ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

Advertisers often use specifically chosen wording to convey the effectiveness of their product and we investigated memory accuracy for the scientific claims put forth by product advertisements. Participants were shown a cognitive enhancement product advertisement and were tested on their memory for various details. Critically, we were interested in participants' memory for a phrase describing the product as either “clinically proven” (indicating the product is effective) or “clinically studied” (which is ambiguous). Generally, both younger and older adults demonstrated poor memory for this detail and were more likely to remember the product as having been “proven” to be effective than to have been “studied”. Thus, we demonstrate the fallibility of memory and the potential for reliance on schematic knowledge in the absence of a veridical record of one's memory for the advertisement. We suggest that ambiguous efficacy claims be carefully considered by consumers so as not to be misled.
RESEARCH ARTICLE
Clinically studied or clinically proven? Memory for claims in
print advertisements
Dillon H. Murphy
1
| Shawn T. Schwartz
1
| Kylie O. Alberts
1
|
Alexander L. M. Siegel
1
| Brandon J. Carone
1
| Alan D. Castel
1
| Aimee Drolet
2
1
Department of Psychology, University of
California, Los Angeles, California, USA
2
Anderson School of Management, University
of California, Los Angeles, California, USA
Correspondence
Alan D. Castel, Department of Psychology,
University of California, Los Angeles, Los
Angeles, CA 90095, USA.
Email: castel@ucla.edu
Funding information
National Institute on Aging, Grant/Award
Number: R01 AG044335
Abstract
Advertisers often use specifically chosen wording to convey the effectiveness of their
product and we investigated memory accuracy for the scientific claims put forth by
product advertisements. Participants were shown a cognitive enhancement product
advertisement and were tested on their memory for various details. Critically, we were
interested in participants' memory for a phrase describing the product as either clini-
cally proven(indicating the product is effective) or clinically studied(which is ambig-
uous). Generally, both younger and older adults demonstrated poor memory for this
detail and were more likely to remember the product as having been provento be
effective than to have been studied. Thus, we demonstrate the fallibility of memory
and the potential for reliance on schematic knowledge in the absence of a veridical
record of one's memory for the advertisement. We suggest that ambiguous efficacy
claims be carefully considered by consumers so as not to be misled.
KEYWORDS
advertising, aging, false memory, metacognition, schematic knowledge
1|GENERAL AUDIENCE SUMMARY
Advertisers, including those touting enhancement supplements, often
use specifically chosen wording to convey the effectiveness of their
product (in some cases, regardless of the product's actual efficacy),
while maintaining conformity with legal standards for example, Food
and Drug Administration (FDA) regulations. In the current study, we
tested how people may remember or misremember a claim regarding a
product's efficacy presented in an advertisement for a cognitive
enhancement supplement. After viewing the advertisement for 1 min,
participants were asked to remember various features such as the name
of the brand, the target age group, and whether the product was FDA-
approved. Critically, we were interested in participants' memory for a
phrase describing the product's efficacy as either clinically studiedor
clinically proven. Results revealed that both younger and older adults
demonstrated poor memory for this detail and were more likely to
remember the product as having been provento be effective than to
have been studied, indicating that people's memory for claims made
in product advertisements may not always be accurate. We suggest that
ambiguous claims of efficacy such as being scientifically studiedor
clinically studiedbe carefully considered by consumers so as not to
be misled regarding the product's efficacy.
2|INTRODUCTION
Many people, from college students to older adults, are interested in
ways to enhance memory and cognitive functioning. Over-
the-counter dietary supplements have become a popular method of
cognitive enhancement (e.g., MemorAll, RediMind, CogniAid),
Received: 7 November 2020 Revised: 23 January 2023 Accepted: 23 June 2023
DOI: 10.1002/acp.4106
This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivs License, which permits use and distribution in any
medium, provided the original work is properly cited, the use is non-commercial and no modifications or adaptations are made.
© 2023 The Authors. Applied Cognitive Psychology published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
Appl Cognit Psychol. 2023;19. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/acp 1
although there is insufficient evidence that these products improve
cognitive functioning in healthy adults (Butler et al., 2017; Gestuvo &
Hung, 2012). However, advertisements for dietary supplements often
include statements indicating that the product has been empirically
studied or validated (e.g., clinically studied,clinically tested,sci-
entifically proven), signaling to consumers that the product has some
degree of effectiveness.
Although many companies use phrases like clinically studiedin
their advertisements to convey a product's efficacy, this phrase is
ambiguous. Stating that something has been clinically studied only
indicates that a product's effects have been studied in a laboratory; it
does not necessarily mean that the product was effective in doing
whatever was claimed in the advertisement. For instance, the results
could have provided null evidence for the product or even shown that
the product has the opposite of the desired effect, but the product
was still clinically studied. Advertisers are likely aware that millions
of people may misunderstand or not pay attention to ambiguous
claims like clinically studiedor could even later misremember a
product labeled as clinically studiedas having been clinically
proven. Thus, strategically placing ambiguous phrases implying some
degree of efficacy may be a way advertisers take advantage of the
fallibility of memory to establish the credibility of their product.
Misremembering certain claims made by product advertisements
may reflect people's reliance on schematic knowledge when they can-
not successfully retrieve exact perceptual or conceptual information
(e.g., Alba & Hasher, 1983). Using schemascognitive heuristics based
on prior experience that predict what information is likely to be part
of a given eventgenerally aids the encoding and retrieval of memo-
ries. However, much like other cognitive heuristics, the usage of
schemas can also lead to memory errors. Such errors may be most
prevalent for atypical events and information (i.e., information that is
inconsistent with a schema), causing individuals to remember an event
or certain pieces of information based on expectation rather than how
the event was actually experienced (Alba & Hasher, 1983; Brewer &
Treyens, 1981; Castel, 2005; Lew & Howe, 2016; Miller &
Gazzaniga, 1998). For example, schemas can be beneficial in reducing
memory deficits when newly learned information is consistent with
schematic knowledge (e.g., Castel, 2005) but can produce false memo-
ries when information is inconsistent with one's past experience and
schemas (e.g., Brewer & Treyens, 1981; see also Murphy &
Castel, 2021). In a marketing context, advertisers may be aware of
people's tendency to rely on schemas and strategically pursue adver-
tising options that capitalize on such tendencies to portray their prod-
uct in a more favorable light.
Prior work has revealed that marketing communications can generate
expectations that impact the way purchasers remember their experience
with a product (Hoch & Deighton, 1989;LaTour&LaTour,2012). Addi-
tionally, there is evidence that people generate gist-based false memories
in the context of advertising due to the reliance on schemas (Braun-
LaTour & LaTour, 2004;LaTouretal.,2014). In one case, people thought
that they remembered an advertisement for a famous brand name when it
was actually a lesser-known brand or even a brand that was entirely ficti-
tious (Braun-LaTour & LaTour, 2004; Holden & Vanhuele, 1999).
