ArticlePDF Available

Kairos study protocol: a multidisciplinary approach to the study of school timing and its effects on health, well-being and students’ performance

Frontiers
Frontiers in Public Health
Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Recent evidence from chronobiology, chssronomedicine and chronopsychology shows that the organisation of social time (e.g., school schedules) generally does not respect biological time. This raises concerns about the impact of the constant mismatch between students’ social and internal body clocks on their health, well-being and academic performance. The present paper describes a protocol used to investigate the problem of (de) synchronisation of biological times (chronotypes) in childhood and youth in relation to school times. It studies the effects of student chronotype vs. school schedule matches/mismatches on health behaviours (e.g., how many hours students sleep, when they sleep, eat, do physical activity, spend time outdoors in daylight) and learning (verbal expression, spatial structuring, operations) and whether alert-fatigue levels mediate this effect alignments/misalignments on learning (verbal expression, spatial structuring, operations) and their mediation by alert-fatigue levels. The novelty of our protocol lies in its multidisciplinary and mixed methodology approach to a relevant and complex issue. It draws on up-to-date knowledge from the areas of biology, medicine, psychology, pedagogy and sociology. The methods employed include a varied repertoire of techniques from hormonal analysis (cortisol and melatonin), continuous activity and light monitoring, self-registration of food intake, sleep timings, exercise and exposure to screens, alongside with systematic application of cognitive performance tests (e.g., memory, reasoning, calculation, attention) and self-reported well-being. This comprehensive and interdisciplinary protocol should support evidence-based education policy measures related to school time organisation. Appropriate and healthier school timetables will contribute to social change, healthier students and with more efficient learning. The results of studies using a similar methodology in other countries would ensure replication and comparability of results and contribute to knowledge to support policy making.
This content is subject to copyright.
Frontiers in Public Health 01 frontiersin.org
Kairos study protocol: a
multidisciplinary approach to the
study of school timing and its
eects on health, well-being and
students’ performance
DanielGabaldón-Estevan
1
*, DiegoCarmona-Talavera
2,
BelénCatalán-Gregori
3, ElenaMañas-García
1,
VanessaMartin-Carbonell
4, LucíaMonfort
5,
ElviraMartinez-Besteiro
6, MònicaGonzález-Carrasco
7,
MaríaJesúsHernández-Jiménez
8, KadriTäht
9, MartaTalavera
10,
AnaAncheta-Arrabal
11, GuillermoSáez
2,12, NuriaEstany
2,
GonzaloPin-Arboledas
13 and CatiaReis
14,15,16 on behalf of the
Research Group on School Health and Well-being (GISBE)
1 Department of Sociology and Social Anthropology, University of Valencia, Valencia, Spain, 2 Service of
Clinical Analysis, University Hospital Dr. Peset, Valencia, Spain, 3 Department of Education, Valencian
International University, Valencia, Spain, 4 Department of Pediatrics, Dr. Peset University Hospital,
Valencia, Spain, 5 Department of Pediatrics, Clinical University Hospital, Valencia, Spain, 6 Department
of Personality, Assessment and Psychological Treatments, University of Valencia, Valencia, Spain,
7 Research Institute on Quality of Life, University of Girona, Girona, Spain, 8 Faculty of Health Sciences,
Valencian International University, Valencia, Spain, 9 Institute of International Social Studies, School of
Governance, Law and Society, Tallinn University, Tallinn, Estonia, 10 Department of Experimental and
Social Sciences Teaching, University of Valencia, Valencia, Spain, 11 Department of Comparative
Education and History of Education, University of Valencia, Valencia, Spain, 12 Department of
Biochemistry and Molecular Biology, Faculty of Medicine and Dentistry, University of Valencia,
Valencia, Spain, 13 Grupo de Sueño y Cronobiologia de la Asociación Española de Pediatría, Valencia,
Spain, 14 CRC-W- Faculdade de Ciências Humanas, Universidade Católica Portuguesa, Lisbon,
Portugal, 15 Instituto de Medicina Molecular João Lobo Antunes, IMM, Lisboa, Lisbon, Portugal,
16 ISAMB - Faculdade de Medicina Universidade de Lisboa, Lisbon, Portugal
Recent evidence from chronobiology, chssronomedicine and chronopsychology
shows that the organisation of social time (e.g., school schedules) generally
does not respect biological time. This raises concerns about the impact of the
constant mismatch between students’ social and internal body clocks on their
health, well-being and academic performance. The present paper describes a
protocol used to investigate the problem of (de) synchronisation of biological
times (chronotypes) in childhood and youth in relation to school times. It studies
the eects of student chronotype vs. school schedule matches/mismatches
on health behaviours (e.g., how many hours students sleep, when they sleep,
eat, do physical activity, spend time outdoors in daylight) and learning (verbal
expression, spatial structuring, operations) and whether alert-fatigue levels
mediate this eect alignments/misalignments on learning (verbal expression,
spatial structuring, operations) and their mediation by alert-fatigue levels. The
novelty of our protocol lies in its multidisciplinary and mixed methodology
approach to a relevant and complex issue. It draws on up-to-date knowledge
from the areas of biology, medicine, psychology, pedagogy and sociology. The
methods employed include a varied repertoire of techniques from hormonal
analysis (cortisol and melatonin), continuous activity and light monitoring, self-
registration of food intake, sleep timings, exercise and exposure to screens,
alongside with systematic application of cognitive performance tests (e.g.,
OPEN ACCESS
EDITED BY
Kevin Dadaczynski,
Fulda University of Applied Sciences,
Germany
REVIEWED BY
Guadalupe Rodriguez Ferrante,
University of Washington, UnitedStates
Anat Lan,
Academic College Tel Aviv-Yao, Israel
*CORRESPONDENCE
Daniel Gabaldón-Estevan
Daniel.Gabaldon@uv.es
RECEIVED 09 November 2023
ACCEPTED 26 February 2024
PUBLISHED 08 March 2024
CITATION
Gabaldón-Estevan D, Carmona-Talavera D,
Catalán-Gregori B, Mañas-García E,
Martin-Carbonell V, Monfort L,
Martinez-Besteiro E, González-Carrasco M,
Hernández-Jiménez MJ, Täht K, Talavera M,
Ancheta-Arrabal A, Sáez G, Estany N,
Pin-Arboledas G and Reis C (2024) Kairos
study protocol: a multidisciplinary approach
to the study of school timing and its eects
on health, well-being and students’
performance.
Front. Public Health 12:1336028.
doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2024.1336028
COPYRIGHT
© 2024 Gabaldón-Estevan, Carmona-
Talavera, Catalán-Gregori, Mañas-García,
Martin-Carbonell, Monfort, Martinez-Besteiro,
González-Carrasco, Hernández-Jiménez,
Täht, Talavera, Ancheta-Arrabal, Sáez, Estany,
Pin-Arboledas and Reis. This is an open-
access article distributed under the terms of
the Creative Commons Attribution License
(CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction
in other forums is permitted, provided the
original author(s) and the copyright owner(s)
are credited and that the original publication
in this journal is cited, in accordance with
accepted academic practice. No use,
distribution or reproduction is permitted
which does not comply with these terms.
TYPE Study Protocol
PUBLISHED 08 March 2024
DOI 10.3389/fpubh.2024.1336028
Gabaldón-Estevan et al. 10.3389/fpubh.2024.1336028
Frontiers in Public Health 02 frontiersin.org
memory, reasoning, calculation, attention) and self-reported well-being. This
comprehensive and interdisciplinary protocol should support evidence-based
education policy measures related to school time organisation. Appropriate
and healthier school timetables will contribute to social change, healthier
students and with more ecient learning. The results of studies using a similar
methodology in other countries would ensure replication and comparability of
results and contribute to knowledge to support policy making.
KEYWORDS
childhood, adolescence, circadian rhythms, chronotype, school schedule, social
jetlag, well-being
1 Introduction
Since the 1920s, it has been found that circadian rhythms
inuence more than 100 human functions (1). Circadian rhythms are
synchronised to the 24-h light/dark cycle (solar time), the most visible
circadian rhythm being the sleep-awake cycle. However, people adapt
dierently to the environment and we call these dierences
chronotypes. ere is a physiological response to light exposure, and
a later exposure to light reects in a later sleep–wake behaviour.
Residents in countries located more west of their time zones tend to
exhibit a later sleep–wake behaviour (2). e most extreme west
country in the Central European Time zone (CET) is Spain (3, 4),
which makes its residents more vulnerable to be a late sleep
chronotype and, consequently, more vulnerable to circadian
misalignment (abnormal timing between dierent cycles) (5). In fact,
as pointed out by Pin etal. (6), 37% of adolescents aged 12 to 15 from
the Valencia region (Spain) believe they get little sleep during the
week. Higher misalignment between solar time and clock time (solar
jetlag) has been associated with shorter sleep duration, especially in
late chronotypes (7). e group most aected by sleep loss is
adolescents since during this phase of their development they present
an endogenous delay in their biological rhythms and a greater
diculty to build sleep pressure (5) resulting in a later sleep onset
timing and higher circadian misalignment. A highly used proxy for
circadian misalignment is the so called ‘social jetlag’ that is calculated
by the dierence between the midpoint of sleep on free days and
workdays (3). Circadian misalignment can potentially aect health (8,
9) and well-being during adolescence, a life stage characterised by a
decline in subjective well-being (10, 11). e potential eects of
circadian misalignment on both physical and mental health during
adolescence are likely to bereected by high levels of sleep deprivation
or even school absence (12), while a better alignment between
chronotype and school timing is associated with lower grade retention
in adolescents (13).
As was shown in the 2011 Progress in International Reading
Literacy Study (PIRLS) and Trends in International Mathematics and
Science Study (TIMSS) a gap in reading ability and mathematics (11
points dierence in each case) was associated with sleep deprivation
(14, 15). e number of 4th-year (10 year old) students suering sleep
deprivation was 49%, which increases by 10% for 8th grade students
(14 years old). Meijer (16) pointed out that chronic reductions in sleep
can have direct and indirect negative eects on school performance
through the eect on motivation and attention. Several studies show
that later school starting times improve students’ sleep (17),
performance (18) and life satisfaction (19).
School schedules are the main inuence on the organisation of
time in the lives of children and youth. ey aect how much time
they can sleep (1, 16, 2023). is inuence has been already reported
in several studies and for dierent ages (2427). e consequences of
not getting enough rest and going to classes at a time that is out of
kilter with their biological clock (during sleep time) aects students
school performance, with late chronotypes being the most severely
aected. Andrade and Menna-Barreto’s (28) study of adolescent girls
(16 year olds) showed that they tended to achieve worse scores for tests
held in the morning and higher scores for those held in the aernoon.
Wolfson and Carskadon (29) studied 3,120 students aged between 13
and 19 and found that students awarded the lowest grades (C, D, and
E in the North American system) went to bed 40 min later on average
and slept 25 min less a night on average compared to those students
who obtained the highest grades (A and B) (30). ere is other
evidence linking the late chronotype with poorer academic
performance (12, 31) and the positive eect on student grades of
scheduling exams later in the day and the week. For example, van der
Vinne etal. (32) show that the chronotype eect on academic results
disappears if exams are scheduled aer noon and Pin etal.s (33) study
shows that moving exams from Monday mornings to Wednesday’s
mid-morning, increased average scores by 1 point (out of 10). School
performance of late chronotypes becomes increasingly worse with age,
being worst at high school which can aect future academic career
and labour earning opportunities (34). Gromada and Shewbridge (30)
found that students attending the morning school schedule but with
later preferences on study time present higher levels of absenteeism.
Contrary to much debate on school timings, it seems that ‘when’
matters as much or even more than ‘how much’ and, to beinclusive,
school timetables would need to betailored to students’ optimum
study times and learning rhythms. Evidence from recent studies in
Latin America also show similar eects of the dierent school timings
on adolescents chronotype, sleep and performance (26, 27, 3537).
Insucient sleep and being forced to wake in their ‘biological
night’ can also aect students’ nutrition through skipping breakfast
(38) or being forced to eat during their biological night, which aects
insulin sensitivity and increases the risk of diabetes (39). PIRLS 2011
(15) and TIMSS 2011 (14) claim that 27–29% of 4th-year (age 10)
students, on average, are aected ‘somewhat or a lot’ by insucient
food intake before certain classes. is has a negative eect on reading
achievement and mathematics (14, 15). Evening or late chronotype
Gabaldón-Estevan et al. 10.3389/fpubh.2024.1336028
Frontiers in Public Health 03 frontiersin.org
students are the most disadvantaged; a later chronotype has been
linked to a higher probability of obesity and unhealthy behaviours
such as smoking, drinking and excess consumption of caeine (9,
38, 40).
Psychological and bio-psychological studies have been examining
the link between the time of the day that a task is performed and
performance eciency. Gabaldón-Estevan and Obiol-Francés (41)
point to the consensus about the ultradian oscillation of attention
rhythms among primary school students in terms of identication of
two alert cycles. Up to noon, attention (alertness) increases followed
bea decrease until 1,400 and then another increase which extends into
the evening hours. A study by Testu (42) indicates that children aged
between 10 and 11 years, show low levels of alertness between 0800
and 0900 and then increased alertness which peaks between 1,100 and
noon. Aer the school lunch break, alertness levels are again low, but
then increase and peak around 1,600. Similar alertness patterns have
been found to occur among children in the United Kingdom,
Germany, Spain, Israel and the UnitedStates (30).
How people evaluate their lives, regardless of age—in general and
in relation to specic life domains (family, friends, leisure time, etc.)—
has been described as Subjective Well-Being (SWB) (43). SWB has
both a cognitive (life satisfaction) component and a two dimensional
(positive and negative) aective component, which reects the
acknowledged tripartite structure of SWB theory (4446). In a parallel
with SWB, Psychological Well-Being (PWB) is also important for
children’s and adolescents’ overall well-being; several scholars suggest
that SWB and PWB are complementary concepts within the broader
construct of well-being (4749). e current protocol subscribes to
this idea.
