ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Over half of children with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) display interpersonal and social problems. Several lines of research suggest that suboptimal decision making, the ability to adjust choices to different risk-varying options, influences poorer choices made in social interventions. We thus measured decision making and its prediction of social problems longitudinally with the Cambridge Gambling Task in children with ADHD over four years. Children with ADHD had shown suboptimal decision making driven mainly by delay aversion at baseline and we expected this to be a stabile trait which would predict greater parent-reported social problems. From the baseline assessment, (n = 70), 67% participated at the follow-up assessment, 21 from the ADHD group and 26 from the typically developing group. The mean age at the follow-up was 14.5 years old. The results confirmed our expectations that suboptimal decision making was a stabile trait in children and adolescents with ADHD. Although delay aversion did not differ from controls at follow-up it still proved to be the main longitudinal predictor for greater social problems. Our findings indicate that impulsivity in social interactions may be due to a motivational deficit in youth with ADHD.
Content may be subject to copyright.
1
Vol.:(0123456789)
Scientic Reports | (2024) 14:6535 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-57041-x
www.nature.com/scientificreports
Suboptimal decision making
and interpersonal problems
in ADHD: longitudinal evidence
from a laboratory task
L. Sørensen 1*, S. Adolfsdottir
1,2, E. Kvadsheim
3, H. Eichele
4, K. J. Plessen
5 &
E. Sonuga‑Barke
6,7,8
Over half of children with Attention‑Decit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) display interpersonal and
social problems. Several lines of research suggest that suboptimal decision making, the ability to
adjust choices to dierent risk‑varying options, inuences poorer choices made in social interactions.
We thus measured decision making and its prediction of social problems longitudinally with the
Cambridge Gambling Task in children with ADHD over four years. Children with ADHD had shown
suboptimal decision making driven mainly by delay aversion at baseline and we expected this to
be a stabile trait which would predict greater parent‑reported social problems. From the baseline
assessment (n = 70), 67% participated at the follow‑up assessment, 21 from the ADHD group and
26 from the typically developing group. The mean age at the follow‑up was 14.5 years old. The
results conrmed our expectations that suboptimal decision making was a stabile trait in children
and adolescents with ADHD. Although delay aversion did not dier from controls at follow‑up it still
proved to be the main longitudinal predictor for greater social problems. Our ndings indicate that
impulsivity in social interactions may be due to a motivational decit in youth with ADHD.
Over half of the individuals with attention-decit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) have diculties in develop-
ing and maintaining social relationships14. ese interpersonal problems seem to be the result of ill-considered
and poorly timed social interventions and responses rather than a lack of knowledge about appropriate social
conduct1,5,6. For instance, people with ADHD might choose to interject a comment into a conversation that has
an inappropriate content, at an inappropriate moment or in an inappropriate way without thinking through the
consequences of the actions7,8. In this sense interpersonal problems can be conceptualized, from one perspec-
tive, as expressions of suboptimal decision making causing poorer and impulsive choices to be made in social
situations9. However, the question of whether the social and interpersonal problems of people with ADHD are
underpinned by more general decits in basic decision-making skills has not yet been addressed. Investigating
the role of decision making in social problems in ADHD could lead to a complementary understanding compared
to simply focusing on that inattentive, impulsive, and hyperactive people cannot focus, wait, or sit still enough
to develop successful interactions. e core ADHD symptoms are in general found to predict social problems10,
at the same time as, treatments that are eective in reducing ADHD symptoms, such as psychostimulant medi-
cation, only have small eects on enhancing social skills1114. is suggests a dissociation between the core
symptoms of ADHD and other neurocognitive processes linking the diagnosis to social impairment. It is thus
a call for exploring if processes, such as social decision making, might improve the conceptualizations of the
causes for social problems in ADHD and as such, contribute to nding relevant targets for social skill training15.
In the current paper, we examined this question by exploring the prospective predictive relationship between
children’s performance on a widely used and well-validated task measuring risky and impulsive decision making
OPEN
1Department of Biological and Medical Psychology, University of Bergen, Jonas Liesvei 91, 5009 Bergen,
Norway. 2Division of Vision Impairments, Statped – National Service for Special Needs Education, Bergen,
Norway. 3Department of Clinical Medicine, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway. 4Regional Resource Centre for
Autism, ADHD and Tourette Syndrome Western Norway, Division of Psychiatry, Haukeland University Hospital,
Bergen, Norway. 5Division of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry, Lausanne University
Hospital, University of Lausanne, Lausanne, Switzerland. 6Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
King’s College London, London, UK. 7Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Aarhus University, Aarhus,
Denmark. 8Department of Psychology, Hong Kong University, Hong Kong, China. *email: Lin.Sorensen@uib.no
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
2
Vol:.(1234567890)
Scientic Reports | (2024) 14:6535 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-57041-x
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
(Cambridge Gambling Task; CGT)16,17 and parent-reported social and interpersonal problems later in develop-
ment. is task measures the extent to which individuals can adjust their choices to more-or-less-risky options
by integrating external information with internal value systems. e choices are made between response options
with dierent outcome probabilities16,18. Suboptimal decision-making was measured with the CGT scores of:
(1) Risk adjustment (the diculty in adjusting decisions according to level of risk by learning from previous
choices), (2) delay aversion (the choice of the less delayed option rather than the choice with the highest return),
(3) reection time (shorter reection times reect poorer inhibitory control19), and (4) risk proneness (over-
attraction to the risky options). In phase one of the study, we included a cross-sectional baseline analysis of 36
drug-naïve children with ADHD and 34 typically developing peers aged between 8 and 12years old. We observed
that ADHD was associated with fewer optimal decisions (poorer risk adjustment), which was driven primarily
by higher levels of delay aversion20. is nding was consistent with much broader literature highlighting delay
aversion as a core motivational component of ADHD across settings2125 and further, that poorer risk adjustment
in ADHD seems to be due to other processes than being risk prone per se26. ese dierent decision making
parameters may predict greater social problems as part of an overall suboptimal decision making. For instance,
impulsive choices made in social settings have been suggested to be caused predominantly by poorer inhibitory
control27,28 or delay aversion29,30. However, several studies have shown a weak link between inhibitory control
and social problems in ADHD3135. As far as we know, no study has investigated the link between delay aversion
and social problems despite the suggestions that it leads to impulsive and disruptive behavior in subjectively
experienced stimulus-poor environments29,30. Furthermore, ADHD has been associated with increased risk prone
behavior (see36). It is not clear to what extent this risky behavior in youth with ADHD is linked to a dierent
social functioning in comparison with typically peers. One recent study indicated that peer inuence compared
to no such inuence increased risk prone choices during the Balloon Analogue Risk Task across the groups of
adolescents with ADHD and their typically developing peers alike37. is may suggest that risk proneness is
similarly related to social inuence in ADHD as in non-ADHD.
We followed up on the sample aer 4years to study the developmental outcomes associated with subopti-
mal decision making in ADHD. Based on the hypothesis that interpersonal problems in ADHD are driven by
core decits in decision making, we predicted that at follow-up individuals with ADHD displaying suboptimal
decision making at baseline (T1) would (i) show more social and interpersonal diculties and (ii) continue to
display poor risk adjustment and delay aversion on the CGT and at the same time continue to not show dier-
ence from typically developing peers in reection time (inhibitory control) or risk proneness. In addition to
parent-reports of social problems from the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach and Rescorla38), we also
included parent-reports on two other subscales from CBCL of conduct problems and anxiety/depression problem
symptoms due to these problems oen being comorbid with ADHD and recognized in accompanying problems
in social interactions. Children with ADHD oen have social problems due to high frustration levels aecting
their social functioning39. Conduct problems characteristically reect higher levels of irritability, delinquency,
and aggressiveness in the behavior towards peers and others40. Furthermore, problematic symptoms of anxiety
and depression are associated with higher frustration levels and irritability41 as well as with social isolation42.
