthermore, these countries are
also interesting to study because of their diverse histories of scepticism, climate vulner-
ability and policy.
For example, the United Kingdom and the United States are two Anglophone coun-
tries where some forms of climate scepticism have been consistently prevalent in the
media (Boykoff and Farell, 2019; Painter and Gavin, 2015), in contrast to Germany,
France and Japan (Adam et al., 2020; Asayama and Ishii, 2014; Painter and Ashe,
2012). Brazil was chosen because it has traditionally expressed greater concern for
4 new media & society 00(0)
misinformation (Newman et al., 2020); however, relatively little is known about cli-
mate misinformation, specifically with President Jair Bolsonaro, a climate change
sceptic, in power until recently (Ferrante and Fearnside, 2019). India combines high
trust in climate science (Whiting, 2023) with news media portraying nationalistic atti-
tudes towards climate change, resisting global mitigation policies by framing them
anti-nationalist (Billet, 2009). Finally, belief in misinformation is high in Pakistan, it
is one of the only two countries left in the world where polio is still endemic, partly due
to widespread misperceptions about the polio vaccine (Ejaz et al., 2021; Ittefaq et al.,
2021). Moreover, both India and Pakistan, which collectively account for more than
20% of the world’s total population, are often ranked highly vulnerable to the negative
consequences of climate change (Eckstein et al., 2019), making them especially rele-
vant countries to study.
Accordingly, we investigated the following determinants of false beliefs about cli-
mate change across countries: (1) socio-demographic variables such as age, gender and
political orientation, (2) the types of media used to consume information about climate
change (offline, online and social media), (3) reported exposure to specific sources of
information about climate change, and (4) trust in specific sources of information about
climate change. In the section below, we briefly review past literature and introduce our
research questions. We note that given the lack of empirical research on most of these
questions outside of WEIRD countries (and the United States in particular), we formu-
lated research questions instead of hypotheses.
Review of the literature and research questions
First, we investigated the determinants of attitudes towards climate change across coun-
tries. Previous work found that women, more educated and younger participants were
more accepting of the scientific consensus on anthropogenic climate change (Hornsey
et al., 2016). Overall, demographic variables tended to have much smaller effects than
trust or political ideology – with people identifying as right-wing having lower belief in
climate change (Hornsey et al., 2016). Yet, most of these studies were conducted in very
affluent countries, and it has been documented that some demographic effects, such as
the positive effects of education, tend to be stronger in less affluent countries. Moreover,
the issue of climate change is less prone to political instrumentalization in less affluent
countries (Czarnek et al., 2021) and in non-anglophone countries (Smith and Mayer,
2019). Similarly, the right-left wing continuum is a weaker predictor of attitudes towards
climate change in Central or Eastern Europe than Western Europe (McCright et al., 2016;
Poortinga et al., 2019).
RQ1. What socio-demographic variables are associated with false beliefs about cli-
mate change?
Second, we investigated the types of media use associated with false beliefs about cli-
mate change. A survey conducted in 20 countries found that using social media for news
was associated with less climate scepticism, but there was significant heterogeneity
Ejaz et al. 5
across countries (Diehl et al., 2021). In countries with higher-than-average levels of cli-
mate scepticism (e.g. Russia, New Zealand) using social media for news was associated
with less climate scepticism, while in other countries using social media for news was
associated with more climate scepticism (e.g. South Korea, Germany). More broadly, a
lot of correlational evidence points to the idea that social media use is associated with
higher belief in misinformation or higher belief in conspiracy theories (Uscinski et al.,
2022).
Across countries, broadcast news use is associated with less climate scepticism (Diehl
et al., 2021). But the positive effects of news use may differ by medium. For instance, a
survey in Portugal showed that news use was associated with higher level of concern
about climate change, and that consumers of public TV channels were more knowledge-
able about climate change than consumers of commercial TV channels (Cabecinhas
et al., 2008). Similarly, a survey in Germany found that public TV news use was associ-
ated with more awareness of climate problems, while print media news was associated
with less awareness (Arlt et al., 2011).
Other types of evidence suggest that the news may help raise awareness and inform
people about climate change. For instance, longitudinal studies in the United States have
shown that greater coverage of climate change was associated with greater levels of
public concern about climate change (Carmichael and Brulle, 2017). These positive
effects may be weaker in some countries than others. For example, an analysis of print
media in the United States, Brazil, China, France, India and the United Kingdom,
between 2009 and 2010 found that climate change sceptic opinions were mostly voiced
in the United States and the United Kingdom (Painter and Ashe, 2012).
RQ2. What types of media use are associated with false beliefs about climate
change?
Third, we investigated the effect of information sources (e.g. scientists or celebrities) on
false beliefs about climate change. This is particularly relevant given that in recent years
the issue of climate change is being increasingly discussed by laypeople, politicians, or
even celebrities (Anderson, 2011). Overall, paying attention to science and environmen-
tal news is associated with more knowledge about climate change and its effects (Zhao
et al., 2011). However, watching Fox News in the United States has been shown to
increase familiarity with some climate change policies, such as the Green New Deal, but
also increase opposition to it (Gustafson et al., 2019).
RQ3. Which information sources are associated with false beliefs about climate
change?