In much of the literature, the focus has been either on SWB or
PWB, measured using dierent instruments and dierent
theoretical approaches, depending on the particular philosophical
origin [the hedonic tradition focuses on SWB in terms of feelings
of pleasure (happiness)]; the eudaimonic tradition focuses on the
feeling of happiness derived from feeling fullled as an individual.
In contrast to SWB instruments, PWB are not dierentiated by a
more cognitive or aective aspect but by the concrete dimensions
they include (e.g., self-acceptance, positive relationships with
others, autonomy, etc.).
Most protocols to date are either specic in their scope or limited
in their tools. Most studies use either cortisol or melatonin or
actigraphy or questionnaires to determine chronotype. Some of them
use two or three of these tools, but hardly any of them use the four as
in our proposed protocol. Several important studies relate chronotype
to academic performance, but not many include student wellbeing
with objective measures of aptitude. Finally, the protocol wepropose
is unique in that it also includes a complete study of the subjects’ time
use in connection to all the other tools used. Our proposed protocol
should advance research in this area of the School Health Promotion
by providing dierent objective measures (i.e., actimetry, hormonal
and cognitive function assessment) as well as subjective measures (i.e.,
questionnaires, diaries). Weconsider that our protocol could help
future researchers by providing a common measure which will ensure
the replicability of our method. Wecall the protocol Kairos, aer the
Greek mythological god, because weagree with him that the focus
should beon ‘opportunity’ rather than ‘fate.
e aim is to provide a reference protocol to promote the
organisation of school schedules that respect the needs and well-being
of children and adolescents. Figure1 depicts the objectives included
in the protocol.
e specic objectives of the Kairos protocol are:
O1—To characterise the chronotypes in the school population
based on the distribution of chronotypes across a sample of students
and applying objective (cortisol/melatonin in saliva, actigraphy data)
and subjective (questionnaire and diary) measures;
O2—To quantify the social jetlag induced by the mismatch
between student chronotype and social (school) schedule based on an
assessment of the occurrence of social jetlag in a sample of students,
measured as the dierence between the midsleep point on non-school
days (waking up without an alarm) and the midsleep point on
school days;
O3—To measure the eect of both chronotype and social jetlag
on sleep (length and quality), eating/mealtime patterns (eating time
and type of food consumed), active (non-sedentary) time and time
spent outside;
O4—To assess the eects of chronotype and social jetlag on
cognitive status (vigilance, alertness, attention) and skills (motor
coordination, simple calculation, memory tasks);
O5—To measure the eect of both chronotype and social jetlag
on the distribution of time use and satisfaction with performance of
activities in the student sample, through self-reported distribution of
the time devoted to sleeping, meals, playing video games, watching
TV/videos (screen time) and doing homework;
O6—To measure the eect of both chronotype and social jetlag
on students’ well-being, comprised by SWB (overall life satisfaction,
satisfaction with specic life domains, positive aect and negative
aect) -thus expanding the traditional tripartite model discussed
above with the quadripartite model recently proposed by Savahl etal.
(50), and PWB;
O7—To measure the eect of the school schedule on the
parameters included in O1 to O6; In the case of education systems
(e.g., Colombia, Germany, Italy, Mexico, Spain, etc.) where school
schedules may dier within a single education system (e.g., split vs.
compact, morning vs. evening, dierent start and/or break times, etc.),
O7 could include the comparison between these; O8—As stated
earlier, the ultimate aim of this Protocol is to contribute to the
promotion of school schedules that are responsive to, and therefore
respectful of, the needs and well-being of children and young people.
To facilitate the dissemination of results and promote social change,
weencourage the design of student-centred school schedules based
on the knowledge gained from the application of the Protocol.
e Kairos Study Protocol proposes a collaborative strategy
related to study design and collection and analysis of replicable and
comparable data. Section 2 describes the protocol in more detail and
Section 3 discusses its pros and cons.
2 Methods and analysis
e Kairos protocol is aimed at addressing the problem of
(de)-synchronised time experienced by children and youth.
We investigate the eects of the social jetlag of students on health
(how much sleep, timing of sleep, timing and quantity of food, time
spent on physical activity and time spent outdoors in daylight),
learning (verbal expression, spatial structuring), mediated by alert/
activation and fatigue levels, day-time activities (studying, socialising,
Gabaldón-Estevan et al. 10.3389/fpubh.2024.1336028
Frontiers in Public Health 04 frontiersin.org
time spent with family, time watching screens, etc.), and self-reported
well-being. Weexplore the impact of students’ social jetlag on their
lives in terms of health, learning, time use and well-being.
2.1 Target sample
Power analysis can becalculated considering the outcome of
interest and the statistical test to beused (e.g., T-test, ANOVA). e
signicance level is set at p < 0.05, for a power of 0.80. For our study,
the sample was dened as a minimum of 385 measurements/surveys
needed to have a 95% condence level that the true value is within
±5% of the measured/surveyed value. e sample includes school
students (6 to 18 years of age), enrolled in primary schools or
secondary (high) schools. e students are to beselected on the basis
of willingness to participate and agreement from parents or guardians.
e intervention demands informing schools, families and
participants and communication channels with the parties for queries
and to report the general ndings to the schools and participants
involved. Inclusion criteria are children of both sexes over 6 years of
age enrolled in compulsory education, who agree to participate in the
study and are enrolled in schools that agree to participate. Exclusion
criteria will benot in compulsory education, unable or unwilling to
follow the protocol, or not in a school that has agreed to participate.
e most critical aspects are recruitment and respondents’
consistency in following the protocol. To ensure this, it is necessary to
prepare information sheets explaining the use of each instrument, to
hold information meetings with the centre management, with the
teachers of the participating groups, with the students and with the
parents of the students. e schools, teachers, students and parents
must give their consent for the students to participate. is makes
random selection impossible, but to ensure a representative sample,
schools are to beselected based on certain criteria including social
class of students families. e recruitment strategy should
be top-down, rst obtaining permission from the Ministry of
Education to carry out the study, then obtaining the agreement of the
school management and class tutors, and only then recruiting families
and students. To do this, information letters and information sessions
FIGURE1
Specific objectives—the Kairos protocol.
Gabaldón-Estevan et al. 10.3389/fpubh.2024.1336028
Frontiers in Public Health 05 frontiersin.org
should be made available to potential participants, their families
and teachers.
2.2 Logistics for participants
e protocol is suited to both cross sectional and longitudinal
research designs. For example, in the latter case, the rst year’s data
obtained from screening 1 will include the full protocol for children
aged 6, 9 and 12 and the second year’s data obtained from screening 2
will be applied to the same students will then be aged 7, 10 and
13 years, and so on. is allows for collection of longitudinal data
which can beused to propose causality. To compensate for sample
dropout, additional individuals who meet the required criteria can
beincluded in subsequent screenings (see gure Flowchart of study
population in the Supplementary material for target pursue in
Kairos study).
e model explanatory variables are age, gender, chronotype,
social jetlag and school schedule. Our outcome variables are grouped
into health (sleep, eating, activity and time out), learning (cognitive:
wakefulness, alertness and attention; skills: motor coordination,
simple calculations, memory), time use (patterns and satisfaction with
activities) and well-being (SWB and PWB). Figure2 provides an
overview of the protocol.
Figure2 shows that the protocol combines both objective and
subjective measures to assess social jetlag and its eects on students
health, learning, time use and well-being. It uses a total of 11
instruments which capture the complexity of the topic under study. In
our biological measures/assessments (saliva, anthropometry),
weinclude actigraphy monitoring (of sleep, temperature, activity and
exposure to daylight) along with data derived from the sleep and time
use diaries, the cognitive assessment (attention and aptitude) tests and
questionnaires (Munich Chronotype Questionnaire—MCTQ, food/
eating questionnaire, timetable of activities and well-
being questionnaire).
e protocol is designed to beapplied over a period of 11 days to
allow for ve full schooldays plus four full non-school days, to allow
to test students’ attention on two dierent Mondays and on a ursday.
To maintain data condentiality, students are assigned individual
project codes. To allow for follow ups, the school administrations are
given the list of project codes assigned to participants and the parent
or guardian authorizations. Sociodemographic data and some
additional information (school marks, special needs, etc.) is to
beobtained directly from the relevant school administration. In the
succeeding subsections, wedescribe the individual instruments.
2.3 Instruments
2.3.1 Hormonal assessment based on saliva
samples
Previous studies have used the measurement of salivary cortisol
to assess the dierences between its levels and the correlation of these
dierences with the subjects’ chronotype (51). Measured salivary
cortisol on awakening and 1 h later, on two consecutive days, in 112
subjects who had previously been classied as morning (9 subjects) or
evening (29 subjects) chronotypes according to the Horne and
Ostberg Owl-and-Lark-Questionnaire. ey showed that those who
identied themselves as morning chronotypes had higher salivary
cortisol levels on waking (day 1) or 1 h aer waking (day 2) than those
who identied themselves as evening chronotypes. Petrowski etal.
(52) attempted to replicate the ndings of Kudielka etal. (51) in the
FIGURE2
Calendarization of tools.
Gabaldón-Estevan et al. 10.3389/fpubh.2024.1336028
Frontiers in Public Health 06 frontiersin.org
controlled environment of a sleep laboratory. In this study, 1,023
participants were classied as morning (29 subjects) or evening (59
subjects) chronotypes using the Morningness-Eveningness-
Questionnaire (MEQ). Saliva samples were taken on waking and aer
15 and 30 min. eir results showed that chronotype inuences
cortisol levels on awakening and subsequent cortisol levels, which
were higher in subjects with a morning chronotype. Weidenauer etal.
(53) found similar results, but they also looked at dierences between
weekdays and weekends. As Kunz-Ebrecht etal. (54) have already
observed, cortisol levels are higher on weekdays than on weekends.
On the other hand, these studies measured cortisol levels early in the
morning without assessing cortisol curves throughout the day. Miller
etal. (55) gather several results that suggest that more negative diurnal
cortisol slopes correlate with adaptive states of the subjects, while
more positive slopes, closer to zero, could indicate altered states of the
cortisol axis.
Regarding melatonin measurement Griefahn etal. (56) carried
out an hourly measurement for 24-26 h of salivary melatonin in a
controlled environment. e results showed that subjects identied
with morning chronotype (7 subjects) using the MEQ showed a
melatonin secretion prole with maximum concentration about 3 h
earlier than subjects with evening chronotype (14 subjects). Another
study with a very similar design with 33 subjects was carried out by
Liu etal. (57). e results of melatonin measurements every hour for
26 h also showed that subjects with morning chronotype according to
the MEQ have the melatonin peak earlier than those with
evening chronotype.
e same results were obtained by Lack etal. (10), showing 2–3 h
earlier in the peak of melatonin for subjects with morning chronotype.
In this protocol, subjects also underwent 27 h of monitoring and
measurement of cortisol every hour. In our study, wewill use the
information discussed above and the recommendation of Pandi-
Perumal etal. (58) on the indications for obtaining partial melatonin
curves. As the approximate time at which the maximum peak of
melatonin and Dim Light Melatonin Onset (DLMO) is obtained for
both subjects with morning and evening chronotype is known, wewill
reduce the intervention on the subjects since it is not feasible to
perform 24-26 h cortisol curves. In addition, wewill carry out the
measurements on three dierent days of the week, with the aim of
looking for possible dierences, as occurs in the case of cortisol.
Saliva samples are collected to measure Dim Light Melatonin
Onset (DLMO) and cortisol peak. To obtain the daily curve for
cortisol the samples will becollected at 0, 1, 4, 9and 13 h aer waking
(51, 55). e minimum saliva DLMO threshold is 4 pg./mL (59) and
the recommended partial melatonin curve to calculate DLMO should
include values between at least 19 and 23 h (probable delayed sleep
phase) or between 16 and 21 h (probable advanced sleep phase) (58),
for a minimum of ve samples in total (60). e relationship between
DLMO and sleep onset timing is on average ~ 2 h before sleep onset
(61, 62), although this value may vary substantially (e.g., DLMO
ranging from ~4.5 h before to 0.5 h aer sleep onset time) (62),
especially for late chronotypes (63, 64). As so, the ideal collection
timing for hourly saliva collections, would start 5 h before sleep onset
time and ends until 1-2 h aer sleep onset time. However, if having
budget restrictions, wemay reduce sample collections until 3 h before
habitual sleep onset and goes until 1 h aer habitual sleep onset time
in order to comply with the 5 timepoints for DLMO determination.
is allows for a combined salivary cortisol and melatonin collection
procedure based on 8 collection times: sleep time-3, sleep time-2,
sleep time-1, sleep time, sleep time + 1, awake time, awake time + 1 and
awake time + 4 (see Figure3). Cortisol and melatonin are measured at
the same time at 20 (sleep time-1) hours; the remaining sampling is
hormone-specic and takes place during execution of daily activities.
It will beanalysed the maximum cortisol levels rst thing in the
morning, the decline curve of cortisol during the day and the
DLMO. e interpretation of these cortisol and melatonin curves will
provide important information about the chronotype of the subjects
and allow their classication according to Kudielka etal. (51) and
Petrowski etal. (52). In addition, they will allow to delve deeper into
participants state of health as, for example, a more negative slope of
cortisol is associated with better health—according to Miller etal.
(55)—and an altered DLMO is associated with circadian imbalances
and sleep and mood disorders—according to Pandi-Perumal
etal. (58).
If the budget does not allow assessment of melatonin, the protocol
applied only to cortisol provides the daily curve for cortisol and
establishes the better-t saliva curve collection protocol for the
standard cortisol pattern. e sampling (collection of saliva in tubes)
should take place during execution of daily activities.
To reduce the level of intervention for the students, wesuggest
that sampling should beconned to Monday, ursday and Sunday
of the same week (see Figure1). is will allow assessment of the
evolution of cortisol and melatonin levels through a week and, also,
dierences across dierent days.
