ArticlePublisher preview available

The effect of audience–character similarity on identification with narrative characters: A meta-analysis

Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract and Figures

Identification, the experience of a psychological merging between the self and a character, is a key mechanism underlying the power of narratives to influence attitudes, beliefs, intentions, and behaviors in story-consistent ways. Similarity between audience members and characters has been intuitively thought to be an antecedent of identification, but experimental studies have yielded inconsistent findings regarding the effectiveness of manipulating similarity on eliciting identification. The current meta-analysis synthesized and quantified the evidence from 39 studies (k = 50, N = 11,077) and investigated several potential causes of heterogeneity at both the narrative and study levels. The data revealed a small but significant and robust overall effect of similarity on identification (g = 0.19, 95% CI [0.10, 0.28], p < .001), with little evidence of publication bias. A notable narrative-level moderator was type of similarity, with manipulations of psychological similarity yielding larger effects than manipulations of objective similarity. In addition, study design emerged as a significant study-level moderator, with the similarity–identification effect being stronger in studies that manipulated similarity using a within-subjects design than those that used a between-subjects design. Insights gained from this meta-analysis can help to address some ill-defined aspects of the similarity–identification hypothesis, contributing to a better understanding of involvement with narrative characters. Practically, the results can inform the design of more effective targeted and tailored narrative messages that are intended to engage and persuade audiences using the tactic of incorporating similar characters.
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
Vol:.(1234567890)
Current Psychology (2024) 43:7026–7043
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-023-04842-4
1 3
The effect ofaudience–character similarity onidentification
withnarrative characters: Ameta‑analysis
KateY.Huang1 · HeleneH.Fung1· PeifengSun2
Accepted: 5 June 2023 / Published online: 29 June 2023
© The Author(s), under exclusive licence to Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2023
Abstract
Identification, the experience of a psychological merging between the self and a character, is a key mechanism underlying
the power of narratives to influence attitudes, beliefs, intentions, and behaviors in story-consistent ways. Similarity between
audience members and characters has been intuitively thought to be an antecedent of identification, but experimental stud-
ies have yielded inconsistent findings regarding the effectiveness of manipulating similarity on eliciting identification. The
current meta-analysis synthesized and quantified the evidence from 39 studies (k = 50, N = 11,077) and investigated several
potential causes of heterogeneity at both the narrative and study levels. The data revealed a small but significant and robust
overall effect of similarity on identification (g = 0.19, 95% CI [0.10, 0.28], p < .001), with little evidence of publication bias.
A notable narrative-level moderator was type of similarity, with manipulations of psychological similarity yielding larger
effects than manipulations of objective similarity. In addition, study design emerged as a significant study-level moderator,
with the similarity–identification effect being stronger in studies that manipulated similarity using a within-subjects design
than those that used a between-subjects design. Insights gained from this meta-analysis can help to address some ill-defined
aspects of the similarity–identification hypothesis, contributing to a better understanding of involvement with narrative
characters. Practically, the results can inform the design of more effective targeted and tailored narrative messages that are
intended to engage and persuade audiences using the tactic of incorporating similar characters.
Keywords Identification· Audience–character similarity· Meta-analysis· Narratives
During narrative reception, audiences may react to char-
acters in various ways. One such psychological response,
identification, involves a merging of self and character, such
that audience members process and experience thoughts and
emotions toward story events as though they have become
that very character in the story world (Cohen, 2001; Cohen
& Klimmt, 2021). Features of the narrative, characters, audi-
ence, and reception context may all influence identification
(for a review, see Cohen & Tal-Or, 2017). The current paper
focuses in particular on the role of audience–character simi-
larity, and aimed to explore the extent and conditions under
which similarity between audiences and characters elicits
identification.
