ArticlePDF Available

Sounds of danger and post-traumatic stress responses in wild rodents: ecological validity of a translational model of post-traumatic stress disorder

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

In the wild, animals face a highly variable world full of predators. Most predator attacks are unsuccessful, and the prey survives. According to the conventional perspective, the fear responses elicited by predators are acute and transient in nature. However, the long-term, non-lethal effects of predator exposure on prey behavioral stress sequelae, such as anxiety and post-traumatic symptoms, remain poorly understood. Most experiments on animal models of anxiety-related behavior or post-traumatic stress disorder have been carried out using commercial strains of rats and mice. A fundamental question is whether laboratory rodents appropriately express the behavioral responses of wild species in their natural environment; in other words, whether behavioral responses to stress observed in the laboratory can be generalized to natural behavior. To further elucidate the relative contributions of the natural selection pressures influences, this study investigated the bio-behavioral and morphological effects of auditory predator cues (owl territorial calls) in males and females of three wild rodent species in a laboratory set-up: Acomys cahirinus; Gerbillus henleyi; and Gerbillus gerbillus. Our results indicate that owl territorial calls elicited not only “fight or flight” behavioral responses but caused PTSD-like behavioral responses in wild rodents that have never encountered owls in nature and could cause, in some individuals, enduring physiological and morphological responses that parallel those seen in laboratory rodents or traumatized people. In all rodent species, the PTSD phenotype was characterized by a blunting of fecal cortisol metabolite response early after exposure and by a lower hypothalamic orexin-A level and lower total dendritic length and number in the dentate gyrus granule cells eight days after predator exposure. Phenotypically, this refers to a significant functional impairment that could affect reproduction and survival and thus fitness and population dynamics.
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
ARTICLE OPEN
Sounds of danger and post-traumatic stress responses in
wild rodents: ecological validity of a translational model
of post-traumatic stress disorder
Hagit Cohen
1,2
, Michael A. Matar
1
, Doron Todder
1
, Carmit Cohen
1
, Joseph Zohar
3
, Hadas Hawlena
4
and Zvika Abramsky
5
© The Author(s) 2023
In the wild, animals face a highly variable world full of predators. Most predator attacks are unsuccessful, and the prey survives.
According to the conventional perspective, the fear responses elicited by predators are acute and transient in nature. However, the
long-term, non-lethal effects of predator exposure on prey behavioral stress sequelae, such as anxiety and post-traumatic
symptoms, remain poorly understood. Most experiments on animal models of anxiety-related behavior or post-traumatic stress
disorder have been carried out using commercial strains of rats and mice. A fundamental question is whether laboratory rodents
appropriately express the behavioral responses of wild species in their natural environment; in other words, whether behavioral
responses to stress observed in the laboratory can be generalized to natural behavior. To further elucidate the relative contributions
of the natural selection pressures inuences, this study investigated the bio-behavioral and morphological effects of auditory
predator cues (owl territorial calls) in males and females of three wild rodent species in a laboratory set-up: Acomys cahirinus;
Gerbillus henleyi; and Gerbillus gerbillus. Our results indicate that owl territorial calls elicited not only ght or ightbehavioral
responses but caused PTSD-like behavioral responses in wild rodents that have never encountered owls in nature and could cause,
in some individuals, enduring physiological and morphological responses that parallel those seen in laboratory rodents or
traumatized people. In all rodent species, the PTSD phenotype was characterized by a blunting of fecal cortisol metabolite response
early after exposure and by a lower hypothalamic orexin-A level and lower total dendritic length and number in the dentate gyrus
granule cells eight days after predator exposure. Phenotypically, this refers to a signicant functional impairment that could affect
reproduction and survival and thus tness and population dynamics.
Molecular Psychiatry; https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-023-02240-7
INTRODUCTION
Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is a potentially chronic
impairing disorder involving cognitive, emotional, and physiolo-
gical failure to adequately process and/or recover from exposure
to a traumatic experience [1]. The memories of a traumatic event
remain vivid, chronically active, and disruptive over long periods
of time, together with the emotions at the time of the event,
shaping symptoms such as intrusive thoughts, physiological
hyperarousal, active avoidance of traumatic reminders, and
negative alterations in cognitions and mood.
From an evolutionary-ecological perspective, memory is dis-
tinctly advantageous for survival. An organisms ability to form
and retain a record, especially of threatening events, and to
accumulate and maintain this information to allow for ready
access and ongoing updating, that is, to form memories, confers
the ability to anticipate danger and prepare for or avoid it. In
natural habitats, animals are required to continually assess and
evaluate the potential risks they face while balancing the need for
food acquisition with the need for safety, considering the dynamic
changes in resource availability, competition intensity, and
predation risk. Therefore, animals have evolved a variety of
mechanisms for evaluating the potential risk of predation,
implementing antipredator decision-making strategies, and devel-
oping adaptive behavioral responses to these conditions [27]. For
example, when faced with predation risk, some prey individuals
may alter their selection of micro or macro-habitats, seeking
concealment, while others may exhibit morphological changes
such as alteration of coloration, to enhance camouage and
increase the trade-off between food acquisition and safety [6].
According to the conventional perspective, the fear responses
elicited by predators are acute and transient in nature [8]. The prey
identies the predator and initiates a behavioral response (eeing,
ghting, or freezing). Following the encounter, the animal either
survives or dies. If survival occurs, it is commonly assumed that the
animal resumes its normal activities [9] and physiological
equilibrium is restored. However, studies on commercial strains
of rats and mice have shown that non-lethal, long-lasting effects
of predators, such as fear, anxiety, or post-traumatic responses,
Received: 1 February 2023 Revised: 17 August 2023 Accepted: 24 August 2023
1
Faculty of Health Sciences, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Israel & Ministry of Health, Anxiety and Stress Research Unit, Beer-Sheva Mental Health Center, Beer-Sheva, Israel.
2
Department of Psychology, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beer-Sheva, Israel.
3
Post-Trauma Center, Sheba Medical Center, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv 52621, Israel.
4
Mitrani Department of Desert Ecology, The Jacob Blaustein Institutes for Desert Research, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Midreshet Ben-Gurion Israel, Sde Boker 8499000,
Israel.
5
Department of Life Sciences and Ramon Science Center, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beer-Sheva 84105, Israel. email: hagitc@bgu.ac.il
www.nature.com/mp
Molecular Psychiatry
1234567890();,:
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
can have a signicant impact on individual morphology, behavior,
and reproductive success [1014]. A fundamental question is thus
the extent to which experimental ndings using a commercial
strain of rats or mice can be generalized to real-world situations. In
particular, it is not known if there are post-traumasymptoms for
animals in the wild or which behavioral stress responses observed
in the laboratory appear in wild animals in the eld [9,15]. Should
we expect that in natural communities the non-lethal enduring
effects of predators will be signicant as well, and whether these
effects will have a greater impact on wild prey populations (and
on predators) in terms of individual behavior, population
dynamics, and reproductive success compared to the non-lethal
short-term effects and the lethal effects.
To ll the above knowledge gaps, we investigated the effects of
predator stress on the behavioral, molecular, and morphological
responses of wild rodent species in a laboratory setup. We studied
males and females of three rodent species: Acomys Cahirinus
(A-Cahirinus); Gerbillus Henleyi (G-Henleyi); and Gerbillus Gerbillus (G-
Gerbillus) (Supplement #1).
Gerbils are particularly interesting for the study of PTSD as their
adrenal glands are comparable to the adrenals of the humans, due
to their high concentration of ascorbic acid and their secretion of
cortisol, unlike most rodents [1618].
The goal of this study was to evaluate the bio-behavioral and
morphological effects of predator cues (playback of tape-recorded
owl calls) in males and females of three wild rodent species. To
this end, we used (a) the cut-off behavioral criteria (CBC)
classication model. In this model, populations of exposed rodents
are classied based on the degree of their individual behavioral
response, creating three distinct groups: extreme,”“partial,or
minimalbehavioral response (EBR, PBR, and MBR, respectively)
(Supplement #2), (b) measured fecal cortisol metabolite (FCM)
levels 12 h after predator stress, (c) examined orexin-A levels in
the hypothalamus eight days after exposure, and (d) we evaluated
changes in the cytoarchitecture of the hippocampus using the
Golgi-Cox method.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Animals
We used two gerbilline species (G-Henleyi and G-Gerbillus) and one murine
species (A-Cahirinus), which are burrow dwellers of the Negev Desert in
Israel. Twenty-three G-Henleyi (10 females and 13 males), fteen G-Gerbillus
(8 females and 7 males), and thirty-ve A-Cahirinus (19 females and 16
males) specimens were used in this study.
All rodents were descendants of wild rodents captured in the vicinity of
the Negev Desert in Southern Israel. After trapping, the animals were
transported to Midreshet Ben-Gurion, where they were housed in pairs
(male and female) and bred for 68 generations. Considering the small
number of generations compared to the evolution of the species themselves
[19,20], these rodents, which were kept and bred under semi-natural
conditions, can be considered wild rather than domesticated [20].
All rodents used in the experiments were adults and were acclimatized
to the conditions to which they were later exposed for at least one month
prior to the habituation experiments. Rodents were housed in pairs in
plastic cages (60 × 50 × 40 cm) under controlled temperature (25 ± 1 °C)
and humidity (30 ± 5%) conditions, with a photoperiod of 12D:12 L (lights
on at 0700 h) and with sawdust and dried grass as bedding material. They
were provided daily with millet seeds ad libitum and fresh alfalfa
(Medicago sp.) as a water source following Hawlena et al. [21]. All
experimental procedures were conducted between 13:0016:00.
