ArticlePDF Available

Gender-specific associations between sleep quality, sleep duration and cognitive functioning among older Indians: findings from WHO-SAGE study

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Background Sleep is an essential component of human health and well-being, playing a crucial role in several cognitive processes, including attention, memory, and executive function. In this study, we aimed to examine the association between sleep quality, sleep duration and cognitive functioning among older men and women in India. Methods Data come from the World Health Organization’s Study on global AGEing and adult health (WHO-SAGE), India wave-2, which was conducted in 2015 in six selected states of India, representing different country regions. The sample included 6,396 older adults aged 50 years and above. We used multivariable linear regression models to examine the associations between sleep quality, sleep duration and cognitive function, separately among older men and women. Results Older men and women with poor sleep and short duration sleep had lower mean scores of cognition than their peers with good sleep and age-appropriate sleep duration. Poor sleep (aCoef: -5.09, CI: -8.66, -1.51) and short duration sleep (aCoef: -5.43, CI: -7.77, -3.10) were negatively associated with cognitive functioning among older men and the associations remained significant among older men with poor sleep (aCoef: -2.39, CI: -3.78, -1.00) and short duration sleep (aCoef: -4.39, CI: -6.46, -2.31) after adjusting for a large number of socio-demographic, health and behavioral factors. Similarly, poor sleep (aCoef: -3.15, CI: -5.79, -0.52) and short duration sleep (aCoef: -2.72, CI: -4.64, -0.81) were associated with cognitive functioning among older women, however, the associations were insignificant when the potential confounders were adjusted. Conclusions This study provides evidence for the significant association between sleep health and cognitive functioning in older Indian adults, especially older men, with poor sleep quality and insufficient sleep duration being detrimental to their cognitive health. Healthcare providers should routinely screen for sleep quality and age-appropriate sleep duration in their older adult patients and consider sex/gender-tailored sleep interventions as part of cognitive health management strategies.
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
RESEARCH Open Access
© The Author(s) 2024. Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use,
sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and
the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this
article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included
in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will
need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
Muhammad et al. Sleep Science and Practice (2024) 8:6
https://doi.org/10.1186/s41606-024-00100-z Sleep Science and Practice
*Correspondence:
T. Muhammad
muhammad.iips@gmail.com
Full list of author information is available at the end of the article
Abstract
Background Sleep is an essential component of human health and well-being, playing a crucial role in several
cognitive processes, including attention, memory, and executive function. In this study, we aimed to examine the
association between sleep quality, sleep duration and cognitive functioning among older men and women in India.
Methods Data come from the World Health Organization’s Study on global AGEing and adult health (WHO-SAGE),
India wave-2, which was conducted in 2015 in six selected states of India, representing dierent country regions.
The sample included 6,396 older adults aged 50 years and above. We used multivariable linear regression models to
examine the associations between sleep quality, sleep duration and cognitive function, separately among older men
and women.
Results Older men and women with poor sleep and short duration sleep had lower mean scores of cognition than
their peers with good sleep and age-appropriate sleep duration. Poor sleep (aCoef: -5.09, CI: -8.66, -1.51) and short
duration sleep (aCoef: -5.43, CI: -7.77, -3.10) were negatively associated with cognitive functioning among older men
and the associations remained signicant among older men with poor sleep (aCoef: -2.39, CI: -3.78, -1.00) and short
duration sleep (aCoef: -4.39, CI: -6.46, -2.31) after adjusting for a large number of socio-demographic, health and
behavioral factors. Similarly, poor sleep (aCoef: -3.15, CI: -5.79, -0.52) and short duration sleep (aCoef: -2.72, CI: -4.64,
-0.81) were associated with cognitive functioning among older women, however, the associations were insignicant
when the potential confounders were adjusted.
Conclusions This study provides evidence for the signicant association between sleep health and cognitive
functioning in older Indian adults, especially older men, with poor sleep quality and insucient sleep duration
being detrimental to their cognitive health. Healthcare providers should routinely screen for sleep quality and age-
appropriate sleep duration in their older adult patients and consider sex/gender-tailored sleep interventions as part of
cognitive health management strategies.
Keywords Sleep quality, Sleep duration, Cognition, Gender, Older adults
Gender-specic associations between
sleep quality, sleep duration and cognitive
functioning among older Indians: ndings
from WHO-SAGE study
T.Muhammad1* , A. H. SruthiAnil Kumar2 and T. V.Sekher3
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Page 2 of 16
Muhammad et al. Sleep Science and Practice (2024) 8:6
Background
Sleep is an essential component of human health and
well-being, playing a crucial role in several cognitive pro-
cesses, including attention, memory, and executive func-
tion. Changes in sleep patterns, such as decreased sleep
quality and altered sleep duration, are common as people
age (Pace-Schott and Spencer 2011). ese alterations
have been linked to cognitive decline and an increased
risk of neurodegenerative diseases in older adults. Sleep
quality refers to the subjective experience of sleep, which
includes factors such as sleep latency, sleep ecacy, and
the presence of sleep disturbances (Krystal and Edinger
2008). Numerous studies have demonstrated a strong
correlation between poor sleep quality and cognitive
impairment in older individuals (Keage et al. 2012; Beh-
rens et al. 2023; Zhang et al. 2023; Miyata et al. 2013).
Inadequate sleep quality has also been associated
with an increased risk of mild cognitive impairment
(MCI) and dementia, including Alzheimer’s disease
(Rothman and Mattson 2012). Cognitive impairment
is further exacerbated by sleep disorders such as sleep
apnea. (Findley et al. 1986; Gagnon et al. 2014; Vanek et
al. 2020). Further, the duration of sleep that is the total
quantity of time an individual spends asleep also plays a
role in aecting cognition (Fortier-Brochu et al. 2012).
Individual sleep requirements may vary, but most adults
require seven to nine hours of sleep per night for optimal
cognitive function (Hirshkowitz et al. 2015). However,
due to age-related changes in circadian rhythms, medical
conditions, and medication use, senior adults frequently
experience shorter sleep durations (Bombois et al. 2010;
Mattis and Sehgal 2016). Insucient sleep duration has
been linked to decits in cognitive domains such as con-
solidation of memories, attention, and executive function
(Cohen-Zion et al. 2004).
Sleep is necessary for memory consolidation, the pro-
cess by which newly acquired information is transformed
into stable memories. Adequate sleep, specically pro-
found sleep or slow-wave sleep, facilitates the consolida-
tion of memories and improves cognitive performance
(Van Cauter et al. 2000; Nebes et al. 2009). Reduced sleep
duration and quality impede memory retrieval and cogni-
tive performance. Maintaining cognitive health and well-
being in older individuals requires optimal sleep quality
and duration. A meta-analysis of 45 studies on sleep
quality and duration in low and middle income countries
(LMICs) suggests that though sleep health parameters
in LMICs are similar to those in high income countries,
there is huge variability potentially due to specic socio-
cultural and demographic settings (Simonelli et al. 2018).
Other studies also found that individuals in LMICs with
sucient sleep exhibited higher cognitive scores and
those with sleep problems reported higher cognitive
complaints (Gildner et al. 2014; Smith et al. 2022).
The role of gender in sleep and cognition
Understanding the complex connection between sleep
and cognition is essential for promoting healthy aging.
In older individuals, sleep quality, sleep duration, and
cognitive function are intricately linked. However, it is
important to consider how gender may moderate this
relationship. By examining the inuence of gender, we
can gain a deeper understanding of how specic factors
interact and impact cognitive health in dierent popu-
lations. Research indicates that sleep patterns vary by
gender (Quan et al. 2016; Rani et al. 2022). Insomnia and
sleep disturbances are more prevalent among women
than among men, resulting in poorer quality of sleep
(Guidozzi 2015). is disparity in sleep quality between
men and women may contribute to dierences in cogni-
tive performance.
Typically, older women report shorter sleep duration
than older men (Rani et al. 2022). Cognitive impairments
have been associated with sleep deprivation resulting
from insucient sleep duration (Lo et al. 2016). Besides,
gender dierences in sleep duration may contribute to
cognitive function dierences among older adults. In
multiple ways, gender may moderate the relationship
between sleep quality, sleep duration, and cognition.
Importantly, hormonal uctuations in women, such as
those that occur during menopause, can aect the qual-
ity of sleep and cognitive function (Eichling and Sahni
2005). Changes in estrogen levels have been associated
with sleep disorders and cognitive decline (Guidozzi
2013). ese hormonal inuences may partially explain
why women experience poorer sleep quality and cogni-
tive impairments than men. Secondly, gender roles and
social expectations may aect sleep patterns and cogni-
tive performance. Multiple duties and responsibilities,
such as caregiving and housework, are frequently borne
by women, which can result in elevated levels of stress
and sleep disturbances (Cha and Eun 2014).
Additionally, cultural norms may inuence men and
women’s sleeping habits dierently (Maume et al. 2010).
ese factors can contribute to variations in sleep qual-
ity and duration, which in turn impact cognitive func-
tion in older individuals. Also, presence of other health
conditions may impact sleep (Muhammad et al. 2023).
Understanding the role of gender as a moderator in the
association between sleep quality, sleep duration, and
cognition has signicant implications for healthcare
interventions. Adapting sleep interventions to the gen-
der-specic requirements of older adults may improve
sleep quality and cognitive function. us, it is neces-
sary to examine the complex gender-specic relation-
ship between sleep quality, sleep duration, and cognition
in older adults in India, emphasizing the role of various
factors that may inuence and aect optimal sleep pat-
terns for healthy aging. erefore, in this study, we aimed
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Page 3 of 16
Muhammad et al. Sleep Science and Practice (2024) 8:6
to examine the association between sleep quality, sleep
duration and cognitive functioning among older men and
women in India. We also examined the role of several
socio-demographic and health-related variables in these
associations (Fig.1).
Methods
Data
Data come from the World Health Organization’s Study
on global AGEing and adult health (WHO-SAGE), India
wave-2, which was conducted in 2015 in six selected
states of India, representing dierent country regions:
Assam (Northeast), Karnataka (South), Maharash-
tra (West), Rajasthan (North), Uttar Pradesh (Central)
and West Bengal (East), covering a broadly illustrative
aggregate sample of 9,116 respondents aged 18 years
and above. SAGE wave-2 India was a follow-up study of
SAGE wave-1 and covered the same states with the same
primary sampling units (PSU) and the sample house-
holds which were covered in the WHO-World Health
Survey (WHS), 2003. From all the states in India, a sys-
tematic random sample selection procedure was fol-
lowed to select the states in WHS. Two-stage sampling
in rural areas was used where the villages were the PSUs
and households as secondary stage unit (SSU). ree-
stage sampling in urban areas with the selection of wards,
census enumeration blocks, and households in a specic
order was followed. e number of households selected
was in proportion to the respective state population and
was distributed in urban and rural population. More
detailed Information about weights and survey design
is available at https://apps.who.int/healthinfo/systems/
surveydata/index.php/catalog/117. SAGE Wave-2 had
two target populations: a large sample of persons aged
50 years and older, which is the focus of the study, and a
smaller sample of persons aged 18–49 years. e survey
had a response rate of 77% for the individual question-
naire (Arokiasamy et al. 2020).e number of respon-
dents in WHO-SAGE, waive-2 included 1,998 aged
18–49 years and 7,118 older adults aged 50 and above.
is study considered respondents aged 50 years and
above. After removing the sample with missing informa-
tion on cognition, our analytical sample reduced to 6,396
older adults age 50 years and above.
Measurements
Outcome variable
Cognitive functioning was assessed using variables such
as verbal uency, verbal recall, digit span forward and
digit span backward. For assessing verbal recall, inter-
viewer read out a list of ten commonly used words to
the respondents and asked them to repeat again in some
time. For assessing verbal uency, respondents were
asked to produce as many animal names as possible in
one minute time span. Finally for assessing the digit span,
which was utilized to measure working memory, inter-
viewer read a series of digits and asked to immediately
repeat them back. In the backward test, the person must
repeat the numbers in reverse order. A series of number
sequences was presented and the respondent was asked
to reproduce the exact same sequence. Following a cor-
rect recall, longer sequences were given until failure. e
Fig. 1 Conceptual framework
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Page 4 of 16
Muhammad et al. Sleep Science and Practice (2024) 8:6
maximum score for the forward digit count was 9 with
a range of 0–9; the score for the backward digit count
ranged from 0 to 8 and a summary score, created by add-
ing forward and backward counting scores, ranged from
0 to 17. e verbal uency test measured respondents’
ability to retrieve information from semantic memory.
is was a one minute assessment in which respondents
were asked to name as many animals as they could. e
verbal uency score was dened by the number of cor-
rectly named animals. Repeated names were not counted.
e overall cognitive score was obtained by adding
scores of verbal recall, digit counting, forward and back-
ward, and verbal uency tests. e scores on these four
tests, which were in dierent scales, were standardised
by rescaling them to have a mean of zero and a stan-
dard deviation of one, and z-score was generated for
each measures. Further, a composite cognitive score was
created using a principal components analysis (PCA).
