ArticlePublisher preview available

Stigma Consciousness: The Psychological Legacy of Social Stereotypes

Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the author.

Abstract

Whereas past researchers have treated targets of stereotypes as though they have uniform reactions to their stereotyped status (e.g., J. Crocker & B. Major, 1989; C. M. Steele & J. Aronson, 1995), it is proposed here that targets differ in the extent to which they expect to be stereotyped by others (i.e., stigma consciousness). Six studies, 5 of which validate the stigma-consciousness questionnaire (SCQ), are presented. The results suggest that the SCQ is a reliable and valid instrument for detecting differences in stigma consciousness. In addition, scores on the SCQ predict perceptions of discrimination and the ability to generate convincing examples of such discrimination. The final study highlights a behavioral consequence of stigma consciousness: the tendency for people high in stigma consciousness to forgo opportunities to invalidate stereotypes about their group. The relation of stigma consciousness to past research on targets of stereotypes is considered as is the issue of how stigma consciousness may encourage continued stereotyping.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
1999,
Vol. 76, No. 1, 114-128Copyright 1999 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.
0022-3514/99/S3.00
Stigma Consciousness: The Psychological Legacy of Social Stereotypes
Elizabeth C. Pinel
University of Texas at Austin
Whereas past researchers have treated targets of stereotypes as though they have uniform reactions to
their stereotyped status (e.g., J. Crocker & B. Major, 1989; C. M. Steele & J. Aronson, 1995), it is
proposed here that targets differ in the extent to which they expect to be stereotyped by others (i.e., stigma
consciousness). Six studies, 5 of which validate the stigma-consciousness questionnaire (SCQ), are
presented. The results suggest that the SCQ is a reliable and valid instrument for detecting differences
in stigma consciousness. In addition, scores on the SCQ predict perceptions of discrimination and the
ability to generate convincing examples of such discrimination. The final study highlights a behavioral
consequence of stigma consciousness: the tendency for people high in stigma consciousness to forgo
opportunities to invalidate stereotypes about their group. The relation of stigma consciousness to past
research on targets of stereotypes is considered as is the issue of how stigma consciousness may
encourage continued stereotyping.
Innocent chatter, the currency of ordinary social life, or a compliment
("You don't think like a woman"), the well-intentioned advice of
psychologists, the news item, the joke, the cosmetics advertisement
none of these is what it is or what it was. Each reveals
itself,
depending on the circumstances in which it appears, as a threat, an
insult, an affront, as a reminder, however subtle, that I belong to an
inferior caste.
—S.
L. Bartky, "Toward a Phenomenology
of Feminist Consciousness"
And I always feel this with.. .people—that whenever they're being
nice to me, pleasant to me, all the time really, underneath they're only
assessing me as a criminal and nothing else. It's too late for me to be
any different now to what I am, but I still feel this keenly, that that's
their only approach, and they're quite incapable of accepting me as
anything else.
—E.
Goffman, Stigma: Notes on the Management
of Spoiled Identity
For some targets of stereotypes, prejudice and discrimination al-
ways seem to be "out
there."
It is easy to understand why. Researchers
have documented the pervasiveness of stereotypes in our society, both
in terms of the number of groups that are stereotyped and the number
of people who endorse stereotypes about these groups (cf. Crocker &
Major, 1989). From this perspective, it is surprising that targets of
Elizabeth C. Pinel, Department of Psychology, University of Texas at
Austin.
This research was supported by National Science Foundation Grant
SBR-9319570. Portions of this research were conducted in partial fulfill-
ment of my doctoral dissertation at the University of Texas at Austin.
I thank Jennifer Bosson, Bob Helmreich, Kelli Keough, Blake Shepard,
and especially Bill Swann for their helpful comments on an earlier version
of this article. I also thank Janet Spence for her invaluable input during the
early stages of this work. Finally, I thank Janel Seagal and Demetrice Davis
for their assistance in the data collection phase of Study 3.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Elizabeth
C. Pinel, who is now at the Department of Psychology, Pennsylvania State
University, 543 Moore Building, University Park, Pennsylvania 16802.
Electronic mail may be sent to ecp6@psu.edu.
stereotypes would ever think that their stereotyped status does not
influence how people treat them.
Empirical research corroborates the claim that targets of stereo-
types recognize that their group membership plays a role in how
people interact with them. For instance, when attractive students
received positive feedback, they were more likely to attribute the
feedback to their appearance when their evaluator could see them
than when their evaluator could not (Major, Carrington, &
Camevale, 1984). Similarly, when participants with cosmetically
applied facial scars interacted with a confederate, they later re-
ported that the scar influenced their interaction—even when the
scar had surreptitiously been removed prior to the interaction
(Kleck & Strenta, 1980)!
