ArticlePDF Available

Statistical analysis of flood risk perception: a case study for Eastern Black Sea Basin, Turkey

Springer Nature
Natural Hazards
Authors:

Abstract

It is very essential in terms of flood risk management to consider social expectations such as risk perception, flood awareness, preparedness, and socio-economic dynamics together with engineering designs. Understanding the way people perceive flood risk can enhance our capability of improving existing flood risk management methods, thus helps us creating disaster resilient societies. In this study, results of a questionnaire which was used for a previous study and had been administered to participants from Eastern Black Sea Region of Turkey were further investigated using statistical methods. The main aim was to understand how demographic factors such as age, gender and education level affect people’s flood risk perception. It was also desired to see that whether they were aware of the parties responsible for taking mitigation measures, or whether they know about possible flood mitigation measures or not. Using the same data with the previous study, but in addition using SPSS software to do statistical analysis, questionnaire results were investigated using convenient statistical tests for each parameter, analysis results were interpreted, and conclusions were drawn. Same tests were conducted using weight coefficients adopted using a certain methodology which is explained in the paper, in order to make a better investigation. Also, results were compared with the results of the previous study. It was seen that there were some consistencies and contradictions between the results of the previous study and this study’s results.
ORIGINAL PAPER
Received: 7 December 2023 / Accepted: 2 March 2024 / Published online: 21 April 2024
© The Author(s) 2024
Tuğçe Anılan
tugcekoc@ktu.edu.tr
Selahattin Bayram
bayram.selahattin@hotmail.com
Mahmut Cenk Sayıl
cenksyl@gmail.com
Osman Yüksek
osmanyk20@gmail.com
1 Karadeniz Technical University Civil Engineering Department, Trabzon, Türkiye
61080, Turkey
2 Bursa Uludağ University Civil Engineering Department, Bursa, Türkiye 16059, Turkey
Statistical analysis of ood risk perception: a case study for
Eastern Black Sea Basin, Turkey
TuğçeAnılan1· SelahattinBayram1· Mahmut CenkSayıl1· OsmanYüksek2
Natural Hazards (2024) 120:8743–8760
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11069-024-06548-7
Abstract
It is very essential in terms of ood risk management to consider social expectations such
as risk perception, ood awareness, preparedness, and socio-economic dynamics together
with engineering designs. Understanding the way people perceive ood risk can enhance
our capability of improving existing ood risk management methods, thus helps us creat-
ing disaster resilient societies. In this study, results of a questionnaire which was used
for a previous study and had been administered to participants from Eastern Black Sea
Region of Turkey were further investigated using statistical methods. The main aim was
to understand how demographic factors such as age, gender and education level aect
people’s ood risk perception. It was also desired to see that whether they were aware
of the parties responsible for taking mitigation measures, or whether they know about
possible ood mitigation measures or not. Using the same data with the previous study,
but in addition using SPSS software to do statistical analysis, questionnaire results were
investigated using convenient statistical tests for each parameter, analysis results were
interpreted, and conclusions were drawn. Same tests were conducted using weight coef-
cients adopted using a certain methodology which is explained in the paper, in order to
make a better investigation. Also, results were compared with the results of the previous
study. It was seen that there were some consistencies and contradictions between the re-
sults of the previous study and this study’s results.
Keywords Risk perception · Flood awareness · Demographic factors · SPSS software
1 3
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Natural Hazards (2024) 120:8743–8760
1 Introduction
Throughout the entire world, oods are one of the most frequently encountered natural
disasters and are highly destructive. Especially in Turkey, oods cause loss of life and
damages to properties as much as earthquakes do and are in the second place in terms of
destructiveness. For these reasons, it is of vital importance for societies to employ a series
of measures, called “ood risk management” to reduce the possible undesired consequences
of oods.
While ood risk management could only be consisting of some structural and non-struc-
tural measures that can be taken in order for us to mitigate ood damages, unless some
social factors aecting people’s responses before, during and after oods are not considered,
the desired mitigation might just not be achieved since it is argued that correct behavioral
responses people show might enable us to reduce ood damages at a ratio of 80% (Groth-
mann and Reusswig 2006; Santoro et al. 2022). Hence, ood risk management policies have
been evolving to a state where social factors aecting people’s responses to ood, such as
ood risk perception, awareness and preparedness are also included in addition to the tradi-
tional measures (Lechowska 2022; Santoro et al. 2022). It can be argued that the way people
perceive ood risks, level of knowledge they have on ooding, their awareness about the
results of ooding and actions they are willing to take to be prepared for the disaster are as
important as any of the structural or non-structural ood mitigation measures.
Flood risk is dened using dierent approaches in the literature. In general, it is said that
it has three main components, namely ood hazard, vulnerability, and ood risk (Oubenna-
ceur et al. 2022). Flood hazard is the possible life and property losses that might be caused
by future oods and vulnerability might be explained as the potential risk associated with
lack of mitigation measures in a region which is ood prone (Liu et al. 2022). Flood risk
can be thought of as a combined eect of all negative consequences of oods and vulner-
ability. Flood risk perception, on the other hand, is a subjective concept; it can be dened
as the way people perceive the extent of the negative consequences of oods (Bubeck et al.
2012; Becker et al. 2014). That is why it can dier from person to person, being aected by
some demographic, psychological factors, and other factors such as ood experience. It can
be said that demographic factors such as age, gender, education level and income level are
highly determinant for ood risk perception.
The perceived probability and seriousness of a threat are both factors in risk perception.
For initiating the risk-reduction process and motivating action, it is regarded as a legitimate
predictor variable (Becker et al. 2014). The perception of ood threats has gained impor-
tance among policymakers recently who are concerned with risk management and safety
issues. Bubeck et al. (2012) examines inuences on private ood mitigation behavior have
been reviewed. Risk awareness analysis and strategy design and implementation are also
crucial steps in ood risk management, as they facilitate response to ood warnings and
the development of initiatives to improve community preparedness (Bodoque et al. 2019).
Dealing with ood hazard and risk, it is important to employ methodologies that combine
knowledge from natural and social sciences, which in turn promotes the ongoing discussion
on socio-hydrology. Communities have various options available to them, including both
non-structural and structural measures, to reduce the risk of ooding (Fuchs et al. 2017;
Bera and Danek 2018; Rana et al. 2020).
1 3
8744
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Natural Hazards (2024) 120:8743–8760
Modern ood risk management considers understanding how the public perceives risk to
be essential since it directs the creation of eective and ecient ood mitigation solutions.
In order to searched for to examine at risk perception and disaster preparedness, information
about respondents’ sociodemographic characteristics and life experiences was also gath-
ered. The majority of respondents appeared to be fairly well equipped to handle a future
ood disaster according to the overall ndings. The results of correlational and regression
analysis showed that risk perception and disaster preparedness were positively correlated
(Miceli et al. 2008; Kellens et al. 2011). Additionally, prior experiences and demographic
characteristics, followed by civil society and the inuence of public institutions, have the
greatest impact on how people perceive the risk of ooding. Given that these are variables
that can be improved, Ardaya et al. (2017) concentrated on the analysis of civil society and
governmental institutions’ inuence. To create a complete framework for ood risk, it is
ideal to consider all potential harms (direct and indirect), including the social, psychologi-
cal, and environmental eects of ood losses (Shen et al. 2020; Oubennaceur et al. 2022).
Numerous connections between the aforementioned parameters have been discovered in
earlier investigations. For instance, it has been discovered that demographics, psychometric
variables, or prior ood experiences might predict behavior, awareness, risk perception, and
the possibility of future oods (Diakakis et al. 2018; Lechowska 2018; Huang et al. 2020).
Eryılmaz Türkkan and Hırca (2021) suggests building a ood risk framework for the social
characteristics of individuals that would make ood management plans more ecient and
sustainable.
In Anılan and Yüksek (2017), in order to evaluate the way people perceive ood risk and
level of knowledge they have on ooding, a questionnaire consisting of four Yes/No and
two open-ended questions was designed and administered to over 1000 participants from
Eastern Black Sea Basin (EBSB) in Turkey. After eliminating invalid responses, a group of
data gathered from 897 participants was obtained. Using these data, ood risk perception
level of the people and their knowledge about possible ood mitigation measures and par-
ties responsible for taking them were determined. As independent variables, demographic
factors age, gender, and education level, and also ood experience and expectations were
used to determine risk perception. However only graphs were obtained, percentage distribu-
tion of responses was given, and conclusions were drawn.
