ArticlePDF Available

Does Scientific Research Change Minds? Linking Criminology and Public Perceptions of Policing

Authors:

Abstract

Research summary This study investigates the impact of scientific research findings on public views of policing topics. Specifically, we conducted an original survey experiment to determine whether research information treatments influence respondents’ views on the effectiveness of the police in reducing crime, defunding and refunding police budgets, and use of body‐worn cameras. Our results indicated that presenting confirmatory research information had a significant positive impact on perceptions of police effectiveness in reducing crime and use of body‐worn cameras compared to comparison groups. Conversely, presenting “negative” research information had a significant negative effect on these perceptions. Interestingly, neither positive nor negative research information treatments related to defunding versus refunding the police had a statistically significant impact on respondents compared to comparison groups, suggesting that research findings have limited effects on more ideologically complex policing topics. Policy implications Scientific research can effectively shape public perceptions of police effectiveness in reducing crime and the use of body‐worn cameras, but it has limited effects on politically charged issues, such as defunding and refunding the police. To enhance the impact of evidence‐based policing, we suggest that police administrators collaborate with researchers to evaluate new policies and disseminate these findings widely to the public. Additionally, researchers should strive to make their research more accessible to the general public, beyond academic journals, scientific conferences, and paywalls. We recommend using open‐access platforms, social media, and other media outlets to disseminate unbiased, evidence‐based research on policing that is digestible to the public.
1
Does Scientific Research Change Minds? Linking Criminology and Public Perceptions of
Policing
Research Summary:
This study investigates the impact of scientific research findings on public views of policing
topics. Specifically, we conducted an original survey experiment to determine whether research
information treatments influence respondents views on the effectiveness of the police in
reducing crime, defunding and refunding police budgets, and the use of body-worn cameras. Our
results indicate that presenting confirmatory research information has a significant positive
impact on perceptions of police effectiveness in reducing crime and use of body-worn cameras.
Conversely, presenting negative research information has a significant negative effect on these
perceptions. Interestingly, neither positive nor negative research findings related to defunding
versus refunding the police had a statistically significant impact on respondents, suggesting that
research has limited effects on more ideologically complex policing topics.
Policy Implications:
Scientific research can effectively shape public perceptions of police effectiveness in reducing
crime and the use of body-worn cameras, but it has limited effects on politically charged issues,
such as defunding and refunding the police. To enhance the impact of evidence-based policing,
we suggest that police administrators collaborate with researchers to evaluate new policies and
disseminate these findings widely to the public. Additionally, researchers should strive to make
their research more accessible to the general public, beyond academic journals, scientific
conferences, and paywalls. We recommend using open-access platforms, social media, and other
media outlets to disseminate unbiased, evidence-based research on policing that is digestible to
the public.
Keywords: police budgets, body-worn cameras, police effectiveness, public perceptions of
police, survey experiment
2
INTRODUCTION
As one measure of scholarly success, scientific communities evaluate the ability of
individual scholars, journals, or disciplines to “impact” the broader communities in which they
operate. For example, Worrall and Gordon (2022) recently considered the impact of a flagship
American Society of Criminology journal (Criminology & Public Policy); they asked, Has its
scholarship reached the masses? Has it been influential from a policy standpoint?” (p. 840).
These are fundamental questions to answer for criminology broadly, as the topical concerns of
scholars are often located in critically important questions involving state policing power,
violence, trauma, and death (Clear, 2010). While the question of “impact” can be construed in
many ways, one obvious potential target of influence is the general public, which has at least
normative power to shape public policy related to policing in democratic locales (Nix et al.,
2021). Crime, and by extension policing, are areas of democratic contention, and many scholars
espouse a desire to inform these areas. Still, scholars harbor doubts about the accomplishments
of “public criminology” (Loader, Ian & Sparks, 2013), leading Austin (2003) to comment that
“…in terms of having any effect on criminal justice policy, there is little evidence that any
criminologist’s career has made much of a difference” (p. 557).
Criminologists did not stand idle in the face of these critiques, as scholars made urgent
calls for greater collective effort to translate criminology research into public action. Sherman
(2005) urged criminology scholars to consider public views of the field as one measure of how
the field was advancing. He suggested that the “future success of the field may depend on a
growing public image based on experimental results, just as advances in treatment attract funding
for basic science in medicine” (Sherman, 2005, p. 131). One example of this link between
experimental emphasis and the general publics view of criminology is the concerted effort to
3
develop a rigorous evidence base for the effectiveness of policing operations. The evidence-
based policing (EBP) movement is grounded in the belief that using scientifically-supported
methods and strategies can improve the effectiveness and efficiency of policing (Lum & Koper,
2017). A potential impact of EBP is that emerging scientific evidence can improve, or degrade,
public opinion on police-related subjects, with Sherman (2015) noting that EBP can “increase
public support for what police do” (p. 18). However, scholarship is limited as to whether
providing the public with knowledge of policing research actually impacts their perception of the
police.
In 2009, Clear (2010) spoke before the American Society of Criminology, declaring that
a new age of evidence-based criminology had arrived. He urged the Society to take a leading role
in shaping the use and nature of evidence in policymaking, emphasizing the importance of
making research available to policymakers and the general public. This call to action implies that
empirical evidence can inform and guide decisions made for the greater good. When it comes to
distribution, we know that social media can be an effective channel for disseminating research to
the public and potentially to policymakers (Worrall & Gordon, 2022). However, it remains
uncertain whether the public is even swayed by such evidence when it comes to issues
surrounding law enforcement and the criminal justice system. Recent findings reveal that certain
topics in policing, such as police shootings, confront deeply ingrained beliefs seemingly
impervious to factual information. For instance, Schiff et al. (2022) show that even when
presented with precise statistical data on police shootings, individuals beliefs about the rates of
these incidents remained unchanged and were primarily driven by political affiliations and race.
Similarly, Mourtgos and Adams (2020) observed that widening discrepancies between legal and
4
community standards for police use of force are correlated with non-policy factors such as
political ideology, race, and gender.
Using a multi-arm information provision survey experiment, we investigate whether
making respondents knowledgeable of police-related research can, in fact, impact their
perceptions of three salient policing topics: police effectiveness, police budgets, and body-worn
cameras (BWCs). We deploy our experiment across heads of households in the state of South
Carolina. Respondents were randomized and presented with either confirmatory, negative, or
mixed scientific evidence regarding these policing topics. Findings from our sample of roughly
1,800 South Carolinians suggest that in certain conditions, knowledge of research can impact
public perceptions of the police. Our findings have implications for researchers, including calls
for greater dissemination of research findings, and the importance of police-researcher
collaborations. Criminology research can bring value by informing the public of evidence-based
practices, thereby shaping public opinion. We begin by first reviewing the importance of public
opinion in shaping policy and current knowledge surrounding the impact of scientific evidence
on public opinion.
2. PERCEPTIONS OF POLICE IN AFFECTING POLICY CHANGE
Public perceptions of police matter a great deal, as public trust in police motivates pro-
social behavior and serves as a critical foundation for an effective criminal justice system (Tyler,
1990, 2004). While perceptions of the police may vary by civilians race (Pickett et al., 2022),
most Americans tend to be supportive and generally hold positive perceptions of the police
(Callanan & Rosenberger, 2011; Wentz & Schlimgen, 2012). For example, a nationally
representative poll recently found that about 69% of Americans hold a “great deal” or a “fair
5
amount” of confidence in police officers, which is in line with the 77% who possess positive
views toward scientists, and well above levels for journalists (40%) and elected officials (24%)
(Kennedy et al., 2022).
However, public views on law enforcement have declined in the aftermath of highly
publicized police misconduct in recent years, resulting in what some have deemed a police
legitimacy crisis. Following the killing of George Floyd in 2020, some scholars have suggested
that anti-police sentiment has reached unprecedented levels (Brenan, 2020; Cassella et al., 2022;
Reny & Newman, 2021; Washburn, 2023; White et al., 2021). In support of this claim, scholars
have linked increases in public hostility to increased numbers of officers leaving agencies
(Mourtgos et al., 2022), and even a short-lived increase in the number of officers shot on duty
(Sierra-Arévalo et al., 2023). Thus, police may be experiencing greater hostility, animosity,
resistance, and violent assaults from civilians daily (Boehme & Kaminski, 2023; Sierra-Arévalo
et al., 2023), while simultaneously struggling to maintain a staffed police force in order to
effectively serve the public (Archbold, 2022).
The growing polarization of American politics, as well as the emergence of the COVID-
19 pandemic, has arguably made matters worse for law enforcement. Political leanings seem to
be a key driver in public perceptions and support of police (Brown, 2017; Fine et al., 2019; Liu
& Cheng, n.d.; Mourtgos & Adams, 2020). Americans are generally skeptical of governmental
power (Cook & Gronke, 2005) and distrust in public and private institutions has increased over
time (Citrin & Stoker, 2018; Kennedy et al., 2022). The pandemic further perpetuated mistrust
and scrutiny of governmental action (Airoldi & Vecchi, 2021). Policethe most visible agents
of governmental coercive power (Bittner, 1990; Lipsky, 1973)may bear the brunt of scrutiny
that is inherently directed at other governmental entities (Reiner, 2010; Sharp & Johnson, 2009).
6
Given this context, it is imperative to understand what factors may influence the public’s
perceptions of police, particularly during a police legitimacy crisis (Nix & Wolfe, 2017; Todak,
2017) and in an era of misinformation (Brashier & Schacter, 2020). It is possible that providing
the public with evidence-based findings about police may shape public opinion of police
(Bennell et al., 2021), which would be noteworthy in today’s political climate. In this
circumstance, Clear’s (2010) message of using evidence to inform policymaking becomes
actionable, given that the public can be swayed by evidence in updating their opinions, including
opinions about police.
2.1 Information provision of criminal justice research on public perceptions
It is unclear whether research evidence can alter public opinion within the larger
scientific context. The COVID-19 pandemic, for instance, challenged the scientific community
in its attempts to influence the public to make sound health decisions regarding the COVID-19
virus (McLaughlin et al., 2021). Many civilians continued their everyday lives, rejecting peer-
reviewed scientific knowledge about the virus, benefits of mask-wearing, and vaccines/boosters
(Eberl et al., 2021). Thus, while scientists investigated the negative effects of unhealthy lifestyles
and the COVID-19 virus, empirical research did not sway all Americans' perceptions, beliefs,
and behaviors (Kroke & Ruthig, 2022). Similarly, despite scientific evidence that points to the
negative effects of smoking, unhealthy diets, and a lack of exercise, Americans continue to
ignore this scientific knowledge (Kabat, 2017). Cigarette smoking and obesity are two leading
causes of preventable deaths in the United States (Wang et al., 2020).
Scholars have previously examined the impact of information provisions on public
perceptions of police effectiveness in executing their duties, BWCs, and police reform (Demir,
7
2019; Pickett & Ryon, 2017; Singer & Cooper, 2009; Vaughn et al., 2022). For example,
Donovan and Klahm IV (2018) found that presenting respondents about wrongful convictions
impacted respondents’ willingness to admit that police misconduct (e.g., obtaining false
confessions through threat or use of force) may contribute to the problem of wrongful
convictions. With respect to BWCs, Demir (2019) distributed surveys to drivers who were pulled
over by police who were randomly assigned BWCs, and found that drivers who were pulled over
by police wearing BWCs had improved perceptions of officer behavior, treatment, and lower
beliefs that police are corrupt and/or lawless.
Turning attention to police reforms, Vaughn and colleagues (2022) experimentally
surveyed Americans by presenting the slogan (e.g., “defund the police”) and substance
(describing to respondents the goals of each movement) of the police reform, defunding the
police, and abolishing the police movements. Other than the police reform movements, the
authors found that respondents largely did not support the defund and abolish the police
movement in both slogan and substance (see other work on criminal justice reforms in Pickett &
Ryon, 2017). Schiff and colleagues (2022) experimentally informed respondents about the
reported number of police shootings in their city and asked about their support of five proposed
police reforms. The findings revealed that providing statistics on police shootings did not impact
respondent support for policies around police reform. Instead, political partisanship and race
were the key driving factors regarding support of police reform, and the presentation of scientific
evidence did not influence deeply rooted beliefs about police reforms. It may be that deeply
rooted and ideologically based beliefs are harder to change with the presentation of scientific
evidence (see other relevant experiments about various policing topics Mullinix et al., 2021;
Mummolo, 2018a, 2018b; Nix et al., 2021; Wozniak et al., 2021). Given some of the mixed
8
findings, more research is needed on how the presentation of information and scientific evidence
affects public perceptions of police.