Across the world, scientists remain the most trusted source of information about climate
change (Brewer and Ley, 2013). Trust in scientists is associated with more correct beliefs
about climate change, such as increased certainty that climate change is happening
(Arbuckle et al., 2015). A meta-analysis found that trust in scientists is one of the strong-
est predictors of acceptance of climate change (Hornsey et al., 2016). Across 20
6 new media & society 00(0)
countries higher trust in science is associated with less climate scepticism (Diehl et al.,
2021). Another meta-analysis of 141 correlations from 51 studies found that trust in sci-
entists and trust in environmental groups were the forms of trust the most strongly asso-
ciated with climate-friendly behaviours (Cologna and Siegrist, 2020). While general
trust and trust in institutions to provide relevant information were only weakly correlated
with climate-friendly behaviour, trust in industry was not significantly correlated with
climate-friendly behaviour. Yet, studies looking specifically at trust in fossil fuel indus-
tries found negative correlations with climate change mitigation behaviours (Dietz et al.,
2007; Leonard and Leviston, 2017).
Most work on trust and climate change attitudes and behaviours has been conducted
in North America and Western Europe, and recent meta-analyses suggest that ‘Future
efforts should be dedicated to disentangling trust-behaviour relationships in Eastern
Europe, as well as Asia, Africa and South America’ (Cologna and Siegrist, 2020).
RQ4. How is trust in different information sources associated with false beliefs about
climate change?
Data
Ipsos was commissioned to survey 8541 online respondents across Brazil, France,
Germany, India, Japan, Pakistan, the United Kingdom and the United States. Data were
collected between the 26th of August and 21st September 2022 as part of a the Oxford
Climate Journalism Network (OCJN) project at the Reuters Institute for the Study of
Journalism at the University of Oxford.
Ipsos applied quotas for age, gender and region to match the national population.
However, because questionnaires were fielded online, our samples can only be repre-
sentative of the online population. In France, Germany, Japan, the United Kingdom and
the United States, where Internet penetration is high, this is unlikely to have a large
impact on the results. However, in India and Pakistan, and to a lesser extent Brazil, where
Internet penetration is lower, there are relatively large differences between the online
population and the national population. In addition, because the survey was fielded in
English in Pakistan and India, the data from these countries are representative of younger
English speakers and not the national population because it is not possible to reach other
groups in a representative way using an online survey. In India and Pakistan, the data are,
at best, representative of the online, English-speaking populations (see Appendix I for
distribution of the sample).
Measures
Dependent variables
Our measure of holding false beliefs about climate change draws on the work of Coan
et al. (2021). They analysed large corpora of contrarian claims about climate change and
identified five ‘super-claims’ representative of climate change contrarianism.
Ejaz et al. 7
Accordingly, participants were asked the extent to which they agreed (‘strongly disagree’
[1], ‘tend to disagree’ [2], ‘neither agree nor disagree’ [3], ‘tend to agree’ [4] and ‘strongly
agree’ [5]) with these claims: (1) global warming is not happening (M = 2.19, SD = 1.37),
(2) greenhouse gases are not causing global warming (M = 2.50, SD = 1.37), (3) actions
being taken to prevent climate change won’t work (M = 3.12, SD = 1.20), (4) climate
change will not have a negative impact on my country (M = 2.30, SD = 1.38) and (5) cli-
mate change will not have a negative impact on the world (M = 2.22, SD = 1.38). Given
that all these claims are contrary to scientific evidence, we take agreement with them as
evidence of holding false beliefs about climate change. We averaged agreement across
these five claims to build the false beliefs about climate change scale (α = .88, M = 2.46,
SD = 1.11) (Table 1).
Independent variables
We measured self-reported exposure to different types of climate change news (e.g.
offline media, social media), self-reported exposure to different sources of information
in climate change news (e.g. scientists, celebrities), and trust in different sources of
information.
The media through which people consume climate change news was measured with
the following question: Thinking specifically about the news or information about cli-
mate change you saw, read or heard within the last week. Where did you see, read or hear
this? Respondents chose between: Television news, Radio news, Printed newspapers and
news magazines, Websites/apps of newspapers and news magazines, Websites/apps of
TV or radio news companies, Websites/apps of other news outlets, social media (e.g.
Facebook, Twitter, Instagram, TikTok, etc.). We divided media use in three categories:
offline (TV, radio and print newspaper), online (websites of newspaper, TV/radio and
digital news outlets) and social media. For descriptive information on the use of these
media across countries, see Appendix II and III in supplementary information.
Table 1. Sample sizes, Internet penetration and mean false beliefs about climate change by
country.
Country NInternet
penetrationa (%)
Percentage of agreement with
false claimsb (%)
UK 1126 95 23
US 1209 91 28
France 1100 85 18
Germany 1100 90 23
Japan 1000 90 12
Brazil 1000 81 25
India 1006 43 47
Pakistan 1000 25 24
aSource: International Telecommunication Union (ITU) World Telecommunication/ICT Indicators Database.
bAverage percentage of agreement across five false statements (Tend to agree and strongly agree).
8 new media & society 00(0)
The sources of climate change information were measured with the following questions
(a separate question for each news medium responents said they had used): Aside from any
journalists, reporters, presenters, etc. that may have delivered the news or information,
which types of organisations or individuals do you recall commenting or being mentioned
as the source on Television news, Radio news, Printed newspapers and news magazines,
Websites/apps of newspapers and news magazines, Websites/apps of TV or radio news com-
panies, Websites/apps of other news outlets, and social media? Participants chose between:
Politicians or political parties, Scientists, Environmental activists, Celebrities, The
Government, Official international institutions, Charities, and Energy companies. We aver-
aged responses for each source across types of medium (i.e., offline, online, and social). For
instance, scientists as a source of information about climate change have three scores: scien-
tists on offline media (average of TV, radio and print), scientists on online media (average of
websites, TV/radio, and digital news outlets) and scientists on social media. Appendix IV
and V show the distribution of respondents recalling respective information sources across
different platforms.
