ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

Overconfidence bias is a well-documented phenomenon in the field of psychology and behavioral finance. It refers to the tendency of individuals to overestimate their abilities and the accuracy of their predictions. This bias can have a significant impact on decision-making, particularly in the context of risk-taking. The current prevailing research has shown that overconfidence bias can lead individuals to take on more risk than they should, leading to negative outcomes such as financial losses or physical harm. Overconfidence can also lead individuals to ignore or downplay potential risks, leading to poor decision-making. This behavior can lead to a positive feedback loop, where the overconfident investors increased trading activity further reinforces their belief in their abilities, leading to even more aggressive trading. The literature on overconfidence bias highlights the importance of recognizing and managing this bias in decision-making, particularly in contexts where risk-taking is involved. Strategies such as seeking out diverse opinions and feedback, and taking a more objective approach to decision-making, can help individuals mitigate the negative impact of overconfidence bias.
Education and Society/UGC CARE Listed Journal/Year:46/Issue: 3/Vol.: II/April-June 2023/ISSN:2278-6864 1
Education and Society UGC CARE Listed Journal
(  ) ISSN 2278-6864
Education and Society
Since 1977
The Quarterly dedicated to Education through Social Development
And Social Development through Education
April-June 2023
Year: 46, Issue-3/ Volume-II
Indian Institute of Education
J. P. Naik Path, Kothrud, Pune- 38
Education and Society/UGC CARE Listed Journal/Year:46/Issue: 3/Vol.: II/April-June 2023/ISSN:2278-6864 2
Indian Institute of Education
Founder of the Institute
Prof. J. P. Naik and Dr. Chitra Naik
Education and Society
Editorial Board:
Prin. (Dr.) Jayasing Kalake, Chief Editor
Dr. Prakash B. Salavi, Executive Editor
Mrs. Shailaja D. Sawant, Secretary
Publisher:
Indian Institute of Education
J. P. Naik Path, Kothrud, Pune- 38
Contact Numbers: 8805159904, 9834109804
Web-site: www.iiepune.org
Email: educationandsociety1977@gmail.com, iiepune1948@gmail.com
----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
Education and Society, the educational quarterly is owned, printed and
published by the Indian Institute of Education, Pune. It is printed at Pratima
Mudran, 1-B, Devgiri Istate, Survey No. 17/1-B, Plot no. 14, Kothrud Industrial
Area, Kothrud, Pune 38. It is published by the Editor Dr. Jaysing Kalake at Indian
Institute of Education J. P. Naik Path, Kothrud, Pune- 38. Opinions or views or
statements and conclusions expressed in the articles that are published in this issue
are personal of respective authors. The editor, editorial board and the institution
will not be responsible for the same in any way.
Education and Society/UGC CARE Listed Journal/Year:46/Issue: 3/Vol.: II/April-June 2023/ISSN:2278-6864 395
Managing Overconfidence Bias in Decision Making: A
Review of the Literature
Dr. Vijaykumar Dhannur
Assistant Professor,
Shubham Mohan Kusane
Research Scholar
Dept. of Management Studies, Visvesvaraya Technological University, Belagavi
-------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
Abstract:
Overconfidence bias is a well-documented phenomenon in the field of
psychology and behavioral finance. It refers to the tendency of individuals to
overestimate their abilities and the accuracy of their predictions. This bias can have
a significant impact on decision-making, particularly in the context of risk-taking.
The current prevailing research has shown that overconfidence bias can lead
individuals to take on more risk than they should, leading to negative outcomes
such as financial losses or physical harm. Overconfidence can also lead individuals
to ignore or downplay potential risks, leading to poor decision-making. This
behavior can lead to a positive feedback loop, where the overconfident investors'
increased trading activity further reinforces their belief in their abilities, leading to
even more aggressive trading. The literature on overconfidence bias highlights the
importance of recognizing and managing this bias in decision-making, particularly
in contexts where risk-taking is involved. Strategies such as seeking out diverse
opinions and feedback, and taking a more objective approach to decision-making,
can help individuals mitigate the negative impact of overconfidence bias.