It is relevant to note that other studies have used information presentation to assess the
impact on public perceptions of other criminal justice related topics, such as sexual assault
myths, punitive attitudes towards offenders, the death penalty, criminal justice system
effectiveness, and crime prevention strategies (Bohm et al., 1991; Bohm & Vogel, 2004;
Cochran & Chamlin, 2005; Indermaur et al., 2012; McMahon, 2010; O’Donohue et al., 2003;
Pickett et al., 2020; Wozniak et al., 2022). For instance, focusing on the death penalty, Norris
and Mullinix (2020) experimentally presernted wrongful conviction exoneration statistics and
found that respondents’ support for the death penalty decreased, while trust in the criminal
justice system was also reduced. Providing wrongful conviction narratives (e.g., stories about
wrongfully convicted individuals) influenced attitudes toward the death penalty and support for
police reform but had little effect on trust in the criminal justice system. In relation to restrictive
housing, Rydberg and colleagues (2018) randomly presented criminal justice actor experiences
or a summary of research findings regarding the struggles of residence restrictions for sex
offenders as a means of assessing its effect on public perceptions of the effectiveness of such
laws. Regardless of experimental condition, participants were unmoved by the stimuli and still
had high levels of support for residence restriction laws (also see Novick et al., 2022 for study on
a similar topic).
3. DATA AND METHODS
In the interest of further understanding how scientific evidence shapes public opinion, the
present study uses a survey experiment designed to test whether providing various police
9
research information influences public opinion on three timely topics: 1) police effectiveness in
reducing crime, 2) perceptions of police budgets, and 3) perceptions of body-worn cameras.
America has experienced a recent surge in violent crime rates nationally (Brantingham et al.,
2021), with some scholars promoting public health approaches as a solution compared to police
initiatives (Cerdá et al., 2018). Calls to defund and abolish the police reached new levels in the
summer of 2020, and though much waned, remain a nationally relevant and politically charged
topic (Baranauskas, 2022; Vaughn et al., 2022). Further, body-worn cameras were originally
aimed at improving police accountability and reducing use of force (Lum et al., 2016), though
the impact of this technology has been uneven (Gaub & White, 2020; Lum et al., 2019).
Substantiating the link between scholarly research on these topics may help police
administrators, policy makers, and researchers in efforts to shape public opinion of the police.
We test two related hypotheses on the causal relationship between scientific evidence and
public opinion on policing topics.
H1: Respondents exposed to confirmatory research findings (versus mixed research
findings) about police effectiveness, budgetary questions, and body-worn cameras
will express more positive views of those subjects.
H2: Respondents exposed to negative research findings (versus mixed research findings)
about police effectiveness, budgetary questions, and body-worn cameras will express
more negative views of those subjects.
3.1 Data and distribution
To test the above hypotheses, we conducted an original survey using statewide head of
household contact data from Mailers Haven (2022), a third-party mailing list provider. Mailers
10
Haven makes multiple attempts per year to verify the accuracy of household addresses and
identifies the head of household using various databases (e.g., United States Postal Service).
Addresses that cannot be verified, unoccupied households, and households that place themselves
on “no contact” lists are excluded. Additionally, we are able to see the age, race, email addresses
(if available), and zip code of each head of household. The initial dataset provided a total of
1,723,480 head of household mailing addresses in South Carolina.
1
Within this data, 680,745 of
the mailing addresses had an email address for the head of household, which is the sampling
frame for this study. Although this sampling frame may look different that the full list of all
households (including those without email addresses), there is evidence to suggest that our
probability sample that excluded those without email addresses still elicits externally valid
results, even if there are slight demographic differences between the two groups (Keeter &
McGeeney, 2015; McMaster et al., 2017; Patten & Perrin, 2015).
Invitations to participate were emailed via Qualtrics starting on October 12, 2022 and
ended on November 12, 2022 with four periodic reminder emails sent throughout the timeframe.
Of the original emails, 650,154 were delivered (though an unknown number were seen, and
many can be assumed to have landed in spam folders). A total of 2,094 respondents started the
survey (0.32%), while 1,814 completed the survey (87% completion rate).
2
Among those who
were administered the survey but did not complete it, the mean age was 52, with 79%, 16%, and
4% identifying as White, Black, and Hispanic, respectively. Alternatively, those who took the
survey reported an average age of 58, with 88%, 8%, and 3% identifying as White, Black, and
1
According to the United States Census Bureau (2022), there are a total of roughly 1,976,447 households in the
state of South Carolina, so the list captures about 87% of these households.
2
We conducted an a priori test in G*Power assuming an effect size of .10 (small effect), an alpha level of .05, and 3
groups. The results revealed that a sample size of 969 (which is half the achieved sample size) would ensure greater
than 80 percent statistical power to detect the main effects of the treatments.
11
Hispanic, respectively. Essentially, those who completed the survey were older and identified
more often as White versus Black (there was an equivalent percentage of Hispanic individuals in
both groups) than those who started the survey and did not complete it.
We relied upon the American Association of Public Opinion Research calculator
(AAPOR, 2023) to estimate a response rate using the RR2 calculation, which came to 0.3%.
Although a low response rate, studies have shown that the response rate is not linked to
nonresponse bias (Pickett et al., 2018; Pickett, 2017). Further, based on an internal beta-testing
experiment, we found that only 20% of study team affiliates who were sent emails using the
same distribution method received the invitation email in their primary inbox, highlighting that
our response rate is likely higher among those who actually received an invitation (i.e., it did not
go to spam or an alternative folder). Also, we used an experimental survey method to ensure that
a low response rate does not interfere with the ability to derive accurate estimates of the causal
link between our treatments and respondents’ perceptions. However, that link rests on the
assumption that the observed characteristics of respondents are balanced across treatments. We
find that respondents' characteristics were well-balanced and report those results in Appendix
Table A1.
Respondent characteristics are reported in Table 1. Our average respondent was a 59-
year-old white male with a four-year college degree and self-identifying as having a conservative
ideology. The average respondent was married, without children in the home, and had not been
the victim of a crime in the last twelve months. These respondent characteristics do contrast to
the latest South Carolina US Census demographics, whereby 51.4% of the population identifies
12
as female, while 68.6% identified as White, with a median age of 39.7
3
. As such, we are careful
about generalizing the findings to the larger South Carolina population.
[Insert Table 1 about here]
3.2 Research design and survey development
The study's outcome measures focused on three themes: 1) police effectiveness in
reducing crime, 2) police budgets, and 3) BWCs. Prior to administering questions about each
topic, all respondents were randomly presented with either "confirmatory," "negative," or
"mixed" research findings. Respondents were randomly assigned research findings within each
topic, such that respondents received an information treatment for each topic and answered
questions about each topic after being presented with the randomized treatments. Respondents
were presented with the research information once on its own page and then a second time at the
top of the page above the relevant themed questions. Note in Table 2, that we highlighted
“buzzwords” in all caps to draw the attention of the respondent to the key research findings. The
"mixed" treatment was employed as the control condition, representing exposure to mixed
scientific evidence on policing that the general public is likely to encounter. We recognize that
using a control condition with no information may have implied complete unawareness of
policing operations, which is unlikely for the general public. Table 2 reports each experimental
condition. The information treatments were kept brief, to reflect how many Americans consume
information through platforms like Twitter (Beck et al., 2017).
Following the randomized treatment, respondents were asked questions to evaluate their
perceptions of each topic. Treatment varied for each respondent, based on the outcome of
3
According to Pew Research Center (2023), about 62% of South Carolinians identify as conservative.
13
interest. For example, one respondent might receive confirmatory research findings about police
effectiveness in reducing crime, followed by questions about perceptions of police effectiveness.
Subsequently, the same respondent might be provided with a confirmatory, negative, or mixed
research finding about police budgets, followed by questions about police budgets. This process
was repeated throughout the survey. Question ordering was also randomized for each outcome of
interest, so that respondents were asked questions about police effectiveness, budgets, and BWCs
in a varied order.
[Insert Table 2 about here]
To develop the survey items on police legitimacy, trust in police, police effectiveness,
police budgets, body-worn cameras, and demographics, we synthesized previous empirical
literature on these topics. We conducted a pilot test of the survey at the University of South
Carolinas Patient Engagement Studio (PES), which assembles a diverse and representative
group of patients (i.e., South Carolinian residents) to provide feedback on the survey, discuss
survey dissemination logistics, and offer any other suggestions for the project. Following the
feedback from the PES, we made the necessary modifications to the survey and conducted a
second pilot test with various faculty, graduate students, and undergraduate students from
different departments within the university. The survey was then updated once more in
consideration of the pilot results and feedback received. The study, including the treatment
structure, was approved by the University’s Institutional Review Board.
3.3 Variables
Dependent variables
14
We created four dependent variables by combining items into indices measuring
respondent perceptions of police effectiveness, police funding (defund/more funding), and
BWCs. Scale reliability was assessed using Cronbach’s alpha (Santos, 1999). Police
effectiveness was measured by combining five survey items: 1) police play a key role in
preventing crime, 2) police play a key role in maintaining law and order, 3) police are effective
at fighting crime, 4) the more visible police are in my neighborhood, the safer it is, and 5) more
police in my neighborhood makes my neighborhood safer (a = 0.924). Refund police was
measured with three survey items: 1) more money from my local government should be allocated
to hiring, retaining, and training police officers, 2) police officers in my neighborhood should be
paid more, and 3) more money from my local government should be spent on providing officers
in my neighborhood with more equipment (e.g., vehicles, technology, safety equipment (a =
0.856). Defund police was measured with three survey items: 1) money should be taken from my
local police department’s budget and given over to social services, 2) in general, I agree with the
aims of the “defund the police” movement, and 3) in general, I agree with the aims of the
“abolish the police” movement (a = 0.857). Perceptions of BWC were measured using five
survey items: 1) BWCs should be worn by all police officers, 2) I would feel safer in my
neighborhood if I knew police officers were wearing BWCs, 3) I trust police actions more when
I know they wear a BWC, 4) using BWCs will make officers act more professionally, and 5) the
use of BWCs will reduce complaints against the police (a = 0.833). Survey items were measured
on a five-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree), such that higher values
represent greater perceptions of police effectiveness, more support for funding the police, more
support for defunding the police, and more support for BWCs, respectively.
Independent/Control Variables
15
The main independent variable of interest was whether the respondent received a
confirmatory, negative, or mixed research finding before each block of questions, denoted as
confirmatory information, negative information, and mixed information. Each condition was
coded as a dummy variable, with the mixed condition used as the reference category. We also
added several observational control variables to sharpen parameter estimates in the model,
account for theoretically important effects, and test for heterogeneity in the treatment effects
(Kern et al., 2016).
We controlled for respondent pre-existing perceptions of police procedural justice,
obeying the police, and trust in police, which were a series of questions asked at the beginning of
the survey before the experiment began. Each of the measures was drawn from previous research
operationalizing these concepts (Pickett et al., 2018; Pryce et al., 2017). Police procedural justice
was measured by combining six survey items, where respondents were asked if police 1) treat
everyone equally, 2) clearly explain the reasons for their actions, 3) treat people with dignity and
respect, 4) treat people fairly, 5) respect people’s rights, and 6) listen to suspects before making
any decisions about how to handle a case (α = 0.961). Police legitimacy was captured in the form
of both obedience and trust. Respondent legal orientation to obey the police and law was
measured using four survey items: 1) people should obey the law even if it goes against what
they think is right, 2) I always try to follow the law even when I think it is wrong, 3) you should
do what the police tell you even if you disagree, and 4) you should accept police decisions even
if you think they are wrong (α = 0.802). Trust in police was measured using four survey items: 1)
the police protect people’s basic rights, 2) the police are generally honest, 3) most police officers
do their jobs well, and 4) the police can be trusted to do what’s right for my neighborhood (α =
0.940). Responses were on a 5-point Likert scale (strongly disagree to strongly agree), such that
16
higher values on each of these indices suggest greater procedural justice perceptions, obedience
toward the police and law, and perceptions of trust in the police.