In this study, which can be thought of an extension of the mentioned previous study,
it was aimed to use statistical methods to further investigate the eects of some demo-
graphic and other factors on ood risk perception and mitigation measure knowledge of
897 participants, using the data obtained in Anılan and Yüksek (2017), and also to make
a comparison between the results of this study and of Anılan and Yüksek (2017). Data set
was analyzed using dierent statistical tests through SPSS software. Independent Sample
T-test was employed to determine whether ood risk perception (FRP) level was aected
by gender, ood experience, the fact that the resident is under risk, damage expected after
a probable future ood and belief in parties which are responsible for damage mitigation.
One-way ANOVA and Post Hoc tests were conducted to see whether FRP level was aected
by age and education level; while two-way ANOVA and Post Hoc tests were used to exam-
ine the combined eects of education level and age, education level and gender, and age
and gender on FRP level. Since Likert Scale had not been employed in Anılan and Yüksek
(2017), data was digitized using dierent methods to make it possible to use it in SPSS soft-
ware. Same analyses were conducted in two ways: In one, weighting coecients were not
1 3
8745
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Natural Hazards (2024) 120:8743–8760
used and in the other they were. A comparison of results obtained by the analyses without
and with weighting coecients was made; also a comparison of the results of this study and
of them was made.
2 Data collected and methodology
2.1 Questionnaire design
In Anılan and Yüksek (2017), a questionnaire which was administered to 897 people was
used to investigate the ood risk perception and knowledge level of participants from East-
ern Black Sea Basin (EBSB) of Turkey. Flood disasters frequently occur in the EBSB. This
region is classied as a ood basin, and it poses a serious threat to residents in the nearby
settlement area. It has a mountainous topography and high amount of precipitation. The
detailed features and maps of the basin can be found in (Anılan et al. 2020).
The questionnaire included six items, four of them being Yes/No questions, and the rest
being open ended (Table 1). In this study, it is aimed to further investigate perceptions of
ood risk and ood damage mitigation measures of those participants through statistical
methods using SPSS software. Since the questionnaire used in Anılan and Yüksek (2017)
did not employ Likert Scale, in order for the data to be used for a statistical analysis, it had
to be converted into scaled data, that is, it had to be numerical. For these reasons, to digitize
the data, an approach based on Liu and Li (2015) and Liu et al. (2018) was taken as basis.
Since demographic factors such as age, gender and education level are of utmost importance
for the determination of FRP scores, they were also considered.
With the rst four questions, it was tried to determine the participants’ ood experiences,
personal awareness, risk and concern perception and potential of coping prociency with
Yes/No questions. In the fth open-ended question given to measure the level of knowl-
edge, options stream improvement, aorestation, preventing settlement in stream beds or
relocation, infrastructure works, awareness of public and other were completed. In the 6th
open-ended question asked to measure expectations, options state, municipality, citizen,
general directorate of state hydraulic works, district governorate, governorate and other
were given. It was possible to make an explanation with the other option in questions 5 and
Questions Purpose Choice
Have you ever experienced a ood? Flood
experience
Yes/no
Do you think that your residence is under
risk?
Personal
awareness
Yes/
No
If a ood occurs, do you think that
you will experience any loss of life or
property?
Risk and con-
cern perception
Yes/
No
Do you believe that any measure may be
taken to mitigate ood damages?
Potential
of coping
prociency
Yes/
No
Which measures may be taken to mitigate
ood damages?
Knowledge
level
Open
ended
Who is responsible for the measure? Expectation Open
ended
Table 1 Determinants of ood
risk perception (N = 897)
1 3
8746
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Natural Hazards (2024) 120:8743–8760
6. The demographic structure of the participants was determined according to age, gender
and educational status which is presented in Table 2.
2.2 Fundamental statistical analysis
In Tables 3 and 4, the fundamental statistical results and frequency analyzes of the answers
are given for the rst 4 questions, respectively. According to the answers given, it was
revealed that most of the participants had experienced (62%) oods before and considered
themselves at risk of oods (60%). In addition, a large majority of the participants think that
they will experience losses in case of any ood (90%) and that precautions can be taken to
prevent this situation (85%).
Flood
experience
Personal
awareness
Risk and
concern
perception
Potential
of coping
prociency
NValid 896 893 897 871
Miss-
ing
14 0 26
Fre-
quen-
cy
No 341 (38.0%) 354 (39.5%) 354 (17.4%) 129
(14.4%)
Yes 555 (61.9%) 539 (60.1%) 539 (82.6%) 742
(82.7%)
Table 4 Frequency analysis for
Yes/No questions (Q1-Q4)
Question Yes No
Have you ever experienced a ood? 62 38
Do you think that your residence is under risk? 60 40
If a ood occurs, do you think that you’ll experience any
loss of life or property?
90 10
Do you believe that any measure may be taken to miti-
gate ood damages?
85 15
Table 3 Fundamental statistics
(%) for Yes/No questions (1–4),
(N = 897)
Participants Number %
Age interval
15–24 139 16
25–34 189 21
35–49 261 29
50+308 34
Gender
Male 777 87
Female 120 13
Education
Elementary School 248 28
Secondary School 119 13
High School 299 33
University 231 26
Table 2 Demographics structure
of participants (N = 897)
1 3
8747
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Natural Hazards (2024) 120:8743–8760
According to the answers to the open-ended questions, the results of which are given
in Table 5, preventing settlement in stream beds or relocation (36.9%) is seen as the most
eective method to reduce ood risk among the participants. In the second open-ended
question, which asks about the people responsible for taking measures, the state comes rst
with 45.7%.
All digitized data had a mean value of 5.37 and a standard deviation. Adopting the
“mean ± 1 standard deviation” approach, interval of the scores was determined to have the
minimum value of 3.31 and maximum value of 7.44 (Liu et al. 2018, 2022). Grouping was
made so as to the values below 3.31 were to be “low”, values above 7.44 were to be “high”,
while the values in between were to be “moderate”. Accordingly, 137 participants had low
risk perception, while 649 and 110 participants had moderate and high-risk perceptions,
respectively.
3 Hypotheses and method
Since for each parameter that might aect the FRP level, the nature of the data is dierent,
some of them required dierent types of statistical tests to be correctly analyzed. Since it
was convenient to use independent sample T-test for the investigations of the eects of gen-
der, ood experience, residence risk, expectation of loss and belief in responsible; T-tests
were performed to investigated these. One-way ANOVA was suitable for the investigation
of the eects of age and gender on FRP level, while Two-way ANOVA was convenient for
the determination of the combined eects of demographic factors age, gender, and educa-
tion level on FRP level. Hypotheses and types of tests used to evaluate them are shown in
Table 6. In each case, null hypothesis “H0 says that there is no dierence between vari-
ances, which is the parameter of interest does not aect the dependent variable which is FRP
level. On the other hand, alternative hypothesis “H1says that the variances dier signi-
cantly, that is; variables aect the FRP level in a statistically signicant manner.
Options NPercent (%)
Stream improvement 143 13.1
Aorestation 173 15.9
Preventing settlement in stream beds or
relocation
402 36.9
Infrastructure works 41 3.8
Awareness of public 131 12.0
Other 199 18.3
Total 1089 100.0
State 526 45.7
Municipality 134 11.6
Citizen 268 23.3
General directorate of state hydraulic
works (DSI)
42 3.6
District governate 36 3.1
Governate 60 5.2
Other 85 7.4
Total 1151 100.0
Table 5 Fundamental statis-
tics for open-ended questions
(Q5-Q6)
1 3
8748
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Natural Hazards (2024) 120:8743–8760
In order to analyze the data in the SPSS package program, it must be converted into a
software language. 4 categories were arranged for the independent variables of age and
educational status, and 2 categories were arranged for the independent variable of gender.
In classifying the dependent variables for the program, 2 categories were determined for the
rst 4 questions, which are Yes/No questions, and 6 and 7 categories were determined for
the last 2 questions respectively, which are open-ended questions. In the rst 4 questions,
‘1’ was assigned to the yes answer and ‘0’ was assigned to the no answer. In open-ended
questions, ‘1’ was assigned to the options that the participant chose for each option, and ‘0’
was assigned to the options that the participant did not choose (Table 7).
