ArticlePublisher preview available

Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) Comprehend the Referential Character of the Human Pointing Gesture

American Psychological Association
Journal of Comparative Psychology
Authors:
  • The Dolphin Institute
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

The authors tested a dolphin's (Tursiops truncatus) understanding of human manual pointing gestures to 3 distal objects located to the left of, to the right of, or behind the dolphin. The human referred to an object through a direct point (Pd), a cross-body point (Px), or a familiar symbolic gesture (S). In Experiment 1, the dolphin responded correctly to 80% of Pds toward laterally placed objects but to only 40% of Pds to the object behind. Responding to objects behind improved to 88% in Experiment 2 after exaggerated pointing was briefly instituted. Spontaneous comprehension of Pxs also was demonstrated. In Experiment 3, the human produced a sequence of 2 Pds, 2 Pxs, 2 Ss, or all 2-way combinations of these 3 to direct the dolphin to take the object referenced second to the object referenced first. Accuracy ranged from 68% to 77% correct (chance = 17%). These results established that the dolphin understood the referential character of the human manual pointing gesture.
Journal
of
Comparative Psychology
1999, Vol.
113,
No. 4,
347-364
Copyright
1999
by the
American Psychological Association,
Inc.
0735-7036/99/S3.00
Dolphins
(Tursiops
truncatus}
Comprehend
the
Referential Character
of
the
Human Pointing Gesture
Louis
M.
Herman
University
of
Hawaii
at
Manoa
and
Kewalo
Basin Marine Mammal Laboratory
Sheila
L.
Abichandani,
Ali N.
Elhajj,
Elia
Y. K.
Herman,
and
Juliana
L.
Sanchez
Kewalo
Basin Marine Mammal Laboratory
Adam
A.
Pack
Kewalo
Basin Marine Mammal Laboratory
and The
Dolphin Institute
The
authors
tested
a
dolphin's
(Tursiops
truncatus) understanding
of
human manual pointing
gestures
to 3
distal objects located
to the
left
of, to the right of, or
behind
the
dolphin.
The
human
referred
to an
object through
a
direct point
(Pd),
a
cross-body point
(Px),
or a
familiar
symbolic gesture
(S).
In
Experiment
1,
the
dolphin responded correctly
to 80% of Pds
toward
laterally placed objects
but to
only
40% of Pds to the
object behind. Responding
to
objects
behind
unproved
to 88% in
Experiment
2
after exaggerated pointing
was
briefly instituted.
Spontaneous comprehension
of Pxs
also
was
demonstrated.
In
Experiment
3, the
human
produced
a
sequence
of 2
Pds,
2
Pxs,
2 Ss, or all
2-way
combinations
of
these
3 to
direct
the
dolphin
to
take
the
object referenced second
to the
object referenced
first.
Accuracy ranged
from
68% to 77%
correct (chance
=
17%).
These
results
established
that
the
dolphin
understood
the
referential character
of the
human manual pointing gesture.
Manual
pointing
(use
of the
deictic gesture)
by
humans
serves
to
call another's attention
to an
object,
an
event,
or a
place
of
interest
to the
pointer.1
Human pointing
is
thus
a
social
triadic
transaction, involving
the
coordination
of the
attention
of the
pointer
and the
observer
to the
same target
or
event.
Implicit
in
adult human pointing behavior
is not
only
the
intent
to
manage
the
attention
of
another individual
but
also
an
expectation that
the
individual understands
the
referring
function
of the
pointing behavior. Likewise,
the
human
observer understands
the
intent
of the
pointer
to
direct attention toward
a
particular object, event,
or
place.
The
understanding
of the
referential
function
of the
Louis
M.
Herman, Department
of
Psychology, University
of
Hawaii
at
Manoa
and
Kewalo Basin Marine Mammal Laboratory,
Honolulu, Hawaii; Sheila
L.
Abichandani,
Ali N.
Elhajj,
Elia
Y. K.
Herman,
and
Juliana
L.
Sanchez, Kewalo Basin Marine Mammal
Laboratory, Honolulu, Hawaii; Adam
A.
Pack, Kewalo Basin
Marine Mammal
Laboratory,
Honolulu,
Hawaii,
and The
Dolphin
Institute, Honolulu, Hawaii.
Ali
N.
Elhajj
is now at the
Department
of
Psychology, University
of
Hawaii
at
Manoa.
Elia
Y. K.
Herman
is now at the
Independent
Studies Concentration, Brown University.
This project
was
funded
in
part
by
grants
from
Earthwatch
and
its
Research
Corps;
by financial
support
from
The
Dolphin
Institute;
and by
equipment donations
from
Apple Computer,
Inc.,
and
Mitsubishi,
Inc.
