ArticlePDF Available

The role of hydrodynamics in collective motions of fish schools and bioinspired underwater robots

The Royal Society
Journal of the Royal Society Interface
Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Collective behaviour defines the lives of many animal species on the Earth. Underwater swarms span several orders of magnitude in size, from coral larvae and krill to tunas and dolphins. Agent-based algorithms have modelled collective movements of animal groups by use of social forces , which approximate the behaviour of individual animals. But details of how swarming individuals interact with the fluid environment are often under-examined. How do fluid forces shape aquatic swarms? How do fish use their flow-sensing capabilities to coordinate with their schooling mates? We propose viewing underwater collective behaviour from the framework of fluid stigmergy , which considers both physical interactions and information transfer in fluid environments. Understanding the role of hydrodynamics in aquatic collectives requires multi-disciplinary efforts across fluid mechanics, biology and biomimetic robotics. To facilitate future collaborations, we synthesize key studies in these fields.
Content may be subject to copyright.
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rsif
Perspective
Cite this article: Ko H, Lauder G, Nagpal R.
2023 The role of hydrodynamics in collective
motions of fish schools and bioinspired
underwater robots. J. R. Soc. Interface 20:
20230357.
https://doi.org/10.1098/rsif.2023.0357
Received: 23 June 2023
Accepted: 2 October 2023
Subject Category:
Life SciencesEngineering interface
Subject Areas:
biomechanics, biomimetics, biophysics
Keywords:
fluid stigmergy, collective behaviour,
fluid mechanics, fish school
Author for correspondence:
Hungtang Ko
e-mail: hk1581@princeton.edu
The role of hydrodynamics in collective
motions of fish schools and bioinspired
underwater robots
Hungtang Ko
1
, George Lauder
2
and Radhika Nagpal
1,3
1
Mechanical and Aerospace Engineering, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, USA
2
Organismic and Evolutionary Biology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA
3
Computer Science, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, USA
HK, 0000-0001-6250-6144; GL, 0000-0003-0731-286X; RN, 0000-0001-9756-0167
Collective behaviour defines the lives of many animal species on the Earth.
Underwater swarms span several orders of magnitude in size, from coral
larvae and krill to tunas and dolphins. Agent-based algorithms have
modelled collective movements of animal groups by use of social forces,
which approximate the behaviour of individual animals. But details of
how swarming individuals interact with the fluid environment are often
under-examined. How do fluid forces shape aquatic swarms? How do fish
use their flow-sensing capabilities to coordinate with their schooling
mates? We propose viewing underwater collective behaviour from the
framework of fluid stigmergy, which considers both physical interactions
and information transfer in fluid environments. Understanding the role
of hydrodynamics in aquatic collectives requires multi-disciplinary
efforts across fluid mechanics, biology and biomimetic robotics. To facilitate
future collaborations, we synthesize key studies in these fields.
1. Introduction
Animal collective behaviour has long been a fascination and inspiration for
humans [1]. Underwater swarms span several orders of magnitude in size,
from clouds of coral larvae on the millimetre scale to pods of dolphins on the
metre scale (figure 1). In groups, animals achieve functions that individuals
cannot achieve, forming a whole greater than the sum of its parts. Coordination
between animals in large groups requires unique strategies [2]. Biological organ-
isms have limited sensing capabilities and individuals do not know the precise
states (location, velocity, orientation, etc.) of other group members. How do
they respond to the sensory information available to them in a way that gives
rise to emergent behaviour on the collective level?
This question has been investigated by the use of agent-based models,
which simulate emergent group behaviour based on assumed rules for individ-
ual agents. Such models often impose a social force that either attracts or repels
an individual to its neighbour based on their distance as if they were connected
via a virtual spring (figure 2). Social forces like this are a crucial element of the
Boids model [3] and the Viscek model [4] as they facilitate generating coherent
movements in simulations. Despite its effectiveness, social force is only a
reduced-order approximation of animal behaviour, and limitations of animals
sensing and locomotion are rarely considered (figure 2). While social force may
be fitted from experiments, there is no guarantee that its form and magnitude
apply to swarms of different sizes, densities and speeds: the social force is itself
an emergent property.
Going beyond such simplifications, the sensory systems of swarming
animals must be considered. Aquatic organisms perceive the world through
synthesizing a wide range of sensory information, including visual,
© 2023 The Authors. Published by the Royal Society under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution
License http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/, which permits unrestricted use, provided the original
author and source are credited.
hydrodynamic, proprioceptive, electric and magnetic cues.
Classical models such as [5,6] were among the first to
consider a limited field of view. Others limited the
number of perceived neighbours [7], or designed agent
behaviours entirely based on vision [8]. The roles of sensing
modes other than vision have rarely been considered in
existing models of swarms. However, many aquatic species
have limited vision or live in dark environments. These
organisms cannot sense a neighbour a few body lengths
away as assumed in vision-based models. They must rely
on other sensory inputs like hydrodynamic cues and use
coordination strategies that are more short-range and
environment-dependent.
A crucial aspect often neglected in fish school models is
the effect of the fluid environment (figure 2). The concept
of environment-mediated coordination dates back more
than 60 years. Stigmergy was initially coined to describe the
collective construction of termite mounds [911]. Instead of
exchanging information explicitly with each other, termites
communicate and coordinate indirectly by leaving and
sensing traces in the environment. A similar principle is
also used by ants to follow trails [12], by paper wasps to
construct nests [13], and by honeybees to locate their
queens [1416]. Recently, stigmergy has been extended
beyond social insects to bacterial colonies [17,18], spatio-
temporal patterns of animal territories [19], cognition
[20,21] and swarm robots [2224].
In this perspective, we propose to view aquatic collective
systems through the lens of fluid stigmergy,whichurges
considerations of the dynamic fluid environment and the
information it carries. How are movements of underwater
collectives affected by fluid forces? How can individuals in
groups use hydrodynamic information for coordination? The
wake behind a fish carries information about the individuals
state, much like the antspheromone trails. In addition,
spatialtemporal features of the vortex street are advected
(a)
(c)(d)
(b)
(e)
Figure 1. Underwater swarms in nature span several orders of magnitude, from (a) dolphins and (b) giant trevally on the metre scale, to (c) squid and (d)mackerel
on the centimetre scale, to (e) coral larvae on the millimetre scale. Images obtained through Education licences from Adobe Stock.
traditional models
sensing
states of
neighbours
muscle
actuation
locomotion
fluid
environment
fluid stigmergy
social
force
models
fluid–agent
interactions
lateral line
systems
other senses
e.g. vision
Figure 2. Different constructs of swarm models. Agent interactions in traditional models depend on relative distance and locations, sometimes considering vision
and a limited field of view. The perspective of fluid stigmergy includes the fluid environment and emphasizes fluidagent interactions and coordination
strategies based on flow-sensing.
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rsif J. R. Soc. Interface 20: 20230357
2
and diffused according to the same principle that transports
airborne pheromones of social insects like honeybees [1416].
This flow information is critical for underwater collective
movements, especially for animals with limited long-range
sensing capabilities. How stigmergy facilitates collective
behaviour likely depends on the scale and the Reynolds
number (Re), which defines the ratio between inertia and vis-
cous effects. For low Re organisms such as bacteria (Re <1),
disturbances diffuse almost instantaneously but may be
sensed from afar. For larger Re swimmers such as schooling
fish (Re =10
4
10
6
), vortices retain their structures longer but
their influence is short-ranged. In this regime, while a swim-
ming fish is difficult to detect a few body lengths away, its
follower who swims through vortices may obtain rich infor-
mation about its states. Indeed, it has been demonstrated
that with only hydrodynamic information, a blindfolded fish
can school with its neighbours [25]. The relative role of
vision and flow sensing depends on the species and their
native habitats. But fluid stigmergy (figure 2) is likely used, at
least partially, by all schooling species.
Compounding the complexity of underwater coordi-
nation are the physical fluidagent interactions. Passive
agents such as dead fish or granular materials can exhibit
non-intuitive behaviours under fluid forces. Just as hydro-
dynamic interactions can influence the collective behaviour
of microscopic organisms [26], repulsion and attraction
between a pair of fish can emerge in the absence of social
interactions and social forces [27,28]. In the wake behind an
obstacle, a dead fish can undulate its body and keep up
with the flow as if it were alive and swimming [29]. Recently,
it was shown that rheotaxic behaviour may not require any
sensory feedback either [30]. These hydrodynamic factors
led to the classical conjecture posed by Weihs & Lighthill,
which stated that a diamond formation of fish schools is opti-
mal because it would lead to constructive vortex interactions
and allow schooling individuals to save energy [31,32].
In addition, Lighthill surmised that such a formation is
stableif a fish deviates from the diamond formation, fluid
forces will push it back to its original position. Whether a dia-
mond formation is truly hydrodynamically optimal and
whether fish schools in nature prefer such a formation
remains an open question. However, the key notion stands:
hydrodynamic interactions could lead to self-organizing
effects in spite of social interactions. Coupling with these
effects, movements of active agents can give rise to
unexpected collective behaviours [33].
Fluidagent interactions affect not only natural swarms but
also underwater robot collectives. Recently, swarm robotics
have received increasing interest because they benefit from
low cost, versatility and robustness [34,35]. Most current
robot swarms are controlled using social force models.
Taking advantage of technologies such as WiFi, Bluetooth
and centralized tracking systems, terrestrial robots can per-
ceive neighbourslocations better than biological organisms,
making coordination simpler. However, underwater robots
do not enjoy the same benefits. Electromagnetic waves attenu-
ate rapidly and sonar suffers from low bandwidth and high
latency [36]. As such, low-cost and low-power underwater
swarms must rely on visual and hydrodynamic cues much
like schooling fish. Recently, a vision-based underwater robot
swarm has been demonstrated to achieve collective behaviours
such as milling [37]. Future designs that include a flow sensing
system may enable robot swarms to save energy by taking
advantage of the fluidagent interactions and using fluid
stigmergy for coordination.
In this review, we discuss findings and methods in fluid
mechanics, biology and robotics with the perspective of
fluid stigmergy. We limit our scope to underwater systems
since most aerial animals such as starlings possess acute
vision and unmanned aerial vehicles often rely on wireless
communication. In the next section, we start by examining
the fluid field around fish schools, discussing what hydro-
dynamic information a swimming fish leaves behind, and
how swarming individuals may interact with each other
through fluid forces. In §3, we discuss what is known
about how fish sense their fluid environment. In §4, we
review robotic platforms with flow-sensing capabilities and
summarize what behaviours flow-sensing enables. Finally,
in §5, we highlight key challenges and opportunities for
understanding underwater systems in the future.
2. Understanding fluid flow
To start, we must understand the fluid mechanics of fish
schools. What is the flow signature a fish leaves behind?
How is that information preserved and propagated to
another fish nearby? What schooling formations are hydro-
dynamically efficient and stable? The complex interaction
between fish schools and water flow has been an ongoing
research topic within the fluid mechanics community, and
progress has been made using both experiments and
simulations [3841,42, ch. 3].
When a fish swims through the water, it leaves two rows
of vortices behind (figure 3a). Each vortex forms as the tail
swings from one side to the other and sheds when it reverses
course. Vortices alternate in their direction (clockwise versus
counterclockwise). As they travel downstream, vortices dissi-
pate energy and break down into smaller eddies. The rate of
energy dissipation and the angle of the vortex street depends
on various factors such as the swimming velocity and the
flow regime (turbulent versus laminar). The width and wave-
length of the wake decrease as the tail-beat frequency
increases and smaller vortices develop when the frequency
is low [46]. The flow signature behind a swimming fish has
a strong frequency component. Recently, it has been shown
that fluctuations of the fluid field preserve information
about a fishs relative position, phase differences and tail-
beat frequency [44].
The simplest way to generate a wake is by placing an
obstacle in a current. The flow over a fixed blunt object
produces alternating vortices, called the Kármán vortex street
[47](figure 3a). This flow pattern can be found behind pillars
under bridges or behind rocks in rivers. The rotation directions
of the vortices in Kármán vortex street are the opposite of
those in a fishs wake. Therefore, the wake of a fish is also
referred to as the reverse Kármán vortex street. Fundamentally,
the arrangement of the wake vortices relates to whether the
object is producing drag (pillars) or thrust (fish) [48]. A key
parameter that determines the force balance is Strouhal
number, St =fL/U, or frequency fnon-dimensionalized by
the length scale Land flow velocity U. The emergent vortex-
shedding frequency of a Kármán vortex street behind a cylin-
der results in St 0.20.3. Remarkably, for fish swimming at
higher speeds, St is also in this same range [38,39,41,49].
Now defined with tail-beat amplitude as the length scale
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rsif J. R. Soc. Interface 20: 20230357
3
and swimming speed as the velocity scale, the non-dimen-
sional tail-beat frequency St 0.3, with most aquatic species
ranging narrowly within 0.20.4 [38,39,41,49]. This relationship
can be used to estimate an undulatory swimmers speed based
on its beating amplitude and frequency.
Fish wakes can be further abstracted using the potential
flow theory in fluid mechanics [50]. Flow around a cylinder
can be expressed analytically as a dipole. Since the theory
neglects viscous effects, the flow around a dipole does not
shed periodic vortices and only approximates the time-
averaged fluid field (figure 3a). To better mimic the wake
behind a fish, in both experiments and simulations, research-
ers use oscillating spheres and dipoles. However, even in
such a simplified setting, complexity quickly arises when
there is more than one body in the fluid domain. Placing a
sensor or a fish body (dead or alive) will instantaneously
change the flow field, including the structure of the vortex
street. Indeed, the hydrodynamic interactions between a
fish body and its own fins can already lead to non-intuitive
results [51,52]. To study what a fish senses from its neigh-
bours wake, it is crucial to include multiple fish in the
fluid field.
The hydrodynamics of fish schools remain an active
area of research. The elegant conjecture that Weihs and
Lighthill proposed about the diamond formation (figure 3b)
has been challenged with animal experiments [5355], and
numerical simulations [27,56,57]. Weihsargument for a dia-
mond pattern is based upon constructive vortex interaction in
directions both parallel and perpendicular to the swimming
directions, assuming the formation remains unchanged.