Finally, the generated index was converted into a 0 (worst
cognition) to 100 (best cognition) scale which facilitates
easier interpretation of the data.
Key explanatory variables
Main predictor variables in this study were sleep quality
and sleep duration. Sleep quality was assessed using the
question, “Please rate the quality of your sleep last night.
Was it very good, good, moderate, poor or very poor?”
Sleep duration was assessed using the question, “How
many hours did you sleep last night?” e responses in
the format of minutes were converted into hours and the
duration was classied as per the total number of hours
the respondent slept in the last night. Less than seven
hours was classied as short duration sleep, 7–8 h was
considered as normal sleep and 9 and more hours was
considered as long duration sleep among older adults in
this study, as recommended by the National Sleep Foun-
dation (Hirshkowitz et al. 2015).
Covariates
Health and behavioral factors included nutritional intake,
assessed by the total number of fruit and vegetable serv-
ings per day (recoded as no for four or less servings, and
yes for more than 4 times per day) (Patel et al. 2019),
body mass index (measured based on height and weight,
and classied as per WHO criteria; less than 18.5 kg/
m2 as underweight, 18.5–24.9 kg/m2 as normal, 25.0–
29.9kg/m2 as overweight, and 30.0kg/m2 as obese),
and physical activity (vigorous, moderate, light and no
activity). For physical activity, the questions assessed the
duration of activity (minutes and/or hours) on a typi-
cal day. e duration of activity included: (i) activities at
the workplace, (ii) activities done as part of travel to and
from places, and (iii) leisure time or recreational physi-
cal activities. We followed the WHO global guidelines
on physical activity for adult health, categorized as vig-
orous activity, moderate activity and light activity and
physical inactivity (Organization 2020). Vigorous activity
includes individuals spending at least 75min on a vigor-
ously intensive activity on a typical day. Moderate activ-
ity includes individuals spending time at least 150min on
moderately intensive activity on a typical day. Light activ-
ity include any activity that does not fall in the above two
categories.
Self-rated health was assessed using a single overall
self-rated general health question used in SAGE: “In gen-
eral, how would you rate your health today?” with a ve-
point response scale from very good to very bad. Number
of chronic conditions was classied into none, single, two
and three and more. Chronic conditions in this study
included having diagnosed with any of the following con-
ditions; hypertension, diabetes, stroke, arthritis, angina,
asthma and chronic lung disease. e question format
used was, “Have you ever been diagnosed with the con-
dition?” for each health condition. Smoking status was
recoded into never smoked, currently not smoking and
currently smoking.
Socio-demographic variables in this study included
age (grouped as 50–59, 60–69, 70–79, 80 + years), sex
(male and female), educational level (no education, less
than primary, primary, secondary and higher), and cur-
rent marital status (married, widowed and others which
include never married/ separated/ divorced). Household
related variables included household wealth index (com-
puted based on a detailed list of items of household assets
and was available on ve quintiles and lowest represents
the quintile with the poorest households and high-
est represents the quintile with the richest households),
religious groups (Hindus, Muslims and others), social
groups (scheduled castes and scheduled tribes [both are
socioeconomically most disadvantaged] and others),
place of residence (urban and rural), and states (Assam,
Karnataka, Maharashtra, Rajasthan, Uttar Pradesh and
West Bengal).
Statistical analysis
We conducted the descriptive statistics to present the
characteristics of the study sample. Further, we presented
the mean scores of cognitive functioning among older
adults by explanatory variables, including sleep quality
and duration, along with 95% condence intervals (CIs).
Finally, we used multivariable linear regression models
to examine the association between sleep quality, dura-
tion and cognitive function. We employed four models
to examine the unadjusted and adjusted linear regression
estimates of cognitive functioning by sleep quality and
duration. First model provides the unadjusted estimates,
second model is adjusted for the selected socio-demo-
graphic variables, third model is additionally adjusted
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Page 5 of 16
Muhammad et al. Sleep Science and Practice (2024) 8:6
for the selected household-related variables and fourth
or nal model is a full model adjusted for all the selected
covariates including the health and behavioral factors.
Survey weights were applied to account for the com-
plex survey design and to provide the estimates at pop-
ulation level. Regression diagnostics such as variance
ination factor (VIF) for multicollinearity (Table S1) and
tests for linearity and normality of residuals (Figures S1
& S2) were carried out and found no violation of basis
assumptions of regression (see Supplemental material).
We report the results in the form of weighted means, and
unadjusted and adjusted coecients (aCoef) with 95%
CIs. All the analyses were carried out in Stata version
15.1 (StataCorp 2017).
Results
Table1 presents the sample characteristics. Around 5%
of the sample age 80 + years, 47.98% of the participants
had no formal education and a total of 23.39% of the
participants were widowed in this study. Around 19% of
older participants reported 5 + intake of fruit and veg-
etables per day, 27.3% were underweight, 15.54% were
overweight and 3.61% were obese in this study. A large
proportion of the sample (43.98%) reported that they
were engaged in none of the physical activities whereas,
21.08% of males and 10.38% of females reported engag-
ing in vigorous physical activity. Around 18% of the par-
ticipants had a bad or very bad self-rated health whereas,
around 42% of the participants had at least one chronic
condition.
Table2 provides the mean scores of cognitive function-
ing (on a scale of 0-100) among older adults by selected
background variables. Older men and women with a
poor sleep and short sleep duration had lower mean
scores of cognition than their peers with good sleep and
age-appropriate sleep duration. Older men consistently
exhibited higher mean scores for cognitive functioning
across all age groups. For instance, men in the 50–59
years age group had a mean score of 62.83 (CI: 61.55,
64.11), while women had a mean score of 56.41 (CI:
55.41, 57.42). is is consistent across all age categories.
Men had higher mean scores across dierent education
levels as compared to women except in the high school
level, where men scored 62.53 (CI: 61.24, 63.83) com-
pared to women’s score of 67.62 (CI: 63.71, 71.53), and
college level, where men scored 67.34 (CI: 64.20, 70.47)
compared to women’s score of 69.86 (CI: 66.29, 73.42).
Table 3 presents the multivariable linear regression
estimates of cognitive functioning among older men.
Poor sleep (aCoef: -5.09, CI: -8.66, -1.51) and short
sleep (aCoef: -5.43, CI: -7.77, 3.10) was negatively asso-
ciated with cognitive functioning among older men.
e associations remained signicant among older men
with poor sleep (aCoef: -2.39, CI: -3.78, -1.00) and short
sleep (aCoef: -4.39, CI: -6.46, -2.31) after adjusting for a
large number of socio-demographic and health-related
variables. In comparison to the reference group (50–59
years), there is a decline in cognitive functioning in older
men belonging to 60–69 age group (aCoef: -3.09, CI:
-4.46, -1.72), 70–79 age group (aCoef: -4.49, CI: -6.30,
-2.68), and 80 + age group (aCoef: -9.22, CI: -12.2, -6.19).
Further, there was a positive association between cog-
nitive functioning and education across all levels, with
the highest likelihood observed among those who have
attended college (aCoef: 11.5, CI: 8.49, 14.4). Widowhood
is linked to a notable decrease in cognitive functioning
(aCoef: -2.52, CI: -4.34, -0.69), whereas no signicant
association was found for other marital statuses. Over-
weight (aCoef: 2.18, CI: 0.23, 4.12) and obesity (aCoef:
3.67, CI: 0.52, 6.81) were signicantly associated with
improved cognition whereas, physical inactivity was
signicantly associated with poor cognitive function
among older men (aCoef: -1.34, CI: -2.77, -0.088). Self-
rated health demonstrates a signicant association with
cognitive functioning, particularly in the moderate and
bad health categories (aCoef: -3.93, CI: -6.98, -0.87 and
aCoef: -4.44, CI: -7.83, -1.04, respectively). e cogni-
tive functioning was much lower for older men in rural
areas (aCoef: -2.24, CI: -4.03, -0.46) compared to those
in urban areas. Nutritional intake, chronic conditions,
smoking status, wealth quintile, religion, social group,
and states did not exhibit signicant associations with
cognitive functioning.
Table 4 presents the multivariable linear regression
estimates of cognitive functioning among older women.
Poor sleep (aCoef: -3.15, CI: -5.79, -0.52) and short sleep
(aCoef: -2.72, CI: -4.64, -0.81) was negatively associated
with cognitive functioning among older women, how-
ever, the associations were insignicant when the poten-
tial health and behavioral confounders were adjusted.
Compared to the reference group (50–59 years), cogni-
tive functioning decreased in 60–69 age group (aCoef:
-1.32, CI: -2.58, -0.065), 70–79 age group (aCoef: -4.20,
CI: -5.82, -2.57) and 80 + age group (aCoef: -3.34, CI:
-6.89, -0.22). A positive association of cognitive function-
ing with all levels of education was observed, with the
highest levels observed for the college-educated group
(aCoef: 13.2, CI: 9.31, 17.1). Being widowed had a sig-
nicant eect on declining cognitive functioning among
older women (aCoef: -1.54, CI: -2.83, -0.26). Having
a higher nutritional intake had a signicantly positive
eect on the levels of cognitive functioning (aCoef: 2.83,
CI: 1.27, 4.40). Compared to being underweight, nor-
mal, overweight, and obese categories had signicantly
increased levels of cognitive functioning. Moderate phys-
ical activity had a signicant positive eect on cognitive
functioning (aCoef: 2.56, CI: 0.32, 4.81). Self-rated health,
chronic conditions, smoking status, wealth quintile,
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Page 6 of 16
Muhammad et al. Sleep Science and Practice (2024) 8:6
Variables Men Women Total
Age (in years)
50–59 1046 (35.37) 1568 (46.07) 2614 (40.95)
60–69 1178 (38.87) 1157 (34.2) 2335 (36.43)
70–79 609 (19.82) 536 (15.54) 1145 (17.59)
80+ 181 (5.94) 121 (4.2) 302 (5.03)
Level of education
No formal education 887 (26.48) 2295 (67.7) 3182 (47.98)
Less than primary 480 (15.72) 361 (10.99) 841 (13.26)
Primary 545 (17.91) 360 (10.18) 905 (13.88)
Secondary 424 (14.83) 189 (5.25) 613 (9.83)
High school 400 (13.96) 98 (3.29) 498 (8.39)
College 278 (11.09) 79 (2.59) 357 (6.66)
Current marital status
Married 2663 (87.96) 2122 (63.09) 4785 (74.98)
Widowed 293 (9.81) 1211 (35.84) 1504 (23.39)
Others 58 (2.23) 49 (1.07) 107 (1.62)
Nutritional intake (> 4 times per day)
No 2412 (79.52) 2798 (82.24) 5210 (80.94)
Yes 602 (20.48) 584 (17.76) 1186 (19.06)
Body mass index
Underweight 791 (27.17) 856 (27.43) 1647 (27.3)
Normal 1700 (57.41) 1705 (50) 3405 (53.54)
Overweight 377 (13.38) 551 (17.53) 928 (15.54)
Obese 68 (2.04) 170 (5.05) 238 (3.61)
Physical activity
Vigorous 654 (21.08) 374 (10.38) 1028 (15.51)
Moderate 536 (17.82) 1079 (31.74) 1615 (25.07)
Light 534 (20.43) 330 (11.43) 864 (15.74)
None 1278 (40.67) 1576 (46.45) 2854 (43.68)
Self-rated health
Very good 136 (4.45) 102 (2.83) 238 (3.61)
Good 986 (33.73) 988 (29.3) 1974 (31.42)
Moderate 1437 (46.33) 1649 (48.01) 3086 (47.21)
Bad 416 (13.93) 603 (18.46) 1019 (16.29)
Very bad 38 (1.55) 40 (1.39) 78 (1.47)
Chronic conditions
Zero 1698 (59.98) 1772 (56.37) 3470 (58.1)
Single 837 (25.25) 1019 (27.91) 1856 (26.64)
Two 319 (9.67) 442 (11.74) 761 (10.75)
Three and more 160 (5.1) 149 (3.98) 309 (4.52)
Smoking
Never 1625 (48.79) 3085 (81.83) 4710 (66.10)
Currently not 489 (14.48) 335 (8.26) 824 (11.22)
Currently smoking 1217 (36.72) 352 (9.91) 1569 (22.68)
Wealth quintile
Poorest 560 (18.62) 672 (21.1) 1232 (19.91)
Poor 543 (18.14) 628 (18.05) 1171 (18.09)
Middle 571 (18.2) 622 (18.2) 1193 (18.2)
Rich 606 (20.49) 703 (21.54) 1309 (21.04)
Richest 734 (24.55) 757 (21.12) 1491 (22.76)
Religion
Hindu 2512 (84.5) 2834 (84.96) 5346 (84.74)
Muslim 378 (12.3) 415 (11.99) 793 (12.14)
Table 1 Sample characteristics
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Page 7 of 16
Muhammad et al. Sleep Science and Practice (2024) 8:6
religion, social group, place of residence, and states did
not show signicant eects on cognitive functioning.