We also asked about respondents’ political leanings, gender, race, education, age and if
the respondent had been a victim of a crime in the past year. A long line of previous research
expects these factors might contribute to baseline differences in an individual’s views on
criminal justice matters, and are commonly included in scholarship interested in respondent’s
views on policing matters (e.g., Metcalfe & Pickett, 2022). For example, Mourtgos and Adams
(2020) find that race, political ideology, and education were all significantly related to
respondents’ views on the reasonableness of police use of force. Similarly, we asked respondents
about their personal experience with crime in the last 12 months because of the known
relationship between crime victimization and views on crime (Unnever et al., 2007).
4. RESULTS
We used the following general model specification to identify treatment effects of
interest:
𝑂𝑢𝑡𝑐𝑜𝑚𝑒 = 𝜷𝟎+ 𝜷𝟏𝐶𝑜𝑛𝑓𝑖𝑟𝑚𝑖𝑛𝑔 + 𝜷𝟐𝑁𝑒𝑔𝑎𝑡𝑖𝑣𝑒 + 𝑿  + 𝝐
where Confirming, and Negative corresponded with the two treatments providing information on
scientific evidence framed around the outcomes, compared to a mixed treatment condition. We
identified treatment effects on the four outcome variables of interest described above, and X
referred to the vector of covariates that we included, namely the demographic characteristics of
our respondents, their self-identified partisanship leanings, whether they have been the victim of
a crime in the last twelve months, and their perceptions of police procedural justice, obeying the
law, and trust in police.
17
We report the controlled results of the experiment in Table 3 below. After listwise
deletion of observations with missing information on the outcomes and control variables, the
final sample size was 1,519 respondents. Recognizing that the inclusion of control variables can
introduce bias (Berk et al., 2013; Freedman, 2008; Lin, 2013), the uncontrolled experimental
results are also reported in Appendix Table A6. Results were consistent between the two models,
though we elected to concentrate on our controlled model given the interesting findings
regarding respondent perceptions, demographics, and experience. Variance inflation factors
(VIF) suggest low potential for multicollinearity, with VIF results for each model reported in
Appendix Table A7. In general, we find that when presented with either confirming or negative
scientific information related to police effectiveness and BWCs, respondents’ perceptions of
those topics were altered. However, we failed to support the hypothesis that respondents
perceptions related to “defunding” or “refunding” the police were altered when presented with
scientific information that conflicted or supported those policy options. These results are
reviewed in detail below.
[Insert Table 3 about here]
4.1 Perceptions of effectiveness and body-worn cameras are causally altered by scientific
information
We found experimental evidence that the public’s views on police effectiveness are
causally altered through exposure to information about scientific findings on the subject. In
short, public opinion on police effectiveness was more positive when exposed to confirmatory
scientific information, and more negative when exposed to information that undercuts police
effectiveness. Our analysis supported the hypothesis that respondents beliefs about police
effectiveness were significantly affected by the provision of relevant scientific information. Both
18
the confirming information (beta = 0.13, 95% CI [0.06, 0.20], p < .001; ß = 0.07, 95% CI [0.03,
0.11]) and negative information (beta = -0.18, 95% CI [-0.25, -0.11], p < .001; ß = -0.10, 95% CI
[-0.13, -0.06]) experimental treatments were significant in the hypothesized directions. Effect
sizes for the experimental treatments were interpreted as very small (confirming information) and
small (negative information) using guidelines suggested by Funder and Ozer (2019).
We found a similar pattern in the effect of scientific information regarding BWCs on how
the public views these cameras. Public opinion on BWCs was more positive when presented with
confirmatory scientific information, just as it was more negative when confronted with negative
scientific information. Our analysis supported the hypothesis that respondents’ beliefs about
police body-worn cameras were significantly affected by the provision of scientific information
on BWCs, although the r-squared value suggests that other factors not accounted influence these
perceptions. Both the confirming information (beta = 0.13, 95% CI [0.04, 0.22], p = 0.004; ß =
0.08, 95% CI [0.03, 0.14]) and negative information (beta = -0.23, 95% CI [-0.32, -0.14], p <
.001; ß = -0.15, 95% CI [-0.20, -0.09]) experimental conditions were significant in the
hypothesized directions. Effect sizes for the experimental treatments were interpreted as very
small (confirming information) and small (negative information).
4.2 “Defund” and “refund” perceptions are unaffected by scientific information
Based on our hypotheses, we expected uniform impact of scientific information on public
opinion regarding each of the policing topics. We rejected those hypotheses, in part, as our
experimental evidence showed a null effect on the highly politicized topic of police budgets (i.e.,
“defund” versus “refund” the police) (Baranauskas, 2022; Vaughn et al., 2022). However, we did
not find evidence that respondents beliefs about defunding the police were significantly affected
19
by the provision of relevant scientific information. The effect of confirming information was
statistically non-significant and negative (beta = -0.03, 95% CI [-0.11, 0.05], p = 0.422; ß = -
0.02, 95% CI [-0.06, 0.02]), and the effect of negative information was statistically non-
significant and negative (beta = -0.00, 95% CI [-0.08, 0.08], p = 0.995; ß = -0.00, 95% CI [-0.04,
0.04]).
Though effects were in the hypothesized direction, we did not find evidence that
respondents’ beliefs about refunding the police were significantly affected by the provision of
relevant scientific information. The effect of confirming information was statistically non-
significant and positive (beta = 0.05, 95% CI [-0.03, 0.13], p = 0.243; ß = 0.03, 95% CI [-0.02,
0.07]), and the effect of negative information was statistically non-significant and negative (beta
= -0.02, 95% CI [-0.10, 0.07], p = 0.714; ß = -0.00, 95% CI [-0.05, 0.03]).
4.3 Controls
As expected by previous literature, respondents’ pre-existing perceptions of police
procedural justice and legitimacy, including their belief in obeying the law and trust in the
police, had significant associations with our outcomes of interest. We caution that because these
were non-experimental variables, they cannot be interpreted as causally affecting the outcomes
of interest and instead are associational. Respondents who emphasized the importance of
obeying the law endorsed the effectiveness of police, rejected defunding, supported refunding the
police, and considered BWCs to be an important police tool. Higher levels of pre-existing beliefs
in police procedural justice was associated with endorsing the effectiveness of police and a
greater willingness to refund police. Finally, respondents with higher levels of trust in the police
20
endorsed police effectiveness, rejected defunding, supported refunding police, and believed that
BWCs are an important police tool.
At baseline levels, respondents in the control group who identified as moderate (b=
0.249, p < 0.001) or conservative (b= 0.310, p < 0.001) had significantly higher perceptions of
police effectiveness compared to liberals. Conservatives (b= -0.917, p < 0.001) and moderates
(b= -0.571, p < 0.001) were less supportive of the defund movement, and more supportive of
“refunding” through higher budgetary support for police. In comparison to liberals, these two
groups also did not view BWCs as an important police technology. Compared to males, females
were more supportive of both the defund (b= 0.187, p < 0.001) and refund (b= 0.121, p < 0.001)
policy options. Higher levels of education were associated with small but significant decreases in
the willingness to refund police (b= -0.035, p < 0.01) and importance of BWCs (b= -0.034, p <
0.05). Older respondents were more willing to endorse police effectiveness (b = 0.005, p
<0.001), “refunding” the police (b = 0.005, p <0.001), and BWCs (b = 0.004, p <0.001), while
less willing to support defunding police (b = -0.006, p <0.001), though these statistically
significant effect sizes were all very small. Contrary to expectations, respondents who had been
criminally victimized in the last twelve months did not significantly differ in any outcome
compared to those who had not been victimized.
4.4 Treatment heterogeneity by sex, race, and partisan identification
One possible story about our main findings is that observational variables were
interacting with the treatments to affect the results. For example, it is theoretically possible that a
null experimental result is masking small, statistically significant results that are roughly equal
and opposed, drawn along categorical differences in our sample such as sex, race, and partisan
21
identification (Kam & Trussler, 2017). Using interactive model specifications, we conducted
exploratory tests of heterogeneity to consider whether different characteristics of respondents
were associated with differential responses to information about scientific findings related to
policing. These tests were not designed to establish causal moderation, but rather are meant to
consider differences in treatment across different groups (e.g., heterogeneity tests), thus a parallel
estimation approach was not adopted (Bansak, 2021).
The gender gap in college completion is growing, as women ages 25 to 34 are more
likely to have a college degree (Parker, 2021). Therefore, we hypothesized that men and women
might update their priors about police differently when presented with our treatment. We found
little support for this hypothesis. As reported in Appendix Table A2, women and men largely
responded similarly to the informational treatments, except there does appear to be heterogeneity
in how female respondents, compared to male respondents, responded to negative scientific
information on the impact of BWCs (b = -.192, p < .05).
Next, we explored whether respondents with different political orientations responded
differently to informational treatments. Political orientation is a strong predictor of generalized
perceptions of police (Gamson & McEvoy, 2017; Pickett, 2016), but is also strongly related to
perceptions of scientific evidence. For example, researchers have found that liberals are more
trusting of scientific institutions and scientific findings compared to their conservative peers
(Agley, 2020), and this gap has been growing (Nadeem, 2020). As reported in Appendix Table
A3, we found little support for this hypothesis in our effectiveness, defund, or refund outcomes.
However, there does appear to be heterogeneity in how conservative-identifying respondents,
compared to liberal-identifying respondents, respond to negative scientific information on the
impact of BWCs (b=.308, p < 0.05).
22
Finally, we investigated whether there were differences in how white and non-white
respondents responded to our informational treatments. Research has convincingly demonstrated
there are racial divides in Americans’ trust of policing (Pickett et al., 2022) and scientific
institutions (Gramlich & Funk, 2020). One possible story about our main results, therefore, is
that there are differential responses to informational treatment between individuals identifying as
white versus non-white. As reported in Appendix Table A4, we found no evidence of treatment
heterogeneity by race. White and non-white individuals responded similarly to both confirming
and negative scientific information across all four outcomes.
In sum, while there was a baseline relationship between race, sex, political ideology, and
the outcomes in the main results, but heterogeneity tests did not indicate that our experimental
treatments differentially impacted the views of people of color, women, and
conservatives/moderates. However, we caution against strongly interpreting these exploratory
results. First, the relationship between partisanship and opinion is endogenously related to media
exposure, and shifts in supply and demand for national news has demonstrable impacts on how
that news is covered (Martin & McCrain, 2019). It may be that these types of effects would be
found in police-related coverage, as well as which types of scientific research would be sought
out or supplied. Analytically, our statistical power here is low to support the interactions
explored, and therefore it is possible that a true statistically significant, but small, effect is
masked. Taken together, these heterogeneity analyses should be considered exploratory only.
Future research interested in this specific result should be careful to construct very large samples
capable of detecting very small effects. Future studies might also consider causal moderation
(Bansak, 2021).
23
5. DISCUSSION
The present study employed an original experimental survey to assess the effect of
providing respondents with scientific research related to policing on their perceptions of law
enforcement. We found partial support for our two hypotheses, indicating that presenting
scientific information can impact how the public thinks about policing topics, but that effect
varies by the topic at hand. On specific issues, scientific research influenced respondents' pre-
existing beliefs about the police. For instance, we found that presenting confirmatory scientific
information about police effectiveness and body-worn cameras led respondents to express
support for the police as effective crime reducers and body-worn cameras as a useful tool for law
enforcement. Alternatively, more highly politicized policing issues, such as those related to
defunding or refunding the police, appeared to be resistant to change in the face of scientific
information. Criminologists should strive to develop an evidence base that is informative to
public discourse. However, it remains an open question whether criminological findings have the
power to affect public opinion. Our contribution to this debate is the provision of causal evidence
that the public can be responsive to generalized descriptions of competing scientific evidence, at
least in some relevant areas of policing research and in the short-term.