Identication andnarrative persuasion
Narratives, such as those in the form of short stories, nov-
els, movies, or television programs, are ubiquitous and likely
encountered on a daily basis by most individuals. As Lewis
(2011) asserts, “story is central to human understanding—it
makes life livable, because without a story, there is no identity,
no self, no other” (p. 505). A large body of scholarship has
emerged on the topic of narrative persuasion, the idea that nar-
ratives can influence audiences’ attitudes, beliefs, intentions,
and behaviors in story-consistent ways (Braddock & Dillard,
2016). Meta-analytic evidence suggests that audiences’ involve-
ment with characters, especially in the form of identification,
plays a key role in such persuasive effects (r = 0.26; Tukachin-
sky & Tokunaga, 2013). The potency of identification may be
explained by the bonds that audiences form with characters
(Cohen etal., 2018), which foster greater attention and reduce
psychological reactance toward underlying narrative messages
(Bandura, 2001; Moyer-Gusé, 2008). Given the major role that
identification plays in narrative persuasion, research efforts have
* Kate Y. Huang
katehuang@link.cuhk.edu.hk
1 Department ofPsychology, The Chinese University ofHong
Kong, HongKongSAR, China
2 Department ofGeography andResource Management, The
Chinese University ofHong Kong, HongKongSAR, China
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
... The finding that people referred more to learning about products (e.g., "I didn't know about probiotic cleaners" female, [30][31][32][33][34][35][36][37][38][39][40][41][42][43][44][45][46][47][48][49] rather than practices or behaviours implies a higher level of knowledge around the impact that hotter or longer washing has on the body or environment, but lower levels of knowledge concerning the potential to replace harsh chemicals with low-impact and/or home-made cleaning solutions ("I liked the stories that described natural cleaning products-something I hadn't thought of" female, [30][31][32][33][34][35][36][37][38][39][40][41][42][43][44][45][46][47][48][49]. ...
... The finding that people referred more to learning about products (e.g., "I didn't know about probiotic cleaners" female, [30][31][32][33][34][35][36][37][38][39][40][41][42][43][44][45][46][47][48][49] rather than practices or behaviours implies a higher level of knowledge around the impact that hotter or longer washing has on the body or environment, but lower levels of knowledge concerning the potential to replace harsh chemicals with low-impact and/or home-made cleaning solutions ("I liked the stories that described natural cleaning products-something I hadn't thought of" female, [30][31][32][33][34][35][36][37][38][39][40][41][42][43][44][45][46][47][48][49]. ...
... Similarly, the finding that people referred more to being made to think about health impacts versus environmental impacts implies an existing level of knowledge that many of our practices are not good for our natural environment, but a lower level of knowledge that those practices could also be bad for personal health (e.g., "Hadn't considered laundry liquid / shower gel having a negative effect on my body" female, 18-29; "Knowing that stripping good bacteria from our homes does more harm than good and understanding where probiotic cleaners can help too" female, [30][31][32][33][34][35][36][37][38][39][40][41][42][43][44][45][46][47][48][49]. This is indicative of the persistent myth that complete removal of germs from the household is necessary (or even beneficial) for human health [3]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Fact-based information campaigns aimed at encouraging more sustainable behaviour have typically resulted in minor effects that tend not to last. Scholars in the fields of entertainment education have proposed storytelling as an alternative strategy. Most existing studies have focused on health communication, but there is increasing interest in exploring storytelling to promote pro-environmental behaviours. Our focus in this study are behaviours which have both health and environmental implications: personal cleaning, household cleaning, and laundry. In a study using both quantitative and qualitative analyses (from a survey to which 77 individuals responded), we find that messages embedded into a short story significantly changed behavioural intentions in readers across all three domains. Readers reported gaining knowledge from the stories, in terms of the specific products and practices that one could undertake and with respect to the commonly held misconception that aggressive cleaning practices in the home (e.g., high temperatures, strong chemicals) are beneficial to human health. Results have implications for interventions aimed at promoting behaviours that have joint benefits for human and environmental health.
... In the aforementioned meta-analysis, Tukachinsky (2014) found no statistically significant effects of shared demographics on identification, following the analysis of six effects reported in four studies manipulating similarity. A recent meta-analysis by Huang et al. (2023) challenged those findings and based on 50 effects reported in 39 studies, concluded that similarity affects identification. Notably, there were differences between these two meta-analyses. ...
... Notably, there were differences between these two meta-analyses. Huang et al. (2023) included more recent studies and examined the effects of different types of similarities. The authors observed that deep-level similarity pertaining to emotions, attitudes, and personality elicits a higher effect size on identification than demographic similarity. ...
... While similarities between audiences and characters from different parts of the world are possible, they are more likely to stem from deep-level traits rather than just demographics. Still, considerably fewer studies investigated similarity in psychological traits, concerning deep-level shared characteristics than demographic similarity (i.e., 11 samples vs. 37 samples, according to Huang et al., 2023). This numerical discrepancy is understandable since the experimental manipulation of demographic traits is easier. ...