Taking the methodology of Liu et al. (2018) as the basis for the determination of FRP,
scores for each individual were calculated and categorized into three classes as “high”,
“moderate” and “low”, based on some fundamental statistical approaches (regarding mean
and standard deviation values). Each participants’ impression of ood risk was calculated
using the mean (MV) and standard deviation (SD) of the FRP scores. If the FRP score was
Table 7 Values used to digitize the data for SPSS
Age Gender Education Q1 Q2 Q3 Q4 Q5 Q6
15–24 (1) Male
(1)
Elementary
school (1)
Yes
(1)
Yes
(1)
Yes
(1)
Yes
(1)
Stream improvement
(1 0)
State (1 0)
25–34 (2) Female
(2)
Secondary
school (2)
No
(0)
No
(0)
No
(0)
No
(0)
Aorestation (1 0) Municipal-
ity (1 0)
35–49 (3) High school
(3)
Preventing settlement in
stream beds or relocation
(1 0)
Citizen
(1 0)
50+ (4) University
(4)
Infrastructure works (1 0) DSI* (1 0)
Awareness of public (1 0 ) District
governorate
(1 0)
Other (1 0) Governorate
(1 0)
Other (1 0)
* General directorate of state hydraulic works
Independent T-test
Is FRP aected by gender?
Is FRP aected by ood experience? (Q1)
Is FRP aected by the fact that the residence is under risk? (Q2)
Is FRP aected by expected loss of life or property? (Q3)
Is FRP aected by the belief that the responsible are taking the
necessary measures? (Q4)
One-way ANOVA, Post Hoc Test
Is FRP aected by age?
Is FRP aected by education level?
Two-way ANOVA, Post Hoc Test
Is FRP aected by a combination of education level and age?
Is FRP aected by a combination of education level and gender?
IS FRP aected by a combination of age and gender?
Table 6 Hypotheses and tests
1 3
8749
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Natural Hazards (2024) 120:8743–8760
larger (lesser) than 1 SD from the MV, a household’s opinion of its ood risk fell into the
high (low) category, whereas other participants fell into the moderate category ‘moderate’.
In this instance, the FRP scores’ SD, MV, minimum, and maximum values were 2.06, 5.37,
0, and 12, respectively. Therefore, the ranges for participants’ perceptions of low, moderate,
and high ood risks were (0, 3.31), (3.31, 7.44), and (7.44, 12), respectively.
According to the ndings, 650 participants (72.5%) of all questioned respondents indi-
cated that there was a moderate probability of ooding. Only 110 and 137 participants, or
roughly 12.3% and 15.3% of all surveyed individuals, respectively, reported high and low
levels of ood risk perception. A statistical analysis was made using only calculated scores;
and as proposed in Ullah et al. (2020), using weighting coecients, another analysis was
performed for comparison purposes. Signicance level was selected as 0.05 (5%), meaning
that when the signicance value is lower than 5% this means there is dierence between
variances and the eects are statistically signicant, and vice versa.
4 Results
Although there are many studies about ood risk perceptions (Liu et al. 2018; Wang et al.
2018; Ullah et al. 2020; Liu et al. 2022), there is no clear consensus on whether weights
should be used in statistical approaches. Therefore, in this study, analysis was conducted
using both without weighting FRP values and weighting coecient values. In this way, it
was possible to compare results without weighing and weighting coecient analysis.
4.1 Statistical analysis without weighting coecients
4.1.1 Independent T-test results
Table 8 shows the independent sample T-test results. It is seen that signicance and two-
tailed signicance values are 0.324 and 0.411 for the variable gender, respectively. First one
being higher than 0.05 implies that the distribution is homogeneous. The fact that two-tailed
signicance value is greater than 0.05 reveals that there is no statistically signicant dier-
ence between variances; that is, independent variable gender does not aect the dependent
variable FRP level. In this case, we fail to reject to null hypothesis. When the eects of
other parameters on FRP levels are investigated in a similar manner, it can be seen that all
other independent variables aect FRP levels signicantly; that is, when ood experience,
risk of the residence, expectations of life and property loss and belief in responsible parties’
change, FRP levels also change.
It can be said that those who had past ood experiences naturally felt themselves under
higher risk, because of the fact that they had witnessed the destructive aspect of the disaster
in real life. For the third parameter, it can easily be said that when the person thinks that his/
her residence is under risk, he/she would perceive the risk as higher. It can be said that when
people expect loss of life and property in the case of a possible future ood, they might see
themselves under a higher risk. It can even be said that the opposite of this might be the case;
that is, when the risk perception is higher, they might expect more losses. It can be argued
that when people think that the responsible parties are fullling their roles properly, they
tend to feel safer, leading to a lower level of risk perception.
1 3
8750
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Natural Hazards (2024) 120:8743–8760
4.1.2 One-way ANOVA test results
Table 9 shows the one-way ANOVA test results. It is seen that variances are homogeneous
for the parameter age. However, signicance value is 6.9% which is greater than 5%, so
we fail to reject the null hypothesis, saying that age does not aect the FRP level. When it
comes to the investigation of the eects of education level on FRP, it is seen that the homo-
geneity of variances does not exist since P-value is lower than 0.05. When signicance
value is examined, it is seen that the value is much smaller than 0.05 variances dier greatly
and there is a statistically signicant dierence between them. So, we accept the alterna-
tive hypothesis, saying that the education level indeed aects FRP level. When a further
investigation is conducted using Tukey’s test, it is seen that the dierences are between the
groups “elementary school-university” and “high school-university.” Further, Tamhane test
Is FRP
aected by
gender?
Gender NSig. Sig.
(2-tailed)
Women 120 Equal vari-
ances assumed
0.324 0.411
Man 777 Equal vari-
ances not
assumed
0.420
Is FRP
aected by
ood ex-
perience?
(Q1)
Flood
experience
NSig. Sig.
(2-tailed)
Yes 555 Equal vari-
ances assumed
0.000 0.000
No 341 Equal vari-
ances not
assumed
0.000
Is FRP
aected
by the fact
that the
residence
is under
risk? (Q2)
Personal
awareness
NSig. Sig.
(2-tailed)
Yes 539 Equal vari-
ances assumed
0.000 0.000
No 354 Equal vari-
ances not
assumed
0.000
Is FRP
aected by
expected
loss of
life or
property?
(Q3)
Risk and
concern
perception
NSig. Sig.
(2-tailed)
Yes 741 Equal vari-
ances assumed
0.000 0.000
No 156 Equal vari-
ances not
assumed
0.000
Is FRP
aected by
the belief
that the
respon-
sibles are
taking the
necessary
measures?
(Q4)
Potential
of coping
prociency
NSig. Sig.
(2-tailed)
Yes 742 Equal vari-
ances assumed
0.122 0.000
No 129 Equal vari-
ances not
assumed
0.000
Table 8 Independent T-test
results
1 3
8751
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Natural Hazards (2024) 120:8743–8760
suggests that there are dierences between groups “elementary school-secondary school,”
“elementary school-university” and “high school-university”.
There might be numerous factors lying behind these dierences; one might argue that
those who are better educated have a better understanding of the causes and consequences
of the oods and of how to prevent the damages to the life and property, and consequently
have a lower risk perception. Another reason might be that those who are better educated
are generally the ones with higher income, so that they might think coping with the results
of the oods, especially with damage to their properties, can be easier.
4.1.3 Two-way ANOVA test results
Table 10 shows the two-way ANOVA test results. Two-way ANOVA tests were conducted
in order to investigate the combined eects of the demographic factors considered in this
study, namely age, gender and education level. It is seen that the homogeneity of variances
does exist except for the case of the combined eects of education level and gender. Since
this homogeneity of variance is violated, this test is said to be a failed one. It is seen that
parameters age and education level separately eects FRP level since the signicance val-
ues for them are 0.037 and 0.025, respectively. However, when their combined eects are
examined, it is clear that their combination does not aect FRP level since the signicance
is greater than 0.05. When the separate and combined eects of age and gender parameters
are examined, it can be seen that all signicance values are much greater than 0.05 and
thus it can be concluded that they do not have a separate or combined eect on FRP level
according to this test.
4.2 Statistical analysis with weighting coecients
4.2.1 Independent T-test results
In Ullah et al. (2020); 1, 0.8, 0.6, 0.4 and 0.2 weighting coecients were adopted for ve
dierent suggested risk perception groups, namely extremely high, high, moderate, low, and
very low. Since in our study there are three groups, namely high, moderate, and low; it is
assumed to be convenient to use weighting coecients 1, 0.6 and 0.2 for them, respectively.