We measured trust in sources of information about climate change by asking partici-
pants about the extent to which they generally trust or distrust each source mentioned
above on a scale from ‘strongly distrust’ [1] to ‘strongly trust’ [5]: Politicians or political
parties (M = 2.61, SD = 1.26), Scientists (M = 3.84, SD = 1.10), Environmental activists
(M = 3.32, SD = 1.28), Celebrities (M = 2.70, SD = 1.23), The government (M = 2.98,
SD = 1.29), Official international institutions (M = 3.44, SD = 1.20), Charities (M = 3.25,
SD = 1.13) and Energy companies (M = 2.94, SD = 1.24). Following previous studies (e.g.
Hobolt and Tilley, 2018; Mattes and Bratton, 2007), ‘don’t know’ responses were recoded
as ‘neither trust nor distrust’ [3] and ‘prefer not to say’ responses were excluded from our
analysis to ensure analytical robustness and minimize data loss. To view the distribution of
trust for each climate information source across countries, please refer to Appendix VI in
the supplementary materials.
We measured trust in the news about climate with the following question: would you
generally trust or distrust news media (organization delivering news to public via radio,
TV, newspapers, or online) as a source of news or information about climate change.
Respondents indicated their level of trust in news media (M = 3.28, SD = 1.17) on a scale
from ‘Strongly distrust’ [1] to ‘Strongly trust’ [5].
Last, we measured socio-demographic variables including age (M = 41.51, SD = 14.97),
gender (49.1% male), education, and political ideology. Education was treated as a cat-
egorical variable: bachelor/higher degree (42%) versus no bachelor/higher degree (58%)
to standardize the data across countries. Political ideology was measured on a 7-point left
to right scale, which we categorized into left (collapsing values from 1 to 3 – 23.3%),
right (collapsing values from 5 to 7 – 29.1%) and centre (value 4, accounting for 28% of
responses). Responses were recoded in this way to remove the effect of different response
styles in different countries. As on average, 9.3% selected ‘don’t know’ and 10.3% ‘pre-
fer not to say’, we included these categories to prevent exclusion of cases.
Results
First, we investigated the socio-demographic variables associated with false beliefs
about climate change (RQ1) in Model 1 of Table 2. We found that men (β = .06, p < .001),
Ejaz et al. 9
younger people (β = .13, p < .001), participants who identified as right-wing (β = .24,
p < .001), and those who either identified as being in the centre (β = .05, p < .001) or
preferred not to answer (β = .05, p < .001) were more likely to agree with climate change-
related misinformation. These effects persist across all models, notwithstanding observed
variation between different countries. Education was not significantly associated with
misperceptions about climate change, except in Model 4, where a positive but weak
association was observed between individuals with a university degree and belief in
misinformation (β = .03, p = .003).
Second, we built on the previous model and investigated the effect of media use for
climate change news on false beliefs about climate change (RQ2, Model 2). We found
that the use of offline news media platforms (TV, radio and print newspaper) (β = −.15,
p < .001) and social media (β = −.05, p < .001) was associated with lower false beliefs
about climate change. We did not find any significant association between online news
media use and false beliefss about climate change (β = −.01, p = .432). The effect of
offline news use is consistent across countries (Figure 1) and holds when controlling for
trust in information sources (Model 3 and 4), while the effect of social media is inconsist-
ent across models and countries, as, for instance, social media use is no longer statisti-
cally significant after controlling for trust (Model 4). The addition of media use in the
model explained an additional 3pp of the variance compared to Model 1 containing only
demographics and countries (from 14% to 17%).
Third, building on previous models, we examined the role of exposure to specific
sources of information about climate change on holding false beliefs about climate change
(RQ3, Model 3). We found that respondents who recalled being exposed to climate change
information from scientists on offline media (β = −.04, p = .006), online media (β = −.08,
p < .001), and social media (β = −.03, p = .015) showed lower levels of false belief.
Similarly, respondents who recalled exposure to climate change information from environ-
mental activists on social media (β = −.04, p = .007) were less likely to hold false beliefs. In
contrast, respondents who recalled being exposed to climate change information from
celebrities on offline (β = .07, p < .001) and online media (β = .05, p < .001), international
organizations on social media (β = .03, p = .025), and energy companies on offline media
(β = .03, p = .047) and online media (β = .03, p = .047), were more likely to hold false beliefs
about climate change. These effects weaken, or are no longer statistically significant, in
subsequent models that include trust in specific sources of information about climate
change (Model 4). The addition of exposure to specific sources of information in the model
only explained an additional 1pp of the variance compared to Model 2 (from 17% to 18%),
suggesting that reported exposure to specific sources of information about climate change
explains little to no variance in false beliefs about climate change.
Fourth, we examined the role of trust in different sources of information on false
beliefs about climate change (RQ4, Model 4). We found that higher trust in scientists
(β = −.21, p < .001), environmental activists (β = −.10, p < .001), international organiza-
tions (β = −.12, p < .001), and the news media, was associated with lower false beliefs
about climate change. On the other hand, trust in politicians (β = .18, p < .001), celebri-
ties (β = .18, p < .001), and energy companies (β = .18, p < .001) were associated with
stronger false beleifs. Trust in government (β = −.01, p = .55) and charities (β = −.01,
p = .39) had no statistically significant effect. The lack of significance for trust in
10 new media & society 00(0)
Table 2. Hierarchical linear regression predicting false beliefs about climate change.
Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4
Constant 2.776*** 2.794*** 2.804*** 3.192***
Block 1: Demographics
Gender [female] −0.138*** −0.155*** −0.167*** −0.135***
Age −0.010*** −0.008*** −0.007*** −0.004***
Education [no degree] −0.029 −0.003 0.009 0.061**
Political ideology [left]
Right 0.594*** 0.567*** 0.536*** 0.340***
Centre 0.116*** 0.104** 0.084** −0.027
Don’t know 0.055 0.014 −0.011 −0.116**
Prefer not to say 0.196*** 0.130** 0.111** −0.053
Block 2: Media use
Offline −0.629*** −0.688*** −0.485***
Online −0.042 −0.007 0.105
Social media −0.163*** −0.139** −0.056
Block 3: Information sources on different types of media
Politicians on offline media 0.014 0.005
Politicians on online media 0.019 −0.003
Politicians on social media 0.049 0.027
Scientists on offline media −0.084** −0.027
Scientists on online media −0.191*** −0.109**
Scientists on social media −0.155* −0.101+
Environmental activists on offline media 0.034 0.03
Environmental activists on online media −0.008 0.007
Environmental activists on social media −0.162** −0.109+
Celebrities on offline media 0.224*** 0.140***
Celebrities on online media 0.154*** 0.076+
Celebrities on social media −0.041 −0.04
Government on offline media −0.033 −0.053+
Government on online media −0.06 −0.052
Government on social media 0.095 0.012
International organizations on offline media −0.011 0.003
International organizations on online media 0.015 0.008
International organizations on social media 0.155* 0.131*
Charities on offline media 0.024 0.044
Charities on online media 0.016 −0.022
Charities on social media 0.01 0.031
Energy companies on offline media 0.077w+0.045
Energy companies on online media 0.089* 0.048
Energy companies on social media −0.011 −0.078
Block 4: Trust in information sources
News media −0.050***
Politicians & political parties 0.161***
Scientists −0.214***
(Continued)
Ejaz et al. 11
government can be partly attributed to its strong correlation with trust in politicians and
political parties, suggesting similar associations with false beliefs. The addition of trust
in sources of information about climate change in the model explained an additional
15pp of the variance compared to Model 3 (from 18% to 33%), suggesting that trust
explains a large share of the variance in false beliefs about climate change.
Cross-country similarities and differences
Given national differences in media and political systems, some of the aggregated effects
reported above do not hold in every country. In this section, we discuss cross-country
similarities and differences (for separate models by country see Appendix VIII in sup-
plementary material).
Regarding cross-country differences, Indian respondents had stronger false beliefs
about climate change compared with those in other countries. This result aligns with past
work (Climate Action Against Disinformation (CADD), 2022), and may reflect national-
istic narratives in India that disproportionately blame Western countries for climate
change, obscuring the nuanced climate change debate (Billett, 2009) and India’s role in
emitting greenhouse gases, potentially contributing to public confusion and the spread of
misinformation (Murali et al., 2021: 18). Though this result could also be due to a form
of acquiescence bias, as we discuss below. In India and France, online news use was
positively associated with false beliefs about climate change. Only in Japan and Germany
was trust in the news associated with lower false beliefs.
Regarding cross-country similarities, trust in scientists was associated with lower
false beliefs about climate change, while the opposite was observed for trust in celebri-
ties. In all countries except for Germany, trust in politicians was positively associated
with false beliefs about climate change. Except for Pakistan, greater trust in international
Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4
Environmental activists −0.088***
Celebrities 0.159***
Government −0.007
International organizations −0.111***
Charities −0.010
Energy companies 0.158***
N 8359 8359 8359 7861
R20.140 0.169 0.185 0.331
R2 Adj. 0.139 0.168 0.181 0.327
Note: Scores are unstandardized regression coefficients. For estimates and standard errors look at Appen-
dix VII in supplementary material.
+ p < 0.1, * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001. There are fewer observa-
tions in Model 4 because we excluded participants who selected prefer not to say to the trust questions.
Note that each country was also added as a fixed effect in each model, but for clarity we report differences
in belief in climate change-related misinformation across countries in Table 1.
Table 2. (Continued)
12 new media & society 00(0)
Figure 1. Predictors of false beliefs about climate change, including trust in various information sources and different types of news platform use.
Error bars represent the 95% confidence interval. Note: Coefficients presented are standardized.
Note: For the full models see Table 2.
Ejaz et al. 13
organizations was associated with lower false beliefs. Apart from Japan, higher trust in
energy companies was associated with higher false beliefs. In all countries except Brazil,
India, and Pakistan, respondents identifying as right-wing (compared with left-wing)
held stronger false beliefs about climate change. Finally, in every country but Brazil,
offline news use was associated with weaker false beliefs about climate change (see
Figure 1).
Discussion
The present study aimed to address several gaps in the existing literature regarding cli-
mate news consumption, trust in different sources, misinformation about climate change,
and underrepresentation of non-WEIRD countries. While previous research has exten-
sively examined the positive effects of news use on political knowledge and climate
change-related attitudes, the role of news media in protecting individuals from belief in
misinformation about climate change remains uncertain, especially in countries beyond
the European and US contexts, such as Brazil, India and Pakistan, which despite being
ranked highly in the climate vulnerability index remain under-researched. Thus, includ-
ing these countries allows a more comprehensive understanding of the global dynamics
of climate change misinformation.
Overall, we found that trust in sources of information about climate change is a
stronger predictor of false beliefs about climate change than the type of media used to
consume information about climate change (offline, online and social media) and the
reported exposure to specific sources of information about climate change (e.g. scientists
on social media or celebrities on offline media).