-------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
Keywords: Overconfidence bias, Behavioral Finance, Stock Market, Decision
Making and Cognitive Bias
-------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
Introduction:
Investment decisions refer to the choices made by individuals or
organizations regarding the allocation of financial resources into various assets
with the aim of generating a return on investment. These decisions can have a
significant impact on an individual's financial future, as well as the success of a
company. When making investment decisions, it is important to consider several
factors such as the investor's financial goals, risk tolerance, and the current
economic factors. Financial literacy is becoming increasingly important as
governments are prioritizing financial education to enhance the well-being of the
population. It involves a collection of competencies and comprehension required
to make informed financial judgments (Corsini & Giannelli, 2021). Despite that
Education and Society/UGC CARE Listed Journal/Year:46/Issue: 3/Vol.: II/April-June 2023/ISSN:2278-6864 396
there has always been irrationality among the investor courtesy behavioral bias
(Baker et al., 2019), (Paisarn et al., 2021). Of the many biases that researchers
work, one bias that has been very lucrative and spoken about is overconfidence.
Overconfidence bias is a psychological tendency or cognitive bias in which an
individual overestimates their abilities, knowledge, or performance in a particular
area. This bias can have significant consequences in many areas of life, including
decision-making, investment choices, and financial planning (Statman et al.,
2006). In the context of decision-making, overconfidence bias can lead individuals
to overestimate their ability to predict future outcomes or events. For example, an
individual may feel overly confident in their ability to predict the outcome of a
sporting event, political election, or stock market performance. This can lead to
poor decision-making, such as making risky bets or investments based on
incomplete or inaccurate information (Musah et al., 2023). Overconfidence bias
can also impact investment choices and financial planning. Investors who exhibit
overconfidence bias may believe that they have superior knowledge or insight into
a particular company or industry, leading them to make investment decisions
based on incomplete or inaccurate information. This can result in the investor
taking on greater risk or investing in assets that may not align with their overall
financial goals. Additionally, overconfidence bias can lead to the disposition
effect, which is the tendency for investors to hold onto losing investments for too
long and sell winning investments too quickly (Cueva et al., 2019). This can result
in investors missing out on potential gains and experiencing greater losses over
the long term (Tekçe & Yılmaz, 2015).
There are several factors that can contribute to overconfidence bias,
including past successes, social norms, and exposure to media and advertising. For
example, an individual who has experienced past successes may be more likely to
exhibit overconfidence bias, as they may believe that they have a track record of
making successful decisions. Additionally, exposure to media and advertising that
promote the idea of "expert" knowledge or "winning" strategies can reinforce
feelings of overconfidence (Zia et al., 2017). By giving more weight to their
personal signals than the overall market consensus, traders who are overconfident
can cause asset prices to deviate from their true values. This results in excessive
volatility in the market as asset prices become distorted. It is important to note that
such price distortions can lead to the formation of bubbles in asset prices
(Scheinkman & Xiong, 2003). Traces of overconfidence biases have been all the
segment of assets in the capital market like equity (Gupta et al., 2018), commodity
(Yung & Liu, 2009), ETFs (Kunjal and Peerbhai 2021). The bias has its own
influence on the risk preference and tolerance of investors. Overconfidence can
lead individuals to take on risks that are beyond their actual capabilities. For
example, an individual who is overconfident in their ability to invest in the stock
Education and Society/UGC CARE Listed Journal/Year:46/Issue: 3/Vol.: II/April-June 2023/ISSN:2278-6864 397
market may take on more risky investments than they can handle, leading to losses.
Also, overconfidence can lead individuals to ignore or downplay potential risks.
This can lead to poor decision-making, as individuals may not take into account
all the relevant information when assessing the risk of a particular action(Chen et
al., 2017) (Trinugroho & Sembel, 2011). When investors are overconfident, they
tend to link positive market returns with their own skills in selecting profitable
securities. This can result in more aggressive trading behavior during periods of
market gains. Consequently, when overconfidence is prevalent among investors,
past market returns can have a positive impact on current trading activities
(Gervais & Odean, 2001).