Discussion
e ndings of this study provide substantial evidence
for the association between sleep and cognitive func-
tioning in older individuals. Bivariate results suggest that
both poor sleep quality and reduced sleep duration were
associated with reduced cognitive performance in both
men and women. ese results are consistent with previ-
ous research and emphasize the importance of sleep for
maintaining optimal cognitive function (Engleman et al.
2000; Saint Martin et al. 2012). Sleep disturbances can
impair cognitive processes such as memory consolidation
and neural repair, resulting in cognitive decline (Aly and
Moscovitch 2010).
It is noteworthy that, across all age categories, men
consistently demonstrated higher mean cognitive func-
tioning scores. is suggests that men may be more resis-
tant than women to the cognitive eects of increasing
age. is nding is in line with other studies that found
that older women often scored less in cognitive scores as
compared to older men (Zhang 2006; Wang et al. 2020).
e ndings also reveal gender-specic dierences in
the association between sleep and cognitive function-
ing. Unlike older men, cognitive eects of poor sleep
quality and short sleep were insignicant among older
women after adjusting for health and behavioural fac-
tors. is suggests an independent association between
sleep and cognition among men but not women. Addi-
tional research is required to comprehend these gender
dierences and their underlying mechanisms. Hormonal
variations, caregiving responsibilities, and societal
expectations may contribute to men’s and women’s dis-
tinctive sleep patterns and cognitive outcomes (Roepke
and Ancoli-Israel 2010; Mallampalli and Carter 2014).
While the association between sleep and cognitive
functioning is well-established, this study extends the
understanding by examining the role of various back-
ground factors. Age was found to signicantly moderate
the relationship between sleep and cognitive functioning,
with higher cognitive decline observed among women as
age increased which is supported by previous research
(Dzierzewski et al. 2018). is nding underscores the
need for targeted interventions to support cognitive
health in older women, particularly as they age. Educa-
tion level also emerged as a signicant factor, indicating
that higher educational attainment acts as a protective
factor against cognitive decline in older adults, and in
women in particular, cohesive with the extensive body
of literature (van Hooren et al. 2007; Tripathi et al. 2014;
Muhammad et al., 2022a). Education provides individu-
als with cognitive reserve, enhances access to healthcare,
promotes engagement in mentally stimulating activities,
and encourages the adoption of healthier lifestyles. ese
factors collectively contribute to better cognitive func-
tioning and may mitigate the negative impact of poor
sleep on cognitive health, especially among women due
to gender-specic behavior and societal roles.
Marital status demonstrated signicant relationship
with cognitive functioning. Specically, widowhood was
signicantly associated with decreased levels of cognitive
functioning, though observed in both men and women.
is nding has been supported by existing research (Xu
et al. 2021). e negative impact of widowhood on cog-
nitive functioning may be attributed to factors such as
Variables Men Women Total
Others 124 (3.2) 133 (3.05) 257 (3.12)
Social group
Scheduled castes 217 (5.93) 265 (6.99) 482 (6.48)
Scheduled tribes 478 (14.4) 571 (15.18) 1049 (14.81)
Other backward classes 1397 (49.4) 1558 (49.71) 2955 (49.56)
Others 922 (30.26) 988 (28.12) 1910 (29.15)
Place of residence
Urban 580 (26.96) 711 (27.85) 1291 (27.42)
Rural 2434 (73.04) 2671 (72.15) 5105 (72.58)
States
Assam 326 (5.27) 351 (5.3) 677 (5.29)
Karnataka 274 (8.45) 340 (9.18) 614 (8.83)
Maharashtra 514 (23.07) 569 (21.52) 1083 (22.26)
Rajasthan 623 (12.26) 736 (13.1) 1359 (12.7)
Uttar Pradesh 690 (32.63) 680 (31.01) 1370 (31.78)
West Bengal 587 (18.33) 706 (19.9) 1293 (19.15)
Total 3014 (100) 3382 (100) 6396 (100)
Percentages a re weighted to account for comp lex survey design and to acc ount for population es timates
Table 1 (continued)
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Page 8 of 16
Muhammad et al. Sleep Science and Practice (2024) 8:6
Variables Men Women Total
Mean 95% Condence intervals Mean 95% Condence intervals Mean 95% Condence intervals
Sleep quality
Good 57.48 56.57, 58.38 52.18 51.38, 52.97 54.81 54.19, 55.43
Moderate 53.9 52.37, 55.43 50.1 48.95, 51.24 51.69 50.74, 52.64
Poor 52.95 49.59, 56.30 49.79 47.45, 52.14 51.42 49.30, 53.55
Sleep duration
Normal 57.28 56.06, 58.50 51.68 50.77, 52.59 54.38 53.59, 55.16
Short 51.91 50.01, 53.80 48.95 47.36, 50.54 50.34 49.09, 51.59
Long 57.01 56.05, 57.97 52.2 51.12, 53.28 54.5 53.75, 55.24
Age (in years)
50–59 62.83 61.55, 64.11 56.41 55.41, 57.42 59.07 58.23, 59.90
60–69 58.21 56.98, 59.44 53.28 52.15, 54.41 55.8 54.94, 56.65
70–79 55.83 54.05, 57.61 48.68 47.40, 49.96 52.53 51.31, 53.75
80+ 49.6 46.82,52.38 46.43 43.22, 49.64 48.22 46.13, 50.31
Level of education
No formal education 51.32 50.33, 52.31 50.4 49.69, 51.10 50.64 50.06, 51.22
Less than primary 57.18 55.07, 59.28 56.46 54.33, 58.58 56.87 55.35, 58.38
Primary 59.15 57.62, 60.67 59.75 58.06, 61.45 59.38 58.23, 60.52
Secondary 63.97 61.77, 66.17 62.49 60.14, 64.83 63.56 61.82, 65.29
High school 62.53 61.24, 63.83 67.62 63.71, 71.53 63.57 62.20, 64.95
College 67.34 64.20, 70.47 69.86 66.29, 73.42 67.85 65.26, 70.44
Current marital status
Married 59.55 58.68, 60.41 55.57 54.74, 56.40 57.8 57.18, 58.42
Widowed 53.07 51.31, 54.83 50.44 49.36, 51.52 50.97 50.03, 51.90
Others 57.22 52.84, 61.61 54.61 49.96, 59.26 56.33 52.95, 59.71
Nutritional intake (> 4 times per day)
No 58.52 57.57, 59.48 53.06 52.31, 53.81 55.63 55.01, 56.25
Yes 60.17 58.87, 61.48 56.78 55.28, 58.28 58.52 57.52, 59.53
Body mass index
Underweight 54.55 53.51, 55.60 49.6 48.47, 50.74 51.96 51.16, 52.76
Normal 60.06 58.93, 61.19 54.18 53.31, 55.05 57.2 56.45, 57.95
Overweight 63.27 61.05, 65.50 58.3 56.35, 60.24 60.35 58.83, 61.87
Obese 63.46 59.21, 67.71 59.4 56.94, 61.86 60.5 58.38, 62.62
Physical activity
Vigorous 59.44 58.25, 60.64 52.3 50.14, 54.45 56.95 55.80, 58.10
Moderate 59.67 57.55, 61.79 56.1 54.94, 57.26 57.32 56.24, 58.39
Light 60.68 58.96, 62.41 54.07 51.88, 56.26 58.19 56.80, 59.57
None 57.31 55.94, 58.68 52.41 51.49, 53.34 54.6 53.77, 55.43
Self-rated health
Very good 66.57 62.16, 70.97 60.87 57.33, 64.40 64.23 61.15, 67.32
Good 61.74 60.16, 63.32 56.12 54.86, 57.38 59 57.94, 60.06
Moderate 57.56 56.62, 58.49 53.33 52.43, 54.22 55.31 54.65, 55.97
Bad 54.08 52.42, 55.73 50.47 49.02, 51.92 51.94 50.83, 53.05
Very bad 56.2 51.92, 60.48 45.53 39.37, 51.69 50.93 46.59, 55.27
Chronic conditions
Zero 59.33 58.19, 60.48 53.26 52.36, 54.16 56.26 55.50, 57.02
Single 58.46 57.27, 59.65 53.58 52.31, 54.84 55.79 54.88, 56.70
Two 58.38 56.39, 60.38 55.62 53.98, 57.27 56.81 55.53, 58.09
Three and more 56.23 52.52, 59.93 55.67 51.77, 59.57 55.97 53.29, 58.65
Smoking
Never 58.71 57.89, 59.53 54.65 53.83, 55.47 56.05 55.33, 56.76
Currently not 58.13 56.65, 59.62 53.37 51.34, 55.41 56.24 54.79, 57.68
Currently smoking 57.85 56.68, 59.01 51.59 49.49, 53.69 56.49 55.24, 57.74
Table 2 Weighted mean scores of cognitive functioning (on a scale of 0-100) among older adults by their background characteristics
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Page 9 of 16
Muhammad et al. Sleep Science and Practice (2024) 8:6
social isolation, loss of support networks, and emotional
stress (Förster et al. 2021). ese factors may interact
with poor sleep quality or shorter sleep duration, exacer-
bating cognitive decline in older men. On the other hand,
no signicant associations were found for other mari-
tal statuses, indicating that the inuence of marital sta-
tus on cognitive functioning may be more nuanced and
multifaceted.
Nutritional intake was signicantly associated with
cognitive functioning in this study, and should not be
overlooked as an important factor for overall health and
well-being. Adequate nutrition is essential for optimal
cognitive function (Selvamani and Singh 2018; Khan
2022), and it may interact with sleep patterns and qual-
ity to inuence cognitive outcomes indirectly. Future
research should explore the complex interplay between
sleep, nutritional intake, and cognitive functioning to
gain a more comprehensive understanding of their com-
bined eects. Further, physically inactive older men had
poor cognition and older women who engaged in mod-
erate physical activity had improved cognition in this
study which corresponds to previous studies (Barha et al.
2017; Castells-Sanchez et al. 2021; Sekher and Muham-
mad 2023). Self-rated health demonstrated a signicant
eect among older men but not women, particularly in
the moderate and bad health categories. Older men who
rated their health as moderate or bad exhibited a lower
level of cognitive functioning which suggest dierential
eects of one’s perceived health on their cognitive func-
tioning among men and women which require further
investigation. Poor sleep quality or shorter sleep duration
may contribute to worse self-rated health (Frange et al.
2014; Simoes Maria et al. 2020), leading to a vicious cycle
of deteriorating cognitive health. Interventions aimed at
improving sleep quality and duration may have indirect
benets by enhancing overall health and well-being, thus
positively impacting cognitive functioning.
