Available via license: CC BY 4.0
Content may be subject to copyright.
Characterising time-on-task effects on oscillatory and aperiodic EEG
components and their co-variation with visual task performance.
Martina Kopčanová 1, Gregor Thut2, Christopher SY Benwell1*, Christian Keitel1*
@ corresponding author - email: mkopcanova@dundee.ac.uk
* joint senior authors
Affiliations
1 Psychology, University of Dundee, Nethergate, DD1 4HN Dundee, UK | 2 Centre for Cognitive
Neuroimaging, University of Glasgow
#
Author
Affiliation
ORCID
Social Media
1
Martina Kopčanová
1
0009-0004-0300-3343
2
Gregor Thut
2
0000-0003-1313-4262
3
Christopher SY Benwell
1
0000-0002-4157-4049
4
Christian Keitel
1
0000-0003-2597-5499
@ckeitelsci.bluesky.social
(BlueSky)
Keywords
EEG, neural oscillations, alpha, time on task, reaction time
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
1
Abstract
Fluctuations in oscillatory brain activity have been shown to co-occur with variations in
task performance. More recently, part of these fluctuations has been attributed to long-term
(>1hr) monotonous trends in the power and frequency of alpha oscillations (8 – 13 Hz). Here
we tested whether these time-on-task changes in EEG activity are limited to activity in the
alpha band and whether they are linked to task performance. Thirty-six participants performed
900 trials of a two-alternative forced choice visual discrimination task with confidence ratings.
Pre- and post-stimulus spectral power (1-40Hz) and aperiodic (i.e., non-oscillatory)
components were compared across blocks of the experimental session and tested for
relationships with behavioural performance. We found that time-on-task effects on oscillatory
EEG activity were primarily localised within the alpha band, with alpha power increasing and
peak alpha frequency decreasing over time, even when controlling for aperiodic contributions.
Aperiodic, broadband activity on the other hand did not show time-on-task effects in our data
set. Importantly, time-on-task effects in alpha frequency and power explained variability in
single-trial reaction times. Moreover, controlling for time-on-task effectively removed the
relationships between alpha activity and reaction times. However, time-on-task effects did not
affect other EEG signatures of behavioural performance, including post-stimulus predictors of
single-trial decision confidence. Therefore, our results dissociate alpha-band brain-behaviour
relationships that can be explained away by time-on-task from those that remain after
accounting for it - thereby further specifying the potential functional roles of alpha in human
visual perception.
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
2
Introduction
Neural oscillations arising from fluctuations in synchronised networks represent a fundamental
characteristic of the brain across species (Buzsáki et al., 2013). This oscillatory activity,
measurable with EEG, has repeatedly been linked to cognitive functions and behaviour in
various domains (Başar et al., 2001; Fries, 2015; Thut et al., 2012; Ward, 2003). Hence, neural
oscillations have been proposed to play a crucial role in global functions such as encoding and
transfer of information between large-scale neural networks (Fries, 2015; Keitel et al., 2022).
For instance, fluctuations in both spontaneous (pre-stimulus) and stimulus-related changes in
alpha band activity (8-13Hz) have been linked to numerous behavioural outcomes in e.g.
perception (Babiloni et al., 2006; Benwell et al., 2022; Busch et al., 2009; Chaumon & Busch,
2014; Craddock et al., 2017) and memory (Hanslmayr et al., 2012; Klimesch et al., 2006), and
have been assigned different functional roles, such as functional inhibition (Foxe & Snyder,
2011) and information gating (Jensen & Mazaheri, 2010).
In addition to spontaneous and state-/stimulus-dependent fluctuations, oscillations exhibit
systematic non-stationarities on shorter timescales, in the order of seconds (Cohen, 2014) and
across the lengths of standard EEG experiments (>1h, Benwell et al., 2019) that may be of
functional importance. For instance, Benwell et al. (2019) showed evidence for partially
independent drifts in alpha power and peak frequency over the course of an ~1h experimental
session, in line with previous studies examining the effects of sustained task performance on
EEG activity (Boksem et al., 2005; Stoll et al., 2016). These findings highlight that changes in
alpha oscillations cannot be assumed to be purely stochastic (i.e., due to trial-by-trial
variability) but are rather partially deterministic, implying sources of variance that act on longer
time scales of several seconds to hours, e.g., vigilance decrements (Hemmerich et al., 2023;
Martel et al., 2014; Pershin et al., 2023) or fluctuations in arousal. Separating the relative
influences of stochastic versus deterministic sources of alpha variability on perception and
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
3
behaviour is thus crucial for understanding its functional role(s). Importantly, in cases where
the behaviour of interest also shows non-stationarity across the experiment (e.g., Benwell et
al., 2018; Doll et al., 2015; Fründ et al., 2011), any brain-behaviour relationships observed may
be confounded, arising due to time-on-task effects on both variables. Without accounting for
time-on-task, such effects may be misinterpreted to represent functional relationships (Benwell
et al., 2018; Schaworonkow et al., 2015). Alternatively, systematic changes in alpha activity
over time may represent a neural mechanism governing systematic changes in behaviour.
It remains unclear whether EEG time-on-task effects extend beyond the alpha band (though
see Arnau et al., 2021) and to what extent they differ depending on the task performed. Previous
research examining time-on-task effects on EEG activity has not considered the broadband
aperiodic component, which is superimposed with oscillatory activity in the power spectrum
(Donoghue, Haller, et al., 2020). Aperiodic activity often confounds estimates of band-specific
spectral power (Donoghue, Dominguez, et al., 2020; Kopčanová et al., 2024) raising the
possibility that previously observed alpha-band time-on-task effects can be explained by
changes in the aperiodic component. Moreover, aperiodic activity itself has been proposed to
index neural excitation/inhibition balance (Gao et al., 2017), and, accordingly, has been found
to co-vary with cognitive performance (Ouyang et al., 2020; Waschke et al., 2021).
Here we examined time-on-task related fluctuations in behaviour and oscillatory activity while
participants performed a 2-alternative forced choice visual discrimination task with confidence
ratings. This allowed us to investigate the presence of any systematic non-stationarities in
accuracy (type-1), confidence (type-2 decisions) and reaction times as well as their relationship
to spectral EEG measures. We tested for time-on-task effects across a wide 1-40Hz frequency
spectrum and parameterized the EEG spectra to determine whether any time-on-task effects
were driven by periodic or aperiodic changes. Single-trial multiple regression analyses were
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
4
used to test whether time-on-task effects confounded any observed brain-behaviour
relationships.
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
5
Methods
Participants
Thirty-seven participants completed a visual 2-AFC discrimination task while their EEG
was recorded. Participants were included if they were 18-40 years old, had normal or corrected-
to-normal vision, no colour-blindness, and no history of neurological disorders. After task
performance checks (see Behavioural analysis), 36 participants were included in the analysis
(27 female, 33 right-handed, age: M = 22.9, SD = 4.4, range = 18-35). They reported sleeping
7.3 hours on average prior to testing, and 17 wore glasses or contact lenses. We obtained written
informed consent from all participants according to the Declaration of Helsinki. All
participants were financially compensated for their time (£20). The School of Social Sciences
Ethics Committee and the University of Dundee approved the study (UoD-SHSL-PSY-TPG-
2022-220).
Task and experimental design
The study involved a 2-alternative forced choice (2-AFC) discrimination task (adapted
from Benwell et al., 2022) with confidence and attentional ratings, as well as simultaneous 32-
channel EEG recording. Participants were required to discriminate numbers from letters and
report confidence in their decisions (see Figure 1). On a subset of trials, they additionally rated
their attention to the task on a continuous scale (1 to 10) – however, this data will not be used
for the present analysis.
We used the PsychoPy software (Peirce et al., 2019) to present the stimuli on a 53x30cm
monitor with 1920x1080 resolution and 60Hz refresh rate. Recordings took place in a sound-
attenuated booth in our local EEG laboratory. Participants sat 57cm from the screen and the
room was dimly lit. Each trial begun with a black fixation circle (1x1 cm) on a gray background
presented for a duration randomly varying between 3000-3500ms. The fixation was followed
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
6
by the stimulus (2x1.3 cm), either a letter randomly selected from 18 consonants (B, C, D, F,
G, H, J, K, L, M, N, P, R, S, T, V, X, Z) or a number selected from digits 1-9. Numbers or
letters presentations were equiprobable. The stimuli were briefly presented for 2 frames (~=
33.3ms), and immediately masked with an overlay of all possible letters and numbers (duration
= 500ms, 2x1.3 cm). Difficulty was manipulated by changing the contrast of the stimuli to 5
difficulty levels, selected prior to the experiment. We created five difficulty levels by varying
the opacity of the stimuli in PsychoPy to the following levels (hardest to easiest): 0.075, 0.15,
0.225, 0.3, 0.375. This range of stimulus contrast levels was selected after running a
behavioural pilot with 15 levels (0.05-0.4, in increments of 0.025) and selecting the range with
optimal psychometric functions. The mask was followed by a response-cue fixation circle
(half-filled circle) that stayed on the screen until the participants made their 1st-order response.