Experimental design
The base level of anxiety-related behavior (and the intensity of the
response to stressor) were determined using the open eld test (OFT). This
test was chosen over the elevated plus maze (EPM) because repeated
testing on the same animals reduces its validity [22]. After being exposed
to predator cues for 10 min, fresh feces were collected to test for cortisol
levels. Behavioral tests were conducted using the OFT, EPM, and ASR tests
on day 7. Exploratory behavior in the EPM serves as the main platform for
the assessment of overall behavior, and the ASR paradigm provides a
precise quantication of hyperalertness in terms of the magnitude of
response and habituation to the stimulus. These data were used to classify
the animals into behavioral response groups. The rats were then sacriced,
and brains were collected for morphology analysis. The baseline FCM
levels were evaluated in G-Gerbillus specimens.
Stress exposure
Individual rodents were exposed to tape-recorded territorial calls of owls.
Stress was induced by placing the test animals in a plastic cage
(40 × 40 × 40 cm) that was situated on a yard paving stone for 10 min in
a closed environment. Sound was transmitted into the arena using a small
loudspeaker (2.5 inch, 10 Ω, 0.2 W max) placed on the rear wall, 45 cm
above the oor, and connected to a cassette recorder. The sound levels of
the tape-recorded owl calls were standardized to approximately 6070 dB.
After 10 min of playback of the owl calls, we turned off the recording
device and left the rodents in the arena for another 5 min of silence. This
period simulates the hunting pattern of the owl, which, following a period
of territorial calling, ies to a hunting perch where it waits in complete
silence to pounce on its prey [23].
Behavioral assessments
All behavioral tests were video-recorded using the ETHO-VISION program
(Noldus), by an investigator blinded to the experimental protocol.
Behaviors of specimens were assessed using OFT, EPM and ASR, as
described previously [2426]. Detailed protocols are described in
Supplementary Information #3:1.
Fecal cortisol metabolites
This non-invasive technique for measuring steroid metabolites in fecal
samples has been established in an increasing number of species [2733].
Fresh fecal samples were collected, and any feces contaminated with urine
was excluded from the analysis. The fecal samples were placed in 2 mL
microfuge tubes and immediately frozen at 20 °C for subsequent
analysis. Hormones were extracted using the method described by
Gutman et al. [34]. Fecal cortisol metabolites were measured using a
radioimmunoassay kit (ICN Biomedical, Inc.) in duplicate.
Brains
24-hours after the behavioral tests, between 12:00 and 14:00, the animals
were deeply anesthetized and perfused intracardially with saline. The
brains were immediately removed, and the hypothalamus was dissected.
Each hypothalamus was washed in saline, weighed, and frozen at 80 °C
until later use. Brains (without hypothalamus) were prepared using a rapid
Golgi kit (FD Neurotechnologies, USA) according to the manufacturers
instructions (Supplementary Information 3:2).
Orexin-A levels in the hypothalamus
The hypothalamus samples were homogenized (PRO Scientic Inc., CT,
USA) at 24,000 rpm for 2 min in ice-cold PBS and centrifuged at 15,000 rpm
for 30 min at 4 °C. The supernatant was separated and stored at 80 °C.
Orexin-A levels in brain tissue were measured using a commercially
available enzyme-linked immunoassay kit (MyBioSource, Inc., San Diego,
CA, USA) according to the manufacturers protocol, in duplicates.
Statistical analyses
All data are expressed as mean±standard error of the mean (SEM), and
statistical analyses were performed using a two-way analysis of variance
(ANOVA). For the OFT, statistical analyses were performed using repeated
measures (RM)-ANOVA. Where signicant group effects were detected, the
Bonferroni test assessed signicant post-hoc differences between groups.
The prevalence of PTSD-classied groups was tested in relation to rodent
species group or sex using cross-tabulation, chi-square tests, and logistic
regression analysis. To gain an additional understanding of the relationship
between behavioral and molecular/morphology measures, Pearsons
correlation analysis was performed.
RESULTS
To preclude the possible effects of basal pre-trauma anxiety,
which can be a risk factor for the development and persistence of
H. Cohen et al.
2
Molecular Psychiatry
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
PTSD, animals were rst evaluated in the OFT under basal
conditions. We found that the level of anxiety-related behavior
was low: the path length or thigmotaxis all indicated a low level of
anxiety-related behavior without any differences among the
species or between sexes. In all species, exposure to the owl cues
reduced the percentage of time spent in the inner and middle
zones and increased the time spent in the outer zone, which are
common indices of anxiety-related behavior (Supplementary
Information #4:0).
In terms of anxiety index (Fig. 1A), two-way ANOVA revealed a
signicant effect of species (F(2,67) =10.2, p< 0.00015, respec-
tively) and sex (F(2,67) =4.9, p< 0.035, respectively). Bonferroni
test conrmed that female A-Cahirinus exposed to predator stress
spent signicantly less time in the open arms, entered the open
arms less frequently, and exhibited a higher anxiety index than
female G-Gerbillus (p< 0.005, p< 00002, and p< 0.0035, respec-
tively) (Supplementary Information #4:1 for all EPM parameters).
No species or sex differences in the ASR or startle habituation
(Fig. 1B, C) were observed.
Relative prevalence rates according to CBC-classication
The species didnt differ in their overall responses to imposed
predator cue. There were no signicant differences in the
prevalence of EBR (PTSD phenotype) (Fig. 1D), MBR (Fig. 1E), or
PBR (Fig. 1F) among species or between sexes. No predictor
variable reached signicance.
The prevalence of EBR among A-Cahirinus females was 26.3%
(5/19), and 25.0% (4/16) among A-Cahirinus males. Moreover, in
A-Cahirinus, 2 females (10.5%) and 1 male (6.25%) fullled the
criteria for MBR, and 12 females (63.15%) and 11 males (68.75%)
were classied as PBR. In G-Henleyi, the prevalence of EBR in
females was 20.0% (2/10) and 23.0% (3/13) among males.
Moreover, 3 females (30.0%) and 1 male (7.69%) fullled the
criteria for MBR, and 5 females (50.0%) and 9 males (69.23%) were
classied as PBR. In G-Gerbillus, the prevalence of EBR females was
12.5% (1/8) and 42.85% (3/7) among males, without signicant
differences, likely due to the small sample size. Moreover, 3
females (37.5%) and 1 male (14.3%) fullled the criteria for MBR,
and 4 females (50.0%) and 3 males (37.5%) were classied as PBR.
In G-Gerbillus, although a χ2 analysis indicated that sex did not
affect the prevalence of the extremes in the behavioral response
to stress (Fisher exact p=0.023, not signicant), marked
behavioral differences were evident between female and male
rodents. Male G-Gerbillus showed a higher prevalence of EBR than
female rats.
Overall, the prevalence of EBR in A-Cahirinus species was 25.71%
(9/35), while in G-Henleyi or G-Henleyi species, it was 21.7% (5/23)
and 26.67% (4/15), respectively.
FCM
In A-Cahirinus and G-Henleyi, measurements were made only after
exposure to stress, that is, without basal level measurements.
Overall, two-way ANOVA revealed a signicant effect of species on
FCM levels (F(1,67) =6.85, p< 0.015).
In A-Cahirinus, FCM concentrations were not homogeneous and
wide scattering was detected (Fig. 2A). No signicant differences
were found in FCM concentrations between females and males,
but there was a trend of lower FCM concentrations in males
(F(1,29) =2.9, p=0.09). Looking at FCM concentrations between
PTSD-classied groups (Fig. 2B), two-way ANOVA revealed a
1)
FMFMFM
0.0
0.2
0.4
0.6
0.8
1.0
)U.A(xednIyteixnA
p<0.0035
A.Cahirinus G. Henleyi G.Gerbilus
FMFMFM
0
200
400
600
800
1000
)U.A(esnopseReltratS
A.Cahirinus G. Henleyi G.Gerbilus
FMFMFM
0
10
20
30
40
50
60
70
80
)%(noitautibaHeltratS
A.Cahirinus G. Henleyi G.Gerbilus
A) B) C)
D) E) F)
FM FM FM
0
10
20
30
40
50
60
70
80
90
100
RBMfoecnelaverP
A.Cahirinus G. Henleyi G.Gerbilus
FM FM FM
0
10
20
30
40
50
60
70
80
90
100
RBEfoecnelaverP
A.Cahirinus G. Henleyi G.Gerbilus
FM FM FM
0
10
20
30
40
50
60
70
80
90
100
RB
Pf
o
ecne
lave
r
P
A.Cahirinus G. Henleyi G.Gerbilus
Fig. 1 The long-term effects of predator cue exposure on behavior. The top panel (1) depicts the experimental protocol. The red circle
signies the behavioral test performed, for which the results are shown. AAnxiety Index which integrates the measured EPM behavioral
measures. BStartle amplitude in the acoustic startle response (ASR) paradigm. CPercentage of startle habituation in the ASR paradigm.
DPrevalence of extreme behavioral response (EBR) (in percentages), EPrevalence of minimal behavioral response (MBR) (in percentages),
FPrevalence of partial behavioral response (PBR) (in percentages). Owl territory calls had long-lasting inuences on rodent behavior; all
species reacted signicantly to predator cue stress in terms of anxiety-related behavior in the EPM and ASR paradigms 7 days after exposure.
In addition, predator cue exposure causes post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD)-like behavioral responses in wild rodents that have never
encountered owls. Data are presented as data points and mean ± SEM and percentage.
H. Cohen et al.
3
Molecular Psychiatry
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
signicant difference in FCM concentrations between groups
(F(2,29) =9.7, p< 0.0006). In females, Bonferroni post-hoc tests
conrmed signicantly lower FCM concentrations in EBR than in
PBR (p< 0.0003) and MBR (p< 0.004) rodents. In males, Bonferroni
tests conrmed signicantly lower FCM concentrations in EBR
than in PBR (p< 0.05) rodents.