From the Table 11, it can be seen that in all of the cases except where the eect of ood
experience on FRP level is examined homogeneity of variances does exist. When two-tailed
Sig. ANOVA
Sig.
Is FRP
aected
by age?
Homoge-
neity of
variances
Based on mean 0.168 0.069
Based on median 0.572
Based on median and with
adjusted df
0.572
Based on trimmed mean 0.213
Is FRP af-
fected by
education
level?
Homoge-
neity of
variances
Based on mean 0.049 0.000
Based on median 0.020
Based on median and with
adjusted df
0.020
Based on trimmed mean 0.037
Table 9 One-way ANOVA
results
1 3
8752
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Natural Hazards (2024) 120:8743–8760
signicance values are examined, it is clearly seen that all studied items except gender does
have eects on FRP level. This was the same case for the analysis which was performed
without using weighting coecients.
4.2.2 One-way ANOVA test results
Table 12 shows one-way ANOVA test results. Homogeneity of variances is violated for
parameter education level. For both parameters, signicance values are lower than 0.05,
thus it can be said that they both aect FRP level. Further investigation (Tukey’s test,
P = 0.033; Tamhane test, P = 0.041) tells that there are dierences between age groups 25–34
and 40–64. On the other hand, for education level parameter, dierences were observed
between groups “elementary school-secondary school”, “elementary school-high school”
and “elementary school-university” according to Tukey’s test. Tamhane test revealed that
dierences observed between groups “elementary school-secondary school”, “elementary
school-high school” and “elementary school-university”.
Sig. Tests of between-
subjects eects
Is FRP
aected
by a
combina-
tion of
education
level and
age?
Homoge-
neity of
variances
Based on
mean
0.084 Sig
Based on
median
0.200 age 0.037
Based on
Median
and with
adjusted df
0.201 edu_sta 0.025
Based on
trimmed
mean
0.077 age * edu_sta 0.168
Is FRP
aected
by a
combina-
tion of
education
level and
gender?
Homoge-
neity of
variances
Based on
mean
0.038 Sig.
Based on
median
0.041 edu_sta -
Based on
median
and with
adjusted df
0.041 gender -
Based on
trimmed
mean
0.036 edu_sta*gender -
IS FRP
aected
by a
combina-
tion of
age and
gender?
Homoge-
neity of
variances
Based on
mean
0.292 Sig.
Based on
median
0.708 gender 0.718
Based on
median
and with
adjusted df
0.708 age 0.131
Based on
trimmed
mean
0.336 gender * age 0.736
Table 10 Two-way ANOVA
results
1 3
8753
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Natural Hazards (2024) 120:8743–8760
Sig. ANOVA
Sig.
Is FRP
aected
by age?
Homoge-
neity of
variances
Based on mean 0.096 0.022
Based on median 0.196
Based on median and with
adjusted df
0.196
Based on trimmed mean 0.102
Is FRP af-
fected by
education
level?
Homoge-
neity of
variances
Based on mean 0.001 0.000
Based on median 0.002
Based on median and with
adjusted df
0.002
Based on trimmed mean 0.002
Table 12 One-way ANOVA
results
Is FRP
aected by
gender?
Gender NSig. Sig.
(2-tailed)
Women 120 Equal vari-
ances assumed
0.061 0.314
Man 777 Equal vari-
ances not
assumed
0.355
Is FRP
aected by
ood ex-
perience?
(Q1)
Flood
Experience
NSig. Sig.
(2-tailed)
Yes 555 Equal vari-
ances assumed
0.008 0.000
No 341 Equal vari-
ances not
assumed
0.000
Is FRP
aected
by the fact
that the
residence
is under
risk? (Q2)
Personal
awareness
NSig. Sig.
(2-tailed)
Yes 539 Equal vari-
ances assumed
0.300 0.000
No 354 Equal vari-
ances not
assumed
0.000
Is FRP
aected by
expected
loss of
life or
property?
(Q3)
Risk and
concern
perception
NSig. Sig.
(2-tailed)
Yes 741 Equal vari-
ances assumed
0.910 0.000
No 156 Equal vari-
ances not
assumed
0.000
Is FRP
aected by
the belief
that the
respon-
sibles are
taking the
necessary
measures?
(Q4)
Potential
of coping
prociency
NSig. Sig.
(2-tailed)
Yes 742 Equal vari-
ances assumed
0.000 0.000
No 129 Equal vari-
ances not
assumed
0.000
Table 11 Independent T test
results
1 3
8754
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Natural Hazards (2024) 120:8743–8760
Comments made for the analysis made without weighting coecients as to why these
groups would dier from each other in their risk perception levels might be valid for this
analysis as well.
4.2.3 Two-way ANOVA test results
Table 13 shows two-way ANOVA test results. In this test, only the combined eects of
gender and age parameters were successfully tested because other two combinations, homo-
geneity of variances was violated. From the Table 13, it is seen that neither age and gender
parameters’ separate nor combined eects had any inuence on FRP level of participants.
Sig. Tests of between-
subjects eects
Is FRP
aected
by a
combina-
tion of
education
level and
age?
Homoge-
neity of
variances
Based on
mean
0.005 Sig
Based on
median
0.032 age -
Based on
median
and with
adjusted df
0.032 edu_sta -
Based on
trimmed
mean
0.013 age * edu_sta -
Is FRP
aected
by a
combina-
tion of
education
level and
gender?
Homoge-
neity of
variances
Based on
mean
0.002 Sig.
Based on
median
0.005 edu_sta -
Based on
median
and with
adjusted df
0.005 gender -
Based on
trimmed
mean
0.005 edu_sta*gender -
IS FRP
aected
by a
combina-
tion of
age and
gender?
Homoge-
neity of
variances
Based on
mean
0.039 Sig.
Based on
median
0.130 gender 0.976
Based on
median
and with
adjusted df
0.130 age 0.080
Based on
trimmed
mean
0.046 gender * age 0.710
Table 13 Two-way ANOVA
results
1 3
8755
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Natural Hazards (2024) 120:8743–8760
5 Discussion
The results obtained as a result of the analyzes are compiled in the Table 14. It can be seen
that use of weighting coecients does not cause major dierences in FRP scores; only dif-
ference was observed while testing the hypothesis “Is FRP aected by age?” using one-way
ANOVA test. The study aimed to further investigate the results of a previous study Anılan
and Yüksek (2017), in which a questionnaire consisting of four Yes/No and two open-ended
questions administered to 897 participants from Eastern Black Sea Region of Turkey to
determine their ood risk perceptions. Dierent from the previous study, SPSS software
was used to examine the relationships between various factors and ood risk perception of
each participant.
In Anılan and Yüksek (2017), answers “yes” given to questions to determine whether
participants had had any ood experience and whether they had thought that their residence
was under risk were nearly equal, as mentioned by the authors. In consistency with this, in
our study it is clear that ood experience inuences participants’ risk perceptions. In Anılan
and Yüksek (2017), it was observed that, in general, women were more conscious about
ood damage mitigation measures, as they mentioned more sustainable and environmen-
tally friendly measures while responding to the open-ended questions. It was also observed
that many of the participants did not know who was responsible for taking measures, but in
general women were again more conscious and also aware of their self-responsibilities. It
was concluded that male participants’ risk perception was lower since they had a tendency
to expect less loss. However, in this study, it seems these dierences were not reected into
Independent T test FRP FRP (weight)
Is FRP aected by gender? No No
Is FRP aected by ood experi-
ence? (Q1)
Yes Yes
Is FRP aected by the fact that
the residence is under risk? (Q2)
Yes Yes
Is FRP aected by expected loss
of life or property? (Q3)
Yes Yes
Is FRP aected by the belief
that the responsibles are taking
the necessary measures? (Q4)
Yes Yes
One-way ANOVA, Post Hoc
Test
Is FRP aected by age? No (not
homogeneous)
Yes (homo-
geneous)
Is FRP aected by education
level?
