Content uploaded by Alberth --
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Alberth -- on Dec 17, 2023
Content may be subject to copyright.
(2023), 6 (2): 380–393
380
P-ISSN 2623-0356
E-ISSN 2654-5586
HOW IMPORTANT IS COMMUNICATING WITH NATIVE ENGLISH
SPEAKERS TO EFL LEARNERS’ SELF-CONFIDENCE IN THEIR ENGLISH
LANGUAGE PROFICIENCY?
Alberth
Universitas Halu Oleo
SUBMISSION TRACK
A B S T R A C T
Submitted
:
7 July 2023
Research evidence shows that communicating and interacting with
native speakers could boost students’ self-confidence in their
language proficiency. If this claim proves to be veritable, students
who have the experience communicating and interacting with
native speakers, either online or offline, would expectedly feel
more confident than those who do not. The present study sought to
examine whether significant differences existed in the self-
confidence level of EFL (English as a Foreign Language) learners
who (a) had the experience communicating with native English
speakers and who did not; (b) had the intention to communicate
with native English speakers and who did not; (c) were males and
females, and (d) who were in different academic years (year 1, year
2, and year 3 respectively). Independent samples t-test revealed
that, whereas males and females were equally confident, learners
who had the experience interacting, or who had the intention
communicating, with native English speakers were significantly
more confident in their English language proficiency than those
who had never interacted, or who did not have the intention to
communicate, with native English speakers. No significant
differences were observed based on year of enrollment.
Implications of this study on the English language teaching and
learning will be critically discussed in this paper.
Accepted
:
22 July 2023
Published
:
25 July 2023
KEYWORDS
Self-confidence, native
speaker, social interaction,
communication
CORRESPONDENCE
E-mail: alberth@uho.ac.id
Introduction
Self-confidence has long been identified as one of the critical success factors in
language learning (Huang, 2014: Hastowohadi & Kumaini, 2019). By and large, highly
confident and less anxious language learners are more likely to succeed in their endeavors
to learn a foreign language than those who are less confident and more anxious (Lee,
2019). Self-confidence has also been known to dictate whether or not language learners
would be willing to use the target language (Yashima, 2002). In fact, self-confidence is
considered to be more essential than language proficiency level when it comes to
communicating using a foreign language. Language proficiency level does not
necessarily translate to willingness to use the language being learned (MacIntyre,
Clément, Dörnyei, & Noels, 1998). It is not surprising that language learners who have a
lower language proficiency level, but are highly confident, are more likely to
communicate using the target language than those who are more proficient, but lacking
self-confidence (de Saint Léger & Storch, 2009).
In view of the critical role of self-confidence in foreign language learning, various
strategies have been introduced in the literature to boost students’ self-confidence levels.
These include, but are not limited to, the use of positive feedback (Peñaloza & Salamanca,
2019), team project (Shokri, 2010), encouragement (Wu, 2019), games (Cine, 2021), and
the use of blog (Watkins, 2016). Interestingly, it has also been suggested that getting
students to interact with native speakers (NSs) of the target language could potentially
boost their self-confidence (Archangeli, 1999). However, much of this claim is based on
(2023), 6 (2): 380–393
381
P-ISSN 2623-0356
E-ISSN 2654-5586
either anecdotal or insufficient empirical evidence, and has not been thoroughly
scrutinized across a wide array of socio-cultural contexts. If interacting with NSs does, in
fact, result in students having a higher level of self-confidence, we would expect that
students who have previous experience interacting with NSs, either online or offline,
would be more confident in their target language proficiency than those who do not share
such experience in the first place. Alas, little research has explored this very important
issue. Conducted in a socio-cultural context where there is a notable dearth of research
on this particular topic, the present study endeavoured to scrutinize whether having the
experience interacting and communicating with native English speakers (NESs) does, in
fact, impinge on students’ perceived self-confidence. Thus, this research has both
practical and theoretical implications on foreign language teaching, especially English.
In the following section, literature concerning how communicating with NSs could be
beneficial for foreign language learning will be examined, including how interacting and
communicating with NSs could enhance language learners’ self-confidence in using the
target language. Finally, the importance of self-confidence will also be touched on.
The Role of Social Interactions with NSs in Foreign Language Learning
By and large, research studies have shown that social interaction and
communication involving NSs are critical to foreign language learning. In particular, real-
life and authentic communication with NSs of the target language is of immense
importance as they help language learners develop their proficiency in the target language
(Hedayati, Nur, Emery, & Pavlyshyn, 2016; Jiang & Li, 2018). For example, it has been
reported that social interaction with NSs results in improved motivation, communicative
competencies, language skills, and interest in learning a foreign language (Wang, 2014).
Additionally, being involved in social interactions with NSs encourages ‘negotiation of
meaning’ between language learners and NSs, thus improving language proficiency of
the learners (Hedayati et al., 2016; Ueno, 2019).