The findings of this study are important as they provide evidence that scientific research
can influence public opinion on policing topics. This is particularly relevant today, with issues
related to policing and law enforcement becoming highly politicized. The fact that scientific
information can impact beliefs about the police has important implications for policymakers and
law enforcement agencies. The use of scientific research could potentially lead to more informed
decision-making and policy development in the field of policing, as politicians are aware of
public preferences on certain criminal justice policies (Vaughn et al., 2022). For example,
24
politicians are generally likely to follow the sentiment of the public on certain topics such as
crime rates (Pickett, 2019), even if public perception is skewed by the media (and
misinformation). While there is debate as to whether all politicians make evidence-informed
legislative decisions (Parkhurst, 2017), informing the public of scientific research findings may
affect public sentiment towards policing policies, motivating lawmakers to pass laws that align
with this public sentiment. Nonetheless, there is a consistent feedback loop between public
sentiment and political decision-making (Gastil & Richards, 2017), whereby a more informed
public can influence policy.
Findings from this study should motivate researchers to promote their scholarship beyond
paywalls that often accompany peer-reviewed journals. The field of criminology and its
academic institutions have struggled to effectively disseminate and reward research with
practitioners and the public to perpetuate evidence-based policies and practices (Austin, 2003;
Currie, 2007). Still, policymakers and criminal justice administrators should be consistently
apprised of the latest scholarly evidence and base policies and practices on evidence-based
approaches (Lum & Koper, 2015).
Our findings lend support to the notion that dissemination of these findings can have an
influence. One option may involve, time permitting, creating personal websites to upload
accepted publications, technical reports, and other research that informs evidence-based policing,
which may help the public and invested stakeholders have access to up-to-date research. There
are open-science collective action efforts underway in this area, such as the CrimRxiv website
(https://www.crimrxiv.com/), which provides a centralized repository for criminology
researchers to easily translate their published work (including pre- and post-prints) into publicly
accessible webpages, which can directly and easily be shared with policy makers, practitioners,
25
and the public without worry for paywalls. As Worral and Gordon (2022) demonstrate, social
media may serve as a valuable platform to deliver evidence-based policing research to the
publicin fact, our information treatments were similar to what could be expected from a
Twitter post. Other outlets include ResearchGate and socarxiv.org, which are publicly accessible
research forums to upload research. Nonetheless, we align with open science literature to
encourage transparency with the public and policy makers (Ashby, 2020; Chin et al., 2021) and
possibly reduce the cost for open access so as to incentivize researchers to pursue this route.
It is relevant to note that our research treatments were either one to two brief sentences,
which were enough to affect respondent perceptions. This finding suggests that short posts on
social media (e.g., Twitter) of research findings can potentially sway public opinion. While
outside the scope of our study, scholars have made efforts to promote lower word counts/page
lengths of manuscripts, so that manuscript development and reviewer turnaround time is more
efficient, more research is published, and manuscripts are more digestible for public and
interested stakeholders (Maddan, 2018). Our findings lend support to the fact that even brief one
to two sentence synopses of the research can be informative to outside readers. Academia largely
does not incentivize op-eds, technical reports, and other translational pieces that are more
digestible to the general public and may not result in peer-reviewed publications (Lum et al.,
2012). While the public may be susceptible to short headlines (some of which may include faulty
information), academics can promote credible research which may help the public parse out this
faulty information. Essentially, promoting rigorous research findings through media outlets can
be avenues in promoting evidence-based scholarship and the diffusion of research to the public.
Our findings also have implications for the practitioner-researcher relationship. Local law
enforcement agencies may be positively impacted in collaborating with researchers to inform
26
evidence-based policies and practices, and this evidence, to a certain extent, can bring the public
in line with policy initiatives made by the police. Researchers can help design, implement, and
evaluate new and existing policies/practices to assess effectiveness. Based on the evaluation,
police agencies can distribute findings to the public to inform the public of (in)effective
policies/practices. These findings can be frequently disseminated on law enforcement and
researcher institutional websites, as well as social media accounts, to keep the public apprised of
effective police strategies that are implemented locally. Importantly, police agencies
collaborating with researchers may bring forth evidence-based practices, potentially increasing
police legitimacy to the public. That is, if the public is aware that police departments are
engaging in evidence-based efforts with researchers, the public may see such policies as
trustworthy, potentially encouraging police legitimacy (Sherman, 2013; Telep, 2016).
Additionally, providing practitioners with relevant scientific information may help inform local
politicians of the relevant scientific evidence, influencing evidence-informed policies.
5.1 Limitations and Future Research
While our study presents some positive implications for public dissemination of research
findings, it is not without its limitations. Our study design causally linked the provision of
scientific information to public opinion but did not explain the exact mechanism as to why this
effect varied by topic. We have some speculations that may prove useful to future research in
this area. Topics like police effectiveness and body-worn cameras may have a more personal or
immediate (micro-level) effect on respondents compared to police budgets. These topics are
likely to impact civilians more regularly, either directly or vicariously, as opposed to the
potential long-term consequences of police budgeting. For example, respondents may personally
27
or vicariously (e.g., read about in the media) experience or witness crime. Therefore, when
presented with scientific research on police effectiveness in reducing crime, respondents may be
personally invested in that knowledge. Additionally, since police are the most visible agents of
the criminal justice system, interacting with police wearing body-worn cameras (e.g., during a
traffic stop or on the street) may be a relatively common experience for respondents, and thus
more “front of mind,” compared to questions of police budgets.
Additionally, discussions around defunding or refunding the police have become highly
politicized in recent years (Jackson et al., 2022), with opinions about these topics more
ideologically ingrained. In this way, scientific research findings related to police budgets may
not be able to adjust these opinions. Police budgeting is a national debate that is split across
political lines. While defunding/abolishing the police has become an emerging political
discussion, Americans largely do not support the goals of these movements. For example,
Vaughn and colleagues (2022) experimentally surveyed U.S. adults and found that respondents
opposed defunding and abolishing police in slogan, substance, and because these proposals
suggest removing police from their regular police roles. In other words, it appears respondents
feel strongly about the topic of police budgets, and as such, we did not find that people given
confirmatory or negative research findings about police budgets were affected. Unfortunately,
we did not ask about existing participant knowledge about these topics, which could have given
us some insight into this issue. Moreover, the concept of defunding the police is not clearly
conceptualized or defined, leading to difficulty in understanding what, and how, to measure such
efforts (Koziarski & Huey, 2021; Lum et al., 2022).
In addition to this limitation, relevant questions about each topic were asked immediately
following each information treatment. In this respect, the study was not designed to test whether
28
these treatments have lasting impacts on respondent perceptions of police. An important follow-
up to our study would be an assessment, perhaps longitudinally, of whether the presentation of
this information is fleeting, as well as what factors make it more likely for information to remain
salient. Based on our current findings, we can only suggest that researchers consistently promote
their research findings so that the evidence remains salient in people’s minds. One possible
model of this type of effort is the Criminal Justice Expert Panel (2022) series. This model brings
together identified experts in criminology, economics, political science, and affiliated fields to
answer questions of public import. For example, on the topic of policing and public safety, the
series asks experts to weigh in on the topic: “Do police actually make communities safer? And
are there other ways to achieve that goal?” Within that topic, experts are asked to give their
opinions on the following: Increasing police budgets will improve public safety” and
Increasing social service budgets (e.g. housing, health, education) will improve public safety.
By weighting their confidence in these statements, experts can signal how strongly they feel
about these policies in the context of available evidence. This model allows any member of the
public to quickly ascertain, for example, that most serious scholars agree that increasing funding
of policing would likely benefit public safety, but that it is critical to direct those funds carefully
towards police activities known to lower serious crime. Not only could this approach of
presenting scientific evidence influence public opinion over time, it would continuously dispel
misinformation from media and other outlets which skews public opinion (Altheide, 2018).
We focused on heads of household in South Carolina that had email addresses available.
The internal validity of a survey experiment is well known, as are the challenges in
understanding the external validity of the findings. It is worth noting that the findings may not be
applicable to the entire United States or even the general population of South Carolina, as the
29
study was not conducted on a representative sample of either area. Further replication is needed
to confirm the external validity of the findings, as well as the reliability of the findings across
various populations. Further, given that political polarization may be at play in influencing some
of the findings, we expect that the exact relationship between scientific information and public
opinion will vary across time politicization varies the tenor and focus of public debates
depending on the most highly salient topics and that saliency varies over time.
One further limitation related to public impact is the question of whether, and how, public
opinion translates to policy maker action, whether it be through appointed positions such as
police chiefs and city managers, or elected officials such as sheriffs and senators. This question
represents longstanding debates within political science scholarship. Recent scholarship suggests
that even amongst policymakers with longstanding resistance to democratic reforms, such as
police executives’ pushback to Civilian Review Boards (CRBs), strong indicators of local
opinion in favor of CRBs is moderately effective at softening police executives’ opinions
(Adams et al., 2022; McCrain et al., 2020). It is outside the scope of our study to
comprehensively map out the pathways from public sentiment to actionable policy. However,
our findings can point to the effectiveness of the science to public sentiment pathway, which
would necessarily be involved in evidence-based policy.
Finally, this study raises questions about the broader implications of how criminology
research is presented and communicated to the public. The use of short, Twitter-like statements
to inform respondents was intentional, as it emulates how many Americans ingest information.
Further, we capitalized buzzwords to draw attention to the important aspects of the statements
and did not include a pure control condition. However, it is unclear whether this method is the
most effective way to communicate scientific research on policing topics and whether including
30
a pure control condition would have made a difference. Further research is needed to determine
the most effective methods for presenting scientific research to the public, and whether different
strategies are needed for different types of topics or audiences. Overall, these findings contribute
to the growing body of research on the intersection of science and public opinion and highlight
the importance of considering how scientific research is communicated to the public.
6. CONCLUSION
This study highlights the importance of researchers engaging with the public and sharing
their work, because it can have an impact on public opinion related to policing issues. This
finding is particularly relevant in todays society, where debates about policing and criminal
justice reform are ongoing and often highly contentious. By sharing their findings with the
public, researchers can help inform these discussions and contribute to a better understanding of
the complexities and nuances of policing issues.
Moreover, the findings underscore the importance of conducting high-quality research on
policing, even on the most politically charged issues. Research can have significant impacts on
policy and other important outcomes, and it is important for researchers to provide evidence-
based insights that can inform these decisions. While it may be more challenging to influence
public opinion on highly politicized issues, there are still other avenues, such as policymaking
bodies, where research can have a meaningful impact. We have presented compelling evidence
that police research can play a valuable role in shaping public opinion, and it is advisable for
researchers to consider how they present their research to the public. By doing so, researchers
can help to inform public discourse and contribute to a better understanding of policing issues
among the general public.
31
References
AAPOR. (2023). American Association for Public Opinion Research. https://www-
archive.aapor.org/Education-Resources/For-Researchers/Poll-Survey-FAQ/Response-
Rates-An-Overview.aspx
Adams, I. T., McCrain, J., Schiff, D. S., Schiff, K. J., & Mourtgos, S. M. (2022). Public Pressure
or Peer Influence: What Shapes Police Executives’ Views on Civilian Oversight?
SocArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31235/osf.io/mdu96
Agley, J. (2020). Assessing changes in US public trust in science amid the COVID-19 pandemic.
Public Health, 183, 122125. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.puhe.2020.05.004
Airoldi, G., & Vecchi, D. (2021). The road from evidence to policies and the erosion of the
standards of democratic scrutiny in the COVID-19 pandemic. History and Philosophy of
the Life Sciences, 43(2), 15.
Altheide, D. L. (2018). Creating fear: News and the construction of crisis. Routledge.
Archbold, C. A. (2022). A Look at Police Accountability Through the Lens of the George Floyd
Case. In Rethinking and Reforming American Policing (pp. 259288). Springer.