They were required to use their left hand to press ‘a’ if they saw a number, or ‘s’ if they saw a
letter. Afterwards, a confidence-cue fixation appeared (fully filled circle) and remained on the
screen until participants rated their confidence. To do so, with their right hand, participants
pressed ‘k’ for confident in the choice being correct or ‘l’ for not confident. There were 900
trials in total, split into 5 blocks of 180 trials and the trial order was fully randomised within
blocks. On 15 trials within each block (75 trials total), an attentional rating scale appeared after
the participants rated their confidence. Participants were asked to what extent they were
focused on or off the task during the preceding trial on a scale of 1 to 10 (1 = complete off task
attention, 10 = complete on task attention). The attentional probes were spaced every 6-18 trials
(with delays sampled from a uniform distribution, mean delay 11 trials). To familiarise
themselves with the task, all participants completed 20 practice trials at the beginning of the
experiment with feedback. Overall, the experiment took ~2-2.5 hours including the EEG set-
up. Participants took self-paced breaks between blocks.
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
7
Figure 1. Task design. The task began with a fixation circle presented for a varying duration
of 3000-3500ms on a gray background. It was followed by the stimulus (either a number or a
letter) presented for ~33ms and quickly masked with a black rectangle for 500ms. Participants
could respond immediately. A half-filled fixation circle remained on the screen until they
responded and was followed by a black filled circle indicating that a confidence rating should
now be given. On a subset of trials participants also rated their attention on a continuous scale
from 1 to 10.
Behavioural analysis
We evaluated the effectiveness of the task difficulty manipulation by plotting the
psychometric functions for both accuracy and confidence across all stimulus contrast levels.
One participant was excluded from the study at this stage due to chance performance
throughout the task (mean proportion correct = 50%). Additionally, we tested for time-on-task
effects on behaviour by calculating the accuracy (type-1), confidence (type-2), and response
RT psychometric functions for each of the five blocks separately. To statistically test for main
effects of time-on-task (i.e., block) and stimulus contrast (i.e., difficulty) and their interaction,
we used 5x5 within-participant ANOVAs with block number and stimulus contrast level as the
independent variables and proportion correct/confident, or mean RT, as the dependent
variables. One-way ANOVAs with corrections for multiple comparisons (Tukey HSD) were
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
8
used to follow up any significant main and/or interaction effects. We applied the Greenhouse-
Geisser correction (pgg) to any models where the Mauchly’s test of sphericity indicated unequal
variances (p < .05).
EEG acquisition and pre-processing
We recorded continuous EEG with a 32-channel ActiveTwo system (Biosemi, The
Netherlands) at 512Hz sampling rate. We placed the scalp electrodes according to the 10-20
International system (Oostenveld & Praamstra, 2001) and four electrooculographic electrodes
were additionally placed at the outer canthi of each eye and above and below the right eye.
We pre-processed the EEG data offline with custom-written scripts in MATLAB
(R2022a) (Mathworks, USA), using Fieldtrip (Oostenveld et al., 2011) and EEGLAB (Delorme
& Makeig, 2004). A zero-phase 2nd-order Butterworth filter was used to apply low-pass
(100Hz) and high-pass (0.5Hz) filters to the data. We then obtained 4-second epochs spanning
from -2.5 to 1.5s relative to stimulus onset on every trial. Excessively noisy channels were
removed without interpolation after visual inspection (sample mean = 0.22 removed channels,
range = 0-3). Next, we average-referenced the data and removed excessively noisy trials with
a semi-automated artifact rejection procedure. Noisy trials were selected based on 1) extreme
amplitudes ( ± 75 µV), 2) joint probability (‘pop_jointprob’ (Delorme & Makeig, 2004), with
3.5SD single-channel limit and 3SD for global limit), 3) kurtosis (local threshold 5SD, global
threshold 3SD, ‘pop_rejkurt’ (Delorme & Makeig, 2004)), and 4) visual inspection. We then
used the ‘runica’ function in EEGLAB (Delorme & Makeig, 2004) to run an independent
component analysis (ICA) using the default settings. Afterwards, we used the Multiple Artifact
Rejection Algorithm (MARA) (Winkler et al., 2011) to semi-automatically remove the
components corresponding to ocular and muscle activity, or transient channel noise. Any
remaining noisy epochs were then removed resulting in retaining 870 of 900 trials per
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
9
participant on average (range = 824-897). Lastly, we interpolated any removed channels with
a spherical spline method to allow group analysis and plotting topographies.
EEG spectral decomposition
To obtain single-trial power spectra for both the pre-stimulus (-1 to 0s) and post-stimulus
time (0 to 1s) periods separately, we used an FFT applied to the Hanning-tapered data using
the ‘mtmfft’ function from Fieldtrip (Oostenveld et al., 2011). Prior to the FFT data were zero-
padded to a length of 10s to achieve a 0.1Hz frequency resolution across the range of 1-40Hz.
Spectral Parameterization
Single-trial power spectra were then averaged across all 32 electrodes (separately for pre-
and post-stimulus data) and used for spectral parameterization. We used the ‘specparam’
toolbox (Donoghue, Haller, et al., 2020) to isolate the periodic and aperiodic activity in the
power spectra. Parametrization was applied to the frequency range of 3-40Hz, using the ‘fixed’
mode, with a peak detection threshold of 2 SD, peak width limits set to 2-15Hz, and a maximum
number of to-be-detected peaks of 4, similar to previous studies (Donoghue, Dominguez, et
al., 2020; Donoghue, Haller, et al., 2020; Robertson et al., 2019). The single trial fits were very
good overall for both pre-stimulus spectra (grand mean R2 (error) = 0.924 (0.139)), with a range
of R2 = 0.645-0.985 across participants, and post-stimulus spectra (grand mean R2(error) =
0.923 (0.142), with a range of R2 = 0.655-0.984). The single-trial estimates of aperiodic
exponents were then extracted and used in subsequent statistical analyses.
The parameterized spectra were also used to identify single-trial oscillatory peaks falling
into an extended alpha range (6-15Hz). Where an alpha peak was detected (pre-stimulus: mean
= 823 trials, range = 671-889; post-stimulus: mean = 784 trials, range = 546-877), we extracted
its center frequency (calculated by ‘specparam’) and aperiodic-adjusted power.
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
10
Time-frequency analysis
We also performed a Fourier-based time-frequency decomposition on the artefact-free
single-trial data for each electrode using the ‘ft_freqanalysis’ function and ‘mtmconvol’
method (Fieldtrip, Oostenveld et al., 2011). We maintained temporal resolution by
decomposing overlapping 0.5-second sections of time-series data that were consecutively
shifted forward by 0.02 s. We Hanning tapered the data segments and zero-padded to a length
of 1 second to achieve a frequency resolution of 1 Hz over a range of 1-40Hz. To obtain single-
trial spectral power values, we squared the absolute values of the complex-valued time-
frequency estimates. Data epochs from -1 to 1s relative to stimulus onset were then used in
subsequent statistical analyses.
Statistical analyses
We performed all statistics in MATLAB 2022a (Mathworks, USA) using the
implementations of standard procedures (ANOVA, regression) where applicable, in
combination with custom-written code for data preparation.
Time-on-task effects on EEG power spectra
To investigate how EEG power spectra changed with time-on-task we adopted a non-
parametric cluster-based permutation approach (Maris & Oostenveld, 2007). Specifically, we
tested whether the power spectra differed significantly across the five experimental blocks.
This analysis was performed separately for pre- and post-stimulus power spectra.