However, a recent study suggested that the constructive
fluidstructure interaction is more reliable in the fishs lateral
direction [56]. Therefore, the side-by-side phalanxformation
is more efficient than the diamond formation (figure 3b). This
claim is supported by experiments of schools of red nose tetra
Hemigrammus bleheri, who form increasingly one-dimensional
patterns as swimming speed increases. This phalanx for-
mation allowed the tetras to reach faster swimming speeds
with lower tail-beat frequencies [55,58]. However, exper-
iments with five species of rainbowfish (Melanotaenia)
arrived at a conflicting conclusion, showing more alignment
along the swimming direction (figure 3b) as their speed
increases [59]. To add to the puzzle, using two-dimensional
simulations with periodic boundaries, research suggests
that hydrodynamic benefits are easier to obtain than pre-
viously thought [57]. Fish schools are more efficient than
individuals regardless of the formations (diamond, rectangu-
lar, side-by-side and inline) [57]. It is unclear if and how the
preferred schooling formations depend on species, especially
regarding the relative importance of the senses (hydro-
dynamic versus vision) for coordination. Perhaps more
critically, whether fish can maintain fixed formations for pro-
longed periods at all is questionable. Both early literature and
empirical observations suggest that fish school formations are
transient and dynamic [60].
How do fluid forces push fish in and out of transient for-
mations? Investigating the hydrodynamic stability of various
formations has gained considerable interest. For a pair of
hydrofoils in tandem with a constant frequency, amplitude
and spacing, two stable swimming speeds exist [61]. The
two emergent modes, fast and slow modes, correspond to
when the flappers are out-of-phase and in-phase, respectively.
If the spacing is allowed to vary spontaneously, configurations
with synchronized phases (slow modes) are always preferred
and are hydrodynamically stablefluid force will restore
their states if perturbed [62]. As more degrees of freedom are
added, a stability diagram may be obtained [63]. When the
frequencies of the two hydrofoils are matched, stable configur-
ations exist for a range of heaving amplitudes. Intriguingly,
an oscillatory mode also exists in the regime where the follower
streamlines around a dipole
Kármán vortex street
wake behind a fish
diamond
inline
phalanx
(a)
(d)(e)
(b)
(c)
Figure 3. (a) Fluid fields behind a swimming fish, behind an obstacle, and around a dipole. Red vortices rotate clockwise while blue vortices rotate counterclock-
wise. (b) Hydrodynamic arguments support either a diamond formation, a phalanx formation or an inline formation. (c) The three-dimensional fluid field around a
swimming fish can be characterized by tomographic PIV (permission from [43]), and hydrodynamic interactions between a pair of fish can be studied using (d) fluid
simulations (permission from [44]) and (e) hydrofoil experiments (permission from [45]).
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rsif J. R. Soc. Interface 20: 20230357
4
heaves wider but less frequently. In this mode, fluid forces
orchestrate the periodic movement of the follower, at times
closing the gap and at others failing to keep up. These beha-
viours have been observed in numerical simulations where
the swimmers deform their bodies like fish and are allowed
to rotate [64,65]. Oscillatory modes have also been found for
two simulated fish starting from side-by-side formations [66].
For up to four swimmers, researchers have discovered more
than a dozen equilibrium formations, including the diamond
formation that Weihs suggested [67]. However, simulations
in [67] are two-dimensional, and swimmers can only move
freely along the streamwise direction. Recent studies using
three-dimensional simulations [27] and hydrofoils free to
move in both directions [28]showedthatveryfewformations
are truly stable.
These studies of hydrodynamic performance and stability
of fish schools have generated considerable insight into
schooling strategies. The fact that stable equilibrium states
are rare suggests a stabilitymanoeuvrability trade-off
similar to bird flight [68,69]: if fish are attracted strongly to
equilibrium positions by fluid forces, they will lose the
ability to change directions rapidly. However, while a bird
can engineer the stability landscape by changing its body
conformation and movement frequency, the stability land-
scape of a fish is determined by the movements of its
schooling mates! The scarcity of stable formations also
suggests that realistic formations of underwater swarms
must be ever-changing (unless deliberate efforts and costs
are paid to maintain them). This, however, does not rule
out the possibility of identifying any patterns in transient
schools since dynamic oscillatory modes are also likely.
To study the hydrodynamics of fish schools, a wide range
of experimental and numerical tools may be adopted. Particle
image velocimetry (PIV) is widely used in experiments to
characterize fluid flows around live fish [7073]. The technique
has been extended to study the three-dimensional flow field
around individual swimming fish (figure 3c)[43,7476].
PIV has also been applied to multiple interacting hydro-
foils (figure 3d)[28,45,61,62]. Numerically, researchers
have used methods such as direct numerical simulations
with immersed boundaries (figure 3e)[27,44,56,66,67,7779],
multi-particle collision dynamics model [57], and potential
flow based solvers [50,80]. However, most existing simulations
prescribe the movements of the fish bodies, and the force
balance around a fish is not always strictly enforced. As
these computational techniques advance, realistic simulations
with multiple interacting individuals may soon be achieved.
Further, interfacing fluid simulations with agents that can
actively sense and react to their fluid surroundings is a fruitful
direction for future work.
3. Flow-sensing in fish schools
The sense of touch (mechano-sensation) is universal for ani-
mals both on land and underwater, but the form of
mechanical forces differs significantly. Aquatic animals
adopt unique strategies to sense fluid flows around them
and can sense touch at a distance[42]. Their mechano-
sensory organ, the lateral line system, senses perturbations
across a wide range of frequencies, from steady currents
to acoustic waves. Lateral line systems have attracted signifi-
cant research since the seventeenth century and have
been reviewed excellently by Coombs and co-workers
[42,81]. Here, we focus on the mechanical properties of lateral
line systems as they relate to the function of sensing fluid
movement and their role in implicit coordination.
Lateral line systems can be divided into two subgroups:
superficial neuromasts (SN) and canal neuromasts (CN)
(figure 4a). They consist of dome-shaped epithelial structures
(cupula) covering hair cells that deflect under fluid flows.
Both types of neuromasts sense fluid shear to infer flow
intensity. SNs protrude from the surface and directly sense
the flow velocity in their vicinity. CNs sense fluid flow
within the lateral line canal. Since the flow in the canal is
dominated by viscous effects, its speed is proportional to
the pressure difference between pores that are connected
to the exterior environment. Therefore, CNs effectively
sense the pressure drop in the external flow. SNs often
locate in proximity to CNs [83], and canals can be found
both on the head (cranial canals) and along the body
of a fish (trunk canal ). Canals are denser on the head, sur-
rounding the eye and extending from the cheek to the jaw
(figure 4b), providing hydrodynamic velocity information
from all directions. The difference among those signals may
be used to infer the direction of flow or to locate prey
[82,83]. With only two types of fluid sensors available to
them (SNs and CNs), how do fish arrange them to maximize
their sensing capabilities? The placement and relative abun-
dance of SNs and CNs are extremely diverse (figure 4b)[42,
ch. 2; 81, ch. 10; 8284], but it has been noted that fish
living in fast and turbulent waters tend to have only a few
or no SNs [82]. This trend may be explained by considering
the boundary layer that forms around the body of a fish
(figure 4a)[73,8587]. The boundary layer is thinner for fast
and turbulent flows. In these environments, the boundary
layer can shrink to thicknesses comparable to the size of neu-
romasts, and the protrusion of SNs would significantly alter
the flow. Sheltered by the canal, the performance of CNs is
less affected by boundary layers in fast currents.
Hydrodynamic information produced by a moving fish is
intrinsically periodic. While a single tail beat only creates
small disturbances that dissipate rapidly with distance and
time, periodic tail beats produce pressure waves that can
travel much farther than the scale of the animal. Indeed,
swimming fish create acoustic waves! It is unclear if such a
wave is strong enough to be distinguished from background
ocean noise, but research has long shown that fish can use lat-
eral line systems to sense low-frequency vibrations [42]. In
fact, during most of the nineteenth and twentieth centuries,
lateral lines were considered auditory organs, and researchers
have investigated how neuromasts respond to oscillatory
flow [42]. Like trees and buildings, neuromasts have natural
frequencies, and their oscillatory movements depend on iner-
tia and rigidity. As a result, signals within a certain frequency
range are amplified, while others are attenuated (figure 4c).
Cave-dwelling populations of Mexican cavefish (Astyanax
mexicanus) have taller SNs and are more responsive to
lower frequency stimuli compared with stream-dwelling
populations that possess functional visual systems [88].
Fluid properties also affect neuromastsfrequency response.
As the perturbation frequency increases, inertial effects
begin to dominate over viscous effects, and boundary
layers form. This can be explained by Stokess second pro-
blem, which suggests that the fluid shear next to SNs
increases with the square root of frequency, ffiffi
f
p. For CNs,
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rsif J. R. Soc. Interface 20: 20230357
5
the channel flow profile also depends on the frequency,
adding a third layer of filtering. Astonishingly, the combi-
nations of frequency filtering result in a constant sensitivity
to flow velocity in SNs of zebrafish (Danio rerio)[89] and a
constant sensitivity to flow acceleration in CNs of ruffe (Gym-
nocephalus cerua)[90] for a wide range of frequencies. This
indicates that SNs and CNs are indeed suitable for sensing
velocity and pressure, respectively.
Flow sensing of the lateral line systems enables beha-
viours such as prey detection, obstacle avoidance and
rheotaxis [81, ch. 3]. Early studies focused primarily on
how lateral line systems sense perturbations from small
prey. Nocturnal predators such as the Lake Michigan mottled
sculpin (Cottus bairdi) rely heavily if not exclusively on the lat-
eral line [91,92]. Species that typically use vision to locate
prey, such as bluegill sunfish (Lepomis macrochirus)[93], large-
mouth bass [94] and blacktip sharks [95] can still detect prey
without vision as long as the prey is within half a body length
away. Surface-feeding fish use lateral line systems to detect
the pressure variation caused by prey at the water surface.
Blind cavefish Astyanax mexicanus rely on hydrodynamic sen-
sing to follow walls [96,97], avoid obstacles [72] and detect
prey [88]. Sensing flow allows fish to hold their position
either in free streams as they orient towards the incoming
flow [98,99] or in regions behind an obstacle [100102].
More recent studies have focused on the role of hydro-
dynamic sensing in dynamic manoeuvres. Kármán gaiting
describes a synchronized movement between a fish and the
vortex street behind an obstacle drag wake [103,104].
In such a gait, fish slalom in between the shed vortices to
take advantage of the propulsion created by the fluid
pressure gradient. This behaviour is only possible when
fish can access the local hydrodynamic information [100].
Sensing vortex streets also allows European catfish (Silurus
glanis) to follow the hydrodynamic trail of their prey
[105,106]. Recently, vortex-matching has been identified as a
coordination strategy for fish in pairs [46,107,108]. Surpris-
ingly, it was concluded that neither vision nor lateral line
systems are required for such behaviour [108], suggesting
that it can arise from hydrodynamic interactions alone.
Only a few studies exist on the role of hydrodynamic sen-
sing during schooling. Classical experiments were conducted
in the late 1970s. Pitcher & Patridge investigated how a fish in
a school behaves differently when it is deprived of vision or
fluid sensing capabilities [25,109]. They showed that a blind-
folded fish can still school, but with a different structure
[109]. When vision is deprived, the fish prefer to stay closer
to their neighbours. They concluded that social attraction,
i.e. the active strategy to cohere as a group, is mediated by
vision, whereas collision avoidance is mediated by lateral
line information. Recent studies on firehead tetras (Hemigram-
mus bleheri), yellow-eyed mullets (Aldrichetta forsteri) and
giant danios (Devario aequipinnatus) also showed that lateral
line ablation disrupts schooling [110112]. Without lateral
line information, fish align less and collide more with other
fish [110,111]. More recently, researchers demonstrated that
while vision is sufficient for schooling in rummy-nose tetras
(Hemigrammus rhodostomus) as they burst and coast in still
water [113,114], the lateral line systems in giant danio (Devario
aequipinnatus) is crucial for tail synchronization [115]. Exper-
imental evidence also demonstrated that giant danio
(Devario aequipinnatus) can form coherent schools in a dark
room [116] and coordinate with a flapping hydrofoil or a
robot that produces fish-like wakes [117,118]. It appears
unquestionable that a certain level of schooling can be
achieved in the absence of visual cues.
In addition to vision, inner ear hair cells sense the
acceleration of the animal and the proprioceptors in muscles
and connective tissues detect body deformation and fin
deflection. These sensory modes provide supplemental
information on the fluid environment that fish may use
for coordination. Future animal experiments are required
(a)
(b)
SN
CN
(c)fluid filtering structural resonance
superfacial
canal
low freq. high freq.
low freq. natural freq.
frequency
frequency
deflection/local flow velocity
local flow velocity/freestream velocity
frequency
deflection/freestream velocity
high freq.
CN
SN ~f2
const. response to acceleration
const. response to velocity
Figure 4. (a) Superficial neuromasts (SN, red) and canal neuromasts (CN, blue) and their relative relationship with the boundary layer flow. Grey arrows indicate
fluid velocity. (b) Interspecies diversity in trunk canal placements (adapted from [82]). (c) Frequency response of SN and CNs (adapted from [42]).
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rsif J. R. Soc. Interface 20: 20230357
6
to elucidate the role of lateral line systems in schooling
behaviour.
4. Flow-sensing in underwater robots
Inspired by lateral line systems, engineers have developed var-
ious electronic platforms that sense flows. These mechanical
systems provide powerful tools to study fluid-mediated coordi-
nation in both natural and artificial systems. Fluid flow around
fish schools depends only on the boundary conditions. There-
fore, by mimicking the shape and movement of fish, a robot
can reproduce the same wake, and sensors placed at locations
of real fishs lateral line systems will receive the same hydrodyn-
amic information a fish would access. Hypotheses about how
fish coordinate with each other can then betested. Furthermore,
incorporating flow sensing into underwater robot swarms
allows artificial systems to adopt fish behaviour such as
rheotaxis, obstacle avoidance and coordination.
On the smallest scale, lateral line systems inspire the
designs of miniature flow sensors called ALL (artificial lateral
line) sensors [119122]. Taking advantage of recent advances
in MEMS and NEMS (micro- and nano-electro-mechanical
systems) manufacturing, new designs of flow sensors mimic
structures of SNs and CNs and often include a single or an
array of micro-pillars. Micro-pillars bend under fluid flow
much like SNs, and piezoresistive, piezoelectric, capacitive
or optical components can be used to convert mechanical
deformation into electrical signals [120]. Since the same
mechanism was used, these SN-mimicking sensors also
respond differently to stimuli at different frequencies. In the
past few years, there has been an increasing interest in devel-
oping CN-inspired sensors that measure the pressure
difference between canal outlets [40]. Like in fish, artificial
CNs are more suitable compared to artificial SNs when the
boundary layer is thin. Protected by microfluidic channels,
CN-inspired sensors are also less prone to damage. While
ALL sensors closely mimic the mechanisms of lateral line
systems and are customizable, they are difficult to manufac-
ture and often require additional tuning and calibration.