Furthermore, while people may initially notice, accurately recall, and/or rec-
ognize some aspects of visually presented advertisements, the specific
details are often rapidly forgotten (Bagozzi & Silk, 1983). Thus, people may
be prone to misremembering advertisements and their efficacy.
Memory for features of a product depicted in an advertisement
may also be susceptible to misinformation. For example, Braun and
Loftus (1998) demonstrated that recollection of a chocolate bar's
packaging color was influenced by misleading information contained
in the advertisement. Similarly, Sherman et al. (2015) reported that
presenting participants with similar television advertisements created
false memories for competitor brands. Other research has shown that
the visual/pictorial component of a print advertisement is encoded
more elaboratively and distinctively than the verbal component and the
verbal aspects may be more prone to false memories (Childers, 1986).
Furthermore, one's goals, age, and interest when studying an advertise-
ment can also bias memory (e.g., Fung & Carstensen, 2003; Martín-
Luengo et al., 2015). Other individual differences, such as one's level of
commitment to a particular brand, have also been shown to influence
memory, with committed brand consumers more likely to form false
memories about a product experience than less-committed consumers
(Montgomery & Rajagopal, 2018). As a result, consumers may misre-
member an experience with a product that was similar to what the
advertisement suggested rather than recalling their own experience
(Braun, 1999; Cowley & Mitchell, 2003). Taken together, these findings
suggest that several traits and environmental factors can influence
memory accuracy for advertisements.
Evidence indicating that advertisements can lead to the expression
of false memories fits with the general notion that memory retrieval is a
reconstructive process (Hasher & Griffin, 1978). Specifically, when a
memory is retrieved, it is a reconstruction that blends elements of what
truly happened with what we believe happened. These reconstructive
processes may be further distorted by other intervening cognitive func-
tions such as prior retrieval attempts, distraction, imagination, and social
influences (Roediger & McDermott, 2000; Schacter, 1999; see also
Gallo, 2010). Thus, through reconstructive memory processes, familiar-
ity, and the spreading of semantic activation (Collins & Loftus, 1975),
advertisement information likely influences consumers' memory. We
sought to determine if peoplespecifically younger and older adults
are prone to misremembering certain scientific terms, claims, and
research conclusions when presented with an advertisement that uses
vague wording to support the product's efficacy.
3|THE CURRENT STUDY
In the current study, we examined how younger and older adults
(those over age 65 who may be especially interested in consuming
memory-enhancing supplements) would remember or misremember
different types of information depicted in a cognitive enhancement
product advertisement. We also investigated how the information
that was remembered would affect perceptions of the product. Partic-
ipants viewed a product advertisement and then completed a ques-
tionnaire containing a series of multiple-choice questions regarding
2MURPHY ET AL.
10990720, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/acp.4106, Wiley Online Library on [15/07/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
the advertisement. Questions covered aspects of the advertisement
like the name of the brand, the presence or absence of a medical doc-
tor in the advertisement, and how much they expected the product to
cost. The primary question of interest tested whether participants
could correctly identify whether the phrase clinically studiedor
clinically provenwas included within the advertisement.
Consistent with the idea that memory for product features can
be biased by advertisements (e.g., Braun & Loftus, 1998), we pre-
dicted that participants would often misremember the phrase clini-
cally studiedas clinically proven, indicating that consumers may be
prone to misremembering products as being more effective than they
were advertised. Previous work has established that the commonality of
words in the English language can impact how consumer-related informa-
tion is processed and subsequently remembered in both an explicit and
implicit manner (Krishnan & Shapiro, 1996; Shapiro & Krishnan, 2001),
and this may be more pronounced in older age. As such, in addition to
younger adults, we were also interested in investigating memory for
product claims in older adults who may be both consumers of these
types of products (i.e., cognitive enhancement supplements) and more
prone to reconstructive memory biases like relying on gist-based memory
(generally remembering the main message but misremembering details)
and schematic knowledge (Castel, 2005; Flores et al., 2017; Gallo
et al., 2019;Schacteretal.,1997;Umanath,2016;Umanath&
Marsh, 2014).
Older adults are particularly prone to falsely remember verbal infor-
mation and rely on gist-based memory (Devitt & Schacter, 2016). For
example, fuzzy-trace theory suggests that older adults have memory
impairments for verbatim details (the exact content and details associ-
ated with a memory) but can access and retrieve more gist-based
information in a variety of circumstances (Brainerd & Reyna, 2004;
Reyna, 2012; Reyna et al., 2016). Moreover, prior knowledge and sche-
matic support can enhance memory as older adults often use schemas
that may lead to accurate memory for schema-consistent information
(Castel, 2005;Floresetal.,2017; Gallo et al., 2019;Hess,2005). How-
ever, schemas can also be misleading, causing inaccuracies that may be
consistent with prior knowledge when to-be-remembered information
is schema-inconsistent (Umanath, 2016).
There is also evidence suggesting that older adults' gist-based
memory for health-related information can be differentially influenced
by certain factors relative to their verbatim memory (Friedman
et al., 2015; Hargis & Castel, 2018;Morrowetal.,2019). Specifically,
older adults may hold specific schemas for health-related information
that can aid or hinder their memory (Heller et al., 2017;Morrow
et al., 1991; Rice & Okun, 1994). Older adults may also be more likely
to form false memories due to their reduced ability to bind information
to its source (e.g., the context in which the information was learned;
Lyle et al., 2006; Mitchell et al., 2000). Given this reliance on gist-based
memory and propensity to falsely remember information, older adults
may be particularly prone to misremembering the precise terms used to
describe the claims regarding a product, consistent with gist-based
memory reliance in older age (e.g., Gallo et al., 2019). This could provide
important insight regarding how aging influences what information is
retained when presented with a product's claims in an applied context.
3.1 |Method
3.1.1 | Participants
An a priori power analysis was conducted using G*Power (Faul
et al., 2007). For a 2 (age: old, young)3 (label: clinically studied, clini-
cally proven, control [i.e., none]) between-subjects ANOVA, assuming
alpha =.05 and power =.95, 297 participants would be needed to
reliably detect a medium effect size (η
p2
=.05). A total sample of
316 younger (n=150; M
age
=24.79, SD
age
=3.60) and older adults
(n=166; M
age
=72.42, SD
age
=5.29) was recruited from CloudRe-
search (www.cloudresearch.com), a website that allows users to com-
plete small tasks for pay (Chandler et al., 2019). Participants were
required to have normal or corrected to normal vision (self-reported)
to participate. Participants were excluded from analysis if they admit-
ted to cheating (e.g., writing down answers) in a post-task question-
naire (they were told they would still receive payment if they
cheated). This exclusion process resulted in three exclusions from the
younger adult group and two exclusions from the older adult group.