In G-Henleyi, no signicant differences were found in FCM levels
between females and males (Fig. 2C). Two-way ANOVA revealed a
signicant effect of groups (F(2,17) =8.3, p< 0.0035) on FCM
concentrations (Fig. 2D). In females, Bonferroni tests conrmed
signicantly lower FCM concentrations in EBR than in PBR
(p< 0.005) and MBR (p< 0.05) rodents. In males, Bonferroni post-
hoc tests conrmed signicantly lower FCM concentrations in EBR
than in PBR (p< 0.03).
In G-Gerbillus,werst monitored baseline FCM concentrations
and subsequently measured the post-stress FCM response. RM-
ANOVA revealed a signicant effect of sex (F(1,16) =8.3, p< 0.015)
and stress (F(1,16) =26.2, p< 0.00025) (Fig. 2E). In both females
and males, stress signicantly increased FCM concentration
relative to the baseline conditions. Bonferroni tests indicated that
all exposed rodents exhibited signicantly elevated mean FCM
concentrations compared with those in the control group
(females: p< 0.0001 and males: p< 0.015). Moreover, both under
baseline conditions and stress conditions the FCM concentrations
were signicantly higher in females than in males (p< 0.003 and
p< 0.05, respectively). Two-way ANOVA revealed a signicant
effect of groups (F(2,9) =7.76, p< 0.015) on FCM concentrations
(Fig. 2F). No effects were observed for sex, but there was a trend of
lower FCM concentrations in males (F(1,9) =3.6, p=0.09). In
females, Bonferroni tests conrmed signicantly lower FCM
concentrations in EBR than in PBR (p< 0.05) and MBR (p< 0.01).
In males, Bonferroni tests conrmed signicantly lower FCM
concentrations in EBR than in PBR (p< 0.02).
We conducted regression analyses to further understand the
relationship between FCM levels and behavioral measures,
regardless of CBC classication (Table 1). In A-Cahirinus,
G-Henleyi and G-Gerbillus Pearsons correlation analysis revealed
that FCM concentrations were signicantly and negatively
correlated with the anxiety index.
Brain orexin-A levels
Changes in total orexin-A levels in rodent brains following stress
exposure are shown in Fig. 3. Overall, no signicant differences
were found in orexin-A levels among species or between sexes.
In A-Cahirinus, no signicant differences were found in orexin-A
levels between females and males (Fig. 3A). Two-way ANOVA
revealed a signicant effect of groups (F(2,29) =38.2, p< 0.00016)
(Fig. 3B). In both sexes, Bonferroni tests conrmed signicantly
lower orexin-A levels in EBR than in PBR (p< 0.0001 for both
females and males) and MBR (females: p< 0.0025 and males:
p< 0.035).
In G-Henleyi, no signicant differences were found in orexin-A
levels between females and males (Fig. 3C). Two-way ANOVA
revealed a signicant effect of groups (F(2,17) =15.2, p< 0.0002)
Female Male
0
50
100
150
200
250
300
350
400
(slevellositroclaceF )Ld/g
A.Cahirinus: 1-2 h after predator exposure
Female Male
0
50
100
150
200
250
300
G. Henleyi: 1-2 h after predator exposure
(s
leve
ll
os
it
roc
lace
F)Ld/g
Female Male Female Male
0
20
40
60
80
100
120
140
160
180
200
220
Baseline 1-2 h after stress
G. Gerbilus
(slevellositroclaceF )Ld/g
P<0.05
P<0.003
P<0.015
P<0.0001
EBR PBR MBR EBR PBR MBR
0
50
100
150
200
250
300
350
p<0.05
p<0.0003
A.Cahirinus: 1-2 h after predator exposure
p<0.004
slevellositroClaceF(g/dL)
Females Males
EBR PBR MBR EBR PBR MBR
0
50
100
150
200
250
300
350
Females Males
p<0.02
p<0.005
G. Henleyi: 1-2 h after predator exposure
p<0.03
slevellositroClaceF(g/dL)
EBR PBR MBR EBR PBR MBR
0
50
100
150
200
250
300
350
Females Males
p<0.05 p<0.02
G. Gerbilus: 1-2 h after predator exposure
p<0.01
slevellositroClaceF(g/dL)
1)
A)
B)
C)
D)
E)
F)
Fig. 2 The effects of predator cue exposure on fecal cortisol metabolite (FCM) levels. The top panel (1) depicts the experimental protocol.
The red circle signies the behavioral test performed, for which the results are shown. AThe FCM levels, collected 12 h after predator stress
exposure, in female and male Acomys cahirinus.BThe FCM levels in Acomys cahirinus according to the affected groups (classied using the cut-
off behavioral criteria (CBC) method). CThe FCM levels, collected 12 h after predator stress exposure, in female and male Gerbillus henleyi.
DThe FCM levels in Gerbillus henleyi according to the affected groups (classied using the CBC method). EThe FCM levels, collected 12 h after
predator stress exposure, in female and male Gerbillus gerbillus.FThe FCM levels in Gerbillus gerbillus according to the affected groups
(classied using the CBC method). Although we tested basal FCM concentrations in G. gerbillus before predator exposure and found that the
FCM concentrations were relatively low and signicantly elevated in response to the stressor compared to their baseline levels, it is still
unclear whether the poor FCM response to predator stress and the dysregulation observed in the acute aftermath of trauma represent an
existing pre-trauma vulnerability trait or develops from the exposure to the trauma itself. Data are presented as data points and mean ± SEM.
H. Cohen et al.
4
Molecular Psychiatry
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
(Fig. 3D). In females, Bonferroni tests conrmed signicantly lower
orexin-A levels in EBR than in MBR (p< 0.05). In males, Bonferroni
post-hoc tests conrmed signicantly lower orexin-A levels in EBR
than in PBR (p< 0.0015).
In G-Gerbillus, no signicant differences were found in orexin-A
levels between females and males (Fig. 3E). Two-way ANOVA
revealed a signicant effect of groups (F(2,9) =14.4, p< 0.002) on
orexin-A levels (Fig. 3F). In males, Bonferroni tests conrmed
signicantly lower orexin-A levels in EBR than in PBR (p< 0.015).
In A-Cahirinus,G-Henleyi and Gerbillus-G Pearsons correlation
analysis revealed that orexin-A levels were signicantly and
negatively correlated with the anxiety index (Table 1).
Morphology of DG granular neurons
Overall, no signicant differences were found in the total dendritic
length or number among species or between sexes (Figs. 45).
In A-Cahirinus, no signicant differences were found in the total
dendritic length (Fig. 4A) or number (Fig. 4C) between females
and males. Eight days after predator stress, the total dendritic
length (Fig. 4B) and total dendritic number (Fig. 4D) were
signicantly lower in both female and male EBR animals than in
the PBR group (p< 0.0001 and p< 0.00025, respectively).
In G-Henleyi, no signicant differences were found in the total
dendritic length (Fig. 4E) or number (Fig. 4G) between females and
males. The total dendritic length (Fig. 4F) and the total dendritic
number (Fig. 4H) were signicantly lower in both female and male
EBR animals than in the PBR (p< 0.0001 and p< 0.0002,
respectively) and MBR (p< 0.0001 and p< 0.02, respectively)
groups.
In G-Gerbillus, no signicant differences were found in the total
dendritic length (Fig. 4I) or number (Fig. 4K) between females and
males. The total dendritic length (Fig. 4J) and the total dendritic
number (Fig. 4L) were signicantly lower in both female and male
EBR animals than in the PBR (p< 0.0065 and p< 0.00015,
respectively) and MBR (p< 0.0001 and p< 0.00065, respectively)
groups.
Female Male
0
20
40
60
80
100
120
140
160
180
200
)gm/gp(A-nixerOcimalahtopyH
A.Cahirinus: 8 days after predator exposure
Female Male
0
20
40
60
80
100
120
140
160
180
200
G. Henleyi: 8 days after predator exposure
)gm/gp(A-nixerOcimalahtopyH
Female Male
0
20
40
60
80
100
120
140
160
180
200
)gm/gp(A-nixerOcimalahtopyH
G. Gerbilus: 8 days after predator exposure
EBR PBR MBR EBR PBR MBR
0
20
40
60
80
100
120
140
160
180
200
p<0.0025
p<0.0001
p<0.0001
A.Cahirinus: 8 days after predator exposure
p<0.035
A-
ni
x
er
Ocim
ala
ht
o
p
y
H(l
m
/
g
p)
Females Males
EBR PBR MBR EBR PBR MBR
0
20
40
60
80
100
120
140
160
180
200
Females Males
p<0.05
G. Henleyi: 8 days after predator exposure
p<0.0015
A-nixerOcimalahtopyH(lm/gp)
EBR PBR MBR EBR PBR MBR
0
20
40
60
80
100
120
140
160
180
200
Females Males
G. Gerbilus: 8 days after predator exposure
A-nixerOcimalahtopyH(lm/gp)
p<0.015
1)
A)
B)
C) E)
D) F)
Fig. 3 The long-term effects of predator cue exposure on hypothalamic levels of orexin A. (1) The top panel depicts the experimental
protocol. The red circle signies the test performed whose results are shown. AHypothalamic levels of orexin-A (ORX-A) in females and males
Acomys cahirinus species. BHypothalamic levels of ORX-A in Acomys cahirinus species according to the affected groups (according to the cut-
off behavioral criteria (CBC) method). CHypothalamic levels of ORX-A in females and males Gerbillus henleyi species. DHypothalamic levels of
ORX-A in Gerbillus henleyi species according to the affected groups (according to the CBC method). EHypothalamic levels of ORX-A in females
and males Gerbillus gerbillus species. FHypothalamic levels of ORX-A in Gerbillus gerbillus species according to the affected groups (according
to the CBC method). Data are presented as data points and mean ± SEM.
