Access to this full-text is provided by Frontiers.
Content available from Frontiers in Public Health
This content is subject to copyright.
Frontiers in Public Health 01 frontiersin.org
Protecting public’s wellbeing
against COVID-19 infodemic: The
role of trust in information sources
and rapid dissemination and
transparency of information over
time
YingnanZhou
1,2, 3, AirongZhang
2, XiaoliuLiu
6, XuyunTan
4,
RuikaiMiao
7, YanZhang
3,4 and JunxiuWang
3,4, 5*
1 School of Sociology and Ethnology, University of Chinese Academy of Social Sciences, Beijing, China,
2 Health and Biosecurity, CSIRO, Brisbane, QLD, Australia, 3 School of Mental Health, Wenzhou Medical
University, Wenzhou, China, 4 Institute of Sociology, Chinese Academy of Social Sciences, Beijing,
China, 5 School of Psychology, Inner Mongolia Normal University, Hohhot, China, 6 Faculty of Ideological
and Political Education and Moral Education, Beijing Institute of Education, Beijing, China, 7 Mental
Health Education Center, Shijiazhuang Tiedao University, Shijiazhuang, China
Objectives: This study examined how trust in the information about COVID-19
from social media and ocial media as well as how the information was
disseminated aect public’s wellbeing directly and indirectly through perceived
safety over time.
Methods: Two online surveys were conducted in China, with the first survey
(Time1, N = 22,718) being at the early stage of the pandemic outbreak and the
second one (Time 2, N = 2,901) two and a half years later during the zero-COVID
policy lockdown period. Key measured variables include trust in ocial media
and social media, perceived rapid dissemination and transparency of COVID-
19-related information, perceived safety, and emotional responses toward the
pandemic. Data analysis includes descriptive statistical analysis, independent
samples t-test, Pearson correlations, and structural equation modeling.
Results: Trust in ocial media, perceived rapid dissemination and transparency
of COVID-19-related information, perceived safety, as well as positive emotional
response toward COVID-19 increased over time, while trust in social media and
depressive response decreased over time. Trust in social media and ocial media
played dierent roles in aecting public’s wellbeing over time. Trust in social media
was positively associated with depressive emotions and negatively associated
with positive emotion directly and indirectly through decreased perceived
safety at Time 1. However, the negative eect of trust in social media on public’s
wellbeing was largely decreased at Time 2. In contrast, trust in ocial media was
linked to reduced depressive response and increased positive response directly
and indirectly through perceived safety at both times. Rapid dissemination and
transparency of COVID-19 information contributed to enhanced trust in ocial
media at both times.
Conclusion: The findings highlight the important role of fostering public trust
in ocial media through rapid dissemination and transparency of information in
mitigating the negative impact of COVID-19 infodemic on public’s wellbeing over
time.
OPEN ACCESS
EDITED BY
Xue Yang,
The Chinese University of Hong Kong, China
REVIEWED BY
David Conversi,
Sapienza University of Rome, Italy
Gour Gobinda Goswami,
North South University, Bangladesh
Hui Zhang,
Nanjing Normal University, China
*CORRESPONDENCE
Junxiu Wang
casswjx@163.com
SPECIALTY SECTION
This article was submitted to
Public Mental Health,
a section of the journal
Frontiers in Public Health
RECEIVED 11 January 2023
ACCEPTED 27 March 2023
PUBLISHED 17 April 2023
CITATION
Zhou Y, Zhang A, Liu X, Tan X, Miao R,
Zhang Y and Wang J (2023) Protecting public’s
wellbeing against COVID-19 infodemic: The
role of trust in information sources and rapid
dissemination and transparency of information
over time.
Front. Public Health 11:1142230.
doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2023.1142230
COPYRIGHT
© 2023 Zhou, Zhang, Liu, Tan, Miao, Zhang and
Wang. This is an open-access article distributed
under the terms of the Creative Commons
Attribution License (CC BY). The use,
distribution or reproduction in other forums is
permitted, provided the original author(s) and
the copyright owner(s) are credited and that
the original publication in this journal is cited,
in accordance with accepted academic
practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is
permitted which does not comply with these
terms.