Informed consent was acquired and the study was completed in
accordance with the UCLA Institutional Review Board.
3.1.2 | Materials
The advertisement used was for a cognitive enhancement product
from a popular nutritional supplement company. The advertisement
was obtained from the back cover full-page format of Psychology
Today published in 2018 (and has also appeared in other similar peri-
odicals) and was displayed to participants on their computer screens
in a similar full-page format.
Within the advertisement are various features describing the
product and its uses. At the top is the company logo and product line
Dr. Formulated Brain Health. Just below, the main slogan reads
Sharpen your memory & focuswith three boxes of the relevant
products presented underneath. Below the boxes and presented in a
green font is the critical phrase of interest in the current study. The
phrase either states Clean, clinically studied whole food ingredients
to support brain health at three stages of life,Clean, clinically
proven whole food ingredients to support brain health at three stages
of life,orClean, whole food ingredients to support brain health at
three stages of life(i.e., the control condition). Beneath the critical
phrase, the target demographic is identified as Kids Young Adults
Adults 40+. In the bottom left-hand corner is the image of a physi-
cian and his credentials and in the bottom right-hand corner are the
symbols designating the product as United States Department of
Agriculture (USDA) Organic and non-genetically modified-organism
(non-GMO) project verified. Finally, the background in the top half of
the advertisement contains a greyscale, stylized image of neurons
while the bottom half is a plain white background.
The questionnaire contained multiple-choice questions asking
about the name of the brand, the main caption/slogan, the number of
boxes of the product visible in the advertisement, whether a doctor
MURPHY ET AL.3
10990720, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/acp.4106, Wiley Online Library on [15/07/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
was present, whether the product was USDA organic, the target age
group(s), the presence and identity of a background image, and
whether they would purchase the product. The critical question
asked, Which of the following phrases was present?with the fol-
lowing answer choices: Clinically studied whole food ingredients to
support brain health,Clinically proven whole food ingredients
to support brain health,Scientifically studied whole food ingredients
to support brain health,Scientifically proven whole food
ingredients to support brain health, and None of these. For each
multiple-choice response, participants were also asked to indicate
their confidence in the correctness of their response on a scale from
1 (low confidence) to 10 (high confidence). Next, participants were
asked to describe the product in a free-response format and to pro-
vide an estimate of the cost of the product (i.e., one box of the prod-
uct as shown in the advertisement).
After answering questions about the product, we asked partici-
pants questions about how seriously they took the advertisement as
well as their beliefs in their memory abilities. Specifically, on a 5-point
Likert scale, we asked participants How seriously did you engage
with the advertisement?,In general, how is your overall memory
ability relative to other people your age?, and How worried/
concerned are you about changes in your memory abilities?. Finally,
we administered Obermiller and Spangenberg's (1998) Advertising
Skepticism (SKEP) scale, which is comprised of nine questions with
responses on a 5-point Likert scale. On all questionnaires, the order of
questions and choices within each question was the same for every
participant, and each question was presented sequentially with partic-
ipants responding at their own pace.
3.1.3 | Procedure
After completing an initial demographics questionnaire, participants
were instructed that they would be viewing an advertisement for a
product. They were told In this task, you will be shown an advertise-
ment for a product for 1 min. After viewing the advertisement, you will
be asked some questions about it, including the specific content
shown.When participants indicated that they understood the instruc-
tions, they advanced to the next page where the advertisement was dis-
played. The advertisement remained on the screen for 1 min at which
point the screen automatically progressed. Participants then completed
a 1-min distractor task where they were asked to rearrange the digits of
several three-digit numbers in descending order (e.g., 456would be
rearranged to 654; adapted from Rohrer & Wixted, 1994;
Unsworth, 2007). Participants were given 5 s to view each of the
12 three-digit numbers and subsequently rearrange the digits. After
completing this distractor task, participants completed the question-
naire. Participants were required to answer all questions and provide
confidence ratings (where applicable) before advancing to the next
question. Data were collected online using Collector, an open-source
program for presenting web-based psychological experiments (Garcia &
Kornell, 2015). All materials and procedures used in the current study
were approved by the UCLA Institutional Review Board.
3.2 |Results
Descriptive statistics for responses to each question in the question-
naire are shown in Table 1. The results are divided into three primary
sections. First, we examined memory accuracy and confidence for
the phrase clinically studied,clinically proven, or neither within
the advertisement. Next, we investigated the propensity to remem-
ber the advertisement containing the word proven. Finally, we
examined differences in confidence as a function of participants'
answer selection and accuracy. We provide the full dataset including
responses to the other questions as well as exploratory correlations
between age, the correctness of memory for the critical phrase,
whether participants remembered the phrase proven, price estima-
tions, SKEP scores, self-reported worry about cognitive decline, and
participants' ratings of how seriously they engaged with the adver-
tisement on OSF. An examination of participants' responses regard-
ing purchasing behavior and price estimation is also available
on OSF.
3.2.1 | Critical phrase memory and confidence
The frequency of selected choices on the critical question Which of
the following phrases was present?is depicted in Figure 1. To examine
the accuracy of participants' memory for the critical phrase in the adver-
tisement (see Figure 2a),
1
we conducted a 2 (age: old, young)3 (label:
clinically studied, clinically proven, none) between-subjects ANOVA.
Results revealed a main effect of the label used in the advertisement
[F(2, 310) =9.25, p< .001, η
p2
=.06] such that participants who were
shown the advertisement with the phrase clinically provenwere more
accurate (M=.38, SD =.49) than participants who were shown the
advertisement with the phrase clinically studied(M=.27, SD =.44),
[p
holm
=.049, d=.28] and participants who were shown the control
advertisement with neither phrase (M=.14, SD =.25), [p
holm
< .001,
d=.59]; additionally, participants who were shown the advertisement
with the phrase clinically studiedwere more accurate than partici-
pants shown the advertisement with neither phrase [p
holm
=.049,
d=.31]. Moreover, younger (M=.31, SD =.46) and older adults
(M=.22, SD =.42) were similarly accurate [F(1, 310) =3.12, p=.078,
η
p2
=.01]. Label interacted with age group [F(2, 310) =4.67, p=.010,
η
p2
=.03] but no comparisons of interest reached significance.