Table 1. Correlation analysis between anxiety index and fecal cortisol levels.
Anxiety index
Acomys Cahirinus Gerbillus Henleyi Gerbillus Gerbillus
Fecal cortisol levels (µg/dL) r=0.57, p< 0.0004, n=35 r=0.42, p< 0.05, n=23 r=0.8, p< 0.0004, n=15
Orexin-A levels (pg/ml) r=0.53, p< 0.001, n=35 r=0.61, p< 0.0025, n=23 r=0.81, p< 0.0003, n=23
DG dendritic Number r=0.68, p< 0.0025, n=24 r=0.61, p< 0.003, n=23 r=0.89, p< 0.0001, n=15
DG dendritic Length (µm) r=0.57, p< 0.004, n=24 r=0.55, p< 0.007, n=23 r=0.75, p< 0.002, n=15
Marked correlations are signicant at p< 0.05.
H. Cohen et al.
5
Molecular Psychiatry
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
In A-Cahirinus,G-Henleyi and Gerbillus-G Pearsons correlation
analysis revealed that dendritic number and length were
signicantly and negatively correlated with the anxiety index
(Table 1).
DISCUSSION
This study aimed to explore the application of our standard model
and procedures for translational studies of PTSD in laboratory
rodents to three groups of wild rodents of both sexes, specically
A-Cahirinus,G-Henleyi, and G-Gerbillus, in an adapted format. This
study employed recordings of owl territorial calls as the trigger, to
which they had never previously been exposed in their lifetime.
The results indicate that this evolutionary trauma cue indeed
elicited not only the acute ght or ightresponses to the
potential predator threat, but also signicant long-term beha-
vioral, neurobiological, and morphological sequelae that are
completely in line with our previous ndings in laboratory
rodents. The results similarly mirror ndings from clinical studies
in traumatized patients, changes that could signicantly compro-
mise functions related to survival and reproduction.
All species reacted to the playback of owl vocalizations
signicantly in terms of anxiety-related behavior in the EPM,
OFT, and ASR paradigms and in the overall pattern of resultant
FCM concentrations, orexin-A levels, and DG dendritic arboriza-
tion, validating the potentially traumatizing effect of the stressor.
However, bio-behavioral stress responses showed extensive
individual phenotypic heterogeneity at the baseline and after
predator exposure within the species. Marked behavioral (i.e.,
phenotypic) differences were evident in all paradigms among
individuals within each species after stress exposure.
Female Male
0
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
rebmunciti
r
dn
e
d
l
atoT
A.Cahirinus
Female Male
0
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
G. Henleyi:
rebmuncitirdnedlatoT
Female Male
0
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
rebmuncitirdnedlatoT
G.Gerbilus
EBR PBR MBR EBR PBR MBR
0
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
p<0.00025
p<0.0001
A.Cahirinus
rebmuncitirdnedlatoT
Females Males
EBR PBR MBR EBR PBR MBR
0
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
p<0.02
p<0.0002
p<0.0001
G. Henleyi
p<0.0001
rebmuncitirdnedlatoT
Females Males
EBR PBR MBR EBR PBR MBR
0
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
p<0.0001
p<0.00065
p<0.0015
p<0.0065
G. Gerbilus
rebmuncitirdnedlatoT
Females Males
Female Male
0
300
600
900
1200
1500
1800
2100
2400
2700
3000
(htgnelcitirdnedlatoT m)
G.Gerbilus
Female Male
0
300
600
900
1200
1500
1800
2100
2400
2700
3000
G. Henleyi
(htgnelcitirdnedlatoT m)
Female Male
0
300
600
900
1200
1500
1800
2100
2400
2700
3000
(
ht
g
nelci
t
ird
n
edla
t
o
Tm)
A.Cahirinus
EBR PBR MBR EBR PBR MBR
0
300
600
900
1200
1500
1800
2100
2400
2700
3000
p<0.0003
p<0.0001
p<0.0001
A.Cahirinus
htgnelcitirdnedlatoT(m)
Females Males
EBR PBR MBR EBR PBR MBR
0
300
600
900
1200
1500
1800
2100
2400
2700
3000
p<0.0075
p<0.005
p<0.0007
G. Henleyi
p<0.0005
ht
g
nelci
t
i
r
dn
e
dlatoT (m)
Females Males
EBR PBR MBR EBR PBR MBR
0
300
600
900
1200
1500
1800
2100
2400
2700
3000
p<0.04
G. Gerbilus
htgnelcitirdnedlatoT(m)
Females Males
1)
A) B) C) D)
E) F) G) H)
I) J) K) L)
Fig. 4 The long-term effects of predator cue exposure on morphology of dentate gyrus (DG) granular neurons. (1) The top panel depicts
the experimental protocol. The red circle signies the test performed, for which the results are shown. AQuantitative analysis of total
dendritic length (μm), and (C) total dendritic number of dentate gyrus granule cells from the suprapyramidal blade in female and male Acomys
cahirinus,Gerbillus henleyi (E,G, respectively), and Gerbillus gerbillus (I,K, respectively). BQuantitative analysis of total dendritic length (μm), and
(D) total dendritic number of dentate gyrus granule cells according to the affected groups in Acomys cahirinus,Gerbillus henleyi
(F,Hrespectively), and Gerbillus gerbillus (J,L, respectively). Data are presented as data points and mean ± SEM.
Fig. 5 Computer-generated plots of reconstructions of the dendritic
trees from granule cells of female and male Acomys cahirinus,
Gerbillus henleyi, and Gerbillus gerbillus.
H. Cohen et al.
6
Molecular Psychiatry
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Separating out the more clearly affected animals using CBC
enabled a more precise assessment of the magnitude and
character of the predator effect [25]. Accordingly, the prevalence
of severely behaviorally affected animals (PTSD phenotype) in
A-Cahirinus was 26.3% in females and 25% in males, whereas in
G-Henleyi it was 20% in females and 23% in males. The prevalence
of the PTSD phenotype in G-Gerbillus was 12.5% in females and
41.85% in males. Nevertheless, there were no statistically
signicant differences in the prevalence rates of PTSD phenotype
among species or between sexes, but it is possible that the
absence of species and sex differences is a result of the low
statistical power due to the limited sample size. Future studies
should use larger sample sizes. Taken together, predator cue
exposure induces PTSD-like symptoms in wild rodents, similar to
those in laboratory rats [3537] or mice [38].
Our results corroborate previous results on possible PTSD-like
symptoms in adult female wild captive wolves (Canis lupus),
elephants, chimpanzees and birds (black-capped chickadees,
Poecile atricapillus)[3942].
HPA-axis
Retrospective analysis of FCM concentrations collected 12 hours
after predator exposure showed that among all wild rodent
species, the PTSD phenotype individuals were typied by a
blunting of the FCM response to the stressor, which was
signicantly different from those observed in less or non-
affected groups. As expected, rodents with less extreme patterns
of behavioral responses (partial or minimal behavioral disruption)
displayed a signicant increase in FCM stress response. The
validity of these ndings was supported by the results of the
Pearson correlation analysis performed irrespective of the CBC
classication, which revealed that FCM concentrations were
signicantly and negatively correlated with the anxiety index,
indicating that lower FCM concentrations shortly after exposure
predicted higher anxiety levels overall.
The blunted cortisol response indicates an underlying dysfunc-
tion in the overall dynamic modulation action of the HPA axis in
rodents with the PTSD phenotype. This blunted FCM concentra-
tion could prolong the availability of norepinephrine to synapses
in both the periphery and brain [4345], which, in turn, might
affect the consolidation of the memory of the traumatic event.
Adrenergic activation in the face of low cortisol levels facilitates
fear memory in animals [46]. Additionally, glucocorticoids play an
important role during foraging, as their release controls appetite
and food intake [47]. Allenbys gerbils implanted with cortisol
foraged longer, but harvested food more slowly due to greater
vigilance and apprehension than placebo-treated gerbils [48]. The
authors suggested that glucocorticoids affect energy acquisition
and provide a physiological context to explain how foragers
manage risks and the trade-off between food and safety [48].
Gutman et al. [34] studied FCM levels and foraging of nocturnal
A-Cahirinus and diurnal Acomys russatus and found that both
species exhibited high FCM concentrations and reduced foraging
when the moon was full, suggesting that reduced foraging may
be mediated by increased glucocorticoid concentrations [34]. Tree
lizards treated with exogenous glucocorticoids responded more
quickly to predator stress and hid for a longer duration (decreased
feeding effort/reduced foraging) than did control lizards [49].
Moreover, the HPA axis of wild strain birds reacted more quickly to
capture and handling (corticosterone levels increase within 250 s)
and more strongly (higher corticosterone peak values) than the
domesticated strain [50]. Together with our previous results and
the ecology literature (which is not limited to laboratory rodents),
a clearer picture of the HPA axis as a contributing factor to the
development of PTSD is starting to emerge. Based on this
evidence, we hypothesize that during an acute threat, the quick
initiation and strong modulation action of the HPA axis should
also entail rapid termination of the physiological stress response
through negative feedback inhibition (short-term duration) once
the treatment has passed [51]. Rapid return to the basal state
renews foraging behavior via its effect on locomotor activity,
which leads to better foraging capacity and increased dispersal
[5153]. Increased dispersal may be an optimal strategy to escape
stressful small-scale events that persist over long periods of time
[52]. All the factors mentioned above could lead to a return to
routine, that is, faster recovery and preparedness to deal with
subsequent stressors. Quick initiation accompanied by rapid
termination of the HPA axis can directly or indirectly modify
behavior and enhance individual survival. In contrast, in the
absence of a rapid and strong HPA-axis response, rodents with the
PTSD phenotype are at greater risk of predation. However,
because predators represent an emergency that may require
split-second responses for survival, a delayed, slower, or faulty
release of glucocorticoid modulation action may result in costly or
even fatal consequences.
