Conference PaperPDF Available

Preventing Scenario Recognition in Human-in-the-Loop Air Traffic Control Research

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

In academic air traffic control research, traffic scenarios are often repeated to increase the sample size and enable paired-sample comparisons, e.g., between different display variants. This comes with the risk that participants recognize scenarios and consequently recall the desired response. In this paper we provide an overview of mitigation techniques found in literature and conclude that rotating scenario geometries is most frequently used. The potential impact of these transformations on participant behavior, as described in this paper, is however not sufficiently addressed in most studies. As an example we, therefore, analyze previously collected eye tracking data from ten professional air traffic controllers, each presented with three repetitions in various rotations of several distinct scenarios. Results imply that researchers wishing to repeat scenarios should more carefully consider whether mitigation techniques might have an impact on their results.
Content may be subject to copyright.
PREVENTING SCENARIO RECOGNITION IN
HUMAN-IN-THE-LOOP AIR TRAFFIC CONTROL RESEARCH
Gijs de Rooij, Clark Borst, M. M. (Ren´
e) van Paassen and Max Mulder
Aerospace Engineering - Delft University of Technology, Delft, The Netherlands
In academic air traffic control research, traffic scenarios are often repeated to increase the sample
size and enable paired-sample comparisons, e.g., between different display variants. This comes
with the risk that participants recognize scenarios and consequently recall the desired response. In
this paper we provide an overview of mitigation techniques found in literature and conclude that ro-
tating scenario geometries is most frequently used. The potential impact of these transformations on
participant behavior, as described in this paper, is however not sufficiently addressed in most studies.
As an example we, therefore, analyze previously collected eye tracking data from ten professional
air traffic controllers, each presented with three repetitions in various rotations of several distinct
scenarios. Results imply that researchers wishing to repeat scenarios should more carefully consider
whether mitigation techniques might have an impact on their results.
Introduction
In air traffic controller (ATCO) training and airspace redesign trials, simulation scenarios are designed to be as realistic
as possible with many different flights over a prolonged period of time. High face validity enables the ATCOs to
execute their tasks as they would in an operational setting. Academic research, however, often benefits from simplified,
more constrained scenarios that are presented to novices or experts while tracking their behavior e.g., when using
different display variants. Constructing alike scenarios, where the scenario itself is not an independent variable, is
a major task, requiring considerable effort and input from subject matter experts. As an alternative, identical traffic
scenarios are, therefore, often repeated to obtain paired-samples at the risk of scenario recognition. Depending on the
aim of the study, this can be undesirable as participants may recall their earlier responses rather than coming up with an
independent solution, aggravating learning effects. This applies especially to studies that measure ATCO consistency,
such as in the personalization of conflict resolution advisories (Westin et al., 2016). Finding a balance between using
alike scenarios and preventing recognition is not trivial.
In this paper we, for the first time, provide an overview of techniques used to mitigate scenario recognition
in existing air traffic control (ATC) studies. A straightforward and frequently employed method is to rotate and/or
mirror scenarios. While these transformations result in identical scenarios in terms of conflict angles, traffic densities
and patterns etc., the change in orientation may unconsciously impact participant behavior. This may not reveal itself
in the final outcome, e.g., solving a conflict, but it can elicit different visual scan patterns to arrive at this outcome.
Visual search is an essential process that ATCOs use to continuously update their mental picture (Fraga et al., 2021).
Changes in this process may lead to faster or slower conflict detection in otherwise identical scenarios, affecting
related objective measures. Furthermore, perceived workload may be affected (e.g., due to unusual traffic directions,
especially for experts) and action sequences or conflict resolutions might change due to different fixation orders.
These effects are, to the best of our knowledge, not sufficiently identified and recognized in literature. Authors
often merely mention that scenarios are transformed to ‘prevent recognition’ without further detailing their consider-
ations or the transformation’s implications. In addition to our literature survey on mitigation techniques, we therefore
analyze eye tracking data from a previously executed experiment that featured scenario transformations. The data con-
sists of ten professional ATCOs who each performed conflict detection and resolution in 15 distinct scenarios, of which
five were selected for this analysis. Each scenario was presented three times to them with different transformations.