Results
Sample characteristics
From the original sample (N = 70) at baseline performing the CGT (T1), 67% (N = 47) participated at the follow-
up assessment (T2); 58% (n = 21) from the ADHD group and 77% (n = 26) from the typically developing group
(see Supplemental Fig.1). At T2, the age range was 11–17years old with a mean age of 14.5 (SD = 1.31). e two
groups did not dier signicantly in age or gender distribution (see Table1). e mean T1 and T2 interval was
4.5years (SD = 0.7) with no signicant dierence in interval between the two groups. At T2, all the adolescents
in the ADHD group still met the criteria for an ADHD diagnosis, and 48% of these had a comorbid disorder;
six had anxiety; six had oppositional deant disorder (ODD), two had tics, one had depression, and one obses-
sive–compulsive disorder. In the control group, two had an anxiety disorder. In the ADHD group, 76% (n = 16)
used CNS stimulants to treat their ADHD. Of these, 88% conducted a washout period of at least 24h before test-
ing, whereas two participants had a washout period of at least 12 and 18h, respectively. ese are all acceptable
washout periods due to the two that had a shorter period used methylphenidate, which has a half-life of two to
three hours and the active ingredient eliminated aer 10–15 h43,44. Children with ADHD who remained in the
study at T2 showed no signicant dierences in scores on the CGT, Full-Scale IQ, or ADHD symptoms at T1
compared to their counterparts who dropped out (see Supplemental Table1).
Did decision making decits seen at baseline persist to follow up?
Cross-sectionally, the ADHD group showed poorer risk adjustment compared with the control group at both T1
and T2, higher delay aversion only at T1, and higher risk proneness only at T2 (see Table1). No cross-sectional
group dierences appeared in relation to reection times (inhibitory control). Longitudinally (alpha levels (α)
were Bonferroni corrected for conducting four ANOVAs; p < 0.013), including both ADHD and the two time
points as factors, there was a main eect (F(1,90) = 8.91, p = 0.004, ηp2 = 0.09) of group on decision making quality
with the ADHD sample showing poorer risk adjustment at both baseline and follow-up (see Fig.1 and Supple-
mental Table2). ere was no signicant eect of time and no interaction between time and group. Furthermore,
the risk adjustment scores at baseline and follow-up were signicantly correlated in the whole sample (r = 0.36,
p = 0.014) and in the ADHD group (r = 0.51, p = 0.018) but not in the control group alone (r = 0.20). However,
stability was less clear for the other decision elements with the ADHD group showing more delay aversion than
controls at baseline than follow-up (main eect of time points: F(1,90) = 18.16, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.20). Still, in the
whole sample (r = 0.34, p = 0.02) and in the ADHD group (r = 0.45, p = 0.041), greater delay aversion at both time
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
3
Vol.:(0123456789)
Scientic Reports | (2024) 14:6535 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-57041-x
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
points correlated signicantly, except in the control group (r = 0.29). No signicant eect of ADHD appeared on
risk proneness or in reection times (inhibitory control) neither at baseline nor follow-up when the two time
points were included as a factor in addition to ADHD. Risk proneness as measured at the two time points did
not correlate, whereas reection times correlated highly between the two timepoints in the whole sample and in
both subgroups. See Supplemental Table4 for all the intercorrelations of the CGT parameters cross-sectionally
and longitudinally.
Did adolescents with ADHD show more social problems?
Parents reported that children with ADHD showed greater social problems and greater problems of conduct
behavior and anxiety/depression at both time points compared with control children (see Table1). At follow
up all three problem areas of social problems, conduct behavior, and anxiety/depression correlated highly with
each other (see Table2).
Table 1. Demographic information, CGT parameter scores, and social problem scores in the groups of
adolescents with ADHD and healthy controls. DM decision making, anx. anxiety, dep. Depression, sympt.
symptoms, ^inhibitory control. e CGT parameters are in z scores and the social problem scores are in
percentile scores. **p < 0.01; *p < 0.05.
Variables
ADHD (n = 21) Controls (n = 26) Group analysis
T1 T2 T1 T2 T1 T2
Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD t t
Age 9.88 1.25 14.33 1.50 10.08 1.00 14.58 1.15 0.20 0.64
T1–T2 interval 4.46 0.91 4.5 0.56 0.21
Full-scale IQ 94 7.03 109 10.42 5.70**
Risk adjustment −0.54 0.73 −0.10 0.77 −0.00 0.92 0.50 1.13 2.19* 2.06*
Delay aversion 0.80 0.99 −0.36 0.81 0.04 0.91 −0.39 0.88 −2.74** −0.10
Reection time^0.61 1.14 −0.47 0.59 0.58 0.68 −0.75 0.39 −0.08 −1.97
Risk proneness −0.00 0.99 0.29 0.82 0.26 1.17 −0.26 0.95 0.09 −2.11*
Social problems 80.10 16.24 81.48 13.34 53.92 7.97 53.88 9.72 −7.23** −8.20**
Anx./dep. sympt 69.00 18.90 77.38 18.89 57.58 14.34 58.35 13.91 −2.36* −3.98**
Conduct problems 65.43 8.91 63.33 11.33 50.85 1.74 51.85 5.27 −8.18** −4.60**
Sex n males n females n males n females X2
15 6 15 11 9.49
Figure1. Group eects shown at baseline and follow-up on the CGT parameters of (a) risk adjustment, (b)
delay aversion, (c) reection time (inhibitory control), and (d) risk proneness.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
4
Vol:.(1234567890)
Scientic Reports | (2024) 14:6535 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-57041-x
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Were social problems correlated with decision making on the CGT cross sectionally and
longitudinally?
Table2 shows the correlations between social problems at follow-up and CGT parameters at baseline and follow-
up. Cross-sectional associations at follow-up were signicant between the CGT parameters of poorer risk adjust-
ment and longer reection times (inhibitory control), and further, between poor risk adjustment and greater
social problems, and greater anxiety/depression problems. Furthermore, the CGT parameters did not correlate
with conduct problems nor did risk proneness correlate with social problems or problems of conduct behavior
and anxiety/depression at follow-up. Longitudinally, signicant correlations were seen for both the baseline
CGT parameters of poorer risk adjustment and greater delay aversion with greater social problems at follow-
up. Baseline reection times (inhibitory control) and risk proneness did not correlate with social problems at
follow-up. Further, only the baseline CGT parameter of greater delay aversion correlated with higher anxiety/
depression problems at follow-up, and only longer reection times (inhibitory control) at baseline correlated
with greater conduct problems at follow up. To test for the specicity of the eect baseline CGT parameters had
on social problems and problems of conduct behavior and anxiety/depression at follow up, we included the three
baseline CGT parameters that correlated longitudinally with these problems as predictors (risk adjustment,
delay aversion, and reection times) in three multiple linear regression analyses with the three CBCL scores as
outcome variables [alpha level (α) was Bonferroni corrected for conducting three regression analyses; p < 0.017].
e results showed that only greater delay aversion at baseline, and not poorer risk adjustment or reection
times (inhibitory control), predicted greater social problems at follow up (see Table3 and Fig.2). None of the
CGT parameters specically predicted level of conduct problems or anxiety/depression problems at follow-up.
Table 2. Bivariate correlations between the CGT parameters at both time points and parent-reported social
problems and conduct and anxiety/depression problems at follow up. **p < 0.01; *p < 0.05. DM decision
making, ^Inhibitory control.
T2 social T2 anxiety/ T2 conduct
problems depression problems
T2 anxiety/depression 0.60**
T2 conduct problems 0.67** 0.56**
T1 CGT risk adjustment −0.32* −0.23 −0.14
T1 CGT delay aversion 0.43** 0.40 0.22
T1 CGT reection time^−0.11 −0.03 0.31*
T1 CGT risk proneness −0.20 −0.28 0.01
T2 CGT risk adjustment −0.40** −0.37* −0.22
T2 CGT delay aversion 0.02 0.17 −0.29
T2 CGT reection time^0.33* 0.27 0.20
T2 CGT risk proneness 0.16 0.006 −0.05
Table 3. e longitudinal prediction of baseline CGT parameters of poorer risk adjustment, delay aversion,
and reection time (inhibitory control) on parent-reported social problems and conduct and anxiety/
depression problems at follow-up. *Bonferroni corrected p-level (0.05/3) = 0.017. DM decision making;
^Inhibitory control.