Therefore, most existing designs of autonomous robots use
commercial flow sensors instead of ALL.
Beyond bio-mimicry, robotics has emerged as a powerful
tool for generating and testing biologically relevant hypotheses.
BlueSwarm is the first autonomous underwater robot swarm
capable of decentralized coordination (figure 5a)[37]. It demon-
strates that robot fish can display milling behaviour by using
vision alone. Nevertheless, the robots are limited to still
waterand slow speeds, where flow sensing may play a less sig-
nificant role. One of the earliest robot fish with hydrodynamic
sensing capabilities is the prototype under project FILOSE
(Robotic FIsh LOcomotion and SEnsing) [123,127130]. In
2011, Kruusmaa et al. reported how a robot fish could use one
single pressure sensor to control its tail-beat frequency to keep
up with the flow speed in the water channel [123](figure 5b).
By incorporating a controller inspired by the Braitenberg
vehicle, this robot can perform rheotaxis by sensingthe pressure
difference between the two sides of its head [128]. With all
five pressure sensors activated, the robot fish can differentiate
between the flow behind an obstacle, i.e. Kármán vortex
street, and an unobstructed laminar flow. In both cases, it is
capable of exhibiting station-holding behaviour using
hydrodynamic sensory information [130].
Sensing robots without fish-like beating tails benefited from a
higher signal-to-noise ratio and enabled theoretical analyses.
Researchers have increased the number of pressure sensors
from 9 [124,131], 11 [132,133]to20[134](figure 5c), making
them effective flow sensing probes for measurements in the
field [132,133]. The robotic platforms developed in [135137]
also lack actuated tails. Using a pairof Joukowski foils constrained
to planar translation and pitching, researchers implement
model-based control for the follower to perform Kármán gaiting
[137]. Untethered robots can also benefit from sensing the fluid
environment. Flow sensing enables free-swimming robots
to estimate the robots own swimming speed and direction
[138,139] and to follow the wall like a cavefish [140,141]. An eel-
like underwater robot has been developed to coordinate its
body undulations based on hydrodynamic feedback [142].
Flow sensing has also been used in robots to gather infor-
mation about their neighbours. For a pair of robots towed in a
leader follower
flow sensors
10.4 cm
(a)(c)
(d)
(b)
(e)
12
3
4
5
Figure 5. Underwater robots with onboard sensing capabilities. (a) Vision-based coordination in BlueSwarms, reproduced with permission from [37]. (be) Indi-
vidual robots with flow sensors (permission from [123] for b, and [124] for c). (d,e) Robots that use flow-sensing to detect their neighbours (permission from [125]
for d, and [126] for e).
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rsif J. R. Soc. Interface 20: 20230357
7
flume, the follower robot can use the sensed hydrodynamic
pressure variations to estimate the states of the leader
robot, including its relative location, orientation, oscillating
frequency, amplitude, etc. (figure 5d)[125,143]. A free-
swimming robot can also locate its leader (figure 5e)[126].
The estimation of the neighbours location is more accurate
when the neighbour is in front of the follower compared to
when the neighbour is beside or behind the robot [126].
These studies have suggested that hydrodynamic information
can indeed be used for coordination. Furthermore, they show
the potential of using robots as a powerful tool for extracting
hydrodynamic information and using it to achieve fish-like
behaviour. However, free-swimming robots are rare and
most studies are limited to only two sensing robots.
Adding artificial lateral lines to platforms like BlueSwarms
[37] remains an exciting future direction.
5. Challenges and opportunities
Understanding underwater collectives requires knowledge
from various disciplines. Research using live animals and
robots presents two distinct philosophies for tackling
complexity. In animal research, it is often difficult to control
and isolate variables of interest, leading to an often messy
but realistic picture. On the other hand, a biomimetic
approach builds the complex system from the ground up
and is excellent for studying each contributing factor in
isolation. However, constructing these underwater systems
is challenging and the fidelity of bio-mimicry is always com-
promised to some extent. Future studies that join different
approaches would reveal novel insights into underwater
collectives (figure 6).
5.1. Building realistic models of swarms
Agent-based models are critical for exploring and under-
standing collective phenomena, but the dynamics of the
fluid environment has often been ignored. While this may
be valid for vision-dominated animals such as starlings
[144147], underwater organisms have to interact with
much larger fluid forces and the visibility of their environ-
ment is often lower. To investigate underwater systems, it is
essential to couple agent-based models with computational
fluid dynamic simulations (figure 6a). Such a coupled
model will capture the realistic interaction between agents
and their fluid environments: agentsmovements disturb
the fluid field, and the fluid in turn propels the agents. Incor-
porating fluid field simulations also creates the opportunity
to model agentsbehavioural rules based on their fluid sur-
roundings. Such sensory-based multi-agent models will
enable studies of how agents react to hydrodynamic cues,
and how variations of this feedback mechanism affect the
cohesiveness of the school (figure 6b).
The most glaring challenge with coupling collective
behaviour to a flow solver is the computational cost. Current
advances in computational fluid dynamics allow resolutions
of fine details in flow structures. However, high-fidelity
algorithms require a considerable amount of computational
resources and time. One direct numerical simulation of a
fish school swimming for a few seconds can take weeks to
complete on a computer cluster. To reduce the computational
cost, most existing fluid simulations of fish schools prescribe
the fishs movements and ignore the force balance around
each individual and Newtons second law. If the prescribed
conditions such as the formation, tail-beat amplitude and
frequency are inherently unstable, the formation cannot
be sustained and any solutions reached would not be
representative of biological collectives.
To ensure a force balance around individual agents while
keeping computational cost low, the fidelity of the simulations
must be compromised. This can be done by using potential
flow theories [50,148]. However, since agents under these
assumptions do not leave periodic wakes, the treatment is
only applicable for sparse underwater collectives. An alterna-
tive method is by directly modelling the vortex street behind
a two-dimensional swimmer [149]. In addition, adaptive
mesh refinement and simplified agents have been used to con-
duct a parameter study of insect swarms under airflow [33].
Another promising approach that does not require significant
sacrifice in flow field accuracy is to prescribe the trajectories
theory
pressure
explore agent rules based on
hydrodynamic and visual information
school cohesiveness
agents’ reactivity to information
(a)
(b)
biology
characterize large three-dimensional
schools of freshwater and marine species
tracks of individuals
time
probability in diamond formation
time
(c)
(d)
robotics
design flexible autonomous fish-like
robots that can sense flow in fish schools
time
sensed pressure
in school
free swimming
(e)
(f)
Figure 6. Future directions and goals for understanding underwater collectives using (a,b) theoretical, (c,d) biological and (e,f) robotic approaches.
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rsif J. R. Soc. Interface 20: 20230357
8
and kinematics tracked from animal experiments. This
approach will ensure that the simulation satisfies physical con-
straints automatically. The force balance around individuals in
the simulation will be left as a check for numerical accuracy.
Striking a balance between the computational cost associated
with fluid simulation and reality (deformable body, uncon-
strained movement, large number of individuals, behavioural
complexity) is critical for future research.
5.2. Characterizing collective movements through
experiments
One fundamental challenge of studying fish schools is
the difficulty of conducting animal experiments. Keeping,
experimenting with, and tracking fish schools in three dimen-
sions even in a laboratory environment requires expertise in
both biology and computer vision (figure 6c). Most existing
experiments used small freshwater fish (a few centimetres
in length) in shallow aquaria, forcing formations to be
planer which made tracking manageable [55,150]. While
much progress has been made in animal pose tracking algor-
ithms using approaches such as contrast-based segmentation
[151153] and machine learning [154156], their performance
in tracking dense fish schools remains less than ideal. Using
one of the most popular tools in multi-animal tracking
in DeepLabCut, tracking near planar shoals of silversides
Menidia beryllina yields at best 70% mean average precision
(mAP) [155]. For schools in three dimensions, fish frequently
overlap with each other in any given video view. Isolating
individuals from overlapping clusters requires ingenious
solutions. Future tracking algorithms may benefit from
using multiple cameras and stereo vision to simultaneously
acquire a depth map from the camera [52,115], or from incor-
porating knowledge of fish swimming kinematics which have
been reported to be generally similar across a diversity of
fish families [157]. The recent development of large models
for general computer vision such as [158] also provides
tremendous opportunities for three-dimensional tracking
fish schools.
To gain a deeper understanding of underwater collective
behaviour, studies of a wider range of species are also critical
[159]. Our knowledge of marine species that migrate in
enormous groups, such as tuna and sardines, remains limited.
Field experiments and novel approaches are essential. Using
a towed net, a recent comparative study examined the swarm-
ing patterns of California market squid (Doryteuthis opalescens)
and Pacific sardine (Sardinops sagax), which have distinct pro-
pulsive mechanisms [160]. Using rectified videos, another
team of researchers measured the duration and size of fish
shoal disturbances [161]. Adaptive resolution imaging sonar
(ARIS) [162,163] and drone footage [164167] have also
emerged as powerful tools in identifying schooling structures
in the field.
Studies of underwater swarms can also benefit from rig-
orous characterizations of their spatialtemporal features.
Many influential studies that shaped our understanding of
schooling formations use experiments that are based on
trials under a minute long [55,59]. At this timescale, the
habituation of the animals and the initialization of flow
field may both affect the schoolsstructures. Recordings
that are around an hour long allowed researchers to apply
statistical methods and analyse the state of the school
[150,168]. Even longer recordings that are multiple hours or
even days long are necessary to fully characterize fish
school formations and answer fundamental research ques-
tions: How frequent are fish schools in certain formations
(figure 6d)? How stable are these formations? It will be inter-
esting to see if other patterns of coherent movement will
emerge from systematic studies like these.
Tracking fish schools provides a crucial step toward under-
standing their collective behaviour. Since the flow field
depends only on the movement of the school, it can be recon-
structed using fluid simulations or robotic experiments once
the tracks are obtained. Through such hybrid approaches,
one can obtain detailed flow information such as the fluid
stress and pressure field next to each individuals lateral line
systems without directly experimenting with the animals. In
addition, other sensory information such as vision and
proprioception may also be inferred. Without further measure-
ments, it will be tangible to derive a mapping from what a fish
may sense to its behaviour. Note that both the input and
output spaces of this mapping are large and the relationship
may be nonlinear. Searching for low-dimensional structures
from high-dimensional data is a recurring theme in biophysics
[169]. Dimension-reduction techniques such as principal
component analysis and proper orthogonal decomposition
may be used [170,171]. Statistical methods such as correlation
functions and transfer entropy may also be applied [117,172].
Furthermore, future research may adopt cutting-edge
model-discovery approaches such as sparse identification of
nonlinear dynamical systems (SINDy) [173] and knowledge-
based neural ODE (KNODE) [174,175]toinferthebehavioural
strategy of schooling fish. Application of these methods
on thousands of tracks in a large school is an exciting
future direction.
5.3. Designing flow-sensing robot collectives
Bioinspired robots enable rigorous studies of hydrodynamic
effects on underwater swimmers, isolating factors related
to animal behaviour. While tethered robots and hydrofoils
provide better control for experimental work, they prevent
natural movements that arise from hydrodynamic inter-
actions. Air bearing systems allow a tethered object to move
freely in two dimensions, but movements in the other four
degrees of freedom are constrained. Critically, the pitch
angles of hydrofoils are often controlled. In fact, rigid hydro-
foils can only generate thrust by pitching, which is driven by
the motor it is attached to. This is distinct from fish, who gen-
erate thrust by contracting their muscles and deforming their
body. In the absence of external control, free-swimming
robots have to generate thrust through deformation and
thus move more like real fish [37,176178]. It has also been
discovered that tuning flexibility is essential to match the kin-
ematics of fish [177,179183]. Future studies that deploy
groups of autonomous free-swimming robots may lead to
discoveries of metastable schooling configurations that
cannot be observed with tethered hydrofoils.
Bioinspired robots also provide unique opportunities to
peek into the sensory world of biological organisms. If the
morphology and kinematics of fish are mimicked, a fish
robot in a swarm will sense the same fluid pressure and
shear that a fish in such a school must sense. While electronic
sensors may differ from neuromasts in their sensitivity and
range, the hydrodynamic information accessible to them is
identical. Adding arrays of flow sensors to systems like
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rsif J. R. Soc. Interface 20: 20230357
9
BlueSwarm [37] would reveal detailed information about the
flow field around the fish robot. Hybrid experiments that
swim fish robots among living fish school will provide
insights into the roles of sensing in coordination and school
formation control (figure 6e,f). With the added flow-sensing
capabilities and knowledge of fishs hydrodynamic feedback
algorithm, future robots may be able to perform vortex
matching and increase energy efficiency. Development of
miniature artificial lateral line sensors that are scalable and
easily deployable will also significantly advance our under-
standing of the hydrodynamic information embedded in
the environment.
6. Concluding remarks
Fish schools are mesmerizing. They have inspired people
from all walks of life and are an excellent example of collec-
tive behaviour. They are also easy to observe; most pet stores
sell dozens of tropical species that readily school in aquaria.
Despite the ubiquity, it remains mysterious how they coordi-
nate with each other so well and how fluid environment
dictates their collective behaviour.
To some, the mystery may seem to have been solved
when the Vicsek and Boids models showed that by keeping
a certain distance from their neighbour, a flock of agents
could avoid obstacles and move cohesively. Nonetheless,
these models overlooked limitations of sensing in biological
organisms. The range, resolution and speed of information
acquisition are all limited; an animal cannot perfectly esti-
mate its neighbourslocations and their distances from it.
This gap in our understanding of swarms becomes glaring
as roboticists struggle to translate the social force models to
robotsrobots also have limited sensory capabilities.
In bridging the gap, interdisciplinary collaboration is
essential. Fluid mechanics researchers have long pondered
the fluidstructure interactions of hydrofoils; biologists have
meticulously described the morphology and sensing mechan-
ism of lateral line systems; roboticists can now assemble fleets
of underwater vehicles. To gain deeper insights into under-
water coordination, we must gather a diverse team of
experts from different backgrounds.