Ashby, M. P. (2020). The open-access availability of criminological research to practitioners and
policy makers. Journal of Criminal Justice Education, 32(1), 121.
Austin, J. (2003). Why criminology is irrelevant. Criminology & Public Policy, 2(3), 557564.
Bansak, K. (2021). Estimating causal moderation effects with randomized treatments and non-
randomized moderators. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series A: Statistics in
Society, 184(1), 6586.
Baranauskas, A. J. (2022). Racial resentment, crime concerns, and public attitudes toward
defunding the police. Journal of Ethnicity in Criminal Justice, 20(1), 4872.
Beck, A. L., Lakkaraju, K., & Rai, V. (2017). Small is big: Interactive trumps passive
information in breaking information barriers and impacting behavioral antecedents. PloS
One, 12(1), e0169326.
Bennell, C., Alpert, Geoffrey P., Andersen, J. P., Arpaia, J., Huhta, J.-M., Kahn, K. B.,
Khanizadeh, A.-J., McCarthy, M., McLean, K., Mitchell, R. J., Nieuwenhuys, A., Palmer,
A., & White, M. D. (2021). Advancing police use of force research and practice: Urgent
issues and prospects. Legal and Criminological Psychology, n/a(n/a).
https://doi.org/10.1111/lcrp.12191
Berk, R., Pitkin, E., Brown, L., Buja, A., George, E., & Zhao, L. (2013). Covariance adjustments
for the analysis of randomized field experiments. Evaluation Review, 37(34), 170196.
Bittner, E. (1990). Florence Nightingale in Pursuit of Willie Sutton. In Aspects of Police Work.
Northeastern University Press.
Boehme, H. M., & Kaminski, R. J. (2023). Suspect resistance, police use of force, and officer
injuries in a post-Floyd era: An analysis of two large police departments. Police Practice
and Research, 19.
Bohm, R. M., Clark, L. J., & Aveni, A. F. (1991). Knowledge and death penalty opinion: A test
of the Marshall hypotheses. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 28(3), 360
387.
Bohm, R. M., & Vogel, B. L. (2004). More than ten years after: The long-term stability of
informed death penalty opinions. Journal of Criminal Justice, 32(4), 307327.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2004.04.003
32
Brantingham, P. J., Carter, J., MacDonald, J., Melde, C., & Mohler, G. (2021). Is the recent
surge in violence in American cities due to contagion? Journal of Criminal Justice, 76,
101848.
Brashier, N. M., & Schacter, D. L. (2020). Aging in an era of fake news. Current Directions in
Psychological Science, 29(3), 316323.
Brenan, M. (2020). Amid Pandemic, Confidence in Key U.S. Institutions Surges. Gallup.
https://news.gallup.com/poll/317135/amid-pandemic-confidence-key-institutions-
surges.aspx
Brown, A. (2017). Republicans more likely than Democrats to have confidence in police. Pew
Research Center. https://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2017/01/13/republicans-more-
likely-than-democrats-to-have-confidence-in-police/
Callanan, V. J., & Rosenberger, J. S. (2011). Media and public perceptions of the police:
Examining the impact of race and personal experience. Policing & Society, 21(2), 167
189.
Cassella, C., Epp, D., Fredriksson, K., Roman, M., & Walker, H. (2022). The George Floyd
Effect: How Protests and Public Scrutiny Change Police Behavior in Seattle.
Cerdá, M., Tracy, M., & Keyes, K. M. (2018). Reducing urban violence. Epidemiology, 29(1),
142150.
Chin, J. M., Pickett, J. T., Vazire, S., & Holcombe, A. O. (2021). Questionable research practices
and open science in quantitative criminology. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 131.
Citrin, J., & Stoker, L. (2018). Political trust in a cynical age. Annual Review of Political
Science, 21, 4970.
Clear, T. R. (2010). Policy and evidence: The challenge to the American Society of Criminology:
2009 presidential address to the American Society of Criminology. Criminology, 48(1),
125.
Cochran, J. K., & Chamlin, M. B. (2005). Can information change public opinion? Another test
of the Marshall hypotheses. Journal of Criminal Justice, 33(6), 573584.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2005.08.006
Cook, T. E., & Gronke, P. (2005). The Skeptical American: Revisiting the Meanings of Trust in
Government and Confidence in Institutions. The Journal of Politics, 67(3), 784803.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2508.2005.00339.x
Criminal Justice Expert Panel. (2022). Criminal Justice Expert Panel.
https://cjexpertpanel.org/surveys/
Currie, E. (2007). Against marginality: Arguments for a public criminology. Theoretical
Criminology, 11(2), 175190.
Demir, M. (2019). Citizens’ perceptions of body-worn cameras (BWCs): Findings from a quasi-
randomized controlled trial. Journal of Criminal Justice, 60, 130139.
Donovan, K. M., & Klahm IV, C. F. (2018). How priming innocence influences public opinion
on police misconduct and false convictions: A research note. Criminal Justice Review,
43(2), 174185. https://doi.org/10.1177/0734016817707809
Fine, A. D., Rowan, Z., & Simmons, C. (2019). Do politics Trump race in determining
America’s youths’ perceptions of law enforcement? Journal of Criminal Justice, 61, 48
57.
Freedman, D. A. (2008). On regression adjustments to experimental data. Advances in Applied
Mathematics, 40(2), 180193.
33
Funder, D. C., & Ozer, D. J. (2019). Evaluating effect size in psychological research: Sense and
nonsense. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 2(2), 156168.
Gamson, W. A., & McEvoy, J. (2017). Police violence and its public support. In Collective
violence (pp. 329342). Routledge.
Gastil, J., & Richards, R. C. (2017). Embracing digital democracy: A call for building an online
civic commons. PS: Political Science & Politics, 50(3), 758763.
Gaub, J. E., & White, M. D. (2020). Open to Interpretation: Confronting the Challenges of
Understanding the Current State of Body-Worn Camera Research. American Journal of
Criminal Justice, 45(5), 899913. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12103-020-09518-4
Gramlich, J., & Funk, C. (2020). Black Americans face higher COVID-19 risks, are more
hesitant to trust medical scientists, get vaccinated. Pew Research Center.
https://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2020/06/04/black-americans-face-higher-covid-
19-risks-are-more-hesitant-to-trust-medical-scientists-get-vaccinated/
Indermaur, D., Roberts, L., Spiranovic, C., Mackenzie, G., & Gelb, K. (2012). A matter of
judgement: The effect of information and deliberation on public attitudes to punishment.
Punishment & Society, 14(2), 147165. https://doi.org/10.1177/1462474511434
Jackson, J., Fine, A., Bradford, B., & Trinkner, R. (2022). Social Identity and Support for
Defunding the Police in the Aftermath of George Floyd’s Murder.
Kam, C. D., & Trussler, M. J. (2017). At the Nexus of Observational and Experimental
Research: Theory, Specification, and Analysis of Experiments with Heterogeneous
Treatment Effects. Political Behavior, 39(4), 789815. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-
016-9379-z
Keeter, S., & McGeeney, K. (2015). Coverage error in internet surveys: Who web-only surveys
miss and how that affects results. Pew Research Center.
https://www.pewresearch.org/methods/2015/09/22/coverage-error-in-internet-surveys/
Kennedy, B., Tyson, A., & Funk, C. (2022). Americans’ trust in scientists, orther groups
declines. Pew Research Center.
https://www.pewresearch.org/science/2022/02/15/americans-trust-in-scientists-other-
groups-declines/
Kern, H. L., Stuart, E. A., Hill, J., & Green, D. P. (2016). Assessing Methods for Generalizing
Experimental Impact Estimates to Target Populations. Journal of Research on
Educational Effectiveness, 9(1), 103127.
https://doi.org/10.1080/19345747.2015.1060282
Koziarski, J., & Huey, L. (2021). #Defund or #Re-Fund? Re-examining Bayley’s blueprint for
police reform. International Journal of Comparative and Applied Criminal Justice.
http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/01924036.2021.1907604
Kroke, A. M., & Ruthig, J. C. (2022). Conspiracy beliefs and the impact on health behaviors.
Applied Psychology: Health and Well‐Being, 14(1), 311328.
Lin, W. (2013). Agnostic notes on regression adjustments to experimental data: Reexamining
Freedman’s critique.
Lipsky, M. (1973). Law and Order: Police Encounters. Transaction Publishers.
Liu, S., & Cheng, T. (n.d.). How Online Media Shapes Polarization Towards Policing.
Loader, Ian, & Sparks, R. (2013). Public Criminology? Routledge.
https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203846049
Lum, C., & Koper, C. S. (2015). Evidence-based policing. Waveland Press Illinois, IL.
Lum, C., & Koper, C. S. (2017). Evidence-based policing. Oxford Univ. Press.
34
Lum, C., Koper, C. S., & Wu, X. (2022). Can we really defund the police? A nine-agency study
of police response to calls for service. Police Quarterly, 25(3), 255280.
Lum, C. M., Koper, C. S., Gill, C., Hibdon, J., Telep, C., & Robinson, L. (2016). An Evidence-
assessment of the Recommendations of the President’s Task Force on 21st Century
Policing: Implementation and Research Priorities. International Association of Chiefs of
Police Alexandria, VA.
Lum, C., Stoltz, M., Koper, C. S., & Scherer, J. A. (2019). Research on body-worn cameras:
What we know, what we need to know. Criminology & Public Policy, 18(1), 93118.
Lum, C., Telep, C. W., Koper, C. S., & Grieco, J. (2012). Receptivity to research in policing.
Justice Research and Policy, 14(1), 6195.
Maddan, S. (2018). Presidential address: The declining significance of the literature review in
criminal justice scholarship: Towards a new paradigm. American Journal of Criminal
Justice, 43(4), 745753.
Mailers Haven. (2022). The highest quality: Most responsive mailing lists.
https://www.mailershaven.com/
Martin, G. J., & McCrain, J. (2019). Local News and National Politics. American Political
Science Review, 113(2), 372384. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0003055418000965
McCrain, J., Adams, I. T., Mourtgos, S. M., Schiff, D. S., & Schiff, K. J. (2020, December 8).
Voters Support Civilian Oversight of Policing. Data For Progress.
https://www.dataforprogress.org/blog/2022/12/voters-support-civilian-oversight-of-
policing
McMahon, S. (2010). Rape myth beliefs and bystander attitudes among incoming college
students. Journal of American College Health, 59(1), 311.
McMaster, H. S., LeardMann, C. A., Speigle, S., & Dillman, D. A. (2017). An experimental
comparison of web-push vs. Paper-only survey procedures for conducting an in-depth
health survey of military spouses. BMC Medical Research Methodology, 17(1), 19.
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12874-017-0337-1
Metcalfe, C., & Pickett, J. T. (2022). Public fear of protesters and support for protest policing:
An experimental test of two theoretical models*. Criminology, 60(1), 6089.
https://doi.org/10.1111/1745-9125.12291
Mourtgos, S. M., & Adams, I. T. (2020). Assessing Public Perceptions of Police Use-of-Force:
Legal Reasonableness and Community Standards. Justice Quarterly, 37(5), 869899.
https://doi.org/10.1080/07418825.2019.1679864
Mourtgos, S. M., Adams, I. T., & Nix, J. (2022). Elevated police turnover following the summer
of George Floyd protests: A synthetic control study. Criminology & Public Policy, 21(1),
933.
Mullinix, K. J., Bolsen, T., & Norris, R. J. (2021). The feedback effects of controversial police
use of force. Political Behavior, 43(2), 881898. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-020-
09646-x
Mummolo, J. (2018a). Militarization fails to enhance police safety or reduce crime but may harm
police reputation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(37), 9181
9186. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1805161115
Mummolo, J. (2018b). Modern police tactics, police-citizen interactions, and the prospects for
reform. The Journal of Politics, 80(1), 115. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/694393
Nadeem, R. (2020). Trust in Medical Scientists Has Grown in U.S., but Mainly Among
Democrats. Pew Research Center.