Article
Full-text available
Current understandings of similarity with media characters often focus on visible attributes including gender and race, yet overlook deep-level characteristics such as personality, attitudes, and experiences. In the present research, we address this limitation and develop and validate the Character Recognizability Scale (CRS), which captures different ways in which audiences can recognize themselves in characters. Based on a previous interview study, we formulated 26 scale items. Subsequently, we conducted two studies. In Study 1, we used a sample of 219 university students in the Netherlands to conduct an exploratory factor analysis. We determined the reliability, as well as criterion and convergent validity of the entire scale and the retained factors. In Study 2, we used a sample of 247 respondents in the United States to conduct a confirmatory factor analysis and replicate the results of the reliability and validity analyses. Based on Study 1, we kept 20 items. In both studies, the overall CRS scale as well as its subscales for Personality Recognizability (CRS-p), Attitudinal Recognizability (CRS-a), and Experiential Recognizability (CRS-e) showed a good internal consistency. They also showed criterion validity through an association with perceived similarity. Finally, the CRS and its subscales correlated positively with media engagement and exposure measures, thus demonstrating convergent validity.
... Unexpectedly, we did not find the anticipated effects of narrative voice and group cue. Previous studies suggesting the first person increases identification with the protagonist (for a meta-analysis on health messaging, see Chen & Bell, 2022) and the narrative voice interacts with similarity (see the meta-analysis of Huang et al., 2023) were not validated in our experiment. Furthermore, there was no interaction between framing, similarity (via group cue), and narrative voice; surprisingly, considering that the relation between similarity and increased identification has been largely theorized (Tajfel & Turner, 1979;Turner et al., 1987) and demonstrated (e.g., Igartua & Guerrero-Martín, 2022). ...
... Similar to Experiment 1, testimonies framing immigrants as victims or heroes, as opposed to profiteers, led to stronger identification, more positive outgroup attitudes, and increased intentions to help as hypothesized (e.g., Algoe & Haidt, 2009;Eisenberg & Miller, 1987) and found in previous studies (Igartua & Cachón-Ramón, 2023). Contrary to expectations and existing literature (see the meta-analysis of Huang et al., 2023), however, no significant relationship was found between group cue and narrative voice among Hungarian participants. This (lack of) finding may stem from the heightened sensitivity of the immigration topic in Hungary (Meuleman et al., 2009), potentially overshadowing the influence of certain testimony elements. ...
... Additionally, the point of view or narrative voice employed had no effect on any variable of these experiments. This is surprising due to the accumulated evidence suggesting it is a relevant moderator in narrative persuasion (see Chen & Bell, 2022) and likely to interact with group cue (see Huang et al., 2023). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
While narratives play a ubiquitous role in human life, there has been a recent surge in theoretical and empirical attention dedicated to understanding their impact on varied psychological outcomes. Despite previous attempts to explore the devices that influence narrative effectiveness (e.g., framing, the group cue or narrative voice) and the mechanisms they operate through (e.g., identification or emotional processes), there remains a gap in systematic and simultaneous tests in large samples. In this study, we focus on testimonial narrative messages, employing a pre-registered online experiment in Spain (N = 1502) and replicating it in Hungary (N = 960), examining the impact of narrative frames (immigrant as a profiteer vs victim vs hero), immigrant origin stigma (high vs low), and narrative voice (first- vs third-person) on participants' attitudes and helping intentions towards immigrants. Framing a testimonial in terms of victimization or heroism (compared to profiteering), improved recipients’ attitudes and helping intentions towards immigrants, and produced ripple effects. These effects manifested, primarily, through heightened psychological identification with the protagonist, resulting in increased meaningful affect and cognitive elaboration, alongside decreased counterarguing. These findings offer insights into the dynamics of narrative persuasion models and underscore the significance of testimonial messages in addressing social issues.
... Књижевност, дакле, која је изникла у једном друштвенополитичком контексту, барем делимично под утицајем тог контекста, такође има значајну моћ да обликује вредности, идеологије и погледе на друштвено уређење својих читалаца подстичући их на размишљање о стварном стању друштва и на разматрања алтернатива постојећим системима вредности (Galloway 2009). Кроз слојевите приче, карактере и наративе, писци могу директно или индиректно промовисати одређене идеје које читаоци могу прихватити и инкорпорирати у своје размишљање и понашање (Huang et al. 2024). Овај утицај може бити посебно снажан због емотивне повезаности коју читаоци развијају са ликовима и ситуацијама у књигама. ...
