ArticlePublisher preview available

When Exemplification Fails: Hypocrisy and the Motive for Self-Integrity

Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

Two experiments investigated how the dissonance that follows a hypocritical behavior is reduced when 2 alternatives are available: a direct strategy (changing behavior to make it less hypocritical) or an indirect strategy (the affirmation of an unrelated positive aspect of the self). In Experiment 1, after dissonance was aroused by hypocrisy, significantly more participants chose to reduce dissonance directly, despite the clear availability of a self-affirmation strategy. In Experiment 2, participants again chose direct resolution of their hypocritical discrepancy, even when the opportunity to affirm the self held more importance for their global self-worth. The discussion focuses on the mechanisms that influence how people select among readily available strategies for dissonance reduction.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
1997,
Vol. 72. No. 1. 54-65Copyright 1997 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.
0O22-3514/97/S3.O0
When Exemplification Fails: Hypocrisy and the Motive for Self-Integrity
Jeff Stone
Princeton University
Andrew W. Wiegand
University
of
California, Santa Cruz
Joel Cooper
Princeton University
Elliot Aronson
University
of
California, Santa Cruz
Two experiments investigated how the dissonance that follows a hypocritical behavior is reduced
when 2 alternatives are available: a direct strategy (changing behavior to make it less hypocritical)
or an indirect strategy (the affirmation of an unrelated positive aspect of the self). In Experiment 1,
after dissonance was aroused by hypocrisy, significantly more participants chose to reduce dissonance
directly, despite the clear availability of a self-affirmation strategy. In Experiment 2, participants
again chose direct resolution of their hypocritical discrepancy, even when the opportunity to affirm
the self held more importance for their global self-worth. The discussion focuses on the mechanisms
that influence how people select among readily available strategies for dissonance reduction.
How
do
people choose
to
reduce
the
discomfort that arises
from
a
discrepancy between behavior
and
important attitudes
or self-beliefs?
In
this article
we
investigate
the
reduction
of
psychological discomfort from
the
dissonance theory tradition
(Festinger, 1957;
see
E. Aronson, 1992; Cooper
&
Fazio,
1984;
Steele, 1988). Dissonance theorists maintain that discrepancies
between behavior and belief arouse dissonance (i.e., psychologi-
cal discomfort), which
in
turn motivates activities designed
to
reduce
the
arousal. Dissonance reduction occurs through
a
vari-
ety
of
strategies that,
for our
purposes,
can be
classified into
one
of
two general categories.
First, psychological discomfort
can be
reduced through
a di-
rect strategy—one that involves altering elements
of the dis-
crepant cognitions. For example, people can change
the
discrep-
ant attitude
or
self-belief
(J.
Aronson, Blanton,
&
Cooper,
1995),
make compensation
for the
unwanted behavior (e.g.,
Sherman
&
Gorkin, 1980), reduce
the
importance
of
either
discrepant cognition (i.e., trivialization,
see
Simon, Green-
berg,
&
Brehm, 1995),
or
distort perceptions
of the
circum-
stances that
led to the
discrepancy (i.e., claim unforeseeability
of or low volition
for
the behavior;
see
Cooper & Fazio, 1984).
Jeff Stone and Joel Cooper, Department of Psychology, Princeton
University; Andrew W. Wiegand and Elliot Aronson, Department of
Psychology, University of California, Santa Cruz.
This research was partially supported by a grant from the University
of California Universitywide AIDS Research Program and by a National
Science Foundation predoctoral fellowship award to Andrew W. Wie-
gand, who conducted Experiment I for partial satisfaction of the Master
of Science degree awarded by the University of California, Santa Cruz.
We wish to express our sincere appreciation to Mike Nurse and Nathan
Effron for acting as the experimenters in Study 1 and to Stephen Wright
for his help in collecting the self-concept measures at the University of
California, Santa Cruz. We also extend our thanks to Hart Blanton, Beth
Bennett, Adam Galinsky, Claude Steele, Eddie Harmon-Jones, and Peter
Gollwitzer for their comments on the research presented in this article.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jeff
Stone, Department of Psychology, Princeton University, Princeton, New
Jersey 08544.
Direct strategies
for
dissonance reduction
can be
distinguished
from
a
second general class
of
strategies we call indirect, which
involve activities that reduce dissonance without altering
ele-
ments
of the
discrepancy. These include mis attributing
the dis-
comfort to something other than the discrepancy (Zanna & Coo-
per,
1974) or
reflecting
on
other valued aspects
of the
self
{Steele
& Lui,
1983; Tesser
&
Cornell, 1991).
The
fact that
direct
and
indirect strategies have been shown
to be
effective
for dissonance reduction suggests that people have tremendous
flexibility when
it
comes
to
selecting
a
strategy
for
dissonance
reduction.