In G-Gerbillus, we found signicant intersexual differences in
FCM concentrations, with females demonstrating signicantly
higher levels than males. This is interesting because, in this
species, we also observed fewer PTSD phenotypes in females than
in males. These ndings imply that higher levels of cortisol could
be protective against the development of PTSD and might explain
the disparity in ndings in rodent and human studies. Female
mice and rats generally demonstrate higher plasma and adrenal
corticosterone levels and more adaptive stress responses [5460],
whereas men (humans) have higher basal cortisol levels
associated with a lower prevalence of stress-related psychopathol-
ogy [61,62]. Therefore, one might hypothesize that higher basal
cortisol levels are protective against the development of PTSD.
As the HPA axis may serve as a key mechanism of development,
modulating growth, and maintenance across a diverse array of
taxa, a prolonged glucocorticoid stress response bears the risk of
high stress-induced physiological and energetic costs (depletion
of energetic stores) [5052,63], which could also impair
reproduction.
In the wild, individual basal body condition, which is an
important factor in determining survival, is also a major
component of physiological stress responses [52]. A factor that
is less considered in laboratory conditions (according to ethics
instructions, individuals who lose weight or show signs of body
neglect or illness are excluded from studies).
Orexin-A levels
Next, we analyzed harvested brains to assess the consequences of
predator stress on orexin-A levels in the hypothalamic nucleus.
Orexin neurons, which are considered multi-taskingneurons,
receive a variety of signals related to environmental, physiological,
and emotional stimuli and project broadly to the entire central
nervous system [6467]. Orexin neurons orchestrate various
aspects of survival via regulation of feeding behavior (energy
homeostasis) and sleep-wakefulness [67]. We found that eight
days after stress exposure, orexin-A levels in the hypothalamus
were downregulated in animals whose behaviors were severely
affected by the stressor (PTSD phenotype) in all three species but
did not change in the PBR and MBR groups in all species.
Moreover, there was a striking negative correlation between the
severity of the anxiety index and changes in the hypothalamus
orexin-A levels in all three species. Pearsons correlation analysis
revealed that the downregulation of orexin-A levels was
signicantly correlated with an increased degree of anxiety-
related behaviors, assessed by EPM, across all samples in each
species.
The reduction in hypothalamic orexin-A levels eight days after
stress exposure in PTSD phenotype rodents may have profound
ecological implications. The orexinergic system initiates, coordi-
nates, and maintains survival behaviors and survival-related
processes (unied orexinergic survival theory) [68]. When the
H. Cohen et al.
7
Molecular Psychiatry
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
orexinergic system is activated in response to stress or alerting
stimuli, orexin neurons can initiate (and maintain) behavioral
stress responses by activating arousal, sensory, somatomotor,
visceromotor, hormonal, and other systems, enabling animals to
better prepare for, respond to, and cope with the acute demands
of physical and emotional threats to re-establish homeostasis [69].
In the wild, animals exhibit reactionary prey behaviors when
orexinergic neurons in the lateral hypothalamus of prey animals
are activated by adequate exteroceptive inputs detailing the
presence of a hungry predator [68]. Accordingly, adequate survival
responses require that animals maximize the functioning of their
sensory systems to critically evaluate their environmental changes,
such as threats or other life-threatening circumstances (i.e., food
deprivation) [68]. Thus, the decreased levels of hypothalamic
orexin-A in the PTSD phenotype eight days after predator stress
across all three species may affect survival behaviors and survival-
related processes, such as reduced alertness to potential threats,
diminished ability to respond proportionally to threats, which
together may increase predation risk.
These ndings are supported by our previous report that the
orexinergic system orchestrates various aspects of survival
behaviors in response to predator stress and is related to the
pathophysiology of PTSD [69]. Our ndings are also supported by
previous studies that investigated orexin expression in patients
with PTSD [70]. It has been demonstrated that cerebrospinal uid
(CSF) and plasma orexin-A levels are signicantly lower in patients
with PTSD than in healthy controls, and CSF orexin-A levels are
strongly and negatively correlated with PTSD severity, as
measured by the Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale, in patients
with PTSD [70].
Morphological changes
The morphological characteristics of DG granule cells were
evaluated using the Golgi-Cox method in all animals. All wild
rodent species whose behavior was extremely disrupted (PTSD
phenotype) selectively displayed signicantly lower total dendritic
length along the DG neurons. The implications of these results are
that rodents with the PTSD phenotype were characterized by
severe atrophy in the DG subregion. Since the dendritic arbor is
responsible for receiving and consolidating neuronal information
input [7173], the reduced dendritic arbor in the DG in PTSD
phenotype rodents can have considerable consequences for the
functional properties of cells and neuronal circuitry, including
decreased synaptic plasticity and synaptic strength and impaired
stabilization of synaptic connectivity, which may in turn lead to
vulnerability to psychopathology.
In contrast, rodents whose behavior was minimally affected or
unaffected (MBR) displayed signicantly longer total dendritic
lengths and longer dendritic branches along the DG neurons. In all
wild rodent species that displayed partial behavioral responses,
the morphological response was intermediate or identical to that
of the MBR group. These ndings were consistent with our
previous ndings in rats [73]. Thus, these results suggest that
predator cue exposure leads to dendritic atrophy of dentate
granule cells, which probably decreases the amount of informa-
tion that neurons can obtain from the environment.
One limitation of this study is the relatively small sample sizes
employed. To establish the long-term, non-lethal effects of
predators on wild rodents, further research incorporating larger
sample sizes and longitudinal follow-up tests is necessary.
In summary, in wild rodent species, the prevalence of PTSD
phenotype was found to be 21.7% to 26.7% of the total
population, similar to that seen in laboratory rodents or
individuals with a history of trauma. In all three rodent species,
individuals who developed the PTSD phenotype to the predator
stimuli were characterized by a blunting of the FCM response, a
lower hypothalamic orexin-A level, and lower total dendritic
length in the DG granule cells eight days after exposure.
Phenotypically, this results in a signicant functional impairment,
potentially impacting reproduction and survival, mediating
indirect effects of predators on prey demographics.
Allen et al. [74] experimentally manipulated fear in free-living
wild songbird populations over three annual breeding seasons by
intermittently broadcasting playbacks of either predator or non-
predator vocalizations and comprehensively quantied the effects
on all components of population growth, together with evidence
of a transgenerational impact on offspring survival as adults. They
found that fear itself signicantly reduced the population growth
rate by causing cumulative, compounding adverse effects on
fecundity and every component of offspring survival, resulting in
predator playback parents producing 53% fewer recruits to the
adult breeding population [74].
CONCLUSIONS
Although PTSD is dened in terms of human responses, several
behavioral, physiological, and neurobiological survival responses
are shared between humans and other mammals (i.e., learning
and remembering about danger and responding to or avoiding
situations that present life-threatening risks), suggesting that just
as the adapted stress/threat response has evolutionary roots, the
post-traumatic stress syndrome has evolutionary roots as well.
Gerbils, wolves, elephants, and marine iguanas may seem
evolutionarily far from humans, but there are compelling scientic
reports to hypothesize that the ecology of fear [2], which may
manifest differently across different taxa, may be largely the same.
Contrary to the hypothesis that the impact of a predator on an
individual prey is either fatal or transitory (i.e., ght or ight), it
seems that some individuals that survive predation stress do not
continue their lives as before, but display long-lasting behavioral
and morphological dysfunction, that resemble human PTSD
responses, which could affect reproduction and survival. The
nding that PTSD symptoms may affect 1 in every 5 wild rodents,
which could negatively impact their tness, has signicant
ramications for study animal behavior and population dynamics.
REFERENCES
1. American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders (5th Edition) - DSM-5. 5 ed ed. Washington, DC,: American Psychiatric
Association (2013).
2. Brown JS, Kotler BP, Bouskila A. Ecology of fear: Foraging games between pre-
dators and prey with pulsed resources. Annales Zoologici Fennici. 2001;38:7187.
3. Houston A, McNamara J, Hutchinson J. General results concerning the trade-off
between gaining energy and avoiding predation. Philos Trans R Soc Lond.
1993;341:37597.
4. Hugie DM. The waiting game: a battle of waitsbetween predator and prey.
Behav Ecol. 2003;14:80717.
5. Katz MW, Abramsky Z, Kotler BP, Rosenzweig ML, Alteshtein O, Vasserman G.
Optimal foraging of little egrets and their prey in a foraging game in a patchy
environment. Am Naturalist. 2013;181:38195.
6. Langerhans RB: Evolutionary consequences of predation: avoidance, escape,
reproduction, and diversication. In: Amt E, editor. Predation in organisms: a
distinct phenomenon. Berlin: Heidelberg: Springer pp (2007) 177220.
7. Sih A. To hide or not to hide? Refuge use in a uctuating environment. Trends
Ecol Evol. 1997;12:3756.
8. Zanette LY, Clinchy M. Ecology of fear. Curr Biol: CB. 2019;29:R309r313.
9. Clinchy M, Schulkin J, Zanette LY, Sheriff MJ, McGowan PO, Boonstra R. The
Neurological Ecology of Fear: Insights Neuroscientists and Ecologists Have to
Offer one Another. Front Behav Neurosci. 2010;4:21.
10. Prugh LR, Sivy KJ, Mahoney PJ, Ganz TR, Ditmer MA, van de Kerk M, et al.
Designing studies of predation risk for improved inference in carnivore-ungulate
systems. Biol Conserv. 2019;232:194207.