We
thank
the
staff,
students, interns,
and
volunteer
participants
at
Kewalo Basin Marine Mammal Laboratory
who
contributed
to
this
project.
We are
grateful
to
Matthias
Hoffmann-Kuhnt,
who
pre-
pared
the
computer images from
the
digital
videotape
record.
Correspondence concerning this article should
be
addressed
to
Louis
M.
Herman, Kewalo Basin Marine Mammal Laboratory,
1129
Ala
Moana Boulevard, Honolulu, Hawaii
96814.
Electronic
mail
may be
sent
to
lherman@hawaii.edu.
human
manual pointing gesture follows
a
developmental
progression
in
infants.
Lempers (1979) noted that beginning
at
9
months
of
age,
infants
were able
to
direct their attention
to
objects
50 cm
from
the
extended
arm and
index
finger
of
an
adult
but did not
understand points
to
objects
at a
distance
of 2.5
m
until
the age of 12
months. Lempers
thus
distinguished
between
prereferential
comprehension,
hi
which
the
object
and the
pointing
finger
must
be
close together,
and
referential
comprehension,
in
which
the finger can be
distant
from
the
object.
Butterworth
(1991)
and
Butterworth
and
Grover
(1988,
1990)
reported
findings
similar
to
those
of
Lempers:
Infants
younger than
12
months usually only
looked
at the
extended
finger or at
objects near
it
(see
also
Desrochers,
Morissette,
&
Ricard,
1995),
but
beginning
at
about
12
months, they could
follow
the
direction
of the
pointing
finger
toward more distant objects. Furthermore,
at
15
months,
infants
were able
to
disregard nearby objects
in
the
path
of the
point
to
look
at
more distant objects.
Morissette, Ricard,
and
Decarie
(1995)
generally
confirmed
this
progression, although they
found
a
somewhat longer
developmental
period.
At 12
months,
infants
attended primar-
ily
to
objects close
to the
pointer's hand. Attention
to
more
distant objects
did not
emerge until approximately
15
months
of
age.
The findings of
Morissette
et
al.
were based
on
a
longitudinal
study,
whereas those
of
Butterworth
and
Grover
were based
on a
cross-sectional approach.
In
addi-
tion,
Morissette
et al.
considered
nonresponses
by the
infants
(trials
on
which
the
infants
offered
no
response
to the
adult's
point)
as
incorrect, whereas Butterworth
and
Grover
ex-
cluded nonresponses
from
their data.
The
more complete
data
set of
Morissette
et al. and the
greater control over
1
For a
general discussion
of
deictic
expressions
in
human
language,
see,
for
example, Anderson
and
Keenan
(1985).
347
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
... The problem of cracking animals' codes have become especially attractive since the great "linguistic" potential was discovered in several highly social and intelligent species by means of intermediary artificial languages. Being applied to apes, dolphins and grey parrots, this method has revealed astonishing mental skills in the subjects [9,10,34,21]. It is become possible to demonstrate that animals are capable not only of decision making and using the experience gained in new situations, but also of using simple grammatical rules, visual symbols and number-related skills. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
In this review we integrate results of long term experimental study on ant "language" and intelligence which were fully based on fundamental ideas of Information Theory, such as the Shannon entropy, the Kolmogorov complexity, and the Shannon's equation connecting the length of a message (l) and its frequency (p), i.e. l=logpl = - \log p for rational communication systems. This approach, new for studying biological communication systems, enabled us to obtain the following important results on ants' communication and intelligence: i) to reveal "distant homing" in ants, that is, their ability to transfer information about remote events; ii) to estimate the rate of information transmission; iii) to reveal that ants are able to grasp regularities and to use them for "compression" of information; iv) to reveal that ants are able to transfer to each other the information about the number of objects; v) to discover that ants can add and subtract small numbers. The obtained results show that Information Theory is not only wonderful mathematical theory, but many its results may be considered as Nature laws.
... Studying animals' grammatical and syntax abilities can help us to understand the gap between human linguistic rule-learning skills and those of non-humans (see for example [62,[101][102][103]). Recently, Sainburg and co-authors [64] analysed the sequential dynamics of songs from multiple songbird species and speech from multiple languages by modelling the information content of signals as a function of the sequential distance between vocal elements and revealed functionally equivalent dynamics governed by similar processes. The second approach, which is based on artificial intermediary languages ("language-training experiments"), uncovered significant "linguistic" and cognitive potential in some species, that contrasts limitations in understanding their natural communications [104][105][106]; see also reviews in [31,107,108]. The third approach applies ideas of information theory to studying animal communication. ...