Or
do
they?
The
mere fact that
a
multitude
of
strategies will
reduce dissonance
may not
indicate that
any one
strategy
for
reduction
is
just
as
good
as
another. Indeed, there
is
evidence
indicating that when several options
are
available, people show
preferences
for
using some strategies over others (e.g., Gotz-
Marchand, Gotz, & Irle, 1974; Olson & Zanna, 1979; Scheier
&
Carver, 1980). Festinger (1957) noted that selection
of a
mode
of dissonance reduction
may
depend
on a
number
of
factors,
such as the importance
of
the cognitions underlying
the
discrep-
ancy or the circumstances that
led
to
the
discrepant act.
It
seems
likely that even when multiple alternatives
for
dissonance reduc-
tion
are
available, factors related
to the
discrepancy
and its
antecedent conditions might constrain
the
choice people make
for
how to
reduce their psychological discomfort.
The research
in
this article focuses
on how
people reduce
the
dissonance that follows
a
specific type
of
discrepancy—an
act
of hypocrisy
(E.
Aronson, Fried,
&
Stone,
1991;
Dickerson,
Thibodeau, Aronson,
&
Miller,
1992;
Stone, Aronson, Crain,
Winslow,
&
Fried, 1994).
Our
goal
was to
investigate
how
much flexibility
a
person
who
commits
a
hypocritical
act has
for reducing dissonance when multiple strategies
are
available.
Clues
to
understanding dissonance reduction
in
this case
may
lie
in the
antecedents that lead
to a
hypocritical discrepancy.
Hypocrisy
and the
Failed Exemplifier
A bout with hypocrisy begins when, perhaps through
an act
of moral self-presentation, (i.e., exemplification;
see
Gilbert
&
54
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
... Often, this is explored in terms of moral cleansing and moral licensing (Mullen & Monin, 2016;Sachdeva et al., 2009), or moral balancing (e.g., Ploner & Regner, 2013). Similar research has been conducted in the context of hypocrisy and dissonance reduction (Stone et al., 1997;Stone & Focella, 2011), and in terms of collective action against injustices (e.g., Lodewijkx et al., 2008). Together, these different lines of research (which we refer to under the umbrella of moral self-regulation) suggest that individuals are likely to feel their MSC is threatened when they (or their in-group) are implicated in immoral behavior (e.g., Monin et al., 2008;Sachdeva et al., 2009;Tetlock et al., 2000). ...
Article
Full-text available
Recycling is a fickle behavior: most people think of recycling as important and that it is the “right thing to do,” but they are also quick to justify not doing it. Recent messaging has highlighted recycling failures as a tool for individuals to recycle more and recycle better. But is confronting individuals with past failures an effective method of behavior change? According to research on moral self-regulation, acting immorally, or failing to act morally, threatens a person’s moral self-concept, often motivating compensatory behavior (such as increased recycling). To test this model, participants were randomly assigned to watch a short video highlighting poor levels of recycling or to a control condition. Results showed that participants in the video condition reported greater moral threat and greater guilt, relative to those in the control condition. This effect was stronger for individuals who believed more strongly that recycling is a moral issue. Mean levels of recycling intent did not differ by condition; however, path analyses indicated that moral threat and guilt mediated the relationship between moral beliefs and recycling intent for those in the video condition, but not for those in the control condition. Results are interpreted within a broader framework of moral self-regulation.
... On the contrary, some studies reported that poor oral health literacy was associated with poor oral health choices such as nocturnal bottle feeding and frequent provision of cariogenic snacks [32]. Whilst it is normally perceived that an individual cannot give what s/he does not have, there are many people who teach good practices despite the fact that they still indulge in poor practices themselves [33][34][35] . ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Oral health of preschool children remains a concern globally. Primary healthcare providers are in a vital position to support preventive oral care programmes. This study explored current practices, perception and barriers of primary health care professionals towards oral health promotion program of children in Qatar. Methods The qualitative research used focus group discussions and interviewed a total of 108 participants that were audio recorded and transcribed verbatim. Four major themes emerged and were analysed to explore contextual patterns within the data. Results Participants acknowledged the high prevalence of caries in children and identified the causes in the local context which included parental practices, poor dietary habits, impact of culture lack of oral health knowledge, limitations in the healthcare system, and negative role of the media. However, complex barriers were exposed, including lack of time and ownership, system coordination between organizations, and lack of policy. Conclusion Health professionals and bureaucrats involved in decision-making held a positive attitude towards oral health prevention programs and were enthusiastic to initiate and support these programs.
... Relatedly, threats to self-enhancement can occur based on how others who are close to them present themselves. As Jones and Pittman (1982) theorized, self-presentation tactics such as boasting, intimidation, and exemplification (i.e., attempting to appear as morally superior) all carry social comparison risks-with the latter of these specifically drawing possible charges of hypocrisy (Stone et al., 1997). Moreover, Alicke et al. (2012) found that the air of superiority evinced by hypocrites is an important factor in judgements of hypocritical behavior. ...