Additionally, empirical evidence also suggests that students who experience
frequent communication with NSs reported improvement in foreign language proficiency
more than those who experience less frequent communication (Dewey, Bown, & Eggett,
2012). Above all, talking to NSs could be an enjoyable learning experience. In a study
conducted by Ueno (2019), participants were reported to be very positive about
interacting with NSs beyond formal language learning contexts. Due to these benefits, it
has been argued that foreign language is best acquired through social interaction with NSs
in a less formal, but more natural context (Bahrani & Sim, 2012; Martinsen & Thompson,
2019; Wang, 2014). In this case, foreign language learners acquire the target language in
a more natural, more authentic way than what generally takes place in a brick-and-mortar
formal classroom settings (Zheng, Young, Brewer, & Wagner, 2009).
Advances in the new technology has enabled foreign language learners to
communicate with NSs of the target language using computer-mediated communication
(Jiang & Li, 2018; Martinsen & Thompson, 2019; Zhang, 2016). Computer-mediated
communication with NSs was reported to be motivating, especially for low proficient
learners (Jauregi, De Graaff, Van den Bergh, & Kriz, 2012). What is more, synchronous
communication with NSs can be a fun and engaging experience (Wang, 2014). More
importantly, communicating with NSs enables foreign language learners to acquire
‘comprehensible input’ critical to language acquisition (Krashen, 1985), and also, at the
same time, enables them to experiment with their ‘comprehensible output’ (Swain, 1995)
(2023), 6 (2): 380–393
382
P-ISSN 2623-0356
E-ISSN 2654-5586
in a less intimidating environment (Wang, 2014). Hence, the new technology provides
ample opportunities for foreign language learners to reach out NSs of the target language
without having to travel a great distance to meet one (Alberth, Wiramihardja, & Uden,
2020). Of course, face-to-face encounters are also possible, but in some EFL contexts,
finding a native speaker is quite a challenge and this is exactly what happens in the context
of the present study.
Since research has confirmed the importance of social interaction with NSs, some
educational institutions intentionally arranged class activities in such away that foreign
language learners would have to interact with NSs to complete a given assignment. In a
study by Cadd (2012), for example, foreign language learners were, as a course task,
required to interact with NSs. The study suggests that language learners with a higher
language proficiency level tend to make more gains in terms of linguistic proficiency
while studying abroad as compared to those with a lower proficiency level. These gains
are attributed primarily to more proficient learners being more willing to interact with
NSs (Cadd, 2012). However, other studies also suggest that the benefit of interacting with
NSs is also shared by novice language learners (Jauregi et al., 2012; Satar & Özdener,
2008; Schenker, 2017), presumably because such activities are perceived to be
motivating, fun, and authentic.
Furthermore, in a research study examining the role of interacting with NSs,
language learners were required to interview two NSs (one older speaker and one about
the same age as the interviewer) as part of their study-abroad course requirements in
Salzburg. Results of this study indicated that these assignments resulted in improved self-
confidence and willingness to communicate with NSs of the target language (Archangeli,
1999). In a similar study, Cadd (2012) reported that learners assigned to interview NSs
indicated that their anxiety levels dwindled and that they became more proficient and
more willing to communicate with NSs. They also asserted that they gained a better
understanding of the culture of the speaker of the language being learned. Through
interactions with NSs, language learners can also improve “their communicative
competence at linguistic, sociolinguistic, discursive, and strategic levels” (Hedayati et al.,
2016, Native and non-Native Language Speaker Interaction section, para 1).
Simply put, because communicating with NSs is perceived to be enjoyable/fun
(Wang, 2014), reduce anxiety levels (Cadd, 2012), improve foreign language proficiency
and promote better cross-cultural understanding (Cadd, 2012; Martinsen & Thompson,
2019), it is natural to expect that language learners who have the experience interacting
with NSs would feel more confident in their language proficiency (Archangeli, 1999) than
those who have never interacted with one. A similar case can be argued for language
learners seeking to engage in social interaction with NSs. Instinct tells us that only those
students having a higher level of confidence would be willing to seek the opportunity to
communicate with NSs, especially when such decision is made volitionally without
coercion. In other words, the presence of the intention to engage in social interaction with
NSs itself implies the existence of self-confidence in the target language proficiency.
However, it is worth noting that the conclusions drawn by most of the previous
studies were based on a fairly small number of subjects and, in some cases, only involved
13 participants, for example. This is understandable given the fact that these studies relied
heavily on a qualitative approach. Whereas findings from such studies are valid and they
provide in-depth and rich information, the data obtained from this limited number of
participants could potentially introduce bias, primarily due to “participants’
idiosyncrasies” (Herzberg & Roth, 2006; Stumpf & Muscroft, 2011). Such studies also
(2023), 6 (2): 380–393
383
P-ISSN 2623-0356
E-ISSN 2654-5586
restrict the generalizability of the findings across different research contexts. Note that
previous studies were conducted in a socio-cultural context which is significantly
different from the context of the present study. Needless to say, socio-cultural contexts
also come into play when discussing how language learners perceive social interaction
with NSs. It goes without saying, what works in a particular context may not necessarily
work in others. Thus, it is important that a quantitative approach involving a larger
number of subjects be employed and that research studies be conducted in a socio-cultural
context where there is a notable dearth of research on this particular topic.