Article
This manuscript inquires into the intricate relationship between Serbian literature and the political environment from 1991 to 2021, highlighting how literature not only reflects societal conditions but actively shapes political and cultural discourse. It examines the dual role of literature as both a product of its time and a powerful tool for influencing collective memory and social values. The analysis begins by referencing Plato's critique of poetry in The Republic, where he perceives literature as a mere imitation of reality, inferior to the world of ideas. However, the manuscript counters this view by emphasizing the significant impact literary works have had throughout history in shaping political ideas, social movements, and the destinies of nations. The manuscript underscores how literature and politics are mutually constitutive, with literature serving as a reflection of the political context in which authors live and create. It argues that through their works, writers provide insights into historical epochs, helping readers understand social changes and processes. Literature does not only mirror the political environment but can also be an active participant in shaping it, functioning as a cultural force that socializes generations of citizens. Several examples from Serbian and world literature illustrate how literary works have influenced political realities. For instance, Stowe's Uncle Tom's Cabin played a crucial role in shaping public opinion against slavery in the United States, while Orwell's 1984 and Huxley's Brave New World have had a profound impact on political thought, warning against the dangers of totalitarianism. In the Serbian context, the works of authors like Dobrica Ćosić and Borislav Pekić are highlighted for their significant influence on the national consciousness and political thought. The manuscript discusses the role of literature in times of political crises and upheavals, noting how Serbian writers have often been politically active, with many participating in political movements and organizations. It also touches upon the ideological conflicts within Serbian literature, where different groups of writers are divided by their views on national identity, traditional values, and the role of history in contemporary society. Moreover, the manuscript explores how literature serves as a space for exploring broader themes, using political context as a backdrop. Writers often use fiction to delve into deeper questions, employing symbolism, metaphors, and layered narratives to express political ideas while leaving room for diverse interpretations. This allows literature to remain a potent force in political socialization, subtly shaping readers' beliefs and attitudes. The manuscript also addresses the dynamic interplay between the socio-political environment and literature, emphasizing how social circumstances influence writers and the themes they explore. During times of significant social change, such as the breakup of Yugoslavia, Serbian literature has reflected and critiqued the evolving political landscape, offering unique narratives that resonate with the collective experiences of the people. In conclusion, the manuscript asserts that literature, born from a specific socio-political context, not only reflects the realities of its time but also has the power to shape values, ideologies, and perceptions of social order. Through its rich and layered storytelling, literature invites readers to critically examine societal norms, history, and ideological constructs, potentially altering their views and contributing to the broader cultural and political discourse.
... udes expressed by the roles they identify with. Identification is an important mechanism for persuasion (L. Huang, 2020;Igartua & Guerrero-Martı´n, 2022). When the audience has an identification with a certain identity, they will accept this identity and accept information through this identity, thus achieving the effect of persuasion (Cohen, 2018;K. Y. Huang et al., 2023). In agricultural product live stream, the host tells the story of farmers, and consumers can imagine themselves as a member of the farmer's group or associate themselves with the story of the farmer's group. When consumers abandon their identity consciousness and experience the world through the host's perspective, they generate host id ...