Article
Full-text available
Simple Summary An information theory approach provides new methods for experimental studying and analysis of animals’ communication and behavioural sequences. One of the main problems in studying animal behaviour at different levels of the organisation, from individual to collective behaviour, is searching for a reliable criterion for evaluating the variability and complexity of patterns. The data-compression method based on ideas of Kolmogorov complexity allows us to spot regularities that are difficult to detect otherwise, and that can influence the complexity of behavioural sequences. The information-theory approach allows for a comparative analysis of ethograms and provides a quantitative way to distinguish between innate and experienced behaviour. This approach is focused on studying natural communications by measuring information transmission rates without attempting to decode signals. It has made it possible, in particular, to discover the existence of a developed symbolic “language” in leader-scouting ant species, based on the ability of these ants to transfer abstract information about remote events. Abstract Over the last 40–50 years, ethology has become increasingly quantitative and computational. However, when analysing animal behavioural sequences, researchers often need help finding an adequate model to assess certain characteristics of these sequences while using a relatively small number of parameters. In this review, I demonstrate that the information theory approaches based on Shannon entropy and Kolmogorov complexity can furnish effective tools to analyse and compare animal natural behaviours. In addition to a comparative analysis of stereotypic behavioural sequences, information theory can provide ideas for particular experiments on sophisticated animal communications. In particular, it has made it possible to discover the existence of a developed symbolic “language” in leader-scouting ant species based on the ability of these ants to transfer abstract information about remote events.
... The development of pointing comprehension in humans and nonhuman animals is likely a result of learning, social experience, and interactions (Miklósi & Soproni, 2006). A variety of nondomesticated mammalian taxa, including dolphins (Tursiops truncatus; Herman et al., 1999), elephants (Loxodonta africana; Smet & Byrne, 2013), bats (Pteropus; Hall et al., 2011), and sea lions (Zalophus californianus; Malassis & Delfour, 2015), have demonstrated following some form of human pointing. Several studies have examined the understanding of human pointing cues in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and other great apes, specifically in the object-choice task. ...
Article
Full-text available
A wide range of nonhuman animal species has been shown to be able to respond to human referential signals, such as pointing gestures. The aim of the present study was to replicate previous findings showing cats to be sensitive to human pointing cues (Miklósi et al., 2005). In our study, we presented two types of human pointing gestures-momentary ipsilateral (direct pointing) and momentary cross-body pointing. We tested nine rescue cats in a two-way object-choice task. On a group level, the success rate of cats was 74.4%. Cats performed significantly above chance level in both the ipsilateral and cross-body pointing conditions. Trial number, rewarded side, and type of gesture did not significantly affect the cats' performances in the experiment. On an individual level, five out of seven cats who completed 20 trials performed significantly above chance level. Two cats only completed 10 trials. One of them succeeded in eight and the other in six of these trials. The results of our study replicate previous findings of cats being responsive to human ipsilateral pointing cues and add additional knowledge about their ability to follow cross-body pointing cues. Our results highlight that a domestic species, socialized in a group setting, may possess heterospecific communication skills. Further research is needed to exclude alternative parsimonious explanations, such as local and stimulus enhancements. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
Full-text available
The ability to attribute attentional states to other individuals is a highly adaptive socio-cognitive skill and thus may have evolved in many social species. However, whilst humans excel in this ability, even chimpanzees appear to not accurately understand how visual attention works, particularly in regard to the function of eyes. The complex socio-ecological background and socio-cognitive skill-set of bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp.), alongside the specialised training that captive dolphins typically undergo, make them an especially relevant candidate for an investigation into their sensitivity to human attentional states. Therefore, we tested 8 bottlenose dolphins on an object retrieval task. The dolphins were instructed to fetch an object by a trainer under various attentional state conditions involving the trainer’s eyes and face orientation: ‘not looking’, ‘half looking’, ‘eyes open’, and ‘eyes closed’. As the dolphins showed an increased latency to retrieve the object in conditions where the trainer’s head and eyes cued a lack of attention to the dolphin, particularly when comparing ‘eyes open’ vs ‘eyes closed’ conditions, we demonstrate that dolphins can be sensitive to human attentional features, namely the functionality of eyes. This study supports growing evidence that dolphins possess highly complex cognitive abilities, particularly those in the social domain.