Self-Confidence
In the context of foreign language learning, the term ‘self-confidence’ has been
used to refer to the extent to which language learners feel confident about using the target
language being learned. The term has been used interchangeably with ‘linguistic self-
confidence’ (Lee, 2019). A distinction is made between ‘state’ and ‘trait-like’ self-
confidence (MacIntyre et al., 1998). The former refers to feeling confident in a certain
situation, such as using a foreign language to book a flight, whereas the latter to more
stable personal characteristics.
Research evidence has consistently confirmed the importance of self-confidence
in language learning. By and large, literature suggests that low self-confidence level could
result in a high anxiety level and a high anxiety level, known as debilitating anxiety, is
unfavorable to language learning (Cheng, Horwitz, & Schallert, 1999; Sahlan, Alberth,
Madil, & Hutnisyawati, 2021). For example, without self-confidence, language learners
would not be willing to communicate using the language being learned (Yashima, Zenuk‐
Nishide, & Shimizu, 2004). In fact, learners who have a higher level of self-confidence
are more likely to participate volitionally in class activities, especially those involving
oral activities, such as speaking. By comparison, language learners with a low level of
self-confidence tend to underestimate their potential and consequently develop negative
evaluation of their performance. It is this negative evaluation that develops into anxiety
(MacIntyre, Noels, & Clément, 1997). Self-confidence and anxiety are, thus, negatively
correlated, in that a higher level of anxiety would generally indicate a lower level of self-
confidence and vice-versa (Sahlan et al., 2021).
Furthermore, highly confident language learners are also more likely to take risks
in using the target language, despite committing errors in their attempts (Molberg, 2010).
Less confident learners, on the contrary, are inclined to avoid such activities, for they feel
uncomfortable to be criticized (Al-Sibai, 2005). Hence, highly confident language
learners are more likely to actively pursue chances to engage in communication, including
interacting and communicating with NSs of the target language, the activities of which
may be avoided by learners who are lacking self-confidence. If this conclusion proves to
be valid, we would also expect to see that English language learners who have intention
to communicate and interact with NESs volitionally should be more confident than those
who do not. By the same token, since previous studies suggest that communicating and
interacting with NSs improve learners’ self-confidence and their willingness to
communicate using the target language (Archangeli, 1999; Cadd, 2012), we would also
expect that language learners who have the experience communicating with NSs of the
target language would be more self-confident than those who have never communicated
with one.
Research has also explored such a question as whether or not self-confidence in
language learning is related to gender. It has been suggested that females are more
(2023), 6 (2): 380–393
384
P-ISSN 2623-0356
E-ISSN 2654-5586
talented in language learning and generally score higher than male students. Not
surprisingly, females are also reported to be more self-confident than males in their
competencies to learn a new language (Cui, 2011; Dörnyei, 2001), notwithstanding
females feeling more anxious than males (Abu-Rabia, 2004). However, conflicting
findings have been reported in the literature. For example, as far as language learning is
concerned, males are found to be more confident than females (Piasecka, 2014; Gul, Qazi,
& Shakir, 2021). It appears that, whilst females are probably generally better at learning
a new language, there are variables other than gender alone that is responsible for
language learners’ self-confidence.
Furthermore, the role of seniority (sophomores, juniors, seniors) in self-
confidence in foreign language learning remains one of the under-researched topics. A
question worth investigating is, whether or to what extent, seniority is related to language
learners’ self-confidence in English language learning. Are senior students necessarily
more confident than their junior counterparts because of the duration of time involved in
learning the target language or whether seniority is not related to self-confidence at all?
Surprisingly, to date, little work has attempted to examine this very important issue and
our understanding on these questions is still in its infancy. The objective of the current
study was to address this research gap.
Research Method
Respondents
A total of 402 high school students of SMAN 5 Kendari, Indonesia, were recruited
for the present study using a convenience sampling technique. The study involved 249
females and 153 males comprising year 1, 2 and 3. Year one comprised 98 students (31
males and 67 females); year two 161 students (66 males and 95 females); year three 143
students (56 males and 87 females). As seen above, females were predominant which, in
fact, represented the trend in entire populations. Respondents were between 14 – 20 years
old (M = 16,24, SD = 1.04). Whereas respondents were encouraged to participate in the
present study, participation was completely voluntary and respondents had the option to
discontinue their participation in the stud anytime without the need to justify their
decisions. Additionally, consents from the respondents were sought prior to the study.
Procedures
Participants were required to respond to three different Likert-scale questions. In
the first question, they were required to indicate their self-confidence level by selecting 1
if they were highly unconfident, 2 unconfident, 3 not sure, 4 confident, and 5 highly
confident. In the second question, participants were asked to indicate whether or not they
previously met, interacted or communicated, either online or offline, with NESs by
selecting “Yes” if they did or “No” if they did not. In the third question, participants were
asked to indicate whether or not they intended to meet, interact, or communicate with
NESs, either online or offline, by selecting “Yes” if they did or “No” if they did not.