By comparing the order in and speed at which flights were fixated, we empirically describe the participants’ behav-
ioral consistency when presented with transformed repetitive scenarios. To conclude we argue on the implications that
researchers should consider when repeating scenarios, based on these initial findings.
Mitigation Techniques
A literature survey resulted in the identification of three categories of techniques to prevent scenario recogni-
tion, explicitly described in 20 ATC studies and summarized in Table 1: geometric, textual and temporal. Most studies
used a combination of techniques, with rotating scenarios as the most popular technique, employed in 15 studies.
Table 1: Scenario recognition mitigation techniques explicitly mentioned in existing research.
Geometric Textual Temporal
Renaming Renaming Time
Study Rotation Mirroring callsigns waypoints shifting Reordering
Abdul Rahman, 2014 - - - - -
Albuquerque et al., 2008 - ? - -
Borst et al., 2017 - - - -
Borst et al., 2019 - - - -
Cummings et al., 2005 - - - - -
Harrison et al., 2014 - - - - -
Hilburn et al., 2014 - - - -
IJtsma et al., 2022 - - - - -
Jans et al., 2019 - - - - -
Jasek et al., 1995 - - - - -
Jha et al., 2011 - - -
Kim et al., 2022 - - -
Klomp et al., 2016 -- - -
Major and Hansman, 2004 - - - -
Metzger and Parasuraman, 2006 - - - - -
Rovira and Parasuraman, 2010 - - -
Sollenberger and Hale, 2011 - - - - -
ten Brink et al., 2019 - - - - -
Trapsilawati et al., 2021 - -
Wilson and Fleming, 2002 - - - - -
Geometric When a scenario is rotated or mirrored, its (objective) taskload formed by the traffic density, conflict
geometries etc. remains the same, but its (subjective) workload might change. Especially with experts, ac-
customed to traffic streams from certain directions, changing the principal axis can have an impact on their
perceived workload, as it requires a change in scan pattern.
Geometric transformations can only be done when the sectors are relatively symmetric, which is generally
not the case in operational environments. Furthermore, on a widescreen monitor, rotations other than 180° may
result in a reduced look-ahead range for flights coming towards the sector. Square-shaped monitors (or simulated
windows), as found in many ATC centers, eliminate this problem. Only rotation multiples of 90° were found in
the studies, presumably because this generates sufficient transformations and is easy to execute. Albuquerque
et al. (2008) mention that they ‘invert the route structure’, without further detailing what is meant by that.
Textual Changing callsigns and waypoint names is a simple technique that can be widely applied, does not change
the taskload and has proven to be sufficient on its own in some cases, such as the study by Wilson and Fleming
(2002). When realistic callsigns and aircraft performance data are used, the callsign should match the flight’s
characteristics (e.g., no big airliner for small airlines or non-standard destinations). Similarly, when using
operational airspaces, waypoints may need to be left unaltered to match operational routes. Neither are a problem
when using airspace-naive novices.
Temporal Shifting occurrences of, for example, conflicts in time is a feasible technique for relatively long scenarios,
where chunks of traffic entering the sector can be shuffled (Albuquerque et al., 2008; Trapsilawati et al., 2021).
Such temporal transformations do, however, risk ignoring cognitive built-up and its associated impact on (per-
ceived) workload. This technique is, therefore, mostly used to construct realistic scenarios from recorded flight
data, by shifting flights to create a plausible scenario that is denser or has more conflicts than the recording.
When an experiment consists of multiple scenarios per test condition, their order can be changed. If, for exam-
ple, display variants are tested that are sufficiently distinct from each other, participants may be predominantly
occupied by the changed visuals and/or tasks, making it even less likely for them to recognize repeated scenarios
at all (Jasek et al., 1995).
An extreme case of re-ordering chunks of traffic is to add dummy scenarios in between measurement scenarios,
as done by Borst et al. (2017). If planning allows, measurements for each participant can even be split over
multiple days. This requires good planning (difficult when using experts) and is more prone to introducing con-
founds due to a lack of control over variables such as participant energy levels or between-session (professional)
experiences. It is therefore not often used, except in longitudinal studies such as by Hilburn et al. (2014).