Yes (not
homogeneous)
Yes (not ho-
mogeneous)
Two-way ANOVA, Post Hoc
Test
Is FRP aected by a combina-
tion of education level and age?
only age aects,
combination does
not (homogeneous)
not
homogeneous
Is FRP aected by a combina-
tion of education level and
gender?
not homogeneous not
homogeneous
IS FRP aected by a combina-
tion of age and gender?
no (homogeneous) no (homoge-
neous)
Table 14 Summary of statistical
results
1 3
8756
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Natural Hazards (2024) 120:8743–8760
the participants’ ood risk perception levels because statistical analyses yielded no eects
of gender on FRP levels.
In Anılan and Yüksek (2017), age seemed to aect awareness on possible ood miti-
gation measures and parties responsible for taking them. In general, younger participants
seemed to be more aware of modern mitigation measures and responsible parties. In this
study, only the eects on FRP level were examined and seen that age aected FRP level
when the weighting coecients used in one-way ANOVA and two-way ANOVA tests. This
might arise from the dierent opinions of participants from dierent age groups on ood-
ing. In Anılan and Yüksek (2017), it was observed that people with higher education levels
mentioned more environmentally friendly solution for ood damage mitigation and were
aware of their self-responsibilities more. In this study, our ndings showed that education
level signicantly aects FRP level. This might be the result of the dierent opinions and
awareness of people having dierent educational levels.
When a comparison is made between the results of Anılan and Yüksek (2017) and this
study, there are some consistencies and disagreements. In both studies, ood experience
aected FRP levels. In Anılan and Yüksek (2017), women were observed to be more aware
of ood mitigation measures and their self-responsibilities and men had less risk perception
since they had expected less losses. However, our study found no statistically signicant
Relationship between gender and FRP. In Anılan and Yüksek (2017), it was observed that
younger people were more aware of modern mitigation measures and parties responsible for
taking them. However, in this study, only the eects of age on FRP levels were examined
and seen that age aected FRP levels when the weighting coecients used in one-way
ANOVA and two-way ANOVA tests. Also in both studies, education level seemed to have
eects on FRP levels and awareness.
Although the tting results of the model with the previous study of Anılan and Yüksek
(2017) are encouraging, this study still has some limitations. There was a signicant per-
centage of participants were men which might cause some uncertainty in the results. Future
research should be more comprehensive. Additionally, there might be other factors which
might be ignored.
6 Conclusions
In this study, results of a previous study (Anılan and Yüksek 2017) were used, a dierent
method was adopted to interpret the results. In their study, a questionnaire was administered
to over 1000 participants from Eastern Black Sea Region of Turkey, to investigate their
ood risk perception and knowledge level. The questionnaire consisted of six questions,
four of them being Yes/No questions and the rest of them being open-ended. Using the
responds, some conclusions were drawn but no statistical method was used to interpret the
responds. It was aimed to perform some statistical tests using SPSS software to adopt a dif-
ferent approach and further investigate the questionnaire results.
In Anılan and Yüksek (2017) a Likert Scale was not employed, for this reasons data had
to be converted to scaled data in order for it to be used for statistical analyses. An approach
based on Liu and Li (2015) and Liu et al. (2018) was taken as basis for this purpose. Demo-
graphic factors such as age, gender and education level were also considered since they are
known to aect risk perception and awareness of people greatly. Same statistical analyses
1 3
8757
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Natural Hazards (2024) 120:8743–8760
were performed both with and without weight coecients to allow for a comparison of
results between them as well.
When weight coecients were not used; independent sample t-test was performed to see
whether FRP changed with gender, ood experience, risk of the residence, expectations of
life and property loss and belief in responsible parties; it was seen that only gender had no
eects on FRP, all the other parameters signicantly aected FRP levels of the participants.
It can be argued that people who had had ood experience could felt themselves under
more risk. It was also expected for residence risk to have an eect on FRP since people in
risky zones would naturally feel themselves under higher risk. It can also be said that when
people think that the responsible parties are fullling their roles properly, they tend to feel
safe and have a lower risk perception. One-way ANOVA tests were performed to investigate
the eects of age and education level on risk perception. It was seen that age did not have
any eects on FRP, while education level did. To see how FRP diered between dierent
subgroups of education level parameter, Tukey’s test was performed. It was observed that
the dierences were between “elementary school-university” and “high school-university”.
Tamhane Test was also applied and showed that there were dierences between groups “ele-
mentary school-secondary school”, “elementary school-university” and “high school-uni-
versity”. Two-way ANOVA tests were used to examine the combined eects of education
level and age, education level and gender, and age and gender. Since for the combination of
education level and gender, the homogeneity of variances was violated, these results could
not be interpreted. The other combinations did not have eects on FRP levels.
When weighting coecients were used; independent t-test results showed that except
gender, all other parameters had eects on FRP levels. This was in consistency with the
results of the same tests when weighting coecients were not used. When one-way ANOVA
was performed, it was seen that both age and education level aected FRP levels. Tukey’s
test suggested that the dierences caused by education level were between groups “ele-
mentary school-secondary school”, “elementary school-high school” and “elementary
school-university”. Tamhane Test results were in agreement with these. When two-way
ANOVA was performed, only in the case where the combined eects of gender and age the
homogeneity of variances was not violated. Thus eects of only this combination could be
examined and it was seen that both separate and combined eects of these parameters had
any inuence on FRP levels of the participants. As a results, it can be said that among the
cases where the homogeneity of variances was not violated, the only dierence between the
results of the tests where the weighting coecients were used and not used, was the eects
age on FRP. Normally age did not have any eect on FRP, but when weighting coecients
were used, it was seen that age aected FRP levels of participants.
In particular, this article has the potential for wider readership and to make contributions
to hazards professionals, GIS professionals, and those working on perception of risk among
a variety of hazard domains. It would be more complete research if sensitivity analysis or
similar were performed to see if any of the variables oered interaction or if the variables
selected were signicant in overall prediction. Based on the results of this study, training
programs should be carried out in order to increase the ood risk awareness of the people of
the region. There was a signicant percentage for whom this was not accessible that major-
ity of respondents were men. Policy makers should work on equalize participant gender. In
addition, surveys and their statistical analysis could also be done periodically to determine
the level of awareness and preparedness of the population.
1 3
8758
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Natural Hazards (2024) 120:8743–8760
In this study, which was conducted with reference to the survey results compiled by
Anılan and Yüksek (2017), it was aimed to obtain in-depth examinations based on numer-
ous statistical studies mentioned in the literature. The results demonstrated vast parallelism
with previous studies. Overall, the ndings of the study are at a level that can give insight
and be useful in terms of ood management.
Funding The authors declare that no funds, grants, or other support were received during the preparation of
this manuscript.
Open access funding provided by the Scientic and Technological Research Council of Türkiye (TÜBİTAK).
Data availability Data and models that support the ndings of this study are available from the corresponding
author upon reasonable request.
Declarations
Competing interests The authors have no relevant nancial or non-nancial interests to disclose.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License,
which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as
you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons
licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are
included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material.
If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted
by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the
copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
References
Anılan T, Yüksek Ö (2017) Perception of ood risk and mitigation: survey results from the East-
ern Black Sea Basin, Turkey. Nat Hazards Rev 18(2):05016006. https://doi.org/10.1061/(ASCE)
NH.1527-6996.0000232
Anılan T, Nacar S, Kankal M, Yüksek Ö (2020) Prediction of maximum annual ood discharges using arti-
cial neural network approaches. Građevinar 72(03):215–224. https://doi.org/10.14256/JCE.2316.2018
Ardaya AB, Evers M, Ribbe L (2017) What inuences disaster risk perception? Intervention measures, ood
and landslide risk perception of the population living in ood risk areas in Rio De Janeiro state, Brazil.