First, for each participant, we computed the mean amplitude at each channel-frequency
point separately for each block. To test for systematic group-level effects, we then combined
the average amplitudes across all participants and compared them with a dependent-samples
F-test (‘ft_freqstatistics’, method: ‘montecarlo’ in Fieldtrip (Oostenveld et al., 2011)). Based
on the initial F-tests, clusters were formed by grouping all neighbouring significant F-values
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
11
(p <.05). Clusters needed to consist of at least two neighbouring electrodes. Spatially, we
defined the neighbours based on the ‘biosemi32-neighb.mat’ template from Fieldtrip
(Oostenveld et al., 2011), with a mean of 6.0 neighbours per electrode. The significance of the
summed F-values (i.e., cluster statistics) was determined against a data-driven null-distribution
obtained by repeating the whole procedure 2000 times while randomly permuting the data. The
cluster-level significance threshold was p < .05.
To test the relationships between time-on-task and the alpha peak power, frequency, and
the aperiodic exponent, we averaged the single-trial estimates of each measure for each block.
Then, a within-participant ANOVA with block as independent variable and EEG measure as
dependent variable was used to test for time-on-task effects. Again, this analysis was performed
separately for both pre- and post-stimulus EEG measure estimates.
Testing whether time-on-task accounts for EEG-behaviour relationships
To investigate whether any time-on-task effects on EEG activity affected brain-
behaviour relationships we performed a single-trial multiple regression analysis (for similar
approach see Benwell et al., 2017, 2022; Kopčanová et al., 2023). We used a hierarchical two-
stage estimation (Friston, 2008) to incorporate participant level variability into group level
statistics.
Within-participant relationships between spectral (time-frequency) power and each of
the behavioural variables (accuracy, confidence, response RT), as well as trial-order and
stimulus contrast level were tested with a multiple regression for each time-frequency-
electrode combination. This allowed us to statistically separate the unique effects of time-on-
task and behaviour on the EEG outcome. The regression model was applied using the ‘fitlm’
function using the least-squares criterion (Mathworks, USA) and was defined as follows:
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
12
𝐸𝐸𝐺 = 𝑎 + 𝑏!"#$ ∗ 𝐶𝑜𝑛𝑓𝑖𝑑𝑒𝑛𝑐𝑒 + 𝑏%&$$ ∗ 𝐷𝑖𝑓𝑓𝑖𝑐𝑢𝑙𝑡𝑦 + 𝑏!"'' ∗ 𝐴𝑐𝑐𝑢𝑟𝑎𝑐𝑦 + 𝑏() ∗𝑅𝑇
+ 𝑏)") ∗𝑇𝑟𝑖𝑎𝑙𝑂𝑟𝑑𝑒𝑟 +𝑒𝑟𝑟𝑜𝑟
We thus obtained the regression coefficients (𝑏!"#$, 𝑏!"'', 𝑏%&$$, 𝑏() , 𝑏)")) for the EEG-
behaviour/time-on-task relationships that represented the strength and direction of the
association between spectral power and each predictor while the other predictors were
controlled for. All regression models were computed without trial-order first, and then trail-
order was added as an additional predictor in a separate model to test whether accounting for
trial order would abolish any observed brain-behaviour relationships. Additionally, predictive
performance of both models was compared by first evaluating the subject-level Akaike
Information Criterion (Cavanaugh & Neath, 2019), then contrasting resulting AIC values by
means of a paired t-test (two-tailed).
Group-level statistics were computed using cluster-based permutation testing of the
obtained regression coefficients against zero (‘ft_freqstatistics’, two-tailed dependent-samples
t-test, method: ‘montecarlo’ in Fieldtrip (Oostenveld et al., 2011)). The procedure was similar
to that described above for EEG power spectra obtained from the FFT analysis with a cluster
p-value threshold = .025.
We used this multiple regression approach to examine whether any time-on-task effects
influenced the relationships between peak alpha power/frequency and behaviour as well.
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
13
Results
Behavioural results
We first evaluated the effectiveness of the stimulus contrast manipulation and time-on-
task effects on three behavioural measures: accuracy (type-1 performance), confidence (type-
2 performance), and response RTs. Figure 2 shows the psychometric functions for each
behaviour type, plotted separately for each task block.
A 5-by-5 ANOVA showed that accuracy increased with higher stimulus contrast as
expected: F(1.49,52.1) = 494.761, pgg < .0001, ηp2 = .934 (see also Figure 2A). The main effect
of time-on-task on accuracy was also significant, F(1.49,52.1) = 3.703, pgg = .018, ηp2 = .096,
whilst the interaction between block and stimulus contrast level was not significant,
F(5.95,208.4) = 1.212, pgg = .284. A follow-up one-way ANOVA with multiple comparison
correction testing the effect of block on accuracy across all evidence levels (F(2.67,93.31) =
3.703, pgg = .018, η2 = .097) showed that the proportion of correct responses increased at the
beginning of the task from block 1 to block 2 (p = .016) and from block 1 to block 3 (p =
.0008), but no other comparisons were significant (Figure 2B).
Like accuracy (type-1 performance), the proportion of confident responses also
increased as a function of stimulus contrast, F(0.88,30.63) = 372.427, pgg <.0001, ηp2 = .914
(Figure 2C). However, there was no significant main effect of time-on-task on confidence,
F(0.88,30.63) = 1.291, pgg = .282 (Figure 2D). The effect of stimulus contrast on confidence
did not vary across blocks, F(3.5,122.52) = 1.766, pgg = .071.
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
14
Figure 2: Time-on-task and stimulus contrast effects on behaviour. (A) Mean proportion
of correct responses per stimulus contrast level within each of the five blocks. Error bars denote
the between-participant standard error of the mean. (B) Mean proportion correct responses per
block. (C) Mean proportion of confident responses per stimulus contrast level in each block.
(D) Mean proportion of confident responses per block (E) Mean response RTs per stimulus
contrast level and block. (F) Mean RTs per block. * p < .05 ** p < .001 *** p < .0001
As Figure 2E shows, participants’ responses got faster with increasing stimulus contrast,
F(0.68,23.81) = 18.590, pgg <.0001, ηp2 = .347, as well as with time-on-task, F(0.68,23.81) =
35.690, pgg <.0001, ηp2 = .505. A follow up one-way ANOVA with multiple comparisons of
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
15
the main time-on-task effect on RTs (F(2.82,98.68) = 35.673, pgg <. 0001, η2 = .505) showed
that RTs were significantly slower in block 1 compared to blocks 2, 3, 4, and 5 (p’s <.0001)
and no other comparisons were significant (see also Figure 2F). Additionally, there was a
significant interaction between block and stimulus contrast on RTs, F(2.72,95.22) = 4.696, pgg
=.0006, ηp2 = .118. Follow up one-way ANOVAs with stimulus contrast as independent
variable and RT as dependent variable carried out separately at each block showed that this
interaction is driven by differences in the effect of the stimulus contrast manipulation on RTs
across blocks. Specifically, as Table S1 the Supplementary Material shows, block 4 did not
show the main effect of stimulus contrast on RTs. No differences in the effect of block on RTs
across the stimulus contrast levels were found (see Supplementary Table S2).
In summary, the task difficulty manipulation was effective. Importantly, the
improvement in accuracy and speed-up of RTs, primarily shown in comparisons between the
first and later blocks, may indicate practice effects on behaviour. Confidence ratings did not
show systematic variability between blocks.
EEG results
Time-on-task effects on spectral power are strongest in the alpha band.
We next tested time-on-task effects on the EEG power spectra across the 1-40Hz range.
Figure 3A-B plots the full scalp mean spectra for each task block separately for 1-sec pre- and
post-stimulus periods. To statistically test for any significant differences in power spectra
across all electrodes we used a cluster-based permutation F-test. We found significant clusters
in alpha frequencies in both the pre-stimulus, Fsum = 13,192, pclust = .0005 (3.7-11.9Hz), and
the post-stimulus, Fsum = 11,188, pclust =.001 (5.2- 12.4Hz), periods. Additionally, there were
significant clusters in beta frequencies, with an Fsum = 1,684 and pclust = .0435 (12.7-18.7Hz)
in the pre-stimulus period, and with an Fsum = 2,711 and pclust = .024 (13-18.9Hz) in the post-
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
16
stimulus period. The topographies in Figure 3C-D show that the grand-average spectral power
in both the alpha and beta bands showed similar topographical distributions between the first
and the last blocks, but the overall amplitudes increased with time. The alpha time-on-task
effect was significant across the whole scalp in both pre- and post-stimulus periods, whilst the
beta effect was constrained to frontal electrodes in the pre-stimulus period, and to frontal and
parieto-occipital electrodes in the post-stimulus period.