Our results also revealed that place of residence played
a moderating role in the association between sleep and
cognitive functioning. Notably, older men residing in
urban areas consistently displayed higher mean scores for
cognitive functioning compared to those in rural areas
which is consistent with ndings from previous research
(Xu et al. 2018; Srivastava and Muhammad 2022) ese
ndings suggest that the urban environment may provide
more favorable conditions for cognitive health, poten-
tially due to better access to healthcare facilities, social
engagement opportunities, and a more stimulating liv-
ing environment. Further research is needed to examine
the specic mechanisms underlying this association and
Variables Men Women Total
Mean 95% Condence intervals Mean 95% Condence intervals Mean 95% Condence intervals
Wealth quintile
Poorest 53.37 52.08, 54.66 50.1 48.88, 51.31 51.56 50.68, 52.45
Poor 57.4 55.54, 59.27 51.07 49.90, 52.25 54.11 52.93, 55.29
Middle 58.43 56.74, 60.13 53.09 51.29, 54.88 55.64 54.37, 56.92
Rich 59.37 57.81, 60.92 54.1 52.75, 55.45 56.55 55.49, 57.62
Richest 64 62.16, 65.84 59.77 58.27, 61.26 61.95 60.73, 63.17
Religion
Hindu 59.11 58.22, 60.00 53.57 52.83, 54.32 56.21 55.61, 56.81
Muslim 58.22 56.45, 59.99 54.58 52.88, 56.28 56.35 55.12, 57.57
Others 54.81 50.40, 59.21 54.42 52.03, 56.81 54.61 52.13, 57.09
Social group
Scheduled castes 54.42 52.42, 56.41 51.29 49.65, 52.94 52.66 51.38, 53.94
Scheduled tribes 55.74 54.40, 57.09 50.43 49.17, 51.68 52.9 51.96, 53.84
Other backward classes 59.09 57.83, 60.35 53.85 52.76, 54.94 56.35 55.49, 57.20
Others 60.85 59.43, 62.26 55.88 54.74, 57.01 58.34 57.41, 59.28
Place of residence
Urban 63.49 61.23, 65.75 56.87 54.98, 58.76 59.98 58.44, 61.52
Rural 57.15 56.60, 57.71 52.51 51.97, 53.05 54.74 54.35, 55.14
States
Assam 55.93 54.74, 57.12 52.74 51.61, 53.87 54.26 53.43, 55.09
Karnataka 55.08 53.36, 56.80 54.8 52.95, 56.65 54.93 53.65, 56.20
Maharashtra 62.62 60.17, 65.06 54.61 52.53, 56.70 58.58 56.86, 60.30
Rajasthan 58.71 57.86, 59.57 52.07 51.24, 52.90 55.14 54.51, 55.77
Uttar Pradesh 57.97 56.88, 59.06 52.23 51.02, 53.43 55.05 54.21, 55.88
West Bengal 58.41 57.02, 59.80 55.94 54.80, 57.07 57.07 56.18, 57.96
Total 58.86 58.06, 59.66 53.72 53.05, 54.39 56.18 55.64, 56.72
Table 2 (continued)
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Page 10 of 16
Muhammad et al. Sleep Science and Practice (2024) 8:6
Variables Unadjusted coecients
(95% CI)
Model 1 Model 2 Model 3
Adjusted coecients
(95% CI)
Adjusted coecients
(95% CI)
Adjusted coe-
cients (95% CI)
Sleep quality
Good Ref. Ref. Ref. Ref.
Moderate -3.85*** (-5.69 - -2.02) -2.56*** (-4.11 - -1.01) -2.82*** (-4.24 - -1.39) -2.39*** (-3.78 - -1.00)
Poor -5.09*** (-8.66 - -1.51) -3.24* (-6.48–0.0037) -3.75** (-6.63 - -0.87) -2.34 (-5.41–0.74)
Sleep duration
Normal Ref. Ref. Ref. Ref.
Short -5.43*** (-7.77 - -3.10) -3.92*** (-6.11 - -1.72) -4.35*** (-6.39 - -2.32) -4.39*** (-6.46 - -2.31)
Long 0.47 (-1.13–2.06) -0.0016 (-1.38–1.38) 0.14 (-1.10–1.38) 0.10 (-1.11–1.31)
Age (in years)
50–59 Ref. Ref. Ref. Ref.
60–69 -4.41*** (-6.11 - -2.71) -3.97*** (-5.50 - -2.43) -3.53*** (-4.88 - -2.17) -3.09*** (-4.46 - -1.72)
70–79 -6.71*** (-8.81 - -4.61) -5.16*** (-7.20 - -3.13) -5.53*** (-7.30 - -3.76) -4.49*** (-6.30 - -2.68)
80+ -12.7*** (-15.6 - -9.76) -10.4*** (-13.7 - -7.04) -10.5*** (-13.6 - -7.38) -9.22*** (-12.2 - -6.19)
Level of education
No formal education Ref. Ref. Ref. Ref.
Less than primary 5.65*** (3.43–7.88) 5.50*** (3.26–7.74) 4.59*** (2.68–6.49) 4.50*** (2.56–6.45)
Primary 7.56*** (5.82–9.30) 6.88*** (5.10–8.65) 5.78*** (4.15–7.41) 5.67*** (4.03–7.30)
Secondary 12.2*** (9.89–14.5) 11.7*** (9.46–13.9) 9.75*** (7.87–11.6) 9.77*** (7.96–11.6)
High school 10.8*** (9.27–12.4) 10.0*** (8.34–11.7) 8.71*** (6.94–10.5) 8.26*** (6.51–10.0)
College 15.5*** (12.3–18.6) 14.6*** (11.5–17.7) 12.1*** (9.25–15.0) 11.5*** (8.49–14.4)
Current marital status
Married Ref. Ref. Ref. Ref.
Widowed -6.22*** (-8.10 - -4.33) -2.89*** (-4.74 - -1.03) -2.56*** (-4.39 - -0.73) -2.52*** (-4.34 - -0.69)
Others -2.26 (-6.54–2.02) -2.29 (-6.55–1.97) -1.08 (-5.24–3.08) -0.92 (-5.16–3.32)
Nutritional intake (> 4 times per
day)
No Ref. Ref.
Yes 1.59** (0.046–3.14) -0.0016 (-1.37–1.37)
Body mass index
Underweight Ref. Ref.
Normal 5.30*** (3.83–6.77) 1.80*** (0.56–3.03)
Overweight 8.39*** (6.04–10.7) 2.18** (0.23–4.12)
Obese 8.56*** (4.38–12.7) 3.67** (0.52–6.81)
Physical activity
Vigorous Ref. Ref.
Moderate 0.23 (-2.10–2.56) -0.20 (-1.89–1.50)
Light 1.20 (-0.81–3.20) 0.39 (-1.29–2.07)
None -2.05** (-3.79 - -0.31) -1.34* (-2.77–0.088)
Self-rated health
Very good Ref. Ref.
Good -4.63** (-9.11 - -0.15) -2.63 (-5.90–0.64)
Moderate -8.64*** (-12.9 - -4.33) -3.93** (-6.98 - -0.87)
Bad -12.0*** (-16.5 - -7.49) -4.44** (-7.83 - -1.04)
Very bad -9.98*** (-15.9 - -4.09) -3.84 (-9.57–1.89)
Chronic conditions
Zero Ref. Ref.
Single -0.81 (-2.39–0.78) -0.48 (-1.67–0.71)
Two -0.88 (-3.09–1.32) -1.06 (-3.03–0.90)
Three and more -2.93 (-6.64–0.78) -2.56 (-5.66–0.54)
Smoking
Never Ref. Ref.
Currently not -1.37 (-3.24–0.50) -0.91 (-2.42–0.60)
Table 3 Estimates from general linear models of cognitive functioning by background variables among older men SAGE- 2015
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Page 11 of 16
Muhammad et al. Sleep Science and Practice (2024) 8:6
explore potential interventions to address cognitive dis-
parities between urban and rural populations. e role
of other background factors, such as chronic conditions,
smoking status, wealth quintile, religion, social group,
and states, did not show consistent signicant eects
on cognitive functioning. ese ndings suggest that
the relationship between sleep and cognitive function-
ing may be less inuenced by these background factors.
However, it is important to consider that these factors
may still indirectly impact cognitive health through their
interactions with one’s health and wellbeing.
Limitations and direction for future research
It is important to acknowledge the limitations of this
study. First, the data relied on self-reported measures,
which may be subject to recall and social desirability
biases. Future studies should consider objective mea-
sures of sleep to provide more accurate assessments.
Second, there was a higher proportion of women in our
sample without any formal education or primary educa-
tion that would aect their cognitive function and inu-
ence our ndings. ird, this study focused on a specic
set of background characteristics, and there may be other
factors, such as genetics, lifestyle factors, or comorbidi-
ties, that could inuence the relationship between sleep
and cognitive functioning. Additionally, the detrimental
impact of poor sleep quality on cognitive functioning dif-
ferently among men and women can be attributed to sev-
eral contextual and environmental factors such as indoor
air quality (Hunter et al. 2018; C.-C. Lo et al. 2022; Saenz
et al. 2021), which needs further investigation. Simi-
larly, sleep disturbances, such as frequent awakenings
and fragmented sleep were not considered in this study,
which could disrupt the restorative processes necessary
for memory consolidation and neural repair (Cellini
2017; Zisapel 2007). As documented, inadequate sleep
Variables Unadjusted coecients
(95% CI)
Model 1 Model 2 Model 3
Adjusted coecients
(95% CI)
Adjusted coecients
(95% CI)
Adjusted coe-
cients (95% CI)
Currently smoking -1.08 (-2.81–0.65) 0.35 (-0.98–1.68)
Wealth quintile
Poorest Ref. Ref. Ref.
Poor 3.90*** (1.72–6.08) 1.33 (-0.35–3.00) 1.21 (-0.50–2.92)
Middle 4.90*** (2.86–6.94) 2.16** (0.40–3.92) 1.95** (0.20–3.70)
Rich 5.81*** (3.88–7.75) 2.14** (0.31–3.98) 1.85** (0.020–3.68)
Richest 10.2*** (8.09–12.4) 3.76*** (1.75–5.77) 3.47*** (1.45–5.49)
Religion
Hindu Ref. Ref. Ref.
Muslim -0.85 (-2.75–1.04) 2.18** (0.50–3.85) 2.37*** (0.69–4.06)
Others -4.13* (-8.44–0.19) -3.49 (-8.06–1.08) -3.89* (-8.39–0.60)
Social group
Scheduled castes Ref. Ref. Ref.
Scheduled tribes 1.28 (-1.03–3.60) 1.57 (-0.50–3.64) 1.33 (-0.77–3.43)
Other backward classes 4.50*** (2.23–6.76) 1.89* (-0.011–3.79) 1.75* (-0.20–3.70)
Others 6.19*** (3.84–8.54) 2.27** (0.21–4.32) 2.20** (0.086–4.30)
Place of residence
Urban Ref. Ref. Ref.
Rural -6.10*** (-8.32 - -3.88) -2.25** (-4.07 - -0.43) -2.24** (-4.03 - -0.46)
States
Assam Ref. Ref. Ref.
Karnataka -0.78 (-2.79–1.22) -3.53*** (-5.73 - -1.33) -4.30*** (-6.50 - -2.10)
Maharashtra 6.38*** (3.77–8.98) 4.02*** (1.99–6.05) 2.77*** (0.79–4.76)
Rajasthan 2.67*** (1.26–4.07) 1.67* (-0.0037–3.35) 1.12 (-0.61–2.85)
Uttar Pradesh 1.96** (0.42–3.51) -0.59 (-2.37–1.19) -1.13 (-3.02–0.76)
West Bengal 2.43*** (0.68–4.19) -0.20 (-2.17–1.77) -0.49 (-2.57–1.59)
Constant 56.7*** (55.1–58.3) 54.8*** (51.7–57.8) 58.2*** (53.6–62.8)
Observations 3,000 3,000 2,909
R-squared 0.224 0.273 0.296
Notes: Model 1 is adjusted f or age, education and mar ital status; Model 2 is ad ditionally adjusted fo r other socio-de mographics such as hous ehold wealth quintile,
religion, s ocial group, place of re sidence and states; M odel 3 is fully adjus ted model, i.e., a dditionally adjus ted for the health va riables such as nutri tional intake, bod y
mass index , physical activit y, self-rate d health, chronic condit ions and smoking stat us; CI: Condence interva l; *** p < 0.001, ** p < 0.01, * p < 0.05
Table 3 (continued)
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Page 12 of 16
Muhammad et al. Sleep Science and Practice (2024) 8:6
Variables Unadjusted coecients
(95% CI)
Model 1 Model 2 Model 3
Adjusted coecients
(95% CI)
Adjusted coecients
(95% CI)
Adjusted coe-
cients (95% CI)
Sleep quality
Good Ref. Ref. Ref. Ref.