11. Lima SL. Predators and the breeding bird: behavioral and reproductive exibility
under the risk of predation. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc. 2009;84:485513.
12. Verdolin JL. Meta-analysis of foraging and predation risk trade-offs in terrestrial
systems. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2006;60:45764.
13. Lima SL, Rattenborg NC, Lesku JA, Amlaner CJ. Sleeping under the risk of pre-
dation. Anim Behav. 2005;70:72336.
H. Cohen et al.
8
Molecular Psychiatry
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
14. Lima SL. Stress and Decision Making under the Risk of Predation: Recent
Developments from Behavioral, Reproductive, and Ecological Perspectives. Adv
Study Behav. 1998;27:21590.
15. Matar MA, Zohar J, Cohen H. Translationally relevant modeling of PTSD in
rodents. Cell Tissue Res. 2013;354:12739.
16. Hammadi S, Chan O, Abdellali M, Medjerab M, Agoun H, Bellahreche Z, et al.
Hyperactivation of the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenocortical axis in streptozotocin-
diabetic gerbils (Gerbillus gerbillus). Int J Exp Pathol. 2018;99:1729.
17. Poulet S: La gerbille un nouveau rongeur de compagnie. Touleuse National
school of veterinary (2004).
18. Mallek A, Movassat J, Ameddah S, Liu J, Semiane N, Khalkhal A, et al. Experimental
diabetes induced by streptozotocin in the desert gerbil, Gerbillus gerbillus, and
the effects of short-term 20-hydroxyecdysone administration. Biomedicine
Pharmacother =Biomedecine Pharmacotherapie. 2018;102:35461.
19. Kondrakiewicz K, Kostecki M, Szadzińska W, Knapska E. Ecological validity of
social interaction tests in rats and mice. Genes, brain, Behav. 2019;18:e12525.
20. Künzl C, Kaiser S, Meier E, Sachser N. Is a wild mammal kept and reared in
captivity still a wild animal? Hormones Behav. 2003;43:18796.
21. Hawlena H, Bashary D, Abramsky Z, Krasnov B. Benets, Costs and Constraints of
AntiParasitic Grooming in Adult and Juvenile Rodents. Ethology. 2007;113:394402.
22. Bourin M. The test retest model of anxiety: An appraisal of ndings to explain
benzodiazepine tolerance. Pharmacol, Biochem, Behav. 2019;178:3941.
23. Eilam D, Dayan T, Ben-Eliyahu S, Schulman II, Shefer G, Hendrie CA. Differential
behavioural and hormonal responses of voles and spiny mice to owl calls. Anim
Behav. 1999;58:108593.
24. Cohen S, Kozlovsky N, Matar MA, Kaplan Z, Zohar J, Cohen H. Post-exposure sleep
deprivation facilitates correctly timed interactions between glucocorticoid and
adrenergic systems, which attenuate traumatic stress responses. Neuropsycho-
pharmacology. 2012;37:2388404.
25. Cohen H, Matar MA, Joseph Z. Animal models of post-traumatic stress disorder.
Curr Protoc Neurosci 2013;Chapter 9:45.
26. Arluk S, Matar MA, Carmi L, Arbel O, Zohar J, Todder D, et al. MDMA treatment
paired with a trauma-cue promotes adaptive stress responses in a translational
model of PTSD in rats. Transl Psych. 2022;12:181.
27. Moss AM, Clutton-Brock TH, Monfort SL. Longitudinal gonadal steroid excretion
in free-living male and female meerkats (Suricata suricatta). Gen Comp Endo-
crinol. 2001;122:15871.
28. Brown JL, Wasser SK, Wildt DE, Graham LH, Monfort SL. Faecal steroid analysis for
monitoring ovarian and testicular function in diverse wild carnivore, primate and
ungulate species Mamm. Biol Suppl II. 1997;62:2731.
29. Touma C, Sachser N, Möstl E, Palme R. Effects of sex and time of day on meta-
bolism and excretion of corticosterone in urine and feces of mice. Gen Comp
Endocrinol. 2003;130:26778.
30. Novakova M, Palme R, Kutalova H, Jansky L, Frynta D. The effects of sex, age and
commensal way of life on levels of fecal glucocorticoid metabolites in spiny mice
(Acomys cahirinus). Physiol Behav. 2008;95:18793.
31. Teskey-Gerstl A, Bamberg E, Steineck T, Palme R. Excretion of corticosteroids in
urine and faeces of hares (Lepus europaeus). J Comp Physiol B, Biochem, Sys-
temic, Environ Physiol. 2000;170:1638.
32. Harper JM, Austad SN. Fecal glucocorticoids: a noninvasive method of measuring
adrenal activity in wild and captive rodents. Physiol Biochem Zool : PBZ.
2000;73:1222.
33. Navarro-Castilla Á, Garrido M, Hawlena H, Barja I. Non-Invasive Monitoring of
Adrenocortical Activity in Three Sympatric Desert Gerbil Species. Anim : Open
Access J MDPI. 2021;11:75.
34. Gutman R, Dayan T, Levy O, Schubert I, Kronfeld-Schor N. The effect of the lunar
cycle on fecal cortisol metabolite levels and foraging ecology of nocturnally and
diurnally active spiny mice. PloS one. 2011;6:e23446.
35. Cohen H, Zohar J, Matar M. The relevance of differential response to trauma in an
animal model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psych. 2003;53:46373.
36. Cohen H, Zohar J, Matar MA, Kaplan Z, Geva AB. Unsupervised fuzzy clustering
analysis supports behavioral cutoff criteria in an animal model of posttraumatic
stress disorder. Biol Psych. 2005;58:64050.
37. Cohen H, Zohar J, Matar MA, Zeev K, Loewenthal U, Richter-Levin G. Setting apart
the affected: the use of behavioral criteria in animal models of post traumatic
stress disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29:196270.
38. Cohen H, Geva AB, Matar MA, Zohar J, Kaplan Z. Post-traumatic stress behavioural
responses in inbred mouse strains: can genetic predisposition explain phenotypic
vulnerability? Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2008;11:33149.
39. Zanette LY, Hobbs EC, Witterick LE, MacDougall-Shackleton SA, Clinchy M.
Predator-induced fear causes PTSD-like changes in the brains and behaviour of
wild animals. Sci Rep. 2019;9:11474.
40. Bradshaw GA, Capaldo T, Lindner L, Grow G. Building an inner sanctuary: complex
PTSD in chimpanzees. J trauma Dissociation : Off J Int Soc Study Dissociation
(ISSD). 2008;9:934.
41. Bradshaw GA, Schore AN, Brown JL, Poole JH, Moss CJ. Elephant breakdown.
Nature. 2005;433:807.
42. Mallonée JS, Joslin P. Traumatic stress disorder observed in an adult wild captive
wolf (Canis lupus). J Appl Anim Welf Sci : JAAWS. 2004;7:10726.
43. Yehuda R. Post-traumatic stress disorder. N Engl J Med. 2002;346:10814.
44. Jeong KH, Jacobson L, Pacak K, Widmaier EP, Goldstein DS, Majzoub JA. Impaired
basal and restraint-induced epinephrine secretion in corticotropin-releasing
hormone-decient mice. Endocrinology. 2000;141:114250.
45. Pacak K, Palkovits M, Kopin IJ, Goldstein DS. Stress-induced norepinephrine
release in the hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus and pituitary-adrenocortical
and sympathoadrenal activity: in vivo microdialysis studies. Front Neuroendo-
crinol. 1995;16:89150.
46. Cahill L, Prins B, Weber M, McGaugh JL. Beta-adrenergic activation and memory
for emotional events. Nature. 1994;371:7024.
47. Romero LM, Wikelski M. Corticosterone levels predict survival probabilities of
Galapagos marine iguanas during El Nino events. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA.
2001;98:736670.
48. Sargunaraj F, Kotler BP, Juliana JRS, Wielebnowski NCJEER. Stress as an adapta-
tion II: Does experimental cortisol supplementation affect predation risk assess-
ment in foraging gerbils? Evolut Ecol Res. 2017;18:58798.
49. Thaker M, Lima SL, Hews DK. Acute corticosterone elevation enhances anti-
predator behaviors in male tree lizard morphs. Hormones Behav. 2009;56:5157.
50. Homberger B, Jenni-Eiermann S, Jenni L. Distinct responses of baseline and
stress-induced corticosterone levels to genetic and environmental factors. Gen
Comp Endocrinol. 2015;210:4654.
51. Romero LM. Physiological stress in ecology: lessons from biomedical research.
Trends Ecol Evol. 2004;19:24955.
52. Cote J, Clobert J, Meylan S, Fitze PS. Experimental enhancement of corticosterone
levels positively affects subsequent male survival. Hormones Behav. 2006;49:3207.
53. Lin H, Decuypere E, Buyse J. Oxidative stress induced by corticosterone admin-
istration in broiler chickens (Gallus gallus domesticus) 1. Chronic exposure. Comp
Biochem Physiol Part B, Biochem Mol Biol. 2004;139:73744.
54. Iwasaki-Sekino A, Mano-Otagiri A, Ohata H, Yamauchi N, Shibasaki T. Gender
differences in corticotropin and corticosterone secretion and corticotropin-
releasing factor mRNA expression in the paraventricular nucleus of the hypo-
thalamus and the central nucleus of the amygdala in response to footshock
stress or psychological stress in rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34:22637.
55. Handa RJ, Burgess LH, Kerr JE, OKeefe JA. Gonadal steroid hormone receptors
and sex differences in the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis. Hormones Behav.
1994;28:46476.
56. Viau V, Bingham B, Davis J, Lee P, Wong M. Gender and puberty interact on the
stress-induced activation of parvocellular neurosecretory neurons and
corticotropin-releasing hormone messenger ribonucleic acid expression in the
rat. Endocrinology. 2005;146:13746.