Finally, we look forward to applications of fluid stigmergy
in other collective systems where signals are transported by
dynamic fluid fields. In fact, many organisms from micro-
scopic to macroscopic coordinate with each other using
chemical signals that are suspended in fluids like water or
air. The role of fluid flow in these collectives remains to be
investigated.
6.1. Citation diversity statement
This statement is inspired by a recent initiative [184]. The accu-
mulated knowledge in the topic reviewed here joins not only a
diversity of disciplines but also contributions from researchers
of different genders and ethnic origins. Recent work revealed
that women and other minority scholars are under-cited even
as factors representing seniority and productiveness are
accounted for (see [185] for a detailed analysis). Here, we pro-
vide a breakdown of our references using the tool https://
github.com/dalejn/cleanBib. Our references contain 8.2%
woman (first author)/woman (last author), 12.0% man/
woman, 18.6% woman/man and 61.2% man/man. By race,
our references contain 14.8% author of colour (first)/author
of colour (last), 6.0% white author/author of colour, 18.0%
author of colour/white author, and 61.2% white author/
white author. Following the original method, a colour
author can be Asian, Hispanic, or Black. Of all 366 first/last
authors (counting duplicates), 73.2% are white, 22.4% are
Asian and only 4.4% are Hispanic and Black combined. This
method is limited in that the predictions made by gender-
api.com are not always correct. We have found that for a few
authors of Asian origins, gender-api.com predicted the incor-
rect gender while claiming to have a near-perfect confidence
score. We manually corrected a few predictions upon looking
up researchersprofiles, but the results cannot be assumed to
be completely accurate. The statistics we report fail to include
intersex, non-binary, transgender people, indigenous and
mixed-race authors, or those who may face differential
biases due to the ambiguous racialization or ethnicization of
their names. Under-representations of other dimensions such
as sexual orientation, (dis)ability, class and their intersections
are difficult to reveal. We look forward to future work that
could help us better understand how to support equitable
practices in science.
Data accessibility. This article has no additional data.
Declaration of AI use. We have not used AI-assisted technologies in creat-
ing this article.
Authorscontributions. H.K.: conceptualization, funding acquisition,
investigation, project administration, writingoriginal draft, writ-
ingreview and editing; G.L.: conceptualization, writingreview
and editing; R.N.: conceptualization, writingreview and editing.
All authors gave final approval for publication and agreed to be
held accountable for the work performed therein.
Conflict of interest declaration. We declare we have no competing interests.
Funding. H.K. is supported by the James S. McDonnell Foundations
Postdoctoral Fellowship for Understanding Dynamic & Multi-scale
Systems. We also acknowledge the support from ONR MURI grant
no. N00014-22-1-2616 to co-PIs R.N. and G.L.
Acknowledgements. We thank Dr Merihan Alhafnawi for providing feed-
back on the manuscript.
References
1. Camazine S, Deneubourg JL, Franks NR, Sneyd J,
Theraula G, Bonabeau E. 2020 Self-organization in
biological systems. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
University Press.
2. Gordon DM. 2010 Ant encounters: interaction
networks and colony behavior. Princeton, NJ:
Princeton University Press.
3. Reynolds CW. 1987 Flocks, herds and schools: a distributed
behavioral model. In Proc. 14th Annual Conf. on Computer
Graphics and Interactive Techniques, SIGGRAPH 87, pp.
2534. New York, NY: ACM. (doi:10.1145/37402.37406)
4. Vicsek T, Czirók A, Ben-Jacob E, Cohen I, Shochet O.
1995 Novel type of phase transition in a system of
self-driven particles. Phys. Rev. Lett. 75, 12261229.
(doi:10.1103/PhysRevLett.75.1226)
5. Aoki I. 1982 A simulation study on the schooling
mechanism in fish. Nippon Suisan Gakkaishi 48,
10811088. (doi:10.2331/suisan.48.1081)
6. Huth A, Wissel C. 1992 The simulation of the
movement of fish schools. J. Theor. Biol. 156,
365385. (doi:10.1016/S0022-5193(05)80681-2)
7. Strandburg-Peshkin A et al. 2013 Visual sensory
networks and effective information transfer in
animal groups. Curr. Biol. 23, R709R711. (doi:10.
1016/j.cub.2013.07.059)
8. Bastien R, Romanczuk P. 2020 A model of
collective behavior based purely on vision.
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rsif J. R. Soc. Interface 20: 20230357
10
Sci. Adv. 6, eaay0792. (doi:10.1126/
sciadv.aay0792)
9. Grassé PP. 1959 La reconstruction du nid et les
coordinations interindividuelles chezBellicositermes
natalensis etCubitermes sp. la théorie de la
stigmergie: essai dinterprétation du comportement
des termites constructeurs. Insectes Soc. 6,4180.
(doi:10.1007/BF02223791)
10. Theraulaz G, Bonabeau E. 1999 A brief history of
stigmergy. Artif. Life 5,97116. (doi:10.1162/
106454699568700)
11. Heylighen F. 2016 Stigmergy as a universal
coordination mechanism I: definition and
components. Cogn. Syst. Res. 38,413. (doi:10.
1016/j.cogsys.2015.12.002)
12. Garnier S, Gautrais J, Theraulaz G. 2007
The biological principles of swarm intelligence.
Swarm Intell. 1,331. (doi:10.1007/s11721-007-
0004-y)
13. Downing H, Jeanne R. 1988 Nest construction by
the paper wasp, Polistes: a test of stigmergy theory.
Anim. Behav. 36, 17291739. (doi:10.1016/S0003-
3472(88)80112-X)
14. Nguyen DMT, Iuzzolino ML, Mankel A, Bozek K,
Stephens GJ, Peleg O. 2021 Flow-mediated olfactory
communication in honeybee swarms. Proc. Natl
Acad. Sci. USA 118, e2011916118. (doi:10.1073/
pnas.2011916118)
15. Nguyen DMT, Fard GG, Iuzzolino ML, Peleg O. 2022
Robustness of collective scenting in the presence of
physical obstacles. Artif. Life Rob. 27, 286291.
(doi:10.1007/s10015-021-00712-z)
16. Nguyen DMT, Fard GG, Atkins A, Bontempo P,
Iuzzolino ML, Peleg O. 2023 Honey bees find the
shortest path: a collective flow-mediated approach.
Artif. Life Rob. 28,17. (doi:10.1007/s10015-022-
00816-0)
17. Gloag ES, Javed MA, Wang H, Gee ML, Wade SA,
Turnbull L, Whitchurch CB. 2013 Stigmergy.
Commun. Integr. Biol. 6, e27331. (doi:10.4161/cib.
27331)
18. Gloag ES, Turnbull L, Whitchurch CB. 2015 Bacterial
stigmergy: an organising principle of multicellular
collective behaviours of bacteria. Scientifica 2015,
e387342. (doi:10.1155/2015/387342)
19. Giuggioli L, Potts JR, Rubenstein DI, Levin SA. 2013
Stigmergy, collective actions, and animal social
spacing. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 110,
16 90416 909. (doi:10.1073/pnas.1307071110)
20. Ricci A, Omicini A, Viroli M, Gardelli L, Oliva E. 2007
Cognitive stigmergy: towards a framework based on
agents and artifacts. In Environments for multi-
agent systems III (eds D Weyns, HVD Parunak, F
Michel), pp. 124140. Berlin, Germany: Springer.
(doi:10.1007/978-3-540-71103-2_7)
21. Heylighen F. 2016 Stigmergy as a universal
coordination mechanism II: varieties and evolution.
Cogn. Syst. Res. 38,5059. (doi:10.1016/j.cogsys.
2015.12.007)
22. Holland O, Melhuish C. 1999 Stigmergy,
self-organization, and sorting in collective
robotics. Artif. Life 5, 173202. (doi:10.1162/
106454699568737)
23. Werfel J, Nagpal R. 2006 Extended stigmergy in
collective construction. IEEE Intell. Syst. 21,2028.
(doi:10.1109/MIS.2006.25)
24. Werfel J, Petersen K, Nagpal R. 2014 Designing
collective behavior in a termite-inspired robot
construction team. Science 343, 754758. (doi:10.
1126/science.1245842)
25. Pitcher TJ, Partridge BL, Wardle CS. 1976 A blind
fish can school. Science 194, 963965. (doi:10.
1126/science.982056)
26. Vicsek T, Zafeiris A. 2012 Collective motion.
Phys. Rep. 517,71140. (doi:10.1016/j.physrep.
2012.03.004)
27. Li G, Kolomenskiy D, Liu H, Thiria B, Godoy-Diana R.
2019 On the energetics and stability of a minimal
fish school. PLoS ONE 14, e0215265. (doi:10.1371/
journal.pone.0215265)
28. Kurt M, Ormonde PC, Mivehchi A, Moored KW. 2021
Two-dimensionally stable self-organization arises in
simple schooling swimmers through hydrodynamic
interactions. (http://arxiv.org/abs/2102.03571)
29. Beal D, Hover FS, Triantafyllou MS, Liao JC, Lauder
GV. 2006 Passive propulsion in vortex wakes. J. Fluid
Mech. 549, 385402. (doi:10.1017/
S0022112005007925)
30. Porfiri M, Zhang P, Peterson SD. 2022 Hydrodynamic
model of fish orientation in a channel flow. eLife
11, e75225. (doi:10.7554/eLife.75225)
31. Weihs D. 1973 Hydromechanics of fish schooling.
Nature 241, 290291. (doi:10.1038/241290a0)
32. Lighthill SJ. 1975 Mathematical biofluid dynamics.
Philadelphia, PA: Society for Industrial and Applied
Mathematics.
33. Ko H, Cassidy GJ, Shishkov O, Aydin E, Hu DL,
Goldman DI. 2021 Air-fluidized aggregates of black
soldier fly larvae. Front. Phys. 9, 734447. (doi:10.
3389/fphy.2021.734447)
34. Yim M, Duff D, Roufas K. 2000 PolyBot: a modular
reconfigurable robot. In Proc. 2000 ICRA. IEEE Int. Conf.
on Robotics and Automation Symp. Proc., San Francisco,
CA, USA, 2428 April 2000,vol.1,pp.514520.
(doi:10.1109/ROBOT.2000.844106)
35. Brambilla M, Ferrante E, Birattari M, Dorigo M. 2013
Swarm robotics: a review from the swarm
engineering perspective. Swarm Intell. 7,141.
(doi:10.1007/s11721-012-0075-2)
36. Kaushal H, Kaddoum G. 2016 Underwater optical
wireless communication. IEEE Access 4, 15181547.
(doi:10.1109/ACCESS.2016.2552538)
37. Berlinger F, Gauci M, Nagpal R. 2021 Implicit
coordination for 3D underwater collective behaviors
in a fish-inspired robot swarm. Sci. Rob. 6,
eabd8668. (doi:10.1126/scirobotics.abd8668)
38. Triantafyllou MS, Triantafyllou GS, Yue DKP. 2000
Hydrodynamics of fishlike swimming. Annu. Rev.
Fluid Mech. 32,3353. (doi:10.1146/annurev.fluid.
32.1.33)
39. Fish F, Lauder G. 2006 Passive and active flow
control by swimming fishes and mammals. Annu.
Rev. Fluid Mech. 38, 193224. (doi:10.1146/
annurev.fluid.38.050304.092201)
40. Triantafyllou MS, Weymouth GD, Miao J. 2016
Biomimetic survival hydrodynamics and flow
sensing. Annu. Rev. Fluid Mech. 48,124. (doi:10.
1146/annurev-fluid-122414-034329)
41. Smits AJ. 2019 Undulatory and oscillatory
swimming. J. Fluid Mech. 874, P1. (doi:10.1017/
jfm.2019.284)
42. Coombs S, Bleckmann H, Fay RR, Popper AN (eds).
2014 Thelaterallinesystem, vol. 48. Springer Handbook
of Auditory Research. New York, NY: Springer.
43. Flammang BE, Lauder GV, Troolin DR, Strand TE.
2011 Volumetric imaging of fish locomotion. Biol.
Lett. 7, 695698. (doi:10.1098/rsbl.2011.0282)
44. Li G, Kolomenskiy D, Liu H, Thiria B, Godoy-Diana R.
2022 Hydrodynamical fingerprint of a neighbour in
a fish lateral line. Front. Rob. AI 9, 825889. (doi:10.
3389/frobt.2022.825889)
45. Kurt M, Eslam Panah A, Moored KW. 2020 Flow
interactions between low aspect ratio hydrofoils in
in-line and staggered arrangements. Biomimetics 5,
13. (doi:10.3390/biomimetics5020013)
46. Maertens AP, Gao A, Triantafyllou MS. 2017 Optimal
undulatory swimming for a single fish-like body and
for a pair of interacting swimmers. J. Fluid Mech.
813, 301345. (doi:10.1017/jfm.2016.845)
47. Vogel S. 2020 Life in moving fluids: the physical
biology of flow, 2nd edn. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
University Press.
48. Jones KD, Dohring CM, Platzer MF. 1998
Experimental and computational investigation of
the Knoller-Betz effect. AIAA J. 36, 12401246.
(doi:10.2514/2.505)
49. Gazzola M, Argentina M, Mahadevan L. 2014
Scaling macroscopic aquatic locomotion. Nat. Phys.
10, 758761. (doi:10.1038/nphys3078)
50. Tsang ACH, Kanso E. 2013 Dipole interactions in
doubly periodic domains. J. Nonlinear Sci. 23,
971991. (doi:10.1007/s00332-013-9174-5)
51. Feilich KL, Lauder GV. 2015 Passive mechanical
models of fish caudal fins: effects of shape and
stiffness on self-propulsion. Bioinspir. Biomimet. 10,
036002. (doi:10.1088/1748-3190/10/3/036002)
52. Mendelson L, Techet AH. 2020 Jumping archer fish
exhibit multiple modes of finfin interaction.
Bioinspir. Biomimet. 16, 016006. (doi:10.1088/1748-
3190/abb78e)
53. Partridge BL, Pitcher TJ. 1979 Evidence against a
hydrodynamic function for fish schools. Nature 279,
418419. (doi:10.1038/279418a0)
54. Partridge BL, Johansson J, Kalish J. 1983 The
structure of schools of giant bluefin tuna in Cape
Cod Bay. Environ. Biol. Fishes 9, 253262. (doi:10.
1007/BF00692374)
55. Ashraf I, Bradshaw H, Ha TT, Halloy J, Godoy-Diana
R, Thiria B. 2017 Simple phalanx pattern leads to
energy saving in cohesive fish schooling. Proc. Natl
Acad. Sci. USA 114, 95999604. (doi:10.1073/pnas.