Article
Full-text available
Plain Language Summary The problem of food waste is getting worse. The emergence of agricultural product live stream has found a new solution to food waste. In China, the development of agricultural product live stream has been rapid and has achieved amazing sales results. The host’s role is very important in the agricultural product live stream, which runs through the whole process of the broadcast and is an important feature of the anchor’s narrative. This study aims to explore the effect of host’s narrative identity on consumers’ purchase intention. This study aims to investigate the relationships between narrative role, empathy, host identification, spatial distance, and purchase intention. This study conducts three experiments with 460 participants to analyze the influencing mechanism of narrative role (in-group vs. out-group) on consumers’ purchase intentions from the perspective of empathy and host identification through three experiments in different spatial distances (distant vs. close). (a) The study found that in the context of agricultural product live stream, hosts taking an in-group character are more likely to trigger consumers’ purchase intentions than hosts taking an out-group character. (b) Empathy and host identification play a mediating role in the impact of the narrative role on consumers’ purchase intentions. (c) The impact of the host’s narrative role on purchase intention does not significantly differ in a spatially distant location. Compared to the host as an out-group character, the host as an in-group character is more likely to promote consumers’ purchase intention when the spatial distance is close. This study explores the boundary effects of the theory of perspective taking, the study found that the persuasive effect of the narrative, based on the theory of perspective taking, is different under the moderating role of spatial distance
Article
Full-text available
Proposes a developmental model of story liking for suspense stories. The model predicts that (a) reader identification increases with greater perceived similarity between character and reader; (b) increased identification leads to greater suspense; (c) liking of outcome is a joint function of character valence (good or bad character) and outcome valence (positive or negative outcome); and (d) overall liking of story increases with greater identification, greater suspense, and greater liking of outcome. The model was tested by having 44 2nd, 64 4th, and 64 6th graders rate suspense stories on 10 affective scales. Results show that similarity to character increased reader identification, and this, in turn, produced more suspense. A strong developmental trend in evaluations of story endings was found: Young Ss preferred positive outcomes regardless of the valence of the character, but older Ss liked positive endings for good characters and negative endings for bad characters. This finding is interpreted as evidence for acquisition of the "just world" belief. Overall story liking was predicted by path analysis of independent contributions of character identification, suspense, and liking of outcome. Results support the proposed model of story appreciation. (29 ref)
Article
Full-text available
Addressing the high prevalence of depression among university students and the need to motivate professional help seeking, we examine whether recovery stories featuring a similar - compared to a dissimilar - person sharing their personal experience of coping with depression will reduce stigmatization (attitudes, social distance, prosocial intentions) and the self-stigma of help seeking as mediated by perceived similarity and identification. We conducted a one-factorial between-subject experiment to test the effects of lifestyle similarity on stigmatization and self-stigma of help seeking. 169 students (74.6% female; Mage = 23.5, SD = 3.5) were randomly assigned to read either a recovery story about a similar or dissimilar author’s reflections on their experience of coping with depression or a control story. Contrary to expectations, recovery stories did not reduce stigmatization nor the self-stigma of help seeking, irrespective of whether they were told by a similar or dissimilar person. However, a significant indirect effect on prosocial intentions through perceived similarity and identification as well as several interesting exploratory results point to potential beneficial and detrimental effects of recovery stories and similarity that should be explored in future research.
Article
Full-text available
One possible way of enhancing the effectiveness of health narratives is by using tailoring. However, evidence of the effectiveness in narrative tailoring is mixed. Some studies have found tailoring to be effective, while others have found no difference between tailored and non-tailored stories. One explanation for these mixed results is that much of the previous research in this area has focused on purely demographic factors. This study aimed to determine whether or not adding theoretically derived tailoring dimensions provides benefits above and beyond demographic tailoring. Participants (N = 812, aged 18-26) were assigned to either a facts only control condition, a non-tailored narrative, a demographically tailored narrative, or a demographically and theoretically tailored narrative. Across all conditions, the stimuli focused on the benefits of the HPV vaccine. Results found that the narrative conditions outperformed the control, but there was no significant difference between tailoring conditions on vaccination expectations, narrative transportation, identification, or perceived personalization. Further analysis showed that perceived personalization and narrative transportation predicted vaccination expectations across all narrative conditions.
Article
Full-text available
Previous studies have shown that identification and self-referencing can both function as mechanisms of narrative persuasion. However, it is not yet clear whether they are compatible and can work together in bringing about persuasive effects of narratives, or not. Therefore, this study examines both identification and self-referencing and studies their relation and effects. A 2x2 between-subjects experiment was conducted among 185 student participants, with the factors ‘perspective’ (1st vs. 3rd person) to influence identification and ‘similarity’ (young student protagonist vs. older working protagonist) to influence self-referencing. Results showed that identification mediated indirect effects of perspective on story-consistent belief and self-referencing mediated indirect effects of similarity on story-consistent belief. Moreover, identification and self-referencing serially mediated indirect effects on story-consistent belief. These results indicate that identification and self-referencing are compatible processes which can work together as mechanisms of narrative persuasion.