Next, to examine participants' confidence in their selection of the
critical phrase in the advertisement (see Figure 2b), we conducted a
2(age:old, young)3 (label: clinically studied, clinically proven, none)
between-subjects ANOVA. Results did not reveal a main effect of the
label used in the advertisement [F(2, 310) =.48, p=.618, η
p2
<.01]
such that participants shown the advertisement with the phrase clini-
cally proven(M=5.89, SD =2.32), clinically studied(M=5.77,
SD =2.47), or neither phrase (M=5.57, SD =2.32) were similarly con-
fident in their memory for the phrase. Furthermore, younger (M=5.89,
SD =2.40) and older adults (M=5.61, SD =2.33) were similarly confi-
dent in their memory [F(1, 310) =.99, p=.320, η
p2
< .01]. Label did
not interact with age group [F(2, 310) =1.63, p=.197, η
p2
=.01].
4MURPHY ET AL.
10990720, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/acp.4106, Wiley Online Library on [15/07/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
3.2.2 | Selecting proven
We next examined participants' propensity to select either scientifi-
cally provenor clinically provenas the critical phrase present in the
advertisement. We believed remembering that the product has been
provenis more critical to viewers' opinions of the product than
whether the product was examined scientificallyversus clinically,
which are essentially synonyms in this instance. To examine the likeli-
hood of participants selecting a phrase containing the word proven
(see Figure 3), we conducted a 2 (age: old, young)3 (label: clinically
studied, clinically proven, none) between-subjects ANOVA. Results did
not reveal a main effect of the label used in the advertisement [F
(2, 310) =.75, p=.475, η
p2
=.01] such that participants shown the
advertisement with the phrase clinically proven(M=.56, SD =.50),
clinically studied(M=.49, SD =.50), or neither phrase (M=.50,
SD =.50) were similarly likely to select an option containing the term
proven. Moreover, younger (M=.51, SD =.50) and older adults
(M=.52, SD =.50) were similarly likely to select an option that used
the term proven[F(1, 310) =.01, p=.915, η
p2
< .01]. Label did not
interact with age group [F(2, 310) =1.41, p=.245, η
p2
=.01].
We were also interested in whether participants were more likely
to remember the word provenor studiedin the advertisement
(again, we collapsed across responses containing the term clinically
and scientifically). Among participants whose advertisement included
the claim clinically proven, selecting a response using the phrase
proven(56%) was more likely than selecting a response using the
phrase studied(34%), [χ
2
(1) =8.07, p=.004]. Similarly, among partic-
ipants whose advertisement included the claim clinically studied,
selecting a response using the phrase proven(49%) was more likely
than selecting a response using the phrase studied(39%), [χ
2
(1)
=3.93, p=.048]. Additionally, among participants whose advertise-
ment did not include the claim clinically provenor clinically studied
(i.e., the control group), selecting a response using the phrase proven
(50%) was more likely than selecting a response using the phrase stud-
ied(36%), [χ
2
(1) =4.19, p=.041]. Thus, participants were more likely
to remember the product as being proventhan to have been stud-
ied, regardless of the claim actually present in the ad (and even if there
was not an efficacy claim present in the ad). However, this tendency
does not appear to differ as a function of the label used in the ad
because each group demonstrated a similar propensity to select an
option containing proven.
3.2.3 | Differences in confidence based on
selections and accuracy
We were also interested in differences in confidence based on partici-
pants' answer selection. We conducted a one-way ANOVA which
revealed differences in confidence between groups [F(4, 311) =5.41,
p<.001,η
p2
=.07] such that participants selecting none of the above
were less confident than participants selecting any other answer [all
p
holm
< .025]; there were no other pairwise differences [all p
holm
>.902].
We also looked at differences in confidence based on accuracy; how-
ever, participants who selected the correct phrase from the advertise-
ment were similarly confident as those who were incorrect [F(1, 314)
=1.00, p=.317, η
p2
<.01].
4|GENERAL DISCUSSION
The current study aimed to examine how people remember specific
aspects of advertisements. Specifically, we used a cognitive enhance-
ment supplement advertisement to determine if people misremember
a critical efficacy phrase that is often common when presenting
TABLE 1 Means and standard deviations (in parentheses) for responses to each question in the questionnaire.
Clinically
proven
Younger adults
clinically studied Control
Clinically
proven
Older adults
clinically studied Control
Brand name accuracy 45% (50%) 45% (50%) 36% (49%) 66% (48%) 57% (50%) 37% (49%)
Brand name confidence 6.51 (2.75) 6.69 (2.64) 6.00 (2.47) 7.00 (2.89) 6.83 (2.99) 6.04 (3.06)
Slogan accuracy 51% (51%) 41% (50%) 54% (50%) 61% (49%) 60% (49%) 61% (49%)
Slogan confidence 7.75 (2.21) 7.10 (2.50) 7.56 (2.29) 7.30 (2.26) 7.15 (2.46) 7.19 (2.58)
Critical efficacy phrase accuracy 45% (50%) 39% (49%) 8% (27%) 32% (47%) 15% (36%) 19% (40%)
Critical efficacy phrase confidence 6.26 (2.15) 5.55 (2.59) 5.84 (2.45) 5.55 (2.42) 5.96 (2.37) 5.33 (2.20)
Number of boxes accuracy 75% (44%) 65% (48%) 74% (44%) 80% (40%) 79% (41%) 83% (38)
Number of boxes confidence 7.55 (2.69) 7.65 (2.87) 7.94 (2.37) 8.59 (2.19) 8.49 (2.04) 8.60 (2.11)
Doctor pictured accuracy 75% (44%) 63% (49%) 70% (46%) 71% (46%) 83% (38%) 72% (45%)
Doctor pictured confidence 8.26 (2.36) 8.24 (2.47) 8.26 (2.02) 8.49 (1.80) 9.14 (1.48) 8.62 (1.81)
Organic accuracy 67% (48%) 59% (50%) 58% (50%) 48% (50%) 42% (50%) 47% (50%)
Organic confidence 8.40 (2.06) 7.70 (2.56) 7.24 (2.29) 7.73 (2.35) 7.76 (2.07) 7.90 (2.29)
Target age accuracy 69% (47%) 69% (47%) 70% (46%) 73% (45%) 77% (42%) 72% (45%)
Target age confidence 8.51 (2.13) 7.96 (2.61) 8.12 (2.40) 8.66 (1.87) 8.74 (2.16) 8.39 (2.45)
Would buy 29% (46%) 33% (47%) 38% (49%) 9% (29%) 25% (43%) 14% (35%)
Product price $47 ($113) $83 ($212) $57 ($198) $25 ($20) $25 ($17) $33 ($23)
MURPHY ET AL.5
10990720, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/acp.4106, Wiley Online Library on [15/07/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
scientific information in consumer settings. To test this, we had partic-
ipants view an ad containing a claim regarding the product's efficacy
(either clinically studiedor clinically proven); we also included a
control group with no efficacy claim. We then asked participants whether
they believed that the claim that was present in the advertisement was
scientifically studied,scientifically proven,clinically studied,clini-
cally proven, or none of these. Despite being able to remember certain
aspects of the advertisement (such as visual information regarding the
layout of the ad and the presence of a photo of a doctor), participants
demonstrated poor memory for this critical efficacy phrase (only 26% of
participants accurately remembered the phrase) which was also reflected
in their confidence ratings (i.e., participants were similarly confident
whether they accurately remembered the critical phrase or not). Most
importantly, regardless of which ad participants studied, participants dem-
onstrated a greater propensity to remember the critical phrase as contain-
ing the word proventhan studiedwhich may represent a greater
reliance on gist-based memory and schematic knowledge over the precise
recollection of the exact phrase that was in the ad.