A technique not explicitly found in literature is the shifting of all flights up or down in altitude. The individual
contribution might be marginal, as people predominantly recognize plan-view patterns, but in combination with other
techniques it can require participants to not completely rely on their memory. Care must be taken not to alter the
altitudes too much, as changes in flight level have an effect on ground speeds and thus closing rates, impacting the
time a loss of separation occurs and/or conflict warnings will be issued.
Data Description
As an example of the potential impact of scenario transformations, we revisit and analyze eye tracking data
from a previously executed experiment designed for task analyses. To prevent scenario recognition it involved static
scenarios featuring several geometric and textual transformations, dummy scenarios and a varying scenario order.
Participants and Apparatus
Ten professional en-route ATCOs (age: µ= 42.7,
σ= 6.8, years of experience: µ= 18.8,σ= 6.2),
from Eurocontrol’s Maastricht Upper Area Control Centre
(MUAC) voluntarily participated in a simulator experiment,
as approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee of
TU Delft under number 2754. All participants provided
written informed consent. A TU Delft-built medium-fidelity
simulator was designed to mimic the MUAC interface on a
1920 x 1920 pixels 27” display with a computer mouse for
control inputs, shown in Fig. 1. Although the scenarios were
static, participants could measure predicted minimum separa-
tion between flights and display extended flight labels.
Figure 1: Experiment set-up with participant (left)
and observer (right) positions.
Gaze data was recorded using a head-worn Pupil Labs Core eye tracker (Kassner et al., 2014) with Pupil
Capture v3.5.1. The forward facing scene camera recorded at 30 Hz and the pupils were recorded at 120 Hz. Eight
AprilTag markers were placed along the edges of the screen to relate gaze to screen pixels. Clusters of gaze points
that were close in location and time were classified as fixations through the Python version of I2MC by Hessels et al.
(2017), with a minimal duration threshold of 60 ms as used by Fraga et al. (2021). The fixations were correlated to
flights by drawing voronoi-like areas of interest around each flight’s symbol, speed vector and label.
Scenarios
Participants assessed five distinct static scenarios, intermingled with ten dummy scenarios that were not
included in the current analysis. Each scenario was shown three times with different transformations and featured
an artificial, octagonal 80 x 80 NM sector, with four waypoints in the cardinal directions. This made sure that the
professionals would not fully rely on their trained scan patterns and that scenarios could not be recognized based on
the sector shape. Four flights were present on direct routes to their exit points. In the dummy scenarios there were only
two or three flights, for which measures like fixation orders would be less robust. Variants were created by applying
any (combination) of the following transformations:
Rotation: 90, 180 or 270 degrees,
Mirroring: flipping along the x- or y-axis,
Altitude shift: all flights up or down by 1,000 or 2,000 ft.
Callsigns were randomized for all variants and flight labels were always placed at a 90 degree offset to the direction
of travel. Figure 2 shows an example of a scenario with corresponding transformations. Note that flights in the center
of the sector were invariant to all geometric transformations and always appeared at the same location on the screen.
VLG3877
350 -
RYR925W
320 -
34
SAS8937
310 -
LOT9YZ
290 -
27
BAW476
340 -
ANA35CT
310 -
33
WZZ10UL
300 -
RYR8WA
280 -
26
SAS868
360 -
JAL7MF
330 -
35
AFR113
320 -
VLG355H
300 -
28
Variant a Variant b Variant c
Figure 2: Three transformations of Scenario 5. Colors relate to the same flights in each transformation.
All participants got to see the same order of transformations, but the scenario ordering was counterbalanced between
them to account for learning effects. This ordering of scenarios was defined in the previously executed experiment and
might, in hindsight, have been suboptimal for the current study. The experiment started with six training scenarios.