TYPE Original Research
PUBLISHED 17 April 2023
DOI 10.3389/fpubh.2023.1142230
Zhou et al. 10.3389/fpubh.2023.1142230
Frontiers in Public Health 02 frontiersin.org
KEYWORDS
psychological stress, trust, media sources, information dissemination, perceived safety,
wellbeing, information transparency
1. Introduction
COVID-19 has been constantly evolving since its outbreak in
early 2020. At the beginning, there was limited scientic understanding
and knowledge about the coronavirus. Due to the unknown nature of
the novel virus, misinformation and rumors were widely spread across
social media platforms, which instilled a strong sense of out-controlled
crisis (1–6). Over 2 years into the pandemic, scientic understanding
of COVID-19 has been advanced, and vaccines have been developed.
Protective measures such as wearing mask, sanitizing hands, and
keeping social distance have been commonly adopted in daily life,
which is regarded as a “new normal.” While the virus has been
constantly mutating, so were rumors and misinformation, especially
regarding the COVID-19 vaccines. For example, exaggeration of side
eects (e.g., infertility, chronic illness, mental illness) as well as distrust
in vaccine development (e.g., crucial trials skipped) were widespread
on social media, leading to vaccine hesitancy (7–13). Meanwhile, the
preventive measures and COVID-19-related policies taken by
governments were also changing over time and dierent from country
to country. While most of countries have reopened by early to
mid-2022, strict lockdown and COVID-zero policy were still in place
in China. Such misinformation and dierences in government policies
have kept sending confusing message to the public. is situation
highlights the remarkable characteristics of the concurrence of
virology and virality of COVID-19, where fast virus spreading is
coupled with rapidly spreading of information and misinformation
(14). Precisely as WHO Director-General Dr. Ghebreyesus pointed
out, “We’re not just ghting an epidemic; we are ghting an
infodemic” (15).
Extensive empirical studies from dierent countries have
demonstrated that a broad range of rumors and misinformation about
COVID-19 spread across social media, which negatively impacted
public’s wellbeing and posited challenge for pandemic control (1–7).
Research has shown that trust in COVID-19 information from social
media was negatively linked to accurate knowledge about COVID-19
(16), positively linked to beliefs in COVID-19 myths and false
information (17) as well as vaccine hesitancy (5, 18, 19). Moreover,
rumors and misinformation fueled fears and led to psychological
distress among the public over the course of COVID-19 pandemic
(20–29). Frequently using social media as an information source for
COVID-19 was signicantly related to poorer psychological wellbeing
(28, 30–32). Moreover, erroneous, inconsistent, unveried, and oen
conicting news and messages led to uncertainty, which caused
intense stress to the public (33). Emerging research indicates that
perceived vulnerability to COVID-19 mediated the relationship
between exposure to COVID-19 news and depressive symptoms (34).
In addition, when people used social media to obtain COVID-19-
related information, their perceived risk of being infected heightened
as the level of concern increased (35). In turn, higher risk perception
and lack of perceived safety toward COVID-19 led to increased
anxiety and depressive symptoms (36–39). ose ndings suggest that
the conicting information and uncertainty on social media made
people feel unsafe as it is not clear how to protect oneself. is led to
fear and stress, and hence, impacted wellbeing. However, how trust in
social media aect public’s wellbeing during COVID-19 pandemic,
and the mediating role of perceived safety are not yet directly
examined. Informed by the research reviewed above,
wehypothesized that:
H1: Trust in COVID-19-related information from social media
was negatively associated with positive emotional response and
positively associated with depressive emotional response toward
COVID-19.
H2: Perceived safety mediates the relationship between trust in
social media with positive and negative responses toward COVID-
19, respectively.
To minimize public fear and confusion caused by social media,
transparency and rapid dissemination of information by government
agencies has been suggested crucial (40–42). e role of transparency
and trust was also demonstrated in managing public fear and panic in
SARS outbreak in Singapore (43) as well as during other outbreaks
including Ebola in West Africa and MERS-CoV in South Korea (44).
Indeed, timely, accurate and transparent information from ocials is
foundational for the public to implement protective measures,
mitigate the negative impact of the pandemic, and to reduce
psychological distress in the crisis (45, 46). e satisfaction with
governments’ communication about COVID-19 was linked to public
trust in government (47, 48). ese ndings suggest that transparency
and rapid dissemination of information about COVID-19 is the key
factors to build public trust in ocial media. erefore,
wehypothesized:
H3: Perceived rapid dissemination and transparency of the
information about COVID-19 are positively related to trust in
ocial media.