It is recommended that saliva is collected in Sarsted Salivette
Cortisol® tubes, following their sampling protocol. e students
should have not taken in any liquid or solid food (including chewing
gum) for at least 60 min before the samples are taken and must rinse
their mouths with water 10 min before sampling. e evening saliva
collections should beperformed under dim light (< 5lux). e swab
should remain in the mouth for 2 min to allow for maximum possible
sample volume. e tube must record the time of collection and the
nal sample should berefrigerated as soon as possible aer sampling.
Processing of the sample should beas per the tube manufacturer’s
instructions. Samples are centrifuged for 2 min at 1000 g. and the swab
then discarded safely. e sample can then be frozen (but never
refrozen) before the cortisol and melatonin concentrations are
determined, which requires that the samples are completely thawed
and homogenised. Determination of salivary cortisol is performed
using the Roche Cobas 6,000® and electrochemiluminescence
technology. Salivary melatonin is determined using the DRG-SLV-
4779 DRG® Melatonin direct (Salivary) ELISA (Enzyme-Linked
ImmunoSorbent Assay) kit; the plate is read using the Grifols
Trit u r u s ® analyser.
Saliva sampling is non-invasive and is performed by the student
(study subjects); it does not involve blood samples or any other clinical
intervention. In all cases, students or tutors will beinformed verbally
and through a detailed written protocol with pictograms about the
appropriate collection of the sample. In addition, sample collection
during school hours will besupervised by the research group. Finally,
they will beinstructed to remain seated or inactive as much as possible
and to collect saliva samples to measure melatonin in dim light. e
study protocol described above has been approved by the Dr. Peset
Hospital Bioethics Committee, Valencia (Spain).
Regarding the interpretation of the results: Axelsson etal. (65)
and Kudielka etal. (51) showed that early morning cortisol levels
Gabaldón-Estevan et al. 10.3389/fpubh.2024.1336028
Frontiers in Public Health 07 frontiersin.org
were higher in subjects with morning chronotype compared to
subjects with aernoon chronotype. Furthermore, several studies
summarised in Miller etal. (55) indicated that a greater (negative)
slope of cortisol decline during the day is indicative of subjects’
adaptation, whereas atter slopes of cortisol decline could indicate
deregulation of the axis. erefore, interpreting the curves with ve
daily cortisol points on three dierent days will allow to know the
patients’ chronotype more accurately. On the one hand, the
concentration of cortisol on waking or the maximum cortisol level
is measured rst thing in the morning, which allows to distinguish
between the morning and evening chronotype of each subject. On
the other hand, the slope of the curve will bestudied to assess the
health status of the subjects and to dierentiate those with alterations
in the cortisol secretion axis that may becaused, for example, by
stress. Furthermore, comparing this information between the
continuous shi and split shi groups can provide information that
allows us to assess which type of shi is most benecial for the
subjects. e data obtained will provide the students’ salivary
cortisol and DLMO and morning melatonin values and allow them
to belinked to their health, stress and sleep quality states. It will
show how these hormone levels are inuenced by or inuence the
other instruments (see Discussion section).
2.3.2 Actigraphy
Actigraphy is used to record the body rhythms that are controlled
by the circadian system (66, 67). ese rhythms are used as markers
and provide information on the working of the circadian system.
ese rhythms are inuenced, also, by other variables, such as light
and sleep, so we recommend recording more than one rhythm
simultaneously. To allow correct measurements and result, these
rhythms must bemonitored over several full days with high sampling
frequency, to reduce the variability inherent in individual lifestyles.
Alongside salivary secretion of melatonin and cortisol, the most
frequent rhythms are related to physical activity and body temperature
and are monitored using actigraphy. Actigraphy is a convenient and
cost-ecient method to record (activity-inactivity, temperature, and
light exposure, body position) over multiple days in a natural
environment. It is especially useful for samples that include infants
and young children whose sleep is likely to beaected by a laboratory
environment (6870).
e participants follow their usual routines at home and in their
school environment. eir activity is sampled at 10 Hz and stored in
every 30 s epochs. Measurements taken when the actimetry watch is
not being worn are excluded from the analysis and if in 1 day wehave
more than 4 h of missing data that day is eliminated for analysis.
Wecalculate sleep onset, sleep oset and sleep duration for schooldays
and non-school days according to Madrid-Navarro etal. (71). Wealso
measure nap behaviour since many adolescents nap to make up for
lack of sleep at night; this is an indirect indicator of how tired they are,
and the total sleep time should beestimated for the 24 h period. All
individuals involved in the actimetry test will berequired to use the
event marker of the device to provide the following information: sleep
onset and sleep oset times.
To record circadian rhythms and sleep, weuse the actigraphy
device Kronowise Feedback® multichannel device, which allows
simultaneous recording of skin temperature rhythms, physical activity,
position, light exposure (infrared, blue and total visible light) and
sleep over a prolonged time period, allowing, in turn, evaluation of
appearance of chronobiological changes (including sleep changes) and
follow-up over a full week.
e Kronowise Feedback® device (Kronohealth SL, Spain)
includes: 1. a temperature sensor with precision ±0.1°C at 25°C and
0.0635°C resolution, housed in a separate chamber to avoid thermal
interference from the battery and electronic components, and attached
to a metal plate that is in contact with the skin; 2. a triaxial MEMS
(Micro ElectroMechanical System) accelerometer calibrated with
linear and equal sensitivity along all three axes, within the range ± 2 g.
and sensitivity 0.001 g.; 3. 50/60 Hz.
FIGURE3
Saliva sampling procedure.
Gabaldón-Estevan et al. 10.3389/fpubh.2024.1336028
Frontiers in Public Health 08 frontiersin.org
e device records approximately 23 million raw data points
related to 14 primary variables and 1 estimated variable (sleep) that
reect the functioning of the circadian system. e information
obtained includes: (1) daily physical rhythms [integrated variable
TAPL—rist skin temperature (T), motor activity (A), body position
(P) and light exposure (L)] (7274); (2) sleep parameters such as sleep
latency, total sleep time, counts of Waking Aer Sleep Onset (WASO),
sleep eciency, activity during sleep and the timing of sleep (onset,
oset and mid-time); (3) data to allow calculation of the main
indicators of circadian health (regularity, contrast between day and
night, synchronisation and quality of rest).
e main circadian indexes will bedetermined by non-parametric
analysis, including:
VM5: average value of the variable during the 5 consecutive
hours of maximum values. It applies to variables that increase
during sleep, such as temperature and sleep;
HM5: central time of the 5 consecutive hours of maximum
values. Values that increase during rest periods;
VL10: Average value of the variable during the 10 consecutive
hours of minimum values. Variables such as temperature and
sleep, whose values decrease during awake periods;
HL10: central time of the 10 consecutive hours of minimum
values. Values, such as temperature and sleep, that decrease
during wake periods;
VM10: average value of the variable during the 10 consecutive
hours of maximum values. Variables such as motor activity,
position and light exposure, whose values increase during
awake periods;
HM10: central time of the 10 consecutive hours of maximum
values, for values, such as motor activity, position and light
exposure, that increase during awake periods;
VL5: average value of the variable during the 5 consecutive hours
of minimum values in relation to those variables, such as motor
activity, position and light exposure, whose values decrease
during sleep;
HL5: central time of the 5 consecutive hours of minimum values.
Values such as motor activity, position and light exposure, that
decrease during rest periods;
IS (Interdaily Stability): a score of the regularity of circadian
pattern over dierent days. is varies between 0 for a Gaussian
noise to 1 for a total stability, where the rhythm repeats itself
exactly, day aer day, weassess it for skin temperature, physical
activity, position, light exposure (infrared, blue and total visible
light) and sleep;
RA (Relative Amplitude): for variables that increase during
resting times or during activity times, we assess it for skin
temperature, physical activity, position, light exposure (infrared,
blue and total visible light) and sleep;
ES (Environmental Synchronisation): degree of synchronisation
between the phase marker of a circadian variable during a rest
phase (HM5 or HL5) and the centre of natural darkness taking
as the reference the Central European Daylight Time (CEDT)
and the location where the event is registered. is variable can
express values between 0 and 1. When the centre of a variable
coincides with the centre of natural darkness, ES is 1.
CHS or CHI (Circadian Healthy Score/Circadian Healthy Index):
is the main marker of circadian health. Its values range between
0 and 1. e value 1 indicates a rhythm perfectly synchronised
with the natural light–dark cycle in terms of regularity,
environmental synchronisation and normalised relative
amplitude, as described by Martínez Nicolas etal. (75). is value
is used as a global marker of chronodisruption: the closer to 1,
the less chronodisruption.
All in all, wejustify the use of actigraphy because it gives us the
opportunity to study the person over a long period of time in their
usual environment, clearly identifying moments of activity and rest,
and it is also a small, comfortable and portable device (like a watch).
Also, because it provides objective data that wecan compare with
data obtained from other instruments in the protocol
(triangulation). Finally, it allows us to assess the person’s exposure
to light, which allows us to make a good assessment of possible
circadian problems, and, by measuring temperature, wecan nd out
whether the person’s time to go to sleep coincides with their
biological time for doing so.
2.3.3 Sleep diary
e student or tutor must record what time the sleep ‘ritual’
begins, what time the individual estimates to fall asleep and what time
the student wakes up. Daytime naps and any events that occur during
the night must beregistered. Physical activity and use of technology
must also beregistered. ese data should beregistered over a 10-day
period when the student is wearing the actigraph (i.e., school days and
non-schooldays). ose completing the sleep diaries will be the
subjects themselves in the case of secondary school students, and their
parents or guardians in the case of primary school students.
Information sessions will beheld for both groups and members of the
research team will beavailable to answer any questions.
2.3.4 Munich chronotype questionnaire
e core version of the standard MCTQ (Children and
Adolescents version) is used to assess sleep-awake behaviour on
school days and non-schooldays. e MCTQ asks about sleep and
activity times for schooldays and for school-free days: what time the
individual goes to bed; what time heor she decides to go to sleep; how
long it takes to fall asleep; what time the individual wakes up, what
time the student gets up and if an alarm clock is used. e MCTQ is
a validated widely used method to estimate chronotype, based on the
midpoint between sleep onset and sleep termination on school-free
days, ‘corrected’ for sleep debt accumulated across schooldays; this
provides the chronotype estimation in the form of a local time not a
score [for the exact calculation, see (3)]. Additional variables, such as
sleep duration and social jetlag (absolute dierence between mid-sleep
on non-school and school days, i.e., the dierence between biological
and social time) can beretrieved from the MCTQ. Chronotype and
social jetlag calculations are considered only if the participant reports
not using an alarm clock on non-school days. Additional measures
enabled by the MCTQ are sleep latency, sleep onset and wake up
times, sleep midpoint, time in bed, and sleep duration separately for
school-and non-school days.
Finally, wealso include the item “You hear about ‘morning’ and
‘evening’ types of people. Which of these types do youthink youare?”
with four possible answers because it is an item from the Morningness-
Eveningness Questionnaire, which is widely used as a single item to
reect circadian self-perception.
Gabaldón-Estevan et al. 10.3389/fpubh.2024.1336028
Frontiers in Public Health 09 frontiersin.org
2.3.5 Eating and food questionnaire
Assessment of food intake and timing is required to understand
eating patterns and the contribution of macro and micronutrients at
the individual and collective levels. e dietary questionnaire allows
an assessment of food intake. To estimate the individual’s usual intake,
the respondent is asked to record all food and drink taken at each meal
on a Monday and a ursday (schooldays) and one non-school day,
Sunday. Prior to the start of the study, the researcher will provide
instructions, an example and three templates for these records to both
the participant and an adult member of the household who will
support the student. e document asks about: (1) amount of food
ingested; (2) all the ingredients in a particular meal; (3) the cooking
method; (4) location of the meal (if at school, attach the school meal
menu); timing of the meal; (5) drinks; (6) snacks taken between meals;
and (7) whether the food and drinks were homemade or processed.
ese data allow two calculations. First, the nutritional
composition of the student’s diet can be measured, based on the
dietary record that can becalculated using a soware such as DIAL
(Alce Ingeniería SA Madrid, Spain). e most relevant diet variables
are kilocalories consumed, percentages of proteins, carbohydrates and
lipids, amounts of sugars, total, soluble and insoluble dietary bre,
cholesterol, and saturated, monounsaturated and polyunsaturated
fatty acids. Second, data on chrononutrition and the timing of intake
on school days and non-school days. e tool is very easy to ll out
because it is a description of what they eat. For elementary school
students, it is completed by their parents, and for high school students,
by themselves. Our experience with the tool so far suggests that it is
an easy task for participants to complete if they are reminded to do so.
Studies suggest that recording daily food intake and calculating
calories using food exchange lists and food composition tables to
estimate caloric intake are within an acceptable range of error and
therefore represent a practical approach to estimating caloric
intake (76).
2.3.6 Anthropometric evaluation
Anthropometric evaluation is used to record the following health
parameters weight and height, body mass index, body composition,
skin folds, waist circumference and is applied to the whole sample.
Anthropometric evaluation has been widely use in the literature for
body composition analysis (77, 78). For this protocol participants
must beweighed and measured, by the same person, under the same
conditions in line with International Society for the Advancement of
Kinanthropometry (ISAK) (79) recommendations. A bioelectronic
scale with a body composition analyser should beused to record body
weight and the other body composition parameters. is is a
non-invasive method to allow evaluation of body composition by
measuring the opposition to the ow of a current through the subject’s
body. More resistance (greater bioimpedance) will be linked to
individuals with large amounts of adipose tissue which is a poor
conductor of electricity due to its low water content. Bioimpedance
allows estimation of total body water and fat-free mass and fat mass
segmented by body areas. Accurate measurement of bioelectrical
impedance requires the student to have urinated immediately before,
bedressed only in underwear, with bare feet, with nothing metallic on
their person or clothing, standing upright with arms outstretched.