Of course, targets of stereotypes do not always interpret their
experiences in terms of their group membership. One important
determinant of this interpretive tendency seems to be the percep-
tions of the probability of being stereotyped. In one test of this
hypothesis, women learned that they would soon receive evalua-
tions from eight male judges and that either 100%, 75%, 50%,
25%,
or 0% of these judges were known to have discriminated
against women (Ruggiero & Taylor, 1997, Study 1). These women
then received a poor evaluation (i.e., a grade of F on a test of their
future prospects). Only when the women were certain that their
evaluators were sexist did they attribute their poor performance to
discrimination. Other, more naturalistic methods for increasing the
perceived probability of being stereotyped seem to have the same
effect. For example, women who anticipated being the sole woman
in a group expected to be stereotyped more than women who did
not expect to occupy such a solo status (Cohen & Swim, 1995).
There may also be stable individual differences in the extent to
which targets expect to be stereotyped or discriminated against.
For example, targets who remain largely insulated from out-group
members would have few occasions on which to reflect on their
stereotyped status (e.g., McGuire & McGuire, 1981). Presumably,
such targets should perceive less of a probability of being stereo-
typed than targets who were raised in a community composed
primarily of out-group members (see Crosby, 1982; Major, 1994).
Whether the perceived probability of being stereotyped is situ-
114
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
... Questionnaire (Pinel, 1999) 0.74-0.81 ...
... Furthermore, four of the scales focused on individuals who may be the targets of stigmatisation. The Stigma Consciousness Questionnaire (Pinel, 1999), Internalised Stigma of Mental Illness (Ritsher & Phelan, 2004) and the Stigma Scale (King et al., 2007) explore the individual experience of stigmatisation. In addition, the Questionnaire on Anticipated Discrimination (Gabbidon et al., 2013) focuses on measuring anticipated discrimination. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Stigma can have a significant impact on the lives of those inflicted, and stigmatisation can occur at any time when individuals commonly label behaviours and characteristics of others as offensive. Many attributes, such as homosexuality, ethnicity, race, and mental illness, have been explored using existing instruments. However, there are currently no standardised measures of stigma towards specific behaviours such as naturism. Naturism is the practice of public nudity without any intention of sexual stimulation. Though a global phenomenon, data suggest that almost seven million individuals in the UK alone identify as naturists. The rising figures of those engaging in stigmatised behaviour and the negative representations of this in the media contribute to the necessity for standardised instruments to measure stigma towards naturism. Method: The Naturism Stigma Scale (NSS) was adapted from the Depression Stigma Scale. This 18-item scale has two subscales which measure personal and perceived stigma. The sample consisted of 449 participants recruited by convenience sampling. Results: The analysis indicated that the scale had high reliability for both subscales (Personal Cronbach's α = 0.91, Perceived Cronbach's α = 0.93). It can be concluded that the NSS is a reliable psychometric instrument. Conclusions: Overall, this research assists in further understanding the stigma towards this minority group. Further research is necessary to explore the psychometric properties of NSS among different community samples.
... Gamers frequently perceive these criticisms as politically motivated forms of scapegoating (Ferreira, 2019), particularly when broader issues, such as gun control or social inequalities, are overlooked in favor of blaming VVGs (Kramer & Harlan, 2019;Missiaen, 2023;Phelps, 2018). This resentment can extend to scientific research on VVGs, as gamers may conflate academic findings with biased political rhetoric, fostering mistrust and defensive reactions toward the research community (Major & O'Brien, 2005;Pinel, 1999). And this sense of being unfairly judged by research can trigger defensive responses, such as motivated reasoning (e.g., Chung et al., 2019;Kumar, 2017;Sanjuan et al., 2012), where individuals dismiss research that they see as a threat to their social identity by questioning the credibility and motives of the scientists behind it (Altenmüller et al., 2021). ...
Preprint
We explore how habitual players of violent video games (VVGs) form perceptions of researchers and engage with further scientific literature on this topic when exposed to studies linking VVGs to increased aggression. Across two experimental studies (Total N = 1,576), we examined whether these players exhibit biased judgments of researchers' competence and friendliness and whether they demonstrate selective exposure to further research. In Study 1, participants were presented with fictional, standardized research summaries that varied only in their conclusions about the effects of VVGs. In Study 2, real published studies were used to provide ecological validity. Results from both experiments revealed that habitual players formed more negative perceptions of researchers when the researchers’ conclusions conflicted with their beliefs. Causal inference and mediation analyses showed that these judgments were causally driven by participants' agreement with the conclusions, independent of the researchers’ methods or credentials. Furthermore, habitual players demonstrated selective exposure, showing less interest in engaging with research that concluded VVGs increase aggression. This disengagement was partially explained by their negative evaluations of researchers' competence. These findings reveal a reinforcing cycle in which biased perceptions of VVG researchers drive avoidance of opposing evidence, ultimately reducing engagement with the broader body of VVG research.