Predictors Adj. R2df F p B β t p 95% CI for B
T2 parent-reported social/interpersonal problems from CBCL
T1 risk adjustment 0.17 3/43 4.14 0.012* −2.52 −0.12 −0.80 0.430 −8.89 to 3.86
T1 delay aversion 6.99 0.40 2.59 0.013* 1.54 to 12.43
T1 reection time^2.78 0.14 1.00 0.325 −2.85 to 8.40
T2 parent-reported anxiety/depression problems from CBCL
T1 risk adjustment 0.11 3/43 2.84 0.049 −1.56 −0.07 −0.45 0.653 −8.47 to 5.36
T1 delay aversion 6.87 0.37 2.34 0.024 0.96 to 12.79
T1 reection time^0.17 0.01 0.06 0.957 −5.93 to 6.27
T2 parent-reported conduct problems from CBCL
T1 risk adjustment 0.10 3/43 2.73 0.055 0.44 0.04 0.23 0.817 −3.34 to 4.22
T1 delay aversion 2.78 0.28 1.73 0.09 −0.45 to 6.01
T1 reection time^3.93 0.35 2.38 0.022 0.60 to 7.26
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
5
Vol.:(0123456789)
Scientic Reports | (2024) 14:6535 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-57041-x
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Discussion
In the current study, children with ADHD were followed up longitudinally to study the development of subop-
timal decision making over time and its prediction of social problems between childhood and adolescence. We
originally found that the children with ADHD displayed suboptimal choices as measured with risk adjustment at
baseline, driven primarily by delay aversion on the CGT compared with typically developing peers. At follow-up
four years later, we expected that individuals with ADHD would still show poorer risk adjustment and greater
delay aversion than their typically developing peers, and that baseline suboptimal decision making would predict
greater parent-reports of social problems and conduct and anxiety/depression problems. As expected, the results
showed that poorer risk adjustment was a stable trait over 4years in children and adolescents with ADHD. Delay
aversion, on the other hand, was no longer signicantly dierent at follow-up in the ADHD group compared
with the control group. Still, greater delay aversion along with poorer risk adjustment correlated from baseline to
follow-up only in the ADHD group and not in the control group. e CGT parameters of poorer risk adjustment,
greater delay aversion, and longer reection times (inhibitory control) were correlated both cross-sectionally
and longitudinally with greater interpersonal problems and conduct and anxiety/depression problems. However,
when including these CGT parameters in the same statistical model, greater delay aversion at baseline (T1) was
the only CGT parameter that four years later (T2) predicted greater social problems (see Fig.3). Furthermore,
none of these CGT parameters at baseline predicted conduct or anxiety/depression problems four years later.
Risk proneness was not dierent in the ADHD group at T1 but showed a tendency to be higher in this group at
T2. It did, however, not correlate with social problems or conduct or anxiety/depression problems. Reection
time (inhibitory control) was not dierent between the groups at either time point.
Parents and teachers consistently report that children with ADHD struggle with interpersonal interactions
and relationships1,4,6,35, as is the case in the current study. ese reports typically coincide with the perception
Figure2. Scatterplots of the longitudinal relationship between the baseline CGT parameters of risk adjustment,
delay aversion, and reection time (inhibitory control) and parent-reported social problems at follow-up. Delay
aversion was the only predictor of these CGT parameters that predicted signicantly greater social problems at
follow-up.
Figure3. Hypothesized models illustrating (a) the expected prediction of baseline suboptimal decision making
on longitudinal interpersonal problems, and the eect of delay aversion on interpersonal problems via poorer
risk adjustment, and (b) the role of baseline delay aversion both as contributing to suboptimal decision making
and directly predicting interpersonal problems four years later.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
6
Vol:.(1234567890)
Scientic Reports | (2024) 14:6535 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-57041-x
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
reported by peers that children with ADHD are more oen rejected and neglected by peers, having fewer friends
and fewer and less diverse social activities13,31,45,46. Our results indicate that these problems are predicted by
diculties in adapting dynamically to changing contextual conditions. On the CGT this can be observed by dif-
culties in adapting choices to the changing patterns of outcome probabilities (poorer risk adjustment). In the
current study, the motivational style of escaping delays was the main driver for these problems. Delay aversion
has previously been suggested to cause social problems by leading to impulsive and disruptive behavior29,30.
Importantly, our study appears, as far as we know, to be the rst to experimentally investigate and link delay
aversion with greater interpersonal problems. is is in line with the dominant causal hypothesis of ADHD in
children in that they tend to choose immediate rewards more consistently when this choice gives escape from
delay compared to when the same choice does not escape delay23. is drive for escape when children with ADHD
perceive situations as tedious (stimulus-poor) is suggested to be associated with a negative aective state (high
frustration), which is supported by fMRI studies of adolescents25,47 and adults48 with ADHD.
In line with ndings from several previous studies3135, the children with ADHD did not show suboptimal
decision making or social problems due to poorer inhibitory control as reected in shorter reection times.
Rather, it was a general nding that longer reection times correlated with greater social problems, which prob-
ably are linked to challenges of processing information fast enough in social situations49. Important to note,
though, is that a previous study has shown that the delay aversion score from the CGT can be challenging to
distinguish from poor inhibitory control in ADHD22. In our original study20, we therefore tested for this and
found that (a) the delay aversion score was linked to shorter test duration time—showing the CGT delay aver-
sion score to be linked to escape of delay and not just an impulsive drive for immediate reward (see23,50), and
(b) the prediction of delay aversion in explaining suboptimal decision making in ADHD was not explained by
poorer inhibitory control (as measured with the Cambridge Stop Signal Test) or by level of intelligence and
working memory capacity. Inhibitory control, intelligence and working memory capacity did not either covary
in our original study with suboptimal decision making when testing the group dierences between ADHD and
control children20.
Our ndings showed that delay aversion at baseline tended to specically predict greater interpersonal prob-
lems in general and not conduct or anxiety/depression problems. Both conduct and anxiety/depression problem
scores were signicantly higher in the ADHD group and highly correlated with social problems. In relation to
the CGT scores, greater delay aversion correlated with higher anxiety/depression problem symptoms, and longer
reection times with greater conduct problems. is supports delay aversion being described as expressing nega-
tive aectivity48—typically associated with higher levels of anxiety and/or depression51. Longer reection times
have also been found to be associated with lower motivation to do tasks as instructed52, which higher conduct
problems can reect via non-compliance with external expectations. However, including the CGT parameters
that correlated with social problems and the problem scores of conduct behavior and anxiety/depression in the
same regression models, showed that none of them specically predicted higher conduct or anxiety/depression
problems.
To our knowledge, this is the rst study that has investigated the link between suboptimal decision making
and interpersonal problems in ADHD. However, in adults with schizophrenia53 and in young healthy adults54,
both studies using a dierent test method than CGT, found that suboptimal decision making was associated with
poorer social skills and interpersonal relationships. In children with ADHD, one study investigated the ability of
teacher-reported ADHD symptoms to predict parent-reports on the social problem subscale from the CBCL and
performance on a decision making task using aective cues two years later55. e ADHD symptoms predicted
both greater social problems and poorer decision making. However, the cross-sectional correlation coecient
between social problems and the aective cuing decision making task scores was low (r = 0.21). Future studies are
encouraged to further test the prediction of suboptimal decision making on social problems in ADHD. e low
sample size in the current study may rise issues of low statistical power when testing longitudinal associations
despite the a priori power analysis showed the sample size to be sucient for avoiding a type II error.