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
17
Figure 3. Time-on-task effect on 1-40Hz power spectra across blocks. (A-B) Scalp mean
grand-average power spectra plotted separately for each experimental block. The black and
gray horizontal lines indicate the frequency ranges included in the two significant clusters
found when testing the differences in amplitude across all 5 blocks with a dependent-samples
F-test (α = .05). The top inset plots the power spectra in the beta frequencies. (A) Pre-stimulus
power and (B) post-stimulus power. (C-D) Topographies of mean raw power in block 1 (top
row) and block 5 (bottom row) as well as the mean F-values. Topographies are plotted
separately for alpha and beta frequencies. The electrodes in white denote the channels that were
included in the significant alpha/beta clusters.
Figure 4 further plots the within-block changes in alpha and beta power across the
experiment. It shows that both alpha and beta band power increased linearly throughout the
experiment, however, alpha power showed greater fluctuations within the blocks than beta
power. Notably, alpha power was drastically reduced for the first trial bin in each block,
suggesting a relative ‘resetting’ of power during breaks which quickly increased again with
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
18
time-on-task. This pattern of changes was similar in the pre-stimulus (Fig. 4A) and post-
stimulus (Fig. 4B) periods.
Figure 4. Within-block evolutions of alpha and beta band power. Mean alpha and beta
power (z-scored) were calculated for each trial bin based on the significant clusters showing
the effect of block on spectral power in Figure 3. Each block of trials was split into 15 bins so
that the trial numbers in each bin were approximately equal. The number of trials in each bin
ranged from 10-12. Vertical black lines denote block onset. A pre-stimulus power. B post-
stimulus power.
Time-on-task changes in peak alpha frequency and power are oscillatory and not influenced
by aperiodic activity.
Whilst the analysis above and previous research (Benwell et al., 2019; Stoll et al., 2016)
suggest power changes with time-on-task, they do not account for potential non-stationarities
in the broadband aperiodic activity. Hence, focusing on the activity in the alpha band that
showed the strongest time-on-task alterations, we next investigated time-on-task effects on
purely oscillatory alpha activity, as parameterized by ‘specparam’ (Donoghue, Haller, et al.,
2020), as well as any possible non-stationarities in the aperiodic exponent itself.
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
19
Figure 5 Aperiodic-adjusted alpha peak power and frequency across blocks. (A) Pre-
stimulus scalp averaged mean alpha power (8-13) for each block. Vertical dashed lines denote
the point at which the adjusted-alpha power is the highest. (B) Post-stimulus aperiodic-adjusted
alpha power and its peak frequencies across blocks. (C) Pre-stimulus and (D) post-stimulus
mean ‘specparam’ identified alpha peak center frequency per block decreased significantly
with time-on-task. (E) Pre-stimulus and (F) post-stimulus mean alpha peak power increased
across blocks. Horizontal black lines indicate significant post-hoc comparisons.
Figure 5A-B shows the full-scalp mean aperiodic-adjusted power in the alpha band (8-
13Hz) for each experimental block, separately for pre- and post-stimulus windows. The vertical
dashed lines additionally illustrate the mean alpha peak frequency shift across the five task
blocks. As expected, Figure 5A-B illustrates that alpha power increased over blocks whilst
peak alpha frequency decreased in both the pre- and post-stimulus periods. To test whether
these shifts were statistically significant, we extracted the trials for which an alpha (6-15Hz)
peak was identified and compared the mean center frequency and peak power (over and above
the aperiodic component) across the five blocks with a one-way ANOVA. We found that alpha
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
20
center frequency decreased significantly with time in both the pre-stimulus, F(4,140) = 20.752,
p <.0001, η2 = .372 (all post-hoc pairwise comparisons p < .05 except block , 2 vs 3, and 4 vs
5), as well as post-stimulus windows, F(4,140) = 5.429, p = .0004, η2 = .134 (only blocks 2 and
5 differed significantly, p = .019, all other comparisons p > .05) (Figure 5C-D). In line with the
aperiodic-uncorrected results above, alpha peak power also showed a significant increase in
both the pre-stimulus, F(4,140) = 21.238, p < .0001, η2 = .378 (all post-hoc pairwise
comparisons p <.05, except block 3 vs 4, 3 vs 5, and 4 vs 5), as well as the post-stimulus period,
F(4,140) = 10.484, p < .0001, η2 = .231 (post-hoc pairwise comparisons showed block 5
differed significantly from blocks 1, 2, and 3; and block 4 differed from blocks 1 and 2) (Figure
5E-F).
Only trials with an identifiable alpha peak (identified with ‘specparam’) were included
in this analysis. Overall, trials in the second half of the experiment were more likely to contain
an alpha peak (on average, ~64% of trials with no pre-stimulus alpha were from the 1st half of
the experiment, while ~36% from the 2nd half). While this may have slightly imbalanced the
precision of the power and frequency estimates across the experiment it will not have
systematically biased them.
While aimed at confirming the oscillatory nature of the alpha-band time-on-task effects,
‘specparam’ also allowed quantifying the aperiodic component of the spectrum and testing
whether this showed similar effects. Figure 6A-B, shows the mean aperiodic exponent
estimates for each block, plotted separately for pre- and post-stimulus data. One-way within-
participant ANOVAs indicated that there were no significant time-on-task changes in either
pre-stimulus, F(4,140) = 2.032, p = .093, or post-stimulus, F(4,140) = 0.994, p = .413, period
derived aperiodic exponents.
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
21
Figure 6. Aperiodic exponent across task blocks. (A) The mean aperiodic exponent for each
block estimated for full-scalp averaged FFT spectra in the pre-stimulus period. (B) Post-
stimulus exponent over blocks.
Taken together, this indicates that the non-stationarities in the alpha band across the
experimental session are driven by oscillatory changes, whilst we found no evidence for time-
on-task effects on the broadband aperiodic activity.
Time-on-task confounds relationships between alpha activity and reaction time, but not
confidence.
Time-frequency analysis
To investigate the unique relationships between EEG power and both time-on-task and
behaviour with added temporal and spatial resolution, we performed a single-trial multiple
regression analysis on time-frequency power data spanning -1s to 1s relative to stimulus onset.
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
22
Figure 7. Unique relationships between EEG power and behaviour before and after
including time-on-task. The time-frequency plots illustrate the mean t-values (two-tailed
dependent samples t-tests of single-trial regression coefficients against zero) across all 32
electrodes that denote the strength and direction of the relationship between each behavioural
measure/trial-order and spectral power. Significant clusters (p <.025) are outlined in black. The
vertical line denotes stimulus onset. A-C Shows the unique relationships between accuracy
(A), RTs (B), and confidence (C) without controlling for time-on-task. D-G Plot these
relationships after time-on-task was controlled for. The unique effects of (D) time-on-task, (E)
accuracy, (F) RTs, and (G) confidence.
We first tested the unique relationships between EEG power and accuracy, RTs and
confidence without including time-on-task in the model. As Figure 7A shows, accuracy was
unrelated to spectral power in both pre- and post-stimulus windows. Figure 7B shows RTs
were significantly negatively linked to both pre- and post-stimulus alpha power, with a tsum = -
48,922 and pclust = .0085. Additionally, a significant positive cluster linking higher RTs to
higher pre- and post-stimulus beta and low power in the gamma frequency range was also
found, tsum = 31,563 and pclust = .0215. Higher confidence was inversely related to post-stimulus
alpha and beta power (negative cluster tsum = -21,819, pclust = .0025) and positively related to
low frequency post-stimulus power (positive cluster tsum = 12,942, pclust = .0135) (Figure 7C).
All of the above clusters included all electrodes.
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
23
To test whether any of these effects were independent from non-stationarities in the time-
frequency power across the experiment, we re-ran the regression models with trial order added
as another predictor. In line with the spectral analyses above, we found that time-frequency
resolved power increased with time-on-task in the alpha and beta bands, with this relationship
spanning across the entire pre- and post-stimulus windows in the alpha band, and being
disrupted around stimulus onset in the lower beta band (Figure 7D). This effect was significant
even after controlling for effects of the behavioural measures, tsum = 117,107 and pclust = .002.