Regarding media use, offline news use was consistently associated with lower false
beliefs about climate change across countries, while online and social media use were
not. This is potentially very challenging, given mounting evidence that most people with
access to the Internet are opting for digital media at the expense of legacy media like
television, radio, and printed newspapers (Newman et al., 2023). However, media use
only explained a small additional part of the variance in false beliefs (3pp).
This finding is in line with the idea that the news media protects people (at least a lit-
tle) from misinformation by exposing them to reliable information and/or by debunking
false information (Altay et al., 2023). Tsfati et al. (2020) may still be right that the news
media contributes to amplifying misinformation by exposing people to false information,
but increasing exposure is not sufficient to increase belief, and recent evidence suggests
that in Western democracies the news media may very well increase awareness of misin-
formation while reducing belief in it (Altay et al., 2023).
However, the news media is not a homogeneous category, and the positive effects of
media use reported here are exclusive to offline news media. This could be because there
is more climate change denialism news on online news media than offline news media in
the countries surveyed. Thus, the effect of online news use may be more heterogonous
than the effect of offline news use given increased opportunities to consume climate
contrarian information online than offline. Moreover, the effect of offline news likely
hides disparities across news outlets given that some prominent news outlets have
engaged in climate change denialism news coverage, such as right-wing cable channels
14 new media & society 00(0)
in the United States and right-wing newspapers in many English-language countries.
Finally, social media use was associated with lower false beliefs about climate change,
but this effect did not hold after controlling for trust in specific sources of climate change
information. The benefits of social media use may be weaker and/or more heterogeneous
than the benefits of offline news use. For instance, while social media use may help peo-
ple with low trust in scientists find climate denialist news, it may also help people with
high trust in scientists to learn more about climate change.
Self-reported exposure to some specific sources of information about climate change
was associated with false beliefs about climate change in different directions. For
instance, exposure to information about climate change from scientists on offline, online,
and social media was associated with lower misinformation belief about climate change.
While exposure to information about climate change from celebrities on offline and
social media was associated with higher belief with climate change-related misinforma-
tion. However, these effects were weak, highly inconsistent across countries and models,
and only explained a small additional part of the variance (1pp).
These findings suggest that while the specific sources of information about climate
change matter, their effects on false beliefs about climate change may vary depending on
the context and country. Future studies could rely on a combination of survey and track-
ing data to measure the effect of climate change information exposure from specific
sources and actors more accurately.
Trust in specific sources of information about climate change explained a large part of
the variance in false beliefs about climate change across countries (15pp). Higher trust in
scientists, and international organizations were associated with lower belief in misinfor-
mation about climate change, while higher trust in politicians, celebrities and energy
companies was associated with higher belief in climate change-related misinformation.
The strong predictive value of trust on false beliefs about climate change, and the
cross-cultural robustness of this finding, suggests that trust is central to understanding
misperceptions about climate change. Trust is known to play an important role in the
acceptance of communicated information: people are more likely to accept information
from trusted sources and largely disregard information communicated by distrusted
sources. In our case, trust in scientists may have reduced climate change misperceptions
by increasing exposure to and acceptance of reliable information about climate change
(and vice versa for trust in celebrities or politicians). However, it could also be that peo-
ple who trust scientists the most also happen to be more knowledgeable about climate
change because of their socio-economic status, or that people whose views align with the
scientific consensus, e.g., because they are concerned about the planet and want to pre-
serve the environment, happen to trust scientists the most (and vice versa for trust in
celebrities or politicians). Future research should try to disentangle the causal pathways
through which different forms of trust may affect false beliefs climate change. Yet, the
fact that trust captures a share of the variance in false beliefs not accounted for by demo-
graphic variables (such as education or political orientation) or media use (such as social
media use) suggests that it is unlikely to be a mere self-selection effect.
To maximize their global effectiveness, interventions aimed at reducing belief in mis-
information about climate change may want to target the most vulnerable groups, such
as people with low trust in scientists and/or high trust in celebrities. Future studies should
Ejaz et al. 15
manipulate experimentally whether increasing or decreasing trust in these sources affects
the acceptance of information from these sources, and ultimately, false beliefs about
climate change. Overall, our findings extend previous work on the importance of trust in
scientists to understand attitudes and beliefs about climate change, notably through its
cross-cultural scope and the variety of determinants and forms of trust included in the
survey.
This study has limitations. First, as with all surveys on news consumption, there
are questions over whether people are able to accurately describe their own news use
(Fletcher and Nielsen, 2018; Prior, 2009). Past research has shown that imperfect
recall by respondents may lead them to misrepresent their media use, potentially bias-
ing estimates in statistical analyses (Guess, 2015). However, due to a different set of
challenges and limitations with passive web tracking data (Bosch et al., 2023; Pew
Research Center, 2020),1 researchers lack a news use ‘ground truth’, and it remains
unclear if and when people underestimate or overestimate their news. In the present
study, if people are overestimating their offline news use (as Prior, 2009 suggests), it
may be that the negative association between it false beliefs about climate change is
weaker or would disappear with more precise measurement. Similarly, underestima-
tion of online news use (as recently found by UK media regulator Ofcom, 2022) could
be hiding a significant relationship with false beliefs. Yet, because over/underestima-
tion may vary among different demographic groups, it is difficult to be sure.