... Es wird darum empfohlen, für eine systematische Altenberichterstattung zum historischen Wandel des sehr hohen Alters in NRW wenige Kriterien festzuschreiben, die in zukünftigen Erhebungen miterfasst werden sollen, um auch über Dieser Bereich der Qualität des eigenen Lebens für Andere wird in der sozialwissenschaftlichen Lebensqualitätsforschung bislang nur selten betrachtet (Neise et al., 2019). Doch beschäftigt es Personen durchaus, wie Andere über sie denken, und wünschen sie sich, positiv bewertet zu werden (Finkelstein et al., 2013;Frey et al., 2006;Pinel, 1999;Vorauer, 2001;Vorauer et al., 1998;Vorauer et al., 2000). Externale Bewertungen hängen zudem mit weiteren Aspekten von Lebensqualität zusammen. ...
... In addition, our measure of stigma included an item that measured how stigma has changed over time in one's country, which may have introduced noise into the measurement by conflating past and present perceptions of stigma. Future research should measure present-day experiences of stigma using established measures such as stigma consciousness (Pinel, 1999) or expectations of rejection (Meyer, 2003). More generally, while our focus was on the LGBTIQ+ context, our theorising may extend to other disadvantaged groups, including women and racial or ethnic minorities. ...
Article
The stigma faced by gender and sexual minorities varies across countries. How does stigma relate to collective action for social change? We propose a curvilinear link (i.e., an inverted U-curve) between stigma and collective action such that increased stigma is associated with increased collective action up to a point, beyond which, increased stigma is associated with reduced collective action. Using Bayesian inference with a sample of over 3,000 lesbian, gay, bisexual, transgender, intersex, and queer (LGBTIQ+) individuals across 25 countries, analyses showed that at the between-country level (but not at the within-country level), there was preliminary evidence of a curvilinear effect. There was also an indirect effect of institutional stigma on collective action outcomes via perceived stigma, suggesting that structural-level factors can trickle down to shape individuals' social change intentions and behaviours. These findings raise the importance of reconsidering the assumption of linearity between stigma and collective action.
... For example, psychologists have argued that 'concealable stigmatised identities' shape stigma in important ways that are not present in contexts where the stigmatised trait or identity cannot be hidden (Quinn and Earnshaw, 2013). For example, whether a person conceals or discloses their stigmatised identity in particular settings, and how this aspect of the person's identity fits within their overall sense of self, shapes how psychological distress is experienced (Pinel, 1999). ...
Article
Full-text available
Patient work refers to the quotidian labour undertaken by individuals to manage health, often unrecognised by health systems. This article argues that anticipated and received stigma and inclusivity labour comprise additional forms of patient work specific to minority populations. We draw on a case study of gay and bisexual men's experiences accessing healthcare services related to HIV prevention and testing in New South Wales (NSW), Australia's most populous state. Although new HIV diagnoses have reduced in NSW, these declines have not been uniformly observed. This study aimed to understand experiences of stigma related to accessing healthcare amongst two priority populations identified in the state's HIV strategy: gay and bisexual men who are young or who are living in regional and outer metropolitan suburbs. We interviewed 32 participants in 2023, recruited via social media advertisements and email invitations, and analysed data thematically. Our findings emphasise how disclosure of sexual orientation and/or HIV status operates as a form of inclusivity labour, in which patients look for cues from health providers that disclosure will be safe and respected. Other forms of patient work required to navigate access to HIV prevention services included finding appropriate providers likely to prescribe HIV pre-exposure prophylaxis (PrEP) and managing service refusal from general practitioners. Patient work appeared to also be compounded by intersecting issues of anticipated and vicarious stigma, unavailability of sexual health services in regional areas, long waiting times, and increased costs of healthcare services. Although experiences of enacted stigma in healthcare were infrequently reported, interview accounts suggested that participants undertook extensive patient work to minimise or avoid stigmatising encounters with health providers. Focusing on patient work in the context of stigma illuminates the labour of underserved populations required to access safe and culturally competent healthcare services (including HIV prevention and testing), suggesting areas of unmet need from health systems.