Participants responded to these questions via a hyperlink provided in a Google Form, and
they completed the survey outside the class time.
Data Analysis
An independent samples t-test was performed to investigate potential significant
variations in the self-confidence level among EFL learners who (a) had the experience
interacting with NESs, either online or offline, and who did not; (b) had the intention to
(2023), 6 (2): 380–393
385
P-ISSN 2623-0356
E-ISSN 2654-5586
communicate with NESs and who did not; (c) were males and females. Additionally, a
one-way ANOVA was also run to examine whether significant differences exist in the
self-confidence level of year 1, year 2, and year 3 students. Prior to conducting each
analysis, thorough checks were performed to ensure that all statistical assumptions were
met without any violations. Examination of the assumptions related to normality and
homogeneity of variance revealed no violations. This, in turn, justified the application of
independent samples t-test and ANOVA with the data.
Result and Discussion
Result
To begin with, the number of students who reported that they had the experience
communicating with NESs and that they did not by gender is provided in the following
table.
Status of communication
Gender
Total
Male
Female
Have communicated with NESs
61
81
142
Have never communicated with NESs
92
168
260
Total
153
249
402
Table 1. Status of communication with NESs by gender
As seen from the above table, about 35% of the students participating in the present
study reported that they have had the experience communicating with NESs, whereas
65% have not. As far as gender is concerned, around 40% of males and 33% of females
reported having communicated with NESs. When broken down into year of enrolment,
the following trends emerge.
Status of communication
Year of enrolment
Total
Year1
Year2
Year3
Have communicated with NESs
36
56
50
142
Have never communicated with
NESs
62
105
93
260
Total
98
161
143
402
Table 2. Status of communication with NESs by year of enrolment
As seen from the above table, around 37% of year 1 students reported to have
communicated with NESs compared to 35% of year 2 and 35% for year 3 students. Thus,
it appears that the percentage of students who have been involved in social interaction
with NESs is similar across the three different cohorts.
Interestingly, more than 70% of the total participants indicated that they intend to
communicate and interact with NESs and only less than 30% reported that they had no
intention to do so.
Intention to communicate with NSs
Gender
Total
Male
Female
Intend to communicate with NESs
97
195
292
Do not intend to communicate with
NESs
56
54
110
Total
153
249
402
Table 3. Intention to communicate with NESs by gender
(2023), 6 (2): 380–393
386
P-ISSN 2623-0356
E-ISSN 2654-5586
When asked whether or not they intend to communicate with NESs, around 63% of
males and 78% of females indicate that they do. In other words, the percentage of female
who have the intention to engage in social interaction with NESs is higher than that of
males. Whereas this intention to interact with NESs varies considerably with gender, this
does not appear to vary with year of enrolment.
Intention to communicate with
NSs
Year of enrolment
Total
Year1
Year2
Year3
Intend to communicate with
NESs
76
116
100
292
Do not intend to communicate
with NESs
22
45
43
110
Total
98
161
143
402
Table 4. Intention to communicate with NESs by year of enrolment
As shown from the above table, overall, 78% of year 1, 72% of year 2, and 70% of
year three students express their intention to engage in communication with NESs and
only less than 30% for the three groups express no intention to meet NESs. Note that, as
students are becoming more senior, the percentage of those who intend to communicate
with NESs also decreases gradually. This finding is interesting and is worth further
investigation in future research. Nonetheless, a question arises whether students who have
the experience interacting, or who have the intention communicating, with NESs have a
similar level of self-confidence.
An independent samples t-test was conducted on students’ self-confidence scores
to examine whether significant differences exist in the self-confidence level of students
who have previously interacted with NESs, either online or offline, and who have not
(Research Question #1). Independent samples t-test suggests that t (400) = 3.84, p (0.00)
< 0.05. This suggests that there exists a notable disparity in the self-confidence level of
students who have previously interacted or communicated with NESs (M = 3.11, SD =
0.79) and who have not (M = 2.78, SD = 0.81), in that students who have previously
interacted with NSs are more confident in their English language proficiency than those
who have not.
Similarly, an independent samples t-test was run on students’ self-confidence scores
to examine whether significant differences exist in the self-confidence level of students
who intend to interact with NESs, either online or offline, and who do not (Research
Question #2). Independent samples t-test indicates that t (400) = 2.74, p (0.00) < 0.05. It
can, therefore, be concluded that there is a significant difference in the self-confidence
level of students who intend to interact with NESs (M = 2.97, SD = 0.82) and who do not
(M = 2.72, SD = 0.79), in that students who intend to interact with NESs are more
confident in their English language proficiency than those who do not.
A separate independent samples t-test was performed to assess the existence of
possible significant differences in the self-confidence level between male and female
students. (Research Question #3). Independent samples t-test suggests that t (347.82) = -
1.50, p (0.13) > 0.05. It can, therefore, be concluded that the self-confident level of males
(M = 2.97, SD = 0.76) and females (M = 2.85, SD = 0.85) was not significantly different
– they were equally confident.