Article
Full-text available
Many argue that the animal understanding of human referential communication is a by-product of domestication. However, the domestication hypothesis is not unanimously supported as some nondomesticated species such as sea lions, dolphins, or African elephants perform well in the understanding of human pointing gesture. There is a need to study species with different levels of domestication across different taxa to understand the emerging communicative sociocognitive skills in animals that provide them with the ability to comprehend human-given cues. We conducted a pilot study to assess the performance of eight sledging reindeer following a commonly used human-given cue (a pointing gesture associated with gaze at the target and local enhancement) in a two-way choice task. Domestic reindeer are considered semicaptive, because of their husbandry system in free-ranging conditions, with limited control of their reproduction. We observed that the willingness of the reindeer to participate in the test was age-related, with the younger individuals which lack experience with humans being reluctant to approach the experimenters. Within the more experienced working sledging reindeer, two individuals showed excellent skills and followed the human-given cues 9 out of 10 times. Reindeer show great potential in following a human indication to locate hidden food with minimal training when properly tamed. The effect of previous experience with humans requires further investigation. This is the first demonstration in cervids of an ability to make use of experimenter-given cues in an object-choice task.
Article
Full-text available
Multimodal imitation of actions, gestures and vocal production is a hallmark of the evolution of human communication, as both, vocal learning and visual-gestural imitation, were crucial factors that facilitated the evolution of speech and singing. Comparative evidence has revealed that humans are an odd case in this respect, as the case for multimodal imitation is barely documented in non-human animals. While there is evidence of vocal learning in birds and in mammals like bats, elephants and marine mammals, evidence in both domains, vocal and gestural, exists for two Psittacine birds (budgerigars and grey parrots) and cetaceans only. Moreover, it draws attention to the apparent absence of vocal imitation (with just a few cases reported for vocal fold control in an orangutan and a gorilla and a prolonged development of vocal plasticity in marmosets) and even for imitation of intransitive actions (not object related) in monkeys and apes in the wild. Even after training, the evidence for productive or “true imitation” (copy of a novel behavior, i.e., not pre-existent in the observer’s behavioral repertoire) in both domains is scarce. Here we review the evidence of multimodal imitation in cetaceans, one of the few living mammalian species that have been reported to display multimodal imitative learning besides humans, and their role in sociality, communication and group cultures. We propose that cetacean multimodal imitation was acquired in parallel with the evolution and development of behavioral synchrony and multimodal organization of sensorimotor information, supporting volitional motor control of their vocal system and audio-echoic-visual voices, body posture and movement integration.
Article
Bottlenose dolphins are a large-brained, long-lived, highly social species, operating within a fission-fusion society characterized by broad multi-level social networks, extensive care giving and teaching of offspring, cooperative and diverse hunting tactics, long-term alliances, and learned vocal signals that broadcast an individual’s identity, can be used in referential exchanges and can be imitated by close associates. Observations of behavior and social interactions in the wild suggest that social cognition in the bottlenose dolphins is well developed. Over the past thrity years, experimental studies have revealed that bottlenose dolphins have decades long social memories of associates, can develop a broad concept of imitation that extends to arbitrary novel sounds and social behaviors presented in a variety of contexts as well as to self-initiated behaviors. Dolphins have also been shown to be able to learn about and appreciate the social requisites of cooperative behavior, can spontaneously understand the referential character of human-initiated social signals involving pointing and gazing, and can employ pointing productively in communicative exchanges with humans to achieve goals. Collectively, dolphin social-cognition abilities are sophisticated and similar in several aspects to those of other species living within complex social networks, such as elephants, chimpanzees, and humans.
Article
For this special issue in honor of Dr. Sarah (Sally) Boysen’s career, we review studies on point following in nonhuman animals. Of the 126 papers that we documented on this topic published since the publication of Povinelli, Nelson, and Boysen (1990, Journal of Comparative Psychology, 104, 203–210), 94 (75%) were published in the past 15 years, including 22 in the past 5 years, indicating that this topic is still an active area of interest in the field of animal behavior and cognition. We present results of a survey of publication trends, discussing the species tested and the sample sizes, and we note methodological considerations and current multilaboratory approaches. We then categorize and synthesize the research questions addressed in these studies, which have been at both the ultimate level (e.g., questions related to evolutionary adaptiveness and phylogenetic differences) and proximate level (e.g., questions related to experiential and temperamental processes). Throughout, we consider future directions for this area of research.
Article
Full-text available
The spontaneous index finger and other referential pointing in 3 adult, laboratory chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) who have not received language training is reported. Of 256 total observed points, 254 were emitted in the presence of a human to objects in the environment; therefore, the points were communicative. Indicators of intentional communication used by the subjects included attention-getting behaviors, gaze alternation, and persistence until reward. Thus, pointing by these chimpanzees was intentionally communicative. These data imply that perspective-taking and referential communication are generalized hominoid traits, given appropriate eliciting contexts. Index finger pointing was more frequent with the subjects' dominant hands. This study refutes claims that indexical or referential pointing is species-unique to humans or dependent on linguistic competence or explicit training.