Participant Task
Participants were asked to first indicate for each scenario whether there were any conflicts and to consequently
solve these through altitude clearances only. Some flights had to leave the sector at a different flight level, requiring
a clearance that would generally also solve any conflict(s). An intermediate level was needed in some cases to not
create a conflict. If there were no (remaining) conflicts and all flights were at or cleared to the correct flight level, the
participant could advance to the next scenario by clicking a button in the lower right corner of the screen. This button
was carefully placed to ensure a common first fixation point, not related to any flights, when a scenario loaded.
Results and Discussion
After the experiment, some participants mentioned that they did recognize the repetition of certain conflict
geometries, but none of them recalled that it were identical scenarios apart from the applied transformation(s). Our
present analysis stays away from concluding whether the recognition mitigation has worked and instead focuses on
the consistency of fixation behavior. Since participants showed vastly individualized behavior, no between-participant
comparisons are performed and all observations discussed here relate to the three scenario repetitions per individual.
Fixation Order
Conflict detection time is directly driven by the order in which flights receive attention, especially when
scenarios include many flights. After all, if an ATCO fixates flights in a different order, he/she might observe a
conflicting pair earlier or later. To this end, Fig. 3 shows for each scenario’s three repetitions the flight that was
first fixated by each ATCO. The level of consistency, in terms of identical first fixations for all three transformations
(visible as a row of three similarly colored squares), varied per ATCO from zero (Participants 5 and 10) to three
scenarios (Participants 7 and 8). A similar variance can be seen between the scenarios, with consistent first fixations
for one (Scenarios 1 and 2) to five (Scenario 5) ATCOs. This suggests that the rotations may have had an impact on the
fixation order, and that this can differ per individual and traffic layout. On closer inspection, in 80% of the runs, the
first fixated flight in Scenario 5 was located in the center of the sector (and therefore in the exact same location for all
repetitions). Conversely, Scenario 1, the only one with no flight near the center, shows the lowest level of consistency.
To illustrate individual differences, complete orders of fixation for two participants on either extremes of
the aforementioned consistency scale are shown in Fig. 4. Note how Participant 8’s complete fixation sequence is
consistent for all variants of Scenario 3. This, in combination with the inconsistent fixation orders seen in other
scenarios or with other participants, further hints at a non-negligible influence of scenario rotation on the processing of
traffic scenarios. For more insight in the relevant mechanisms, an analysis of scan patterns at different transformations
would be useful, but this requires scenarios with more flights. The static, low density scenarios used in this study
imply that the results are not necessarily applicable to dynamic and/or denser scenarios.
a b c a b c a b c a b c a b c
Scenario
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
Participant
12345
Figure 3: First fixated flight per participant. Colors
represent specific flights in a scenario (see Fig. 2 for
Scenario 5).
a b c a b c a b c a b c a b c
Scenario
1
2
3
4
1
2
3
4
Flight fixation order
12345
Participant 8
Participant 10
Figure 4: Complete flight fixation orders of two
participants. Colors represent specific flights in a
scenario (see Fig. 2 for Scenario 5).
Fixation Speed
To further illustrate the potential influence of rotations on fixation sequences and duration, Fig. 5 shows the
standardized time till specific flights in Scenarios 3 and 5 had been first fixated. Results imply that the rotational-
influence on this measure is dependent on the researcher’s flight of interest. This is most visible in Scenario 5b, where
Flight 1 shows significantly different means compared to the other two rotations. Akin to the fixation order, differences
between individuals are again considerable, reflected in the wide spread of most data.
1234
2
1
0
1
2
3
Standardized time
Flight
abc abc abc abc
Scenario 3
1234
2
1
0
1
2
3
Standardized time
Flight
abc abc abc abc
Scenario 5
Figure 5: Standardized (per participant) time till flights have been first fixated in two scenarios, split per
transformation. Colors represent specific flights in each scenario (see Fig. 2 for Scenario 5).
While the order of scenarios was counterbalanced between participants, the order of their repetitions was
not (i.e., all participants first saw a, followed by b and then c). While this resulted in a clearly visible reduction in
total fixation time over the three repetitions, this reduction is not (always) reflected in the results presented here. We
therefore conclude that this speed-up was mostly caused by the participants getting more acquainted with the task
at hand and advancing to the next scenario, rather recognizing the specific scenarios. To further isolate the effect of
purely the transformation, future studies should include duplicate scenarios where no transformation has been applied.