Finally, A one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted to investigate
whether significant differences exist in the self-confidence level of students belonging to
the first, second, and third year, respectively (Research Question #4). No significant
(2023), 6 (2): 380–393
387
P-ISSN 2623-0356
E-ISSN 2654-5586
differences were observed across the three different cohorts – they were all equally
confident, F (2,399) = 1.03, p (.36) > .05.
A visual comparison of self-confidence level of (a) students who have the
experience interacting with NESs and those who do not, (b) students who have the
intention to communicate with NESs and those who do not, (c) male and female, and (d)
first, second, and third year students is presented in the following bar graphs.
Figure 1. Mean score of self-confidence:
Interaction and no Interaction with NESs
Figure 2. Mean score of self-confidence:
Intention and no Intention to communicate with
NESs
Figure 3. Mean score of self-confidence: Male
and Female
Figure 4. Mean score of self-confidence: Year 1,
Year 2 and Year 3
Discussion
The present study examined whether significant differences existed in the self-
confidence level of EFL learners who (a) had the experience interacting with NESs, either
online or offline, and who did not, (b) had the intention to communicate with NESs and
who did not, (c) were males and females, and (d) who were in different academic years
(first, second, and third year respectively). As reported earlier, whereas no significant
differences were observed in the self-confidence level of males and females and the self-
confidence level of students in different academic years, respondents who have the
experience interacting with NESs or who have the intention communicating with NESs
are significantly more confident in their English language proficiency than those who
either have never been engaged in social interaction with NESs, or who do not have the
intention to interact with NESs.
3.11
2.78
Interaction No Interaction
2.97
2.72
Intention No Intention
2.97
2.85
Male Female
2.82
2.96
2.88
Year 1 Year 2 Year 3
(2023), 6 (2): 380–393
388
P-ISSN 2623-0356
E-ISSN 2654-5586
These findings confirm the widely-held belief concerning the advantages of
engaging with NSs in terms of improved self-confidence level (e.g. Archangeli, 1999;
Hedayati et al., 2016; Jiang & Li, 2018). As mentioned earlier, self-confidence has been
identified as a pivotal determinant of success in language learning, particularly with
regard to the realm of foreign language acquisition (Huang, 2014). Individuals who
possess heightened self-assurance and reduced anxiety levels among language learners
are, by and large, more likely to succeed in their endeavors compared to individuals
characterized by diminished self-assurance and heightened anxiety levels (Lee, 2019).
Self-confidence also has a direct bearing to willingness to communicate (Yashima, 2002),
in that more confident learners are more willing to use the target language.
As stated earlier, social interaction involving NSs offer various benefits for
language learning. For example, not only does it motivate students to learn the target
language, but it also improves language skills, communicative competencies and
students’ interest in learning the language (Wang, 2014). This is owing to the fact that
social interaction with NSs occurs in a more informal, more authentic, and more
contextual way and, above all, it is a more natural way of learning a language (Bahrani &
Sim, 2012; Martinsen & Thompson, 2019; Wang, 2014). This is often missing in a
conventional formal brick and mortar classroom settings (Zheng et al., 2009). Through
social interaction with NSs, foreign language learners are also exposed to natural
expressions. In this case, the focus of the communication is on meaning (communication),
rather than on form (grammar). In fact, this is how we acquire our first language. As a
result of this social encounters with NSs, learners’ self-confidence improves. The fact that
native interlocutors could understand the target language being spoken somehow boosts
the learners’ self-confidence in using the language being learned.
Interestingly, students who have the intention to communicate with NESs also
exhibit a higher self-confidence level than those who do not have the intention to embark
on such endeavors. This finding is not unexpected. Needless to say, the desire to engage
in social interaction involving NESs is born out of a high self-confidence level. Language
learners who have a lower self-confidence level tend to avoid participating in simple class
activities (Marlow, 2010), let alone seek NESs to communicate. Note that there is a
significant difference between low proficient and low confident language learners.
Unconfident learners are not necessarily lacking proficiency. However, they tend to be
“pessimistic about everything”, “predict only the bad things” that are going to happen
with their learning and “give up easily because they feel afraid and insecure” (Aguebet,
2017 p. 20). It is due to this feeling of insecurity that learners with a low self-confidence
level tend to avoid, when possible, interacting with NSs and this contention lends support
from this study. What is more, unconfident learners often feel demotivated and,
consequently, do not perform well in their learning (Lu, Chang, & Chen, 2007). Thus, it
is not surprising that those who reported that they had the intention to interact with NESs
also demonstrated an elevated level of self-assurance and the reverse is also true.
One practical implication of this finding is that it is important that teachers of
English as a foreign language encourage their students to interact and communicate with
NESs. One way to do this is to assign learners, including unconfident ones, to chat or
interview NESs as part of the course requirements (as practiced by Cadd, 2012).
However, in doing this, the teacher should ensure that extensive support is particularly
provided for unconfident students to enable them to do the task until they have gradually
improved their level of confidence. In other words, the teacher could provide
‘scaffolding’ to these unconfident students until they can do the task on their own. Making
(2023), 6 (2): 380–393
389
P-ISSN 2623-0356
E-ISSN 2654-5586
a special arrangement with native speakers whom the unconfident students are going to
chat with would be a good idea to begin with.