Figure 7E confirms that no significant accuracy effects were present. As Figure 7F illustrates,
the reaction time effects on alpha and lower beta power (in both the pre- and post-stimulus
period) disappeared after the time-on-task related variance was accounted for (max negative
cluster pclust = .028). However, RTs correlated with higher frequency activity (higher beta and
low gamma power) in both pre- and post-stimulus periods after controlling for time on task
(tsum = 69,595, pclust = .001) (Figure 7F). In contrast, confidence remained a significant predictor
of post-stimulus alpha/beta desynchronisation and low frequency power even after time-on-
task was accounted for (negative cluster tsum = -17,455, pclust = .0025; positive cluster tsum =
14,377, pclust = .006; Figure 7G), suggesting these effects are more likely due to trial-by-trial
variability in spectral power rather than due to monotonous time-on-task effects. All reported
clusters included all 32 electrodes.
Overall, we find that alpha power increases with time-on-task and that this effect is
sustained throughout the whole pre- and post-stimulus (-1 to 1s) periods. Similarly, beta power
increases with time-on-task in pre- and post-stimulus windows, although this effect is
interrupted by the stimulus onset. Moreover, both these effects showed a relationship with RTs.
Importantly, these effects dissociated from the relationships between confidence ratings and
spectral power regarding frequencies, timing, sign, and dependence on time on task.
Aperiodic corrected analysis
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
24
We additionally sought to examine whether the identified time-on-task effects on purely
periodic alpha activity (both frequency and power) confounded or explained relationships with
behavioural performance. The behavioural measures were single-trial accuracy, reaction times,
and confidence ratings, while trial stimulus contrast was controlled for in the regression
models.
First, we tested the unique relationships between behaviour and both peak alpha
frequency and power without accounting for time-on-task. To do so, we performed single-trial
multiple regressions, on trials where an alpha peak was identified in the EEG data, separately
for each participant. We then compared the resulting regression coefficients against zero to test
for systematic group-level effects. As the pink bars in Figure 8 show, we found that in the pre-
stimulus period (Fig. 8A-B), RTs were significantly positively related to alpha peak frequency
(t=4.34, p<.001) and negatively related to alpha peak power (t=-3.07, p = .011). In contrast,
neither confidence nor accuracy showed significant relationships with alpha activity in the pre-
stimulus window (all p’s > .05). In the post-stimulus data (Fig. 8D-E), RTs showed a significant
positive relationship with alpha peak frequency (t=3.43, p = .008), whilst the other behavioural
measures were unrelated to alpha frequency. Post-stimulus alpha peak power, on the other
hand, was significantly negatively linked to single trial confidence ratings (t=-5.25, p <.001)
and RTs (t=-3.21, p= .011), but unrelated to accuracy (p = .198).
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
25
Figure 8. A-B Pre-stimulus and D-E post-stimulus comparisons of results of multiple
regression testing (with group-level t-tests) for unique effects of behaviour on EEG with (in
blue) and without (in pink) controlling for time-on-task (ToT). * p <.05; ** p <.001, *** p
<.0001 (after FDR multiple comparisons corrections (Benjamini & Hochberg, 1995)). C (pre-
stimulus) and F (post-stimulus) show the difference in AIC between the models without and
with ToT – positive ΔAIC denote better model fits with ToT included.
Hence, alpha power and frequency, in both pre- and post-stimulus periods, were
significantly correlated with a time-varying behavioural measure, namely response RTs.
However, given the time-on-task effects on alpha activity and response speed these
relationships may be confounded by trial order.
We therefore repeated the regression analyses whilst also including trial order as an
additional predictor in the models. The blue bars in Figure 8 show the results of the group-level
t-tests of regression coefficients against zero. We found that when time-on-task was controlled
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
26
for, neither pre-stimulus alpha peak frequency nor peak power predicted RTs (p = .572, p =
.874). However, time-on-task itself remained a significant unique predictor of both pre-
stimulus alpha frequency (t=-7.32, p <.0001) and power (t=6.32, p <.0001) even after
controlling for the effects of behavioural measures. No other significant relationships were
found with confidence or accuracy in the pre-stimulus period (all p’s > .05).
Like the pre-stimulus effects, the previously significant relationships between post-
stimulus alpha frequency (p = .264) and power (p = .065) and RTs did not reach significance
after time-on-task effects were accounted for. Additionally, post-stimulus alpha frequency
decreased significantly with time-on-task (t=-3.49, p = .005) and alpha power increased
(t=4.42, p = .0005) even when all behavioural measures were controlled for. Whilst post-
stimulus alpha frequency was unrelated to any behavioural measures (p’s > .05), alpha power
remained inversely related to confidence after accounting for time-on-task effects (t=-5.17, p
<.0001).
To formally test whether the inclusion of time-on-task significantly improved the
models, we calculated the difference in AIC values for the model fits with and without time-
on-task included (Figure 8C&F). We found that the regression models including time-on-task
had better model fit than the models without time-on-task. This result was found for both alpha
power and frequency in the pre- and post-stimulus time windows (all p’s <.005; in t-tests
comparing ∆AIC against 0 (i.e., no improvement in fit)).
Together these results suggest that time-on-task effects fully accounted for relationships
between EEG alpha activity and RTs, while post-stimulus relationships between EEG alpha
power and confidence occurred independently of time-on-task.
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
27
Discussion
We examined how time-on-task influences both periodic and aperiodic EEG activity across the
course of a typical experimental session, and whether time-on-task trends potentially confound
apparent relationships between EEG and behaviour. During performance of a visual
discrimination task, neural time-on-task trends were found in the alpha (8-12Hz), and beta
bands (13-20Hz). Specifically, alpha (and beta) power systematically increased over the course
of task performance, whereas alpha frequency systematically decreased. The effects represent
changes in oscillatory rather than aperiodic activity: aperiodic activity did not vary
systematically across the experiment. Importantly, time-on-task confounded apparent pre- and
post-stimulus relationships between single-trial alpha activity and reaction times which were
no longer significant after time-on-task was accounted for. These results establish the
oscillatory nature of long-term EEG non-stationarities in the alpha band and emphasise the
need to account for time-on-task when testing oscillatory brain-behaviour relationships. We
argue that controlling for time-on-task ultimately allows for a better understanding of the
functional roles of oscillations in cognition as it may remove potentially spurious correlations
and allow to tease apart different oscillatory contributors to behavioural variability.
The increases in alpha and beta power across task blocks are in line with previous studies that
found time-on-task spectral changes (Benwell et al., 2019; Boksem et al., 2005; Cajochen et
al., 1995; Sadaghiani et al., 2010; Stoll et al., 2016). These power increases across longer
timescales have previously been linked to fatigue and lapses in sustained attention (Boksem et
al., 2005; Cajochen et al., 1995). Additionally, Benwell et al. (2019) showed that alpha band
non-stationarities likely arise from different underlying mechanisms, with potentially separable
functions, as some alpha components showed changes in power, frequency, or combination of
both in their study. Here, by examining the time-on-task effects across a wider frequency
spectrum, we confirm the previous findings and show that alpha band activity experiences a
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
28
relatively steep increase, accompanied by a shallower increase in the beta band (see Figure 4).
Whilst our task did not elicit a decline in performance across the experimental session (i.e.,
more errors/longer response times) with time-on-task on the group level, it is possible that the
alpha power increase may at least partially reflect increasing fatigue within-participants across
the experimental session.
The increases in beta power are also somewhat in line with Stoll et al.’s (2016) findings of
time-on-task effects on beta activity in monkeys, who argued that this trend may reflect
increasing attentional effort required to maintain cognitive performance. It remains unclear
whether the beta effect is a distinct phenomenon or a consequence of alpha harmonics that
carry the alpha power trend into the beta band. However, the differences in slope, with alpha
power showing a steeper increase, and different topographical representations may suggest at
least partially independent processes.