Conclusion
Scenario transformations such as rotation and mirroring are proven techniques to create paired-samples in
human-in-the-loop ATC research, but the potential impact on results is not always sufficiently recognized. We showed
that the most popular technique, rotating scenarios, does risk eliciting different eye fixation behavior from participants,
potentially confounding objective measures such as conflict detection time. Whether this is a problem strongly depends
on the research at hand and requires careful consideration. No definitive conclusions regarding the size of these effects
can be made on the basis of the limited analysis presented here. The first indications do warrant further research with
more elaborate, potentially dynamic, traffic scenarios and a tailored experiment design.
Acknowledgments
The authors express their gratitude to all involved ATCOs and MUAC for facilitating the experiment.
References
Abdul Rahman, S. (2014). Solution Space-based Approach to Assess Sector Complexity in Air Traffic Control (Doctoral disserta-
tion). Delft University of Technology.
Albuquerque, E. A. F., Trabasso, L. G., Sandes, A., de Ara´
ujo, M., Li, L., & Hansman, R. J. (2008). Experimental Setup for Air
Traffic Control Cognitive Complexity Analysis. Symposium of Operational Applications in Areas of Defense, 342–347.
Borst, C., Bijsterbosch, V. A., van Paassen, M. M., & Mulder, M. (2017). Ecological interface design: supporting fault diagnosis of
automated advice in a supervisory air traffic control task. Cognition, Technology and Work,19(4), 545–560.
Borst, C., Visser, R. M., van Paassen, M. M., & Mulder, M. (2019). Exploring Short-Term Training Effects of Ecological Interfaces:
A Case Study in Air Traffic Control. IEEE Transactions on Human-Machine Systems,49(6), 623–632.
Cummings, M. L., Tsonis, C. G., & Cunha, D. C. (2005). Complexity Mitigation Through Airspace Structure. International Sym-
posium on Aviation Psychology, 159–163.
Fraga, R. P., Kang, Z., Crutchfield, J. M., & Mandal, S. (2021). Visual Search and Conflict Mitigation Strategies Used by Expert en
Route Air Traffic Controllers. Aerospace,8(7).
Harrison, J., Izzetoˇ
glu, K., Ayaz, H., Willems, B., Hah, S., Ahlstrom, U., Woo, H., Shewokis, P. A., Bunce, S. C., & Onaral, B.
(2014). Cognitive workload and learning assessment during the implementation of a next-generation air traffic control
technology using functional near-infrared spectroscopy. IEEE Transactions on Human-Machine Systems,44(4), 429–
440.
Hessels, R. S., Niehorster, D. C., Kemner, C., & Hooge, I. T. (2017). Noise-robust fixation detection in eye movement data:
Identification by two-means clustering (I2MC). Behavior Research Methods,49(5), 1802–1823.
Hilburn, B., Westin, C., & Borst, C. (2014). Will Controllers Accept a Machine That Thinks like They Think? The Role of Strategic
Conformance in Decision Aiding Automation. Air Traffic Control Quarterly,22(2), 115–136.
IJtsma, M., Borst, C., van Paassen, M. M., & Mulder, M. (2022). Evaluation of a Decision-Based Invocation Strategy for Adaptive
Support for Air Traffic Control. IEEE Transactions on Human-Machine Systems,52(6), 1135–1146.
Jans, M., Borst, C., van Paassen, M. M., & Mulder, M. (2019). Effect of ATC Automation Transparency on Acceptance of Resolu-
tion Advisories. IFAC PapersOnLine,52(19), 353–358.
Jasek, M., Pioch, N., & Zeltzer, D. (1995). Enhanced Visual Displays for Air Traffic Control Collision Prediction. IFAC Proceedings
Volumes,28(15), 553–558.
Jha, P. D., Bisantz, A. M., Parasuraman, R., & Drury, C. G. (2011). Air traffic controllers’ performance in advance air traffic
management system: Part I-performance results. International Journal of Aviation Psychology,21(3), 283–305.