Article
Full-text available
Few studies have focused on the conditions in which individuals perceive hypocrisy in others. The current study introduces and tests the Motivated Appeal to Hypocrisy (MAtH) hypothesis. This hypothesis examines core social psychological motivational threats and asks (a) whether these are related to the accounts of individuals in charging others with hypocrisy, and (b) whether these perceptions of hypocrisy are associated with reductions in the persuasiveness of persons targeted as hypocrites. Study 1 ( N = 201 ) was based on qualitative coding of stories and revealed, as expected, that violations of core social motives involving belongingness, understanding, control, self-enhancement, and trust are involved in participants’ stories of hypocrisy. Study 2 ( N = 237 ) used a multilevel correlational approach and demonstrated that violations of core social motives significantly predict perceptions of hypocrisy and the rejection of a person’s message or advice. The relation between social motive violations and message rejection was mediated by perceptions of hypocrisy.
Article
Full-text available
Research has shifted from examining the harmful effects of workplace deviance to focusing on preventing or mitigating it through a perpetrator-centered perspective. However, findings on whether perpetrators subsequently engage in reparative behaviors remain inconsistent. We rely on moral cleansing theory to postulate that deviant peer pressure is a significant contextual factor in determining perpetrators’ future moral choices. We propose that when deviant peer pressure is low, employees are more likely to experience guilt after perpetrating workplace deviance, thereby inducing their helping behaviors. Simultaneously, employees would also perceive a loss of moral credits, resulting in reduced engagement in workplace deviance. Empirical evidence from an experiment and a three-wave field study involving multiple sources supports our hypotheses. These findings offer novel insights into resolving conflicting results in the workplace deviance literature and highlight the crucial role of deviant peer pressure in shaping responses to workplace deviance. We also discuss theoretical and practical implications.
Article
This study explores the effectiveness of AI chatbots in public safety advertising campaigns against marijuana-impaired driving, specifically focusing on the role of hypocrisy induction, gender, and linguistic styles. In our online experiment, participants engaged with a chatbot delivering either hypocrisy-inducing or neutral messages about driving under marijuana influence. The chatbot, characterized as male or female, utilized either formal or casual language. Results revealed that hypocrisy induction is most effective when delivered by a casually speaking female chatbot or a formally speaking male chatbot. The heightened guilt among participants explains the effectiveness of this approach. The Language Expectancy Theory (LET) serves as a theoretical framework, elucidating how the alignment of chatbot gender and linguistic style enhances the hypocrisy induction effect. This research highlights the potency of AI chatbots in leveraging public safety advertising campaigns and offers guidance on optimizing their design by aligning gender and language style with campaign goals.
Article
Compensatory consumption theory posits that individuals tend to engage in status consumption as a response to experiencing upward social comparison. However, it is unclear whether upward social comparison can influence status consumption in four lifestyle domains: clothing, eating, living, and travelling. Therefore, we conducted two separate studies with a sample of Chinese consumers. In Study 1, we investigated the effects of upward social comparison on status consumption in four domains by comparing participants with a specific individual. In Study 2, we examined the effects of upward social comparison on status consumption in four domains by comparing participants with a group. Additionally, Study 2 examined the mediating role of perceived self‐improvement and perceived superiority. The results revealed that participants exhibited higher purchase intentions and preferences for status products and services in four consumption domains when exposed to upward social comparison. Furthermore, the results also indicated that perceived self‐improvement and perceived superiority mediated the positive effects of status consumption in four domains under upward social comparison. These findings contribute to the broader applicability of compensatory consumption theory.
Chapter
This chapter considers when and under what conditions will laws lead to attitudinal and behavioral change in the long run. First, it considers the conditions necessary for continued compliance through acquiescence, identification, and internalization. Second, it considers the conditions under which individuals who may have complied with the law through acquiescence (fear of legal sanction) or weak identification (fear of social sanctions) can be motivated to comply with laws voluntarily (through strong identification and/or internalization). In the process, it identifies the social and institutional conditions that societies and policymakers can focus on to create a general culture in favor of legal compliance. As importantly, it highlights the limits of using only laws and formal enforcement mechanisms for creating lasting attitudinal change. In fact, as the chapter discusses in the last sections, over-emphasizing the role of law and legal enforcement can have the perverse effect of undermining conditions necessary for future compliance with the law.