In Indian Context the traces of the bias have been studied and found
among the investors in Bombay Stock Exchange (BSE), the analysis of returns
impulse responses to private and public information shocks, it has been observed
that returns tend to overreact to private information and underreact to public
information. This means that the market tends to place more weight on information
that is only available to a select few individuals, while not fully incorporating
information that is publicly available to everyone. As a result, the market may be
slower to adjust to new public information, leading to potential inefficiencies and
market anomalies. This behavior can be attributed to factors such as information
asymmetry and cognitive biases, which can lead to suboptimal decision-making
(Mushinada & Veluri, 2018). The question is what boosts overconfidence among
the investors? The answer to the above question can be simply attributed to two
situations firstly, when investors make accurate forecasts of future returns, they
tend to become overconfident and trade more frequently in the following periods.
This behavior can be attributed to self-attribution bias, where investors attribute
their successful forecast to their own skill or knowledge, leading them to believe
that they can continue to achieve similar results. Conversely, when investors make
incorrect forecasts, their overconfidence may decrease slightly, as they may
recognize that their forecast was flawed. These findings highlight the importance
of understanding the role of cognitive biases in investment decision-making,
particularly in contexts where market gains may influence investor behavior. By
recognizing and managing biases such as self-attribution bias, investors can make
more informed and objective decisions that are not solely driven by
overconfidence or emotions (Chuang & Lee, 2006).
While overconfidence bias can have negative consequences, there are
several strategies that can help individuals mitigate its effects. One such strategy
is to seek out diverse perspectives and opinions when making decisions. Many
Fintech Companies have adopted newer technologies like automation,
gamification etc. that assist investors to make informed decisions. Investors active
participation in gamification can help reduce overconfidence bias and disposition
Education and Society/UGC CARE Listed Journal/Year:46/Issue: 3/Vol.: II/April-June 2023/ISSN:2278-6864 398
effect. This suggests that these biases can be mitigated through increased
engagement with gamified learning experiences (Şenol & Onay, 2023). Another
experiment that was conducted based on the gamification model suggested that by
using the gamified methods of learning, the results can enhance by at least 14.98%
and helps in judgmental forecasting and developing cautions traits among the
individuals (Legaki et al., 2021). The incorporation of gamification elements in
Personal Financial Management (PFM) apps can meet users' desire for
competency and independence, resulting in increased motivation to use them. This
motivation positively influences users' perception of the apps' usefulness and ease
of use, leading to more favorable attitudes towards them. The study further
revealed a positive correlation between users' attitudes towards PFM apps and their
behavioral intention to use them. Overall, these findings suggest that gamification
can enhance the effectiveness of PFM apps and promote user engagement (Bitrián
et al., 2021).
Conclusion:
Overconfidence bias can be both beneficial and dangerous for rational
decision making, depending on the context and situation. On the one hand,
overconfidence bias can lead individuals to take on challenges and pursue
opportunities that they may have otherwise avoided, leading to positive outcomes
and achievements. However, on the other hand, overconfidence bias can lead to
poor decision-making and negative outcomes, particularly in the context of risk-
taking. Overconfident individuals may underestimate the likelihood of negative
outcomes, leading to excessive risk-taking and potential harm. Additionally,
overconfidence bias can lead individuals to ignore or downplay potential risks,
leading to poor decision-making. Furthermore, studies have shown that
overconfidence bias is prevalent among investors, particularly those who are
actively trading in the stock market. Overconfident investors tend to associate
positive market returns with their own security-picking abilities, leading them to
trade more aggressively during periods of market gains. This behavior can lead to
a positive feedback loop, where the overconfident investors' increased trading
activity further reinforces their belief in their abilities, leading to even more
aggressive trading. Understanding the impact of overconfidence bias on decision-
making is crucial for making informed and objective decisions. By recognizing
and managing biases such as overconfidence, individuals can mitigate the negative
impact of these biases and make better decisions that are more aligned with their
goals and objectives.