FIGURE 1 The percentage of
participants in each condition (whether
the studied advertisement contained the
phrase Proven,Studied, or no efficacy
phrase (control condition)) selecting each
of the options on the questionnaire
regarding the advertisement.
FIGURE 2 The probability of selecting the correct answer
regarding which phrase was present in the advertisement (a) as well
as confidence in participants' responses (b) as a function of age and
the phrase present when viewing the advertisement. Error bars reflect
the standard error of the mean.
FIGURE 3 The probability of selecting an answer containing the
term provenas a function of age and the phrase present when
viewing the advertisement. Error bars reflect the standard error of
the mean.
6MURPHY ET AL.
10990720, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/acp.4106, Wiley Online Library on [15/07/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Regarding participants' confidence ratings, the lack of differences
in confidence for the various answer choices suggests that partici-
pants were generally unsure of which phrase was present. We found
it especially surprising that participants who were correct were no
more confident in their memory than those who remembered a
phrase including entirely absent words, providing evidence of the
frailty of memory. The present results are also consistent with
the fuzzy-trace theory such that biases may influence the reconstruc-
tion of one's memory for this type of advertising claim (Brainerd &
Reyna, 2002,2004; Reyna, 2012; Reyna et al., 2016).
It could be the case that participants misremembered the criti-
cal phrase in the advertisement because they may not have
attended to and/or encoded the phrase in the first place. Interest-
ingly, we found that many participants remembered other visual
aspects of the ad, such as whether a picture of a doctor was pre-
sent or the number of boxes presented in the ad, although partici-
pants showed poorer memory performance for more specific verbal
information such as the brand name and slogan. Specifically, as can
be observed in Table 1, some questions were answered much more
accurately (e.g., the target age groups were correctly remembered
by 72% of participants). Thus, participants did accurately remember
certain contents of the advertisement but memory for the critical
efficacy phrase was relatively poor (26%). As such, the efficacy
phrase was not well encoded (or perhaps even received no top-
down attentional allocation at all), leading to poor memory for this
information.
In terms of potential age-related differences and similarities for
various aspects of the ad, both younger and older adults were similarly
inaccurate in their memory for the critical phrase and were also simi-
larly likely to remember the term provenin the advertisement. Older
adults remembered information fairly well relative to younger adults,
and this may be due to experience and the use of context that can
help older adults remember relevant information, perhaps by relying
on schematic support (Castel, 2005; Hess, 2005). However, age-
related differences may emerge if memory were tested at a longer
delay and/or involved remembering specific associations between
similar advertisements (Naveh-Benjamin, 2000). In general, older
adults may remember gist information as well as certain aspects that
are important and this could be useful in terms of making informed
decisions regarding consumer products, but future research is needed
to determine how well older adults can accurately remember specific
details that may be relevant to consumer behavior.
Future work should aim to replicate these findings in a more
diverse array of advertising phrases and contexts. However, we antici-
pate that similar results would be found with other advertisements
such that participants would demonstrate poor memory for many spe-
cific details of the adparticularly efficacy claimsand generally rely
on schematic knowledge in the absence of verbatim memory for cer-
tain pieces of information. It would also be informative to implement
eye-tracking technology to study how top-down and bottom-up
attention predicts subsequent memory for components of a product
advertisement (Bott et al., 2017; Hervet et al., 2011; Wedel &
Pieters, 2007). We also note that the fixed order with which
participants viewed the multiple-choice options for each question
may be a limitation; future work could randomize the order of the
options to avoid potential confounds.
In sum, understanding memory for health-relevant information
is critical given the prevalence of health information (and misinfor-
mation) abound on news and social media platforms that could have
adverse effects on public health (Bode & Vraga, 2018;Chou
et al., 2018; Wang et al., 2019). In the present study, we examined
memory for an efficacy claim in a health-related product advertise-
ment. Results indicated that advertisers may be able to take advan-
tage of people's poor memory when attempting to convince
consumers of the efficacy of health products. Specifically, if a prod-
uct has been scientificallyor clinicallyproven, this implies that
the product has been tested for effectiveness and the results indi-
cated that the product yielded benefits compared to controls or
competing products. However, simply stating that something has
been studiedonly implies that a study was conductedthe results
could have indicated that the product does not work or even has
negative effects. Yet, even if this is the case, the product has still
been studied. Importantly, if consumers are unable to differentiate
between this terminology (studiedversus proven)intheirmem-
ory for a product, advertisers may be able to mislead consumers
regarding the efficacy of the product. We suggest that ambiguous
claims of efficacy such as being scientifically studiedor clinically
studiedbe carefully considered by consumers so as not to be mis-
led regarding a product's efficacy. While the present results may be
limited to a particular domain (i.e., memory for dietary supplement
information), we highlight the way human memoryeven for conse-
quential health-related informationmay be malleable and
inaccurate.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The authors would like to thank Mary Hargis, Tyson Kerr, Catherine
Middlebrooks, Julia Schorn, Katie Silaj, Mary Whatley, and the rest of
the members of the Memory and Lifespan Cognition Laboratory at
UCLA for their guidance and support. We would also like to thank
Matt Rhodes for helpful comments on a previous version of the
manuscript.
FUNDING INFORMATION
This research was supported in part by the National Institutes of
Health (National Institute on Aging; Award Number R01 AG044335
to Dr. Alan Castel).
CONFLICT OF INTEREST STATEMENT
The authors certify that they have no affiliations with or involvement
in any organization or entity with any financial or non-financial inter-
est in the subject matter or materials discussed in this manuscript.
DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT
The stimuli and data from the present study have been made available
on the Open Science Framework: https://osf.io/6hya3/?view_only=
503efdc974d54e83879c8bbfdb3abb2b.