Chapter
This chapter utilizes the integrated approach to compliance developed in Chaps. 2 and 3 to understand the role and limits of legal expression in changing behaviors and attitudes. After briefly describing the existing theoretical and empirical research on legal expression, it embeds the explanation within the integrated approach. It is evident that legal expression can create compliance through weak and strong identification within the integrated approach. However, this exercise also reveals the conditions necessary for legal expression to change behaviors and attitudes. Why mere expression has so effectively changed behaviors and attitudes in some contexts but not others can be better understood when we take into account the social and institutional conditions in which this interaction takes place. As importantly, the chapter highlights how legal expression (both when successful and unsuccessful) changes the conditions in which individuals will interact with future laws. The advantages, disadvantages, and success of legal expression as a tool for behavioral change, therefore, cannot be estimated without taking into account a more integrated approach.
Article
Full-text available
Brand activism, i.e., taking a public stance towards sociopolitical issues, is a controversial strategic marketing tool for firms. Prior work shows that at best, it has no significant positive impact on consumer support and outcomes for brands. In this research, we propose and explore how advice-giving impacts consumer brand activism support and brand liking. Across four studies we find that advice-giving leads to more successful brand activism by increasing brand activism support behavior, and also has benefits for the brand. We show that advice-giving drives higher levels of normative influence which leads to higher brand activism support behavior. We also study a boundary condition and show that even when brands do not follow consumers’ advice, advice-giving has no negative behavioral and attitudinal consequences for the brand. Overall, we contribute to literature on brand activism and advice-giving and help marketers in strategically and effectively designing their brand activism activities.
Article
Full-text available
Recent experiments involving hypocrisy have been theorized to be a form of cognitive dissonance induction. As such, hypocrisy would constitute a new experimental paradigm for dissonance induction. However, these hypocrisy studies are open to alternative explanations. The present study attempts to rule out these alternative explanations by providing independent evidence that hypocrisy is a form of cognitive dissonance arousal. In the present study, a 2×2 design manipulates whether hypocrisy is induced and whether subjects are given an opportunity to misattribute their arousal to external factors. If hypocrisy is a form of dissonance, the misattribution manipulation should reduce behavior changes brought on by the induction of hypocrisy. The results support the predictions.
Article
Full-text available
This experiment applied a new twist on cognitive dissonance theory to the problem of AIDS prevention among sexually active young adults. Dissonance was created after a proattitudinal advocacy by inducing hypocrisy-having subjects publicly advocate the importance of safe sex and then systematically making the subjects mindful of their own past failures to use condoms. It was predicted that the induction of hypocrisy would motivate subjects to reduce dissonance by purchasing condoms at the completion of the experiment. The results showed that more subjects in the hypocrisy condition bought condoms and also bought more condoms, on average, than subjects in the control conditions. The implications of the hypocrisy procedure for AIDS prevention programs and for current views of dissonance theory are discussed.
Article
Self-affirmation processes are being activated by information that threatens the perceived adequacy or integrity of the self and as running their course until this perception is restored through explanation, rationalization, and/or action. The purpose of these constant explanations (and rationalizations) is to maintain a phenomenal experience of the self-self-conceptions and images as adaptively and morally adequate—that is, as competent, good, coherent, unitary, stable, capable of free choice, capable of controlling important outcomes, and so on. The research reported in this chapter focuses on the way people cope with the implications of threat to their self-regard rather than on the way they cope with the threat itself. This chapter analyzes the way coping processes restore self-regard rather than the way they address the provoking threat itself.
Article
In an early revision of Festinger's theory of cognitive dissonance, Aronson argued that dissonance was aroused when an important element of the self-concept was violated. More recently, Cooper and Fazio's "New Look" version of the theory maintains that dissonance is the result of feeling personally responsible for an aversive outcome. This article examines the claims of these two positions, ultimately arguing for the superiority of Aronson's formulation. The authors maintain that research findings in support of the New Look can also be explained by the self-concept formulation. In addition, they argue that the twin postulates of the New Look position-personal responsibility and aversive consequences-lack adequate conceptual grounding without reference to the self-concept. Finally, previous research is examined that is incompatible with the New Look, as well as new data suggesting that aversive consequences are not necessary for dissonance arousal.
Article
In 1957, Leon Festinger's theory of cognitive dissonance burst on the scene and revitalized social psychology with its deft blend of cognition and motivation. For the next two decades, the theory inspired an extraordinary amount of exciting research leading to a burgeoning of knowledge about human social behavior. The theory has been referred to as "the most important single development in social psychology to date" (Jones, 1976, p. x). But, by the mid-1970s the allure of the theory began to wane as interest in the entire topic of motivation faded and the journals were all but overwhelmed by the incredible popularity of purely cognitive approaches to social psychology. Recently, social psychologists seem to have rediscovered motivation and several mini- theories have emerged blending cognition with motivation-in much the same way that Festinger did some 35 years ago. This article traces the history of these developments and attempts a synthesis of some of the newer theories with the dissonance research of the late 1950s and early 1960s.