References:
1. Baker, H. K., Kumar, S., Goyal, N., & Gaur, V. (2019). How financial literacy and
demographic variables relate to behavioral biases. Managerial Finance, 45(1), 124146.
https://doi.org/10.1108/MF-01-2018-0003
Education and Society/UGC CARE Listed Journal/Year:46/Issue: 3/Vol.: II/April-June 2023/ISSN:2278-6864 399
2. Bitrián, P., Buil, I., & Catalán, S. (2021). Making finance fun: the gamification of
personal financial management apps. International Journal of Bank Marketing, 39(7),
13101332. https://doi.org/10.1108/IJBM-02-2021-0074
3. Chen, X., Wang, Z., Deng, S., & Fang, Y. (2017). Risk measure optimization:
Perceived risk and overconfidence of structured product investors. Journal of Industrial &
Management Optimization, 13(5), 120. https://doi.org/10.3934/jimo.2018105
4. Chuang, W.-I., & Lee, B.-S. (2006). An empirical evaluation of the overconfidence
hypothesis. Journal of Banking & Finance, 30(9), 24892515.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbankfin.2005.08.007
5. Corsini, L., & Giannelli, G. C. (2021). Economics education and financial literacy
acquisition: Evidence from a field experiment. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental
Finance, 32, 100556. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbef.2021.100556
6. Cueva, C., Iturbe-Ormaetxe, I., Ponti, G., & Tomás, J. (2019). An experimental
analysis of the disposition effect: Who and when? Journal of Behavioral and Experimental
Economics, 81, 207215. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socec.2019.06.011
7. Gervais, S., & Odean, T. (2001). Learning to Be Overconfident. Review of Financial
Studies, 14(1), 127. https://doi.org/10.1093/rfs/14.1.1
8. Gupta, S., Goyal, V., Kalakbandi, V. K., & Basu, S. (2018). Overconfidence, trading
volume and liquidity effect in Asia’s Giants: evidence from pre-, during- and post-global
recession. DECISION, 45(3), 235257. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40622-018-0185-9
9. Kunjal, D., & Peerbhai, F. (2021). Investor overconfidence in the South African
exchange traded fund market. Cogent Economics & Finance, 9(1).
https://doi.org/10.1080/23322039.2021.1978190
10. Legaki, N.-Z., Karpouzis, K., Assimakopoulos, V., & Hamari, J. (2021). Gamification
to avoid cognitive biases: An experiment of gamifying a forecasting course.
Technological Forecasting and Social Change, 167, 120725.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.techfore.2021.120725
11. Musah, G., Domeher, D., & Frimpong, J. M. (2023). Presidential election uncertainty
and investor overconfidence bias in sub-Saharan African stock markets. African Journal
of Economic and Management Studies, 14(1), 7085. https://doi.org/10.1108/AJEMS-03-
2022-0075
12. Mushinada, V. N. C., & Veluri, V. S. S. (2018). Investors overconfidence behaviour
at Bombay Stock Exchange. International Journal of Managerial Finance, 14(5), 613632.
https://doi.org/10.1108/IJMF-05-2017-0093
13. Paisarn, W., Chancharat, N., & Chancharat, S. (2021). Factors Influencing Retail
Investors’ Trading Behaviour in the Thai Stock Market. Australasian Business,
Accounting & Finance Journal, 15(2), 2637. https://doi.org/10.14453/aabfj.v15i2.3
14. Scheinkman, J. A., & Xiong, W. (2003). Overconfidence and Speculative Bubbles.
Journal of Political Economy, 111(6), 11831220. https://doi.org/10.1086/378531
15. Şenol, D., & Onay, C. (2023). Impact of gamification on mitigating behavioral biases
of investors. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Finance, 37, 100772.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbef.2022.100772
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any citations for this publication.
Article
Full-text available
Exchange Traded Funds (ETFs) have proven to be extremely popular amongst both retail and institutional investors. The increasing interest in this asset class may incite overconfidence in its’ investor base, which could lead to undesirable market effects such as security mispricing, excess trading volumes, and exacerbated market volatility. This study aims to examine the South African ETF market for presence of investor overconfidence. To achieve this objective, Vector Autoregression (VAR) models and their associated impulse response functions are employed to examine the relationship between the current trading activity and the historical market return. Consistent with the overconfidence hypothesis, a positive and significant relationship between current market turnover and lagged market returns is found for both ETFs with domestic benchmarks and ETFs with international benchmarks. Further analysis of panel VAR models and their associated impulse response functions suggest that the overconfidence bias also influences the trading activities of individual ETFs. These findings have important implications for various market participants.