Int J Disaster Risk Reduct 25:227–237. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijdrr.2017.09.006
Becker G, Aerts JC, Huitema D (2014) Inuence of ood risk perception and other factors on risk-reduc-
ing behavior: a survey of municipalities along the Rhine. J Flood Risk Manag 7(1):16–30. https://doi.
org/10.1111/jfr3.12025
Bera MK, Daněk P (2018) The perception of risk in the ood-prone area: a case study from the Czech munici-
pality. Disaster Prev 27(1):2–14. https://doi.org/10.1108/DPM-01-2017-0004
Bodoque JM, Díez-Herrero A, Amérigo M, García JA, Olcina J (2019) Enhancing ash ood risk perception
and awareness of mitigation actions through risk communication: a pre-post survey design. J Hydrol
568:769–779. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhydrol.2018.11.007
Bubeck P, Botzen WJW, Aerts JC (2012) A review of risk perceptions and other factors that inuence ood mit-
igation behavior. Risk Anal: Int J 32(9):1481–1495. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.2011.01783.x
Diakakis M, Priskos G, Skordoulis M (2018) Public perception of ood risk in ash ood prone areas of
Eastern Mediterranean: the case of Attica Region in Greece. Int J Disaster Risk Reduct 28:404–413.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijdrr.2018.03.018
Eryılmaz Türkkan G, Hırca T (2021) The investigation of ood risk perception as a quantitative analy-
sis from socio-demographic perspective. Nat Hazards 106(1):715–733. https://doi.org/10.1007/
s11069-020-04488-6
Fuchs S, Karagiorgos K, Kitikidou K, Maris F, Paparrizos S, Thaler T (2017) Flood risk perception and adap-
tation capacity: a contribution to the socio-hydrology debate. Hydrol Earth Syst Sci 21(6):3183–3198.
https://doi.org/10.5194/hess-21-3183-2017
1 3
8759
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Natural Hazards (2024) 120:8743–8760
Grothmann T, Reusswig F (2006) People at risk of ooding: why some residents take precautionary action
while others do not. Nat Hazards 38:101–120. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11069-005-8604-6
Huang J, Cao W, Wang H, Wang Z (2020) Aect path to ood protective coping behaviors using SEM based
on a survey in Shenzhen, China. Int J Environ Res Public Health 17(3):940. https://doi.org/10.3390/
ijerph17030940
Kellens W, Zaalberg R, Neutens T, Vanneuville W, De Maeyer P (2011) An analysis of the pub-
lic perception of ood risk on the Belgian coast. Risk Anal: Int J 31(7):1055–1068. https://doi.
org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.2010.01571.x
Lechowska E (2018) What determines ood risk perception? A review of factors of ood risk perception
and relations between its basic elements. Nat Hazards 94(3):1341–1366. https://doi.org/10.1007/
s11069-018-3480-z(01234
Lechowska E (2022) Approaches in research on ood risk perception and their importance in ood risk man-
agement: a review. Nat Hazards 111(3):2343–2378. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11069-021-05140-7.9()
Liu DL, Li Y (2015) Social vulnerability of rural households to ood hazards in western mountainous regions
of Henan Province. China Nat Hazards Earth Syst Sci Discuss 3:6727–6744. https://doi.org/10.5194/
nhessd-3-6727-2015
Liu D, Li Y, Shen X, Xie Y, Zhang Y (2018) Flood risk perception of rural households in western mountain-
ous regions of Henan Province, China. Int J Disaster Risk Reduct 27:155–160. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
ijdrr.2017.09.051
Liu D, Li M, Li Y, Chen H (2022) Assessment of public ood risk perception and inuencing factors: an
example of Jiaozuo City, China. Sustainability 14(15):9475. https://doi.org/10.3390/su14159475
Miceli R, Sotgiu I, Settanni M (2008) Disaster preparedness and perception of ood risk: a study in an alpine
valley in Italy. J Environ Psychol 28(2):164–173. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2007.10.006
Oubennaceur K, Chokmani K, Lessard F, Gauthier Y, Baltazar C, Toussaint JP (2022) Understanding
ood risk perception: a Case Study from Canada. Sustainability 14(5):3087. https://doi.org/10.3390/
su14053087
Rana IA, Jamshed A, Younas ZI, Bhatti SS (2020) Characterizing ood risk perception in urban communities
of Pakistan. Int J Disaster Risk Reduct 46:101624. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijdrr.2020.101624
Santoro S, Totaro V, Lovreglio R, Camarda D, Iacobellis V, Fratino U (2022) Risk perception and knowl-
edge of protective measures for ood risk planning. The case study of Brindisi (Puglia region). Saf Sci
153:105791. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssci.2022.105791
Shen Y, Lou S, Zhao X, Ip KP, Xu H, Zhang J (2020) Factors impacting risk perception under typhoon
disaster in Macao SAR, China. Int J Environ Res Public Health 17(20):7357. https://doi.org/10.3390/
ijerph17207357
Ullah F, Saqib SE, Ahmad MM, Fadlallah MA (2020) Flood risk perception and its determinants among rural
households in two communities in Khyber Pakhtunkhwa, Pakistan. Nat Hazards 104:225–247. https://
doi.org/10.1007/s11069-020-04166-7
Wang Z, Wang H, Huang J, Kang J, Han D (2018) Analysis of the public ood risk perception in a ood-
prone city: the case of Jingdezhen city in China. Water 10(11):1577. https://doi.org/10.3390/w10111577
Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and
institutional aliations.
1 3
8760
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
Terms and Conditions
Springer Nature journal content, brought to you courtesy of Springer Nature Customer Service Center
GmbH (“Springer Nature”).
Springer Nature supports a reasonable amount of sharing of research papers by authors, subscribers
and authorised users (“Users”), for small-scale personal, non-commercial use provided that all
copyright, trade and service marks and other proprietary notices are maintained. By accessing,
sharing, receiving or otherwise using the Springer Nature journal content you agree to these terms of
use (“Terms”). For these purposes, Springer Nature considers academic use (by researchers and
students) to be non-commercial.
These Terms are supplementary and will apply in addition to any applicable website terms and
conditions, a relevant site licence or a personal subscription. These Terms will prevail over any
conflict or ambiguity with regards to the relevant terms, a site licence or a personal subscription (to
the extent of the conflict or ambiguity only). For Creative Commons-licensed articles, the terms of
the Creative Commons license used will apply.
We collect and use personal data to provide access to the Springer Nature journal content. We may
also use these personal data internally within ResearchGate and Springer Nature and as agreed share
it, in an anonymised way, for purposes of tracking, analysis and reporting. We will not otherwise
disclose your personal data outside the ResearchGate or the Springer Nature group of companies
unless we have your permission as detailed in the Privacy Policy.
While Users may use the Springer Nature journal content for small scale, personal non-commercial
use, it is important to note that Users may not:
use such content for the purpose of providing other users with access on a regular or large scale
basis or as a means to circumvent access control;
use such content where to do so would be considered a criminal or statutory offence in any
jurisdiction, or gives rise to civil liability, or is otherwise unlawful;
falsely or misleadingly imply or suggest endorsement, approval , sponsorship, or association
unless explicitly agreed to by Springer Nature in writing;
use bots or other automated methods to access the content or redirect messages
override any security feature or exclusionary protocol; or
share the content in order to create substitute for Springer Nature products or services or a
systematic database of Springer Nature journal content.
In line with the restriction against commercial use, Springer Nature does not permit the creation of a
product or service that creates revenue, royalties, rent or income from our content or its inclusion as
part of a paid for service or for other commercial gain. Springer Nature journal content cannot be
used for inter-library loans and librarians may not upload Springer Nature journal content on a large
scale into their, or any other, institutional repository.
These terms of use are reviewed regularly and may be amended at any time. Springer Nature is not
obligated to publish any information or content on this website and may remove it or features or
functionality at our sole discretion, at any time with or without notice. Springer Nature may revoke
this licence to you at any time and remove access to any copies of the Springer Nature journal content
which have been saved.
To the fullest extent permitted by law, Springer Nature makes no warranties, representations or
guarantees to Users, either express or implied with respect to the Springer nature journal content and
all parties disclaim and waive any implied warranties or warranties imposed by law, including
merchantability or fitness for any particular purpose.
Please note that these rights do not automatically extend to content, data or other material published
by Springer Nature that may be licensed from third parties.
If you would like to use or distribute our Springer Nature journal content to a wider audience or on a
regular basis or in any other manner not expressly permitted by these Terms, please contact Springer
Nature at
onlineservice@springernature.com
... A large number of authors have addressed the issue of risk perception and sustainable management of basins [39][40][41][42][43][44][45]. These studies, while employing varied methodological approaches, have consistently focused on establishing effective governance, management and risk communication policies. ...
... Anilan et al. [43] worked with variables such as familiarity with risk, fear, history of disasters, trust in institutions and understanding of risk. Similarly, other research groups used familiarity with risk, past history of disasters, trust in institutions, understanding of risk, catastrophism and the role of the press. ...