Second, the research on measuring public attitudes related to climate change using sur-
veys has cautioned about the issue of social desirability bias, which fails to fully reveal the
true attitudes of citizens (Beiser-McGrath and Bernauer, 2021) and even inflate self-
reported green attitudes (Larson and Kinsey, 2019). However, despite their limitations,
recent research suggests surveys may not be as problematic as commonly perceived (Jones-
Jang et al., 2020; Kirkizh et al., 2024), and nonetheless remain a realistic method for data
collection across multiple countries. Third, to assess respondents’ agreement with various
climate change-related misinformation, the questions were framed negatively, in line with
previous work (Capstick and Pidgeon, 2014; Coan et al., 2021). However, this negative
framing may have influenced respondents’ perceptions (Fischer et al., 2019), particularly
given the likelihood of varied response styles across different countries, where individuals
in some countries may exhibit a greater susceptibility to acquiescence bias compared with
others, which could have happened in the case of India. Hence, it is important to consider
the said limitations when interpreting the findings.
To conclude, we found that across eight countries the strongest determinants of false
beliefs about climate change were identifying as right-wing (compared with left-wing),
consuming less offline news, having less trust in scientists, environmental activists, and
international organizations, while having more trust in politicians, celebrities, and energy
companies as sources of information about climate change. Overall, the results highlight
that it’s the characteristics, attitudes and beliefs of people – especially their attitudes
towards specific information sources – and not simply exposure (whether it occurs
online, offline or on social media) that are central for understanding the effects of infor-
mation and misinformation. This insight, we hope, will help advance both our scholarly
understanding of factors influencing misinformation spread about climate change as well
as practical attempts to address such belief.
16 new media & society 00(0)
Funding
The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/
or publication of this article: This research was supported by the European Climate Foundation
(ECF).
ORCID iDs
Waqas Ejaz https://orcid.org/0000-0002-2492-4115
Sacha Altay https://orcid.org/0000-0002-2839-7375
Richard Fletcher https://orcid.org/0000-0002-5496-2112
Supplemental material
Supplemental material for this article is available online.
Note
1. Furthermore, relying on tracking data alone also presents challenges, including a limited abil-
ity to examine a range of different attitudes, including related to climate change (Kirkizh
et al., 2024), and a lack of data in certain countries.
References
Adam S, Reber U, Häussler T, et al. (2020) How climate change skeptics (try to) spread their ideas:
using computational methods to assess the resonance among skeptics’ and legacy media.
PLoS ONE 15(10): e0240089.
Altay S, Nielsen RK and Fletcher R (2023) News can help! The impact of news media and digital
platforms on awareness of and belief in misinformation. The International Journal of Press/
Politics 29: 459–484.
Anderson A (2011) Sources, media, and modes of climate change communication: the role of
celebrities. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Climate Change 2(4): 535–546.
Arbuckle JG Jr., Morton LW and Hobbs J (2015) Understanding farmer perspectives on climate
change adaptation and mitigation: the roles of trust in sources of climate information, climate
change beliefs, and perceived risk. Environment and Behavior 47(2): 205–234.
Arlt D, Hoppe I and Wolling J (2011) Climate change and media usage: effects on problem aware-
ness and behavioural intentions. International Communication Gazette 73(1–2): 45–63.
Asayama S and Ishii A (2014) Reconstruction of the boundary between climate science and poli-
tics: the IPCC in the Japanese mass media, 1988–2007. Public Understanding of Science
23(2): 189–203.
Beiser‐McGrath LF and Bernauer T (2021) Current surveys may underestimate climate change
skepticism evidence from list experiments in Germany and the USA. PloS One 16(7):
e0251034. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0251034
Biddlestone M, Azevedo F and Van Der Linden S (2022) Climate of conspiracy: a meta-analysis
of the consequences of belief in conspiracy theories about climate change. Current Opinion
in Psychology 46: 101390.
Billett S (2009) Dividing climate change: global warming in the Indian mass media. Climatic
Change 99(1–2): 1–16.
Bohr J (2020) Reporting on climate change: a computational analysis of U.S. newspapers and
sources of bias, 1997–2017. Global Environmental Change 61: 102038.
Ejaz et al. 17
Bosch OJ, Sturgis P, Kuha J, et al. (2023) Uncovering Digital Trace Data Biases: Tracking
Undercoverage in Web Tracking Data. Charlottesville, VA: OSF.
Boykoff MT (2011) Who Speaks for the Climate? Making Sense of Media Reporting on Climate
Change. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Boykoff M and Farrell J (2019). Climate change countermovement organizations and media atten-
tion in the United States. In Almiron N. and Xifra J. (eds.), Climate change denial and public
relations: Strategic communication and interest groups in climate inaction (pp. 121–139).
Routledge.
Brewer PR and Ley BL (2013) Whose science do you believe? Explaining trust in sources of sci-
entific information about the environment. Science Communication 35(1): 115–137.
Cabecinhas R, Lázaro A and Carvalho A (2008) Media uses and social representations of climate
change. In: Carvalho A (ed.) Communicating Climate Change: Discourses, Mediations and
Perceptions. Braga: Universidade do Minho, pp. 170–189.
Capstick SB and Pidgeon NF (2014) What is climate change scepticism? Examination of the
concept using a mixed methods study of the UK public. Global Environmental Change 24:
389–401.
Carmichael JT and Brulle RJ (2017) Elite cues, media coverage, and public concern: an integrated
path analysis of public opinion on climate change, 2001–2013. Environmental Politics 26(2):
232–252.
Cheng H and Gonzalez-Ramirez J (2020) Trust and the media: perceptions of climate change news
sources among US college students. Postdigital Science and Education 3: 910–933.
Climate Action Against Disinformation (CAAD) (2022) The impacts of climate disinformation
on public perception. Available at: https://caad.info/analysis/reports/the-impacts-of-climate-
disinformation-on-public-perception-2/
Coan TG, Boussalis C, Cook J, et al. (2021) Computer-assisted classification of contrarian claims
about climate change. Scientific Reports 11(1): 22320.