Privacy should bemaintained by the use of a 3-leaf paraban or similar.
A stadiometer with 0.1 cm fraction precision should beuse for the
height measurements. Students should bebare foot and standing with
their feet together, with their heels, buttocks, back and occipital region
in contact with the vertical plane of the stadiometer, with the feet
together. When taking the reading, the student must inhale and keep
his or her head in line with the Frankfort plane (imaginary horizontal
line between the lower edge of the orbit and the external auditory
canal). Werecommend that height should berecorded in centimetres.
e height and weight data allow calculation of the z-BMI score
and the corresponding percentiles using the SEGHNP2 Nutritional
Application programme and the reference tables for the focal country’s
paediatric population (in our case Spain) to calculate the percentile
and the equivalent SD for each subject. A child with a BMI greater
than or equal to 1.8 standard deviation (SD) above the mean for age
and sex is classed as obese, according to the World Health Organisation
(WHO) (80). e students’ waist should bemeasured manually using
a non-extensible, exible steel tape calibrated in centimetres with
millimetre graduations in line with ISAK (79) protocols. e waist
measurement should betaken by placing the tape 4 centimetres above
the navel with the student standing upright and breathing normally.
e waist measurement is recorded at the end of an outbreath.
Tricipital, bicipital and abdominal skin folds should also bemeasured
to allow calculation of body fat. ree measurements must betaken
of the waist circumference to obtain the average among the values.
ree measurements of each fold should berecorded by the same
person and under ISAK (79) titration.
2.3.7 Attention test
Attention tests allow estimation of optimal attention time slots to
enable optimal student learning. e tests should beadministered to
each student three times to compare results at the beginning and end
of the rst school-day in the week (Monday—1st and last class session)
and to compare those with mid-morning ursday. Dierent attention
tests have been chosen to avoid a training eect derived from
repetition of the same test within a few days. Werecommend that the
Monday 1st class tests should use the Test of Perception of Dierences
(or FACES-R test) and Monday end of the day tests should use the
d2-R and the ursday attention test should bebased on attention
sub-test included in the Aptitude Assessment test (see Section 3.7).
e Faces-R test is used to evaluate perceptual and attentional
aspects among subjects aged between 6 and 18 years old and is
appropriate for large samples. It comprises 60 graphic elements, each
grouped according to three schematic faces, two of which are identical
and a third which has one dierent feature (mouth, eyebrow, hair,
etc.). e d2-R test is a timed test, suitable for subjects aged 6 years and
older and involves a cancellation task to evaluate selective attention. It
measures processing speed, ability to follow instructions and accuracy
of execution of a task involving discrimination among similar visual
stimuli. Both tests provide scores for speed and accuracy, which are
important aspects of the main score—ability to concentrate.
To observe and record attention changes throughout the day and
the week among a group of students requires administration of the
attention test at dierent times and on dierent days. Werecommend
three tests: rst and last classes on Monday and mid-morning on
ursday. is allows observation of attentiveness and activity at the
beginning and end of the rst school day in the week (Monday) which
can becompared with attentiveness and activity in the middle of the
day on the second to last day of the week (ursday). e results of
these tests will indicate the hours when level of attention is highest,
which will favour understanding and internalisation of new learning,
Gabaldón-Estevan et al. 10.3389/fpubh.2024.1336028
Frontiers in Public Health 10 frontiersin.org
and those days in the week when perception is perhaps highest. Both
tests have been used in other protocols with the same population
target (8184).
e test is conducted in the same way for the whole sample,
regardless of age. First, the test should bedistributed to each individual
in the sample; the subjects will beasked to complete the information
requested on the rst page (identication data). e psychologist or
pedagogist then will explain the test. Aer ensuring all the study
subjects have understood what is required the test should start. e
time allocated to completing the test is 3 min.
Aer 3 min, the test pages will becollected. e responses will
be awarded direct scores, percentile scores or STANdard NINes
(stanines). Higher scores indicate higher capacity for attention and
perception and greater control of impulsivity.
Internal consistency of the FACES-R test will beestablished using
Cronbach’s Alpha coecient; weobtained a score of 0.91 for the whole
sample. However, since the Cronbach’s Alpha coecient is below 0.90
for students aged between 6 and 11 years, weneed to apply some
additional instruments to increase reliability.
Based on the results obtained from summing hits and errors, the
following proles can beidentied.
e results of this test should becorrelated with the results of the
aptitude tests such as BADyG (Bateria de Actividades mentales
Diferenciales y Generales or Dierential and General Skills Battery;
see next instrument) used to detect aptitude levels of sample
participants. BADyG also includes an attention subtest that will
beused as the attention test for ursday. It is useful, also, to cross-
check these data against student chronotype, extracted from the
MCTQ, to verify whether the hours of peak attention correspond to
the individual’s periods of peak energy and activity.
2.3.8 Aptitude assessment
Aptitude is a construct that cannot beobserved directly; therefore,
aptitude should bemeasured using a self-reported questionnaire. e
BADyG aptitude test allows the assessment of academic performance
and special educational needs and has been widely used in research
(85, 86). ere are dierent versions of the BADyG test to suit ages 4
to 18 years. e test is copyright protected and must bepurchased by
the researcher. e test shows good psychometric scores, excellent
reliability at all levels and good internal consistency of Cronbach’s
alpha, Spearman-Brown coecient and Guttman’s two halves
coecient. e BADyG is used to measures cognitive development in
verbal, numerical, spatial and logical reasoning skills and ability to
solve verbal, numerical and spatial syllogisms, and the speed and
eciency with which students solve the academic problems they
encounter. It also provides a score for general intelligence.
BADyG logical reasoning includes inductive operations and
abstract concepts based on grouping visuospatial, numerical and
verbal aspects. It requires ability to solve comprehension problems and
access to memory. e logical reasoning score is obtained by summing
the scores for analogue relations, numerical series and logical matrices.
e verbal score is obtained by combining the content of verbal
compression and analogical reasoning to obtain second-order
relationships between pairs of concepts. is ability is basic and
central to most smart activities. e verbal score also involves
Analogue Relations plus Complete Sentences tests. e numerical part
of the test requires access to memory and recovery of previously
learned knowledge, decoding of numerical codes or symbols and the
related operations. e numerical score is obtained from the
Numerical Series plus Numerical Problems test. e visuospatial test
involves representations of gures that rotate a series of degrees to le
or right which must bepositioned appropriately to complete the
painting. is is like Raven’s Progressive Matrices, which are used as
a non-verbal measure of overall intelligence and are obtained from the
Logical Matrices + Fit gures. General intelligence is considered to
combine all mental abilities: the mind’s relational and abstractive
activities. However, it is current intelligence, not something given at
and immutable since birth. It is based on development phases and
interaction with environmental stimuli and is obtained by summing
the verbal analogies, numerical series, logical matrices, complete
sentences, numerical problems and gure tting scores.
e tests should bescheduled to cover a range of times appropriate
to the student’s age: for example, older aged students will require less
time to complete the test. For a sample with an average age of 12 years,
the basic tests will take 67 min and the complementary tests 24 min,
to a total of 91 min. It should benoted that within each level of the
various tests, the questions are organised in order of diculty, from
easier to more dicult. Correct application of these tests requires
subjects to have a test book and another book in which to record their
answers. Each test must beexplained to participants along with the
time available to complete the test. A computerised version of the tests
is also available.
Interpretation of the results is done based on individual answer
books/sheets.
e results allow automatic generation of reports. e collective
report is generated by the direct scores and the group centile score,
which provide scaled and statistical results. Individual proles are
obtained by comparing a specic student’s percentile with the
percentile assigned to each test scale. e comparative intergroup
report of means is extracted by comparing the mean of each group
with that of the corresponding scale; the means of each test are then
compared to allow observation of those skills that are more developed
and those that require interventions. e intragroup level distribution
report is based on the percentile ranking in the 7 levels: a score of 94
or over is Very High, a score of between 93 and 75 is High, 74 to 61 is
Medium-High, 60 to 31 is Medium, 30 to 16 is Medium-Low level, 15
to 7 is Low and the score of less than 7 is Very Low level. Finally, the
interval between general intelligence and intelligence quotient or IQ
indicates the upper and lower limits of student percentile scores.
One of the limitations of the BADyG and similar tests, is the
length of time involved and the need for some breaks during the tests
to avoid a fatigue eect which could lead to lower test scores.
Werecommend a 30-min break halfway through the battery of tests.
2.3.9 Timetable of activities
e student timetable for a normal week captures their
participation in a range of dierent activities (school hours,
extracurricular activities, sports trainings, etc.). It requires them to
indicate start and nish times for their regular activities and provides
an understanding of how primary and secondary school students
allocate their time, on a weekly basis. Also, and in line with
Csíkszentmihályi’s (87) ‘ow’ concept, students are asked to rank their
level of preparedness (skill) for the individual activities (low, medium
or high skill level) and how dicult (low, medium or high) they found
the activity. Measuring the ‘ow’ related to each activity captures the
student’s subjective evaluation of his or her routine activities.
Gabaldón-Estevan et al. 10.3389/fpubh.2024.1336028
Frontiers in Public Health 11 frontiersin.org
2.3.10 Time diary
e Time Diary is used is to measure the students participation
in dierent activities based on information on time spent on learning,
socialising, leisure, self-care and other activities. e Time Diary tool
asks participants to indicate their main and secondary activities, who
is involved in these activities and where they take place, on a scale of
10-minute slots, allowing a degree of objectication of non-or
partially-institutionalised social activities and their duration (88). e
Time Diary from the standard Time Use Survey (TUS) provides a
detailed record of the activities undertaken during one school day and
one non-school day. e TUS is administered in numerous countries
and, in the European Union, is coordinated by the Eurostat Working
Party which devised the Harmonised European Time-use Study
(HETUS) in the late 1990s (89).
In the case of primary age children and adolescents, the interest is
in obtaining information on students’ daily routines, with whom they
interact and their assessed level of satisfaction with these activities. To
respond to the specicities of the study subjects and the research
objectives, the TUS Time Diary can be adjusted to include
these aspects.
Students are asked to record their activities over three specic
days, with support from tutors, parents or guardians. Using a 10-min
interval scale, the students are asked to indicate their activities over
the course of two school days (Monday and Tuesday) and one
non-school day (Sunday). Figure1 shows that these 3 days coincide
with the collection of saliva samples, recording of eating in the feeding
questionnaire, wearing the actimetry watches and recording their
sleep/awake behaviours in the sleep diary. is pattern facilitates data
collection, and the range of data allows for data triangulation.
2.3.11 Well-being questionnaire
e literature recommends the use of more than one type of
instrument to allow comparison and allow collection of
complementary information (90). e wellbeing questionnaire uses
the ve scales employed in the third wave of the Childrens Worlds
Project
1
to assess SWB and PWB. e CW-SWBS, CW-DBSWBS,
CW-PNAS and CW-PSWBS are adapted from previously proposed
scales. It also includes four additional satisfactions with life domains
not being part of any scale. e limitations regarding cross-cultural
comparison are discussed later. All of the instruments are answered
under a self-application form and only for students 8-years-old and
older. ey are the following:
2.3.11.1 Overall life satisfaction scale
e importance of a single item scale for overall satisfaction in the
context of the cognitive dimension of SWB was highlighted by
Campbell etal. (43). Although initially formulated for adults, this scale
has been used widely with samples of children and adolescents and
has shown good performance. Weincluded a question asking about
overall satisfaction with life; 10 to 18 year-old students are asked to
indicate their overall satisfaction with their lives on a scale from 0 to
10, where 0 is Not at all satised and 10 “Totally satised” and a scale
of 5 emoticons (from sad to happy) for 8 to 9 year olds. e questions
posed are: for 10 to 18-year-old students—To what extent are
1 https://isciweb.org
you satised with your life in general? And for 8- to 9-year-old
students—Are youhappy with your life in general?
2.3.11.2 The children’s worlds subjective well-being scale
e CW-SWBS consists of six items measuring cognitive SWB,
based on Huebner’s (91) Student Life Satisfaction Scale, Diener etal.s
(92) Satisfaction With Life Scale, and suggestions made by the children
themselves on how to improve the wording. ey are the following: I
have a good life, e things that happen in my life are excellent,
Iamhappy with my life, Ienjoy my life, My life is going well and Ilike
my life. An 11-point agreement scale was is used from 0 = Not at all
agree to 10 = Totally agree (10 to 18 year olds), and an agreement
5-point scale ranging from “I do not agree” to “Totally agree” (8 to
9 year olds). An overall index is calculated based on summing the six
items and dividing the total by 6.
2.3.11.3 The children’s worlds domain based subjective
well-being scale
e CW-DBSWBS includes ve items measuring domain based
cognitive SWB, based on Seligson etal.s (93) Brief Multidimensional
Student Life Satisfaction Scale (BMSLSS). e ve items refer to
satisfaction with: e people youlive with; Your friends; Your life as a
student; e area youlive in; and How youlook. e CW-DBSWBS is
scored on an 11-point scale from 0 = Not at all satised to 10 = Totally
satised for 10- to18-year-old students and a scale of 5 emoticons
(from sad to happy) for primary 8- to 9-year-olds school students.
Calculation of an overall index is achieved by summing the ve items
and dividing the total by 5. Again, there are some limitations in
relation to cross-cultural comparison and these are discussed later.
Besides the CW-DBSWBS, the protocol also includes additional
satisfaction with life domains not being part of any scale but also
included in the Children’s Worlds project to cover more aspects of
children’s and adolescents’ lives. ey are measured in the same way
as for the CW-DBSWBS and are analysed separately. ey are
satisfaction with how youuse your time, the freedom youhave, your
health and how are youlistened to by adults in general.