35
https://www.pewresearch.org/science/2020/05/21/trust-in-medical-scientists-has-grown-
in-u-s-but-mainly-among-democrats/
Nix, J., Ivanov, S., & Pickett, J. T. (2021). What does the public want police to do during
pandemics? A national experiment. Criminology & Public Policy, 20(3), 545571.
https://doi.org/10.1111/1745-9133.12535
Nix, J., & Wolfe, S. E. (2017). The Impact of Negative Publicity on Police Self-legitimacy.
Justice Quarterly, 34(1), 84108. https://doi.org/10.1080/07418825.2015.1102954
Novick, R., Socia, K. M., & Pickett, J. T. (2022). Asymmetric Compassion Collapse, Collateral
Consequences, and Reintegration: An Experiment. Justice Quarterly, 39(7), 14751498.
https://doi.org/10.1080/07418825.2022.2119157
O’Donohue, W., Yeater, E. A., & Fanetti, M. (2003). Rape prevention with college males: The
roles of rape myth acceptance, victim empathy, and outcome expectancies. Journal of
Interpersonal Violence, 18(5), 513531.
Parker, K. (2021). What’s behind the growing gap between men and women in college
completion? Pew Research Center. https://www.pewresearch.org/fact-
tank/2021/11/08/whats-behind-the-growing-gap-between-men-and-women-in-college-
completion/
Parkhurst, J. (2017). The politics of evidence: From evidence-based policy to the good
governance of evidence. Taylor & Francis.
Patten, E., & Perrin, A. (2015). Who’s left out in a web-only survey and how it affects results.
Pew Research Center. https://www.pewresearch.org/short-reads/2015/09/22/who-s-left-
out-in-a-web-only-survey-and-how-it-affects-results/
Pew Research Center. (2023). Party affiliation among adults in South Carolina by political
ideology. https://www.pewresearch.org/religion/religious-landscape-
study/compare/party-affiliation/by/political-ideology/among/state/south-carolina/
Pickett, J., Cullen, F., Bushway, S. D., Chiricos, T., & Alpert, G. (2018). The response rate test:
Nonresponse bias and the future of survey research in criminology and criminal justice.
Available at SSRN 3103018.
Pickett, J. T. (2016). On the social foundations for crimmigration: Latino threat and support for
expanded police powers. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 32(1), 103132.
Pickett, J. T. (2017). Methodological myths and the role of appeals in criminal justice journals:
The case of response rates. ACJS Today, 41(3), 6169.
Pickett, J. T. (2019). Public opinion and criminal justice policy: Theory and research. Annual
Review of Criminology, 2, 405428.
Pickett, J. T., Graham, A., & Cullen, F. T. (2022). The American racial divide in fear of the
police. Criminology, 60(2), 291320.
Pickett, J. T., Ivanov, S., & Wozniak, K. H. (2020). Selling effective violence prevention policies
to the public: A nationally representative framing experiment. Journal of Experimental
Criminology, 123.
Pickett, J. T., Nix, J., & Roche, S. P. (2018). Testing a social schematic model of police
procedural justice. Social Psychology Quarterly, 81(2), 97125.
https://doi.org/DOI:10.1177/0190272518765134
Pickett, J. T., & Ryon, S. B. (2017). Procedurally just cooperation: Explaining support for due
process reforms in policing. Journal of Criminal Justice, 48, 920.
36
Pryce, D. K., Johnson, D., & Maguire, E. R. (2017). Procedural justice, obligation to obey, and
cooperation with police in a sample of Ghanaian immigrants. Criminal Justice and
Behavior, 44(5), 733755. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093854816680225
Reiner, R. (2010). The Politics of the Police. OUP Oxford.
Reny, T. T., & Newman, B. J. (2021). The opinion-mobilizing effect of social protest against
police violence: Evidence from the 2020 George Floyd protests. American Political
Science Review, 115(4), 14991507.
Rydberg, J., Dum, C. P., & Socia, K. M. (2018). Nobody gives a#% &!: A factorial survey
examining the effect of criminological evidence on opposition to sex offender residence
restrictions. Journal of Experimental Criminology, 14, 541550.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11292-018-9335-5
Santos, J. R. A. (1999). Cronbach’s alpha: A tool for assessing the reliability of scales. Journal
of Extension, 37(2), 15.
Schiff, K. J., Clark, T. S., Glynn, A. N., Owens, M. L., Gunderson, A., & Dobbie, E. (2022).
Knowing is Half the Battle? The Effect of Information about Police Shootings of Civilians
on Public Support for Police Reforms.
https://static1.squarespace.com/static/58d3d264893fc0bdd12db507/t/63e409b790c62c06c
962e9b6/1675889081751/OIS_Info_Survey_Experiment.pdf
Sharp, E. B., & Johnson, P. E. (2009). Accounting for Variation in Distrust of Local Police.
Justice Quarterly, 26(1), 157182. https://doi.org/10.1080/07418820802290496
Sherman, L. W. (2005). The Use and Usefulness of Criminology, 1751-2005: Enlightened
Justice and Its Failures. The ANNALS of the American Academy of Political and Social
Science, 600(1), 115135. https://doi.org/10.1177/0002716205278103
Sherman, L. W. (2013). The rise of evidence-based policing: Targeting, testing, and tracking.
Crime and Justice, 42(1), 377451.
Sherman, L. W. (2015). A tipping point for “totally evidenced policing” ten ideas for building an
evidence-based police agency. International Criminal Justice Review, 25(1), 1129.
Sierra-Arévalo, M., Nix, J., & Mourtgos, S. M. (2023). The “War on Cops,” Retaliatory
Violence, and the Murder of George Floyd. https://jnix.netlify.app/publication/51-crim-
war-on-cops-george-floyd/
Singer, L., & Cooper, S. (2009). Improving public confidence in the criminal justice system: An
evaluation of a communication activity. The Howard Journal of Criminal Justice, 48(5),
485500. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2311.2009.00590.x
Telep, C. W. (2016). Expanding the scope of evidence-based policing. Criminology & Pub.
Pol’y, 15, 243.
Todak, N. (2017). The decision to become a police officer in a legitimacy crisis. Women &
Criminal Justice, 27(4), 250270.
Tyler, T. R. (1990). Why people obey the law. Yale Univ. Press.
Tyler, T. R. (2004). Enhancing police legitimacy. Annals of the American Academy of Political
and Social Science, 593, 8499.
United States Census Bureau. (2022). Quick facts: South Carolina.
https://www.census.gov/quickfacts/SC
Unnever, J. D., Cullen, F. T., & Fisher, B. S. (2007). “A Liberal Is Someone Who Has Not Been
Mugged”: Criminal Victimization and Political Beliefs. Justice Quarterly, 24(2), 309
334. https://doi.org/10.1080/07418820701294862
37
Vaughn, P. E., Peyton, K., & Huber, G. A. (2022). Mass support for proposals to reshape
policing depends on the implications for crime and safety. Criminology & Public Policy,
21(1), 125146.
Washburn, E. (2023). America Less Confident In Police Than Ever Before: A Look At The
Numbers. Forbes. https://www.forbes.com/sites/emilywashburn/2023/02/03/america-
less-confident-in-police-than-ever-before-a-look-at-the-numbers/?sh=3db8d22b6afb
Wentz, E. A., & Schlimgen, K. A. (2012). Citizens’ perceptions of police service and police
response to community concerns. Journal of Crime and Justice, 35(1), 114133.
White, M. D., Orosco, C., & Terpstra, B. (2021). Investigating the Impacts of a Global Pandemic
and George Floyd’s Death on Crime and Other Features of Police Work. Justice
Quarterly, 128.
Worrall, J. L., & Gordon, Q. (2022). Is Criminology & Public Policy “influential?” Answers
from altmetrics. Criminology & Public Policy.
Wozniak, K. H., Drakulich, K. M., & Calfano, B. R. (2021). Do photos of police-civilian
interactions influence public opinion about the police? A multimethod test of media
effects. Journal of Experimental Criminology, 17(2), 127.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11292-020-09415-0
Wozniak, K. H., Pickett, J. T., & Brown, E. K. (2022). Judging Hardworking Robbers and Lazy
Thieves: An Experimental Test of Act-vs. Person-Centered Punitiveness and Perceived
Redeemability. Justice Quarterly, 39(7), 15651591.
TABLES
TABLE 1 Descriptive characteristics of respondents
Variable
Mean
SD
Effectiveness Outcome
4.1
0.9
Refund Outcome
4.1
0.9
Defund Outcome
1.7
0.9
BWC Outcome
3.9
0.8
Police Procedural Justice
1.8
1.0
Obey Police
3.9
0.8
Trust in Police
4.1
0.9
Age
58.8
15.0
Employed
0.5
0.5
N
Percent
Political Ideology
Liberal (Reference category)
181
11.8
Moderate
548
35.8
Conservative
796
52.0
Sex
Male
770
50.3
Female
759
49.6
Race
White
1294
84.5
Non-White
233
15.2
Formal Education
No diploma or GED
9
0.6
Finished High School
135
8.8
Some college
311
20.3
Two-year degree
187
12.2
Four-year degree
496
32.4
Graduate/professional degree
392
25.6
Crime Victim (Last 12 months)
No
1381
90.2
Yes
150
9.8
38
Married
No
544
35.5
Yes
982
64.1
Parent
No
1193
77.9
Yes
335
21.9
Notes: SD = Standard deviation; N = Number of respondents; final sample = 1,519
TABLE 2 Treatment Conditions
Police Effectiveness Treatments
Confirming Info: “Scientific research finds that INCREASING POLICE PRESENCE in neighborhoods leads to
LOWER CRIME rates and MORE ARRESTS of violent criminals.”
Negative Info: “Scientific research finds that INCREASING POLICE PRESENCE in neighborhoods DOES NOT
LOWER CRIME rates and DOES NOT LEAD TO MORE ARRESTS of violent criminals.”
Mixed Info: “Some scientific research finds that increasing police presence in neighborhoods leads to lower crime
rates and more arrests of violent criminals, BUT other scientific research does not find this to be the case.”
Police Budget Treatments
Confirming Info: “Scientific research finds that INCREASING POLICE BUDGETS for hiring, retaining, and
training officers LOWERS CRIME rates within neighborhoods.”
Negative Info: “Scientific research finds that DECREASING POLICE BUDGETS and shifting money to social
services, alcohol/drug rehabilitation, and mental health resources LOWERS CRIME rates in neighborhoods.”
Mixed Info: “Some scientific research finds that increasing police budgets for hiring, retaining, and training
officers lowers crime rates within neighborhoods, BUT other scientific research finds that decreasing police
budgets and shifting money to social services, alcohol/drug rehabilitation, and mental health resources lowers
crime rates in neighborhoods.”
BWC Treatments
Confirming Info: “Scientific research finds police body-worn cameras REDUCE POLICE USE-OF-FORCE used
on citizens and REDUCE COMPLAINTS, as well as IMPROVE TRANSPARENCY and SAFETY for citizens.”
Negative Info: “Scientific research finds police body-worn cameras DO NOT REDUCE USE-OF-FORCE used on
citizens and DO NOT REDUCE COMPLAINTS, as well as DO NOT IMPROVE TRANSPARENCY and
SAFETY for citizens.”
Mixed Info: “Some scientific research finds that police body-worn cameras reduce use-of-force used on citizens
and reduce false complaints, as well as improve transparency and safety for citizens, BUT other scientific research
does not find this to be the case.”