Finding NESs used to be a difficult task some two or three decades ago, but with
advances in computer and information technology, it is now possible for language
learners to communicate with NESs using computer-mediated communication (Jiang &
Li, 2018; Martinsen & Thompson, 2019; Zhang, 2016). As mentioned earlier, being
involved in social interaction with NESs promotes ‘negotiation of meaning’ between
language learners and NESs, thus improving language proficiency of the learners
(Hedayati et al., 2016; Ueno, 2019). Of course, this social interaction has to be “task-
based”, in that the primary objective of interacting with NESs is to complete a task. A
simple example, as mentioned above, would be to get English language learners interview
NESs about their hobbies, about the food they love, about places they would love to visit,
and so on and so forth using social media (Alberth, 2019; Alberth, Mursalim, Siam,
Suardika, & Ino, 2018; Suardika et al., 2020), and present the results of the interview in
the classroom. The interview can be conducted synchronously or asynchronously
(Alberth, Wang, & Wang, 2019), depending on the arrangement. This communication
with NESs is particularly important since research evidence suggests that more frequent
communication with NSs is associated with improved language proficiency (Dewey et
al., 2012), for both low proficient learners (Jauregi et al., 2012; Satar & Özdener, 2008)
and high proficient learners (Cadd, 2012). What is more, social interaction with native
speakers enables language learners to improve their linguistic and cultural understanding
(Cadd, 2012; Martinsen & Thompson, 2019). Of course, not all attempts to interact with
NSs have a happy ending; some students might encounter problems and get frustrated,
primarily owing to cultural differences (Back, 2009). However, with a good arrangement
and extensive support by the teacher, these inconveniences can be anticipated and avoided
in the first place.
Whereas self-confidence is, at times, discussed with respect to gender, the present
study clearly shows that males and females are equally confident. This is also the case
with year of enrollment. There were no notable distinctions detected in the self-confident
level of students who are in the first, second, and third years, respectively. Thus, neither
gender nor year of enrollment dictates respondents’ self-confident level. It is whether or
not they have previous experience interacting with NESs or whether or not they have the
intention to be involved in social interaction with NESs at some point in the future.
To sum up, due to various benefits that foreign language learners can get from
interacting with NSs as mentioned above, it is just natural to expect that their self-
confidence will also increase along the way. Interestingly, it is not just the students who
reported having the experience interacting with NSs that have a higher level of self-
confidence; students who have never met NSs, but have the intention to communicate
with them at some point in the future, also reported a higher level of self-confidence
compared to those who do not have the intention to do so.
Conclusion
The study shows that EFL learners who had the experience communicating and
interacting with NESs, either online or offline, were significantly more confident than
those who did not. This confirms findings reported in previous studies. Interestingly,
students who reported to have the intention to communicate with NESs were also more
confident in their English language proficiency than those who had no intention at all.
Intuitively, communicating with NSs of the target language requires a certain degree of
(2023), 6 (2): 380–393
390
P-ISSN 2623-0356
E-ISSN 2654-5586
self-confidence. Students with a low self-confidence level would less likely seek the
opportunity to communicate with NSs volitionally. Thus, it is critical that teachers
encourage their students to communicate with NSs of the target language through task-
based projects. This could, for example, be done by assigning the students a task whereby
they need to interview NSs, either online or offline, to complete the task. This may be a
bit hard for low confident learners, but with extensive support provided by the teacher
(and possible classmates), these reluctant students could gradually gain confidence in
their language proficiency and continue to take the utmost benefit of interacting with NSs.
This study also demonstrated that there were no significant differences in the self-
confidence level of males and females. Nor were there any significant differences
detected based on year of enrollment. As a result, it can be inferred that differences in
students’ self-confidence level are dictated by neither gender nor year of enrollment. They
are determined by whether the students have previous experience interacting with NSs or
whether they have the intention to communicate with NSs at some point in the future.
Given the ubiquity of social media these days when having access to NSs is at the
learners’ fingertips, the practical implications of this study on foreign language teaching
and learning become immediately discernible.
Limitations
The findings of this study should be interpreted with caution due to the presence
of certain limitations. First, this study relied on a single item measure for each variable of
interest. Use of a single item measure is often criticized in the literature. However, in
some cases, single item measures may also be acceptable such as the case for overall job
satisfaction (Scarpello & Campbell, 1983; Wanous, Reichers, & Hudy, 1997). The self-
confidence question used in the present study works exactly the same way as the
aforementioned job satisfaction measures. The other question exploring whether or not
respondents have previously communicated with NESs measures self-reported fact and
the single question of this type is generally acceptable in research (Wanous, Reichers, &
Hudy, 1997). Secondly, the respondents of the present study were drawn from a particular
school and context which might have introduced bias in the generalization of the findings
across different populations and research contexts. Thirdly, the present study did not
make any attempt to identify the frequency and intensity of social interaction with NESs.