MURPHY ET AL.7
10990720, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/acp.4106, Wiley Online Library on [15/07/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
ORCID
Dillon H. Murphy https://orcid.org/0000-0002-5604-3494
Shawn T. Schwartz https://orcid.org/0000-0001-6444-8451
Alan D. Castel https://orcid.org/0000-0003-1965-8227
ENDNOTE
1
Across all participants and conditions, memory accuracy for the critical
phrase (M=.26, SD =.44) was better than chance (.20), [t(315) =2.53,
p=.012, d=.14].
REFERENCES
Alba, J. W., & Hasher, L. (1983). Is memory schematic? Psychological Bulle-
tin,93, 203231.
Bagozzi, R. P., & Silk, A. J. (1983). Recall, recognition, and the measure-
ment of memory for print advertisements. Marketing Science,2,
95134.
Bode, L., & Vraga, E. K. (2018). See something, say something: Correction
of global health misinformation on social media. Health Communica-
tion,33, 11311140.
Bott, N. T., Lange, A., Rentz, D., Buffalo, E., Clopton, P., & Zola, S. (2017).
Web camera based eye tracking to assess visual memory on a visual
paired comparison task. Frontiers in Neuroscience,11, 370.
Brainerd, C. J., & Reyna, V. F. (2002). Fuzzy-trace theory and false mem-
ory. Current Directions in Psychological Science,11, 164169.
Brainerd, C. J., & Reyna, V. F. (2004). Fuzzy-trace theory and memory
development. Developmental Review,24, 396439.
Braun, K. (1999). Postexperience advertising effects on consumer memory.
Journal of Consumer Research,25, 319334.
Braun, K. A., & Loftus, E. F. (1998). Advertising's misinformation effect.
Applied Cognitive Psychology,12, 569591.
Braun-LaTour, K. A., & LaTour, M. S. (2004). Assessing the long-term
impact of a consistent advertising campaign on consumer memory.
Journal of Advertising,33,4961.
Brewer, W. F., & Treyens, J. C. (1981). Role of schemata in memory for
places. Cognitive Psychology,13, 207230.
Butler, M., Nelson, V. A., Davila, H., Ratner, E., Fink, H. A., Hemmy, L. S.,
McCarten, J. R., Barclay, T. R., Brasure, M., & Kane, R. L. (2017). Over-
the-counter supplement interventions to prevent cognitive decline,
mild cognitive impairment, and clinical Alzheimer-type dementia: A
systematic review. Annals of Internal Medicine,168,5262.
Castel, A. D. (2005). Memory for grocery prices in younger and older
adults: The role of schematic support. Psychology and Aging,20,
718721.
Chandler, J., Rosenzweig, C., Moss, A. J., Robinson, J., & Litman, L.
(2019). Online panels in social science research: Expanding sampling
methods beyond mechanical Turk. Behavior Research Methods,51,
20222038.
Childers, T. L. (1986). Memory for the visual and verbal components of
print advertisements. Psychology & Marketing,3, 137149.
Chou, W. S., Oh, A., & Klein, W. M. P. (2018). Addressing health-related
misinformation on social media. Jama,320, 24172418.
Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory of
semantic processing. Psychological Review,82, 407428.
Cowley, E., & Mitchell, A. A. (2003). The moderating effect of product
knowledge on the learning and organization of product information.
Journal of Consumer Research,30, 443454.
Devitt, A. L., & Schacter, D. L. (2016). False memories with age: Neural and
cognitive underpinnings. Neuropsychologia,91, 346359.
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G* power 3: A
flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behav-
ioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods,39,
175191.
Flores, C. C., Hargis, M. B., McGillivray, S., Friedman, M. C., & Castel, A. D.
(2017). Gist-based memory for prices and better buysin younger
and older adults. Memory,25, 565573.
Friedman, M. C., McGillivray, S., Murayama, K., & Castel, A. D. (2015).
Memory for medication side effects in younger and older adults. Mem-
ory & Cognition,43, 206215.
Fung, H. H., & Carstensen, L. L. (2003). Sending memorable messages to
the old: Age differences in preferences and memory for advertise-
ments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,85, 163178.
Gallo, D. A. (2010). False memories and fantastic beliefs: 15 years of the
DRM illusion. Memory & Cognition,38, 833848.
Gallo, H. B., Hargis, M. B., & Castel, A. D. (2019). Memory for weather
information in younger and older adults: Tests of verbatim and gist
memory. Experimental Aging Research,45, 252265.
Garcia, M., & Kornell, N. (2015). Collector [Computer software]. Retrieved
from https://github.com/gikeymarica/Collector
Gestuvo, M. K., & Hung, W. W. (2012). Common dietary supplements for
cognitive health. Aging Health,8,8997.
Hargis, M. B., & Castel, A. D. (2018). Younger and older adults' associative
memory for medication interactions of varying severity. Memory,26,
11511158.
Hasher, L., & Griffin, M. (1978). Reconstructive and reproductive pro-
cesses in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning
and Memory,4, 318330.
Heller, M. K., Chapman, S. C. E., & Horne, R. (2017). No blank slates: Pre-
existing schemas about pharmaceuticals predict memory for side
effects. Psychology & Health,32, 402421.
Hervet, G., Guérard, K., Tremblay, S., & Chtourou, M. S. (2011). Is banner
blindness genuine? Eye tracking internet text advertising. Applied Cog-
nitive Psychology,25, 708716.
Hess, T. M. (2005). Memory and aging in context. Psychological Bulletin,
131, 383406.
Hoch, S. J., & Deighton, J. (1989). Managing what consumers learn from
experience. Journal of Marketing,53,120.
Holden, S. J. S., & Vanhuele, M. (1999). Know the name, forget the expo-
sure: Brand familiarity versus memory of exposure context. Psychology
and Marketing,16, 479496.
Krishnan, H. S., & Shapiro, S. (1996). Comparing implicit and explicit mem-
ory for brand names from advertisements. Journal of Experimental Psy-
chology: Applied,2, 147163.
LaTour, K. A., & LaTour, M. S. (2012). Can advertising change memory for
even a really discrepant experience? Paradigm issues in the study of
post-experience advertising. Journal of Current Issues and Research in
Advertising,33, 210226.
LaTour, K. A., LaTour, M. S., & Brainerd, C. C. (2014). Fuzzy trace theory
and smartfalse memories: Implications for advertising. Journal of
Advertising,43,317.
Lew, A. R., & Howe, M. L. (2016). Out of place, out of mind: Schema-driven
false memory effects for object-location binding. Journal of Experimen-
tal Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,43, 404421.
Lyle, K. B., Bloise, S. M., & Johnson, M. K. (2006). Age-related binding defi-
cits and the content of false memories. Psychology and Aging,21,
8695.