Moderate -2.36*** (-3.79 - -0.92) -1.30** (-2.59 - -0.017) -1.33** (-2.64 - -0.028) -0.99 (-2.27–0.29)
Poor -3.15** (-5.79 - -0.52) -1.98* (-4.30–0.34) -2.29* (-4.60–0.020) -1.74 (-4.16–0.67)
Sleep duration
Normal Ref. Ref. Ref. Ref.
Short -2.72*** (-4.64 - -0.81) -1.42 (-3.21–0.36) -1.43 (-3.31–0.44) -1.27 (-3.16–0.63)
Long 1.04 (-0.42–2.50) 0.20 (-1.05–1.45) 0.16 (-1.11–1.42) 0.53 (-0.71–1.76)
Age (in years)
50–59 Ref. Ref. Ref. Ref.
60–69 -3.01*** (-4.46 - -1.56) -1.70** (-3.02 - -0.37) -1.74*** (-3.06 - -0.42) -1.32** (-2.58
- -0.065)
70–79 -7.42*** (-8.98 - -5.86) -4.99*** (-6.64 - -3.34) -5.13*** (-6.76 - -3.50) -4.20*** (-5.82 - -2.57)
80+ -9.60*** (-12.8 - -6.37) -5.94*** (-9.35 - -2.53) -6.37*** (-9.79 - -2.94) -3.34* (-6.89–0.22)
Level of education
No formal education Ref. Ref. Ref. Ref.
Less than primary 5.83*** (3.70–7.97) 4.89*** (2.70–7.08) 3.72*** (1.55–5.89) 3.32*** (1.19–5.46)
Primary 9.02*** (7.27–10.8) 8.36*** (6.55–10.2) 6.90*** (5.06–8.73) 6.49*** (4.61–8.38)
Secondary 11.7*** (9.34–14.0) 10.6*** (8.17–13.0) 8.64*** (6.07–11.2) 7.75*** (5.08–10.4)
High school 16.6*** (12.8–20.4) 15.8*** (12.0–19.6) 13.2*** (9.49–16.9) 12.3*** (8.76–15.9)
College 18.8*** (15.3–22.2) 18.3*** (14.7–21.9) 14.9*** (11.0–18.9) 13.2*** (9.31–17.1)
Current marital status
Married Ref. Ref. Ref. Ref.
Widowed -4.92*** (-6.23 - -3.61) -1.97*** (-3.30 - -0.63) -1.89*** (-3.24 - -0.55) -1.54** (-2.83 - -0.26)
Others -0.94 (-5.48–3.59) -1.73 (-6.06–2.60) -1.02 (-5.11–3.07) 0.51 (-3.67–4.69)
Nutritional intake (> 4 times per
day)
No Ref. Ref.
Yes 3.59*** (1.97–5.20) 2.83*** (1.27–4.40)
Body mass index
Underweight Ref. Ref.
Normal 4.41*** (3.03–5.78) 2.14*** (0.81–3.48)
Overweight 8.38*** (6.22–10.5) 3.25*** (1.13–5.38)
Obese 9.44*** (6.84–12.0) 4.04*** (1.34–6.73)
Physical activity
Vigorous Ref. Ref.
Moderate 3.66*** (1.31–6.01) 2.56** (0.32–4.81)
Light 1.69 (-1.26–4.64) 2.24 (-0.58–5.06)
None 0.11 (-2.14–2.37) 0.90 (-1.33–3.13)
Self-rated health
Very good Ref. Ref.
Good -4.54** (-8.14 - -0.94) -0.64 (-4.24–2.96)
Moderate -7.23*** (-10.7 - -3.73) -1.89 (-5.44–1.67)
Bad -9.99*** (-13.7 - -6.32) -2.93 (-6.72–0.86)
Very bad -14.8*** (-21.6 - -7.97) -2.62 (-10.5–5.27)
Chronic conditions
Zero Ref. Ref.
Single 0.34 (-1.15–1.83) -0.62 (-1.97–0.73)
Two 2.28** (0.48–4.09) 0.37 (-1.33–2.07)
Three and more 2.36 (-1.46–6.19) -1.62 (-5.05–1.80)
Smoking
Never Ref. Ref.
Table 4 Estimates from general linear models of cognitive functioning by background variables among older women SAGE- 2015
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Page 13 of 16
Muhammad et al. Sleep Science and Practice (2024) 8:6
duration deprives the brain of sucient time to engage in
these crucial processes, leading to cognitive impairments
over time (Lim and Dinges 2010; Lo et al. 2016), and
these pathways should be considered in future studies.
Furthermore, sleep is closely intertwined with other
physiological and psychological factors that inuence
cognitive health, such as inammation, hormonal regu-
lation, and emotional well-being (Tartar et al. 2015;
ompson et al. 2022). Finally, stressors, such as insuf-
cient or lack of income (Muhammad et al. 2021), child
rearing/caregiving burden (Muhammad and Srivastava
2022), and absence of adult children (Muhammad et al.,
2022b), may induce mental strain, aect sleep and/or lead
to cognitive decits. erefore, future research should
delve deeper into the specic mechanisms through
which sleep disturbances aect cognitive functioning
in older adults, including potential gender dierences.
us, addressing the moderating factors identied in this
study can shed light on direction for future research and
enhancing the eectiveness of interventions targeting
sleep and cognitive functioning.
Conclusions
is study provides evidence for the signicant associa-
tion between sleep health and cognitive functioning in
older Indian adults, especially older men, with poor sleep
quality and shorter sleep duration being detrimental
to cognitive health. e ndings of this study also have
important implications for healthcare providers and
policymakers. Prioritizing the assessment and manage-
ment of sleep disturbances in older adults is crucial for
promoting healthy cognitive aging. Healthcare providers
should routinely screen for sleep quality and age-appro-
priate sleep duration in their older adult patients and
Variables Unadjusted coecients
(95% CI)
Model 1 Model 2 Model 3
Adjusted coecients
(95% CI)
Adjusted coecients
(95% CI)
Adjusted coe-
cients (95% CI)
Currently not -2.51** (-5.01 - -0.0070) -1.09 (-3.20–1.01)
Currently smoking -2.49*** (-4.35 - -0.64) 1.16 (-0.57–2.89)
Wealth quintile
Poorest Ref. Ref. Ref.
Poor 0.96 (-0.67–2.58) 0.35 (-1.37–2.06) 0.28 (-1.40–1.96)
Middle 2.91*** (0.84–4.99) 1.07 (-0.98–3.12) 1.01 (-0.99–3.01)
Rich 3.88*** (2.14–5.62) 1.36 (-0.40–3.12) 0.68 (-1.10–2.45)
Richest 9.33*** (7.48–11.2) 3.60*** (1.74–5.47) 2.94*** (1.04–4.84)
Religion
Hindu Ref. Ref. Ref.
Muslim 0.96 (-0.82–2.75) 1.78* (-0.052–3.61) 1.53 (-0.32–3.38)
Others 0.85 (-1.56–3.26) 1.52 (-0.67–3.70) 1.26 (-0.97–3.49)
Social group
Scheduled castes Ref. Ref. Ref.
Scheduled tribes -0.83 (-2.82–1.15) -0.53 (-2.52–1.45) -0.69 (-2.68–1.30)
Other backward classes 2.48** (0.58–4.37) 0.75 (-1.06–2.57) 0.54 (-1.28–2.37)
Others 4.42*** (2.49–6.34) 1.31 (-0.58–3.20) 1.21 (-0.71–3.12)
Place of residence
Urban Ref. Ref. Ref.
Rural -4.21*** (-6.09 - -2.33) -1.06 (-2.92–0.80) -0.63 (-2.43–1.17)
States
Assam Ref. Ref. Ref.
Karnataka 1.92* (-0.16–4.01) 1.75* (-0.32–3.82) 0.86 (-1.25–2.97)
Maharashtra 1.73 (-0.54–4.00) 1.29 (-0.66–3.24) 0.90 (-1.07–2.86)
Rajasthan -0.70 (-2.04–0.65) 0.69 (-0.89–2.28) 0.31 (-1.34–1.97)
Uttar Pradesh -0.56 (-2.15–1.02) 0.30 (-1.43–2.02) 0.24 (-1.57–2.06)
West Bengal 3.04*** (1.51–4.58) 2.12*** (0.53–3.71) 2.41*** (0.64–4.17)
Constant 53.7*** (52.3–55.0) 51.8*** (48.8–54.7) 49.6*** (44.6–54.6)
Observations 3,364 3,364 3,249
R-squared 0.193 0.210 0.231
Notes: Model 1 is adjusted f or age, education and mar ital status; Model 2 is ad ditionally adjusted fo r other socio-de mographics such as hous ehold wealth quintile,
religion, s ocial group, place of re sidence and states; M odel 3 is fully adjus ted model, i.e., a dditionally adjus ted for the health va riables such as nutri tional intake, bod y
mass index , physical activit y, self-rate d health, chronic condit ions and smoking stat us; CI: Condence interva l; *** p < 0.001, ** p < 0.01, * p < 0.05
Table 4 (continued)
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Page 14 of 16
Muhammad et al. Sleep Science and Practice (2024) 8:6
consider sex/gender-tailored sleep interventions as part
of cognitive health management strategies. Educational
programs aimed at improving cognitive health should be
accessible and tailored to the diverse educational back-
grounds of older adults. Social support interventions,
particularly for widowed older women, can help mitigate
the negative impact of widowhood on cognitive function-
ing. Additionally, comprehensive healthcare approaches
that address overall health, including nutritional intake
and self-rated health, should be integrated into cognitive
health promotion strategies.
Abbreviations
MCI Mild cognitive impairment
WHO-SAGE World Health Organization’s Study on global AGEing and adult
health
PSU Primary Sampling Units
WHS World Health Survey
PCA Principal components analysis
aCoef Adjusted Coecients
CI Condence Interval
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at https://doi.
org/10.1186/s41606-024-00100-z.
Supplementary Material 1
Author contributions
All authors full the criteria for authorship. Conceived and designed the
research paper: T.M.; analyzed the data: T.M.; Wrote the manuscript: T.M., A.A.
and T.V.S; Rened the manuscript: T.M. and T.V.S. All authors read, reviewed and
approved the manuscript.
Funding
The authors received no funding for this research.
Data availability
The dataset analyzed for this study is available at data repository in https://
iipsindia.ac.in/content/SAGE-wave-2
Declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate
The procedures undertaken in this study and the data collection processes
were conducted ethically per the World Medical Association’s Declaration
of Helsinki. Ethical approvals were obtained for SAGE study from the Ethics
Review Committee of the World Health Organization, the Ethics and Protocol
Review Committee of the Ghana Medical School, Accra, Ghana, the Ethics
Committee of the School of Preventive and Social Medicine, and the Russian
Academy of Medical Sciences, Moscow, Russia. Approval was also obtained
for the SAGE 1 study from the Ethics Committee of the Shanghai Municipal
Centre for Disease Control and Prevention, Shanghai, China, Institutional
Review Board of the International Institute of Population Sciences, Mumbai,
India, and nally from the Research Ethics Committee of the Human Sciences
Research Council, Pretoria, South Africa. These approvals also covered all
procedures through which written informed consent was obtained from each
participant. Condential records of participants’ consent were maintained by
SAGE. Further, written informed consents for participating in the SAGE study
were obtained from each participant.
Consent for publication
All authors consent to publish this research article.
Competing interests
The authors declare that there is no competing interest.
Author details
1Department of Human Development and Family Studies | Center for
Healthy Aging, Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA
16802, USA
2WHO-SAGE Project, International Institute for Population Sciences,
Mumbai 400088, Maharashtra, India
3Department of Family and Generations | WHO-SAGE Project,
International Institute for Population Sciences, Mumbai 400088,
Maharashtra, India
Received: 16 January 2024 / Accepted: 19 March 2024
References
Aly M, Moscovitch M. The eects of sleep on episodic memory in older
and younger adults. Memory. 2010;18(3):327–34. https://doi.
org/10.1080/09658211003601548.
Arokiasamy P, Sekher TV, Lhungdim H, Dhar M, Roy AK. Study on global AGEing
and adult health (SAGE) Wave 2 India National Report; 2020.
Barha CK, Davis JC, Falck RS, Nagamatsu LS, Liu-Ambrose T. Sex dierences in
exercise ecacy to improve cognition: a systematic review and meta-
analysis of randomized controlled trials in older humans. Front Neuroendocr.