Article
Social network dependence refers to the behaviour where individuals pay excessive attention to social networks, leading to negative physiological and psychological consequences. This study intended to explore the relationships and mechanisms of internalized homonegativity, sexual orientation concealment, expectations of rejection, self‐esteem and social network dependence among young Chinese gay men. A web sample of 510 young Chinese gay men completed a self‐report questionnaire on internalized homonegativity, sexual orientation concealment, expectations of rejection, self‐esteem and social network dependence. The results showed that internalized homonegativity was positively correlated with social network dependence. The associations between internalized homonegativity and social network dependence were mediated by sexual orientation concealment, expectations of rejection and self‐esteem. In addition, sexual orientation concealment and self‐esteem played a chain mediation role between internalized homonegativity and social network dependence, and expectations of rejection and self‐esteem played a sequential mediation role between internalized homonegativity and social network dependence. The study is conducive to understanding the contributing factors and mechanisms of social network dependence.
Article
A clear and stable notion of one's abilities in a field is an important predictor of interest and motivation. The present study examines whether the development of this clear and stable ability self‐concept is undermined for people facing negative stereotyping because of exposure to daily evaluation threats. As such, the present study examines a novel explanation for the leaky pipeline phenomenon that members of negatively stereotyped groups disproportionally opt out of fields. A daily diary study among 205 female psychology students ( N = 1390 data points) showed that awareness of ingroup‐stereotyping related to more daily fear that others negatively evaluate their statistical abilities. This daily evaluation threat related negatively, within and across days, to fluctuations in clarity and certainty of the ability self‐concept—indicating that the ability self‐concept is more unstable for people experiencing more daily evaluation‐threat. Lower clarity and certainty of the ability self‐concept was, in turn, related to lower statistics interest and motivation, speaking to the importance of a clear and stable ability self‐concept for goal pursuit.
Article
Objective Stigma influences perceptions of mental illness and novel diseases like coronavirus disease-2019 (COVID-19), often impeding healthcare access despite advancements in medical treatment. This study compares the stigma associated with COVID-19 and mental illness to identify factors that could help reduce stigma.Methods An online survey was conducted in May 2023 among 1,500 participants aged 19 to 65 in South Korea, using a panel from Embrain, an online survey service. The survey assessed stigma and distress related to COVID-19 and mental illness using a validated questionnaire. It collected demographic and clinical data, evaluated COVID-19-related stigma, fear, and knowledge, and measured prejudice and attitudes toward psychiatric treatment. Personality traits were assessed using the Big Five Inventory (BFI)-10. Factors significantly associated with stigma scores were entered into linear regression analysis.Results COVID-19-related stigma scores were significantly negatively correlated with knowledge of COVID-19 and positively correlated with fear of infection and age. Individuals with a history of COVID-19 infection had significantly lower scores on COVID-19-related stigma. Similarly, mental illness stigma was negatively correlated with knowledge of psychiatric treatment and positively correlated with age, as well as conscientiousness and neuroticism in the BFI. Personal experience with individuals with mental illness was associated with significantly lower stigma scores.Conclusion This study suggests that the mechanism-making stigma was similar in cases of traditional mental illness and a novel infectious disease. Both cognitive and experiential factors influence stigma. Educating the public about the disease and enabling interactions with affected individuals emerge as effective strategies for stigma reduction.
Article
Full-text available
In this article, we attempt to distinguish between the properties of moderator and mediator variables at a number of levels. First, we seek to make theorists and researchers aware of the importance of not using the terms moderator and mediator interchangeably by carefully elaborating, both conceptually and strategically, the many ways in which moderators and mediators differ. We then go beyond this largely pedagogical function and delineate the conceptual and strategic implications of making use of such distinctions with regard to a wide range of phenomena, including control and stress, attitudes, and personality traits. We also provide a specific compendium of analytic procedures appropriate for making the most effective use of the moderator and mediator distinction, both separately and in terms of a broader causal system that includes both moderators and mediators. (46 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
National sample data were analyzed to describe the identification and class consciousness of people with working- and middle-class occupations, race consciousness of blacks and whites, sex consciousness of women, and age consciousness of people sixty years or older. Stratum consciousness, an ideology about the position of the stratum in society, includes a sense of power discontent, evaluation of the legitimacy of its position, and the view that collective action is the best means to realize its interests. Comparisons of the two sets of subordinate and superordinate strata showed that only along the racial dimension was identification more widespread in the lower-power stratum, but that power discontent and collectivist orientations were stronger among both identified blacks and the working class than among identified whites and the middle class. Comparisons among the identified members of lower-power strata additionally demonstrated that consciousness was strongest among blacks, weakest among women, and moderate among people with working-class occupations and older people. Multivariate analyses further showed that identification, power discontent, and rejection of legitimacy influence the collectivist orientations of women and blacks additively, rather than interactively.