2.3.11.4 The children’s worlds positive and negative
aects scale
e CW-PNAS measures the aective dimension of SWB and is
based on the Core Aect Scale (94, 95). e six items include three for
positive aect (full of energy, happy, calm) and three for negative aect
(stressed, sad, bored). e question posed for 10–18-year-old students
is: Please consider each of these words and then indicate on a scale
from 0 (Not at all) to 10 (Extremely) how much you felt this way
during the last 2 weeks? while 8–9-year-olds students are asked to
indicate frequency of these feelings on a 4-point scale ranging from
Never” to “Always,” with “Sometimes” and “Oen” as intermediate
options. An overall index can becalculated by summing the three
items on positive aect and the three items on negative aect and
dividing the respective scores by 3. e items can also
beanalysed separately.
2.3.11.5 The children’s worlds psychological subjective
well-being scale
e six-item CW-PSWB psychometric scale measures PWB and
is based on Rys (96) model [see (97)]. ey are asked to score the
following items: Ilike being the way Iam; Iamgood at managing my
Gabaldón-Estevan et al. 10.3389/fpubh.2024.1336028
Frontiers in Public Health 12 frontiersin.org
daily responsibilities; People are generally friendly towards me; Ihave
enough freedom of choice about how I spend my time; I feel that
Iamlearning a lot at the moment; and I feel positive about my future
on an 11-point unipolar scale from 0 “Totally disagree” to 10 “Totally
agree.” is scale is only administered to the secondary-education
students (12 to 18 year olds). An overall index summing up the six
items on positive aect, on the one hand, and negative aect, on the
other hand, and dividing the respective total scores by six is
generally calculated.
Casas and González-Carrasco (98) investigated the reliability and
comparability of the CW-SWBS, CW-DBSWBS, CW-PNAS and
CW-PSWBS among the 35 countries included in the third wave of the
Childrens Worlds Project, based on the 10- to 12-year-old age groups.
In the CW-SWBS, they suggested that an abridged version of the ve
items, which excluded the item I like my life, would besuitable for both
10 and 12 year olds. Comparison of the data from the dierent
countries based on multi-group equation modelling showed that the
mean scores for the item e things that happen in my life are excellent
was not strictly comparable among countries for either age group,
although the other items were satisfactory. e ve items in the
CW-DBSWBS were deemed appropriate for both age groups.
However, the authors warned against use of a statistic based on an
overall index for cross country comparison, since the meaning diers
across countries.
For both age groups, the explained variance among the
CW-DBSWBS items may besmaller compared to the CW-SWBS
items. For the pooled sample, the CW-PNAS (positive and negative
aect) models are appropriate for both the 10-year-old and 12-year-
old samples. e multiple-group model is appropriate for the 10-year-
olds, but not the 12-year-old group. Again, a statistic based on an
overall index for the scale should not beused for cross country
comparison. e CW-PSWBS is appropriate for both age groups, but
again no statistic based on an overall index should beused for cross
country comparison. For the CW-DBSWBS, CW-PNAS and
CW-PSWBS cross country comparison can bebased on correlations
and regressions. For the primary age students, conrmatory factor
analysis of the CW-SWBS applied to the 8-year-olds group,
corresponding to the third wave of the Childrens Worlds Project
showed adequate t for structural equation modelling while the multi-
group analyses supported scalar invariance if one of the countries was
excluded from the model. erefore, ndings support application of
the CW-SWBS for 8 year olds in both eastern and western
countries (99).
We need to make some comments on the interpretation of the
results of the above instruments. In general terms it could besaid that
the higher the score the better. However, there is a range of values that
are likely to dene most of the sample population’s responses to the
SWB (there is no equivalence for the PWB level). e theory behind
the predicted normative range for the SWB scores is Cummins (100)
homeostatic model. To explain variations in SWB wesuggest the
analogy of deviations in blood pressure and heart rate, which, in
certain circumstances, due perhaps to dierent external and external
factors (e.g., stress, illness) exhibit minimum and maximum values,
and which under normal circumstances, return to their baseline values.
It is assumed that there is a genetic mechanism that controls SWB
homeostatically. ere may berelatively small variations in SWB levels
among individuals belonging to the same culture which can
beexplained by this mechanism, although with the exception of cases
where inbuilt protection mechanisms fail. e range of values for
adults of between 75 and 90 on a 100-point scale, is due to optimism
bias (100, 101) whereas the normal distribution is around 50.
Although some authors suggest that levels of SWB are similar for
younger children and adolescents (102), there is some evidence
suggesting that these set-points are higher among primary age
children and decrease through adolescence before converging, at some
point, with the adult population (10, 11).
Taking the third wave of the Children’s Worlds project and the
only scale that supports mean comparisons across countries, the ve-
item version of the CW-SWBS, as a reference point, this decreasing
with age tendency reported by González-Carrasco etal. (10, 11) with
longitudinal data is observed for the group of 10-year-olds compared
to 12-year-olds (the same scale is used for both). e mean score for
the 8-year-olds ranges from 3.11 to 3.65 out of 4 (with Spain ranked
3rd—mean of 3.59). e means for the 10-year-olds group oscillates
between 7.94 and 9.71 out of 10 (with Spain ranked 4th position). And
the means in the case of the 12-year-olds group (mean of 8.82 out of
10) with means ranging from 7.25 to 9.55 (with Spain ranked 7th
position) (103).
2.4 Ethics and dissemination
e protocol complies with Spanish legislation (Biomedical
Research Law, BOE July 4, 2007, research collecting data on humans),
and the ethical standards articulated in the Helsinki declaration (104).
All the processes involved comply with international ethics of human
genetic studies recommendations (105). Participation carries no risks
for participants; it involves no invasive procedures and no
interventions with known risky side eects. e saliva samples are
used only to collect hormonal indicators.
e data (salivary hormones, actigraphy data, subjective data
collected through questionnaires and diaries, personal data including
age, sex, socioeconomic background, etc.) are collected in line with
the guidelines set by an Ethics Committee. Use of the personal data
collected during application of the protocol should belimited to
project eldwork and is subject to the participants’ informed consent.
Personal identity will beprotected through use of anonymized codes;
only the schools and high schools involved will beable to related the
codes to actual names. e codes will never bemade public. e
documentation and work data will bestored on secure servers to
which only the research team will have access.
At the end of the project, the anonymized data, will bemade
available to the scientic community in line with national legislation.
During the project the members of the research team will not process
data using personal means and will guarantee security through
conformance to legal requirements acknowledging that resources
containing personal data are condential and restricted. At the end of
the project, any personal data collected will bedestroyed.
2.5 Data analysis
e protocol generates a considerable ow of data, which should
berecorded, organised, and stored in a huge set of quantitative and
qualitative variables of dierent nature. e preliminary analysis
should include the initial treatment of records, data cleaning, and
graphical representations. en, marginal and multivariate analysis of
cross-sectional variables of interest should beperformed by using
Gabaldón-Estevan et al. 10.3389/fpubh.2024.1336028
Frontiers in Public Health 13 frontiersin.org
diverse descriptive and association measures, multiple regression
models, binary choice models, and in the cases of repeated
measurement, panel data methods.
A key assumption in linear regression is that observational units
(e.g., students) are independent, given the values of a set of covariates.
An important violation of the independence assumption is due to
clustered data, when the responses of students are naturally grouped in
classrooms, or schools. e analyst could never hope to observe all the
potentially inuential covariates, such as the classroom atmosphere,
the teacher’s enthusiasm and competence, or the level of parental
involvement, since they cannot bemeasured appropriately. erefore,
there is unobserved heterogeneity between the responses by classrooms
and schools. e consequence is that two observations in same
classroom or school are correlated and more similar than observations
in dierent classrooms or schools, so students in one classroom or
school would tend to have better test results, even aer controlling for
observed covariates, than students in another classroom or school.
Multilevel models allow the processing of data from hierarchical
structures and are especially useful for data processing in educational
research. is analysis complements the previous ones, so that it
considers not only the possibility of treating the rst-level elements that
characterise individual responses, but also the dierent levels of nested
models that can beproposed, considering higher-level eects than the
individual student.
3 Discussion
Epidemiological studies show that chrono disruption is a risk
factor for numerous diseases, including cardiovascular disease,
metabolic syndrome, cognitive and aective disorders, cancer and
immunosuppression, among others (106, 107). Exposure to light and
physical or mental activity at hours when exposure to light is abnormal
for human beings causes alterations to biological rhythms and,
especially, sleep rhythms and biological processes controlled by
endogenous circadian clocks. ese eects are seen particularly
among children and adolescents. e use of electronic technology at
the end of the school day and during extracurricular and family
activities contribute to tiredness during daytime hours and are
contributing factors for school failure (108).
Exposure to articial light (in school, or at home) leads to
changes to clock genes and proteins in the central nervous system and
aects melatonin and other hormones production, which aect the
duration and quality of sleep and changes to central and peripheral
circadian rhythms (109, 110). Activities such as mealtimes and the
type of food consumed also aect peripheral clocks which in turn
aect circadian rhythms (111).
ere are studies that it exemplies some of the eects of the
school schedule in the health, performance and wellbeing of students.
Some studies link delayed intake of food with increased obesity (8, 40)
also in children and youth (112) and some highlight the link between
compact schedule in school with decreased performance and increased
fatigue (113115). Several works focus on the eects of school time
organisation in schools and its relationship with (de)-synchronisation
with students’ internal body clocks (12, 31, 32). With the eect of
school schedule organisation on academic performance (13, 116) and
the impact of school schedules on sleep (23, 26, 117).
e protocol captures several health measures to evaluate the
eect of school schedules on youth health and wellbeing. e protocol
measures circadian health, amount and quality of sleep, body mass
index, body composition, diet, outdoor light exposure, and minutes
per day of moderate-to-vigorous physical activity, as well as
recreational screen time. As for wellbeing questionnaire, as wealready
mentioned, weintegrate the ve scales employed in the third wave of
the Children’s Worlds Project and include four additional satisfactions
with life domains not being part of any scale.
e protocol allows the comparison of dierent school schedules
(extended vs. compact; morning vs. evening) within an education
system. We propose to measure the eects of dierent school
schedules on students’ health, wellbeing and academic performance
by controlling for the other confounding variables included in the
protocol. Although demanding, compliance with the protocol is
possible with a reasonable degree of success if the research team
carries out its tasks of eld preparation, information, support and
instrument collection properly. Webelieve that the more support
given to students and parents in explaining the purpose of the research
and how the protocol works, the greater the success. In some cases, a
second appointment to retake a test or complete a questionnaire must
begranted if the participating student is ill or unable to attend, and if
this happens, it must beon the same day of the week and at the same
time as the original request.
From a scientic-technical perspective, the combination of
expertise and methodologies proposed in this protocol is novel and
should beof value to both researchers and education institutions. In
2023, we implemented the protocol in two high schools with 49
13-year-old students. e protocol included measuring cortisol levels,
but not melatonin due to budget limitations. is prior experience has
enabled us to adjust logistical aspects of the protocol based on our
eld study. e protocol enables the collection of rich and
complementary data from a sample of students, which will provide a
better understanding of the complexities related to the scheduling of
lesson times for children and adolescents. e statistical analysis
should include both a longitudinal description of each variable and a
multivariate description of the relationships between them based on
extensive correlation analysis. e ability to attribute causality, which
is rare in this eld, is enabled by collection of longitudinal data, which
also enable a better mapping of the changes that occur in childhood
and adolescence. For example, the proposed protocol allows collection
of data on children’s cortisol levels, during two dierent days during
the school week and on a non-school day. ese data related to young
people are unique.
Our protocol allows the interlinking and further development of
several research strands (on sleep, eating, activity, learning,
socialisation, well-being and other patterns) within a comprehensive,
interdisciplinary and complex protocol. e ndings will reinforce
existing research and should beinformative for policies in education
and aspects related to school timings. In turn, this should contribute
to the formulation of more healthy, ecient and satisfying school
schedules. e novelty of our protocol should contribute to research
and education worldwide and the well-being of society and, especially,
students, students’ families and teachers. ese benets may
contribute bemanifested in better student performance in school and
a better school climate and improved health literacy.
We are aware of the inherent challenges in achieving full
synchronisation between the school timetable and individual
chronotypes, and weare also aware that getting the school timetable
right for students’ chronotypes is only part of the problem, but a very
important one. What happens outside of school is very relevant to
Gabaldón-Estevan et al. 10.3389/fpubh.2024.1336028
Frontiers in Public Health 14 frontiersin.org
what happens inside, and the pedagogy and internal organisation of
academic activities also play an important role in both student
performance and well-being. We believe that both what happens
outside school and what happens inside school will benet greatly
from a better match between the school timetable and the chronotype
of the student. On the other hand, there is a degree of scepticism
about improving school schedules due to the multifactorial nature of
the educational process, especially in education systems with a weak
tradition of evidence-based practise. We believe that it is only
through the development of research and specic educational studies
that wewill beable to guide teachers, families and policy makers in
a research-action process of social change.
Considering the protocol and the accompanying instruments, the
following strengths and weaknesses are identied. At the application
level, one of the protocol’s strengths is its multidisciplinary approach,
encompassing various disciplines and variables to study a broad age
range, with a sample ranging from 6 to 16 years, considering each
developmental and academic stage. On the ip side, one of the most
critical aspects is the recruitment and consistency of the sample
requiring participants to commit to the protocol with up to 12
instruments applied over 10 days. While it is a strength that applied
as a longitudinal study allows the evaluation of the evolution of many
variables over time, maintaining the sample for several years poses a
signicant challenge. To compensate for sample dropout, additional
individuals who meet the required criteria can be included in
subsequent screenings. Additionally, as a weakness, reliance on the
samples responsibility in completing instruments is acknowledged,
with increased completeness when applied in the school setting
assisted by the class teacher and/or the research team.