1706503114)
56. Daghooghi M, Borazjani I. 2015 The hydrodynamic
advantages of synchronized swimming in a
rectangular pattern. Bioinspir. Biomimet. 10,
056018. (doi:10.1088/1748-3190/10/5/056018)
57. Hemelrijk C, Reid D, Hildenbrandt H, Padding J. 2015
The increased efficiency of fish swimming in a school.
Fish Fisher. 16,511521. (doi:10.1111/faf.12072)
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rsif J. R. Soc. Interface 20: 20230357
11
58. Ashraf I, Godoy-Diana R, Halloy J, Collignon B, Thiria
B. 2016 Synchronization and collective swimming
patterns in fish (Hemigrammus bleheri). J. R. Soc.
Interface 13, 20160734. (doi:10.1098/rsif.2016.0734)
59. Kent MIA, Lukeman R, Lizier JT, Ward AJW. 2019
Speed-mediated properties of schooling. R. Soc.
Open Sci. 6, 181482. (doi:10.1098/rsos.181482)
60. Partridge BL, Pitcher T, Cullen JM, Wilson J. 1980
The three-dimensional structure of fish schools.
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 6, 277288. (doi:10.1007/
BF00292770)
61. Becker AD, Masoud H, Newbolt JW, Shelley M,
Ristroph L. 2015 Hydrodynamic schooling of
flapping swimmers. Nat. Commun. 6, 8514. (doi:10.
1038/ncomms9514)
62. Ramananarivo S, Fang F, Oza A, Zhang J, Ristroph L.
2016 Flow interactions lead to orderly formations of
flapping wings in forward flight. Phys. Rev. Fluids 1,
071201. (doi:10.1103/PhysRevFluids.1.071201)
63. Newbolt JW, Zhang J, Ristroph L. 2019 Flow
interactions between uncoordinated flapping
swimmers give rise to group cohesion. Proc. Natl
Acad. Sci. USA 116, 24192424. (doi:10.1073/pnas.
1816098116)
64. Zhu X, He G, Zhang X. 2014 Flow-mediated
interactions between two self-propelled flapping
filaments in tandem configuration. Phys. Rev. Lett.
113, 238105. (doi:10.1103/PhysRevLett.113.238105)
65. Yu H, Lu XY, Huang H. 2021 Collective locomotion
of two uncoordinated undulatory self-propelled
foils. Phys. Fluids 33, 011904. (doi:10.1063/5.
0036231)
66. Peng ZR, Huang H, Lu XY. 2018 Collective
locomotion of two closely spaced self-propelled
flapping plates. J. Fluid Mech. 849, 10681095.
(doi:10.1017/jfm.2018.447)
67. Dai L, He G, Zhang X, Zhang X. 2018 Stable
formations of self-propelled fish-like swimmers
induced by hydrodynamic interactions. J. R. Soc.
Interface 15, 20180490. (doi:10.1098/rsif.2018.0490)
68. Harvey C, Baliga VB, Wong JCM, Altshuler DL,
Inman DJ. 2022 Birds can transition between stable
and unstable states via wing morphing. Nature 603,
648653. (doi:10.1038/s41586-022-04477-8)
69. Wissa A. 2022 Trade-offs between stability and
manoeuvrability in bird flight. Nature 603,
579580. (doi:10.1038/d41586-022-00638-x)
70. Drucker E, Lauder G. 1999 Locomotor forces on a
swimming fish: three-dimensional vortex wake
dynamics quantified using digital particle image
velocimetry. J. Exp. Biol. 202, 23932412. (doi:10.
1242/jeb.202.18.2393)
71. Lauder GV. 2015 Fish locomotion: recent advances
and new directions. Annu. Rev. Mar. Sci. 7,
521545. (doi:10.1146/annurev-marine-010814-
015614)
72. Windsor SP, Tan D, Montgomery JC. 2008 Swimming
kinematics and hydrodynamic imaging in the blind
Mexican cave fish (Astyanax fasciatus). J. Exp. Biol.
211, 29502959. (doi:10.1242/jeb.020453)
73. Anderson E, McGillis W, Grosenbaugh M. 2001 The
boundary layer of swimming fish. J. Exp. Biol. 204,
81102. (doi:10.1242/jeb.204.1.81)
74. Flammang BE, Lauder GV, Troolin DR, Strand T.
2011 Volumetric imaging of shark tail
hydrodynamics reveals a three-dimensional dual-
ring vortex wake structure. Proc. R. Soc. B 278,
36703678. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2011.0489)
75. Mendelson L, Techet AH. 2015 Quantitative wake
analysis of a freely swimming fish using 3D
synthetic aperture PIV. Exp. Fluids 56, 135. (doi:10.
1007/s00348-015-2003-x)
76. Tu H, Wang F, Wang H, Gao Q, Wei R. 2022
Experimental study on wake flows of a live fish with
time-resolved tomographic PIV and pressure
reconstruction. Exp. Fluids 63, 25. (doi:10.1007/
s00348-021-03378-2)
77. Mittal R, Dong H, Bozkurttas M, Najjar FM, Vargas
A, von Loebbecke A. 2008 A versatile sharp interface
immersed boundary method for incompressible
flows with complex boundaries. J. Comput. Phys.
227, 48254852. (doi:10.1016/j.jcp.2008.01.028)
78. Pan Y, Dong H. 2020 Computational analysis of
hydrodynamic interactions in a high-density fish
school. Phys. Fluids 32, 121901. (doi:10.1063/5.
0028682)
79. Pan Y, Dong H. 2022 Effects of phase difference on
hydrodynamic interactions and wake patterns in
high-density fish schools. Phys. Fluids 34, 111902.
(doi:10.1063/5.0113826)
80. Moored K. 2018 Unsteady three-dimensional
boundary element method for self-propelled bio-
inspired locomotion. Comput. Fluids 167, 324340.
(doi:10.1016/j.compfluid.2018.03.045)
81. Bleckmann H, Mogdans J, Coombs SL (eds). 2014
Flow sensing in air and water. Berlin, Germany:
Springer.
82. Kasumyan A. 2003 The lateral line in fish: structure,
function, and role in behavior. J. Ichthyol. 43, 40.
83. Coombs S, Janssen J, Webb JF. 1988 Diversity of
lateral line systems: evolutionary and functional
considerations. In Sensory biology of aquatic animals
(eds A Atema, RR Fay, AN Popper, WN Tavolga),
pp. 553593. New York, NY: Springer.
84. Webb JF. 1989 Developmental constraintsand evolution
of the lateral line system in teleost fishes. In The
mechanosensory lateral line (eds S Coombs, P Görner,
H Münz), pp. 7997. New York, NY: Springer.
85. Teyke T. 1988 Flow field, swimming velocity and
boundary layer: parameters which affect the
stimulus for the lateral line organ in blind fish.
J. Comp. Physiol. A 163,5361. (doi:10.1007/
BF00611996)
86. Windsor SP, McHenry MJ. 2009 The influence of
viscous hydrodynamics on the fish lateral-line
system. Integr. Comp. Biol. 49, 691701. (doi:10.
1093/icb/icp084)
87. Yanase K, Saarenrinne P. 2015 Unsteady
turbulent boundary layers in swimming rainbow
trout. J. Exp. Biol. 218, jeb108043. (doi:10.1242/jeb.
108043)
88. Yoshizawa M, Jeffery WR, van Netten SM, McHenry
MJ. 2014 The sensitivity of lateral line receptors and
their role in the behavior of Mexican blind cavefish
(Astyanax mexicanus). J. Exp. Biol. 217, 886895.
(doi:10.1242/jeb.094599)
89. McHenry MJ, Strother JA, van Netten SM. 2008
Mechanical filtering by the boundary layer and
fluidstructure interaction in the superficial
neuromast of the fish lateral line system. J. Comp.
Physiol. A 194, 795810. (doi:10.1007/s00359-008-
0350-2)
90. van Netten SM. 2006 Hydrodynamic detection by
cupulae in a lateral line canal: functional relations
between physics and physiology. Biol. Cybern 94,
6785. (doi:10.1007/s00422-005-0032-x)
91. Hoekstra D, Janssen J. 1985 Non-visual feeding
behavior of the mottled sculpin, Cottus bairdi,in
Lake Michigan. Environ. Biol. Fishes 12, 111117.
(doi:10.1007/BF00002763)
92. Coombs S, Braun C, Donovan BL. 2001 The orienting
response of Lake Michigan mottled sculpin is
mediated by canal neuromasts. J. Exp. Biol. 204,
337348. (doi:10.1242/jeb.204.2.337)
93. Enger PS, Kalmijn AJ, Sand O. 1989 Behavioral
investigations on the functions of the lateral line
and inner ear in predation. In The mechanosensory
lateral line (eds S Coombs, P Görner, H Münz),
pp. 575587. New York, NY: Springer.
94. Gardiner JM, Motta PJ. 2012 Largemouth bass
(Micropterus salmoides) switch feeding modalities in
response to sensory deprivation. Zoology 115,
7883. (doi:10.1016/j.zool.2011.09.004)
95. Gardiner JM. 2012 Multisensory integration in shark
feeding behavior. PhD thesis, University of South
Florida, USA.
96. Sharma S, Coombs S, Patton P, de Perera TB. 2009
The function of wall-following behaviors in the
Mexican blind cavefish and a sighted relative, the
Mexican tetra (Astyanax). J. Comp. Physiol. A 195,
225240. (doi:10.1007/s00359-008-0400-9)
97. Patton P, Windsor S, Coombs S. 2010 Active wall
following by Mexican blind cavefish (Astyanax
mexicanus). J. Comp. Physiol. A 196, 853867.
(doi:10.1007/s00359-010-0567-8)
98. Oteiza P, Odstrcil I, Lauder G, Portugues R, Engert F.
2017 A novel mechanism for mechanosensory-based
rheotaxis in larval zebrafish. Nature 547, 445448.
(doi:10.1038/nature23014)
99. Coombs S, Bak-Coleman J, Montgomery J. 2020
Rheotaxis revisited: a multi-behavioral and
multisensory perspective on how fish orient to flow.
J. Exp. Biol. 223, jeb223008. (doi:10.1242/jeb.
223008)
100. Liao JC. 2006 The role of the lateral line and vision
on body kinematics and hydrodynamic preference of
rainbow trout in turbulent flow. J. Exp. Biol. 209,
40774090. (doi:10.1242/jeb.02487)
101. Liao JC. 2007 A review of fish swimming mechanics
and behaviour in altered flows. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B
362, 19731993. (doi:10.1098/rstb.2007.2082)
102. Bleckmann H, Przybilla A, Klein A, Schmitz A, Kunze
S, Brücker C. 2012 Station holding of trout:
behavior, physiology and hydrodynamics. In Nature-
inspired fluid mechanics (eds C Tropea, H
Bleckmann), pp. 161177. Berlin, Germany:
Springer. (doi:10.1007/978-3-642-28302-4_10)
103. Liao JC, Beal DN, Lauder GV, Triantafyllou MS. 2003
Fish exploiting vortices decrease muscle activity.
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rsif J. R. Soc. Interface 20: 20230357
12
Science 302, 15661569. (doi:10.1126/science.
1088295)
104. Akanyeti O, Liao JC. 2013 A kinematic model of
Kármán gaiting in rainbow trout. J. Exp. Biol. 216,
jeb093245. (doi:10.1242/jeb.093245)
105. Pohlmann K, Grasso FW, Breithaupt T. 2001 Tracking
wakes: the nocturnal predatory strategy of
piscivorous catfish. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 98,
73717374. (doi:10.1073/pnas.121026298)
106. Pohlmann K, Atema J, Breithaupt T. 2004 The
importance of the lateral line in nocturnal predation
of piscivorous catfish. J. Exp. Biol. 207, 29712978.
(doi:10.1242/jeb.01129)
107. Gao A, Triantafyllou MS. 2018 Independent caudal
fin actuation enables high energy extraction and
control in two-dimensional fish-like group
swimming. J. Fluid Mech. 850, 304335. (doi:10.
1017/jfm.2018.456)
108. Li L, Nagy M, Graving JM, Bak-Coleman J, Xie G,
Couzin ID. 2020 Vortex phase matching as a
strategy for schooling in robots and in fish. Nat.
Commun. 11, 5408. (doi:10.1038/s41467-020-
19086-0)
109. Partridge BL, Pitcher TJ. 1980 The sensory basis of
fish schools: relative roles of lateral line and vision.
J. Comp. Physiol. A 135, 315325. (doi:10.1007/
BF00657647)
110. Faucher K, Parmentier E, Becco C, Vandewalle N,
Vandewalle P. 2010 Fish lateral system is required
for accurate control of shoaling behaviour. Anim.
Behav. 79, 679687. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.
12.020)
111. Middlemiss KL, Cook DG, Jerrett AR, Davison W.
2017 Morphology and hydro-sensory role of
superficial neuromasts in schooling behaviour of
yellow-eyed mullet (Aldrichetta forsteri). J. Comp.
Physiol. A 203, 807817. (doi:10.1007/s00359-017-
1192-6)
112. Mekdara PJ, Schwalbe MAB, Coughlin LL,
Tytell ED. 2018 The effects of lateral line
ablation and regeneration in schooling giant
danios. J. Exp. Biol. 221, jeb175166. (doi:10.1242/
jeb.175166)
113. McKee A, Soto AP, Chen P, McHenry MJ. 2020 The
sensory basis of schooling by intermittent
swimming in the rummy-nose tetra (Hemigrammus
rhodostomus). Proc. R. Soc. B 287, 20200568.
(doi:10.1098/rspb.2020.0568)
114. Lafoux B, Moscatelli J, Godoy-Diana R, Thiria B.
2023 Illuminance-tuned collective motion in fish.
Commun. Biol. 6, 585. (doi:10.1038/s42003-023-
04861-8)
115. Mekdara PJ, Nasimi F, Schwalbe MAB, Tytell ED.
2021 Tail beat synchronization during schooling
requires a functional posterior lateral line
system in giant danios, Devario aequipinnatus.
Integr. Comp. Biol. 61, 427441. (doi:10.1093/icb/
icab071)
116. Zhang Y, Lauder GV. 2023 Energy conservation by
group dynamics in schooling fish. bioRxiv.(doi:10.