2017;46:71–85.
Behrens A, Anderberg P, Berglund JS. Sleep disturbance predicts worse cognitive
performance in subsequent years: a longitudinal population-based cohort
study. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2023;106:104899. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
archger.2022.104899.
Bombois S, Derambure P, Pasquier F, Monaca C. Sleep disorders in aging and
dementia. J Nutr Health Aging. 2010;14(3):212–7. https://doi.org/10.1007/
s12603-010-0052-7.
Castells-Sanchez A, Roig-Coll F, Lamonja-Vicente N, Toran-Monserrat P, Pera G,
Montero P, Dacosta-Aguayo R, Bermudo-Gallaguet A, Bherer L, Erickson KI.
Sex matters in the Association between Physical Activity and Fitness with
Cognition. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2021;53(6):1252–9.
Cellini N. Memory consolidation in sleep disorders. Sleep Med Rev. 2017;35:101–12.
Cha S-E, Eun K-S. Gender dierence in sleep problems: focused on Time Use
in Daily Life of Korea. Soc Indic Res. 2014;119(3):1447–65. https://doi.
org/10.1007/s11205-013-0550-1.
Cohen-Zion M, Stepnowsky C, Johnson S, Marler M, Dimsdale JE, Ancoli-Israel S.
Cognitive changes and sleep disordered breathing in elderly: dierences
in race. J Psychosom Res. 2004;56(5):549–53. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
jpsychores.2004.02.002.
Dzierzewski JM, Dautovich N, Ravyts S. Sleep and cognition in older adults. Sleep
Med Clin. 2018;13(1):93–106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsmc.2017.09.009.
Eichling PS, Sahni J. Menopause related Sleep disorders. J Clin Sleep Med.
2005;01(03):291–300. https://doi.org/10.5664/jcsm.26347.
Engleman HM, Kingshott RN, Martin SE, Douglas NJ. Cognitive function in the
sleep apnea/hypopnea syndrome (SAHS). Sleep. 2000;23(Suppl 4):S102–8.
Findley LJ, Barth JT, Powers DC, Wilhoit SC, Boyd DG, Suratt PM. Cognitive impair-
ment in patients with obstructive sleep apnea and Associated Hypoxemia.
Chest. 1986;90(5):686–90. https://doi.org/10.1378/chest.90.5.686.
Förster F, Luppa M, Pabst A, Heser K, Kleineidam L, Fuchs A, Pentzek M, Kadusz-
kiewicz H, van der Leeden C, Hajek A, König H-H, Oey A, Wiese B, Mösch E,
Weeg D, Weyerer S, Werle J, Maier W, Scherer M, Riedel-Heller SG. The role of
social isolation and the development of Depression. A comparison of the
widowed and married Oldest Old in Germany. Int J Environ Res Public Health.
2021;18(13). https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18136986. Article 13.
Fortier-Brochu É, Beaulieu-Bonneau S, Ivers H, Morin CM. Insomnia and daytime
cognitive performance: a meta-analysis. Sleep Med Rev. 2012;16(1):83–94.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.2011.03.008.
Frange C, de Queiroz SS, da Silva Prado JM, Tuk S, de Mello MT. The impact of
sleep duration on self-rated health. Sleep Sci. 2014;7(2):107–13. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.slsci.2014.09.006.
Gagnon K, Baril A-A, Gagnon J-F, Fortin M, Décary A, Lafond C, Desautels A, Mont-
plaisir J, Gosselin N. Cognitive impairment in obstructive sleep apnea. Pathol
Biol (Paris). 2014;62(5):233–40. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.patbio.2014.05.015.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Page 15 of 16
Muhammad et al. Sleep Science and Practice (2024) 8:6
Gildner TE, Liebert MA, Kowal P, Chatterji S, Snodgrass JJ. Associations between
Sleep Duration, Sleep Quality, and cognitive test performance among older
adults from six Middle Income countries: results from the study on Global
Ageing and Adult Health (SAGE). J Clin Sleep Med. 2014;10(06):613–21.
https://doi.org/10.5664/jcsm.3782.
Guidozzi F. Sleep and sleep disorders in menopausal women. Climacteric.
2013;16(2):214–9. https://doi.org/10.3109/13697137.2012.753873.
Guidozzi F. Gender dierences in sleep in older men and women. Climacteric.
2015;18(5):715–21. https://doi.org/10.3109/13697137.2015.1042451.
Hirshkowitz M, Whiton K, Albert SM, Alessi C, Bruni O, DonCarlos L, Hazen N,
Herman J, Hillard A, Katz PJ, Kheirandish-Gozal ES, Neubauer L, O’Donnell
DN, Ohayon AE, Peever M, Rawding J, Sachdeva R, Setters RC, Vitiello B, M. V.,
Ware JC. National Sleep Foundation’s updated sleep duration recommenda-
tions: nal report. Sleep Health. 2015;1(4):233–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
sleh.2015.10.004.
Hunter JC, Handing EP, Casanova R, Kuchibhatla M, Lutz MW, Saldana S, Plass-
man BL, Hayden KM. Neighborhoods, sleep quality, and cognitive decline:
does where you live and how well you sleep matter? Alzheimer’s Dement.
2018;14(4):454–61.
Keage HAD, Banks S, Yang KL, Morgan K, Brayne C, Matthews FE. What sleep
characteristics predict cognitive decline in the elderly? Sleep Med.
2012;13(7):886–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2012.02.003.
Khan J. Nutritional status, alcohol-tobacco consumption behaviour and cogni-
tive decline among older adults in India. Sci Rep. 2022;12(1). https://doi.
org/10.1038/s41598-022-25563-x.
Krystal AD, Edinger JD. Measuring sleep quality. Sleep Med. 2008;9:S10–7. https://
doi.org/10.1016/S1389-9457(08)70011-X.
Lim J, Dinges DF. A meta-analysis of the impact of short-term sleep deprivation on
cognitive variables. Psychol Bull. 2010;136(3):375.
Lo JC, Groeger JA, Cheng GH, Dijk D-J, Chee MWL. Self-reported sleep duration and
cognitive performance in older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis.
Sleep Med. 2016;17:87–98. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2015.08.021.
Lo C-C, Liu W-T, Lu Y-H, Wu D, Wu C-D, Chen T-C, Fang Y-T, Lo Y-C, Chen Y-Y, Kang
L. Air pollution associated with cognitive decline by the mediating eects of
sleep cycle disruption and changes in brain structure in adults. Environ Sci
Pollut Res. 2022;29(35):52355–66.
Mallampalli MP, Carter CL. Exploring sex and Gender Dierences in Sleep
Health: a society for women’s Health Research Report. J Women’s Health.
2014;23(7):553–62. https://doi.org/10.1089/jwh.2014.4816.
Mattis J, Sehgal A. Circadian rhythms, Sleep, and disorders of Aging. Trends
Endocrinol Metabolism. 2016;27(4):192–203. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
tem.2016.02.003.
Maume DJ, Sebastian RA, Bardo AR. Gender, work-family responsibilities, and Sleep.
Gend Soc. 2010;24(6):746–68. https://doi.org/10.1177/0891243210386949.
Miyata S, Noda A, Iwamoto K, Kawano N, Okuda M, Ozaki N. Poor sleep quality
impairs cognitive performance in older adults. J Sleep Res. 2013;22(5):535–41.
https://doi.org/10.1111/jsr.12054.
Muhammad T, Srivastava S. Why rotational living is bad for older adults? Evidence
from a cross-sectional study in India. J Popul Ageing. 2022 Mar;15(1):61–78.
Muhammad T, Srivastava S, Sekher TV. Association of self-perceived income status
with psychological distress and subjective well-being: a cross-sectional
study among older adults in India. BMC Psychol. 2021;9(1):82. https://doi.
org/10.1186/s40359-021-00588-5.
Muhammad T, Srivastava S, Sekher TV. Assessing socioeconomic inequalities in
cognitive impairment among older adults: a study based on a cross-sectional
survey in India. BMC Geriatr. 2022a;22(1):389. https://doi.org/10.1186/
s12877-022-03076-6.
Muhammad T, Sulaiman MK, Srivastava S. Migration of adult male children
and associated depression among community-dwelling older parents: a
cross‐sectional gender analysis from Longitudinal Ageing Study in India,
2017–2018. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2022b;37(2):gps5675. https://doi.
org/10.1002/gps.5675.
Muhammad T, Meher T, Siddiqui LA. Mediation of the association between
multi-morbidity and sleep problems by pain and depressive symptoms
among older adults: evidence from the longitudinal aging study in India,
wave- 1. PLoS ONE. 2023;18(2):e0281500. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.
pone.0281500.
Nebes RD, Buysse DJ, Halligan EM, Houck PR, Monk TH. Self-reported Sleep Quality
predicts poor cognitive performance in healthy older adults. Journals Geron-
tology: Ser B. 2009;64B(2):180–7. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbn037.
Organization WH. (2020). WHO guidelines on physical activity and
sedentary behaviour. https://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/han
dle/10665/336656/9789240032170-rus.pdf.
Pace-Schott EF, Spencer RMC. (2011). Chapter 5—Age-related changes in the
cognitive function of sleep. In A. M. Green, C. E. Chapman, J. F. Kalaska, &
F. Lepore, editors, Progress in Brain Research (Vol. 191, pp. 75–89). Elsevier.
https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-444-53752-2.00012-6.
Patel S, Ram F, Patel SK, Kumar K. Association of behavioral risk factors with self-
reported and symptom or measured chronic diseases among adult popula-
tion (18–69 years) in India: evidence from SAGE study. BMC Public Health.
2019;19(1):560. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-019-6953-4.
Quan S-A, Li Y-C, Li W-J, Li Y, Jeong J-Y, Kim D-H. Gender dierences in Sleep
disturbance among Elderly koreans: Hallym Aging Study. J Korean Med Sci.
2016;31(11):1689–95. https://doi.org/10.3346/jkms.2016.31.11.1689.
Rani R, Arokiasamy P, Selvamani Y, Sikarwar A. Gender dierences in self-reported
sleep problems among older adults in six middle-income countries: a cross-
sectional study. J Women Aging. 2022;34(5):605–20. https://doi.org/10.1080/
08952841.2021.1965425.
Roepke SK, Ancoli-Israel S. Sleep disorders in the elderly. Indian J Med Res.
2010;131(2):302.
Rothman SM, Mattson MP. Sleep disturbances in Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s
diseases. Neuromol Med. 2012;14(3):194–204. https://doi.org/10.1007/
s12017-012-8181-2.
Saenz JL, Adar SD, Zhang YS, Wilkens J, Chattopadhyay A, Lee J, Wong R.
Household use of polluting cooking fuels and late-life cognitive function:
A harmonized analysis of India, Mexico, and China. Environ Int. 2021 Nov
1;156:106722.
Saint Martin M, Sforza E, Barthélémy JC, Thomas-Anterion C, Roche F. Does
subjective sleep aect cognitive function in healthy elderly subjects? The
Proof cohort. Sleep Med. 2012;13(9):1146–52. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
sleep.2012.06.021.
Sekher T, Muhammad T. (2023). Yoga/Meditation and Physical Exercises and
Associated Cognitive Function Among Older Adults: Findings from Baseline
Wave of the Longitudinal Aging Study in India. In Handbook of Aging,
Health and Public Policy (pp. 1–23). Springer Nature Singapore. https://doi.
org/10.1007/978-981-16-1914-4_162-1.
Selvamani Y, Singh P. Socioeconomic patterns of underweight and its associa-
tion with self-rated health, cognition and quality of life among older adults
in India. PLoS ONE. 2018;13(3):e0193979. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.
pone.0193979.
Simoes Maria M, Büla C, Santos-Eggimann B, Krief H, Heinzer R, Seematter-
Bagnoud L. Sleep characteristics and self-rated health in older persons. Eur
Geriatr Med. 2020;11(1):131–8. https://doi.org/10.1007/s41999-019-00262-5.
Simonelli G, Marshall NS, Grillakis A, Miller CB, Hoyos CM, Glozier N. Sleep health
epidemiology in low and middle-income countries: a systematic review and
meta-analysis of the prevalence of poor sleep quality and sleep duration.
Sleep Health. 2018;4(3):239–50.