Kassner, M., Patera, W., & Bulling, A. (2014). Pupil: An Open Source Platform for Pervasive Eye Tracking and Mobile Gaze-
based Interaction. Adjunct Proceedings of the 2014 ACM International Joint Conference on Pervasive and Ubiquitous
Computing, 1151–1160.
Kim, M., Borst, C., & Mulder, M. (2022). Situation Awareness Prompts: Bridging the Gap between Supervisory and Manual Air
Traffic Control. IFAC-PapersOnLine,55(29), 13–18.
Klomp, R. E., Borst, C., van Paassen, M. M., & Mulder, M. (2016). Expertise Level, Control Strategies, and Robustness in Future
Air Traffic Control Decision Aiding. IEEE Transactions on Human-Machine Systems,46(2), 255–266.
Major, L. M., & Hansman, R. J. (2004). Human-Centered Systems Analysis of Mixed Equipage in Oceanic Air Traffic Control
(tech. rep.). MIT International Center for Air Transportation. Cambridge, MA, USA.
Metzger, U., & Parasuraman, R. (2006). Effects of automated conflict cuing and traffic density on air traffic controller performance
and visual attention in a datalink environment. International Journal of Aviation Psychology,16(4), 343–362.
Rovira, E., & Parasuraman, R. (2010). Transitioning to future air traffic management: Effects of imperfect automation on controller
attention and performance. Human Factors,52(3), 411–425.
Sollenberger, R. L., & Hale, M. (2011). Human-in-the-Loop Investigation of Variable Separation Standards in the En Route Air
Traffic Control Environment. Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society 55th Annual Meeting, 66–70.
ten Brink, D. S., Klomp, R. E., Borst, C., van Paassen, M. M., & Mulder, M. (2019). Flow-based air traffic control: Human-machine
interface for steering a path-planning algorithm. 2019 IEEE International Conference on Systems, Man and Cybernetics,
SMC 2019, 3186–3191.
Trapsilawati, F., Chen, C. H., Wickens, C. D., & Qu, X. (2021). Integration of conflict resolution automation and vertical situation
display for on-ground air traffic control operations. Journal of Navigation,74(3), 619–632.
Westin, C., Borst, C., & Hilburn, B. (2016). Strategic Conformance: Overcoming Acceptance Issues of Decision Aiding Automa-
tion? IEEE Transactions on Human-Machine Systems,46(1), 41–52.
Wilson, I. A. B., & Fleming, K. (2002). Controller reactions to free flight in a complex transition sector re-visited using ADS-B+.
Proceedings. The 21st Digital Avionics Systems Conference,1, 5–1.
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any citations for this publication.
Article
Full-text available
Air traffic controller workload is a limiting factor in the current air traffic management system. Adaptive support systems have the potential to balance controller workload and gain acceptance as they provide support during times of need. Challenges in the design of adaptive support systems are to decide when and how to trigger support. The goal of this study is to gain empirical insights into these challenges through a human-in-the-loop experiment, featuring a simplified air traffic control environment in which a novel triggering mechanism uses the quality of the controller's decisions to determine when support is needed. The designed system seeks to prevent high workload conditions by providing resolution advisories when the controller exceeds a threshold of “self-complicating” decisions. Results indicate that the new system is indeed capable of increasing the efficiency and safety compared to full manual control without intervention. More adaptive support, however, increased the frustration of participants, decreased acceptance, and did not result in improved workload ratings. These findings suggest that, unless we can better infer human intent in complex work environments, adaptive support at the level of decision-making is problematic. A potentially more fruitful direction is to provide support at the level of information integration, with full decision-making authority with the human.