57. Luine V. Sex differences in chronic stress effects on memory in rats. Stress (Amst,
Neth). 2002;5:20516.
58. Aisa B, Tordera R, Lasheras B, Del Río J, Ramírez MJ. Effects of maternal separation
on hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal responses, cognition and vulnerability to
stress in adult female rats. Neuroscience. 2008;154:121826.
59. Cohen H, Yehuda R. Gender differences in animal models of posttraumatic stress
disorder. Dis Markers. 2011;30:14150.
60. Atkinson HC, Waddell BJ. Circadian variation in basal plasma corticosterone and
adrenocorticotropin in the rat: sexual dimorphism and changes across the
estrous cycle. Endocrinology. 1997;138:38428.
61. Takai N, Yamaguchi M, Aragaki T, Eto K, Uchihashi K, Nishikawa Y. Gender-specic
differences in salivary biomarker responses to acute psychological stress. Ann N.
Y Acad Sci. 2007;1098:5105.
62. Vierhapper H, Nowotny P, Waldhäusl W. Sex-specic differences in cortisol pro-
duction rates in humans. Metab: Clin Exp. 1998;47:9746.
63. Sapolsky RM, Romero LM, Munck AU. How do glucocorticoids inuence stress
responses? Integrating permissive, suppressive, stimulatory, and preparative
actions. Endocr Rev. 2000;21:5589.
64. ChiefS, Carotenuto M, Monda V, Valenzano A, Villano I, Precenzano F, et al.
Orexin System: The Key for a Healthy Life. Front Physiol. 2017;8:357.
65. Soya S, Sakurai T. Evolution of Orexin Neuropeptide System: Structure and
Function. Front Neurosci. 2020;14:691.
66. Sakurai T. The role of orexin in motivated behaviours. Nat Rev Neurosci.
2014;15:71931.
67. Sakurai T. The neural circuit of orexin (hypocretin): maintaining sleep and
wakefulness. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007;8:17181.
68. Chase MH. A unied survival theory of the functioning of the hypocretinergic
system. J Appl Physiol (Bethesda, Md: 1985). 2013;115:95471.
69. Cohen S, Matar MA, Vainer E, Zohar J, Kaplan Z, Cohen H. Signicance of the
orexinergic system in modulating stress-related responses in an animal model of
post-traumatic stress disorder. Transl Psych. 2020;10:10.
H. Cohen et al.
9
Molecular Psychiatry
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
70. Strawn JR, Pyne-Geithman GJ, Ekhator NN, Horn PS, Uhde TW, Shutter LA, et al. Low
cerebrospinal uid and plasma orexin-A (hypocretin-1) concentrations in combat-
related posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2010;35:10017.
71. Sorra KE, Harris KM. Overview on the structure, composition, function, develop-
ment, and plasticity of hippocampal dendritic spines. Hippocampus.
2000;10:50111.
72. Vessey JP, Karra D. More than just synaptic building blocks: scaffolding proteins
of the post-synaptic density regulate dendritic patterning. J Neurochem.
2007;102:32432.
73. Cohen H, Kozlovsky N, Matar MA, Zohar J, Kaplan Z. Distinctive hippocampal and
amygdalar cytoarchitectural changes underlie specic patterns of behavioral
disruption following stress exposure in an animal model of PTSD. Eur Neu-
ropsychopharmacol : J Eur Coll Neuropsychopharmacol. 2014;24:192544.
74. Allen MC, Clinchy M, Zanette LY. Fear of predators in free-living wildlife reduces
population growth over generations. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2022;119:e2112404119.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
CH, ZJ, AZ contributed to conception and design of the study. CH, MM, CC organized
the database. CH, MM, CC conducted the experiments. CH performed the statistical
analysis. CH and MM wrote the rst draft of the manuscript.CH, MM, TD, CC, ZJ, HH and
AZ contributed to manuscript revision and read and approved the submitted version.
COMPETING INTERESTS
The authors declare no competing interests.
ADDITIONAL INFORMATION
Supplementary information The online version contains supplementary material
available at https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-023-02240-7.
Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to Hagit Cohen.
Reprints and permission information is available at http://www.nature.com/
reprints
Publishers note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims
in published maps and institutional afliations.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons
Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing,
adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give
appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative
Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party
material in this article are included in the articles Creative Commons licence, unless
indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the
articles Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutor y
regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly
from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
© The Author(s) 2023
H. Cohen et al.
10
Molecular Psychiatry
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
Terms and Conditions
Springer Nature journal content, brought to you courtesy of Springer Nature Customer Service Center GmbH (“Springer Nature”).
Springer Nature supports a reasonable amount of sharing of research papers by authors, subscribers and authorised users (“Users”), for small-
scale personal, non-commercial use provided that all copyright, trade and service marks and other proprietary notices are maintained. By
accessing, sharing, receiving or otherwise using the Springer Nature journal content you agree to these terms of use (“Terms”). For these
purposes, Springer Nature considers academic use (by researchers and students) to be non-commercial.
These Terms are supplementary and will apply in addition to any applicable website terms and conditions, a relevant site licence or a personal
subscription. These Terms will prevail over any conflict or ambiguity with regards to the relevant terms, a site licence or a personal subscription
(to the extent of the conflict or ambiguity only). For Creative Commons-licensed articles, the terms of the Creative Commons license used will
apply.
We collect and use personal data to provide access to the Springer Nature journal content. We may also use these personal data internally within
ResearchGate and Springer Nature and as agreed share it, in an anonymised way, for purposes of tracking, analysis and reporting. We will not
otherwise disclose your personal data outside the ResearchGate or the Springer Nature group of companies unless we have your permission as
detailed in the Privacy Policy.
While Users may use the Springer Nature journal content for small scale, personal non-commercial use, it is important to note that Users may
not:
use such content for the purpose of providing other users with access on a regular or large scale basis or as a means to circumvent access
control;
use such content where to do so would be considered a criminal or statutory offence in any jurisdiction, or gives rise to civil liability, or is
otherwise unlawful;
falsely or misleadingly imply or suggest endorsement, approval , sponsorship, or association unless explicitly agreed to by Springer Nature in
writing;
use bots or other automated methods to access the content or redirect messages
override any security feature or exclusionary protocol; or
share the content in order to create substitute for Springer Nature products or services or a systematic database of Springer Nature journal
content.
In line with the restriction against commercial use, Springer Nature does not permit the creation of a product or service that creates revenue,
royalties, rent or income from our content or its inclusion as part of a paid for service or for other commercial gain. Springer Nature journal
content cannot be used for inter-library loans and librarians may not upload Springer Nature journal content on a large scale into their, or any
other, institutional repository.
These terms of use are reviewed regularly and may be amended at any time. Springer Nature is not obligated to publish any information or
content on this website and may remove it or features or functionality at our sole discretion, at any time with or without notice. Springer Nature
may revoke this licence to you at any time and remove access to any copies of the Springer Nature journal content which have been saved.
To the fullest extent permitted by law, Springer Nature makes no warranties, representations or guarantees to Users, either express or implied
with respect to the Springer nature journal content and all parties disclaim and waive any implied warranties or warranties imposed by law,
including merchantability or fitness for any particular purpose.
Please note that these rights do not automatically extend to content, data or other material published by Springer Nature that may be licensed
from third parties.
If you would like to use or distribute our Springer Nature journal content to a wider audience or on a regular basis or in any other manner not
expressly permitted by these Terms, please contact Springer Nature at
onlineservice@springernature.com
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any citations for this publication.
Article
Full-text available
MDMA (3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine), a synthetic ring-substituted amphetamine, combined with psychotherapy has demonstrated efficacy for the treatment of chronic posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) patients. This controlled prospective study aimed to assess the bio-behavioral underpinnings of MDMA in a translational model of PTSD. Rats exposed to predator-scent stress (PSS) were subjected to a trauma-cue at day 7 shortly after single-dose MDMA injection (5 mg/kg). The elevated plus maze and acoustic startle response tests were assessed on day 14 and served for classification into behavioral response groups. Freezing response to a further trauma-reminder was assessed on Day 15. The morphological characteristics of the dentate gyrus (DG) and basolateral amygdala (BLA) were subsequently examined. Hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis and 5-hydroxytryptamine involvement were evaluated using: (1) corticosterone measurements at 2 h and 4 h after MDMA treatment, (2) Lewis strain rats with blunted HPA-response and (3) pharmacological receptor-blockade. MDMA treatment was effective in attenuating stress behavioral responses only when paired with memory reactivation by a trauma-cue. The effects of the treatment on behavior were associated with a commensurate normalization of the dendritic cytoarchitecture of DG and BLA neurons. Pretreatment with RU486, Ketanserin, or Pindolol prevented the above improvement in anxiety-like behavioral responses. MDMA treatment paired with memory reactivation reduced the prevalence rate of PTSD-phenotype 14 days later and normalized the cytoarchitecture changes induced by PSS (in dendritic complexities) compared to saline control. MDMA treatment paired with a trauma-cue may modify or update the original traumatic memory trace through reconsolidation processes. These anxiolytic-like effects seem to involve the HPA axis and 5-HT systems.
Article
Full-text available
Significance Accurately evaluating the total impact of predators on prey population growth rates is fundamental to forecasting the consequences of predator conservation and management. That the fear (antipredator responses) predators inspire could contribute to this total impact has only relatively recently been recognized. We experimentally demonstrate that fear itself can impact prey population growth rates in free-living wildlife, extending to transgenerational impacts reducing population growth beyond the parental generation. We report how fear may contribute considerably to the total impact of predators and why this may be the norm in birds and mammals. The critical significance of our work lies in experimentally establishing that inferring the effects of predators using data on direct killing alone risks dramatically underestimating their total impact.