Our results show that systematic non-stationarities in alpha band activity are driven by
oscillatory changes and not due to alterations in the aperiodic signal. This is important given
that previous time-on-task studies (Benwell et al., 2019; Boksem et al., 2005; Cajochen et al.,
1995; Sadaghiani et al., 2010) did not account for the aperiodic exponent, which has been
shown to confound spectral results regarding EEG changes with aging (Merkin et al., 2021;
Tröndle et al., 2022), cognitive performance (Ouyang et al., 2020), and disease states (Johnston
et al., 2023; Pani et al., 2022; Robertson et al., 2019). We found no significant changes in the
aperiodic exponent across blocks. On the physiological level, this implies that the EEG non-
stationarities are unlikely to be due to changes in asynchronous neural spiking (Manning et al.,
2009) or alterations in the balance between excitatory and inhibitory synaptic currents (Gao et
al., 2017), which have both been linked to the aperiodic component. Instead, our results suggest
that time-on-task related changes arise from alteration in neural synchrony in widespread
cortical networks which are thought to give rise to oscillations (Buzsáki et al., 2013; Buzsáki
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
29
& Draguhn, 2004). The concurrent increases in alpha power and decreases in alpha frequency
have previously been attributed to a single process and proposed to reflect fluctuations in the
background excitability (Himmelstoss et al., 2015). However, Benwell et al (2019) showed that
the changes in alpha power and frequency arise from several distinct generating sources with
separable spatial and spectral characteristics. Our results may therefore reflect changes in one
or more oscillators that vary over the course of an experimental session. Considering the
numerous functional roles ascribed to alpha activity, including task-related functional
inhibition (Foxe & Snyder, 2011), information integration and transfer (Griffiths et al., 2019;
Jensen & Mazaheri, 2010), and sensory sampling (Samaha & Postle, 2015), the functional
relevance of the time-varying alpha activity remains to be determined.
Our results highlight the importance of considering time-on-task effects when interpreting
brain-behaviour relationships. In addition to non-stationarities in the EEG signal, we found that
participants’ accuracy improved, and response times became faster over the initial blocks of
the experiment, which could be due to practice effects, whereas confidence remained
unchanged. Previous studies have found that links between some behavioural measures and
EEG activity can be at least partially attributed to deterministic sources of variance, like time-
on-task related shifts in both EEG and behaviour (Benwell et al., 2018; Bompas et al., 2015;
Schaworonkow et al., 2015). Our results are in line with this, as we found that time-on-task
effects confounded relationships between alpha activity and reaction times. When time-on-task
has not been accounted for, numerous studies have found links between reaction times and
alpha power (Dockree et al., 2007; Kelly et al., 2009; Roberts et al., 2014), as well as frequency
(Jin et al., 2006), across a variety of tasks. Accordingly, we also found that higher alpha power
was linked to faster RTs, whereas higher alpha frequency was related to faster RTs across both
pre- and post-stimulus windows. However, adding time-on-task as a regressor in our model
explained away the direct relationship between alpha power and response speed completely.
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
30
This was true in both spatially resolved time-frequency analyses as well as in scalp-averaged
analyses in which only purely oscillatory activity was included. This finding highlights how
correlations between two measures affected by time-on-task can arise spuriously and can be
misinterpreted as a direct functional relationship. Hence, we strongly advise including time-
on-task as a default control parameter when studying oscillation-behaviour links.
In addition to time-on-task related EEG effects, we also found that alpha and beta
desynchronisation uniquely predicted single-trial decision confidence independently of
accuracy, RTs, stimulus contrast, and trial-order. This is in line with previous studies that
showed post-stimulus alpha/beta power correlates with subjective judgements like confidence
and perceptual awareness (Benwell et al., 2022; Faivre et al., 2018; Kopčanová et al., 2023)
and is unrelated to accuracy (Benwell et al., 2022; Kopčanová et al., 2023). Given the
independence of this effect from RTs, it is unlikely it can solely be attributed to motor
preparation (Faivre et al., 2018). Instead, it may reflect fluctuations in excitability and global
arousal (Dahl et al., 2022; Kosciessa et al., 2021) that uniquely affect confidence and not
accuracy (Allen et al., 2016). Importantly, the finding that this effect persists after controlling
for time-on-task, i.e., is not a spurious consequence of both alpha/beta and confidence changing
over time, highlights that this approach can help separating spurious from genuine effects of
neural oscillations on cognitive function. It may also indicate the existence of different alpha-
generating processes that tie in with distinct physiological and cognitive processes.
In addition to time-on-task independent alpha/beta effects, we also found that low frequency
(delta/theta) power predicted single trial confidence judgements independently of time-on-task
and other behavioural measures. It is likely that the positive correlations between delta/theta
power and confidence reflect the previously found relationships between slow ERPs (e.g.,
P300) and accumulating decision evidence as well as confidence judgements (Feuerriegel et
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
31
al., 2022; Gherman & Philiastides, 2018; Herding et al., 2019; Kelly & O’Connell, 2013;
Kopčanová et al., 2023; Trajkovic et al., 2024).
Finally, we also found slower reaction times were linked to increases in high beta/low gamma
power even after time-on-task was controlled for. Increased pre-stimulus gamma power has
previously been linked to slower RTs (Reinhart et al., 2011), whereas others have found a
negative relationship between gamma and RTs (Hoogenboom et al., 2010). Whilst our results
are more in line with Reinhart et al.'s (2011) who attributed it to a state of preparedness that
leads to more complete stimulus processing and slower motor responses, we cannot exclude
the possibility that this effect is driven by muscle-related activity in the current study.
Nevertheless, these effects overall demonstrate that EEG-behaviour relationships outside of the
alpha/beta band are unlikely to reflect deterministic variance due to time-on-task.
Conclusion
We found strong systematic time-on-task trends in the alpha band, with weaker effects present
in the beta band. These trends were absent in aperiodic activity, corroborating earlier findings
of long-term changes in the activity of cortical oscillators during sustained task performance.
Our findings also emphasise the importance of accounting for time-on-task when investigating
brain-behaviour relationships, as monotonous changes in both EEG and behavioural measures
may drive correlations that can be misinterpreted. Finally, we provide evidence that time-
varying performance-related alpha/beta effects can be dissociated depending on whether they
can be accounted for by time on task, here: response speed, or whether they persist after
controlling for it, here: decision confidence.
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank Efstratios Koukouvinis for his help with data collection.
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
32
Funding
MK is funded by an SGSSS (UKRI ESRC: ES/P000681/1) stipend awarded to authors CK, CB
and GT. All authors are members of the Scottish-EU Critical Oscillations Network (SCONe),
funded by the Royal Society of Edinburgh (RSE Saltire Facilitation Network Award to CK,
Reference Number 1963).
Author contributions
Contributions coded according to the CRediT taxonomy (Brand et al. 2015).
Conceptualization: MK, CB, CK; Data curation: MK; Formal analysis: MK; Funding
acquisition: CB, CK, GT; Investigation: MK; Methodology: All authors. Project
administration: MK, CB, CK; Resources: CK, CB; Software: MK, CB, CK; Supervision: CK,
CB, GT; Validation: CB, CK; Visualization: MK, CK, CB; Writing – original draft: MK, CB,
CK; Writing – review & editing: All authors. Final version of the manuscript approved by all
authors.
Declaration of conflicting interests
The authors declare that there were no competing interests with respect to the authorship or
publication of this research article.
Data and code availability
Data and code are openly available online. The data can be accessed through OSF (here), and
the code can be found on GitHub (here).
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
33
References
Allen, M., Frank, D., Schwarzkopf, D. S., Fardo, F., Winston, J. S., Hauser, T. U., &
Rees, G. (2016). Unexpected arousal modulates the influence of sensory noise on confidence.
eLife, 5, e18103. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.18103
Arnau, S., Brümmer, T., Liegel, N., & Wascher, E. (2021). Inverse effects of time-on-
task in task-related and task-unrelated theta activity. Psychophysiology, 58(6), e13805.
https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13805
Babiloni, C., Vecchio, F., Bultrini, A., Romani, G. L., & Rossini, P. M. (2006). Pre- and
poststimulus alpha rhythms are related to conscious visual perception: A high-resolution EEG
study. Cerebral Cortex, 16(12), 1690–1700. https://doi.org/10.1093/CERCOR/BHJ104
Başar, E., Başar-Eroglu, C., Karakaş, S., & Schürmann, M. (2001). Gamma, alpha, delta,
and theta oscillations govern cognitive processes. International Journal of Psychophysiology:
Official Journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology, 39(2–3), 241–248.
https://doi.org/10.1016/s0167-8760(00)00145-8
Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the False Discovery Rate: A Practical
and Powerful Approach to Multiple Testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B
(Methodological), 57(1), 289–300. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x
Benwell, C. S. Y., Coldea, A., Harvey, M., & Thut, G. (2022). Low pre-stimulus EEG
alpha power amplifies visual awareness but not visual sensitivity. European Journal of
Neuroscience. https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.15166
Benwell, C. S. Y., Keitel, C., Harvey, M., Gross, J., & Thut, G. (2018). Trial-by-trial co-
variation of pre-stimulus EEG alpha power and visuospatial bias reflects a mixture of stochastic
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
34
and deterministic effects. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 48(7), 2566–2584.
https://doi.org/10.1111/EJN.13688
Benwell, C. S. Y., London, R. E., Tagliabue, C. F., Veniero, D., Gross, J., Keitel, C., &
Thut, G. (2019). Frequency and power of human alpha oscillations drift systematically with
time-on-task. Neuroimage, 192, 101–101.
https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2019.02.067
Benwell, C. S. Y., Tagliabue, C. F., Veniero, D., Cecere, R., Savazzi, S., & Thut, G.