On the instrument level, strengths include obtaining hormones
through non-invasive saliva samples and the actigraphy as a wearable
instrument capable of registering extensive data over an extended
period in student’s natural environment. e feed questionnaire,
attention tests, and well-being questionnaire stand out for their ease
of application, speed, applicability across a wide age range, and
provision of valuable data for student contextualisation. e time
diary with its 10-min interval scale allows triangulation both the
actimetry and the sleep diary facilitating the assessment of data
consistency. One of the cons is that the BADyG test requires a
signicant amount of time for application, leading to decreased
student attention due to fatigue towards the end if a break is
not schedule.
Ethics statement
e studies involving humans have been approved by Dr. Peset
Hospital Bioethics Committee, Valencia (Spain). e studies will
beconducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional
requirements. Written informed consent will beobtained from the
participants and the participants' legal guardian/next of kin prior
to participation.
Author contributions
DG-E: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Methodology,
Supervision, Writing – original dra, Writing – review & editing,
Project administration, Visualization. DC-T: Methodology,
Visualization, Writing – original dra. BC-G: Methodology, Writing
– original dra. EM-G: Methodology, Writing – original dra. VM-C:
Methodology, Writing – original dra. LM: Methodology, Writing –
original dra. EM-B: Methodology, Writing – original dra. MG-C:
Methodology, Writing – original dra. MH-J: Writing – review &
editing. KT: Supervision, Writing – review & editing. MT: Writing –
review & editing. AA-A: Writing – review & editing. GS: Supervision,
Writing – review & editing. NE: Supervision, Writing – review &
editing. GP-A: Supervision, Writing – review & editing,
Conceptualization. CR: Methodology, Supervision, Writing – original
dra, Writing – review & editing, Conceptualization.
Funding
e author(s) declare that nancial support was received for the
research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. e publication
is part of the ‘Student chronotype (mis)match with school time
organisation: its eects on health, learning, time use and satisfaction
[Kairos]’ project and has beneted for the following support: Grant
PID2021-126846NA-I00 funded by MCIN/AEI/10.13039/501100011033
and by ERDF “A way of making Europe”, RC21-019-Recovery,
Transformation and Resilience Plan—funded by the European Union
Next Generation EU/PRTR, and COST Action CA 18236 ‘Multi-
disciplinary Innovation for Social Change.
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank Ana Adan for her useful comments to a
previous version of the manuscript, Cynthia Little for her English
editing of the manuscript, Olga Minguez for her assistance with the
references management, and Alberto Pérez for his graphic
design assistance.
Conflict of interest
e authors declare that the research was conducted in the
absence of any commercial or nancial relationships that could
beconstrued as a potential conict of interest.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors
and do not necessarily represent those of their aliated organizations,
or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product
that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its
manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Supplementary material
e Supplementary material for this article can befound online
at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpubh.2024.1336028/
full#supplementary-material
Gabaldón-Estevan et al. 10.3389/fpubh.2024.1336028
Frontiers in Public Health 15 frontiersin.org
References
1. Klein J. Planning middle school schedules for improved attention and achievement.
Scand J Educ Res. (2004) 48:441–50. doi: 10.1080/0031383042000245825
2. Roenneberg T, Kumar CJ, Merrow M. e human circadian clock entrains to sun
time. Curr Biol. (2007) 17:R44–5. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.12.011
3. Roenneberg T, Winnebeck EC, Klerman EB. Daylight saving time and articial time
zones – A Battle between biological and social times. Front Physiol. (2019) 10:944. doi:
10.3389/fphys.2019.00944
4. Gabaldón-Estevan D. A deshora en la escuela. Rev Sociol la Educ. (2021) 14:256–71.
doi: 10.7203/RASE.14.3.21626
5. Klerman EB, Brager A, Carskadon MA, Depner CM, Foster R, Goel N, et al.
Keeping an eye on circadian time in clinical research and medicine. Clin Transl Med.
(2022) 12:e1131. doi: 10.1002/ctm2.1131
6. Pin Arboledas G, Cubel Alarcón M, Martin González G, Lluch Roselló A, Morell
SM. Hábitos y problemas con el sueño de los 6 a los 14 años en la Comunidad
Valenciana. Opinión de los propios niños. An Pediatría. (2011) 74:103–15. doi:
10.1016/j.anpedi.2010.08.014
7. Reis C, Pilz LK, Kramer A, Lopes LV, Paiva T, Roenneberg T. e impact of
daylight-saving time (DST) on patients with delayed sleep-wake phase disorder
(DSWPD). J Pineal Res. (2023) 74:e12867. doi: 10.1111/jpi.12867
8. Martínez-Lozano N, Tvarijonaviciute A, Ríos R, Barón I, Scheer FAJL, Garaulet M.
Late eating is associated with obesity, inammatory markers and circadian-related
disturbances in school-aged children. Nutrients. (2020) 12:2881. doi: 10.3390/nu12092881
9. Martínez-Lozano N, Barraco GM, Rios R, Ruiz MJ, Tvarijonaviciute A, Fardy P,
et al. Evening types have social jet lag and metabolic alterations in school-age children.
Sci Rep. (2020) 10:16747. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-73297-5
10. González-Carrasco M, Sáez M, Casas F. Subjective well-being in early adolescence:
observations from a ve-year longitudinal study. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2020)
17:8249. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17218249
11. González-Carrasco M, Casas F, Malo S, Viñas F, Dinisman T. Changes with age in
subjective well-being through the adolescent years: dierences by gender. J Happiness
Stud. (2017) 18:63–88. doi: 10.1007/s10902-016-9717-1
12. Zerbini G, Merrow M. Time to learn: how chronotype impacts education. PsyCh
J. (2017) 6:263–76. doi: 10.1002/pchj.178
13. Rodríguez Ferrante G, Goldin AP, Sigman M, Leone MJ. A better alignment
between chronotype and school timing is associated with lower grade retention in
adolescents. npj Sci Learn. (2023) 8:21. doi: 10.1038/s41539-023-00171-0
14. Mullis IVS, Martin MO, Arora A. TIMSS 2011 international results in mathematics.
TIMSS & PIRLS Int Study Center. (2012)
15. Mullis IVS, Martin MO, Foy P, Drucker KT. PIRLS 2011 international results in
Reading. TIMSS & PIRLS Int Study Center. (2012)
16. Meijer AM. Chronic sleep reduction, functioning at school and school
achievement in preadolescents. J Sleep Res. (2008) 17:395–405. doi:
10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00677.x
17. Minges KE, Redeker NS. Delayed school start times and adolescent sleep: A
systematic review of the experimental evidence. Sleep Med Rev. (2016) 28:86–95. doi:
10.1016/j.smrv.2015.06.002
18. Kelley P, Lockley SW, Foster RG, Kelley J. Synchronizing education to adolescent
biology: ‘let teens sleep, start school later’. Learn Media Technol. (2015) 40:210–26. doi:
10.1080/17439884.2014.942666_1
19. Chan CS, Poon CYS, Leung JCY, Lau KNT, Lau EYY. Delayed school start time is
associated with better sleep, daytime functioning, and life satisfaction in residential
high-school students. J Adolesc. (2018) 66:49–54. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2018.05.002
20. Carskadon M. When worlds collide: adolescent need for sleep versus societal
demands. Phi Delta Kappan. (1999) 80:348–53.
21. Suchaut B. L’organisation et l’utilisation du temps scolaire à l’école primaire: enjeux
et eets sur les élèves. Conférence à l’initiative la V Cran-Gevrier. (2009)
22. Suchaut B. Pour une nouvelle organisation du temps scolaire a l’école primaire.
(2012). Available from: https://shs.hal.science/halshs-00714043/document
23. Gabaldón-Estevan D, Taht K. e school schedule eect on self-reported sleep
length of children and youth in Spain. J Sleep Res. (2020) 29:180–0.
24. Clara MI, Allen GA. An epidemiological study of sleepwake timings in school
children from 4 to 11 years old: insights on the sleep phase shi and implications for the
school starting times’ debate. Sleep Med. (2020) 66:51–60. doi: 10.1016/j.
sleep.2019.06.024
25. Estevan I, Silva A, Vetter C, Tassino B. Short sleep duration and extremely delayed
Chronotypes in Uruguayan youth: the role of school start times and social constraints.
J Biol Rhythms. (2020) 35:391–404. doi: 10.1177/0748730420927601
26. Rodríguez Ferrante G, Goldin AP, Sigman M, Leone MJ. Chronotype at the
beginning of secondary school and school timing are both associated with
chronotype development during adolescence. Sci Rep. (2022) 12:8207. doi: 10.1038/
s41598-022-11928-9
27. Rodríguez Ferrante G, Leone MJ. Solar clock and school start time eects on
adolescents’ chronotype and sleep: A review of a gap in the literature. US: John Wiley and
Sons Inc (2023).
28. Andrade MMM, Menna-Barreto L. Diurnal variation in Oral temperature,
sleepiness, and performance of high school girls. Biol Rhythm Res. (1996) 27:336–42.
doi: 10.1076/brhm.27.3.336.12966
29. Wolfson AR, Carskadon MA. Sleep schedules and daytime functioning in
adolescents. Child Dev. (1998) 69:875–87. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06149.x
30. Gromada A, Shewbridge C. Student learning time: A literature review. OECD
Educ. (2016). doi: 10.1787/5jm409kqqkjh-en
31. Zerbini G, van der Vinne V, Otto LKM, Kantermann T, Krijnen WP, Roenneberg
T, et al. Lower school performance in late chronotypes: underlying factors and
mechanisms. Sci Rep. (2017) 7:4385. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-04076-y
32. van der Vinne V, Zerbini G, Siersema A, Pieper A, Merrow M, Hut RA, et al.
Timing of examinations aects school performance dierently in early and late
chronotypes. J Biol Rhythms. (2015) 30:53–60. doi: 10.1177/0748730414564786
33. Pin G, Gradolí R, García G. Shastu sleep habits in Student’s performance. Final
Report. (2016). Available from: http://shastu.org/wp-content/uploads/2017/02/
SHASTU-FINAL-REPORT-ENGLISH_ok.pdf
34. French MT, Homer JF, Popovici I, Robins PK. What youdo in high school matters:
high school GPA, educational attainment, and labor market earnings as a young adult.
East Econ J. (2015) 41:370–86. doi: 10.1057/eej.2014.22
35. Arrona-Palacios A, Díaz-Morales JF, Parra-Robledo Z, Adan A. Sleep and
depressive symptoms in the Morningness/Eveningness-suicidal ideation relationship
depend on school shi in Mexican adolescents. J Clin Med. (2021) 10:4681. doi: 10.3390/
jcm10204681
36. Carvalho-Mendes RP, Dunster GP, de la Iglesia HO, Menna-Barreto L. Aernoon
school start times are associated with a lack of both social jetlag and sleep deprivation
in adolescents. J Biol Rhythms. (2020) 35:377–90. doi: 10.1177/0748730420927603
37. Estevan I, Silva A, Tassino B. School start times matter, eveningness does not.
Chronobiol Int. (2018) 35:1753–7. doi: 10.1080/07420528.2018.1504785
38. Roenneberg T. Internal time: Chronotypes, social jet lag, and why You’re so tired.
Cambridge, MA (USA): Harvard University Press (2012).
39. Eckel RH, Depner CM, Perreault L, Markwald RR, Smith MR, McHill AW, et al.
Morning circadian misalignment during short sleep duration impacts insulin sensitivity.
Curr Biol. (2015) 25:3004–10. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.10.011
40. Garaulet M, Gómez-Abellán P. Timing of food intake and obesity: A novel
association. Physiol Behav. (2014) 134
41. Gabaldón-Estevan D, Obiol FS. Guía sobre tiempos escolares. Creat Educ Innov
Rev. (2017) 1:12–69. doi: 10.7203/CREATIVITY.1.12062
42. Testu F. Rythmes de vie et rythmes scolaires. aspects chronobiologiques et
chronopsychologiques Masson (2008).
43. Campbell A, Converse PE, Rodgers W. e quality of American life: Perceptions,
evaluations, and satisfactions. New York: Russell Sage (1976).
44. Arthaud-day ML, Rode JC, Mooney CH, Near JP. e subjective well-being
construct: A test of its convergent, discriminant, and factorial validity. Soc Indic Res.
(2005) 74:445–76. doi: 10.1007/s11205-004-8209-6
45. Metler SJ, Busseri MA. Further evaluation of the tripartite structure of subjective
well-being: evidence from longitudinal and experimental studies. J Pers. (2017)
85:192–206. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12233
46. Diener E. Subjective well-being. Psychol Bull. (1984) 95:542–75.
47. Herd SM. Synthesising hedonic and Eudaimonic approaches: A culturally
responsive four-factor model of aggregate subjective well-being for Hong Kong children.
Child Indic Res. (2022) 15:1103–29. doi: 10.1007/s12187-021-09901-5
48. Ryan RM, Deci EL. On happiness and human potentials: A review of research on
hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annu Rev Psychol. (2001) 52:141–66. doi: 10.1146/
annurev.psych.52.1.141
49. Strelhow MRW, Sarriera JC, Casas F. Evaluation of well-being in adolescence:
proposal of an integrative model with hedonic and eudemonic aspects. Child Indic Res.
(2020) 13:1439–52. doi: 10.1007/s12187-019-09708-5
50. Savahl S, Casas F, Adams S. e structure of Children’s subjective well-being. Front
Psychol. (2021) 12:691. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.650691/full
51. Kudielka BM, Federenko IS, Hellhammer DH, Wüst S. Morningness and
eveningness: the free cortisol rise aer awakening in “early birds” and “night owls”. Biol
Psychol. (2006) 72:141–6. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.08.003
52. Petrowski K, Schmalbach B, Stalder T. Morning and evening type: the cortisol
awakening response in a sleep laboratory. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2020) 112:104519.
doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2019.104519
53. Weidenauer C, Vollmer C, Scheiter K, Randler C. Weak associations of
Morningness-Eveningness and stability with skin temperature and cortisol levels. J
Circadian Rhythms. (2019) 17:8. doi: 10.5334/jcr.182
Gabaldón-Estevan et al. 10.3389/fpubh.2024.1336028
Frontiers in Public Health 16 frontiersin.org
54. Kunz-Ebrecht SR, Kirschbaum C, Marmot M, Steptoe A. Dierences in cortisol
awakening response on work days and weekends in women and men from the
Whitehall II cohort. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2004) 29:516–28. doi: 10.1016/
S0306-4530(03)00072-6
55. Miller GE, Cohen S, Ritchey AK. Chronic psychological stress and the regulation
of pro-inammatory cytokines: A glucocorticoid-resistance model. Health Psych ol.