Smith L, Oh H, Jacob L, López-Sánchez GF, Veronese N, Soysal P, Shin JI, Schuch
F, Tully MA, Butler L. Sleep problems and subjective cognitive complaints
among middle-aged and older adults in 45 low-and middle-income coun-
tries. Aging Clin Exp Res. 2022;34(6):1285–93.
Srivastava S, Muhammad T. Rural-urban dierences in food insecurity and associ-
ated cognitive impairment among older adults: ndings from a nationally
representative survey. BMC Geriatr. 2022;22(1):287. https://doi.org/10.1186/
s12877-022-02984-x.
StataCorp L. (2017). Stata statistical software: Release 15 (2017). College Station, TX:
StataCorp LP.
Tartar JL, Fins AI, Lopez A, Sierra LA, Silverman SA, Thomas SV, Craddock TJ.
Sleep restriction and delayed sleep associate with psychological health
and biomarkers of stress and inammation in women. Sleep Health.
2015;1(4):249–56.
Thompson KI, Chau M, Lorenzetti MS, Hill LD, Fins AI, Tartar JL. Acute sleep depriva-
tion disrupts emotion, cognition, inammation, and cortisol in young healthy
adults. Front Behav Neurosci. 2022;16:945661.
Tripathi R, Kumar K, Bharath S, Marimuthu P, Varghese M. Age, education and
gender eects on neuropsychological functions in healthy Indian older
adults. Dement Neuropsychologia. 2014;8(2):148–54. https://doi.org/10.1590/
s1980-57642014dn82000010.
Van Cauter E, Leproult R, Plat L. Age-related changes in slow Wave sleep and REM
sleep and relationship with growth hormone and cortisol levels in healthy
men. JAMA. 2000;284(7):861–8. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.284.7.861.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Page 16 of 16
Muhammad et al. Sleep Science and Practice (2024) 8:6
van Hooren SAH, Valentijn AM, Bosma H, Ponds RWHM, van Boxtel MPJ, Jolles J.
Cognitive functioning in healthy older adults aged 64–81: a Cohort Study
into the eects of Age, Sex, and Education. Aging Neuropsychol Cognition.
2007;14(1):40–54. https://doi.org/10.1080/138255890969483.
Vanek J, Prasko J, Genzor S, Ociskova M, Kantor K, Holubova M, Slepecky M,
Nesnidal V, Kolek A, Sova M. Obstructive sleep apnea, depression and
cognitive impairment. Sleep Med. 2020;72:50–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
sleep.2020.03.017.
Wang J, Xiao LD, Wang K, Luo Y, Li X. Gender dierences in cognitive impairment
among rural Elderly in China. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(10).
https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17103724.
Xu H, Ostbye T, Vorderstrasse AA, Dupre ME, Wu B. Place of Residence and cogni-
tive function among the Adult Population in India. Neuroepidemiology.
2018;50(3–4):119–27. https://doi.org/10.1159/000486596.
Xu P, Wei R, Cheng B, Wang A, -jun, Li X, Li H, Sun L, Du J, Sheng J, Liu K, Tao F,
Yang L. The association of marital status with cognitive function and the role
of gender in Chinese community-dwelling older adults: a cross-sectional
study. Aging Clin Exp Res. 2021;33(8):2273–81. https://doi.org/10.1007/
s40520-020-01743-5.
Zhang Z. Gender dierentials in cognitive impairment and decline of the Oldest
Old in China. Journals Gerontology: Ser B. 2006;61(2):S107–15. https://doi.
org/10.1093/geronb/61.2.S107.
Zhang L, Chen C, Zhang H, Peng B. Longitudinal associations between daytime
napping and cognitive function in Chinese older adults. Arch Gerontol Geri-
atr. 2023;107:104909. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.archger.2022.104909.
Zisapel N. Sleep and sleep disturbances: Biological basis and clinical implications.
Cell Mol Life Sci. 2007;64:1174–86.
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in
published maps and institutional aliations.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
Terms and Conditions
Springer Nature journal content, brought to you courtesy of Springer Nature Customer Service Center GmbH (“Springer Nature”).
Springer Nature supports a reasonable amount of sharing of research papers by authors, subscribers and authorised users (“Users”), for small-
scale personal, non-commercial use provided that all copyright, trade and service marks and other proprietary notices are maintained. By
accessing, sharing, receiving or otherwise using the Springer Nature journal content you agree to these terms of use (“Terms”). For these
purposes, Springer Nature considers academic use (by researchers and students) to be non-commercial.
These Terms are supplementary and will apply in addition to any applicable website terms and conditions, a relevant site licence or a personal
subscription. These Terms will prevail over any conflict or ambiguity with regards to the relevant terms, a site licence or a personal subscription
(to the extent of the conflict or ambiguity only). For Creative Commons-licensed articles, the terms of the Creative Commons license used will
apply.
We collect and use personal data to provide access to the Springer Nature journal content. We may also use these personal data internally within
ResearchGate and Springer Nature and as agreed share it, in an anonymised way, for purposes of tracking, analysis and reporting. We will not
otherwise disclose your personal data outside the ResearchGate or the Springer Nature group of companies unless we have your permission as
detailed in the Privacy Policy.
While Users may use the Springer Nature journal content for small scale, personal non-commercial use, it is important to note that Users may
not:
use such content for the purpose of providing other users with access on a regular or large scale basis or as a means to circumvent access
control;
use such content where to do so would be considered a criminal or statutory offence in any jurisdiction, or gives rise to civil liability, or is
otherwise unlawful;
falsely or misleadingly imply or suggest endorsement, approval , sponsorship, or association unless explicitly agreed to by Springer Nature in
writing;
use bots or other automated methods to access the content or redirect messages
override any security feature or exclusionary protocol; or
share the content in order to create substitute for Springer Nature products or services or a systematic database of Springer Nature journal
content.
In line with the restriction against commercial use, Springer Nature does not permit the creation of a product or service that creates revenue,
royalties, rent or income from our content or its inclusion as part of a paid for service or for other commercial gain. Springer Nature journal
content cannot be used for inter-library loans and librarians may not upload Springer Nature journal content on a large scale into their, or any
other, institutional repository.
These terms of use are reviewed regularly and may be amended at any time. Springer Nature is not obligated to publish any information or
content on this website and may remove it or features or functionality at our sole discretion, at any time with or without notice. Springer Nature
may revoke this licence to you at any time and remove access to any copies of the Springer Nature journal content which have been saved.
To the fullest extent permitted by law, Springer Nature makes no warranties, representations or guarantees to Users, either express or implied
with respect to the Springer nature journal content and all parties disclaim and waive any implied warranties or warranties imposed by law,
including merchantability or fitness for any particular purpose.
Please note that these rights do not automatically extend to content, data or other material published by Springer Nature that may be licensed
from third parties.
If you would like to use or distribute our Springer Nature journal content to a wider audience or on a regular basis or in any other manner not
expressly permitted by these Terms, please contact Springer Nature at
onlineservice@springernature.com
... Older women have poor sleep quality as compared to men (Kohanmoo et al., 2024). Gender differences have also been reported in nighttime sleep and daytime napping as predictors of mortality in older adults (Jung et al., 2013) and in the association between sleep quality and cognitive functioning (Muhammad et al., 2024). Studies also suggest that older men and woman differ in the way they acknowledge and manage sleep problems (Cohen-Mansfield & Jensen, 2005;Pradhan & Saikia, 2024;Venn et al., 2013). ...
... Studies also suggest that older men and woman differ in the way they acknowledge and manage sleep problems (Cohen-Mansfield & Jensen, 2005;Pradhan & Saikia, 2024;Venn et al., 2013). For these reasons, gender-tailored approaches are also recommended for addressing sleep problems of older adults (Muhammad et al., 2024;Chu et al., 2022). ...
Article
As adequate sleep is an indicator of healthy aging, it is of concern that in India, the prevalence of sleep disorders is significant among older women. Using a qualitative approach, we aimed to understand how older Indian women perceive, engage in, and experience sleep and the factors influencing their sleep. Eight urban-residing older women from a southern Indian district were interviewed. Three main themes emerged: perceptions about sleep and sleep disturbances, causes and consequences of disturbed sleep, and sleep practices. Results may guide contextually relevant occupational assessments and interventions to support good sleep and healthy aging of this vulnerable population.
... Insufficient and excessive sleep and poor sleep quality are associated with numerous cardiometabolic conditions, including obesity, diabetes, high blood pressure, stroke, and coronary heart disease [10,15]. They are also linked to increased risks of immunity and hormone dysregulation [16,17], falls [18], disability [19], depression [20], and cognitive disorders, including memory loss [21], cognitive impairment, and dementia [22,23]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Recent research has revealed that today’s older adults report more sleep problems than their predecessors, a trend compounded by expanding social stratification. As such, this study examined the demographic, socioeconomic, and health correlates of sleep quality and sleep duration among community-dwelling older adults in India. Methods The current study sample draws on data from 7118 respondents aged 50 years and over participating in the World Health Organization’s Study on global AGEing and adult health (WHO-SAGE) wave-2 dataset. Sleep quality (good, moderate, and poor) and sleep duration (in hours and minutes) were self-reported. Adjusted multivariable logistic regression models were employed to examine the associations between sleep quality and sleep duration and several demographic, socioeconomic, and health indicators. Results A total of 12.84% and 36.1% of older adults reported long (> 8 h) and short (< 7 h) sleep, respectively. Older adults with primary education had lower odds of poor sleep [aOR: 0.85, CI: 0.73–0.99] than peers with no formal education. The odds of poor sleep were lower among those in higher wealth quintiles than those in the poorest quintile. Older adults with higher education had higher odds of short sleep [aOR: 1.36, CI: 1.06–1.74], and those with primary education had lower odds of long sleep [aOR: 0.70, CI: 0.54–0.91] than those without formal education (base category: age-appropriate sleep, i.e., 7–8 h). Older adults who were widowed had lower odds of both short [aOR: 0.82, CI: 0.68–0.98] and long sleep [aOR:0.74, CI: 0.58–0.95] compared to those who were currently married. Older individuals with adequate nutritional intake reported lower odds of short [aOR:0.59, CI: 0.49–0.72] and higher odds of long sleep [aOR:1.52, CI: 1.20–1.93] relative to their counterparts. Older adults who reported chronic conditions and body pain had higher odds of poor sleep and short sleep than their counterparts. Conclusions We identified significant associations between several unmodifiable factors, including age, education, and marital status, and modifiable factors such as dietary intake, body pain, and pre-existing chronic ailments, and sleep quality and sleep duration. Our findings can assist health care providers and practitioners in developing a more holistic and empathic approach to care. Moreover, that several demographic, socioeconomic, and health-related factors are consequential for older adults’ sleep health suggests that early detection through screening programs and community-based interventions is vital to improving sleep among older Indians who are most susceptible to sleep problems.
Chapter
Full-text available
The treatment of cognitive deficits including dementias through the medical model has been limited due to high cost of therapy, adverse effects of the drugs used, and lack of efficacy in several cases. This chapter explored the association of yoga/meditation and moderate/vigorous physical activities with late-life cognitive functioning and the sex differences in those associations. The study was conducted on a sample of older adults aged 60 years or older, drawn from the baseline wave of Longitudinal Ageing Study in India (2017–2018). Significance level of bivariate associations between explanatory variables and outcome variable (overall cognitive score) was assessed using simple linear regression. Bar graphs and box plots are used to present the sex-stratified estimates. Multivariable linear regression was employed to test the study hypotheses. Older participants who practiced yoga/meditation reported moderate or vigorous physical activity in this study were associated with better cognitive function compared to their respective counterparts. Significant sex differences were also observed in these associations with a female disadvantage. Thus, it would be crucial for policymakers to advocate health-promotional programs such as yoga/meditation and physical exercises that enhance cognitive abilities of older individuals and women in particular and ensure active aging.