(2017). Prestimulus EEG Power Predicts Conscious Awareness But Not Objective Visual
Performance. eNeuro, 4(6). https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0182-17.2017
Boksem, M. A. S., Meijman, T. F., & Lorist, M. M. (2005). Effects of mental fatigue on
attention: An ERP study. Cognitive Brain Research, 25(1), 107–116.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.04.011
Bompas, A., Sumner, P., Muthumumaraswamy, S. D., Singh, K. D., & Gilchrist, I. D.
(2015). The contribution of pre-stimulus neural oscillatory activity to spontaneous response
time variability. NeuroImage, 107, 34–45. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.11.057
Busch, N. A., Dubois, J., & VanRullen, R. (2009). The phase of ongoing EEG
oscillations predicts visual perception. The Journal of Neuroscience : The Official Journal of
the Society for Neuroscience, 29(24), 7869–7876. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0113-
09.2009
Buzsáki, G., & Draguhn, A. (2004). Neuronal Oscillations in Cortical Networks. Science,
304(5679), 1926–1929. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1099745
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
35
Buzsáki, G., Logothetis, N., & Singer, W. (2013). Scaling Brain Size, Keeping Timing:
Evolutionary Preservation of Brain Rhythms. Neuron, 80(3), 751–764.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2013.10.002
Cajochen, C., Brunner, D. P., Krauchi, K., Graw, P., & Wirz-Justice, A. (1995). Power
Density in Theta/Alpha Frequencies of the Waking EEG Progressively Increases During
Sustained Wakefulness. Sleep, 18(10), 890–894. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/18.10.890
Cavanaugh, J. E., & Neath, A. A. (2019). The Akaike information criterion: Background,
derivation, properties, application, interpretation, and refinements. WIREs Computational
Statistics, 11(3), e1460. https://doi.org/10.1002/wics.1460
Chaumon, M., & Busch, N. A. (2014). Prestimulus neural oscillations inhibit visual
perception via modulation of response gain. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 26(11), 2514–
2529. https://doi.org/10.1162/JOCN_A_00653
Cohen, M. X. (2014). Fluctuations in Oscillation Frequency Control Spike Timing and
Coordinate Neural Networks. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(27), 8988–8998.
https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0261-14.2014
Craddock, M., Poliakoff, E., El-deredy, W., Klepousniotou, E., & Lloyd, D. M. (2017).
Pre-stimulus alpha oscillations over somatosensory cortex predict tactile misperceptions.
Neuropsychologia, 96, 9–18. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA.2016.12.030
Dahl, M. J., Mather, M., & Werkle-Bergner, M. (2022). Noradrenergic modulation of
rhythmic neural activity shapes selective attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 26(1), 38–
52. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.TICS.2021.10.009
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
36
Delorme, A., & Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of
single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. Journal of
Neuroscience Methods, 134(1), 9–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.JNEUMETH.2003.10.009
Dockree, P. M., Kelly, S. P., Foxe, J. J., Reilly, R. B., & Robertson, I. H. (2007). Optimal
sustained attention is linked to the spectral content of background EEG activity: Greater
ongoing tonic alpha (∼10 Hz) power supports successful phasic goal activation. European
Journal of Neuroscience, 25(3), 900–907. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05324.x
Doll, R. J., Veltink, P. H., & Buitenweg, J. R. (2015). Observation of time-dependent
psychophysical functions and accounting for threshold drifts. Attention, Perception, &
Psychophysics, 77(4), 1440–1447. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-015-0865-x
Donoghue, T., Dominguez, J., & Voytek, B. (2020). Electrophysiological Frequency
Band Ratio Measures Conflate Periodic and Aperiodic Neural Activity. eNeuro, 7(6).
https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0192-20.2020
Donoghue, T., Haller, M., Peterson, E. J., Varma, P., Sebastian, P., Gao, R., Noto, T.,
Lara, A. H., Wallis, J. D., Knight, R. T., Shestyuk, A., & Voytek, B. (2020). Parameterizing
neural power spectra into periodic and aperiodic components. Nature Neuroscience 2020
23:12, 23(12), 1655–1665. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-020-00744-x
Faivre, N., Filevich, E., Solovey, G., Kühn, S., & Blanke, O. (2018). Behavioral,
Modeling, and Electrophysiological Evidence for Supramodality in Human Metacognition.
Journal of Neuroscience, 38(2), 263–277. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0322-17.2017
Feuerriegel, D., Murphy, M., Konski, A., Mepani, V., Sun, J., Hester, R., & Bode, S.
(2022). Electrophysiological correlates of confidence differ across correct and erroneous
perceptual decisions. NeuroImage, 259, 119447.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.119447
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
37
Foxe, J., & Snyder, A. (2011). The Role of Alpha-Band Brain Oscillations as a Sensory
Suppression Mechanism during Selective Attention. Frontiers in Psychology, 2.
https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00154
Fries, P. (2015). Rhythms for Cognition: Communication through Coherence. Neuron,
88(1), 220–235. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEURON.2015.09.034
Friston, K. (2008). Hierarchical Models in the Brain. PLOS Computational Biology,
4(11), e1000211. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000211
Fründ, I., Haenel, N. V., & Wichmann, F. A. (2011). Inference for psychometric
functions in the presence of nonstationary behavior. Journal of Vision, 11(6), 16.
https://doi.org/10.1167/11.6.16
Gao, R., Peterson, E. J., & Voytek, B. (2017). Inferring synaptic excitation/inhibition
balance from field potentials. NeuroImage, 158, 70–78.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.06.078
Gherman, S., & Philiastides, M. G. (2018). Human VMPFC encodes early signatures of
confidence in perceptual decisions. eLife, 7, e38293. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.38293
Griffiths, B. J., Mayhew, S. D., Mullinger, K. J., Jorge, J., Charest, I., Wimber, M., &
Hanslmayr, S. (2019). Alpha/beta power decreases track the fidelity of stimulus specific
information. eLife, 8. https://doi.org/10.7554/ELIFE.49562
Hanslmayr, S., Staudigl, T., & Fellner, M. C. (2012). Oscillatory power decreases and
long-term memory: The information via desynchronization hypothesis. Frontiers in Human
Neuroscience, 0(MARCH 2012), 1–20.
https://doi.org/10.3389/FNHUM.2012.00074/BIBTEX
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
38
Hemmerich, K., Lupiáñez, J., Luna, F. G., & Martín-Arévalo, E. (2023). The mitigation
of the executive vigilance decrement via HD-tDCS over the right posterior parietal cortex and
its association with neural oscillations. Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y.: 1991), 33(11), 6761–
6771. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhac540
Herding, J., Ludwig, S., von Lautz, A., Spitzer, B., & Blankenburg, F. (2019). Centro-
parietal EEG potentials index subjective evidence and confidence during perceptual decision
making. NeuroImage, 201. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116011
Himmelstoss, N. A., Brötzner, C. P., Zauner, A., Kerschbaum, H. H., Gruber, W.,
Lechinger, J., & Klimesch, W. (2015). Prestimulus amplitudes modulate P1 latencies and
evoked traveling alpha waves. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9.
https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00302
Hoogenboom, N., Schoffelen, J.-M., Oostenveld, R., & Fries, P. (2010). Visually induced
gamma-band activity predicts speed of change detection in humans. NeuroImage, 51(3), 1162–
1167. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.03.041
Jensen, O., & Mazaheri, A. (2010). Shaping functional architecture by oscillatory alpha
activity: Gating by inhibition. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4.
https://doi.org/10.3389/FNHUM.2010.00186
JIN, Y., O’HALLORAN, J. P., PLON, L., SANDMAN, C. A., & POTKIN, S. G. (2006).