Martín-Luengo, B., Luna, K., & Migueles, M. (2015). Effects of interest, the-
matic congruence, and typicality on memory for television, radio, and
press advertisements of new products. Applied Cognitive Psychology,
29, 560572.
Miller, M. B., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (1998). Creating false memories for visual
scenes. Neuropsychologia,36, 513520.
Mitchell, K. J., Johnson, M. K., Raye, C. L., Mather, M., & D'Esposito, M.
(2000). Aging and reflective processes of working memory: Binding
and test load deficits. Psychology and Aging,15, 527541.
Montgomery, N. V., & Rajagopal, P. (2018). Motivated reconstruction: The
effect of brand commitment on false memories. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Applied,24, 159179.
8MURPHY ET AL.
10990720, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/acp.4106, Wiley Online Library on [15/07/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Morrow, D., Azevedo, R. F. L., Garcia-Retamero, R.,
Hasegawa-Johnson, M., Huang, T., Schuh, W., Gu, K., & Zhang, Y.
(2019). Contextualizing numeric clinical test results for gist compre-
hension: Implications for her patient portals. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Applied,25,4161.
Morrow, D., Leirer, V., Altieri, P., & Tanke, E. (1991). Elders' schema for
taking medication: Implications for instruction design. Journal of Geron-
tology: Psychological Sciences,46, 378385.
Murphy, D. H., & Castel, A. D. (2021). Tall towers: Schemas and illusions
when perceiving and remembering a familiar building. Applied Cognitive
Psychology,35, 12361246.
Naveh-Benjamin, M. (2000). Adult age differences in memory perfor-
mance: Tests of an associative deficit hypothesis. Journal of Experimen-
tal Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,26, 11701187.
Obermiller, C., & Spangenberg, E. R. (1998). Development of a scale to
measure consumer skepticism toward advertising. Journal of Consumer
Psychology,7, 159186.
Reyna, V. F. (2012). A new intuitionism: Meaning, memory, and development
in fuzzy-trace theory. Judgment and Decision Making,7,332359.
Reyna, V. F., Corbin, J. C., Weldon, R. B., & Brainerd, C. J. (2016). How
fuzzy-trace theory predicts true and false memories for words, sen-
tences, and narratives. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cog-
nition,5,19.
Rice, G. E., & Okun, M. A. (1994). Older readers' processing of medical
information that contradicts their beliefs. Journal of Gerontology: Psy-
chological Sciences,49, 119128.
Roediger, H. L., III, & McDermott, K. B. (2000). Tricks of memory. Current
Directions in Psychological Science,9, 123127.
Rohrer, D., & Wixted, J. T. (1994). An analysis of latency and interresponse
time in free recall. Memory & Cognition,22, 511524.
Schacter, D. L. (1999). The seven sins of memory: Insights from psychology
and cognitive neuroscience. American Psychologist,54, 182203.
Schacter, D. L., Koutstaal, W., & Norman, K. A. (1997). False memories and
aging. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,1, 229236.
Shapiro, S., & Krishnan, H. S. (2001). Memory-based measures for asses-
sing advertising effects: A comparison of explicit and implicit memory
effects. Journal of Advertising,30,113.
Sherman, S. M., Follows, H., Mushore, A. B., Hampson-Jones, K., &
Wright-Bevans, K. (2015). Television advertisements create false
memories for competitor brands. Journal of Applied Research in Memory
and Cognition,4,17.
Umanath, S. (2016). Age differences in suggestibility to contradictions of
demonstrated knowledge: The influence of prior knowledge. Aging,
Neuropsychology, and Cognition,23, 744767.
Umanath, S., & Marsh, E. J. (2014). Understanding how prior knowledge
influences memory in older adults. Perspectives on Psychological Sci-
ence,9, 408426.
Unsworth, N. (2007). Individual differences in working memory capacity
and episodic retrieval: Examining the dynamics of delayed and contin-
uous distractor free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
Memory, and Cognition,33, 10201034.
Wang, Y., McKee, M., Torbica, A., & Stuckler, D. (2019). Systematic litera-
ture review on the spread of health-related misinformation on social
media. Social Science & Medicine, 112552.
Wedel, M., & Pieters, R. (2007). A review of eye-tracking research in mar-
keting. In N. K. Malhotra (Ed.), Review of marketing research (pp. 123
147). M.E. Sharpe.
How to cite this article: Murphy, D. H., Schwartz, S. T.,
Alberts, K. O., Siegel, A. L. M., Carone, B. J., Castel, A. D., &
Drolet, A. (2023). Clinically studied or clinically proven?
Memory for claims in print advertisements. Applied Cognitive
Psychology,19. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.4106
MURPHY ET AL.9
10990720, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/acp.4106, Wiley Online Library on [15/07/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any citations for this publication.
Article
Full-text available
We investigated how schemas can bias both memory and perception of a frequently seen building leading to a horizontal-vertical illusion. Specifically, undergraduate students (n = 172) were asked to estimate and sketch the dimensions of a highly familiar campus building to determine if they misremember or misperceive the building's features. Despite its cubic dimensions, participants frequently overestimated the building's height to width ratio, both on sketches and estimates, as they were likely biased by the horizontal-vertical illusion and the schema that buildings are often taller than wider. This occurred regardless of whether participants sketched and estimated from memory or completed these tasks while perceiving the building. Additionally, participants were often unable to correctly identify the building's outline on a recognition test, even while looking at it. These results demonstrate that both perceptual and memory accuracy can be impacted by schematic biases and cognitive illusions. This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
Article
Full-text available
Contemporary commentators describe the current period as "an era of fake news" in which misinformation, generated intentionally or unintentionally, spreads rapidly. Although affecting all areas of life, it poses particular problems in the health arena, where it can delay or prevent effective care, in some cases threatening the lives of individuals. While examples of the rapid spread of misinformation date back to the earliest days of scientific medicine, the internet, by allowing instantaneous communication and powerful amplification has brought about a quantum change. In democracies where ideas compete in the marketplace for attention, accurate scientific information, which may be difficult to comprehend and even dull, is easily crowded out by sensationalized news. In order to uncover the current evidence and better understand the mechanism of misinformation spread, we report a systematic review of the nature and potential drivers of health-related misinformation. We searched PubMed, Cochrane, Web of Science, Scopus and Google databases to identify relevant methodological and empirical articles published between 2012 and 2018. A total of 57 articles were included for full-text analysis. Overall, we observe an increasing trend in published articles on health-related misinformation and the role of social media in its propagation. The most extensively studied topics involving misinformation relate to vaccination, Ebola and Zika Virus, although others, such as nutrition, cancer, fluoridation of water and smoking also featured. Studies adopted theoretical frameworks from psychology and network science, while co-citation analysis revealed potential for greater collaboration across fields. Most studies employed content analysis, social network analysis or experiments, drawing on disparate disciplinary paradigms. Future research should examine susceptibility of different sociodemographic groups to misinformation and understand the role of belief systems on the intention to spread misinformation. Further interdisciplinary research is also warranted to identify effective and tailored interventions to counter the spread of health-related misinformation online.