Article
Full-text available
Background There could be several possible mechanisms on how chronic conditions relate to sleep problems in older persons; for instance, pain and sleep have a strong link and depressive symptoms are similarly associated with sleep problems. The present study explored whether pain and depressive symptoms are mediators in the relationship between multi-morbidity and sleep problems among older adults. Methods Study utilized data from the Longitudinal Aging Study in India (LASI) with a sample of 31,464 older adults age 60 years and above. Multivariable logistic regression along with mediation analysis using Karlson–Holm–Breen (KHB) method was conducted. Results A proportion of 14.8% of the participants suffered from sleep problems, whereas, 22.5% and 8.7% of older adults had multi-morbidity and had depressive symptoms, respectively. Also, around 10.3% of older adults reported pain and received no medication for the relief of pain, whereas 29.3% of older adults reported pain and received some type of medication for the relief of pain. Older adults with multi-morbidity had higher odds of suffering from sleep problems [adjusted odds ratio (aOR):1.26, confidence interval (CI):1.10–1.45] than those who had no multi-morbidity. Older adults who reported pain but received no medication for the relief of pain [aOR: 1.90, CI: 1.64–2.22] or reported pain and received medication for the relief of pain [aOR: 1.82, CI:1.62–2.04] and those who had depressive symptoms [aOR: 2.21, CI:1.89–2.57%] had higher odds of suffering from sleep problems compared to those who did not report pain and had no depressive symptoms, respectively. Around 11.2% of the association of multi-morbidity with sleep problems was mediated by pain and 4.3% of such association was mediated by depressive symptoms. Conclusion Pain and depressive symptoms were found to mediate the association between multi-morbidity and sleep problems; therefore, reducing pain and depressive symptoms may be considered to improve sleep in older multi-morbid patients.
Article
Full-text available
Background : Poor sleep is a potential modifiable risk factor for later life development cognitive impairment. The aim of this study is to examine if subjective measures of sleep duration and sleep disturbance predict future cognitive decline in a population-based cohort of 60, 66, 72 and 78-year-olds with a maximal follow up time of 18 years. Methods : This study included participants from the Swedish National Study on Ageing and Care – Blekinge, with assessments 2001-2021. A cohort of 60 (n=478), 66 (n=623), 72 (n=662) and 78 (n=548) year-olds, were assessed at baseline and every 6 years until 78 years of age. Longitudinal associations between sleep disturbance (sleep scale), self-reported sleep duration and cognitive tests (Mini Mental State Examination and the Clock drawing test) were examined together with typical confounders (sex, education level, hypertension, hyperlipidemia, smoking status, physical inactivity and depression). Results : There was an association between sleep disturbance at age 60 and worse cognitive function at ages 60, 66 and 72 years in fully adjusted models. The association was attenuated after bootstrap-analysis for the 72-year-olds. The items of the sleep scale most predictive of later life cognition regarded nightly awakenings, pain and itching and daytime naps. Long sleep was predictive of future worse cognitive function. Conclusion : Sleep disturbance was associated with worse future cognitive performance for the 60-year-olds, which suggests poor sleep being a risk factor for later life cognitive decline. Questions regarding long sleep, waking during the night, pain and itching and daytime naps should be further explored in future research and may be targets for intervention.
Article
Full-text available
Cognition capacity is essentially age-dependent and it is associated with the overall well-being of an individual. The public health aspects of cognitive research primarily focus on the possible delaying of cognitive decline among the older adult population. In this context, using the most recent round of the Longitudinal Ageing Study in India, 2017–2018 data, this study examines the cognition capacity among older adults aged 45 and above subject to their nutritional health and health behaviour (tobacco and alcohol consumption). It is observed that almost one in every tenth individual (10%) above 45 years of age in India shows low cognition scores. Low cognition is much more prevalent among 60 + females than males. Around one-fifth of the underweight older adults (18%) demonstrate low cognition capacity among them. Of those older adults who consume only tobacco, 11% of them demonstrate low cognition than the rest. The partial proportional odds model estimation shows that older adults are at higher risk of developing low cognition with increasing age and beyond age 65, the individuals carry a critically higher risk to experience low cognition. The estimation also shows that with increasing age older adults are higher likely to experience poor cognition independent of nutritional status, but underweight older adults are comparatively more likely to experience low cognition followed by normal and overweight older adults. In terms of alcohol-tobacco consumption behaviour, older adults who consume both are more likely to experience low cognition with increasing age followed by ‘only alcohol consumers’, and ‘only tobacco consumers’.
Article
Full-text available
Chronic sleep deprivation has been demonstrated to diminish cognitive performance, alter mood states, and concomitantly dysregulate inflammation and stress hormones. At present, however, there is little understanding of how an acute sleep deprivation may collectively affect these factors and alter functioning. The present study aimed to determine the extent to which 24-h of sleep deprivation influences inflammatory cytokines, stress hormones, cognitive processing across domains, and emotion states. To that end, 23 participants (mean age = 20.78 years, SD = 2.87) filled out clinical health questionnaires measured by the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index, Morningness Eveningness Questionnaire, and Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale. Actigraph was worn for seven days across testing to record sleep duration. At each session participants underwent a series of measures, including saliva and blood samples for quantification of leptin, ghrelin, IL-1β, IL-6, CRP, and cortisol levels, they completed a cognitive battery using an iPad, and an emotion battery. We found that an acute sleep deprivation, limited to a 24 h period, increases negative emotion states such as anxiety, fatigue, confusion, and depression. In conjunction, sleep deprivation results in increased inflammation and decreased cortisol levels in the morning, that are accompanied by deficits in vigilance and impulsivity. Combined, these results suggest that individuals who undergo 24 h sleep deprivation will induce systemic alterations to inflammation and endocrine functioning, while concomitantly increasing negative emotions.
Article
Full-text available
Abstract Background The rapidly aging population is a major concern for countries, especially where cognitive health in older age is poor. The study examined the socioeconomic and health-related factors associated with cognitive impairment among older adults and the contribution of those factors to the concentration of low cognitive functioning among older adults from economically poor households. Methods Data this study were derived from the “Building Knowledge Base on Population Ageing in India” (BKPAI) survey, which was carried out in seven major states of India. The effective sample size for the analysis was 9176 older adults aged 60 years and above. Results from descriptive and bivariate analysis were reported in the initial stage. Multivariable logistic regression analysis was conducted to explore the associations. Additionally, the concentration index and concentration curve were used to measure socioeconomic inequality in cognitive impairment among older adults. Wagstaff decomposition was employed to explore the key contributors in the concentration index. Results Nearly 60% of older adults suffered from cognitive impairment in the study. The likelihood of cognitive impairment were higher among older adults with a low level of self-perceived income sufficiency [coefficient: 0.29; confidence interval (CI): 0.07- 0.52] compared to older adults with higher levels of perceived income status. Older adults with more than 10 years of schooling were less likely to be cognitively impaired [coefficient: -1.27; CI: − 1.50- -1.04] in comparison to those with no education. Cognitive impairment was concentrated among older adults from households with the lowest wealth quintile (concentration index (CCI): − 0.10: p
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Due to rapid urbanization, Covid-19 pandemic and increasing food prices, a higher rate of food insecurity has been observed in recent years in India. Thus, we aim to study the prevalence of food insecurity among older Indian adults and the association of food insecurity as a modifiable risk factor with late-life cognitive impairment. Method Data for this study were obtained from the recent release of the Longitudinal Ageing Study in India (2017–18). The total sample size for the study was 31,464 older adults aged 60 years and above. Cognitive functioning was measured through five broad domains (memory, orientation, arithmetic function, executive function, and object naming) adapted from the cognitive module of the US Health and Retirement Study (HRS). Descriptive statistics along with cross-tabulation were presented in the study. Additionally, multivariable logistic regression analysis was used to fulfil the objectives of the study. Results It was found that 7.7% of older adults in rural areas reduced their size of meals due to unavailability (urban, 3.2%), 41.2% of them did not eat enough food of their choice (urban, 38.3%), 6.9% were hungry but did not eat food (urban, 2.6%), 5.0% did not eat for whole day (urban, 2.2%), and 6.9% lost weight due to lack of food in their household (urban, 2.9%). It was found that older adults who did not have enough food of their choice had significantly higher odds [AOR: 1.24; CI: 1.14, 1.35] of suffering from cognitive impairment in reference to their counterparts. Similarly, the older adults who were hungry but did not eat were 30% [AOR: 1.30; CI: 1.02, 1.73] more likely to suffer from cognitive impairment in reference to their counterparts. Interaction model revealed that older adults who had food insecurity in rural areas had higher odds of cognitive impairment than older adults who had food insecurity in urban areas. Conclusion The findings of the study highlight that the food security status in older adults may bring about greater challenges due to their limited economic resources. Interventions focusing on food security may have unintended positive impacts on late-life mental wellbeing as the older age is associated with higher cognitive deficits.
Article
Full-text available
The effects of air pollution on sleep and dementia remain unclear. The objective of this study was to investigate the effects of air pollution on cognitive function as mediated by the sleep cycle. A cross-sectional study design was conducted to recruit 4866 subjects on which PSG had been performed. Fifty of them were further given a cognitive function evaluation by the MMSE and CASI as well as brain images by CT and MRI. Associations of 1-year air pollution parameters with sleep parameters, cognitive function, and brain structure were examined. We observed that O3 was associated with a decrease in arousal, an increase in the N1 stage, and a decrease in the N2 stage of sleep. NO2 was associated with an increase in the N1 stage, a decrease in the N2 stage, and an increase in REM. PM2.5 was associated with a decrease in the N1 stage, increases in the N2 and N3 stages, and a decrease in REM. The N1 and N2 stages were associated with cognitive decline, but REM was associated with an increase in cognitive function. The N1 stage was a mediator of the effects of PM2.5 on the concentration domain of the MMSE. O3 was associated with an increase in the pars orbitalis volume of the left brain. NO2 was associated with increases in the rostral middle frontal volume, supramarginal gyrus volume, and transverse temporal volume of the left brain, and the pars opercularis volume of the right brain. PM2.5 was associated with increases in the pars triangularis volume of the left brain and the fusiform thickness of the right brain. In conclusion, we observed that air pollution was associated with cognitive decline by mediating effects on the sleep cycle with changes in the brain structure in controlling executive, learning, and language functions in adults.
Article
Full-text available
Background Currently, a small body of evidence suggests that sleep problems are positively associated with subjective cognitive complaints (SCC). However, no studies on this topic exist from low- and middle-income countries (LMICs). Thus, we investigated the association between sleep problems and SCC in a large sample of middle-age and older adults from 45 LMICs. Methods Cross-sectional, predominantly nationally representative, community-based data were analyzed from the World Health Survey. Sleep problems (such as difficulties falling asleep, waking up frequently during the night or waking up too early in the morning) in the last 30 days were self-reported. Two questions on subjective memory and learning complaints in the past 30 days were used to create a SCC scale ranging from 0 (No SCC) to 100 (worse SCC). Multivariable linear regression was conducted to explore the association between sleep problems (exposure) and SCC (outcome). Results Data on 60,228 adults aged ≥ 50 years were analyzed [mean (SD) age 61.4 (9.9) years; 53.9% females]. After adjustment for potential confounders, compared to those without sleep problems, the mean SCC score for the multivariable model was 13.32 (95% CI 12.01, 14.63), 19.46 (95% CI 17.95, 20.98), 24.17 (95% CI 22.02, 26.33), and 31.39 (95% CI 28.13, 34.65) points higher for mild, moderate, severe, and extreme sleep problems, respectively. Similar results were found for analyses stratified by age and country-income level. Conclusion Sleep problems were positively associated in a dose–response manner with SCC among middle-aged and older adults in multiple LMICs. Addressing sleep problems may aid in the prevention of SCC and ultimately dementia, pending future longitudinal research.
Article
Background: Little was known about the longitudinal associations between daytime napping and cognitive function in China. Thus, the study aimed to explore the cross-sectional and the longitudinal relationship between daytime napping and cognitive performance in the elderly Chinese population. Methods: The data was from the China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study (CHARLS). Daytime napping was self-reported. Cognitive function was assessed via a structured questionnaire in two dimensions: episodic memory and mental status. Linear regression and mixed-effect model were applied to explore the association between daytime napping and cognitive function. Results: A total of 2,875 and 2,440 participants aged over 65 years were included in the cross-sectional and the longitudinal studies, respectively. In the cross-sectional study, non-nappers and extended nappers had significantly lower global cognition scores (P<0.01), as well as significantly lower scores for episodic memory (P<0.05) and mental status (P<0.01), compared with moderate nappers. In the longitudinal analysis, no napping and extended napping were significantly associated with global cognitive decline (P<0.05) and only extended napping showed the significant association for the decline in episodic memory as well as mental status (P<0.01). Limitations: Daytime napping duration was self-reported by participants. Conclusion: The study found a longitudinal association between extended napping duration and worse cognitive function.