(2002) 21:531–41. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.21.6.531
56. Griefahn B, Künemund C, Golka K, ier R, Degen G. Melatonin synthesis: A
possible indicator of intolerance to shiwork. Am J Ind Med. (2002) 42:427–36. doi:
10.1002/ajim.10122
57. Liu X, Uchiyama M, Shibui K, Kim K, Kudo Y, Tagaya H, et al. Diurnal preference,
sleep habits, circadian sleep propensity and melatonin rhythm in healthy human
subjects. Neurosci Lett. (2000) 280:199–202. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00793-X
58. Pandi-Perumal SR, Smits M, Spence W, Srinivasan V, Cardinali DP, Lowe AD, et al.
Dim light melatonin onset (DLMO): A tool for the analysis of circadian phase in human
sleep and chronobiological disorders. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacology Biol Psychiatry.
(2007) 31:1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2006.06.020
59. Keijzer H, Smits MG, Peeters T, Looman CWN, Endenburg SC, Gunnewiek JMTK.
Evaluation of salivary melatonin measurements for Dim Light Melatonin Onset
calculations in patients with possible sleep–wake rhythm disorders. Clin Chim Acta.
(2011) 412:1616–20.
60. Benloucif S, Burgess HJ, Klerman EB, Lewy AJ, Middleton B, Murphy PJ, et al.
Measuring melatonin in humans. J Clin Sleep Med. (2008) 4:66–9. doi: 10.5664/
jcsm.27083
61. Sletten TL, Vincenzi S, Redman JR, Lockley SW, R ajaratnam SMW. Timing of sleep
and its relationship with the endogenous melatonin rhythm. Front Neurol. (2010) 1:137.
doi: 10.3389/fneur.2010.00137
62. Duy JF, Wright KP. Entrainment of the human circadian system by light. J Biol
Rhythms. (2005) 20:326–38. doi: 10.1177/0748730405277983
63. Burgess HJ, Fogg LF. Individual dierences in the amount and timing of salivary
melatonin secretion. PloS One. (2008) 3:e3055. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0003055
64. Kennaway DJ. e dim lig ht melatonin onset across ages, methodologies, and sex
and its relationship with morningness/eveningness. Sleep. (2023) 46:zsad033. doi:
10.1093/sleep/zsad033
65. Axelsson J, Åkerstedt T, Kecklund G, Lindqvist A, Attefors R. Hormonal changes
in satised and dissatised shi workers across a shi cycle. J Appl Physiol. (2003)
95:2099–105. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00231.2003
66. Ancoli-Israel S, Cole R, Alessi C, Chambers M, Moorcro W, Pollak CP. e role
of Actigraphy in the study of sleep and circadian rhythms. Sleep. (2003) 26:342–92. doi:
10.1093/sleep/26.3.342
67. Schoch SF, Kurth S, Werner H. Actigraphy in sleep research with infants and young
children: current practices and future benets of standardized reporting. J Sleep Res.
(2021) 30:e13134. doi: 10.1111/jsr.13134
68. Sadeh A, Lavie P, Scher A, Tirosh E, Epstein R. Actigraphic home-monitoring
sleep-disturbed and control infants and young children: A new method for pediatric
assessment of sleep-wake patterns. Pediatrics. (1991) 87:494–9.
69. Tirosh E. Eects of methylphenidate on sleep in children with attention-decit
hyperactivity disorder. Am J Dis Child. (1993) 147:1313–5.
70. Acebo C, Sadeh A, Seifer R, Tzischinsky O, Wolfson AR, Hafer A, et al. Estimating
sleep patterns with activity monitoring in children and adolescents: how many nights
are necessary for reliable measures? Sleep. (1999) 22:95–103.
71. Rodríguez-Morilla B, Martínez-Madrid MJ, Campos Martínez M, Rol de
Lama MA. Monitorización Circadiana Ambulatoria (MCA). Madrid: Kronoscore
(2021).
72. Ortiz-Tudela E, Martinez-Nicolas A, Campos M, Rol MÁ, Madrid JA. A new
integrated variable based on thermometry, Actimetry and body position (TAP) to
evaluate circadian system status in humans. PLoS Comput Biol. (2010) 6:e1000996. doi:
10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000996
73. Ortiz-Tudela E, Martinez-Nicolas A, Albares J, Segarra F, Campos M, Estivill E,
et al. Ambulatory circadian monitoring (ACM) based on thermometry, motor activity
and body position (TAP): A comparison with polysomnography. Physiol Behav. (2014)
126:30–8. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.12.009
74. Ortiz-Tudela E, Martinez-Nicolas A, Díaz-Mardomingo C, García-
Herranz S, Pereda-Pérez I, Valencia A, et al. The characterization of biological
rhythms in mild cognitive impairment. Biomed Res Int. (2014) 2014:1–7. doi:
10.1155/2014/524971
75. Martinez-Nicolas A, Martinez-Madrid MJ, Almaida-Pagan PF, Bonmati-Carrion
M-A, Madrid JA, Rol MA. Assessing Chronotypes by Ambulatory Circadian Monitor ing.
Front Physiol. (2019) 10:1396. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2019.01396
76. Petersen R, Kaye WH, Gwirtsman HE. Comparison of calculated estimates and
laboratory analysis of food oered to hospitalized eating disorder patients. J AmDiet
Assoc. (1986) 86:490–2. doi: 10.1016/S0002-8223(21)03963-8
77. Fomon SJ, Nelson SE. Body composition of the male and female reference infants.
Annu Rev Nutr. (2002) 22:1–17. doi: 10.1146/annurev.nutr.22.111401.145049
78. Khoury M, Manlhiot C, McCrindle BW. Role of the waist/height ratio in the
Cardiometabolic risk assessment of children classied by body mass index. J AmColl
Cardiol. (2013) 62:742–51. doi: 10.1016/j.jacc.2013.01.026
79. Marfell-Jones M, Olds T, Stewar A, Carter LEL. ISAK manual, international
standards for anthropometric assessment. Int Society for the Advan Kinanthropometry.
(2012). doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.01.001
80. World Health Organization. Growth reference data for 5-19 years. BMI-for-age
(5-19 years) (2007). Available from: https://www.who.int/tools/growth-reference-data-
for-5to19-years/indicators/bmi-for-age
81. Santner A, Kopp M, Federolf P. Partly randomised, controlled study in children
aged 6–10 years to investigate motor and cognitive eects of a 9-week coordination
training intervention with concurrent mental tasks. BMJ Open. (2018) 8:e021026. doi:
10.1136/bmjopen-2017-021026
82. Yato Y, Hirose S, Wallon P, Mesmin C, Jobert M. d2-R test for Japanese adolescents:
concurrent validity with the attention decit-hyperactivity disorder rating scale. Pediatr
Int. (2019) 61:43–8. doi: 10.1111/ped.13735
83. Arboix-Alió J, Sagristà F, Marcaida S, Aguilera-Castells J, Peralta-Geis M, Solà J,
et al. Relación entre la condición física y el hábito de actividad física con la capacidad
de atención selectiva en alumnos de enseñanza secundaria. Cuad Psicol del Deport.
(2022) 22:1–13. doi: 10.6018/cpd.419641
84. Méndez-Giménez A, Pallasá-Manteca M. Efecto de los descansos activos sobre
procesos atencionales y la regulación motivacional en escolares. Apunt Educ Física y
Deport. (2023) 151:49–57. doi: 10.5672/apunts.2014-0983.es.(2023/1).151.05
85. Navarro Soria I, García Fernández JM, Inglés Saura CJ, Real FM. Early detection
of learning diculties using the BADyG-E2r battery during primary education. Psicol
Reexão e Crítica. (2020) 33:4. doi: 10.1186/s41155-020-00143-y
86. Lorca Garrido AJ, López-Martínez O, de Vicente-Yagüe Jara MI. Latent inhibition
as a biological basis of creative capacity in individuals aged nine to 12. Front Psychol.
(2021) 12:650541.
87. Csikszentmihalyi M. Flow: e psychology of optimal experience. New York: Harper
and Row (1990).
88. Chenu A, Lesnard L. Time use surveys: A review of their aims, methods, and
results. Eur J Sociol Eur Sociol. (2006) 47:335–59. doi: 10.1017/S0003975606000117
89. Gershuny J. Time-use surveys and the measurement of national well-being. Centre
for time-use research, Department of sociol. (2011)
90. Casas F. Subjective social indicators and child and adolescent well-being. Child
Indic Res. (2011) 4:555–75. doi: 10.1007/s12187-010-9093-z
91. Huebner ES. Initial development of the Student’s life satisfaction scale. Sch Psychol
Int. (1991) 12:231–40. doi: 10.1177/0143034391123010
92. Diener E, Emmons RA, Larsem RJ, Grin S. e satisfaction with life scale. J Pers
Assess. (1985) 49:71–5. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13
93. Seligson JL, Huebner ES, Valois RF. Preliminary validation of the brief
multidimensional students’ life satisfaction scale (BMSLSS). Soc Indic Res. (2003)
61:121–45. doi: 10.1023/A:1021326822957
94. Russell JA. Core aect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychol Rev.
(2003) 110:145–72. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.110.1.145
95. Feldman Barrett L, Russell JA. Independence and bipolarity in the structure of
current aect. J Pers Soc Psychol. (1998) 74:967–84.
96. Ry CD. Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of
psychological well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol. (1989) 57:1069–81. doi:
10.1037/0022-3514.57.6.1069
97. Nahkur O, Casas F. Fit and cross-country comparability of Children’s worlds
psychological well-being scale using 12-year-Olds samples. Child Indic Res. (2021)
14:2211–47. doi: 10.1007/s12187-021-09833-0
98. Casas F, González-Carrasco M. Analysing comparability of four multi-item well-
being psychometric scales among 35 countries using Children’s worlds 3rd wave 10 and
12-year-olds samples. Child Indic Res. (2021) 14:1829–61. doi: 10.1007/
s12187-021-09825-0
99. Blasco-Belled A, González-Carrasco M, Casas F. Changes in the network structure
of well-being components in adolescents in the school context: A 2-year longitudinal
study. J Sch Psychol. (2024) 102:101255. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2023.101255
100. Cummins R. Normative life satisfaction: measurement issues and homeostatic
model In: B Zumbo, editor. Social indicators and quality of life research methods:
Methodological developments and issues Dordrecht, e Netherlands: Kluwer (2000)
101. Capic T, Li N, Cummins RA. Conrmation of subjective wellbeing set-points:
foundational for subjective social indicators. Soc Indic Res. (2018) 137:1–28. doi:
10.1007/s11205-017-1585-5
102. Cummins RA. Understanding the well-being of children and adolescents through
homeostatic theory In: A Ben-Arieh, F Casas, I Frønes and J Korbin, editors. Handbook
of child well-being. Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands (2014). 635–61.
103. Rees G, Savahl S, Lee BJ, Casas F. Children’s views on their lives and well-being in
35 countries: A report on the Children’s worlds project countries: A report on the Children’s
worlds project, 2016–19. Israel: Jerusalem (2020).
Gabaldón-Estevan et al. 10.3389/fpubh.2024.1336028
Frontiers in Public Health 17 frontiersin.org
104. World Health Organization. World medical association declaration of Helsinki.
JAMA. (2013) 310:2191. doi: 10.1001/jama.2013.281053
105. Beskow LM. Informed consent for population-based research involving genetics.
JAMA. (2001) 286:2315–21. doi: 10.1001/jama.286.18.2315
106. Reiter RJ, Tan D-X, Korkmaz A, Ma S. Obesity and metabolic syndrome:
association with chronodisruption, sleep deprivation, and melatonin suppression. Ann
Med. (2012) 44:564–77. doi: 10.3109/07853890.2011.586365
107. de Souza CM, Hidalgo MPL. e midpoint of sleep on working days: A measure
for chronodisruption and its association to individuals’ well-being. Chronobiol Int.
(2015) 32:341–8. doi: 10.3109/07420528.2014.979941
108. Owens J, Au R, Carskadon M, Millman R, Wolfson A, Braverman PK,
et al. Insufficient sleep in adolescents and young adults: an update on causes and
consequences. Pediatrics. (2014) 134:e921–32. doi: 10.1542/peds.2014-1696
109. Manoogian ENC, Panda S. Circadian rhythms, time-restricted
feeding, and healthy aging. Ageing Res Rev. (2017) 39:59–67. doi: 10.1016/j.
arr.2016.12.006
110. Takahashi JS. Transcriptional architecture of the mammalian circadian clock. Nat
Rev Genet. (2017) 18:164–79. doi: 10.1038/nrg.2016.150
111. Rijo-Ferreira F, Takahashi JS. Genomics of circadian rhythms in health and
disease. Genome Med. (2019) 11:82. doi: 10.1186/s13073-019-0704-0
112. López-Mínguez J, Gómez-Abellán P, Garaulet MTiming of Breakfast, Lunch, and
Dinner. Eects on obesity and metabolic risk. Nutrients. (2019) 11:2624.
113. Caride Gómez JA. A xornada escolar de sesión única en Galicia. Estudio
avaliativo: conclusións xerais e criterios de actuación. Consellería de Educación e
Ordenación Universitaria. (1993). eaau6200.