Article
Full-text available
The question of what contributes to users’ identification with media characters remains an open issue in research. Apart from media interactivity, user characteristics like gender, age, immersive tendencies, and factual transportation into the narrative are promising factors. Yet, research is still in its infancy, and the usage of different media limits cross-study comparability. Hence, the current study set out to examine predictors of character identification using a text-based fiction with purported interactivity which was inspired by interactive fiction (IF) games. In an online experiment, 228 participants aged 15–65 years were randomly assigned to either an active condition where they could choose from different options, or a passive condition where they only read the story. Additionally, participants filled out questionnaires assessing immersive tendencies, level of identification, and narrative transportation. A multiple linear regression model tested for predictors of character identification. Apart from age and gender which remained non-significant, interactivity, immersive tendencies, and transportation into narrative significantly predicted identification with the IF’s main character. The current findings support theoretical models on media interactivity and identification, yet several open issues such as the role of media content (engaging vs. mundane) and character features (e.g., similarity with user) remain to be answered.
Article
Full-text available
The design of campaigns for the improvement of intergroup attitudes requires innovative approaches that consider both the characteristics of the messages and the psychological processes they evoke. This work addresses the study of factors that increase the persuasive effectiveness of testimonial messages aimed at improving attitudes towards stigmatized immigrants. An experiment was conducted using a representative sample of 443 participants of Spanish origin on the effect of similarity to the protagonist and the narrative voice. Two mediating mechanisms (identification with the protagonist and cognitive elaboration) were evaluated, and the indirect effect of the two independent variables was studied with respect to two dependent variables: the attitude towards immigration and the intention to collaborate with NGOs to support immigrants. Similarity to the protagonist of the narrative message increased identification only when the participants read the version written in the first person. In addition, a conditional process model was tested, revealing that identification increased cognitive elaboration, which, in turn, was associated with a more favorable attitude towards immigration and a greater intention to collaborate with immigrant support organizations. This study highlights the relevance of the characteristics of narrative messages to increase affective (identification) and cognitive (elaboration) processes that explain their persuasive impact. The results are discussed in the context of research on narrative persuasion and the design of campaigns for the prevention of racism and xenophobia.
Article
Full-text available
One of the most used methods to examine sources of heterogeneity in meta-analyses is the so-called ‘subgroup analysis’. In a subgroup analysis, the included studies are divided into two or more subgroups, and it is tested whether the pooled effect sizes found in these subgroups differ significantly from each other. Subgroup analyses can be considered as a core component of most published meta-analyses. One important problem of subgroup analyses is the lack of statistical power to find significant differences between subgroups. In this paper, we explore the power problems of subgroup analyses in more detail, using ‘metapower’, a recently developed statistical package in R to examine power in meta-analyses, including subgroup analyses. We show that subgroup analyses require many more included studies in a meta-analysis than are needed for the main analyses. We work out an example of an ‘average’ meta-analysis, in which a subgroup analysis requires 3–4 times the number of studies that are needed for the main analysis to have sufficient power. This number of studies increases exponentially with decreasing effect sizes and when the studies are not evenly divided over the subgroups. Higher heterogeneity also requires increasing numbers of studies. We conclude that subgroup analyses remain an important method to examine potential sources of heterogeneity in meta-analyses, but that meta-analysts should keep in mind that power is very low for most subgroup analyses. As in any statistical evaluation, researchers should not rely on a test and p -value to interpret results, but should compare the confidence intervals and interpret results carefully.
Chapter
This handbook provides a strong collection of communication- and psychology-based theories and models on media entertainment, which can be used as a knowledge resource for any academic and applied purpose. Its 41 chapters offer explanations of entertainment that audiences find in any kind of ‘old’ and ‘new’ media, from classic novels to VR video games, from fictional stories to mediated sports. As becomes clear in this handbook, the history of entertainment research teaches us not to forget that even if a field is converging to a seemingly dominant perspective, paradigm, and methodology, there are more views, alternative approaches, and different yet equally illuminative ways of thinking about the field. Young scholars may find here innovative ways to reconcile empirical-theoretical approaches to the experience of entertainment with such alternative views. And there are numerous entertainment-related phenomena in contemporary societies that still fit the „bread and circuses-“ perspective of the initial Frankfurt School thinking. So while the mission of the present handbook is to compile and advance current theories about media entertainment, scholars active or interested in the topic are invited to also consider the historic roots of the field and the great diversity it has featured over the past nearly 100 years. Many lessons can be learned from this history, and future innovations in entertainment theory may just as likely emerge from refining those approaches compiled in the present handbook as from building on neglected, forgotten, or marginalized streams of scholarship.