In the current study, the children with ADHD were drug naïve at baseline and the majority on medication
at follow-up. is may have aected the longitudinal ndings in that medication according to its objectives is
supposed to have long-term eects on cognition and motivational style56. ese eects may be present even
though we asked the participants to refrain from taking the medicine before the testing. We found, however,
that suboptimal decision making was a stable trait in the ADHD group suggesting that CNS medication did not
have a long-term eect on improving the ability to choose optimal options in ambiguous and risky situations (on
the gambling task)—besides the short-term eects shown previously on the CGT
57. is is in line with a recent
study on the eects of methylphenidate in adolescents with ADHD, in which no long-term eects were found on
any of the outcome measures including on cognitive functioning14. It is possible that medication may have led
to a higher tolerance for delay at the follow-up assessment, or it may be due to the children with ADHD being
older and more mature in their decision making abilities. In relation to social competence and the eect of CNS
medication, one systematic review found that children treated with medication and/or non-pharmacological
interventions for their ADHD had better social functioning compared to untreated children with ADHD58. Other
studies, however, have found that methylphenidate alone, or in combination with cognitive behavioral therapy,
had only a limited eect on social behavior in children with ADHD1114. In boys with ADHD, the positive eect
of methylphenidate was specically observed in higher compliance with expected behavior and a decrease in
aggressive behavior tendencies12.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
7
Vol.:(0123456789)
Scientic Reports | (2024) 14:6535 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-57041-x
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Conclusion
Interpersonal problems are recognized as one of the important impairments negatively aecting quality of life
in children with ADHD58,59. Early predictors and mechanisms of interpersonal problems are thus essential to
identify15. e current study showed that suboptimal decision making driven by delay aversion can be an impor-
tant predictor to address in future studies of interpersonal competence in ADHD. A deeper understanding of
this relationship may initiate specic programs of psychoeducation about living with ADHD for caregivers and
the children. is relationship may also be important to address in training programs to improve quality of life
for children with ADHD.
Methods
Participants
e current study was a follow-up of children 8 to 12years old with ADHD and age-matched typically developing
peers performing the CGT
20. e children were originally included at baseline (T1) with a Full-Scale Intelligent
Quotient (FSIQ) > 8060 and diagnostically evaluated with the Schedule for Aective Disorders and Schizophrenia
for School-Age Children—Present and Lifetime Version (K-SADS)61. e K-SADS was re-administered at the
follow-up assessment (T2). e test administrators of the neuropsychological test battery including the CGT
were blinded to group status both at T1 and T2. e parents reported on the children’s’ mental health and social
functioning at both time points (described below). At T2, children were rst interviewed with the K-SADS
while parents lled out questionnaires, and subsequently the children performed a neuropsychological test bat-
tery and lled out questionnaires while the parents were interviewed with the K-SADS. e study protocol was
approved by the Regional committee for medical research ethics of western Norway (study number: 2014/1304)
and research was conducted according to relevant regulations and guidelines. Informed consent was given both
orally and in written form from all the parents and the adolescents. Both the adolescents’ (at T2) and their parents
(at T1 and T2) signed written consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. e participants received
a reimbursement of $115.
Cambridge gambling task (CGT)
e CGT from the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery, CANTAB; www. camcog. com 16,17,
was administered at T1 and T2 (see Fig.4). e children were rst instructed that a yellow token was hidden
behind either a blue or red box, presented in an array of 10 boxes at the top of the computer screen. Secondly,
they were instructed to make a bet on the likelihood of their decision being correct or not. Points were presented
in a box on the right-hand side in 5-s increments/decrements sequences. e children touched the box to place
a bet. In four of the test blocks, the bets were presented in an ascending order of 5, 25, 50, 75, and 95% por-
tion of the points that the children would “earn” on each trial (displayed on the le-hand side of the computer
screen), and in the other four blocks they were presented in a descending order (95, 75, 50, 25, and 5% portion
of the points). e order of presentation (ascending or descending) was counterbalanced across individuals
within the groups at both time points. e task was administered on a desktop PC and responses recorded via a
touch-sensitive screen (see Sørensen etal.20 for further details). As measures of suboptimal decision making, we
included the CGT generated scores of (a) risk adjustment measuring the ability in adjusting decisions according
to level of risk by learning from previous choices, (b) delay aversion measured the proportion of choices made
for the option available most immediately irrespective of the outcome, (c) reection time (inhibitory control)
measured the time taken to think about the options during the decision phase and was calculated based on the
deliberation time between stimulus presentation and choice outcome, and (d) risk proneness was dened by the
total number of points that were gambled on the most improbable outcome—reecting the overall tendency to
take risks. ese scores were centralized as z-scores.
Social problems and conduct and anxiety/depression problems
Parents completed the Child Behavior Checklist 6–18 (CBCL)38 at T1 and T2. e CBCL is a 113-item rating
scale with a 3-point response scale, from 0 for not true to 2 for very true/oen true. e CBCL is a widely used
Figure4. A screen shot of the Cambridge Gambling Task (CGT). e red and blue boxes at the top of the
screen are hiding a yellow token and the red and blue squares at the bottom of the screen are pushed to guess
which colored box the token is hidden behind. e numbers presented represent percentages of the points
displayed at the le-hand side that are presented either in ascending or descending sequences. Note. e image
is printed with permission from © Copyright 2023 Cambridge Cognition Limited. All rights reserved.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
8
Vol:.(1234567890)
Scientic Reports | (2024) 14:6535 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-57041-x
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
instrument with excellent test–retest reliability, internal consistency, and interrater reliability38. e total score
from the subscale of social problems was used as a measure of interpersonal problems. e items of this subscale
relate to dierent areas of social functioning including peer rejection, interaction style, impact of peer rejection
(feeling lonely), and behaviors that are observed with peer rejection such as clumsiness and speech problems.
e subscales of social problems, anxiety/depression problems, and conduct problems have all been shown to
have high test–retest reliability38 and acceptable internal consistency (≥ 0.70) with the Norwegian translation62.
e standardized percentile scores were included in the statistical analyses.
Statistical analyses
All analyses were performed with SPSS, version 28. e longitudinal sample was rst described by testing group
dierences on sample characteristics with independent sample t-tests. To test for longitudinal eects of ADHD
on decision making, four analyses of variances (ANOVAs) were conducted with a 2 (ADHD vs. no ADHD) × 2
(eects of timepoint vs. no eects of timepoints) factorial design to test for between-group dierences on the
CGT scores of risk adjustment, delay aversion, reection time (inhibitory control), and risk proneness. An
interaction eect between ADHD and time points would suggest that the eect of ADHD on one of the CGT
scores would be specic to one of the time points. Age and gender were not included as covariates in the nal
ANOVA models due to not changing the signicant eects of ADHD on the CGT scores. See Supplemental
Table3 for the age-adjusted ANOVA results in which age only covaried with delay aversion and gender only
with risk proneness (see also Supplemental Tables4 and 5). Further, the eect of ADHD on social problems and
problems of conduct behavior and anxiety/depression was investigated with independent sample t-test analyses.
Bivariate correlations were estimated rst on the relationship between the parent-reported problem scores and
thereaer on the cross-sectional and longitudinal relationship between the CGT scores and the parent-reported
scores of social problems, conduct problems, and anxiety/depression problems. e baseline CGT scores (T1)
that correlated with the social and conduct and anxiety/depression problems at follow-up (T2) were further
included (simultaneous) as predictors in three multiple linear regression analyses with the follow-up parent
reported scores on social problems, conduct problems, and anxiety/depression problems as the outcome vari-
ables, respectively. Age was not included as a covariate in the nal linear regression models due to not covarying
with the parent-reported outcome scores of social problems, conduct problems, or anxiety/depression problems
(see Supplemental Table6 for the bivariate correlations between age and the CBCL scores of social problems,
anxiety/depression problems, and conduct problems).
We adjusted for multiple analyses by using Bonferroni correction of alpha level (α) in the between-group
analyses (ANOVAs); p = 0.05/4, (four CGT outcome scores) which gives an α corrected p level of 0.013, and in
the multiple linear regression analysis with parent-reported problem scores as the outcome variables; p = 0.05/3
(three CBCL outcome scores), which gives an α corrected p level of 0.017. Outliers were dened using a ± 3
standard deviations threshold from the sample mean and replaced and included in the statistical analyses with
a score of ± 3 standard deviations from the sample mean. On the CGT: One child with ADHD showed very long
reection time (z = 4.24) and two typically developing children showed very high scores on risk adjustment
(z = 3.18 and z = 3.52). No outlier scores were detected on the CBCL.