Article
Full-text available
The role of the en route air traffic control specialist (ATCS) is vital to maintaining safety and efficiency within the National Airspace System (NAS). ATCSs must vigilantly scan the airspace under their control and adjacent airspaces using an En Route Automation Modernization (ERAM) radar display. The intent of this research is to provide an understanding of the expert controller visual search and aircraft conflict mitigation strategies that could be used as scaffolding methods during ATCS training. Interviews and experiments were conducted to elicit visual scanning and conflict mitigation strategies from the retired controllers who were employed as air traffic control instructors. The interview results were characterized and classified using various heuristics. In particular, representative visual scanpaths were identified, which accord with the interview results of the visual search strategies. The highlights of our findings include: (1) participants used systematic search patterns, such as circular, spiral, linear or quadrant-based, to extract operation-relevant information; (2) participants applied an information hierarchy when aircraft information was cognitively processed (altitude -> direction -> speed); (3) altitude or direction changes were generally preferred over speed changes when imminent potential conflicts were mitigated. Potential applications exist in the implementation of the findings into the training curriculum of candidates.
Article
Full-text available
In many work domains, the push toward higher levels of automation raises the concern of diminishing human expertise. Ecological interfaces could help operators in retaining and potentially even in acquiring expertise as they are hypothesized to lead to a deeper understanding of the work domain. This study explores the short-term impact of ecological interfaces on knowledge development and compares the results with an instruction-based training method. To monitor and compare students’ progress, their decision-making strategies, identified from verbal comments recorded in “think-aloud” simulator sessions, are mapped onto the decision ladder. This method has been applied to an experiment ( $N=16$ ) aimed at training novices in conflict detection and resolution (CD&R) within a simplified air traffic control context. Results show that the overall CD&R performance in the final measurement sessions, featuring a transfer manipulation, was not significantly different between the “ecological” and “instructional” groups. In terms of cognitive behavior, however, students in the ecological group exhibited more laborious rule- and knowledge-based behaviors that sparked goal-oriented thoughts and corresponding control performances beyond the CD&R task. These findings indicate that ecological interfaces can change how people think and approach a control problem, even after removing the support. It is therefore reasonable to believe that ecological interfaces can play an important role in the early stages of deep knowledge development.
Article
Full-text available
Future air traffic control will have to rely on more advanced automation to support human controllers in their job of safely handling increased traffic volumes. A prerequisite for the success of such automation is that the data driving it are reliable. Current technology, however, still warrants human supervision in coping with (data) uncertainties and consequently in judging the quality and validity of machine decisions. In this study, ecological interface design was used to assist controllers in fault diagnosis of automated advice, using a prototype ecological interface (called the solution space diagram) for tactical conflict detection and resolution in the horizontal plane. Results from a human-in-the-loop simulation, in which sixteen participants were tasked with monitoring automation and intervening whenever required or desired, revealed a significant improvement in fault detection and diagnosis in a complex traffic scenario. Additionally, the experiment also exposed interesting interaction patterns between the participants and the advisory system, which seemed unrelated to the fault diagnosis task. Here, the explicit means-ends links appeared to have affected participants’ control strategy, which was geared toward taking over control from automation, regardless of the fault condition. This result suggests that in realizing effective human-automation teamwork, finding the right balance between offering more insight (e.g., through ecological interfaces) and striving for compliance with single (machine) advice is an avenue worth exploring further.
Conference Paper
Full-text available
A simulation environment was designed in order to understand the Complexity factors and to evaluate the influence of the time-based operations on Cognitive Complexity. Three different Complexity metrics were implemented in this experiment. Several participants were exposed to designed scenarios in different orders. Each scenario corresponds to a combination of independent variables, namely the Control Type and the Schedule Type. Preliminary empirical findings of the influence of scenario's order are also presented in this paper.
Article
Full-text available
Eye-tracking research in infants and older children has gained a lot of momentum over the last decades. Although eye-tracking research in these participant groups has become easier with the advance of the remote eye-tracker, this often comes at the cost of poorer data quality than in research with well-trained adults (Hessels, Andersson, Hooge, Nyström, & Kemner Infancy, 20, 601-633, 2015; Wass, Forssman, & Leppänen Infancy, 19, 427-460, 2014). Current fixation detection algorithms are not built for data from infants and young children. As a result, some researchers have even turned to hand correction of fixation detections (Saez de Urabain, Johnson, & Smith Behavior Research Methods, 47, 53-72, 2015). Here we introduce a fixation detection algorithm-identification by two-means clustering (I2MC)-built specifically for data across a wide range of noise levels and when periods of data loss may occur. We evaluated the I2MC algorithm against seven state-of-the-art event detection algorithms, and report that the I2MC algorithm's output is the most robust to high noise and data loss levels. The algorithm is automatic, works offline, and is suitable for eye-tracking data recorded with remote or tower-mounted eye-trackers using static stimuli. In addition to application of the I2MC algorithm in eye-tracking research with infants, school children, and certain patient groups, the I2MC algorithm also may be useful when the noise and data loss levels are markedly different between trials, participants, or time points (e.g., longitudinal research).