Article
Full-text available
In their daily lives, people are confronted with situations where they need to form a schema of possible future scenarios and the likelihood of them occurring, be it about climate change, economic up- or downturn, or even the potential success of a romantic date. Be these issues of mundane or universal importance, this judgmental forecasting poses people with a difficult pervasive cognitive challenge. Commonly, judgmental forecasting is taught in forecasting courses syllabi, and the pedagogy surrounding it is challenging. However, gamification and game-based learning have risen as promising tools to simulate different kinds of scenarios and stimulate cognitive problem solving. This study investigates the effects of a gamified application with points, levels, challenges, storytelling and leaderboard for teaching judgmental forecasting by conducting a 2×2 between-subjects experiment (treatments: i) read: yes vs no, and ii) gamification: yes vs no), with a sample of 285 students of a School of Electrical and Computer Engineering and a Business Administration Department. The findings indicate that the gamified application improved learning outcomes regarding the heuristics and biases that affect judgmental forecasting by almost 15%, supporting the use of gamification in forecasting education.
Article
Full-text available
This paper investigates trading behaviour among Thai retail investors in 2016. Using detailed survey data from 491 investors, we examine the characteristics and behavioural patterns that lead to investor bias. Empirical results in the behavioural finance literature indicate that retail investors may not behave reasonably. Behavioural biases may influence investor decisions and affect financial markets. These studies, however, are limited to subsamples of the overall investor groups studied and mainly focus on developed markets. We find that biases are common among investors and that men are more overconfident than women. Moreover, we discover that investors with more experience in trading are less likely to hold their stocks for long periods of time. Further, investors aged 45 and younger hold more diversified portfolios. Another finding is that participants with an income of more than 50,000 Baht a month and/or who employ a number of brokers hold more diversified portfolios. This evidence is consistent with the findings that have been reported for Turkey, India, and Vietnam, indicating that demographic factors are useful for distinguishing between investors in terms of the level of overconfidence bias they exhibit. This result confirms that demographic factors play a role in differentiating and classifying retail investors and should motivate future researchers to consider these factors in their research.
Article
Full-text available
Purpose The purpose of the paper is to empirically test the overconfidence hypothesis at Bombay Stock Exchange (BSE). Design/methodology/approach The study applies bivariate vector autoregression to perform the impulse-response analysis and EGARCH models to understand whether there is self-attribution bias and overconfidence behavior among the investors. Findings The study shows the empirical evidence in support of overconfidence hypothesis. The results show that the overconfident investors overreact to private information and underreact to the public information. Based on EGARCH specifications, it is observed that self-attribution bias, conditioned by right forecasts, increases investors’ overconfidence and the trading volume. Finally, the analysis of the relation between return volatility and trading volume shows that the excessive trading of overconfident investors makes a contribution to the observed excessive volatility. Research limitations/implications The study focused on self-attribution and overconfidence biases using monthly data. Further studies can be encouraged to test the proposed hypotheses on daily data and also other behavioral biases. Practical implications Insights from the study suggest that the investors should perform a post-analysis of each investment so that they become aware of past behavioral mistakes and stop continuing the same. This might help investors to minimize the negative impact of self-attribution and overconfidence on their expected utility. Originality/value To the best of the authors’ knowledge, this is the first study to examine the investors’ overconfidence behavior at market-level data in BSE, India.
Article
We investigate whether gamification can help mitigate behavioral biases of investors by conducting a unique stock market experiment that is free from observer-expectancy and subject-expectancy effects. Utilizing the trading data of investors who simultaneously have active portfolios in an investment firm and stock market simulation game, we show that investors have different biases in real versus simulated settings. We find that participating in a stock market game affects all biases differently, with different degrees of participation to the game. While overconfidence bias and disposition effect can be mitigated and decrease with more active participation in the game, familiarity and status quo biases increase. We also show that young, inexperienced investors with average-sized portfolios and men are more likely to participate. These findings will especially be of interest to researchers, financial institutions and governmental bodies that plan to conduct similar experiments and design services promoting better financial decision-making and investment behavior.