These variables may also impinge on the degree of self-confidence. Further studies could
explore these issues further. Nonetheless, findings of this study are well in agreement
with those of studies conducted in other research contexts.
References
Abu-Rabia, S. (2004). Teachers' role, learners' gender differences, and FL anxiety among
seventh-grade students studying English as a FL. Educational Psychology, 24(5),
711-721.
Aguebet, C. R. (2017). Investigating the Effects of Self-Confidence on Students’ Speaking
Performance. (Master). UNIVERSITY 8 MAI 1945, Guelma. Retrieved from
https://dspace.univ-guelma.dz/jspui/bitstream/123456789/2688/1/M821.122.pdf
Al-Sibai, D. (2005). L2 anxiety of Saudi female university students enrolled in speaking
courses. Retrieved on July, 26, 2011.
Alberth. (2019). Use of Facebook, students' intrinsic motivation to study writing, writing
self-efficacy and writing performance. Technology, Pedagogy and Education,
28(1), 21-36. doi:10.1080/1475939X.2018.1552892
(2023), 6 (2): 380–393
391
P-ISSN 2623-0356
E-ISSN 2654-5586
Alberth, Mursalim, Siam, Suardika, I. K., & Ino, L. (2018). Social Media as a Conduit
for Teacher Professional Development: Myths, Promises or Realities? TEFLIN,
29(2).
Alberth, Wang, Y., & Wang, W. Y. C. (2019). Fostering foreign language learning
through a telecollaborative social media-based tandem language learning
approach. In International Workshop on Learning Technology for Education in
Cloud (pp. 362-368): Springer.
Alberth, Wiramihardja, E., & Uden, L. (2020). WhatsApp with English language
teaching? Some practical ideas and strategies. International Journal of
Technology Enhanced Learning, 12(3), 262-274.
Archangeli, M. (1999). Study abroad and experiential learning in Salzburg, Austria.
Foreign Language Annals, 32(1), 115-122.
Back, J.-H. (2009). Korean University Students' Progress in Developing Social
Interaction with Native Speakers in the UK. English Language & Literature
Teaching, 15(3), 1-31.
Bahrani, T., & Sim, T. S. (2012). Informal Language Learning Setting: Technology or
Social Interaction? Turkish Online Journal of Educational Technology-TOJET,
11(2), 142-149.
Cadd, M. (2012). Encouraging students to engage with native speakers during study
abroad. Foreign Language Annals, 45(2), 229-245.
Cheng, Y. s., Horwitz, E. K., & Schallert, D. L. (1999). Language anxiety: Differentiating
writing and speaking components. Language Learning, 49(3), 417-446.
Cine, A. (2021). Spēļošanas elementi runāšanas aktivitātēs angļu valodas runātprasmes
veicināšanai 3. klasē.
Cui, J. (2011). Research on High School Students' English Learning Anxiety. Journal of
Language Teaching & Research, 2(4).
de Saint Léger, D., & Storch, N. (2009). Learners’ perceptions and attitudes: Implications
for willingness to communicate in an L2 classroom. System, 37(2), 269-285.
Dewey, D. P., Bown, J., & Eggett, D. (2012). Japanese language proficiency, social
networking, and language use during study abroad: Learners’ perspectives.
Canadian Modern Language Review, 68(2), 111-137.
Dörnyei, Z. (2001). Teaching and researching motivation. London: Longman.
Gul, S., Qazi, Z., & Shakir, Z. (2021). Investigating Gender-based Speech Variations in
the Discourse of Pakistan Air Force Officers. Psychology and Education Journal,
58(2), 9909-9924.
Hastowohadi, H., & Kumaini, R. (2019). Building Efl Students’self-Confidence,
Motivation, And Ability In Listening English 2.0. Journey: Journal of English
Language and Pedagogy, 2(1), 10-21.
Hedayati, M., Nur, S., Emery, S., & Pavlyshyn, H. (2016). Enriching personal growth
and communicative competence through self-organised interaction between
native and non-native speakers. Paper presented at the Australian Association for
Research in Education (AARE) Conference 2016: transforming education
research.
Herzberg, P. Y., & Roth, M. (2006). Beyond resilients, undercontrollers, and
overcontrollers? An extension of personality prototype research. European
Journal of Personality, 20(1), 5-28.
Huang, Y.-W. (2014). Self and language anxiety. English language and literature studies,
4(2), 66.
(2023), 6 (2): 380–393
392
P-ISSN 2623-0356
E-ISSN 2654-5586
Jauregi, K., De Graaff, R., Van den Bergh, H., & Kriz, M. (2012). Native/non-native
speaker interactions through video-web communication: A clue for enhancing
motivation? Computer Assisted Language Learning, 25(1), 1-19.
Jiang, W., & Li, W. (2018). Linking up learners of Chinese with native speakers through
WeChat in an Australian tertiary CFL curriculum. Asian-Pacific Journal of
Second and Foreign Language Education, 3(1), 1-16.
Krashen, S. D. (1985). The input hypothesis: Issues and implications. London: Addison-
Wesley Longman Limited.