1101/2022.11.09.515731)
117. Zhang P, Krasner E, Peterson SD, Porfiri M. 2019 An
information-theoretic study of fish swimming in the
wake of a pitching airfoil. Physica D 396,3546.
(doi:10.1016/j.physd.2019.02.014)
118. Thandiackal R, Lauder G. 2023 In-line swimming
dynamics revealed by fish interacting with a robotic
mechanism. eLife 12, e81392. (doi:10.7554/eLife.
81392)
119. Bora M, Kottapalli AGP, Miao J, Triantafyllou MS. 2018
Sensing the flow beneath the fins. Bioinspir. Biomimet.
13, 025002. (doi:10.1088/1748-3190/aaa1c2)
120. Zhai Y, Zheng X, Xie G. 2021 Fish lateral line
inspired flow sensors and flow-aided control: a
review. J. Bionic Eng. 18, 264291. (doi:10.1007/
s42235-021-0034-y)
121. Liu G, Wang A, Wang X, Liu P. 2016 A review of
artificial lateral line in sensor fabrication and bionic
applications for robot fish. Appl. Bionics Biomech.
2016, 4732703. (doi:10.1155/2016/4732703)
122. Shizhe T. 2014 Underwater artificial lateral line flow
sensors. Microsyst. Technol. 20, 21232136. (doi:10.
1007/s00542-014-2350-1)
123. Kruusmaa M, Toming G, Salumäe T, Ježov J, Ernits
A. 2011 Swimming speed control and on-board
flow sensing of an artificial trout. In 2011 IEEE Int.
Conf. on Robotics and Automation, Shanghai, China,
913 May 2011, pp. 17911796. (doi:10.1109/
ICRA.2011.5979942)
124. Chambers LD, Akanyeti O, Venturelli R, Ježov J,
Brown J, Kruusmaa M, Fiorini P, Megill WM. 2014
A fish perspective: detecting flow features while
moving using an artificial lateral line in steady and
unsteady flow. J. R. Soc. Interface 11, 20140467.
(doi:10.1098/rsif.2014.0467)
125. Zheng X, Wang M, Zheng J, Tian R, Xiong M, Xie G.
2019 Artificial lateral line based longitudinal
separation sensing for two swimming robotic fish
with leader-follower formation. In 2019 IEEE/RSJ
Int. Conf. on Intelligent Robots and Systems (IROS),
Macau, China, 38 November 2019, pp. 25392544.
(doi:10.1109/IROS40897.2019.8967597)
126. Yen WK, Huang CF, Chang HR, Guo J. 2021
Localization of a leading robotic fish using a
pressure sensor array on its following vehicle.
Bioinspir. Biomimet. 16, 016007. (doi:10.1088/1748-
3190/abb0cc)
127. Kruusmaa M et al. 2014 FILOSE for svenning: a flow
sensing bioinspired robot. IEEE Rob. Autom. Mag.
21,5162. (doi:10.1109/MRA.2014.2322287)
128. Salumäe T, Rañó I, Akanyeti O, Kruusmaa M. 2012
Against the flow: a Braitenberg controller for a fish
robot. In 2012 IEEE Int. Conf. on Robotics and
Automation, Saint Paul, MN, USA, 1418 May 2012,
pp. 42104215. (doi:10.1109/ICRA.2012.6225023)
129. Ježov J, Akanyeti O, Chambers LD, Kruusmaa M.
2012 Sensing oscillations in unsteady flow for better
robotic swimming efficiency. In 2012 IEEE Int. Conf.
on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics (SMC), Seoul,
South Korea, 1417 October 2012, pp. 9196.
(doi:10.1109/ICSMC.2012.6377682)
130. Salumäe T, Kruusmaa M. 2013 Flow-relative control
of an underwater robot. Proc. R. Soc. A 469,
20120671. (doi:10.1098/rspa.2012.0671)
131. Akanyeti O, Chambers LD, Ježov J, Brown J,
Venturelli R, Kruusmaa M, Megill WM, Fiorini P.
2013 Self-motion effects on hydrodynamic pressure
sensing: part I. Forwardbackward motion.
Bioinspir. Biomimet. 8, 026001. (doi:10.1088/1748-
3182/8/2/026001)
132. Strokina N, Kämäräinen JK, Tuhtan JA, Fuentes-
Pérez JF, Kruusmaa M. 2016 Joint estimation of bulk
flow velocity and angle using a lateral line probe.
IEEE Trans. Instrum. Meas. 65, 601613. (doi:10.
1109/TIM.2015.2499019)
133. Tuhtan JA, Fuentes-Perez JF, Toming G, Schneider
M, Schwarzenberger R, Schletterer M, Kruusmaa M.
2018 Man-made flows from a fishs perspective:
autonomous classification of turbulent fishway flows
with field data collected using an artificial lateral
line. Bioinspir. Biomimet. 13, 046006. (doi:10.1088/
1748-3190/aabc79)
134. Venturelli R et al. 2012 Hydrodynamic pressure
sensing with an artificial lateral line in steady and
unsteady flows. Bioinspir. Biomimet. 7, 036004.
(doi:10.1088/1748-3182/7/3/036004)
135. DeVries L, Lagor FD, Lei H, Tan X, Paley DA. 2015
Distributed flow estimation and closed-loop control
of an underwater vehicle with a multi-modal
artificial lateral line. Bioinspir. Biomimet. 10,
025002. (doi:10.1088/1748-3190/10/2/025002)
136. Zhang F, Lagor FD, Yeo D, Washington P, Paley DA.
2015 Distributed flow sensing for closed-loop speed
control of a flexible fish robot. Bioinspir. Biomimet.
10, 065001. (doi:10.1088/1748-3190/10/6/065001)
137. Free BA, Paley DA. 2018 Model-based observer and
feedback control design for a rigid Joukowski foil in
a Kármán vortex street. Bioinspir. Biomimet. 13,
035001. (doi:10.1088/1748-3190/aaa97f)
138. Wang W, Li Y, Zhang X, Wang C, Chen S, Xie G.
2016 Speed evaluation of a freely swimming robotic
fish with an artificial lateral line. In 2016 IEEE Int.
Conf. on Robotics and Automation (ICRA), Stockholm,
Sweden, 1621 May 2016, pp. 47374742. (doi:10.
1109/ICRA.2016.7487675)
139. Zheng X, Wang W, Xiong M, Xie G. 2020 Online
state estimation of a fin-actuated underwater robot
using artificial lateral line system. IEEE Trans. Rob.
36, 472487. (doi:10.1109/TRO.2019.2956343)
140. Yen WK, Guo J. 2016 Wall following control of a
robotic fish using dynamic pressure. In OCEANS
2016, Shanghai, China, 1013 April 2016, pp. 17.
(doi:10.1109/OCEANSAP.2016.7485700)
141. Yen WK, Sierra DM, Guo J. 2018 Controlling a
robotic fish to swim along a wall using
hydrodynamic pressure feedback. IEEE J. Oceanic
Eng. 43, 369380. (doi:10.1109/JOE.2017.2785698)
142. Thandiackal R et al. 2021 Emergence of robust self-
organized undulatory swimming based on local
hydrodynamic force sensing. Sci. Rob. 6, eabf6354.
(doi:10.1126/scirobotics.abf6354)
143. Zheng X, Wang W, Li L, Xie G. 2020 Artificial lateral
line based relative state estimation between an
upstream oscillating fin and a downstream robotic
fish. Bioinspir. Biomimet. 16, 016012. (doi:10.1088/
1748-3190/abb86c)
144. Giardina I. 2008 Collective behavior in animal
groups: theoretical models and empirical studies.
HFSP J. 2, 205219. (doi:10.2976/1.2961038)
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rsif J. R. Soc. Interface 20: 20230357
13
145. Ballerini M et al. 2008 Interaction ruling animal
collective behavior depends on topological rather
than metric distance: evidence from a field study.
Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 105, 12321237. (doi:10.
1073/pnas.0711437105)
146. Ballerini M et al. 2008 Empirical investigation of
starling flocks: a benchmark study in collective
animal behaviour. Anim. Behav. 76, 201215.
(doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.02.004)
147. Young GF, Scardovi L, Cavagna A, Giardina I,
Leonard NE. 2013 Starling flock networks manage
uncertainty in consensus at low cost. PLoS Comput.
Biol. 9, e1002894. (doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.
1002894)
148. Filella A, Nadal F, Sire C, Kanso E, Eloy C. 2018
Model of collective fish behavior with hydrodynamic
interactions. Phys. Rev. Lett. 120, 198101. (doi:10.
1103/PhysRevLett.120.198101)
149. Oza AU, Ristroph L, Shelley MJ. 2019 Lattices of
hydrodynamically interacting flapping swimmers.
Phys. Rev. X 9, 041024. (doi:10.1103/PhysRevX.9.
041024)
150. Katz Y, Tunstrøm K, Ioannou CC, Huepe C, Couzin ID.
2011 Inferring the structure and dynamics of
interactions in schooling fish. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci.
USA 108, 18 72018 725. (doi:10.1073/pnas.
1107583108)
151. Butail S, Paley DA. 2012 Three-dimensional
reconstruction of the fast-start swimming
kinematics of densely schooling fish. J. R. Soc.
Interface 9,7788. (doi:10.1098/rsif.2011.0113)
152. Wang SH, Liu X, Zhao J, Liu Y, Chen YQ. 2016 3D
tracking swimming fish school using a master view
tracking first strategy. In 2016 IEEE Int. Conf. on
Bioinformatics and Biomedicine (BIBM), Shenzhen,
China, 1518 December 2016, pp. 516519.
(doi:10.1109/BIBM.2016.7822572)
153. Voesenek CJ, Pieters RPM, Leeuwen JL. 2016
Automated reconstruction of three-dimensional fish
motion, forces, and torques. PLoS ONE 11,
e0146682. (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0146682)
154. Graving JM, Chae D, Naik H, Li L, Koger B, Costelloe
BR, Couzin ID. 2019 DeepPoseKit, a software toolkit
for fast and robust animal pose estimation using
deep learning. eLife 8, e47994. (doi:10.7554/eLife.
47994)
155. Lauer J et al. 2022 Multi-animal pose estimation,
identification and tracking with DeepLabCut. Nat.
Methods 19, 496504. (doi:10.1038/s41592-022-
01443-0)
156. Pereira TD et al. 2022 SLEAP: a deep learning
system for multi-animal pose tracking. Nat. Methods
19, 486495. (doi:10.1038/s41592-022-01426-1)
157. Di Santo V, Goerig E, Wainwright DK, Akanyeti O,
Liao JC, Castro-Santos T, Lauder GV. 2021
Convergence of undulatory swimming kinematics
across a diversity of fishes. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA
118, e2113206118. (doi:10.1073/pnas.2113206118)
158. Kirillov A et al. 2023 Segment anything. (http://
arxiv.org/abs/2304.02643)
159. Hughey LF, Hein AM, Strandburg-Peshkin A, Jensen
FH. 2018 Challenges and solutions for studying
collective animal behaviour in the wild. Phil.
Trans. R. Soc. B 373, 20170005. (doi:10.1098/rstb.
2017.0005)
160. Burford BP, Williams RR, Demetras NJ, Carey N,
Goldbogen J, Gilly WF, Harding J, Denny MW. 2022
The limits of convergence in the collective behavior
of competing marine taxa. Ecol. Evol. 12, e8747.
(doi:10.1002/ece3.8747)
161. Gómez-Nava L, Lange RT, Klamser PP, Lukas J,
Arias-Rodriguez L, Bierbach D, Krause J, Sprekeler H,
Romanczuk P. 2023 Fish shoals resemble a
stochastic excitable system driven by environmental
perturbations. Nat. Phys. 19, 663669. (doi:10.
1038/s41567-022-01916-1)
162. Kruusmaa M, Rieucau G, Montoya JCC, Markna R,
Handegard NO. 2016 Collective responses of a large
mackerel school depend on the size and speed of a
robotic fish but not on tail motion. Bioinspir.
Biomimet. 11, 056020. (doi:10.1088/1748-3190/11/
5/056020)
163. Handegard NO, Tenningen M, Howarth K, Anders N,
Rieucau G, Breen M. 2017 Effects on schooling
function in mackerel of sub-lethal capture related
stressors: crowding and hypoxia. PLoS ONE 12,
e0190259. (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0190259)
164. Schad L, Fischer J. 2023 Opportunities and risks in
the use of drones for studying animal behaviour.
Methods Ecol. Evol. 14, 18641872. (doi:10.1111/
2041-210X.13922)
165. Harris M, Nelson JA, Rieucau G, Broussard III W.
2019 Use of drones in fishery science. Trans. Am.
Fisher. Soc. 148, 687697. (doi:10.1002/tafs.10168)
166. Torney CJ, Lamont M, Debell L, Angohiatok RJ,
Leclerc LM, Berdahl AM. 2018 Inferring the rules of
social interaction in migrating caribou. Phil.
Trans. R. Soc. B 373, 20170385. (doi:10.1098/rstb.
2017.0385)
167. Koger B, Deshpande A, Kerby JT, Graving JM,
Costelloe BR, Couzin ID. 2023 Quantifying the
movement, behaviour and environmental context of
group-living animals using drones and computer
vision. J. Anim. Ecol. 92, 13571371. (doi:10.1111/
1365-2656.13904)
168. Tunstrøm K, Katz Y, Ioannou CC, Huepe C, Lutz MJ,
Couzin ID. 2013 Collective states, multistability and
transitional behavior in schooling fish. PLoS Comput.
Biol. 9, e1002915. (doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.
1002915)
169. Bialek W. 2022 On the dimensionality of behavior.
Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 119, e2021860119.
(doi:10.1073/pnas.2021860119)
170. Stephens GJ, Johnson-Kerner B, Bialek W, Ryu WS.
2008 Dimensionality and dynamics in the behavior
of C. elegans.PLoS Comput. Biol. 4, e1000028.
(doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000028)
171. Ahamed T, Costa AC, Stephens GJ. 2021 Capturing
the continuous complexity of behaviour in
Caenorhabditis elegans.Nat. Phys. 17, 275283.
(doi:10.1038/s41567-020-01036-8)
172. Sridhar VH, Davidson JD, Twomey CR, Sosna MMG,
Nagy M, Couzin ID. 2023 Inferring social influence
in animal groups across multiple timescales. Phil.
Trans. R. Soc. B 378, 20220062. (doi:10.1098/rstb.
2022.0062)
173. Brunton SL, Proctor JL, Kutz JN. 2016 Discovering
governing equations from data by sparse
identification of nonlinear dynamical systems. Proc.