Cologna V and Siegrist M (2020) The role of trust for climate change mitigation and adaptation
behaviour: a meta-analysis. Journal of Environmental Psychology 69: 101428.
Comfort SE, Tandoc E and Gruszczynski M (2020) Who is heard in climate change journalism?
Sourcing patterns in climate change news in China, India, Singapore, and Thailand. Climatic
Change 158(3–4): 327–343.
Cook J (2020) Deconstructing climate science denial. In: Holmes DC and Richardson LM (eds)
Research Handbook on Communicating Climate Change. Cheltenham: Edward Elgar
Publishing, pp. 62–78.
Cook J, Ellerton P and Kinkead D (2018) Deconstructing climate misinformation to identify rea-
soning errors. Environmental Research Letters 13(2): 024018.
Czarnek G, Kossowska M and Szwed P (2021) Right-wing ideology reduces the effects of educa-
tion on climate change beliefs in more developed countries. Nature Climate Change 11(1):
9–13.
Diehl T, Huber B, Gil de Zúñiga H, et al. (2021) Social media and beliefs about climate change:
a cross-national analysis of news use, political ideology, and trust in science. International
Journal of Public Opinion Research 33(2): 197–213.
Dietz T, Dan A and Shwom R (2007) Support for climate change policy: social psychological and
social structural influences. Rural Sociology 72(2): 185–214.
Dunlap RE and Brulle RJ (2020) Sources and amplifiers of climate change denial. In: Holmes
DC and Richardson LM (eds) Research Handbook on Communicating Climate Change.
Cheltenham: Edward Elgar Publishing, pp. 49–61.
18 new media & society 00(0)
Eckstein D, Winges M, Künzel V, et al. (2019) Global Climate Risk Index 2020 Who Suffers Most
from Extreme Weather Events? Weather-Related Loss Events in 2018 and 1999 to 2018.
Berlin: Germanwatch Nord-Süd Initiative e.V.
Ejaz W, Ittefaq M, Seo H, et al. (2021) Factors associated with the belief in COVID-19 related
conspiracy theories in Pakistan. Health, Risk & Society 23: 162–178.
Ejaz W, Mukherjee M, Fletcher R, et al. (2022) How We Follow Climate Change: Climate News
Use and Attitudes in Eight Countries. Oxford: Reuters Institute for the Study of Journalism.
Ferrante L and Fearnside PM (2019) Brazil’s new president and ‘ruralists’ threaten Amazonia’s
environment, traditional peoples and the global climate. Environmental Conservation 46(4):
261–263.
Fischer H, Amelung D and Said N (2019) The accuracy of German citizens’ confidence in their
climate change knowledge. Nature Climate Change 9(10): 776–780.
Fletcher R and Nielsen RK (2018) Are people incidentally exposed to news on social media? A
comparative analysis. New Media & Society 20(7): 2450–2468.
Guess AM (2015) Measure for measure: an experimental test of online political media exposure.
Political Analysis 23(1): 59–75.
Gustafson A, Rosenthal SA, Ballew MT, et al. (2019) The development of partisan polarization
over the Green New Deal. Nature Climate Change 9(12): 940–944.
Hobolt SB and Tilley J (2018) Fleeing the centre: the rise of challenger parties in the aftermath of
the euro crisis. In: Laffan B (ed.) Europe’s Union in Crisis. London: Routledge, pp. 57–77.
Hornsey MJ, Harris EA, Bain PG, et al. (2016) Meta-analyses of the determinants and outcomes
of belief in climate change. Nature Climate Change 6(6): 622–626.
Ittefaq M, Abwao M, and Rafique S (2021). Polio vaccine misinformation on social media:
Turning point in the fight against polio eradication in Pakistan. Human Vaccines &
Immunotherapeutics 17(8): 2575–2577. DOI: 10.1080/21645515.2021.189489.
Jones-Jang SM, Heo Y, McKeever R, et al. (2020) Good news! Communication Findings may be
underestimated: comparing effect sizes with self-reported and logged smartphone use data.
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 25(5): 346–363.
Kahan DM, Jenkins-Smith H and Braman D (2011) Cultural cognition of scientific consensus.
Journal of Risk Research 14(2): 147–174.
Kirkizh N, Ulloa R, Stier S, et al. (2024) Predicting political attitudes from web tracking data: a
machine learning approach. Journal of Information Technology & Politics. Epub ahead of
print 25 February. DOI: 10.1080/19331681.2024.2316679.
Krange O, Kaltenborn BP and Hultman M (2021) ‘Don’t confuse me with facts’—how right wing
populism affects trust in agencies advocating anthropogenic climate change as a reality.
Humanities and Social Sciences Communications 8(1): Article 1.
Larson RB and Kinsey JD (2019) Culture and sampling issues with “Green” attitude research.
Social Marketing Quarterly 25(2): 91–106. DOI: 10.1177/1524500419838989.
Leonard R and Leviston Z (2017) Applying a model of collective efficacy for understanding con-
sumer and civic pro-environmental actions. Socijalna Ekologija: Časopis Za Ekološku Misao
I Sociologijska Istraživanja Okoline 26(3): 105–123.
McCright AM, Dunlap RE and Marquart-Pyatt ST (2016) Political ideology and views about cli-
mate change in the European Union. Environmental Politics 25(2): 338–358.
Mattes R and Bratton M (2007) Learning about democracy in Africa: awareness, performance, and
experience. American Journal of Political Science 51(1): 192–217.
Murali R, Kuwar A and Nagendra H (2021) Who’s responsible for climate change? Untangling
threads of media discussions in India, Nigeria, Australia, and the USA. Climatic Change
164(3–4): 1–20.