39
TABLE 3 Full OLS Regressions Predicting Attitudes about Police Effectiveness, Defund,
Refund, and Body-Worn Camera
Effectiveness
Defund
Refund
BWC
0.132 (0.034)***
-0.029 (0.039)
0.046 (0.041)
0.135 (0.046)**
-0.179 (0.035)***
-0.004 (0.039)
-0.012 (0.041)
-0.232 (0.046)***
0.109 (0.026)***
-0.042 (0.030)
0.100 (0.031)**
0.014 (0.035)
0.172 (0.022)***
-0.084 (0.025)***
0.193 (0.026)***
0.079 (0.029)**
0.465 (0.031)***
-0.345 (0.035)***
0.374 (0.036)***
0.061 (0.041)
0.248 (0.049)***
-0.570 (0.055)***
0.227 (0.058)***
-0.129 (0.064)*
0.299 (0.050)***
-0.905 (0.057)***
0.326 (0.059)***
-0.238 (0.066)***
0.040 (0.029)
0.165 (0.033)***
0.140 (0.034)***
0.063 (0.038)+
-0.047 (0.042)
0.163 (0.048)***
0.019 (0.050)
0.033 (0.056)
0.010 (0.011)
-0.003 (0.012)
-0.036 (0.013)**
-0.034 (0.014)*
0.005 (0.001)***
-0.006 (0.001)***
0.005 (0.001)***
0.004 (0.001)**
0.078 (0.049)
-0.063 (0.056)
0.065 (0.058)
0.071 (0.065)
1519
1519
1519
1519
0.602
0.504
0.445
0.067
192.132
129.750
102.233
10.063
Notes: Unstandardized coefficients presented with standard errors in parentheses. With respect to the experimental
treatments, the presentation of mixed information was used as the reference category.
+ p < 0.1, * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001 (two-tailed).
40
APPENDIX
Table A1. Balance Table
Characteristic
Negative Info,
N = 479
Confirming Info,
N = 524
Mixed Info,
N = 528
P-value
Political Ideology
0.5
Liberal
57 / 477 (12%)
66 / 523 (13%)
58 / 525 (11%)
Moderate
184 / 477 (39%)
174 / 523 (33%)
190 / 525 (36%)
Conservative
236 / 477 (49%)
283 / 523 (54%)
277 / 525 (53%)
Sex
0.9
Male
240 / 477 (50%)
268 / 524 (51%)
262 / 528 (50%)
Female
237 / 477 (50%)
256 / 524 (49%)
266 / 528 (50%)
Race
0.5
White
397 / 477 (83%)
449 / 524 (86%)
448 / 526 (85%)
Non-White
80 / 477 (17%)
75 / 524 (14%)
78 / 526 (15%)
Education
0.4
No diploma or GED
4 / 478 (0.8%)
1 / 524 (0.2%)
4 / 528 (0.8%)
High school degree
39 / 478 (8.2%)
56 / 524 (11%)
40 / 528 (7.6%)
Some college
89 / 478 (19%)
104 / 524 (20%)
118 / 528 (22%)
Two-year degree
57 / 478 (12%)
66 / 524 (13%)
64 / 528 (12%)
Four-year degree
163 / 478 (34%)
174 / 524 (33%)
159 / 528 (30%)
Graduate/professional
126 / 478 (26%)
123 / 524 (23%)
143 / 528 (27%)
Age
57.98 (15.35)
59.15 (14.45)
59.29 (15.26)
0.5
Married
292 / 477 (61%)
338 / 522 (65%)
352 / 527 (67%)
0.2
Parent
113 / 476 (24%)
121 / 524 (23%)
101 / 528 (19%)
0.2
Crime Victim
53 / 479 (11%)
50 / 524 (9.5%)
47 / 528 (8.9%)
0.5
Employed
238 / 478 (50%)
246 / 523 (47%)
254 / 528 (48%)
0.7
Obey Police
3.90 (0.74)
3.91 (0.78)
3.97 (0.76)
0.3
Police Legitimacy
1.74 (1.00)
1.81 (0.97)
1.76 (1.06)
0.5
Trust in Police
4.09 (0.90)
4.16 (0.87)
4.12 (0.91)
0.9
Note: n / N (%); Mean (SD);Pearson’s Chi-squared test; balance across covariates shown only for treatment on
one outcome (police effectiveness), as respondents received new treatments for each outcome presented. Balance
across other treatment/outcome dyads similar (i.e., balanced).
Table A2. Means and conditional means of outcomes
Outcome
Overall Mean (SD)
Confirming Info.
Negative Info.
Mixed Info.
Effectiveness
4.10 (0.87)
4.26 (0.82)
3.91 (0.91)
4.12 (0.86)
Refund
4.09 (0.88)
4.12 (0.86)
4.03 (0.89)
4.12 (0.88)
Defund
1.65 (0.89)
1.63 (0.87)
1.71 (0.92)
1.63 (0.88)
BWC
3.68 (0.75)
4.04 (0.74)
3.68 (0.76)
3.90 (0.71)
41
Table A3. Treatment heterogeneity by sex
Effectiveness
Defund
Refund
BWC
Confirming Info
0.142 (0.048)**
-0.017 (0.055)
0.040 (0.057)
0.119 (0.064)+
Negative Info
-0.175 (0.050)***
0.012 (0.056)
0.0006 (0.058)
-0.137 (0.065)*
Confirm * Female
-0.024 (0.069)
-0.030 (0.080)
0.017 (0.083)
0.025 (0.092)
Negative * Female
-0.017 (0.071)
-0.025 (0.079)
-0.030 (0.082)
-0.192 (0.092)*
N
1519
1519
1519
1519
R2
0.60
0.50
0.44
0.07
R2 Adj.
0.595
0.497
0.439
0.064
F
172.524
116.291
92.400
8.984
Note: control variables not shown; + p < 0.1, * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001
Table A4. Treatment heterogeneity by partisan identification
Effectiveness
Defund
Refund
BWC
Confirming Info
0.103 (0.100)
0.067 (0.117)
0.076 (0.122)
0.173 (0.133)
Negative Info
-0.105 (0.104)
0.035 (0.112)
0.067 (0.116)
-0.421 (0.134)**
Confirm × Moderate
-0.045 (0.116)
-0.043 (0.134)
0.038 (0.140)
-0.174 (0.154)
Confirm × Conservative
0.080 (0.111)
-0.159 (0.129)
-0.076 (0.134)
0.034 (0.147)
Negative × Moderate
-0.127 (0.119)
-0.024 (0.130)
-0.041 (0.135)
0.071 (0.155)
Negative × Conservative
-0.064 (0.115)
-0.047 (0.124)
-0.126 (0.129)
0.308 (0.148)*
N
1519
1519
1519
1519
R2
0.60
0.50
0.44
0.07
R2 Adj.
0.595
0.497
0.439
0.065
F
149.979
101.005
80.218
8.053
Note: control variables not shown; + p < 0.1, * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001
Table A5. Treatment heterogeneity by race
Effectiveness
Defund
Refund
BWC
Confirming Info
0.139 (0.037)***
-0.017 (0.043)
0.052 (0.045)
0.149 (0.050)**
Negative Info
-0.179 (0.038)***
0.024 (0.043)
-0.008 (0.044)
-0.235 (0.050)***
Confirm * Nonwhite
-0.061 (0.098)
-0.114 (0.114)
-0.032 (0.119)
-0.118 (0.130)
Negative * Nonwhite
-0.032 (0.097)
-0.167 (0.111)
-0.050 (0.116)
0.009 (0.128)
N
1519
1519
1519
1519
R2
0.60
0.50
0.44
0.07
R2 Adj.
0.595
0.498
0.439
0.061
F
172.574
116.619
92.382
8.532
Note: control variables not shown; + p < 0.1, * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001
42
Table A6. Uncontrolled experimental results
Effectiveness
Defund
Refund
BWC
Confirming Info
0.146** (0.053)
0.007 (0.056)
0.003 (0.055)
0.137** (0.046)
Negative Info
-0.212*** (0.054)
0.081 (0.055)
-0.085 (0.054)
-0.220*** (0.046)
N
1531
1531
1531
1531
R2
0.03
0.002
0.002
0.04
R2 Adj.
0.026
0.0004
0.0008
0.037
F
21.717
1.288
1.633
30.528
+ p < 0.1, * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001
Table A7. Variance Inflation Factor (VIF) Check
Effectiveness
Defund
Refund
BWC
Confirming Info
1.15
1.14
1.14
1.16
Negative Info
1.15
1.14
1.14
1.16
Obey
1.19
1.19
1.19
1.19
Legitimate
1.88
1.88
1.88
1.88
Trust
1.94
1.95
1.95
1.95
Ideology
1.07
1.07
1.07
1.07
Female
1.02
1.02
1.02
1.02
Nonwhite
1.07
1.07
1.07
1.07
Education
1.01
1.01
1.01
1.01
Age
1.11
1.11
1.11
1.11
Crime Victim
1.03
1.03
1.03
1.03
GVIF^(1/(2Df)) reported. The GVIF^(1/(2Df)) is an adjusted VIF value that accounts for the degrees of
freedom, and it is more appropriate for comparing multicollinearity across predictors with different numbers
of categories. Unadjusted VIF values all indicate non-problematic multicollinearity.
... The relationship between the format of disseminated information and knowledge gain proved to be more complex than anticipated. Recent findings from Boehme et al. (2024) suggest that brief synopses of scientific research can effectively shape public opinion on specific policing issues, such as police effectiveness and the use of body-worn cameras. However, this influence does not extend uniformly across all topics; for complex and politically sensitive issues like police budgets, brief summaries did not make a significant impact. ...
... However, this influence does not extend uniformly across all topics; for complex and politically sensitive issues like police budgets, brief summaries did not make a significant impact. Boehme et al. (2024) concluded that the efficacy of concise research summaries in influencing public perceptions largely depends on the topic's relevance to the audience and the broader political context, indicating that dissemination strategies may need to be highly tailored to the audience's concerns and realities. ...
... Both executive summaries and full-text articles were shown to have a limited impact on knowledge gain, echoing concerns raised by Telep (2017) and Boehme et al. (2024). While the current study adhered to established guidelines for drafting executive summaries, it does not definitively address whether these guidelines produce the most effective summaries. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objectives This study aims to identify effective strategies for disseminating scholarly evidence to corrections practitioners within the framework of translational criminology. Methods In this experimental study, corrections practitioners were randomly assigned to receive weekly executive summaries, full-text articles, or corrections news for five months. Data were collected through a pre-survey, a 2-month midpoint survey, and a post-survey to evaluate engagement and knowledge uptake. Results Participants receiving executive summaries showed the highest engagement but did not demonstrate enhanced comprehension compared to those receiving corrections news, who exhibited higher comprehension. Those accessing full-text articles reported the least comprehension and lower engagement, despite finding the information highly useful. Conclusions Given the mixed effectiveness of executive summaries and full-text articles, future research should develop tailored dissemination strategies to better meet the needs of corrections practitioners. The study highlights the critical roles of partnerships and pracademics in translating complex research for practical application.
... Wu, 2021;Suiter & Metcalfe, 2023), and information on the race and gender of the defendant and victim as to whether the public would label the offender as a sex offender (Stevenson et al., 2009). In the realm of policing, scholars have experimentally tested the impact of information on various policing outcomes such as police use of force (Mullinix et al., 2021), police effectiveness (Boehme et al., 2023), body-worn cameras (Demir, 2019), and police reform (Vaughn et al., 2022). ...
... This finding aligns with the sentiment of some police, police administrators, and scholars who argue that discretionary traffic stops are a key crime-suppressing tool as they (1) increase presence in the community, (2) may result in contraband seizure, and (3) lead to arrest of potentially violent criminals (Lee, 2022;Wu & Lum, 2019). These findings also align with other studies that view police as playing a vital role in combatting crime (Boehme et al., 2023;Braga & Weisburd, 2020). While outside the scope of this study, these findings may lend support for other policing strategies/tactics that result in crime reductions (e.g., hot spots, focused deterrence), which, if presented to the public, may result in more positive perceptions of police and conducting crime control strategies. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objectives To assess whether data on traffic stop outcomes causally impacts public approval of discretionary traffic stops as a crime control strategy. Methods We distributed an original online survey experiment randomly assigning respondents (N = 4740) into either the (1) contraband condition, (2) disparity condition, or (3) the control condition. Results In comparison to the control condition, the contraband condition significantly increased respondent support of discretionary traffic stops as a crime-fighting tool. Black respondents assigned the contraband treatment were significantly more likely to support the use of discretionary traffic stops compared to Black respondents assigned the control condition. Conclusions Although scrutiny exists regarding the efficacy of discretionary traffic stops, public opinion may be shifted if they are provided with information on the outcomes of such stops. Police agencies should consider coupling evidence-based strategies with data on the outcomes of crime control strategies, which may also address community desire for more transparency. Minority civilians may support crime control strategies if presented with data on such strategies’ effectiveness.