Natl Acad. Sci. USA 113, 39323937. (doi:10.1073/
pnas.1517384113)
174. Jiahao TZ, Pan L, Hsieh MA. 2022 Learning to
swarm with knowledge-based neural ordinary
differential equations. In 2022 Int. Conf. on
Robotics and Automation (ICRA), Philadelphia, PA,
USA, 2327 May 2022, pp. 69126918. (doi:10.
1109/ICRA46639.2022.9811997)
175. Chee KY, Jiahao TZ, Hsieh MA. 2022 KNODE-MPC: a
knowledge-based data-driven predictive control
framework for aerial robots. IEEE Rob. Autom. Lett.
7, 28192826. (doi:10.1109/LRA.2022.3144787)
176. Zhu J, White C, Wainwright DK, Di Santo V, Lauder
GV, Bart-Smith H. 2019 Tuna robotics: a high-
frequency experimental platform exploring the
performance space of swimming fishes. Sci. Rob. 4,
eaax4615. (doi:10.1126/scirobotics.aax4615)
177. White CH, Lauder GV, Bart-Smith H. 2021 Tunabot Flex:
a tuna-inspired robot with body flexibility improves
high-performance swimming. Bioinspir. Biomimet. 16,
026019. (doi:10.1088/1748-3190/abb86d)
178. Li L, Ravi S, Xie G, Couzin ID. 2021 Using a robotic
platform to study the influence of relative tailbeat
phase on the energetic costs of side-by-side
swimming in fish. Proc. R. Soc. A 477, 20200810.
(doi:10.1098/rspa.2020.0810)
179. Salumäe T, Kruusmaa M. 2011 A flexible fin with
bio-inspired stiffness profile and geometry. J. Bionic
Eng. 8, 418428. (doi:10.1016/S1672-
6529(11)60047-4)
180. Marais C, Thiria B, Wesfreid JE, Godoy-Diana R. 2012
Stabilizing effect of flexibility in the wake of a
flapping foil. J. Fluid Mech. 710, 659669. (doi:10.
1017/jfm.2012.390)
181. Jusufi A, Vogt DM, Wood RJ, Lauder GV. 2017
Undulatory swimming performance and body
stiffness modulation in a soft robotic fish-inspired
physical model. Soft Rob. 4, 202210. (doi:10.1089/
soro.2016.0053)
182. Zhong Q, Zhu J, Fish FE, Kerr SJ, Downs AM,
Bart-Smith H, Quinn D. 2021 Tunable stiffness
enables fast and efficient swimming in fish-like
robots. Sci. Rob. 6, eabe4088. (doi:10.1126/
scirobotics.abe4088)
183. Berlinger F, Saadat M, Haj-Hariri H, Lauder GV,
Nagpal R. 2021 Fish-like three-dimensional
swimming with an autonomous, multi-fin, and
biomimetic robot. Bioinspir. Biomimet. 16, 026018.
(doi:10.1088/1748-3190/abd013)
184. Zurn P, Bassett DS, Rust NC. 2020 The citation
diversity statement: a practice of transparency, a
way of life. Trends Cogn. Sci. 24, 669672. (doi:10.
1016/j.tics.2020.06.009)
185. Teich EG et al. 2022 Citation inequity and
gendered citation practices in contemporary
physics. Nat. Phys. 18,11611170. (doi:10.1038/
s41567-022-01770-1)
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rsif J. R. Soc. Interface 20: 20230357
14
... Much of this is of interest for bacteria and other microscopic entities such as light or chemically driven Janus particles. Most of these studies being at low Reynolds numbers, it remains a challenge to gauge how high Reynolds numbers affect the collective behavior of swimmers and what the effects of boundaries are for such conditions 12,13 . Whether this is of interest to the behavior of fish or other aquatic species in natural settings is an important question and a variety of experiments using different types of fish have been carried out revealing among others the role of social interactions, interactions with the environment, or the role of the flow produced by the fish in determining the behavior of the collective [12][13][14][15][16][17][18][19][20] . ...
... Most of these studies being at low Reynolds numbers, it remains a challenge to gauge how high Reynolds numbers affect the collective behavior of swimmers and what the effects of boundaries are for such conditions 12,13 . Whether this is of interest to the behavior of fish or other aquatic species in natural settings is an important question and a variety of experiments using different types of fish have been carried out revealing among others the role of social interactions, interactions with the environment, or the role of the flow produced by the fish in determining the behavior of the collective [12][13][14][15][16][17][18][19][20] . Experiments and simulations using artificial fish like swimmers should allow to shed light on the importance of hydrodynamics 12,21 as well as the presence of boundaries on the behavior of collections of high Reynolds number swimmers. ...
... Whether this is of interest to the behavior of fish or other aquatic species in natural settings is an important question and a variety of experiments using different types of fish have been carried out revealing among others the role of social interactions, interactions with the environment, or the role of the flow produced by the fish in determining the behavior of the collective [12][13][14][15][16][17][18][19][20] . Experiments and simulations using artificial fish like swimmers should allow to shed light on the importance of hydrodynamics 12,21 as well as the presence of boundaries on the behavior of collections of high Reynolds number swimmers. ...
Article
Full-text available
The spatial organization of active particles or swimmers may depend strongly on the nature of the interaction between the particles and the boundary. Here we use robotic fish of several centimeters dimensions that swim at high enough velocities to reach Reynolds numbers Re of order 10310^3 or 10410^4. Under confinement in circular arenas filled with a shallow layer of water, these robots swim mostly near the walls and undergo a gradual transition from swirling motion near the boundaries to large cluster formation as the number of particles in the assembly is increased. This transition is highly dependent on the nature of the walls: for solid impermeable walls this transition occurs for small numbers of fish robots. For porous walls this transition is delayed and occurs at larger numbers. The main reason why the two boundaries affect the swimming differently is the alignment of the fish robots at the wall: for the impermeable boundary the fish robots align with a smaller angle to the wall while for the porous case, the fish robots align with a larger angle at the wall allowing the formation of linear clusters. We carry out numerical simulations of model fish in three dimensions to examine how such experimental results can be understood. The interest of these simulations is that they provide a direct and quantitative view of the properties of the flow engendered by the fish like objects. The interaction of this flow with other fish or with the boundaries is the crucial aspect behind the self organization. These simulations reproduce the main features of the behavior of the swimmers such as their swimming near the walls or their angle with respect to the boundary. By using flexible and free to move arenas in experiments and simulations, we show that the assembly of fish robots is capable of creating large deformations as well as induce mobility of the arenas through the self-organization of the robotic fish opening the possibility of making sub-aquatic flexible robots of robots.
... Thus far, in research experiments on collective behavior, efforts have often been made to reduce the impact of obstacles and use uniform water flow conditions. These experiments mainly involved small freshwater fish (a few centimeters long) in circular shallow-water tanks (Jolles et al. 2017;Ginnaw et al. 2020;Li et al. 2022;Ko et al. 2023) or small rectangular shallow-water flumes (De Bie et al. 2020;Georgopoulou et al. 2022), making tracking easier. Although these previous studies have made significant strides in understanding the collective motion of fish, only a few have tested changes in group cohesion, structure, and dynamics in environments that may interfere with individual collective behavioral capabilities, such as varying the water flow conditions (De Bie et al. 2020;Ginnaw et al. 2020;Li et al. 2020). ...
... Das et al. (2023) further demonstrated that the common stability of in-line swimming patterns in calm water can be attributed to fluid dynamic interactions between fish. In summary, the complex interaction between fish schools and water currents has long been a focal point of fluid dynamics research, with significant advancements made in both experimental and simulation studies (Ko et al. 2023). However, most of these experimental conditions have been limited to uniform flow condition. ...
... Future research using particle image velocimetry (PIV) and particle tracking velocimetry (PTV) to directly measure velocity fields in aquatic animal movement (Tu et al. 2022;Li and Mendelson 2023) will become popular experimental techniques. The development of large-scale general computer vision models will also offer significant opportunities for accurately capturing fish school behavior (Ko et al. 2023). These methods will greatly enhance our understanding of the hydrodynamic response mechanisms of schooling structures. ...
Article
Full-text available
Abstracts Fish often swim in structured group formations (fish schooling). Hydrodynamics is one of the key external factors that might affect an organism’s swimming behavior, along with other aspects such as food resources and predators. Most previous studies on collective behavior of schooling fish have been conducted in relatively simplified tanks, with few focusing on collective behavior in complex flow environments with obstacles. In this study, complex hydrodynamic environments were constructed by arranging staggered cobbles in the flume to investigate the collective behavior and upstream tactics of juvenile fish under different hydrodynamic conditions, including low- (0.4 Ucrit) and high-flow (0.8 Ucrit) conditions. The results indicate the following: (1) Under high-flow conditions, schooling fish tend to swim side-by-side with stronger cohesion when confronted with the impact of high flow velocity; (2) in low-flow conditions, schooling fish display increased polarization, evidenced by reduced bearing angles between neighboring and focal fish, signifying improved group coordination; and (3) in high-flow conditions, schooling fish use the sheltered area behind obstacles to go upstream, while in none-obstructed zone, migrating upstream is challenging. This study provides a new approach for studying fish swimming behavior in natural-like environments and offers insights and theoretical references for fish habitat restoration works.
... Not only do such environments hold the potential to exert exogenous forcings on the individuals, such as when a bird encounters a gust, or when a group of fish encounters a turbulent flow [12], but they also couple the individuals to each other through fluid-mediated interactions. In this work, we focus on the hydrodynamic interactions between swimmers-which are expected to play an outsized role in the collective motion of swimmers [13]-specifically on how they affect group cohesion. ...
Preprint
When groups of inertial swimmers move together, hydrodynamic interactions play a key role in shaping their collective dynamics, including the cohesion of the group. To explore how these interactions influence group cohesion, we develop a three-dimensional, inviscid, far-field model of a swimmer. Focusing on symmetric triangular, diamond, and circular group arrangements, we investigate whether passive hydrodynamics alone can promote cohesive behavior, and what role symmetry of the group plays. While small symmetric (and even asymmetric) groups can be cohesive, larger groups typically are not, instead breaking apart into smaller, self-organized subgroups that are cohesive. Notably, we discover circular arrangements of swimmers that chase each other around a circle, resembling the milling behavior of natural fish schools; we call this hydrodynamic milling. Hydrodynamic milling is cohesive in the sense that it is a fixed point of a particular Poincar\'e map, but it is unstable, especially to asymmetric perturbations. Our findings suggest that while passive hydrodynamics alone cannot sustain large-scale cohesion indefinitely, controlling interactions between subgroups, or controlling the behavior of only the periphery of a large group, could potentially enable stable collective behavior with minimal active input.
... Hydrodynamic signals exhibited a more complex interplay (Ko et al., 2023). While tidal currents appeared structured, their shorter vector lengths and secondary PC2 association suggest a limited, possibly episodic influence, which is consistent with prior observations that tidal periodicity constrains wild Cod displacement but is less influential in farmed settings (Berges et al., 2024;Meager et al., 2009). ...
Article
Full-text available
This study investigated how environmental drivers and feeding practices influence the vertical behavior of farmed Atlantic Cod (Gadus morhua) in a commercial sea cage environment. A 70-day continuous observation was conducted at a farm in northern Norway, utilizing a dual-echosounder setup combined with temperature loggers and model-derived environmental data, including current velocity, salinity, and oxygen profiles. Cod behavior was compared between submerged (5 m) and surface feeding regimes across four sea cages. Fish consistently avoided surface layers above 15 ∘ C, aggregating at cooler depths, and shifted upward as thermal stratification weakened into autumn. Submerged feeding supported tighter aggregation, stronger rhythmicity, and vertical cohesion, while surface feeding induced anticipatory surfacing, behavioral fragmentation, and increased thermal exposure. These patterns were amplified by Cod's physoclistic physiology and anatomical predispositions to strain from repeated vertical movement. Signal processing of depth-use patterns revealed stronger behavioral synchronization under submerged feeding, particularly aligned with the 48-hour feeding cycle. Aggregation dynamics were more closely aligned with residual current patterns and deeper water mass structure than with tidal forcing, and air temperature emerged as a reliable and practical proxy for estimating the vertical center of biomass. This study provides critical insight into farmed Cod's depth preferences and environmental rhythms, which may be essential for enhancing fish welfare, reducing mortality, and improving the sustainability of Cod aquaculture.
... However, this model is computationally complex and, in large-scale swarms, is prone to noise interference, which can destabilize the swarm and hinder its adaptability to complex and dynamic underwater environments. 4,11,16 In contrast, the Vicsek model achieves self-organized coordination through alignment mechanisms among individuals, o®ering higher computational e±ciency and demonstrating robust performance in the face of environmental changes. Nevertheless, the Vicsek model exhibits limited°exibility and adaptability when dealing with dynamic obstacles and diverse task objectives, 16 making it less suitable for complex underwater operational scenarios. ...
Article
Full-text available
As the demand for marine environmental monitoring and deep-sea operations continues to grow, AUV swarm systems have become a research hotspot due to their high flexibility. This paper proposes a navigation control method for multi-AUV swarms that integrates the Social Force Model (SFM) with the Vicsek model to improve collaboration efficiency and motion stability in dynamic underwater environments. The proposed control strategy incorporates both physical forces and alignment mechanisms to achieve dynamic behavioral coordination among the robots in the swarm. The SFM is used to characterize interactions between individuals, ensuring collision avoidance, while the Vicsek model provides a neighborhood-based velocity alignment mechanism to enhance swarm coherence. Additionally, the control framework introduces a leader-follower structure, effectively integrating local perception with global navigation information. Simulation results indicate that the robot swarm can maintain effective obstacle avoidance and structural stability even in complex environments with obstacles. In the absence of the Vicsek model, navigation tasks may fail or significantly increase navigation time. Even with low alignment strength and higher speeds, navigation time can be reduced by 35%, while higher alignment strength combined with lower speeds can shorten navigation time by up to 50%. Tests with large-scale swarms demonstrate that the proposed method exhibits good scalability and effectively prevents excessive aggregation within the group. Future research will focus on integrating intelligent optimization methods, such as deep reinforcement learning, to enhance the generalization ability of the control strategy in unstructured and dynamic environments.