Article
Full-text available
The ability of adolescent chimpanzees and 2- and 3-year-old children to use pointing gestures to locate hidden surprises was examined in two experiments. The results revealed that although young 2-year-old children appeared to have no difficulty extracting referential information from a pointing gesture (independent of gaze or distance cues) and spontaneously using it to search in specific locations, adolescent chimpanzees appeared to rely on cueconfiguration and distance-based rules. Thus, although these chimpanzees were trained to respond appropriately to the pointing gestures of a human by searching in a particular location, this ability did not easily generalize to situations in which the distance between the pointing hand and the location were more distal. Furthermore, even those chimpanzees that were able to generalize in this fashion appeared to use distance-based cues, not ones based on an appreciation of the internal attentional focus or mental referent of the experimenter as indicated by his pointing gesture.
Article
This chapter discusses a group of young chimpanzees in a one-acre field. Three decades ago, Tolman declared that distance and direction are the fundamental concepts of behavior and that all of the important problems of psychology, save perhaps those involving language and society, could eventually be understood in essence by the analysis of the behavior of a rat at a choice point of a maze. Certainly, species variables are more important than the behaviorists of the 1930s admitted, and the maze is an arbitrary and possibly distorted model of the universe. Tolman's lone rat has simply evolved into a group of chimpanzees; the choice-point of a maze has become any given point in an outdoor area as large as the author could reasonably control and survey as a whole with the techniques at his disposal. The major advantage of a spatial, geometrical, or mechanical approach to nature is that its questions, methods, and concepts fully utilize the complexity of the real world in their very construction but are themselves extremely simple and general.
Chapter
The chapter discusses on what is known of the receptive competencies of animals in language like tasks. The emphasis is on the extensive work with apes, as well as the recent work with marine mammals, bottle-nosed dolphins. Sign language projects have given little attention to receptive competencies. Generally, data from these studies are insufficient for judging the degree to which apes understand the signs of their trainers. The most extensive attempt to quantify sentence comprehension in apes was that by Premack. The chimps Sarah, Peony, and Elizabeth were tutored in a system in which plastic symbols of arbitrary shape and color were used to represent objects, properties, and actions within an artificial language. Sentences could be constructed, by the experimenter or by the chimp, by arranging the symbols in a linear array on a board. There is evidence for an asymmetry of comprehension and production in the development and maintenance of language in humans. During early childhood, comprehension generally precedes and exceeds production. The ability of dolphins to understand imperative and interrogative sentences expressed within artificial acoustic or gestural languages is examined. Two young female bottle-nosed dolphins (Tursiops truncatus), housed together in a large seawater tank, were tutored in the artificial languages.
Book
The sonar of dolphins has undergone evolutionary re-finement for millions of years and has evolved to be the premier sonar system for short range applications. It far surpasses the capability of technological sonar, i.e. the only sonar system the US Navy has to detect buried mines is a dolphin system. Echolocation experiments with captive animals have revealed much of the basic parameters of the dolphin sonar. Features such as signal characteristics, transmission and reception beam patterns, hearing and internal filtering properties will be discussed. Sonar detection range and discrimination capabilities will also be included. Recent measurements of echolocation signals used by wild dolphins have expanded our understanding of their sonar system and their utilization in the field. A capability to perform time-varying gain has been recently uncovered which is very different than that of a technological sonar. A model of killer whale foraging on Chinook salmon will be examined in order to gain an understanding of the effectiveness of the sonar system in nature. The model will examine foraging in both quiet and noisy environments and will show that the echo levels are more than sufficient for prey detection at relatively long ranges.
Chapter
All species confront the problem of converting each new generation into the old generation. All the capabilities of the parents—values, knowledge, and skills—must somehow be transferred to the newborn; and this transfer must take place in a constrained period of time: enough of the generation must survive to produce—and even, in some cases, provide parental care for—the next generation.
Chapter
This chapter describes current research in the area of “short term,” or, “working memory” in animals. The memory processes provide a model for the actual functioning of working memory in the “real lives” of many animals, and learn about temporal contingencies in their environment and make appropriate, adaptive response decisions. When the same initial stimuli are used and the memory functions differ, they indicate different memory processes or prospective memory processing with differing degrees of stimulus specificity. The direct procedures involve (1) cues presented in advance of alternative test conditions or trial outcomes, with the attendant use of the probe procedure; (2) changes in the number of initial stimuli or differences among them; and (3) differential opportunities to practice remembering.