A priori estimation of statistical power was performed to determine the sample size needed to estimate longi-
tudinal statistical eects of ADHD on suboptimal decision making. Using g*power, we included the mean scores
of risk adjustment of the ADHD group (0.61) and the controls (1.20), respectively, from T120, and adjusted for
the statistical variance (SD = 0.65) of the ADHD group. We specied the conventional level of sucient power
to be 1−ß = 0.80 and a signicance of α = 0.05. e analysis revealed that a total number of n = 42 young people
would at least be required to avoid a type II error in our study, which is below our sample size of n = 47.
Data availability
e datasets used and analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reason-
able request.
Received: 24 May 2023; Accepted: 13 March 2024
References
1. de Boo, G. M. & Prins, P. J. Social incompetence in children with ADHD: Possible moderators and mediators in social-skills train-
ing. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 27, 78–97. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. cpr. 2006. 03. 006 (2007).
2. Hoza, B. et al. What aspects of peer relationships are impaired in children with attention-decit/hyperactivity disorder?. J. Consult.
Clin. Psychol. 73, 411–423. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/ 0022- 006X. 73.3. 411 (2005).
3. Landau, S. & Miilich, R. Social communication of attention-decit-disordered boys. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 16, 69–81. https://
doi. org/ 10. 1007/ BF009 10501 (1988).
4. Ros, R. & Graziano, P. A. So cial functioning in children with or at risk for attention decit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analytic
review. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 47, 213–235. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1080/ 15374 416. 2016. 12666 44 (2018).
5. Aduen, P. A. et al. Social problems in ADHD: Is it a skills acquisition or performance problem?. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 40,
440–451. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/ s10862- 018- 9649-7 (2018).
6. Huang-Pollock, C. L., Mikami, A. Y., Pner, L. & McBurnett, K. Can executive functions explain the relationship between attention
decit hyperactivity disorder and social adjustment?. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 37, 679–691. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/ s10802- 009-
9302-8 (2009).
7. Cunningham, C. E. & Siegel, L. S. Peer interactions of normal and attention-decit-disordered boys during free-play, cooperative
task, and simulated classroom situations. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 15, 247–268. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/ BF009 16353 (1987).
8. Winsler, A. Parent-child interaction and privarte speech in boys with ADHD. Appl. Dev. Sci. 2, 17–39. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1207/
s1532 480xa ds0201_2 (1998).
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
9
Vol.:(0123456789)
Scientic Reports | (2024) 14:6535 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-57041-x
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
9. Lee, S. S. & Harris, L. T. How social cognition can inform social decision making. Front. Neurosci. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3389/ fnins.
2013. 00259 (2013).
10. ompson, K. N. et al. Do children with attention-decit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms become socially isolated? Longitudinal
within-person associations in a nationally representative cohort. JAACAP Open 1, 12–23. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. jaacop. 2023.
02. 001 (2023).
11. Alkalay, S. & Dan, O. Eect of short-term methylphenidate on social impairment in children with attention decit/hyperactivity
disorder: Systematic review. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry Ment. Health 16, 93. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1186/ s13034- 022- 00526-2 (2022).
12. Granger, D. A., Whalen, C. K., Henker, B. & Cantwell, C. ADHD boys’ behavior during structured classroom social activities:
Eects of social demands, teacher proximity, and methylphenidate. J. Attent. Disord. 1, 16–30. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1177/ 10870 54796
00100 102 (1996).
13. Hoza, B. et al. Peer-assessed outcomes in the multimodal treatment study of children with attention decit hyperactivity disorder.
J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 34, 74–86. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1207/ s1537 4424j ccp34 01_7 (2005).
14. Schweren, L. et al. Long-term eects of stimulant treatment on ADHD symptoms, social-emotional functioning, and cognition.
Psychol. Med. 49, 217–223. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1017/ S0033 29171 80005 45 (2019).
15. Morris, S., Sheen, J., Ling, M., Foley, D. & Sciberras, E. Interventions for adolescents with ADHD to improve peer social function-
ing: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Atten. Disord. 25, 1479–1496. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1177/ 10870 54720 906514 (2021).
16. Clark, L. et al. Dierential eects of insular and ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions on risky decision-making. Brain 131,
1311–1322. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1093/ brain/ awn066 (2008).
17. Rogers, R. D. et al. Dissociable decits in the decision-making cognition of chronic amphetamine abusers, opiate abusers, patients
with focal damage to prefrontal cortex, and tryptophan-depleted normal volunteers: Evidence for monoaminergic mechanisms.
Neuropsychopharmacology 20, 322–339. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/ S0893- 133X(98) 00091-8 (1999).
18. Ochsner, K. N. & Gross, J. J. Handbook of Emotion Regulation Vol. 2nd (e Guilford Press, 2014).
19. Solanto, M. V. et al. Neurocognitive functioning in AD/HD, predominantly inattentive and combined subtypes. J. Abnorm. Child
Psychol. 35, 729–744. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/ s10802- 007- 9123-6 (2007).
20. Sorensen, L. et al. Suboptimal decision making by children with ADHD in the face of risk: Poor risk adjustment and delay aversion
rather than general proneness to taking risks. Neuropsychology 31, 119–128. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/ neu00 00297 (2017).
21. Bitsakou, P., Psychogiou, L., ompson, M. & Sonuga-Barke, E. J. Delay aversion in attention decit/hyperactivity disorder: An
empirical investigation of the broader phenotype. Neuropsychologia 47, 446–456. https:// doi. o r g/ 10. 1016/j . n euro psych ologia. 2008.
09. 015 (2009).
22. Coghill, D. R., Seth, S. & Matthews, K. A comprehensive assessment of memory, delay aversion, timing, inhibition, decision mak-
ing and variability in attention decit hyperactivity disorder: Advancing beyond the three-pathway models. Psychol. Med. 44,
1989–2001. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1017/ S0033 29171 30025 47 (2014).
23. Marco, R. et al. Delay and reward choice in ADHD: An experimental test of the role of delay aversion. Neuropsychology 23, 367–380.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/ a0014 914 (2009).
24. Sonuga-Barke, E., Bitsakou, P. & ompson, M. Beyond the dual pathway model: Evidence for the dissociation of timing, inhibi-
tory, and delay-related impairments in attention-decit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 49, 345–355.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. jaac. 2009. 12. 018 (2010).
25. Van Dessel, J. et al. Delay aversion in attention decit/hyperactivity disorder is mediated by amygdala and prefrontal cortex hyper-
activation. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 59, 888–899. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1111/ jcpp. 12868 (2018).
26. Dekkers, T. J. et al. Decision-making decits in ADHD are not related to risk seeking but to suboptimal decision-making: Meta-
analytical and novel experimental evidence. J. Atten. Disord. 25, 486–501. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1177/ 10870 54718 815572 (2021).
27. Bunford, N. et al. Attention-decit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms mediate the association between decits in executive function-
ing and social impairment in children. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 43, 133–147. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/ s10802- 014- 9902-9 (2015).
28. Rinsky, J. R. & Hinshaw, S. P. Linkages between childhood executive functioning and adolescent social functioning and psycho-
pathology in girls with ADHD. Child Neuropsychol. 17, 368–390. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1080/ 09297 049. 2010. 544649 (2011).
29. Koer, M. J. et al. Working memory decits and social problems in children with ADHD. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 39, 805–817.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/ s10802- 011- 9492-8 (2011).
30. Nixon, E. e social competence of children with attention decit hyperactivity disorder: A review of the literature. Child Psychol.
Psychiatry Rev. 6, 172–179. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1111/ 1475- 3588. 00342 (2001).
31. Flicek, M. Social status of boys with both academic problems and attention-decit hyperactivity disorder. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol.
20, 353–366. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/ BF009 18981 (1992).
32. Hilton, D. C., Jarrett, M. A., McDonald, K. L. & Ollendick, T. H. Attention problems as a mediator of the relation between executive
function and social problems in a child and adolescent outpatient sample. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 45, 777–788. https:// doi. org/
10. 1007/ s10802- 016- 0200-6 (2017).
33. Koer, M. J. et al. Heterogeneity in ADHD: Neurocognitive predictors of peer, family, and academic functioning. Child Neuropsy-
chol. 23, 733–759. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1080/ 09297 049. 2016. 12050 10 (2017).