... Table 1 also illustrates the correspondence between the variables and the questions included in the survey. Anilan et al. [43] worked with variables such as familiarity with risk, fear, history of disasters, trust in institutions and understanding of risk. Similarly, other research groups used familiarity with risk, past history of disasters, trust in institutions, understanding of risk, catastrophism and the role of the press. ...
Article
Full-text available
The Nigua River basin in the Dominican Republic is a critical hydrographic area facing significant environmental challenges, including deforestation, soil erosion and pollution from mining and agricultural activities. This study explores the role of risk perception among local residents in shaping policies for the basin’s sustainable management. The research aims to identify the factors influencing risk perception and propose actionable strategies to improve environmental governance in the region. A “perceived risk profile” methodology was applied, using survey data from 1223 basin residents. The analysis identified key variables that influence risk perception, including demographic factors such as education, gender, and place of residence. The findings reveal that risk underestimation correlates with low awareness of risks, uncertainty about the origins of disasters, fatalism toward natural events, and low trust in institutions. In contrast, risk over-estimation is linked to infrequent risk communication, heightened catastrophism and a strong emphasis on the benefits of environmental protection. The study also highlights significant regional differences in risk perception, with residents of the lower basin exhibiting higher perceptions of risk due to cumulative pollution and frequent disaster impacts. Based on these insights, the study recommends targeted strategies to bridge risk perception gaps, including tailored risk communication, community-based environmental education and stronger institutional trust-building initiatives, all aimed at fostering more effective and inclusive environmental governance in the Nigua basin.
... Par exemple, s'agissant du risque d'inondation, certaines études observent une corrélation positive entre l'âge et la perception du risque (Kellens et al., 2011), tandis que Botzen et al. (2009) ont montré que les personnes plus âgées ont une plus faible perception du risque. Il en va de même pour le genre, puisque toujours pour le risque d'inondation, certaines études montrent que les femmes présentent un niveau plus haut de perception du risque (Kellens et al., 2011;Lindell & Hwang, 2008) alors que d'autres études ne trouvent aucune influence du genre sur la perception de ce même risque (Anılan et al., 2024). Mais d'autres facteurs dispositionnels semblent avoir un effet plus consensuel sur le rapport des habitants au risque auquel ils sont exposés, notamment les expériences passées. ...
Thesis
Full-text available
This thesis explores the relationship between risk perception and adaptation, taking into account the temporal dimension of risk (long-term or sudden) and adaptation (before and during exposure). Four studies have applied this approach to coastal hazards. The first, a qualitative study, provides an overview of the social representations of coastal risks and their prescriptive dimension. The second study determined how risk map communication presenting future projections influences the perception of a long-term risk such as coastal erosion, by measuring risk map observation using eye-tracking. The following two studies then explores the interaction between risk perception and individual and public adaptations according to different temporalities of risk and exposure. The first identified individual reactions to exposure to a sudden risk (tsunami) using a virtual reality simulation, and in particular reconsidered the "myth of panicked victim" in the face of a natural disaster. The second looked at public adaptation to a long-term risk (coastal erosion). It showed that the generally hotly contested strategy of relocating the stakes is ultimately the most accepted, and identified factors in the acceptability of different strategies, with the time step (short or long term) of their application not being one of them.
... This data can be read in light of the flood risk perception, a subjective concept that reflects how individuals assess the potential negative impacts of floods [104,105]. This perception is not uniform [106]; it varies from person to person and is shaped by demographic and psychological factors, along with personal flood experiences. Previous research focused on how demographic variables such as age, gender, education level, and income significantly influence how people perceive this risk are contrasting [107]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Agriculture, livestock, and forestry are crucial in mitigating hydrogeological risks, such as floods, particularly severe in the Mediterranean region. Still, the ecosystem services (ESs) provided by these activities are often undervalued. However, to assign them an economic value and ensure their effective incorporation into decision-making processes and territorial planning, they must first be recognized, appreciated, and deemed necessary by society. Despite several studies on ESs in the primary sector, research on agroecosystem flood regulation is limited, leaving key aspects unaddressed for decision-makers. No previous studies explicitly address the evaluation of ESs provided by agriculture, livestock, and forestry businesses in hydrogeological risky environments, especially in flood-prone areas. This study investigates the perception of the ESs provided by the above activities, focusing on those furnished in areas subject to hydrogeological instability. It also focuses on Sardinia (Italy), which is highly susceptible to hydrogeological instability. Through a quantitative survey of 270 residents and non-residents, the research provides evidence of society’s perception of the above ESs. Supporting ESs obtain greater appreciation in crop activities, particularly concerning the preservation of pollinating insects, soil fertility, biodiversity, and water quality. Among the regulatory Ess, appreciation is most prominent in fire risk management and flood risk regulation. Similar arguments can be made for livestock activities. Forestry activities are perceived as key players in managing flood risk, landslide risk, soil erosion, and climate change. The Multiple Correspondence Analysis indicates that appreciating one ES often leads to the recognition of others. Additionally, a set of Logit Regressions showed that while age and gender do not influence ESs perception, education level and awareness of climate change-related emergencies play a significant role. Those findings support more informed decision-making and fostering sustainable practices in areas at risk of hydrogeological disasters and lead to several important implications for practitioners, academics, and policymakers.
Article
Full-text available
There are many studies showing that public flood risk perception may promote people’s motivation to reduce flood risk and enhance their coping behavior, thus providing useful insights for flood risk management. The purpose of this article is to estimate residents’ flood risk perception in Jiaozuo City and to identify the influencing factors. A questionnaire survey method was used to collect data and a composite index was constructed to measure public risk perception. Each respondent’s grade of flood risk perception was calculated using the relationship between the standard deviation (SD) and the mean value (MV) of flood risk perception index (RPI) scores. Moreover, the hypotheses concerning different groups were tested using an independent sample T-test and one-way ANOVA (analysis of variance), and the group differences in flood risk perception on each observed dependent variable were explored using post hoc tests. The flood risk perception of the total respondents was divided into three levels based on the SD and MV of RPI scores: low (68.4%), moderate (13.7%), and high (17.9%). Respondents with low education, low income, less flood experiences, and who have married, lived in rural areas or near rivers/reservoirs had a higher flood risk perception than others, and respondents who lived in flood storage areas had a lower risk perception. Moreover, the ability to mitigate floods and the trust in flood-control projects were negatively related to the flood risk perception.
Article
Full-text available
Floods are among the most frequent natural hazards, and flood risk management is a paramount task when planning solutions to reduce their impact on communities. In the last decades, policy makers' actions for flood risk management have been redirected from purely physical self-protective measures towards integrated management strategies by including social components. Assessing flood risk perception and the level of knowledge of citizens regarding protective measures is becoming a pillar for generating innovative flood integrated management strategies. This study aims to highlight multiple aspects which can influence flood risk management in urban areas, providing a preliminary assessment of citizens’ flood risk perception and knowledge of protective measures. Proposed methodology is based on E-survey in order to gather data and Mann-Whitney and Kruskal-Wallis tests to analyze them and has been applied to the case study of Brindisi (Puglia region, Southern Italy). The results suggest that flood risk perception depends on intrinsic components of individuals, mainly related to trust in public strategies and risk communication. It depends on hazard proximity but is uniformly distributed over the whole city, demonstrating that the perception of flood risk can not be related only to river floods. Knowledge of protective measures appears uniformly low by category of citizens and territorial area, particularly for teenagers. The methodological approach has allowed to bring out how the different nature of floods could produce a spatial and social heterogeneity in citizens’ flood risk perception and knowledge of protective measures, revealing latent risk features useful for supporting flood risk planning.
Article
Full-text available
In recent years, understanding and improving the perception of flood risk has become an important aspect of flood risk management and flood risk reduction policies. The aim of this study was to explore perceptions of flood risk in the Petite Nation River watershed, located in southern Quebec, Canada. A survey was conducted with 130 residents living on a floodplain in this river watershed, which had been affected by floods in the spring of 2017. Participants were asked about different aspects related to flood risk, such as the flood hazard experience, the physical changes occurring in the environment, climate change, information accessibility, flood risk governance, adaptation measures, and finally the perception of losses. An analysis of these factors provided perspectives for improving flood risk communication and increasing the public awareness of flood risk. The results indicated that the analyzed aspects are potentially important in terms of risk perception and showed that the flood risk perceptions varied for each aspect analyzed. In general, the information regarding flood risk management is available and generally understandable, and the level of confidence was good towards most authorities. However, the experiences of flood risk and the consequences of climate change on floods were not clear among the respondents. Regarding the adaptation measures, the majority of participants tended to consider non-structural adaptation measures as being more relevant than structural ones. Moreover, the long-term consequences of flooding on property values are of highest concern. These results provide a snapshot of citizens’ risk perceptions and their opinions on topics that are directly related to such risks.