... We used an information provision survey experimental design (Boehme et al. 2023) to assess whether information about P2P training improved residents' perceptions of the police. Upon entering the experimental component of the survey, respondents were randomized into either treatment or control conditions based on SurveyMonkey's randomization function. ...
... Although police legitimacy is a complex concept with varying definitions and measures across different researchers, for the purposes of this study, police legitimacy was measured using the trust and confidence scale. This operationalization focuses on police legitimacy as the belief that the actions of legal authorities are "appropriate, proper, and just," (Tyler, 2006, p. 375), and is consistent with measurements used in several recent studies (e.g., Boehme et al., 2023;Thompson & Pickett, 2021). Thus, to assess Police Legitimacy, respondents were asked, how much they agreed or disagreed on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = strong disagree, 5 = strongly agree) with the following statements regarding trust and confidence with the police in their neighborhoods, (1) "The police protect people's basic rights," (2) "The police are generally honest," (3) "Most officers do their jobs well," and (4) "The police can be trusted to do what's right for my neighborhood." ...
Article
Full-text available
Given its empirical and public support, increasing police presence seems to be a viable preventive policy option for police agencies. However, the theoretical and empirical literature is not clear on whether this support would vary along race/ethnicity and perceived neighborhood disadvantage, which is relevant given that policies of this kind would likely have a greater impact on disadvantaged communities of color. Using survey data from a national sample, we found that racial and ethnic identity were unrelated to support for increased police presence, while perceived neighborhood disorder was related to greater support for police presence. We also found that neighborhood disorder, social cohesion, and informal social control were indirectly related to support for increased police presence through perceptions of police legitimacy and fear of crime, demonstrating some evidence of the overpolicing-underpolicing paradox in disadvantaged communities. Policy implications of these findings are discussed.
... The impact of scientific research on shaping public opinion, particularly in the context of highly politicised policing issues such as crime statistics, is emphasised by Boehme et al. (2023). This research uses a longitudinal approach to examine the evolution of livestock theft in South Africa from 2013 to 2023, focussing on a quantitative methodology and involving secondary sources. ...
Article
Full-text available
Livestock theft poses a significant challenge to the agricultural sector in South Africa, impacting farmers’ livelihoods, food security, and economic stability. This article analyses livestock theft in South Africa for the ten-year period from 2013 to 2023. It draws on existing literature, government statistics, and analyses of livestock theft statistics. The study identifies the key factors contributing to livestock theft, including regional disparities, seasonal patterns, and the involvement of organised crime. Key findings reveal a steady decline in reported cases from the 2018/2019 financial reporting year, even though the number of stolen livestock has remained relatively high. This is indicative of underreporting, livestock theft incidents; closed-circuit television (CCTV) and surveillance measures; that have had an influence on the data. Practical recommendations are made for combating livestock theft, including improving reporting mechanisms; conducting targeted research on regional hotspots; implementing interventions during peak theft periods; fostering stakeholder collaboration; and increased investment in the training and capacity-building initiatives for law enforcement agencies. This article contributes to understanding the complexities of livestock theft in South Africa. It offers actionable recommendations for policymakers, law enforcement agencies, and agricultural stakeholders to address this pressing issue and protect the economic livelihoods of farming communities.
... In his concurrence in Furman v. Georgia (1972), Justice Thurgood Marshall famously posited that increased awareness about the death penalty tends to foster opposition to it, except when support is grounded in retributive beliefs. In empirical studies, scientific research has influenced public perceptions on various criminal justice matters, including the "Marshall hypothesis" on support for the death penalty (e.g., Bohm & Vogel, 2004;Cochran & Chamlin, 2005), but also punitive attitudes (Indermaur et al., 2012), the perceived effectiveness of the police and criminal justice system (Boehme et al., 2023), and crime prevention strategies (Pickett et al., 2022). ...
... In this study, approximately half of the participants received a treatment condition and approximately half of the respondents received a control condition (participants only received one of the conditions). Treatment randomization in the current study was accomplished using the Qualtrics randomization feature (Boehme et al. 2023). ...
Article
Full-text available
This study examined whether public opinion on drug addiction, perceived culpability/responsibility, and punishment were impacted by statements from medical associations that drug addiction is a disease and not a choice. We utilized an experimental information provision survey distributed via Qualtrics to heads of household in South Carolina with an associated email address. The randomized treatment variable had two conditions. The first condition presented participants with a statement from several noteworthy medical associations/institutions noting that drug addiction is a disease. The second condition provided no statement (control condition). Results from our sample of over 5000 indicated that a large majority of respondents felt that individuals who have been diagnosed with a drug addiction should be fully responsible for any crimes that they commit. The presentation of official statements that addiction was a disease did not produce meaningful differences from the control group. Additionally, a large majority of respondents did not agree that addiction should relieve a defendant from punishment for drug crimes, theft crimes, or violent crimes. Here again, the presentation of official statements that addiction was a disease did not produce meaningful differences from the control groups. Finally, our results indicated that a majority of respondents viewed addiction as both a choice and disease, and the presentation of official statements that addiction was a disease did not meaningfully alter any responses.
Article
Full-text available
Objectives This study examines the extent to which “mass incarceration” has support among the American public as the lynchpin of correctional policy. Methods Three information experiments embedded in a 2023 nationwide YouGov survey of 1000 Americans tested whether providing facts about the extent and nature of incarceration impacts public opinion. Results Few Americans support the imprisonment binge; many are emotionally bothered by it and believe it is immoral, inhumane, and/or racist. The experimental information treatments had little impact on public opinion, which is consistent with the theory that popular discourse, media coverage, and personal and vicarious experiences with incarceration have increased Americans’ awareness of the carceral state. Conclusions The findings align with broader evidence that public punitiveness has declined in recent years, ushering in a new penal sensibility. The results suggest the “get tough” movement has lost traction in the United States, partly because of growing knowledge about the carceral state.
Article
Full-text available
While the events following the death of Michael Brown in Ferguson, Missouri undoubtedly brought about anti-police sentiment, the death of George Floyd may have brought about even greater hostility towards police. Utilizing data from two large police departments, we test whether there were significant increases in the incidence of civilian resistance, Black civilian resistance, use of force, and officer injuries. Estimation of multiple interrupted time series models revealed that except for officer injuries in one agency, all outcomes significantly increased post-Floyd. Implications for policy and practice are discussed. Eprints -https://www.tandfonline.com/eprint/YB7FSHDRHPQEPPQ9IAIV/full?target=10.1080/15614263.2023.2172010
Article
Full-text available
Public opinion is doubly important for reintegration, as it shapes both the policy and the stigma environments that people with criminal records must face. Nowhere are the policy and stigma environments bleaker than for record holders convicted of sex crimes. Drawing on the theory of compassion collapse (or psychic numbing) and using experimental data from a national survey, we examine the effects of informing members of the public about the hardships faced by record holders convicted of sex crimes, and we compare those effects (or the lack thereof) to the effects of victim discourse. We also randomize the information format: aggregate/statistical versus personal narratives. We find that narratives about crime victims’ suffering matter to the public—increasing aversive emotions, support for collateral consequences, and stigmatization—but narratives about record holders’ suffering do not. We conclude by discussing alternative communication strategies that public criminologists may use to garner public support for progressive criminal justice reforms.
Preprint
Full-text available
Demand for democratic accountability in policing is accelerating, yet little is understood about how law enforcement executives engage in policy learning around civilian oversight. This paper shares the results of a novel survey experiment administered to all U.S. police chiefs and sheriffs. We assess whether police executives’ attitudes towards civilian oversight are responsive to 1) state-level public opinion (drawing on an n=16,840 survey) and 2) prior adoption of civilian review boards in large agencies. Results from over 1,300 police executives reveal that law enforcement leaders are responsive to peer adoption but much less to public opinion, despite overwhelming support amongst voters. Further, we find that agencies with an established oversight board are highly supportive of their existence, while elected sheriffs are much less likely to support civilian oversight. Our results indicate that policy learning and reform around civilian oversight are possible, though sources of reform are not themselves primarily democratic.
Article
Full-text available
In the spring and summer of 2020, police in the United States killed Breonna Taylor, George Floyd and other unarmed people of color. In one of the largest social movements in the nation’s history, thousands engaged in public protests and called to defund or abolish the police. Debate about police racism and the need for reform intensified, with public opinion polls showing how polarised public attitudes were along traditional political lines. Analyzing data from a cross-sectional quota sample survey of 1,500 U.S. residents conducted in summer 2020, our findings confirm the proposition that opposition and support for defunding the police was related to not only political views and superordinate identification with the group that the police prototypically represent, but also polarized intergroup identification with the police and the Black Lives Matter movement, as well as people’s perceptions of police procedural justice and systemic racism.
Book
Today's police agencies are in a period of both crisis and reform as they try to improve their ability to deliver public safety to citizens in ways that are effective, legitimate, and sustainable. Evidence-based policing offers one such solution—an approach which emphasises the value that research can bring to police officers and, by extension, the public they serve. However, evidence-based policing is not just about the process of understanding and evaluating police practices. It is also about translating and using that knowledge in daily police activities. This book examines the scientific evidence for the effectiveness of various police practices and provides tools to help turn research into practice. Part I gives a practitioner's definition of evidence-based policing, a primer on how to judge and interpret research findings, and a review of the Evidence-Based Policing Matrix, a tool for translating research on police crime control interventions. In Part II the chapters review the breadth of knowledge about policing interventions for people, places, communities, and technology, focusing on how to optimize operations based on this information. Tools and ideas that can assist in implementing evidence-based practices into patrol, investigations, supervision, management, crime analysis, and leadership are provided in Part III. Finally, in Part IV the chapters contain conversations with researchers about how they might continue to work with police agencies to advance evidence-based policing.
Article
The police murder of George Floyd sparked nationwide protests in the summer of 2020 and revived claims that public outcry over such high‐profile police killings perpetuated a violent “war on cops.” Using data collected by the Gun Violence Archive (GVA) on firearm assaults of U.S. police officers, we use Bayesian structural time series (BSTS) modeling to empirically assess if and how patterns of firearm assault on police officers in the United States were influenced by the police murder of George Floyd. Our analysis finds that the murder of George Floyd was associated with a 3‐week spike in firearm assaults on police, after which the trend in firearms assaults dropped to levels only slightly above that which were predicted by pre‐Floyd data. We discuss potential explanations for these findings and consider their relevance to the contemporary discussion of a “war on cops,” violence, and officer safety.
Article
We use an altmetric aggregator, the Altmetric Attention Score (AAS), to rank the influence of articles published in Criminology & Public Policy from the journal's inception through July 31, 2022. We also rank articles based on specific AAS components, namely, Twitter, news, and policy document mentions. Last, we regress AASs on article‐level predictors, including research category, funding, open access type, and time since publication. With few exceptions, policing scholarship far outweighs other categories of research in terms of AAS‐measured societal impact. In contrast to bibliometrics (e.g., citation counts), altmetrics measure scholarship's societal impact, including its influence on policy. Since Criminology & Public Policy was initially created with the intention of influencing crime‐related policy, it is important to gauge the extent to which that has occurred. Other policy‐oriented (or perhaps all) criminal justice/criminology journals should evaluate their influence via altmetrics.
Article
This study explores whether Americans’ punitiveness and perceptions of redeemability are shaped more by the type of crime committed or by judgements about an offender’s moral character. Guided by theories of neoliberalism, we focus on laziness as an indicator of flawed character that is independent of criminality. A sentencing vignette experiment administered to a national sample of the U.S. population tested the effects of crime type and a defendant’s employment status, work ethic, and race on respondents’ preferred punishment and perceptions of the defendant’s redeemability. Both crime type and work ethic significantly affect perceived (ir)redeemability and sentencing preferences, but the effects are not identical. Work ethic exerts the largest effect on perceived (ir)redeemability, whereas crime type most strongly influences sentencing preferences. We discuss the implications of our findings for act- vs. person-centered theories of punishment, as well as the role of laziness stigma in social responses to lawbreakers.