... Wake and vortices-Although the torque plays no role in the scaling of Re as a function of Th, it is essential to reproduce the wake at the rear of the swimmer in the inertial regime. The torque dipole can be used to account for flagellum, body undulation, or tail beating to generate a reverse von Karman vortex street, as observed in the wake of a fish at high Re number [15,43]. Figures 3(a) and 3(b) show the typical wakes obtained for a 2D swimmer at Re ¼ 960 and 300. ...
Article
We introduce a minimal model of a swimmer without body deformation based on force and torque dipoles which allows accurate and efficient 3D Navier-Stokes calculations. Our model can reproduce swimmer propulsion for a large range of Reynolds numbers and generate wake vortices in the inertial regime, reminiscent of the flow generated by the flapping tail of real fish. We perform a numerical exploration of the model from low to high Reynolds numbers and obtain universal laws using scaling arguments. Collecting data from a wide variety of microswimmers, we show that our theoretical scaling laws compare very well with experimental swimming performances across the different hydrodynamic regimes, from Stokes to turbulent flows. The simple design of our generic swimmer model paves the way for efficient large-scale simulations of hundreds of individuals, crucial for understanding collective effects within assemblies of aquatic animals.
... There has been a broad variety of self-organizing robots [107][108][109][110][111][112][113][114][115][116][117] , terrestrial, aerial, aquatic, and/or hybrid (for a review see ref. 26). ...
... They can also leverage information encoded in flow structures for perception, especially at short ranges. For instance, fish use superficial and canal neuromasts to detect surface shear flow and pressure gradients (e.g., generated by predators and prey [5]) along their lateral line [6], [7], and swim in wakes for schooling and station-keeping, even without visual cues [8]- [11]. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Fish use their lateral lines to sense flows and pressure gradients, enabling them to detect nearby objects and organisms. Towards replicating this capability, we demonstrated successful leader-follower formation swimming using flow pressure sensing in our undulatory robotic fish (μ\muBot/MUBot). The follower μ\muBot is equipped at its head with bilateral pressure sensors to detect signals excited by both its own and the leader's movements. First, using experiments with static formations between an undulating leader and a stationary follower, we determined the formation that resulted in strong pressure variations measured by the follower. This formation was then selected as the desired formation in free swimming for obtaining an expert policy. Next, a long short-term memory neural network was used as the control policy that maps the pressure signals along with the robot motor commands and the Euler angles (measured by the onboard IMU) to the steering command. The policy was trained to imitate the expert policy using behavior cloning and Dataset Aggregation (DAgger). The results show that with merely two bilateral pressure sensors and less than one hour of training data, the follower effectively tracked the leader within distances of up to 200 mm (= 1 body length) while swimming at speeds of 155 mm/s (= 0.8 body lengths/s). This work highlights the potential of fish-inspired robots to effectively navigate fluid environments and achieve formation swimming through the use of flow pressure feedback.
Article
Full-text available
Fish swimming together in schools interact via multiple sensory pathways, including vision, acoustics and hydrodynamics, to coordinate their movements. Disentangling the specific role of each sensory pathway is an open and important question. Here, we propose an information-theoretic approach to dissect interactions between swimming fish based on their movement and the flow velocity at selected measurement points in the environment. We test the approach in a controlled mechanical system constituted by an actively pitching airfoil and a compliant flag that simulates the behaviour of two fish swimming in line. The system consists of two distinct types of interactions – hydrodynamic and electromechanical. By using transfer entropy of the measured time series, we unveil a strong causal influence of the airfoil pitching on the flag undulation with an accurate estimate of the time delay between the two. By conditioning the computation on the flow-speed information, recorded by laser Doppler velocimetry, we discover a significant reduction in transfer entropy, correctly implying the presence of a hydrodynamic pathway of interaction. Similarly, the electromechanical pathway of interaction is identified accurately when present. The study supports the potential use of information-theoretic methods to decipher the existence of different pathways of interaction between schooling fish.
Article
Full-text available
We propose a hybrid numerical model for the collective motion of fish groups, which integrates an agent-based model with a CFD-based model. In the agent-based model, the fish group is represented by self-propelled particles (SPPs), incorporating social forces with local interactive rules. The CFD-based model treats the fish body with an undulated filament that responds to the hydrodynamic forces imposed by the surrounding fluid flow. These two models are coupled using a central pattern generator (CPG) controller. We test this hybrid model with groups of 30-50 individuals. The results show that the group exhibits various collective behaviours, including tight schooling, sparse schooling, and milling patterns, by adjusting the coefficients in the SPP model. Due to the hydrodynamic interactions, particularly with the obstacle avoidance model, both the individuals' and the group's mean speed fluctuate, differentiating it from traditional SPP models that typically consider volumeless particles. More interestingly, our findings indicate that fish benefit from collective motion in terms of energetic consumption in both schooling and milling patterns. It is important to note that the swimming fish are actuated using a very simple mechanism without any optimisation strategy. An additional study investigates the effects of the Reynolds number, demonstrating the capability of the current hybrid model to account for fish groups of varying body lengths or swimming speeds. Future applications of this model are promising, offering potential insights into the energetic advantages of collective motion in large-scale fish groups.
Article
Full-text available
We experimentally investigate the role of illumination on the collective dynamics of a large school (ca. 50 individuals) of Hemigrammus rhodostomus. The structure of the group, defined using two order parameters, is quantified while progressively altering the visual range of the fish through controlled cycles of ambient light intensity. We show that, at low light levels, the individuals within the group are unable to form a cohesive group, while at higher illuminance the degree of alignment of the school correlates with the light intensity. When increasing the illuminance, the school structure is successively characterized by a polarized state followed by a highly regular and stable rotational configuration (milling). Our study shows that vision is necessary to achieve cohesive collective motion for free swimming fish schools, while the short-range lateral line sensing is insufficient in this situation. The present experiment therefore provides new insights into the interaction mechanisms that govern the emergence and intensity of collective motion in biological systems.
Article
Full-text available
Methods for collecting animal behaviour data in natural environments, such as direct observation and biologging, are typically limited in spatiotemporal resolution, the number of animals that can be observed and information about animals' social and physical environments. Video imagery can capture rich information about animals and their environments, but image‐based approaches are often impractical due to the challenges of processing large and complex multi‐image datasets and transforming resulting data, such as animals' locations, into geographical coordinates. We demonstrate a new system for studying behaviour in the wild that uses drone‐recorded videos and computer vision approaches to automatically track the location and body posture of free‐roaming animals in georeferenced coordinates with high spatiotemporal resolution embedded in contemporaneous 3D landscape models of the surrounding area. We provide two worked examples in which we apply this approach to videos of gelada monkeys and multiple species of group‐living African ungulates. We demonstrate how to track multiple animals simultaneously, classify individuals by species and age–sex class, estimate individuals' body postures (poses) and extract environmental features, including topography of the landscape and animal trails. By quantifying animal movement and posture while reconstructing a detailed 3D model of the landscape, our approach opens the door to studying the sensory ecology and decision‐making of animals within their natural physical and social environments.
Article
Full-text available
Many animal behaviours exhibit complex temporal dynamics, suggesting there are multiple timescales at which they should be studied. However, researchers often focus on behaviours that occur over relatively restricted temporal scales, typically ones that are more accessible to human observation. The situation becomes even more complex when considering multiple animals interacting, where behavioural coupling can introduce new timescales of importance. Here, we present a technique to study the time-varying nature of social influence in mobile animal groups across multiple temporal scales. As case studies, we analyse golden shiner fish and homing pigeons, which move in different media. By analysing pairwise interactions among individuals, we show that predictive power of the factors affecting social influence depends on the timescale of analysis. Over short timescales the relative position of a neighbour best predicts its influence and the distribution of influence across group members is relatively linear, with a small slope. At longer timescales, however, both relative position and kinematics are found to predict influence, and nonlinearity in the influence distribution increases, with a small number of individuals being disproportionately influential. Our results demonstrate that different interpretations of social influence arise from analysing behaviour at different timescales, highlighting the importance of considering its multiscale nature. This article is part of a discussion meeting issue ‘Collective behaviour through time’.
Article
Full-text available
Groups of animals can perform highly coordinated collective behaviours that confer benefits to the participating individuals by facilitating social information exchange and protection from predators¹. Some of these characteristics could arise when groups operate at critical points between two structurally and functionally different states, leading to maximal responsiveness to external stimuli and effective propagation of information2,3. It has been proposed that animal groups constitute examples of self-organized systems at criticality2,3; however, direct empirical evidence of this hypothesis—in particular in the wild—is mostly absent. Here we show that highly conspicuous, repetitive and rhythmic collective dive cascades produced by many thousands of freshwater fish under high predation risk resemble a stochastic excitable system driven by environmental perturbations. Together with the results of an agent-based model of the system, this suggests that these fish shoals might operate at a critical point between a state of high individual diving activity and low overall diving activity. We show that the best fitting model, which is located at a critical point, allows information about external perturbations—such as predator attacks—to propagate most effectively through the shoal. Our results suggest that criticality might be a plausible principle of distributed information processing in large animal collectives.
Article
Full-text available
Schooling in fish is linked to a number of factors such as increased foraging success, predator avoidance, and social interactions. In addition, a prevailing hypothesis is that swimming in groups provides energetic benefits through hydrodynamic interactions. Thrust wakes are frequently occurring flow structures in fish schools as they are shed behind swimming fish. Despite increased flow speeds in these wakes, recent modeling work has suggested that swimming directly in-line behind an individual may lead to increased efficiency. However, only limited data are available on live fish interacting with thrust wakes. Here we designed a controlled experiment in which brook trout, Salvelinus fontinalis, interact with thrust wakes generated by a robotic mechanism that produces a fish-like wake. We show that trout swim in thrust wakes, reduce their tail-beat frequencies, and synchronize with the robotic flapping mechanism. Our flow and pressure field analysis revealed that the trout are interacting with oncoming vortices and that they exhibit reduced pressure drag at the head compared to swimming in isolation. Together, these experiments suggest that trout swim energetically more efficiently in thrust wakes and support the hypothesis that swimming in the wake of one another is an advantageous strategy to save energy in a school.
Article
Full-text available
Honey bees (Apis mellifera L.) are social insects that makes frequent use of volatile pheromone signals to collectively navigate unpredictable and unknown environments. Ants have been shown to effectively use pheromone trails to find the shortest path between two points, the nest and the food source. The ant pheromone trails are accomplished by depositing pheromones which are then diffused passively, creating isotropic (i.e., non-directional and axi-symmetric) signals. In this study, we report the first instance of the honey bees’ ability to solve the shortest path problem to localize the queen and aggregate around her by using a collective flow-mediated scenting strategy. In this strategy, individual bees not only emit pheromones but also fan their wings to actively direct the flow of the signals, providing colony members with directional messages to the queen’s location. We use computer vision and deep learning approaches to perform automatic and accurate image analysis. As a result, we quantify the number of bees in the short and long paths, and show that the short path is frequented by significantly more bees over time. We also reconstruct attractive surfaces using the positions and directions of scenting bees, and show that this surface is more “attractive” along the short path and around the queen as scenting bees send out directional messages and the swarm makes their way to the queen. Overall, we show that honey bees can effectively use the collective scenting behavior to overcome local and volatile pheromone communication and find the shortest path to the queen.
Preprint
Schooling fish move in a viscous fluid environment and are a model system for understanding how group movement dynamics enables energy conservation ¹ . But how group movement in the water reduces locomotor cost remains elusive. We measured both aerobic and anaerobic metabolic contributions to energy use in schools of giant danio, and show an energetic reduction per tail beat of up to 126% compared to solitary fish. Fish in schools used a smaller proportion of their aerobic capacity to swim and reduced the anaerobic cost by 2.8 fold, which lowered the total energy expenditure and cost of transport by up to 112% at higher speeds. Fish schools also exhibit a U -shaped metabolism–speed curve, with a minimum metabolic cost at 1 body length s ⁻¹ , the migratory speed of diverse fish species 2,3 . This finding suggests that physical constraints on movement through water may be a key driver for the evolution of active directional schooling behaviour in fishes. One-Sentence Summary Fish schools reduce the metabolic cost of swimming by up to 112% and the energy used per tail beat of up to 126% compared to solitary fish.
Article
In this study, we numerically investigate the effects of the tail-beat phase differences between the trailing fish and its neighboring fish on the hydrodynamic performance and wake dynamics in a two-dimensional high-density school. Foils undulating with a wavy-like motion are employed to mimic swimming fish. The phase difference varies from 0° to 360°. A sharp-interface immersed boundary method is used to simulate flows over the fish-like bodies and provide quantitative analysis of the hydrodynamic performance and wakes of the school. It is found that the highest net thrust and swimming efficiency can be reached at the same time in the fish school with a phase difference of 180°. In particular, when the phase difference is 90°, the trailing fish achieves the highest efficiency, 58% enhancement compared with a single fish, while it has the highest thrust production, increased by 108% over a single fish, at a phase difference of 0°. The performance and flow visualization results suggest that the phase of the trailing fish in the dense school can be controlled to improve thrust and propulsive efficiency, and these improvements occur through the hydrodynamic interactions with the vortices shed by the neighboring fish and the channel formed by the side fish. In addition, the investigation of the phase difference effects on the wake dynamics of schools performed in this work represents the first study in which the wake patterns for systems consisting of multiple undulating bodies are categorized. In particular, a reversed Bénard–von Kármán vortex wake is generated by the trailing fish in the school with a phase difference of 90°, while a Bénard–von Kármán vortex wake is produced when the phase difference is 0°. Results have revealed that the wake patterns are critical to predicting the hydrodynamic performance of a fish school and are highly dependent on the phase difference.
Article
The under-attribution of women’s contributions to scientific scholarship is well known and well studied. One measure of this under-attribution is the citation gap between men and women: the under-citation of papers authored by women relative to expected rates coupled with an over-citation of papers authored by men relative to expected rates. Here we explore this citation gap in contemporary physics. We find a global bias wherein papers authored by women are significantly under-cited, and papers authored by men are significantly over-cited. Moreover, we find that citation behaviour varies along several dimensions, such that imbalances differ according to who is citing, where they are citing and what they are citing. Specifically, citation imbalance in favour of man-authored papers is highest for papers authored by men, papers published in general physics journals and papers for which citing authors probably have less domain or author familiarity. Our results suggest that although deciding which papers to cite is an individual choice, the cumulative effects of these choices needlessly harm a subset of scholars. We discuss several strategies for the mitigation of these effects, including conscious behavioural changes at the individual, journal and community levels. The under-citation of woman authors in physics is quantified and measures that could overcome this inequity are presented.