Article
Full-text available
The priority of flood management planning is physical victimization and focuses on taking structural measures. Although this approach is an accurate approach, more information is needed in implementing efficient precautionary and planning decisions. It is an indisputable fact that the existence of nothing that is not sustainable in nature cannot continue. Hence, it is necessary to implement a planning decision suitable for the structure of the population living in the region so that the continuity of the policies to be carried out against natural hazards of hydrometeorological origin such as a flood is ensured. How the socio-demographic structures affect the flood risk perception of 245 people living in the city center of Bayburt is examined in this study. It is the first research conducted for the province of Bayburt for this perspective. The participants were asked to fill a questionnaire containing 24 items and consisting of 2 sections. T test and one-way ANOVA (one-way analysis of variance) statistical methods were used to ascertain the difference between the responses of the participants to the questionnaire, based on their demographic structure. As the result of the study, significant differences were observed between the expressions depicting flood risk perception and the participant's age, income levels and educational background. In addition, it has been noted that there is a positive relationship between education and income levels and flood risk perception.
Article
Full-text available
Studying typhoon risk perception and its influencing factors help reveal potential risk factors from the perspective of the public and provide a basis for decision-making for reducing the risk of typhoon disasters. The purpose of this study is to assess the risk perception and related factors of Macao residents in China. Information was collected from 983 participants using a structured questionnaire with an effective utilization rate of 94.2%. Descriptive statistics, univariate analysis and correlation analysis were used to analyze the data. The results show that, on the one hand, there are significant differences in risk perception on the factors included: (1) age, education and other demographic characteristics; (2) health status, occupation, length of stay, residence area, residence floor, family organization structure and individuals monthly income and other personal or family conditions; (3) channels and quantity of typhoon information acquisition; (4) degree of mastery of relevant risk aversion knowledge. On the other hand, some factors still have a moderate or high level of correlation with risk perception: (1) The older the respondent, the lower the education level, the lower the income, the lower the risk perception of property damage, health impact and life threat. (2) The more children or elderly people in the family, the higher the risk perception of respondents. (3) The more risk knowledge, the lower the risk perception. (4) The more channels for obtaining information, the lower the fear level and the overall impact of risk perception. (5) The stronger the risk perception, the more positive disaster response behaviors would be taken by the public. In addition, the more information acquisition channels and the less risk knowledge respondents have, the greater the risk perception of the overall impact and the fear of the typhoon; the fewer information access channels and less risk knowledge respondents have, the greater the risk perceptions of property damage, health effects and life threats.
Article
Full-text available
Risk perception plays a vital part in flood risk management and mitigation strategies. Therefore, this study aims at first to measure the risk perception of the vulnerable households in the rural areas of Pakistan and, second, to assess the determinants and to estimate their impacts on risk perception among different groups of households. Data were collected through questionnaire survey from 382 respondents in two districts of Khyber Pakhtunkhwa province, Pakistan. Risk perception was measured through a Likert scale for a set of indicators, and the composite index was calculated. Moreover, univariate and multivariate logistic models were applied to explore the relationship between dependent and independent variables. Results of risk perception show that 50.52% of the respondents perceived a high risk of floods in the study area. Results of regression models show that the age group 40–50 years was less likely to perceive a high risk of floods. Respondents with flood experience, education (higher secondary and above), household location (near the bank of streams) and household distance from the river (≤ 500) were more likely to perceive high floods risk. Furthermore, respondents in the riverine flood-prone areas perceived high flood risk than respondents in the flash flood-prone regions. The study contributes in terms of useful information about the risk perception of floods and the determinants of flood risk perception in rural areas. These findings can help the provincial disaster management authorities and local disaster management units in understanding flood risk and implementing relevant interventions at the local level that can be used in adaptation to floods and other climate-induced disasters.
Article
Full-text available
The initial concept of flood control has gradually shifted to flood risk management which emphasizes more public participation. Therefore, understanding the public’s protective coping behavioral patterns to floods is significant, and can help improve the effectiveness of public participation and implementation of flood-mitigation measures. However, the quantitative effect of socio-demographic factors on flood risk perception and behaviors is not clear. In this study, the socio-demographic factors are included to explore the quantitative relationship with and the affect path to flood protective coping behaviors with socio-demographic factors are studied. Shenzhen City in China is chosen as the study area, which suffers frequent urban floods every year. Questionnaire surveys are conducted in five flood-prone communities there, and 339 valid questionnaires were collected. The correlations between flood risk perception, flood risk knowledge, flood risk attitude, socio-demographic factors, and protective coping behaviors are analyzed firstly. A structural equation model (SEM) about these factors is then established to verify the correctness of hypothetical paths and discover new paths. The results indicates that socio-demographic factors and flood risk perception do not have impacts on protective coping behaviors directly, but are mediated by flood risk knowledge and flood risk attitude. Flood risk attitude is an important factor that affects protective coping behaviors directly. Moreover, two affect paths to flood protective coping behaviors are proposed. The findings of Shenzhen city in this study can be extended to other cities with similar characteristics, providing support for conducting effective flood mitigation measures.
Article
Full-text available
Flood risk management is gradually shifting from a risk-based approach to an integrated one that, among other elements, considers risk communication (RC) as a means of boosting resilience. Regardless of the above, few scientists have tackled up to now the integration of RC into flood risk management. In this connection, this particular study seeks to check out the potential of a risk dialogue approach (based on an ad hoc RC strategy) to change attitudes and behaviours in relation to flash flood risk. Via a pre-post survey design, we evaluated risk perception and awareness regarding a Civil Protection Plan (CPP) implemented locally (i.e., in the municipality of Navaluenga, central Spain). For this particular objective, a questionnaire survey was created, and 201 adults (representing more than 10% of the population census) were interviewed twice in a one-year period. Before the second survey, an RC strategy was created. The RC strategy comprised briefings, quiz answers, storytelling, a contest of videos and photographs about past floods, and an intergenerational workshop. A t-test for paired sample analyses and a general linear model (GLM) repeated measures ANOVA were applied to identify changes in risk perception and awareness. Our results indicate that the RC strategy did increase flood risk perception in Navaluenga in the long term (lifetime). Also, it increased the level of awareness of the various features that comprise the CPP, enabling people to be more competent in facing a flash flood. Some cognitive biases detected in the perceptual process of human beings may shed some light on the results obtained. The implementation of well-thought-out RC strategies can play a role in improving resilience, particularly in geographic areas such as the Iberian Peninsula, in which climate change scenarios indicate a likely increase in the severity and frequency of flooding.
Article
The applicability of artificial neural network (ANN) approaches for estimation of maximum annual flows is investigated in the paper. The performance of three neural network models is compared: multi layer perceptron neural networks (MLP_NN), generalized feed forward neural networks (GFF_NN), and principal component analysis with neural networks (PCA_ NN). The proposed approaches were applied to 33 stream-gauging stations. It was found that the optimal 3-hidden layered PCA_NN method was more appropriate than the optimal MLP_NN and GFF_NN models for the estimation of maximum annual flows.
Article
Rapid urbanization and climate change have increased flood risk in urban settings. Risk perception is a vital constituent of flood risk management and risk communication. It has become important to understand risk perception, so that appropriate disaster risk reduction strategies can be initiated. Socioeconomic factors influencing risk perception have direct impact on potential adaptive capacities and disaster preparedness. This study gives an insight into psychosocial aspect of multifaceted risk in flood prone urban communities of Punjab, Pakistan. Three urban communities at high flood risk were selected from urban centres of different population size. A sample of 210 was collected using household surveys. Flood risk perception index was constructed using relevant indicators, and classified into high and low perceived risk. Logistic regression model was used to identify determinants of flood risk perception. The results show that past experiences and hazard proximity significantly influence risk perception. The determinants of risk perception also varied among the communities, depicting spatial variation. Findings of this study can help in understanding flood risk perception and its determinants, in order to design proper risk communication strategies and flood risk management plans. In addition, this study can help in understanding multidimensional flood risk and its spatial dynamics from a social science perspective.