Article
Purpose The purpose of this paper is to investigate overconfidence bias and the effect of presidential elections on investor overconfidence bias in sub-Saharan African stock markets. Design/methodology/approach The study uses the vector autoregressive (VAR) model and its associated impulse response functions to investigate overconfidence bias. Furthermore, we make use of OLS regressions to examine the effect of presidential elections on investor overconfidence bias. Findings Investor overconfidence bias is present in the markets of Ghana and Tanzania suggesting that the phenomenon persists in sub–Saharan Africa's small markets. We also find that post-presidential election periods have a dampening effect on investor overconfidence in a country where there is less post-election uncertainty. Originality/value Despite the previous studies on investor overconfidence bias in sub-Saharan Africa, this paper to the best of the authors’ knowledge, is the first to investigate investor overconfidence bias in the context of presidential elections.
Article
We investigate how attending an undergraduate course in economics affects students’ financial literacy. The specific conditions of the course, which was compulsorily taken by law students at the University of Firenze, reproduce a quasi-experiment that enables us to isolate the effect of the course and provides solid evidence on its effect. We find that attending the course has positive effects on financial literacy and that the gain is most relevant for students coming from backgrounds associated with lower financial literacy. The gain is large for topics closer to what was taught in the course but small for other economic issues. From a policy perspective, our evidence provides support for the feasibility and efficacy of policies promoting general economics education, and it also stresses the redistributive and egalitarian character of such policies.
Article
Purpose This study integrates self-determination theory (SDT) and the technology acceptance model (TAM) to explore how gamification increases users' motivation and intention to use personal financial management (PFM) apps, and how it facilitates their adoption. Design/methodology/approach Data from 208 users of the Mint app were analyzed using partial least squares structural equation modeling. Findings The results showed that gamifying PFM apps satisfies users' needs for competence and autonomy and enhances their autonomous motivation to use them. Users' motivation increases their perceptions of ease of use and usefulness of the apps and causes them to develop more favorable attitudes toward them. The findings also confirmed a relationship between users' attitudes toward PFM apps and the behavioral intention to use them. Research limitations/implications To investigate the generalizability of results, studies using other PFM apps would be useful. The cross-sectional nature of the research also limits its causal inference. Practical implications This research provides support for the use of gamification in PFM apps and offers suggestions that may help fintech companies and banks to persuade users to engage with their apps. Originality/value Although gamification is a trending topic, few studies have explored its use in the finance industry. Drawing on SDT and the TAM, this study extends previous research and adds new insights into the effects of gamification in this context.
Article
The disposition effect (DE) is a common bias by which investors tend to sell winning assets too soon and hold losing assets too long. We complement the existing evidence in three directions. First, we check whether the DE is robust to realistic features such as transaction costs and competitive payment schemes. Second, by using a gender-balanced design, we check for gender differences. Third, we search for psychological correlates of the DE. We find that the DE is positive and significant in all our treatments. We do not find significant differences across treatments, although transaction costs significantly reduce the propensity to sell both winners and losers. We find somewhat larger DE in women, but this effect is only significant in the second half of the experiment. On the other hand, women are more reluctant to sell losing assets throughout the experiment. Finally, we find that the most significant psychological predictors of the DE are difficulty recognizing one's mistakes and optimism. Subjects scoring high in these traits are less likely to sell at a loss and therefore exhibit a larger DE. Our results provide further suggestive evidence of cognitive dissonance as an important determinant of the DE.
Article
In financial optimization, it is important to quantify the risk of structured financial products. This paper quantifies the risk of structured financial products by perceived risk measures based on a standard measure of risk, and then we construct the risk perception and decision-making models of individual investors considering structured products. Moreover, based on bullish and bearish binary structured products, we introduce the psychological bias of overconfidence to explore how this bias affects investors' perceived risk. This study finds that overconfident investors believe in private signals and underestimate the variance of noise in private signals, which affects their expectation of the underlying asset price of structured financial products. Furthermore, overconfidence bias leads investors to overestimate the probability of obtaining a better return. With the increase in overconfidence, the overestimation of the probability is intensified, which eventually leads to lower perceived risk.