Lee, J. S. (2019). EFL students’ views of willingness to communicate in the extramural
digital context. Computer Assisted Language Learning, 32(7), 692-712.
Lu, Y.-T., Chang, C.-W., & Chen, G.-D. (2007). Using a programmable storytelling
robot to motivate learning second language. Paper presented at the Seventh IEEE
International Conference on Advanced Learning Technologies (ICALT 2007).
MacIntyre, P. D., Clément, R., Dörnyei, Z., & Noels, K. A. (1998). Conceptualizing
willingness to communicate in a L2: A situational model of L2 confidence and
affiliation. Modern Language Journal, 82, 545-562.
MacIntyre, P. D., Noels, K. A., & Clément, R. (1997). Biases in self‐ratings of second
language proficiency: The role of language anxiety. Language Learning, 47(2),
265-287.
Marlow, D. W. (2010). Engaging syntax: Using a personal response system to encourage
grammatical thought. American Speech, 85(2), 225-237.
Martinsen, R. A., & Thompson, G. L. (2019). Virtual Conversations: Research and
Practice in Online Language Exchanges. Publicaciones: Facultad de Educación
y Humanidades del Campus de Melilla, 49(2), 77-92.
Molberg, H.-K. K. (2010). The consequence of motivation and linguistic self-confidence
in relation to pupils’ oral interaction. Universitetet i Tromsø,
Peñaloza, A. C., & Salamanca, L. C. (2019). Writing from the Self: Reflecting on the
Role of Creative Writing in the Academic Writing Classroom. ENLETAWA
JOURNAL, 12(2), 97-121.
Piasecka, L. (2014). The Effects of Working on Multi-Word Expressions on the
Development of Lexical Competence: A Case of 25 Students Enrolled in One
Course. In Major Trends in Theoretical and Applied Linguistics 3 (pp. 301-314):
De Gruyter Open Poland.
Sahlan, Alberth, Madil, W., & Hutnisyawati. (2021). The effects of modes of test
administration on test anxiety and test scores Issues in Educational Research,
31(3).
Satar, H. M., & Özdener, N. (2008). The effects of synchronous CMC on speaking
proficiency and anxiety: Text versus voice chat. The Modern Language Journal,
92(4), 595-613.
Scarpello, V., & Campbell, J. P. (1983). Job satisfaction: Are all the parts there?
Personnel psychology, 36(3), 577-600.
Schenker, T. (2017). Synchronous telecollaboration for novice language learners: Effects
on speaking skills and language learning interests. Alsic. Apprentissage des
Langues et Systèmes d'Information et de Communication, 20(2).
Shokri, N. M. (2010). Team project facilitates language learning. Procedia-Social and
Behavioral Sciences, 7, 555-564.
(2023), 6 (2): 380–393
393
P-ISSN 2623-0356
E-ISSN 2654-5586
Stumpf, S., & Muscroft, S. (2011). When users generate music playlists: When words
leave off, music begins? Paper presented at the 2011 IEEE International
Conference on Multimedia and Expo.
Suardika, I. K., Alberth, Mursalim, Siam, Suhartini, L., & Pasassung, N. (2020). Using
WhatsApp for Teaching a Course on the Education Profession: Presence,
Community and Learning. International Journal of Mobile and Blended Learning
(IJMBL), 12(1), 17-32.
Swain, M. (1995). Three functions of output in second language learning. Principles and
practice in applied linguistics: Studies in honor of HG Widdowson, 125-144.
Ueno, J. (2019). Developing autonomy through conversation exchange: A case study of
ESL Learners. TESL-EJ, 22(4), n4.
Wang, Y.-C. (2014). Acquiring the English Language through Web-Based Role-Playing
Simulations: Informal Learning in Interactive Environments. International
Journal of English Language Education, 2(1), 100-110.
Wanous, J. P., Reichers, A. E., & Hudy, M. J. (1997). Overall job satisfaction: how good
are single-item measures? Journal of Applied Psychology, 82(2), 247.
Watkins, S. T. (2016). Did you read my blog?: a case study of blogging in the English
language arts classroom. Rutgers University-Graduate School of Education,
Wu, H. (2019). Reticence in the EFL classroom: voices from students in a Chinese
university. International Journal of Applied Linguistics and English Literature,
8(6), 114-125.
Yashima, T. (2002). Willingness to communicate in a second language: The Japanese
EFL context. The Modern Language Journal, 86(1), 54-66.
Yashima, T., Zenuk‐Nishide, L., & Shimizu, K. (2004). The influence of attitudes and
affect on willingness to communicate and second language communication.
Language Learning, 54(1), 119-152.
Zhang, S. (2016). Learning through a CMC-Based tandem project with native speakers:
A descriptive study of beginning CFL learners. Journal of Technology and
Chinese Language Teaching, 7(2), 58.
Zheng, D., Young, M. F., Brewer, R. A., & Wagner, M. (2009). Attitude and self-efficacy
change: English language learning in virtual worlds. CALICO Journal, 27(1), 205-
231.