114. Asensio Aguilera JM. Cronobiología y educación In: P Fermoso Estébanez,
editor. El tiempo educativo y escolar: estudio interdisciplinar. Promociones y
Publicaciones Universitarias, PPU, Barcelona (1993). 75–110.
115. Estaun FS. Cronopsicología y educación In: P Fermoso Estébanez, editor. El
tiempo educativo y escolar: estudio interdisciplinar. Promociones y Publicaciones
Universitarias, PPU, Barcelona (1993). 111–52.
116. Dunster GP, de la Iglesia L, Ben-Hamo M, Nave C, Fleischer JG, Panda S, et al.
Sleepmore in Seattle: later school start times are associated with more sleep and better
performance in high school students. Sci Adv. (2018) 4:eaau6200. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.aau6200
117. Arrona-Palacios A, Díaz-Morales JF. Morningness–eveningness and sleep habits
at school: a comparative study between Mexico and Spain. Biol Rhythm Res. (2017)
48:175–88. doi: 10.1080/09291016.2016.1245459
... Although the current study utilized a common curriculum across various schools, the distinct characteristics of each school type may still contribute to differing response patterns. [33] highlight that even with a standardized curriculum, variations in teaching methods and school culture can lead to diverse student outcomes. Additionally, [8] emphasize the importance of contextual factors in educational assessments, suggesting that school type may inherently affect how students engage with test materials. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study examined the prevalence of aberrant response patterns in mathematics achievement tests among secondary school students in Southwestern Nigeria. The study used a multi-stage sampling technique to select 1800 Senior Secondary School students for its survey research sample. From the six states in Southwestern Nigeria, three states, one senatorial district, and three Local Government Areas were selected from each state using a simple random sampling technique. Using a stratified sampling technique, four secondary schools, and fifty senior secondary school students were selected. The mathematics achievement test was used to collect data for the study. Data collected were analyzed using MATLAB to compute aberrance indices and SPSS. The results showed that there is a high prevalence of aberrant response patterns with W* (x ̅ = 2.07, sd = 1.71) had a lower value of mean and standard deviation than the B* (x ̅= 3.61, sd = 3.34). The study also revealed that there was a significant difference between the aberrant and non-aberrant students for W* (t =-13.91, df =1693, p<0.05) and B* (t = -14.79, df =1693, p< 0.05). The study further revealed that sex (p-value= 0.026, R-squared= 0.003, Adjusted R-squared=0.002), age (p-value = 0.035, R-squared = 0.001, Adjusted R-squared= 0.001), and school type (p-value= 0.044, R-squared= 0.004, Adjusted R-squared= 0.003) have significant effects on students’ aberrant response pattern. The study concluded that there is a high prevalence of aberrant response patterns among secondary school students in Southwestern Nigeria
Article
Full-text available
Disentangling the connections between subjective and psychological well-being may help practitioners identify effective targets of intervention to promote mental health in school settings. Based on theoretical foundations of well-being, the present study utilized psychometric network analysis to explore prospective associations between the subjective and psychological well-being of adolescents over 2 years. To this end, a cross-sectional network was estimated at Time 1 (n = 560) and Time 2 (n = 281), followed by a longitudinal network incorporating individual changes across time points in each component (n = 235). The networks included different indicators of subjective (e.g., life satisfaction, positive affect, negative affect) and psychological well-being measured by means of self-reported questionnaires. The results revealed direct connections between indicators of subjective and psychological well-being over time. Positive affect, especially feeling happy and satisfied, exhibited most of these connections. Only one negative longitudinal association emerged, which involved negative affect (e.g., feeling worried) and psychological well-being. The suitability of the network approach to represent the structure of subjective and psychological well-being can be used to widen research on adolescents' well-being. Considering the longitudinal associations identified, the present study makes an exploratory hypothesis to propose specific connectors between subjective and psychological well-being as potential targets for interventions aimed at promoting adolescents' mental health.
Article
Full-text available
Schools start early in the morning all over the world, contrasting with adolescents’ late chronotype. Interestingly, lower academic performance (i.e. grades or qualifications) was associated with later chronotypes. However, it is unclear whether it is a direct effect of chronotype or because students attend school too early to perform at their best. Moreover, little is known about how this affects students’ academic success beyond their grades. To address this gap in knowledge, we studied how school timing and chronotype affect grade retention (i.e. repeat a year) in a unique sample of students randomly assigned to one of three different school timings (starting at 07:45, 12:40, or 17:20). Even when controlling for academic performance, we found that later chronotypes exhibit higher odds of grade retention only in the morning, but not in later school timings. Altogether, ensuring a better alignment between school timing and students’ biological rhythms might enhance future opportunities of adolescents.
Article
Full-text available
Due to time zones, sun time and local time rarely match. The difference between local and sun time, which we designate by Solar Jet Lag (SoJL), depends on location within a time zone and can range from zero to several hours. Daylight Saving Time (DST) simply adds one hour to SoJL, independent of location. We hypothesized that the impact of DST, is particularly problematic in patients with Delayed Sleep-Wake Phase Disorder (DSWPD), worsening their sleep debt. DSWPD is characterized by a chronic misalignment between the internal and social timing, reflected by an inability to fall asleep and wakeup at conventional or socially acceptable times. We analysed the clinical records of 162 DSWPD patients from a sleep medicine centre in Lisbon, Portugal (GMTzone), and separated them into two groups: the ones diagnosed across DST or Standard Time (ST). We included 82 patients (54.9% male; age: median [Q1 , Q3 ] 34.5 [25.0, 45.3]; range 16-92; 54 in DST and 28 in ST) who had Dim Light Melatonin Onset (DLMO) measured as a marker for the circadian phase and sleep timing (onset, SO, mid-point, MS and end, SE) self-reported separately for work- and work-free days. Differences between ST and DST were compared using Mann-Whitney or Student's t tests. On a weekly average, patients in DST slept an hour less (62 min. p<0.01), mainly due to sleep on workdays (SDw, p<0.01), which also correlated with SoJL (rsp = 0.38, p<0.01). While the time from DLMO to SO was similar in those in ST or those in DST, the time from DLMO to SE was significantly shorter for those in DST. The average duration between DLMO and sleep end was close to 10.5h in ST, the biological night length described in the literature. Our results favour perennial ST and suggest assigning time-zones close to sun time to prevent social jetlag and sleep deprivation. This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
Article
Full-text available
The onset of melatonin secretion, the Dim Light Melatonin Onset or DLMO is a tool for determining the phase of the circadian timing system. While small studies have investigated the impacts of age and methods of calculating DLMO, there is no DLMO reference range. In the current study, the saliva DLMO from 121 published studies (3579 subjects) and plasma DLMO in 31 studies (818 subjects) in healthy control subjects (3 - 73 years) were analysed. In a subset of 53 papers (1749 subjects) individual saliva DLMO and MEQ scores were obtained from authors or mined from publications and a reference range was constructed. Saliva DLMO was earliest in children to 10 years of age and latest around 20 years of age and thereafter advanced with age by 30 minutes in the oldest subjects. Melatonin assay methods and DLMO calculation methods had little effect on the determination of the DLMO. Saliva DLMO was correlated (P < 0.001) with the MEQ score; lower MEQ scores were associated with later DLMO. MEQ scores increased with age, reflecting a tendency towards Morningness. An evaluation of 14 saliva DLMO studies of clinically diagnosed DSWPD patients (mean ages 20 to 31 years) revealed mean saliva DLMO within the reference range albeit at the late extreme. Peak plasma melatonin levels from 179 studies of healthy subjects revealed a high degree of variability within studies and age groups, but only a small decline between the 20 and 50 years and lowest levels after 70 years.
Article
Full-text available
Introducción/objetivo: La realización de actividad física (AF) es considerada una manera muy rentable de mejorar la función neurocognitiva. Tanto la actividad física de intensidad moderada como la vigorosa de corta duración tienen efectos positivos en la función cerebral, la cognición y el rendimiento académico durante la infancia. El objetivo del presente estudio fue analizar el efecto de los descansos activos (DDAA) en la atención y motivación de los estudiantes, así como examinar posibles diferencias en cuanto a sexo y curso. Métodos: Participaron 215 estudiantes (119 niñas) de 2.º a 6.º de primaria, con edades comprendidas entre 7 y 13 años (M = 9.18; DE = 1.55), distribuidos en grupo experimental (n = 108; 62 niñas) y grupo control (n = 107; 57 niñas). Se realizó un diseño cuasi experimental con medidas pre-post y metodología cuantitativa. El grupo experimental recibió un programa de DDAA (20-30/semana; 2-5 minutos cada descanso activo). Se utilizó el Test de caras-R y el PLOC adaptado. Resultados: Los resultados en atención mostraron diferencias significativas entre grupos solo en 3.º, cuyo programa se basó en DDAA de intensidad vigorosa protagonizados por los estudiantes. El grupo experimental reportó niveles elevados de motivación autodeterminada. Los cursos de menor edad se mostraron más autodeterminados. Conclusiones: Los DDAA de intensidad vigorosa pueden provocar efectos positivos sobre la atención y motivación autodeterminada de los estudiantes.
Article
Full-text available
Background: Daily rhythms are observed in humans and almost all other organisms. Most of these observed rhythms reflect both underlying endogenous circadian rhythms and evoked responses from behaviours such as sleep/wake, eating/fasting, rest/activity, posture changes and exercise. For many research and clinical purposes, it is important to understand the contribution of the endogenous circadian component to these observed rhythms. Content: The goal of this manuscript is to provide guidance on best practices in measuring metrics of endogenous circadian rhythms in humans and promote the inclusion of circadian rhythms assessments in studies of health and disease. Circadian rhythms affect all aspects of physiology. By specifying minimal experimental conditions for studies, we aim to improve the quality, reliability and interpretability of research into circadian and daily (i.e., time-of-day) rhythms and facilitate the interpretation of clinical and translational findings within the context of human circadian rhythms. We describe protocols, variables and analyses commonly used for studying human daily rhythms, including how to assess the relative contributions of the endogenous circadian system and other daily patterns in behaviours or the environment. We conclude with recommendations for protocols, variables, analyses, definitions and examples of circadian terminology. Conclusion: Although circadian rhythms and daily effects on health outcomes can be challenging to distinguish in practice, this distinction may be important in many clinical settings. Identifying and targeting the appropriate underlying (patho)physiology is a medical goal. This review provides methods for identifying circadian effects to aid in the interpretation of published work and the inclusion of circadian factors in clinical research and practice.
Article
Full-text available
The misalignment between late chronotypes and early school start times affect health, performance and psychological well-being of adolescents. Here we test whether, and how, the baseline chronotype (i.e. chronotype at the beginning of secondary school) and the school timing affect the magnitude and the direction of the developmental change in chronotype during adolescence. We evaluated a sample of Argentinian students (n = 259) who were randomly assigned to attend school in the morning (07:45 a.m.–12:05 p.m.), afternoon (12:40 p.m.–05:00 p.m.) or evening (05:20 p.m.–09:40 p.m.) school timings. Importantly, chronotype and sleep habits were assessed longitudinally in the same group of students along secondary school (at 13–14 y.o. and 17–18 y.o.). Our results show that: (1) although chronotypes partially align with class time, this effect is insufficient to fully account for the differences observed in sleep-related variables between school timings; (2) both school timing and baseline chronotype are independently associated with the direction and the magnitude of change in chronotype, with greater delays related to earlier baseline chronotypes and later school timings. The practical implications of these results are challenging and should be considered in the design of future educational timing policies to improve adolescents’ well-being.
Article
Full-text available
This paper tested a culturally responsive four-factor model of aggregate subjective well-being (SWB). Hedonic SWB is often presented as the definitive articulation of SWB, whilst overlooking the impact of Eudaimonic well-being (EWB) on Aggregate SWB. Moreover, children from Confucian Heritage Cultures (CHC) are reported as having lower levels of SWB than children from other countries in studies that principally rely on hedonic instruments, thereby ignoring alternative indicators of well-being. While being a vital childhood indicator, hedonic SWB may not entirely capture the essence of well-being in CHC children who have different ontological interpretations of well-being. As an answer to this methodological shortcoming, EWB was included as a complementary component to hedonic SWB with it being hypothesised that EWB could bring added cultural responsiveness to the measurement of SWB and compensate for the under-reporting of levels of aggregate SWB in CHC children. Upon examination of the four components of SWB concurrently, it was possible to verify that the CHC sample of children had higher levels of EWB in absolute terms compared to the hedonic SWB components. Equally, support was found for a four-factor structure of SWB from which a higher order factor of SWB, termed as aggregate SWB, could meaningfully be represented as a combination of the four components. Within this model, EWB accounted for the most variance out of the four factors. These results have implications for the measurement of SWB and better understanding the developmental needs of CHC children thus providing a more attuned and culturally grounded indicator.
Article
Circadian rhythms are entrained by external factors such as sunlight and social cues, but also depend on internal factors such as age. Adolescents exhibit late chronotypes, but worldwide school starts early in the morning leading to unhealthy sleep habits. Several studies reported that adolescents benefit from later school start times. However, the effect of later school start time on different outcomes varies between studies, and most previous literature only takes into consideration the social clock (i.e. local time of school starting time) but not the solar clock (e.g. the distance between school start time and sunrise). Thus, there is an important gap in the literature: when assessing the effect of a school start time on chronotype and sleep of adolescents at different locations and/or seasons, the solar clock might differ and, consistently, the obtained results. For example, the earliest school start time for adolescents has been suggested to be 08:30 hours, but this school start time might correspond to different solar times at different times of the year, longitudes and latitudes. Here, we describe the available literature comparing different school start times, considering important factors such as geographic position, nationality, and the local school start time and its distance to sunrise. Then, we described and contrasted the relative role of both social and solar clocks on the chronotype and sleep of adolescents. As a whole, we point and discuss a gap in literature, suggesting that both clocks are relevant when addressing the effect of school start time on adolescents' chronotype and sleep.