Article
Full-text available
Amazon Mechanical Turk (MTurk) is widely used by behavioral scientists to recruit research participants. MTurk offers advantages over traditional student subject pools, but it also has important limitations. In particular, the MTurk population is small and potentially overused, and some groups of interest to behavioral scientists are underrepresented and difficult to recruit. Here we examined whether online research panels can avoid these limitations. Specifically, we compared sample composition, data quality (measured by effect sizes, internal reliability, and attention checks), and the non-naivete of participants recruited from MTurk and Prime Panels—an aggregate of online research panels. Prime Panels participants were more diverse in age, family composition, religiosity, education, and political attitudes. Prime Panels participants also reported less exposure to classic protocols and produced larger effect sizes, but only after screening out several participants who failed a screening task. We conclude that online research panels offer a unique opportunity for research, yet one with some important trade-offs.
Article
Full-text available
Patient portals to Electronic Health Record (EHR) systems are underused by older adults because of limited system usability and usefulness, including difficulty understanding numeric information. We investigated whether enhanced context for portal messages about test results improved responses to these messages, comparing verbally, graphically, and video-enhanced formats. Older adults viewed scenarios with fictitious patient profiles and messages describing results for these patients from cholesterol or diabetes screening tests indicating lower, borderline, or higher risk levels. These messages were conveyed by standard format (table of numerical test scores) or one of the enhanced formats. Verbatim and gist memory for test results, risk perception, affective response, attitude toward and intention to perform self-care behaviors, and satisfaction were measured. Verbally and video enhanced context improved older adults' gist but not verbatim memory compared to the standard format, suggesting we were successful in designing messages that highlight gist-based information. Little evidence was found for benefits related to the graphically enhanced format. Although verbally and video enhanced formats improved gist memory and message satisfaction, they had less impact on the other responses to the messages. However, these responses reflected level of risk: As risk associated with test results increased, positive affect decreased whereas negative affect, perceived risk, behavioral attitudes, and intentions increased, as predicted by behavioral change theories.
Article
Full-text available
Across 5 studies, we examine the effect of prior brand commitment on the creation of false memories about product experience after reading online product reviews. We find that brand commitment and the valence of reviews to which consumers are exposed, interact to affect the incidence of false memories. Thus, highly committed consumers are more susceptible to the creation of false experience memories on exposure to positive versus negative reviews, whereas low commitment consumers exhibit similar levels of false memories in response to both positive and negative reviews. Further, these differences across brand commitment are attenuated when respondents are primed with an accuracy motivation, suggesting that the biasing effects of commitment are likely because of the motivation to defend the committed brand. Finally, we find that differences in false memories subsequently lead to differences in intentions to spread word-of-mouth (e.g., recommend the product to friends), suggesting that the consequences of false product experience memories can be significant for marketers and consumers. Our findings contribute to the literatures in false memory and marketing by documenting a motivated bias in false memories because of brand commitment.
Article
Background/Study Context: Memory for specific, verbatim details tends to decline with age, and reliance on gist-based information increases. However, instructions that direct attention toward certain types of information can benefit memory accuracy for that information. Previous work has examined gist-based and verbatim memory for images, but little work has utilized stimuli that participants may study in their daily lives, such as a weather forecast. Methods: The current study examined how younger and older adults recall both general, gist-based information and specific, verbatim details of a weather forecast, and whether differences in the task instructions to focus on gist-based information may affect recall. Two study-test cycles with different forecasts were used to determine whether experience with the task may affect performance. Results: While there was no effect of additional gist-based instructions on recall of gist-based information, participants who received the additional instructions recalled fewer verbatim details than those who did not. There were no age-related differences in recall of the gist of the forecast, but younger adults correctly recalled more verbatim details than older adults did. Conclusion: Environmental support and use of gist-based processing can allow both younger and older adults to remember information that can be useful in their daily lives. The current study informs future research on prospective memory and memory for everyday information.
Article
The ubiquitous social media landscape has created an information ecosystem populated by a cacophony of opinion, true and false information, and an unprecedented quantity of data on many topics. Policy makers and the social media industry grapple with the challenge of curbing fake news, disinformation, and hate speech; and the field of medicine is similarly confronted with the spread of false, inaccurate, or incomplete health information.
Article
The authors argue that what consumers learn from the experience of using products is not a simple matter of discovering objective truth. They frame the problem of learning from experience as a four-stage process (hypothesizing—exposure—encoding—integration) with three moderating factors (familiarity with the domain, motivation to learn, and the ambiguity of the information environment). The framework is used to identify where learning from product consumption experience is most open to managerial influence. The authors discuss strategic tools for managing experiential learning and consider applications to the simulation of learning in concept and pre-test-market product testing.
Article
Across 5 studies, we examine the effect of prior brand commitment on the creation of false memories about product experience after reading online product reviews. We find that brand commitment and the valence of reviews to which consumers are exposed, interact to affect the incidence of false memories. Thus, highly committed consumers are more susceptible to the creation of false experience memories on exposure to positive versus negative reviews, whereas low commitment consumers exhibit similar levels of false memories in response to both positive and negative reviews. Further, these differences across brand commitment are attenuated when respondents are primed with an accuracy motivation, suggesting that the biasing effects of commitment are likely because of the motivation to defend the committed brand. Finally, we find that differences in false memories subsequently lead to differences in intentions to spread word-of-mouth (e.g., recommend the product to friends), suggesting that the consequences of false product experience memories can be significant for marketers and consumers. Our findings contribute to the literatures in false memory and marketing by documenting a motivated bias in false memories because of brand commitment.
Article
While older adults face various deficits in binding items in memory, they are often able to remember information that is deemed important. In Experiment 1, we examined how younger and older adults remember medication interactions of varying severity. There were no age differences in overall memory accuracy, but older adults’ performance depended on the severity of the interactions (such that the interactions associated with the most severe health outcomes were remembered most accurately) while younger adults’ did not. In Experiment 2, a similar task was designed to create interference in memory. Even with this more difficult task there were no age differences in recall accuracy, and both age groups remembered the interactions with the severe outcomes most accurately. These findings suggest that, under certain circumstances, older adults do not face deficits in associative recognition accuracy of information that varies in importance.