Alpha Eeg Predicts Visual Reaction Time. International Journal of Neuroscience, 116(9),
1035–1044. https://doi.org/10.1080/00207450600553232
Johnston, P. R., McIntosh, A. R., & Meltzer, J. A. (2023). Spectral slowing in chronic
stroke reflects abnormalities in both periodic and aperiodic neural dynamics. NeuroImage:
Clinical, 37, 103277. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2022.103277
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
39
Keitel, C., Ruzzoli, M., Dugué, L., Busch, N. A., & Benwell, C. S. Y. (2022). Rhythms
in cognition: The evidence revisited. European Journal of Neuroscience, 55(11–12), 2991–
3009. https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.15740
Kelly, S. P., Gomez-Ramirez, M., & Foxe, J. J. (2009). The strength of anticipatory
spatial biasing predicts target discrimination at attended locations: A high-density EEG study.
European Journal of Neuroscience, 30(11), 2224–2234. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-
9568.2009.06980.x
Kelly, S. P., & O’Connell, R. G. (2013). Internal and External Influences on the Rate of
Sensory Evidence Accumulation in the Human Brain. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(50).
https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3355-13.2013
Klimesch, W., Doppelmayr, M., & Hanslmayr, S. (2006). Upper alpha ERD and absolute
power: Their meaning for memory performance. Progress in Brain Research, 159, 151–165.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(06)59010-7
Kopčanová, M., Ince, R. A. A., & Benwell, C. S. Y. (2023). Two distinct stimulus-locked
EEG signatures reliably encode domain-general confidence during decision formation (p.
2023.04.21.537831). bioRxiv. https://doi.org/10.1101/2023.04.21.537831
Kopčanová, M., Tait, L., Donoghue, T., Stothart, G., Smith, L., Flores-Sandoval, A. A.,
Davila-Perez, P., Buss, S., Shafi, M. M., Pascual-Leone, A., Fried, P. J., & Benwell, C. S. Y.
(2024). Resting-state EEG signatures of Alzheimer’s disease are driven by periodic but not
aperiodic changes. Neurobiology of Disease, 190, 106380.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nbd.2023.106380
Kosciessa, J. Q., Lindenberger, U., & Garrett, D. D. (2021). Thalamocortical excitability
modulation guides human perception under uncertainty. Nature Communications 2021 12:1,
12(1), 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-021-22511-7
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
40
Manning, J. R., Jacobs, J., Fried, I., & Kahana, M. J. (2009). Broadband Shifts in Local
Field Potential Power Spectra Are Correlated with Single-Neuron Spiking in Humans. Journal
of Neuroscience, 29(43), 13613–13620. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2041-09.2009
Maris, E., & Oostenveld, R. (2007). Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-
data. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 164(1), 177–190.
https://doi.org/10.1016/J.JNEUMETH.2007.03.024
Martel, A., Dähne, S., & Blankertz, B. (2014). EEG predictors of covert vigilant
attention. Journal of Neural Engineering, 11(3), 035009. https://doi.org/10.1088/1741-
2560/11/3/035009
Mathewson, K. E., Gratton, G., Fabiani, M., Beck, D. M., & Ro, T. (2009). To See or
Not to See: Prestimulus α Phase Predicts Visual Awareness. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(9),
2725–2732. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3963-08.2009
Merkin, A., Sghirripa, S., Graetz, L., Smith, A. E., Hordacre, B., Harris, R., Pitcher, J.,
Semmler, J., Rogasch, N. C., & Goldsworthy, M. (2021). Age differences in aperiodic neural
activity measured with resting EEG (p. 2021.08.31.458328). bioRxiv.
https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.08.31.458328
Oostenveld, R., Fries, P., Maris, E., & Schoffelen, J. M. (2011). FieldTrip: Open source
software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data.
Computational Intelligence and Neuroscience, 2011. https://doi.org/10.1155/2011/156869
Oostenveld, R., & Praamstra, P. (2001). The five percent electrode system for high-
resolution EEG and ERP measurements. Clinical Neurophysiology, 112(4), 713–719.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S1388-2457(00)00527-7
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
41
Ouyang, G., Hildebrandt, A., Schmitz, F., & Herrmann, C. S. (2020). Decomposing alpha
and 1/f brain activities reveals their differential associations with cognitive processing speed.
NeuroImage, 205, 116304. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116304
Pani, S. M., Saba, L., & Fraschini, M. (2022). Clinical applications of EEG power spectra
aperiodic component analysis: A mini-review. Clinical Neurophysiology, 143, 1–13.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2022.08.010
Peirce, J., Gray, J. R., Simpson, S., MacAskill, M., Höchenberger, R., Sogo, H., Kastman,
E., & Lindeløv, J. K. (2019). PsychoPy2: Experiments in behavior made easy. Behavior
Research Methods, 51(1), 195–203. https://doi.org/10.3758/S13428-018-01193-Y
Pershin, I., Candrian, G., Münger, M., Baschera, G.-M., Rostami, M., Eich, D., & Müller,
A. (2023). Vigilance described by the time-on-task effect in EEG activity during a cued
Go/NoGo task. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 183, 92–102.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2022.11.015
Reinhart, R. M. G., Mathalon, D. H., Roach, B. J., & Ford, J. M. (2011). Relationships
between pre-stimulus gamma power and subsequent P300 and reaction time breakdown in
schizophrenia. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 79(1), 16–24.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.08.009
Roberts, D. M., Fedota, J. R., Buzzell, G. A., Parasuraman, R., & McDonald, C. G.
(2014). Prestimulus Oscillations in the Alpha Band of the EEG Are Modulated by the
Difficulty of Feature Discrimination and Predict Activation of a Sensory Discrimination
Process. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 26(8), 1615–1628.
https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00569
Robertson, M. M., Furlong, S., Voytek, B., Donoghue, T., Boettiger, C. A., & Sheridan,
M. A. (2019). EEG power spectral slope differs by ADHD status and stimulant medication
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
42
exposure in early childhood. Journal of Neurophysiology, 122(6), 2427–2437.
https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00388.2019
Sadaghiani, S., Scheeringa, R., Lehongre, K., Morillon, B., Giraud, A.-L., &
Kleinschmidt, A. (2010). Intrinsic Connectivity Networks, Alpha Oscillations, and Tonic
Alertness: A Simultaneous Electroencephalography/Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging
Study. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(30), 10243–10250.
https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1004-10.2010
Samaha, J., & Postle, B. R. (2015). The Speed of Alpha-Band Oscillations Predicts the
Temporal Resolution of Visual Perception. Current Biology, 25(22), 2985–2990.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.10.007
Schaworonkow, N., Blythe, D. A. J., Kegeles, J., Curio, G., & Nikulin, V. V. (2015).
Power-law dynamics in neuronal and behavioral data introduce spurious correlations. Human
Brain Mapping, 36(8), 2901–2914. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22816
Stoll, F. M., Wilson, C. R. E., Faraut, M. C. M., Vezoli, J., Knoblauch, K., & Procyk, E.
(2016). The Effects of Cognitive Control and Time on Frontal Beta Oscillations. Cerebral
Cortex, 26(4), 1715–1732. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhv006
Thut, G., Miniussi, C., & Gross, J. (2012). The Functional Importance of Rhythmic
Activity in the Brain. Current Biology, 22(16), R658–R663.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.06.061
Trajkovic, J., Di Gregorio, F., Thut, G., & Romei, V. (2024). Transcranial magnetic
stimulation effects support an oscillatory model of ERP genesis. Current Biology: CB, 34(5),
1048-1058.e4. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2024.01.069
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint
43
Tröndle, M., Popov, T., Dziemian, S., & Langer, N. (2022). Decomposing the role of
alpha oscillations during brain maturation. eLife, 11, e77571.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.77571
Ward, L. M. (2003). Synchronous neural oscillations and cognitive processes. Trends in
Cognitive Sciences, 7(12), 553–559. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.TICS.2003.10.012
Waschke, L., Donoghue, T., Fiedler, L., Smith, S., Garrett, D. D., Voytek, B., & Obleser,
J. (2021). Modality-specific tracking of attention and sensory statistics in the human
electrophysiological spectral exponent. eLife, 10, e70068. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.70068
Winkler, I., Haufe, S., & Tangermann, M. (2011). Automatic Classification of Artifactual
ICA-Components for Artifact Removal in EEG Signals. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 7(1),
1–15. https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-7-30/FIGURES/9
.CC-BY 4.0 International licensemade available under a
(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is
The copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted April 24, 2024. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.19.590227doi: bioRxiv preprint