34. Tseng, W. L. & Gau, S. S. Executive function as a mediator in the link between attention-decit/hyperactivity disorder and social
problems. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 54, 996–1004. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1111/ jcpp. 12072 (2013).
35. Koer, M. J. et al. Neurocognitive and behavioral predictors of social problems in ADHD: A Bayesian framework. Neuropsychology
32, 344–355. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/ neu00 00416 (2018).
36. Roberts, D. K., Alderson, R. M., Betancourt, J. L. & Bullard, C. C. Attention-decit/hyperactivity disorder and risk-taking: A
three-level meta-analytic review of behavioral, self-report, and virtual reality metrics. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 87, 102039. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 1016/j. cpr. 2021. 102039 (2021).
37. Dekkers, T. J. et al. Risk taking by adolescents with attention-decit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): A behavioral and psycho-
physiological investigation of peer inuence. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 48, 1129–1141. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/ s10802- 020- 00666-z
(2020).
38. Achenbach, T. M. & Rescorla, L. A. Manual for the ASEBA School-Age Forms & Proles (University of Vermont Research Center
for Children. Youth. & Families, 2001).
39. Evans, S. C. et al. Examining ODD/ADHD symptom dimensions as predictors of social, emotional, and academic trajectories in
middle childhood. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 49, 912–929. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1080/ 15374 416. 2019. 16446 45 (2020).
40. Elowsky, J. et al. Dierential associations of conduct disorder, callous-unemotional traits and irritability with outcome expecta-
tions and values regarding the consequences of aggression. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry Ment. Health 16, 38. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1186/
s13034- 022- 00466-x (2022).
41. Savage, J. et al. A genetically informed study of the longitudinal relation between irritability and anxious/depressed symptoms. J.
Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 54, 377–384. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. jaac. 2015. 02. 010 (2015).
42. Brown, V., Morgan, T. & Fralick, A. Isolation and mental health: thinking outside the box. Gen. Psychiatr. 34, e100461. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 1136/ gpsych- 2020- 100461 (2021).
43. Ito, S. Pharmacokinetics 101. Paediatr. Child Health 16, 535–536. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1093/ pch/ 16.9. 535 (2011).
44. Kimko, H. C., Cross, J. T. & Abernethy, D. R. Pharmacokinetics and clinical eectiveness of methylphenidate. Clin. Pharmacokinet.
37, 457–470. https:// doi. org/ 10. 2165/ 00003 088- 19993 7060- 00002 (1999).
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
10
Vol:.(1234567890)
Scientic Reports | (2024) 14:6535 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-57041-x
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
45. Bagwell, C. L., Molina, B. S., Pelham, W. E. Jr. & Hoza, B. Attention-decit hyperactivity disorder and problems in peer relations:
Predictions from childhood to adolescence. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 40, 1285–1292. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1097/ 00004
583- 20011 1000- 00008 (2001).
46. Heiman, T. An examination of peer relationships of children with and without attention decit hyperactivity disorder. School
Psychol. Int. 26, 330–339. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1177/ 01430 34305 055977 (2005).
47. Lemiere, J. et al. Brain activation to cues predicting inescapable delay in adolescent attention decit/hyperactivity disorder: An
fMRI pilot study. Brain Res. 1450, 57–66. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. brain res. 2012. 02. 027 (2012).
48. Wilbertz, G. et al. Neural and psychophysiological markers of delay aversion in attention-decit hyperactivity disorder. J. Abnorm.
Psychol. 122, 566–572. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/ a0031 924 (2013).
49. orsen, A. L., Meza, J., Hinshaw, S. & Lundervold, A. J. Processing speed mediates the longitudinal association between ADHD
symptoms and preadolescent peer problems. Front. Psychol. 8, 2154. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3389/ fpsyg. 2017. 02154 (2017).
50. Sonuga-Barke, E. J. e dual pathway model of AD/HD: An elaboration of neuro-developmental characteristics. Neurosci. Biobehav.
Rev. 27, 593–604. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. neubi orev. 2003. 08. 005 (2003).
51. Iqbal, N. & Dar, K. A. Negative aectivity, depression, and anxiety: Does rumination mediate the links?. J. Aect. Disord. 181,
18–23. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. jad. 2015. 04. 002 (2015).
52. Wolf, C. & Lappe, M. Motivation by reward jointly improves speed and accuracy, whereas task-relevance and meaningful images
do not. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 85, 930–948. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3758/ s13414- 022- 02587-z (2023).
53. Stratta, P., Cella, M., Di Emidio, G., Collazzoni, A. & Rossi, A. Exploring the association between the Iowa Gambling Task and
community functioning in people with schizophrenia. Psychiatr. Danubina 27, 371–377 (2015).
54. Parker, A. M. & Fischho, B. Decision-making competence: External validation through an individual-dierences approach. J.
Behav. Decis. Making 18, 1–27. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1002/ bdm. 481 (2005).
55. Humphreys, K. L., Galan, C. A., Tottenham, N. & Lee, S. S. Impaired social decision-making mediates the association between
ADHD and social problems. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 44, 1023–1032. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/ s10802- 015- 0095-7 (2016).
56. Abiko, H. et al. Symptomatic improvement in children with ADHD treated with long-term methylphenidate and multimodal
psychosocial treatment. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 43, 802–811. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1097/ 01. chi. 00001 28791. 10014. ac
(2004).
57. DeVito, E. E. et al. e eects of methylphenidate on decision making in attention-decit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol. Psychiatry
64, 636–639. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. biops ych. 2008. 04. 017 (2008).
58. Harpin, V., Mazzone, L., Raynaud, J. P., Kahle, J. & Hodgkins, P. Long-term outcomes of ADHD: A systematic review of self-esteem
and social function. J. Atten. Disord. 20, 295–305. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1177/ 10870 54713 486516 (2016).
59. Coghill, D., Danckaerts, M., Sonuga-Barke, E., Sergeant, J. & Group, A. E. G. Practitioner review: Quality of life in child mental
health–conceptual challenges and practical choices. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 50, 544–561. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1111/j. 1469- 7610.
2009. 02008.x (2009).
60. Wechsler, D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children 4th edn. (Psychological Corporation, 2023).
61. Kaufman, J. et al. Schedule for aective disorders and schizophrenia for school-age children-present and lifetime version (K-SADS-
PL): Initial reliability and validity data. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 36, 980–988. https:// doi . org/ 10. 1097/ 00004 583- 19970
7000- 00021 (1997).
62. Jozeak, T., Larsson, B., Wichstrom, L. & Rimehaug, T. Competence and emotional/behavioural problems in 7–16-year-old Nor-
wegian school children as reported by parents. Nord. J. Psychiatry 66, 311–319. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3109/ 08039 488. 2011. 638934
(2012).
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank the children and parents who participated in the study and Dr. Hayley MacDonald for
language editing the manuscript. is work was supported by grants from the Research Council of Norway
(190544/H110), the Western Norway Health Authority (MoodNet and the Network for Anxiety Disorders;
911435, 911607, 911827) to KP and by grants from the Western Norway Health Authority (911460) and the
National Norwegian ADHD network to LS. is paper represents independent research part funded by the NIHR
Maudsley Biomedical Research Centre at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King’s College
London. e views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NIHR or the Department
of Health and Social Care. Figure4 was printed with permission from Cambridge Cognition.
Author contributions
LS: Study design, experimental design, data collection, data analysis, interpretation, writing of the manuscript.
SA: Data collection, interpretation, writing of the manuscript. EK: Data collection, revision of the manuscript.
HE: Data collection, revision of the manuscript. KP: Study design, experimental design, data collection, revision
of the manuscript. ESB: Guiding data analysis, interpretation, writing of the manuscript.
Funding
Open access funding provided by University of Bergen.
Competing interests
Lin Sørensen has received small research funding for speaking and conference support from Medice in 2023.
Edmund Sonuga-Barke declares competing interest during the 3years prior to July 2023: Speaker fees, consul-
tancy or research funding from Medice, Takeda, Neurotech Solutions and QBTech. All other authors declare
no competing interests.