Ejaz et al. 19
Newman N, Fletcher R, Schulz A, et al. (2020) Reuters Institute digital news report 2020. Reuters
Institute for the Study of Journalism. DOI: 10.60625/risj-048n-ap07.
Newman N, Fletcher R, Eddy K, et al. (2023) Digital news report 2023. Reuters Institute for the
Study of Journalism. DOI: 10.60625/risj-p6es-hb13.
Newman TP, Nisbet EC and Nisbet MC (2018) Climate change, cultural cognition, and media
effects: worldviews drive news selectivity, biased processing, and polarized attitudes. Public
Understanding of Science 27(8): 985–1002.
Ofcom (2022) News consumption and media plurality on Twitter in the UK. Available at: https://
www.ofcom.org.uk/research-and-data/multi-sector-research/media-plurality (accessed April
6, 2024).
Painter J and Ashe T (2012) Cross-national comparison of the presence of climate scepticism in
the print media in six countries, 2007–10. Environmental Research Letters 7(4): 044005.
Painter J and Gavin NT (2015) Climate skepticism in British newspapers, 2007–2011.
Environmental Communication 10(4): 432–452.
Painter J, Ettinger J, Holmes D, et al. (2023) Climate delay discourses present in global main-
stream television coverage of the IPCC’s 2021 report. Communications Earth & Environment
4(1): 118.
Petersen AM, Vincent EM and Westerling AL (2019) Discrepancy in scientific authority and
media visibility of climate change scientists and contrarians. Nature Communications 10(1):
Article 1.
Pew Research Center (2020) Measuring news consumption in a digital era. https://www.pewre-
search.org/journalism/2020/12/08/measuring-news-consumption-in-a-digital-era/
Poortinga W, Whitmarsh L, Steg L, et al. (2019) Climate change perceptions and their individual-
level determinants: a cross-European analysis. Global Environmental Change 55: 25–35.
Porter E and Wood TJ (2021) The global effectiveness of fact-checking: evidence from simultane-
ous experiments in Argentina, Nigeria, South Africa, and the United Kingdom. Proceedings
of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 118(37): e2104235118.
Prior M (2009) The immensely inflated news audience: assessing bias in self-reported news expo-
sure. Public Opinion Quarterly 73(1): 130–143.
Schäfer MS and Painter J (2020) Climate journalism in a changing media ecosystem: assessing
the production of climate change-related news around the world. Wiley Interdisciplinary
Reviews: Climate Change 12: e675.
Smith EK and Mayer A (2019) Anomalous Anglophones? Contours of free market ideology,
political polarization, and climate change attitudes in English-speaking countries, Western
European and post-Communist states. Climatic Change 152(1): 17–34.
Supran G and Oreskes N (2017) Assessing ExxonMobil’s climate change communications (1977–
2014). Environmental Research Letters 12(8): 084019.
Tsfati Y, Boomgaarden HG, Strömbäck J, et al. (2020) Causes and consequences of mainstream
media dissemination of fake news: literature review and synthesis. Annals of the International
Communication Association 44(2): 157–173.
Uscinski J, Enders A, Diekman A, et al. (2022) The psychological and political correlates of con-
spiracy theory beliefs. Scientific Reports 12(1): Article 1.
van der Linden S, Leiserowitz A, Rosenthal S, et al. (2017) Inoculating the public against misin-
formation about climate change. Global Challenges 1(2): 1600008.
Vowles K and Hultman M (2021) Scare-quoting climate: the rapid rise of climate denial in the
Swedish far-right media ecosystem. Nordic Journal of Media Studies 3(1): 79–95.
Vraga EK and Bode L (2020) Defining misinformation and understanding its bounded nature:
using expertise and evidence for describing misinformation. Political Communication 37(1):
136–144.
20 new media & society 00(0)
Walter N and Murphy ST (2018) How to unring the bell: a meta-analytic approach to correction of
misinformation. Communication Monographs 85(3): 423–441.
Whiting K (2020) 3 charts that show how attitudes to climate science vary around the world. World
Economic Forum, January 22. Available at: https://www.weforum.org/agenda/2020/01/cli-
mate-science-global-warming-most-sceptics-country/
Whitmarsh L (2011) Scepticism and uncertainty about climate change: dimensions, determinants
and change over time. Global Environmental Change 21(2): 690–700.
Zhao X, Leiserowitz AA, Maibach EW, et al. (2011) Attention to science/environment news posi-
tively predicts and attention to political news negatively predicts global warming risk percep-
tions and policy support. Journal of Communication 61(4): 713–731.
Zhou Y and Shen L (2021) Confirmation bias and the persistence of misinformation on climate
change. Communication Research 49: 500–523.
Author biographies
Waqas Ejaz is a Postdoctoral Research Fellow at the Reuters Institute for the Study of Journalism,
University of Oxford. His research focuses on understanding the consumption of climate change
news, the effects of digital media, and journalism. He holds a PhD in communication science from
the Technical University of Ilmenau, Germany.
Sacha Altay is an experimental psychologist working on misinformation, misperceptions, social
media, and trust. He’s currently a post-doctoral researcher at the University of Zurich within the
political science department, and holds a PhD in cognitive science from the École Normale
Supérieure in Paris.
Richard Fletcher is the Director of Research at the Reuters Institute for the Study of Journalism,
University of Oxford. He is a lead researcher and co-author of the Digital News Report. He is
primarily interested in global trends in digital news consumption, comparative media research and
the relationship between technology and journalism.
Rasmus Kleis Nielsen is the director of the Reuters Institute for the Study of Journalism and profes-
sor of Political Communication at the University of Oxford