A preview of this full-text is provided by American Psychological Association.
Content available from Journal of Experimental Psychology Animal Behavior Processes
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
Journal
of
Experimental
Psychology:
Animal
Behavior
Processes
1994, Vol.
20, No. 1,
51-65
Copyright
1994
by the
American
Psychological
Association,
Inc.
0097-7403/94/S3.00
Context-Specificity
of
Target
Versus
Feature
Inhibition
in a
Feature-Negative
Discrimination
Mark
E.
Bouton
and
James
B.
Nelson
Four experiments with
rats
examined
the
effects
of a
context switch
on
inhibition that
was
acquired
during
a
feature-negative discrimination.
A
target conditioned stimulus
was
paired
with food when
it was
presented alone
but
occurred without food when
it was
combined with
&
feature stimulus.
A
context switch following training
did not
disrupt inhibition conditioned
to the
feature. However,
responding
to the
target
was
more
difficult
to
inhibit when
it was
tested
in a
different context.
It
is
suggested that both
the
target
and the
feature acquired inhibition
and
that
the
target's
inhibition
was
especially
sensitive
to the
context.
The
feature
may
inhibit responding
to the
target
(a) by
directly suppressing
the
representation
of the
food
and (b) by
activating
the
target's
own
inhibitory
association with food, which
is at
least partly context-specific. Implications
for
theories
of
inhi-
bition
and
negative
occasion-setting
are
discussed.
The
results
of a
number
of
experiments suggest that
ex-
tinction
performance
is
relatively specific
to the
context
in
which
it is
learned.
For
example,
if
conditioned
stimulus
(CS)-unconditioned stimulus (US) pairings
are
given
in one
context
and
then
CS-alone
training (extinction)
is
given
in
another context,
a
return
of the CS to the
original condi-
tioning context
can
renew extinguished performance
to the
CS
(e.g., Bouton
&
Holies,
1979; Bouton
&
King,
1983;
Bouton
&
Peck, 1989). Although extinction performance
is
specific
to its
context, conditioning performance
is
much less
so.
When
the
context
is
switched
after
conditioning, there
is
often
relatively little diminution
of the
conditioned response
(CR)
(e.g.,
Bouton
&
King, 1983; Bouton
&
Peck, 1989;
Bouton
&
Swartzentruber,
1986; Hall
&
Honey, 1989; Kaye
&
Mackintosh, 1990; Kaye, Preston, Szabo,
Druiff,
&
Mackintosh, 1987).
It
may be
reasonable
to
suppose that
the CS
acquires
a
second association during extinction
and
that this association
is
especially sensitive
to the
context (Bouton, 1993).
One
possibility
is
that
the
animal learns
a new
inhibitory asso-
ciation with
the CS
(e.g.,
CS-no
US) in
extinction. Such
learning
has
been proposed
by
several conditioning models
(e.g.,
Konorski,
1967;
Pearce,
1987;
Pearce
&
Hall, 1980;
Wagner,
1981;
see
also Pavlov, 1927) that assume that non-
reinforcement
of the CS in
extinction will cause
acquisition
of
a
second inhibitory association instead
of the
unlearning
of
the
original
CS-US
association (e.g., Rescorla
&
Wagner,
1972). According
to
these models,
at the end of
extinction,
the
CS has
both excitatory
and
inhibitory values.
On
this
Mark
E.
Bouton
and
James
B.
Nelson,
Department
of
Psychol-
ogy,
University
of
Vermont.
This
research
was
supported
by
Grants
BNS
89-08535
and
IBN
92-09454
from
the
National
Science
Foundation.
Correspondence
concerning
this
article
should
be
addressed
to
Mark
E.
Bouton,
Department
of
Psychology,
University
of
Vermont,
Burlington,
Vermont
05405.
view,
the
context-specificity
of
extinction could come about
if
inhibition
is
relatively specific
to the
context
in
which
it
is
learned.
The
present experiments were therefore designed
to
test
whether conditioned inhibition
is
reduced with
a
change
of
context.
To
produce inhibition,
we
used
a
feature-negative
discrimination procedure.
In
this sort
of
procedure,
a
target
CS,
such
as a
tone,
is
paired with
a US
when
it is
presented
alone
(T+)
but is
presented
without
a US
when
it is
combined
with
a
"feature" stimulus, such
as a
light
(LT-).
The
feature-
negative procedure
is
widely known
to
produce inhibition
to
the
feature (L). Indeed,
it is
sometimes taken
as the
basic
means
of
producing conditioned inhibition (e.g., Rescorla
&
Wagner,
1972).
If
conditioned inhibition
is
specific
to its
context, then
a
context switch
after
feature-negative training
might
cause
a
loss
of
inhibition
to the
feature
CS.
Recent theories
of
conditioning, however, suggest that
the
target stimulus might also acquire some inhibition
in the
feature-negative
discrimination.
As is
expected when
a CS is
nonreinforced
in
extinction,
the
target itself might acquire
an
inhibitory
association when
it is
nonreinforced
in
compound
with
the
feature
in
this paradigm (e.g., Pearce
&
Hall, 1980;
Wagner,
1981). Thus, both feature
and
target
may
acquire
some inhibition when
the
compound
is
nonreinforced
in the
feature-negative
paradigm.
We
designed
the
present experiments
to
investigate
the
effects
of
switching
the
context
after
feature-negative dis-
crimination training.
We
used
an
appetitive conditioning
preparation
in
which
we
monitored
the
number
of
times
the
rat
entered
the
food
cup or
magazine (magazine
en-
tries) during
the CS
(e.g.,
Hall
&
Channell,
1985; Hall
&
Honey,
1990; Kaye
&
Mackintosh, 1990; Wilson
&
Pearce, 1990).
The
results suggest that inhibition
is
indeed
attenuated
when
the
context
is
switched
after
feature-
negative training. However,
in the
present experiments,
the
context switch reduced inhibition
to the
target
CS but not
inhibition
to the
feature.
The
results have implications
for
the
issue
of
what
is
learned
in
feature-negative discrimina-
tion learning.
51
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.