Additional information
Supplementary Information e online version contains supplementary material available at https:// doi. org/
10. 1038/ s41598- 024- 57041-x.
Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to L.S.
Reprints and permissions information is available at www.nature.com/reprints.
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and
institutional aliations.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
11
Vol.:(0123456789)
Scientic Reports | (2024) 14:6535 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-57041-x
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Open Access is article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International
License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or
format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the
Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. e images or other third party material in this
article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the
material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not
permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from
the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http:// creat iveco mmons. org/ licen ses/ by/4. 0/.
© e Author(s) 2024
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
Terms and Conditions
Springer Nature journal content, brought to you courtesy of Springer Nature Customer Service Center GmbH (“Springer Nature”).
Springer Nature supports a reasonable amount of sharing of research papers by authors, subscribers and authorised users (“Users”), for small-
scale personal, non-commercial use provided that all copyright, trade and service marks and other proprietary notices are maintained. By
accessing, sharing, receiving or otherwise using the Springer Nature journal content you agree to these terms of use (“Terms”). For these
purposes, Springer Nature considers academic use (by researchers and students) to be non-commercial.
These Terms are supplementary and will apply in addition to any applicable website terms and conditions, a relevant site licence or a personal
subscription. These Terms will prevail over any conflict or ambiguity with regards to the relevant terms, a site licence or a personal subscription
(to the extent of the conflict or ambiguity only). For Creative Commons-licensed articles, the terms of the Creative Commons license used will
apply.
We collect and use personal data to provide access to the Springer Nature journal content. We may also use these personal data internally within
ResearchGate and Springer Nature and as agreed share it, in an anonymised way, for purposes of tracking, analysis and reporting. We will not
otherwise disclose your personal data outside the ResearchGate or the Springer Nature group of companies unless we have your permission as
detailed in the Privacy Policy.
While Users may use the Springer Nature journal content for small scale, personal non-commercial use, it is important to note that Users may
not:
use such content for the purpose of providing other users with access on a regular or large scale basis or as a means to circumvent access
control;
use such content where to do so would be considered a criminal or statutory offence in any jurisdiction, or gives rise to civil liability, or is
otherwise unlawful;
falsely or misleadingly imply or suggest endorsement, approval , sponsorship, or association unless explicitly agreed to by Springer Nature in
writing;
use bots or other automated methods to access the content or redirect messages
override any security feature or exclusionary protocol; or
share the content in order to create substitute for Springer Nature products or services or a systematic database of Springer Nature journal
content.
In line with the restriction against commercial use, Springer Nature does not permit the creation of a product or service that creates revenue,
royalties, rent or income from our content or its inclusion as part of a paid for service or for other commercial gain. Springer Nature journal
content cannot be used for inter-library loans and librarians may not upload Springer Nature journal content on a large scale into their, or any
other, institutional repository.
These terms of use are reviewed regularly and may be amended at any time. Springer Nature is not obligated to publish any information or
content on this website and may remove it or features or functionality at our sole discretion, at any time with or without notice. Springer Nature
may revoke this licence to you at any time and remove access to any copies of the Springer Nature journal content which have been saved.
To the fullest extent permitted by law, Springer Nature makes no warranties, representations or guarantees to Users, either express or implied
with respect to the Springer nature journal content and all parties disclaim and waive any implied warranties or warranties imposed by law,
including merchantability or fitness for any particular purpose.
Please note that these rights do not automatically extend to content, data or other material published by Springer Nature that may be licensed
from third parties.
If you would like to use or distribute our Springer Nature journal content to a wider audience or on a regular basis or in any other manner not
expressly permitted by these Terms, please contact Springer Nature at
onlineservice@springernature.com
Preprint
Full-text available
This paper introduces an innovative approach to Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) diagnosis by employing deep learning (DL) techniques on electroencephalography (EEG) signals. This method addresses the limitations of current behavior-based diagnostic methods, which often lead to misdiagnosis and gender bias. By utilizing a publicly available EEG dataset and converting the signals into spectrograms, a Resnet-18 convolutional neural network (CNN) architecture was used to extract features for ADHD classification. The model achieved a high precision, recall, and an overall F1 score of 0.9. Feature extraction highlighted significant brain regions (frontopolar, parietal, and occipital lobes) associated with ADHD. These insights guided the creation of a three-part digital diagnostic system, facilitating cost-effective and accessible ADHD screening, especially in school environments. This system enables earlier and more accurate identification of students at risk for ADHD, providing timely support to enhance their developmental outcomes. This study showcases the potential of integrating EEG analysis with DL to enhance ADHD diagnostics, presenting a viable alternative to traditional methods.
Article
Full-text available
Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is one of the most common disorders in school-age children. In addition to learning difficulties associated with the disorder’s core symptoms of inattention and hyperactivity, children with ADHD display substantial social impairments. Methylphenidate (MPH) in formulations such as Ritalin or Concerta mitigates inattention and hyperactivity, but the effects of the therapy on social behavior in children with ADHD are not clear. This review aims to determine the effectiveness of short term (up to 6 months) MPH treatment on three domains of social skills in children aged 6–14 with ADHD: (i) Recognition of nonverbal emotional expressions, which are a marker of inherent (unlearned) social understanding, (ii) theory of mind (ToM) components that relate to learned cognition and social communication, and (iii) social competence in everyday environments. 15 relevant studies were identified based on inclusion/exclusion criteria. The results show mixed effects: the overall social performance as evaluated by parents, teachers or peers, and some components of ToM, were found to improve following a weeks-long course of MPH treatment. However, the effects of the medication are less clear when evaluating momentary/nonverbal social responses such as reactions to emotional facial expressions. While the findings of this review indicate that an MPH medication regime of order weeks to months could improve, to a degree, social impairment in children with ADHD, more studies are required to identify the medications’ mechanism and confirm such a conclusion.
Article
Full-text available
Visual selection is characterized by a trade-off between speed and accuracy. Speed or accuracy of the selection process can be affected by higher level factors – for example, expecting a reward, obtaining task-relevant information, or seeing an intrinsically relevant target. Recently, motivation by reward has been shown to simultaneously increase speed and accuracy, thus going beyond the speed-accuracy-trade-off. Here, we compared the motivating abilities of monetary reward, task-relevance, and image content to simultaneously increase speed and accuracy. We used a saccadic distraction task that required suppressing a distractor and selecting a target. Across different blocks successful target selection was followed either by (i) a monetary reward, (ii) obtaining task-relevant information, or (iii) seeing the face of a famous person. Each block additionally contained the same number of irrelevant trials lacking these consequences, and participants were informed about the upcoming trial type. We found that postsaccadic vision of a face affected neither speed nor accuracy, suggesting that image content does not affect visual selection via motivational mechanisms. Task relevance increased speed but decreased selection accuracy, an observation compatible with a classical speed-accuracy trade-off. Motivation by reward, however, simultaneously increased response speed and accuracy. Saccades in all conditions deviated away from the distractor, suggesting that the distractor was suppressed, and this deviation was strongest in the reward block. Drift-diffusion modelling revealed that task-relevance affected behavior by affecting decision thresholds, whereas motivation by reward additionally increased the rate of information uptake. The present findings thus show that the three consequences differ in their motivational abilities.
Article
Full-text available
Background Previous work has examined the association of aggression levels and callous-unemotional traits with outcome expectations and values regarding the consequences of aggression. Less work has examined the outcome expectations and values regarding the consequences of aggression of adolescents with Conduct Disorder (CD). Also, no studies have examined links between irritability (a second socio-affective trait associated with CD) and these social cognitive processes despite the core function of anger in retaliatory aggression and establishing dominance. Method The current study, investigating these issues, involved 193 adolescents (typically developing [TD; N = 106], 87 cases with CD [N = 87]). Participants completed an adaptation of the Outcomes Expectations and Values Questionnaire and were assessed for CU traits and irritability via the Inventory of Callous-Unemotional traits and the Affective Reactivity Index. Results While CD was associated with atypical outcome expectations this was not seen within statistical models including CU traits and irritability. CU traits were associated with decreased expectation that aggression would result in feelings of remorse and victim suffering, as well as decreased concern that aggressive acts would result in punishment and victim suffering. Irritability was associated with increased expectations and concern that aggression would result in dominance and forced respect. Conclusions The results suggest that CU traits and irritability, often present in youth with CD, are associated with different forms of maladaptive outcome expectations and values regarding the consequences of aggression. This suggests that the atypical social cognitive processes underlying aggressive behavior among youth exhibiting CU traits may differ from those exhibiting problems regulating anger.