Article
Full-text available
The study of the endocrine status can be useful to understand wildlife responses to the changing environment. Here, we validated an enzyme immunoassay (EIA) to non-invasively monitor adrenocortical activity by measuring fecal corticosterone metabolites (FCM) in three sympatric gerbil species (Gerbillus andersoni, G. gerbillus and G. pyramidum) from the Northwestern Negev Desert’s sands (Israel). Animals included into treatment groups were injected with adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) to stimulate adrenocortical activity, while control groups received a saline solution. Feces were collected at different intervals and FCM were quantified by an EIA. Basal FCM levels were similar in the three species. The ACTH effect was evidenced, but the time of FCM peak concentrations appearance differed between the species (6–24 h post-injection). Furthermore, FCM peak values were observed sooner in G. andersoni females than in males (6 h and 18 h post-injection, respectively). G. andersoni and G. gerbillus males in control groups also increased FCM levels (18 h and 48 h postinjection, respectively). Despite the small sample sizes, our results confirmed the EIA suitability for analyzing FCM in these species as a reliable indicator of the adrenocortical activity. This study also revealed that close species, and individuals within a species, can respond differently to the same stressor.
Article
Full-text available
Orexins are hypothalamic neuropeptides that were initially identified in the rat brain as endogenous ligands for an (previously) orphan G-protein-coupled receptor (GPCR). They are multitasking peptides involved in many physiological functions, including regulation of feeding behavior, wakefulness and autonomic/neuroendocrine functions, and sleep/wakefulness states in mammals. There are two isopeptides of orexin, orexin A and orexin B, which are produced from a common precursor peptide, prepro-orexin. Structures of orexins, as well as orexin genes, are highly conserved throughout mammalian species, suggesting strong evolutionary pressure that maintains the structures. Their lengths and structure suggested that orexin B is the ancestral form of the orexin neuropeptide. In mammals, orexins bind to two subtypes of GPCRs, i.e., orexin 1 receptor (OX1R) and orexin 2 receptor (OX2R). Phylogenetically, the orexin system is present exclusively in vertebrates. In genomes of species outside mammals, there is only one orexin receptor, which is similar to OX2R, suggesting that OX2R is the prototype receptor for orexins. OX1R is likely to have evolved during early mammalian evolution. Orexin-producing neurons (orexin neurons) are mainly located in the lateral hypothalamic area (LHA) in mammals and are also found in hypothalamic regions in many other vertebrates. Orexins are likely to be closely related to the regulation of active, motivated behavior in many species. The orexin system seems to have evolved as a system that supports active and purposeful behavior which is closely related with wakefulness.
Article
Full-text available
Converging evidence indicates that orexins (ORXs), the regulatory neuropeptides, are implicated in anxiety- and depression-related behaviors via the modulation of neuroendocrine, serotonergic, and noradrenergic systems. This study evaluated the role of the orexinergic system in stress-associated physiological responses in a controlled prospective animal model. The pattern and time course of activation of hypothalamic ORX neurons in response to predator-scent stress (PSS) were examined using c-Fos as a marker for neuronal activity. The relationship between the behavioral response pattern 7 days post-exposure and expressions of ORXs was evaluated. We also investigated the effects of intracerebroventricular microinfusion of ORX-A or almorexant (ORX-A/B receptor antagonist) on behavioral responses 7 days following PSS exposure. Hypothalamic levels of ORX-A, neuropeptide Y (NPY), and brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) were assessed. Compared with rats whose behaviors were extremely disrupted (post-traumatic stress disorder [PTSD]-phenotype), those whose behaviors were minimally selectively disrupted displayed significantly upregulated ORX-A and ORX-B levels in the hypothalamic nuclei. Intracerebroventricular microinfusion of ORX-A before PSS reduced the prevalence of the PTSD phenotype compared with that of artificial cerebrospinal fluid or almorexant, and rats treated with almorexant displayed a higher prevalence of the PTSD phenotype than did untreated rats. Activated ORX neurons led to upregulated expressions of BDNF and NPY, which might provide an additional regulatory mechanism for the modulation of adaptive stress responses. The study indicates that the activated ORX system might promote adaptive responses to PSS probably via stimulation of BDNF and NPY secretion, and early intervention with ORX-A reduces the prevalence of the PTSD phenotype and increases the prevalence of adaptive phenotypes. The findings provide some insights into the mechanisms underlying the involvement of the ORX system in stress-related disorders.
Article
Full-text available
Predator-induced fear is both, one of the most common stressors employed in animal model studies of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), and a major focus of research in ecology. There has been a growing discourse between these disciplines but no direct empirical linkage. We endeavoured to provide this empirical linkage by conducting experiments drawing upon the strengths of both disciplines. Exposure to a natural cue of predator danger (predator vocalizations), had enduring effects of at least 7 days duration involving both, a heightened sensitivity to predator danger (indicative of an enduring memory of fear), and elevated neuronal activation in both the amygdala and hippocampus – in wild birds (black-capped chickadees, Poecile atricapillus), exposed to natural environmental and social experiences in the 7 days following predator exposure. Our results demonstrate enduring effects on the brain and behaviour, meeting the criteria to be considered an animal model of PTSD – in a wild animal, which are of a nature and degree which can be anticipated could affect fecundity and survival in free-living wildlife. We suggest our findings support both the proposition that PTSD is not unnatural, and that long-lasting effects of predator-induced fear, with likely effects on fecundity and survival, are the norm in nature.
Article
Full-text available
Quantifying both the lethal and non-lethal (or "risk") effects of predation has emerged as a major research focus in carnivore-ungulate systems. While numerous studies have examined predation risk and risk effects in recent decades, a lack of standardization in approaches has impeded progress in the field. We provide an overview of five major study design considerations involved in assessing predation risk and responses of prey in carnivore-ungulate systems, highlighting how different design choices can impact the strength and scope of inference. First, we stress the importance of distinguishing measures of predation risk (probability of being killed) from measures of risk effects (costs of antipredator behaviors in response to risk). Second, we recommend explicit consideration of spatial and temporal scales using a standardized framework to facilitate cross-study comparisons. Third, ungulates use visual, auditory, and olfactory sensory pathways to evaluate predation risk. Experiments that manipulate signals of risk (e.g., auditory playbacks or application of predator scent) can be powerful approaches, but the dosages and types of cues need to be carefully considered. Fourth, ungulates usually face threats from multiple predators simultaneously, and we highlight the potential for remote cameras and structural equation modeling to help address this challenge. Fifth, emerging technologies may substantially improve our ability to assess risk. We discuss several promising technologies, such as animal-borne video, un-manned aerial vehicles, and physiological sensors. We conclude with general recommendations for study design, which may improve the utility of predation risk research for the conservation and management of carnivore-ungulate systems.
Article
Full-text available
Quantifying both the lethal and non-lethal (or “risk”) effects of predation has emerged as a major research focus in carnivore-ungulate systems. While numerous studies have examined predation risk and risk effects in recent decades, a lack of standardization in approaches has impeded progress in the field. We provide an overview of five major study design considerations involved in assessing predation risk and responses of prey in carnivore-ungulate systems, highlighting how different design choices can impact the strength and scope of inference. First, we stress the importance of distinguishing measures of predation risk (probability of being killed) from measures of risk effects (costs of antipredator behaviors in response to risk). Second, we recommend explicit consideration of spatial and temporal scales using a standardized framework to facilitate cross-study comparisons. Third, ungulates use visual, auditory, and olfactory sensory pathways to evaluate predation risk. Experiments that manipulate signals of risk (e.g., auditory playbacks or application of predator scent) can be powerful approaches, but the dosages and types of cues need to be carefully considered. Fourth, ungulates usually face threats from multiple predators simultaneously, and we highlight the potential for remote cameras and structural equation modeling to help address this challenge. Fifth, emerging technologies may substantially improve our ability to assess risk. We discuss several promising technologies, such as animal-borne video, unmanned aerial vehicles, and physiological sensors. We conclude with general recommendations for study design, which may improve the utility of predation risk research for the conservation and management of carnivore-ungulate systems.
Article
Full-text available
Different rat and mouse models are used in studies of social interactions. Simple behavioral measures, which are commonly used in the laboratory, allow to perform relatively short experiments and use multiple brain manipulation techniques. However, too much focus on the simplest behavioral models generates a serious risk of reducing ecological validity or even studying phenomena which would never happen outside of the laboratory. In this review, we discuss the suitability of mice and rats as model organisms for studying social behaviors, with focus on social transmission of fear paradigms. First, we briefly introduce the concept of domestication and what impact it had on laboratory rodents. Then we present two aspects of social behaviors, sociability and dominance, which are crucial for social organization in these species. Finally, we present experimental models used for studying how animals transmit information about danger between each other, and how these models may reflect what happens in the natural environment. We discuss the difficulties that arise from our limited knowledge of rat and mouse ecology, especially their social life. We also explore the subject of balancing ecological validity and controllability in rodent models of social behaviors, the latter being particularly important for studying brain activity. Though it is very challenging, an efficient program for social neuroscience research should, in our opinion, aim at bridging the gap between laboratory and field studies.
Article
The 'ecology of fear' refers to the total impact of predators on prey populations and communities. The traditional view in ecology is that predators directly kill prey, thereby reducing prey survival and prey numbers - and that this is the limit of their ecological role. The ecology of fear posits that the behavioural, physiological and neurobiological costs of avoiding predation ('fear' for short) may additionally reduce prey fecundity and survival, and the total reduction in prey numbers resulting from exposure to predators may thus far exceed that due to direct killing alone. If this is the case, then failing to consider fear as a factor risks profoundly underestimating the ecological role predators play.