Article

The Future of Psychology: Connecting Mind to Brain

Boston College, Massachusetts General Hospital, and Harvard Medical School.
Perspectives on Psychological Science (Impact Factor: 4.89). 07/2009; 4(4):326-339. DOI: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01134.x
Source: PubMed

ABSTRACT

Psychological states such as thoughts and feelings are real. Brain states are real. The problem is that the two are not real in the same way, creating the mind-brain correspondence problem. In this article, I present a possible solution to this problem that involves two suggestions. First, complex psychological states such as emotion and cognition an be thought of as constructed events that can be causally reduced to a set of more basic, psychologically primitive ingredients that are more clearly respected by the brain. Second, complex psychological categories like emotion and cognition are the phenomena that require explanation in psychology, and, therefore, they cannot be abandoned by science. Describing the content and structure of these categories is a necessary and valuable scientific activity.Physical concepts are free creations of the human mind, and are not, however it may seem, uniquely determined by the external world.-Einstein & Infeld (1938, p. 33)The cardinal passions of our life, anger, love, fear, hate, hope, and the most comprehensive divisions of our intellectual activity, to remember, expect, think, know, dream (and he goes on to say, feel) are the only facts of a subjective order…-James (1890, p. 195).

Download full-text

Full-text

Available from: Lisa Feldman Barrett
  • Source
    • "Emotions are considered by theorists and researchers to fall within the broader concept of affect, but are distinct from other affective phenomena, such as moods, in that they are more intense, have a clearer object-focus, a more salient cause (linked to an event and situation), and are experienced for a shorter duration (Scherer, 2005 ; Shuman & Scherer, 2014 ). A rich history of scholarly interest in emotions (e.g., Darwin, 1872 ; James, 1890 ) has produced a number of theories of emotion (for review see Gendron & Barrett, 2009 ; Gross & Barrett, 2011 ) including theories of basic emotions (e.g., Ekman, 1992 ; Ekman & Cordar, 2011 ; Izard, 1993 ), psychological construction (e.g., Barrett, 2009 ; Russell, 2003 ), and appraisal theories (e.g., Frijda, 1986 ; Lazarus, 1991 ; Scherer, 1984 ). For the purposes of this "
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract]
    ABSTRACT: Emotions serve an important role in learning and performance, yet their role in medical education has been largely overlooked. In this chapter, we examine how multiple research methodologies and measures can be used to detect and analyze emotions within authentic medical learning environments. Our goal is to highlight conceptual, methodological, and practical considerations that should be attended to by researchers, educators, and medical professionals interested in examining the role of emotions within medical education. Findings from our literature review and empirical work suggest that appraisal models that treat emotions as multi-componential (e.g., control-value theory) can provide a fruitful framework for examining links between emotions and learning. In terms of measuring emotions, self-report can be useful with respect to scalability and capturing subjective experience, whereas behavioral and physiological measures provide continuous data streams and are less susceptible to cognitive or memory biases. Other factors researchers and health sciences professionals should take into consideration when selecting a measure of emotion include: efficiency; level of granularity; and person-centered versus group-level analyses. Recent work suggests that multiple measures of emotions can be integrated into affect-aware learning technologies to aid instructional design by detecting, tracing, and modeling emotional processes during learning.
    Full-text · Chapter · Jan 2016
  • Source
    • "Also, again, motivated by technological advances, BE is likely to incorporate advances in techniques designed to measure the neuropsychological underpinnings of choice behaviour. Neuroscience and neuroeconomics already adopt the principle that a deeper understanding of any behavior requires consilience across neuroscience and psychology (Barrett, 2009), and across neuroscience and economics (Camerer & Loewenstein, 2004; Camerer & Loewenstein, & Prelec, 2005). This is likely to continue given the Human Brain Mapping project [a US project set to begin in 2014 involving the National Institutes of Health (NIH), the Defense Advanced Research Projects Agency (Darpa), and the National Science Foundation (NSF)]. "
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract]
    ABSTRACT: The addition of “behavioral” to economics has given rise to a highly successful field of research. But, is it just a fashionable new trend or is it here to stay? More to the point, how does it differ from its close relative psychology? To answer these questions, the present article considers what behavioral economics is, and where it started, with the aim of trying to forecast what the status of it will be in the future. In forecasting where behavioral economics might be heading, the argument proposed here is that the best clues can be found in psychological research. If, as has been proposed here, behavioral economics partners research trends in psychology, then the futures of both will almost certainly be moving in the same direction. Both are beginning to, and will start to rely on online tools/mobile phone applications to collect richer data revealing dynamic tends over long time horizons, and as technology continues to facilitate ways of looking at group behaviour online, then larger scale studies examining interactions amongst multiple groups of people will become the norm rather than the exception. More specifically this article speculates on the future research focus of researchers in behavioral economics and the extent to which this will overlap with psychological research on judgment and decision-making.
    Full-text · Article · Jul 2015 · Psychology
  • Source
    • "Lisa Feldman Barrett makes an interesting case for these elementary features of behaviour. She advocates reducing behaviours or emotions into basic, psychological primitives (Feltman Barrett, 2009). If we want to improve the baking of bread it is not helpful to make slices of bread to study how we can improve it. "
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract]
    ABSTRACT: Psychopharmacology has had some bad publicity lately. Frankly, there have been some major problems along the way in developing new effective drugs for psychiatric disorders. After a prolonged period of high investments but low success rates, big pharmaceutical companies seem to retract their activities in the psychopharmacology field. Yet, the burden of mental disorders is likely to keep on growing in the next decades. In this position paper, we focus on drug development for depression and anxiety disorders, to narrow the scope of the assay. We describe the current situation of the psychopharmacology field, and analyse some of the methods and paradigms that have brought us here, but which should perhaps change to bring us even further. In addition, some of the factors contributing to the current stagnation in psychopharmacology are discussed. Finally, we suggest a number of changes that could lead to a more rational strategy for central nervous system drug development and which may circumvent some of the pitfalls leading to "me too" approaches. Central to the suggested changes, is the notion that mental disorders do not lead to several symptoms, but a network of causally related symptoms convolutes into a mental disorder. We call upon academia to put these changes in the early phases of drug development into effect. Copyright © 2015. Published by Elsevier B.V.
    Full-text · Article · Mar 2015 · European journal of pharmacology
Show more