Article
Full-text available
Adolescents with ADHD demonstrate increased risk-taking behavior (RTB) like substance abuse and dangerous traffic conduct. RTB in adolescence is more likely under peer influence. The current investigation (1) tests the hypothesis that adolescents with ADHD are particularly susceptible to such influence and (2) tests whether groups differed in autonomic reactivity to peer influence. Adolescent boys between 12 and 19 years with (n = 81) and without (n = 99) ADHD performed the Balloon Analogue Risk Task twice. In the peer condition, a highly credible virtual peer manipulation that encouraged risk taking was added, in the solo condition this was absent. Autonomic reactivity was indexed by heart rate (HR), pre-ejection period (PEP) and respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA). All adolescents engaged in more risk taking in the peer condition relative to solo condition. Autonomic differences between groups were only found on PEP: a stronger sympathetic response to peer influence was observed in typically developing adolescents relative to adolescents with ADHD. Increased physiological stress (as indexed by PEP) in the peer relative to the solo condition predicted peer-induced risk taking in all adolescents. We conclude that susceptibility to peer influence is not exaggerated in ADHD but rather reflects a general tendency of adolescents. As adolescents experiencing peer influence as stressful are most susceptible to peer influence, we suggest that increasing resistance to peer influence may be an important treatment aim for these adolescents specifically.
Article
Full-text available
Objective: Peer social functioning difficulties characteristic of ADHD persist into adolescence, but the efficacy of interventions for this age group remains unclear. Method: A systematic search of nonpharmacological interventions for adolescents with ADHD (10–18 years) identified 11 trials addressing social functioning, of which eight were included in meta-analyses. Results: Random effects meta-analyses of four randomized trials found no differences in social functioning between treatment and control groups by parent- ( g = −0.08 [−0.34, 0.19], k = 4, N = 354) or teacher-report ( g = 0.17 [−0.06, 0.40], k = 3, N = 301). Meta-analyses of nonrandomized studies indicated participants’ social functioning improved from baseline to postintervention by parent-report, but not teacher- or self-report. All trials had a high risk of bias. Conclusion: These results highlight the paucity of research in this age group. There is little evidence that current interventions improve peer social functioning. Clearer conceptualizations of developmentally relevant targets for remediation may yield more efficacious social interventions.
Article
Full-text available
The goal of this article is to investigate the symptom dimensions of oppositional defiant disorder (ODD; irritability, defiance) and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD; inattention, hyperactivity-impulsivity) as predictors of academic performance, depressive symptoms, and peer functioning in middle childhood. Children (N = 346; 51% female) were assessed via teacher-report on measures of ODD/ADHD symptoms at baseline (Grades K–2) and academic performance, depressive symptoms, peer rejection, and victimization on 7 occasions over 4 school years (K–2 through 3–5). Self-report and grade point average data collected in Grades 3–5 served as converging outcome measures. Latent growth curve and multiple regression models were estimated using a hierarchical/sensitivity approach to assess robustness and specificity of effects. Irritability predicted higher baseline depressive symptoms, peer rejection, and victimization, whereas defiance predicted higher baseline peer rejection; however, none of these ODD-related effects persisted 3 years later to Grades 3–5. In contrast, inattention predicted persistently poorer academic performance, persistently higher depressive symptoms, and higher baseline victimization; hyperactivity-impulsivity predicted subsequent peer rejection and victimization in Grades 3–5. In converging models, only inattention emerged as a robust predictor of 3-year outcomes (viz., grade point average, depressive symptoms, peer rejection, and relational victimization). Broadly, ODD dimensions—particularly irritability—may be linked to acute disturbances in social-emotional functioning in school-age children, whereas ADHD dimensions may predict more persistent patterns of peer, affective, and academic problems. By examining all 4 ODD/ADHD symptom dimensions simultaneously, the present analyses offer clarity and specificity regarding which dimensions affect what outcomes, and when. Findings underscore the importance of multidimensional approaches to research, assessment, and intervention.
Article
Full-text available
Objective: ADHD is related to decision-making deficits in real-life (e.g., substance abuse) and on experimental tasks (increased preference for risky options). In most tasks, risk and expected value are confounded (risky options have lowest expected value), making it impossible to disentangle risky from suboptimal (i.e., not choosing highest expected value) decision-making. We differentiated between risky and suboptimal decision-making in ADHD in two studies. Method and results: First, on a multilevel meta-regression analysis ( k = 48, n_ADHD = 1,144, n_Control = 1,108), ADHD and controls differed if the risky option was suboptimal (ADHD choosing more risky/suboptimal), whereas groups performed similar if the risky option was not suboptimal. Second, an empirical study showed that adults with ADHD ( n = 40) made more suboptimal, but not more risky choices than controls ( n = 40). Conclusion: These results contribute to a growing body of evidence that decision-making deficits in ADHD are driven by suboptimal decision-making and not by risk seeking.
Article
Objective: This study examined longitudinal associations between attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms and social isolation across childhood. The study tested the direction of this association across time, while accounting for preexisting characteristics, and assessed whether this association varied by ADHD presentation, informant, sex, and socioeconomic status. Method: Participants included 2,232 children from the Environmental Risk (E-Risk) Longitudinal Twin Study. ADHD symptoms and social isolation were measured at ages 5, 7, 10, and 12. Random-intercept cross-lagged panel models were used to assess the directionality of the association across childhood. Results: Children with increased ADHD symptoms were consistently at increased risk of becoming socially isolated later in childhood, over and above stable characteristics (β = .05-.08). These longitudinal associations were not bidirectional; isolated children were not at risk of worsening ADHD symptoms later on. Children with hyperactive ADHD presentation were more likely to become isolated, compared with inattentive presentation. This was evident in the school setting, as observed by teachers, but not by mothers at home. Conclusion: The study findings highlight the importance of enhancing peer social support and inclusion for children with ADHD, particularly in school settings. This study adds explanatory value beyond traditional longitudinal methods, as the results represent how individual children change over time, relative to their own preexisting characteristics. Diversity & inclusion statement: We worked to ensure sex and gender balance in the recruitment of human participants. We worked to ensure that the study questionnaires were prepared in an inclusive way. One or more of the authors of this paper self-identifies as a member of one or more historically underrepresented sexual and/or gender groups in science. We actively worked to promote sex and gender balance in our author group. The author list of this paper includes contributors from the location and/or community where the research was conducted who participated in the data collection, design, analysis, and/or interpretation of the work.
Article
Meta-analytic methods were used to examine ADHD-related risk-taking in children and adults, and to compare the magnitude of risk taking across behavioral, self-report, and virtual reality metrics. Potential moderators of effect size heterogeneity were also examined via a three-level multi-level approach and a hybrid meta-analytic/systematic review approach. Aggregated effect sizes obtained from 56 behavioral-task studies (82 effect sizes; ADHDN = 2577; TDN = 2606), 51 self-report studies (130 effect sizes; ADHDN = 18,641; TDN = 113,163), and 8 virtual reality studies (16 effect sizes; ADHDN = 382; TDN = 436) suggest that children and adults with ADHD exhibit moderately more risk-taking compared to children and adults without the disorder. Notably, the aggregated effect size obtained from virtual reality simulations (Hedges', g = 0.43) was 30–40% larger than effect sizes obtained from self-report and behavioral task metrics (Hedges' g = 0.31 and 0.27), respectively. Suboptimal Decision Making was the only significant moderator identified via multi-level modeling; however, comparison of subgroup effect sizes revealed potential moderating effects of ADHD presentation and trial-by-trial feedback on behavioral tasks. Collectively, findings suggest that ADHD is reliably associated with small to moderate magnitude greater risk-taking behavior and virtual reality simulations appear be the most sensitive currently available metric.