Article
Full-text available
Cognitive engineering researchers have long studied the complexity and reliability of human–automation interaction. Historically, though, the area of human–automation decision-making compatibility has received less attention. Paradoxically, this could in the future become one of the most critical issues of all, as mismatches between human and automation problem-solving styles could threaten the adoption of automation. This paper presents the concept of strategic conformance as a potential key factor influencing initial acceptance of automation, specifically decision aiding systems capable of guiding decision and action. Here, strategic conformance represents the match in problem-solving style between decision aiding automation and the individual operator. The theoretical foundation builds on the compatibility construct found in technology acceptance theories such as the innovation diffusion and technology acceptance models. The paper concludes with a critical discussion on the limitations and drawbacks of strategic conformance. It is proposed that the construct would be most applicable at the introductory phase of new decision aiding automation, in helping to foster operators’ initial acceptance of such automation.
Article
Full-text available
The introduction of 4-D trajectory-based operations will require the development of new and more advanced “human-centered” decision support tools for future air traffic controllers. One approach to the design of human-centered decision aids is ecological interface design, which focuses on visualizing the boundaries of safe system performance rather than prescribing predetermined strategies or discrete solutions. Previous studies with ecological interfaces in the aviation domain revealed that humans sometimes opted for control actions close to these boundaries, giving rise to a general concern about the robustness of control actions. The goal of this study has been to empirically investigate how effectively an ecological interface for 4-D trajectory management, as developed in a previous study, supports the preservation of airspace robustness. For this purpose, a metric has been developed to evaluate both minimum and average sector-based and control-based robustness. Special attention was paid to quantifying and measuring the effect of expertise level on the robustness of human-generated control actions. Results of a human-in-the-loop experiment indicate that expert participants were most robust in their control actions, as compared with either skilled or novice participants. This result suggests that boundary-seeking control actions with ecological interfaces are mainly dependent on the level of expertise and the control strategies of the end user.
Article
Full-text available
In a series of real time trials, we simulated sophisticated air traffic man· agement conflict resolution automation using unrecognizable replays of controllers' own performance. Using a fairly novel experimental design and a prototype air traffic control interface, we explored with operational controllers the interactive effects of traffic complexity, level of automation, and "strategic conformance" (defined as the match between human and machine solution strategy) on a number of dependent measures. Conformal advisories (exact replays of a given controller's previous solution) were accepted more often, rated higher, and responded to faster than were non· conformal advisories (replays of a colleague's different solution). In the end, one result stood out in particular: roughly 24% of conformal advisories were rejected by controllers. How could it be that controllers, in effect, disagreed with their very own solutions roughly one quarter of the time? The project is currently exploring this and other related issues through extended human-in-the-loop simulations.
Article
Both conflict resolution aid (CRA) and vertical situation display (VSD) systems may contribute to air traffic control (ATC) operations. However, their effectiveness still needs to be examined before being widely adopted in ATC facilities. This study aims to examine empirically the use of CRA and VSD as well as the systems’ interaction in ATC operations. It was found that CRA benefited conflict resolution performance by 13⋅7% and lowered workload by 46⋅4% compared with manually performing the task. The VSD could also reduce the air traffic controllers’ (ATCOs) workload and improve their situation awareness. Ultimately, when the first CRA failure occurred, the situation awareness supported by VSD offset the performance decrements by 30%. The findings from this study demonstrate that integrating VSD with CRA would benefit ATC operations, regardless of the CRA's imperfection.