With respect to how trust in ocial media would aect public’s
wellbeing, existing literature pointed to dierent directions. Some
studies indicated that ocial media in some countries applied a fear-
based communication strategy (e.g., showing realistic pictures and
giving direct information on COVID-19 death statistics) and
suppressed scientic debate to persuade public to adhere to
recommended health behaviors such as wearing mask, practicing social
distance, and getting vaccinations (49–52). Such fear-inducing
approach can increase levels of perceived threat, cause psychological
distress, and aect wellbeing among the public (51, 53–55). In this case,
trust in ocial media would negatively aect public’s wellbeing through
decreasing perceived safety from being infected. Meanwhile, other
studies suggested the opposite. ese studies found trust in the
Zhou et al. 10.3389/fpubh.2023.1142230
Frontiers in Public Health 03 frontiersin.org
government and obtaining information from ocial media reduced
perceived risk toward COVID-19, mitigated mental distress, and
improved psychological wellbeing among the public (35, 56–58). ese
ndings suggested that receiving information from trusted and
authoritative source would give people certainty and ecacy, hence,
increasing perceived safety and enhancing mental wellbeing. In
summary, the research ndings reviewed above indicate that trust in
COVID-19-related information from ocial media could either
positively, or negatively aect public’s wellbeing and that perceived
safety might play a mediating role. Hence, weproposed that trust in
ocial media was signicantly related to public’s wellbeing both directly
and indirectly through perceived safety (Hypotheses 4 and 5), but
wele the direction of the relationships (i.e., positive or negative) open.
H4: Trust in COVID-19-related information from ocial media was
signicantly (either positively or negatively) associated with positive
and depressive emotional responses toward COVID-19 respectively.
H5: Perceived safety mediates the relationship between trust in
ocial media with positive and negative responses toward
COVID-19 respectively.
The present study
e present study aimed to investigate how trust in the information
about COVID-19 from ocial media and social media aect public’s
wellbeing (i.e., positive response and depressive response) through
perceived safety, and how the dissemination of information impact
public trust in ocial media both at the early stage of COVID-19
outbreak and 2 years later in China. To our best knowledge, this is the
rst study to examine the impacts of trust in media sources on public’s
wellbeing toward COVID-19 over time. e insights developed through
this study will help policy makers and health intervention initiatives
develop targeted strategies to address the mental health challenges
presented by the COVID-19 pandemic and protect public’s wellbeing.
Figure1 presents a path model which summarizes the hypotheses
proposed above. In this model, wepropose that trust in COVID-19
information received from social media was negatively associated with
positive emotional response and positively associated with depressive
emotional response toward COVID-19 both directly and indirectly
through decreased perceived safety (H1-H2); that perceived
transparency and rapid dissemination of COVID-19-related
information are positively related to trust in ocial media (H3). In
turn, trust in ocial media was either positively or negatively
associated with positive and depressive response toward COVID-19
both directly and indirectly through perceived safety (H4, H5).
ough the scientic understanding of COVID-19 has been
advanced over 2 years into the pandemic, the “infodemic” wasn’t over.
Rumors and misinformation about the virus and vaccine were still
widespread across social media (8). In addition, the mental health
symptoms were still quite prevalent among public in the “new normal”
era (59). erefore, it’s important to examine the mechanism of trust
in media sources aect public’s wellbeing over time. e path
framework weproposed allows the examination of how the key factors
aect public’s wellbeing both at the early stage of COVID-19 outbreak
and post COVID-19 era and also allows to make comparisons of the
changes in eects. e developed insights on what has changed over
time will inform policy makers to adjust the risk communication
strategies accordingly.
2. Materials and methods
2.1. Procedure and participants
National online surveys in China were conducted at the early
stage of COVID-19 outbreak and 2 years later. Time 1 survey was
FIGURE1
An integrative model to predict emotional responses toward the COVID-19.
Zhou et al. 10.3389/fpubh.2023.1142230
Frontiers in Public Health 04 frontiersin.org
carried out between 24-Jan to 10-Feb, 2020, which was right aer
China’s ocial announcement of COVID-19 outbreak (on January 20,
2020) and deployed lockdown measures (on January 23, 2020). Time
2 survey was conducted between 21-Apr to 4-May, 2022, when Delta
and Omicron variants were widely spread around the world and
COVID-zero policy was still in place in China (60). e study was
conducted in compliance with the ethical standards specied in the
Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct by the
American Psychological Association (61) and in 1964 Helsinki
declaration and its later amendments (62). Two private research
survey companies (Intell-vision for Time 1, ePanel for Time 2) were
engaged to recruit participants and conduct data collection through
convenance sampling. e survey link was sent to users of the online
survey platforms of the two companies. Aer presenting a brief
description of the study, participants were informed that no personal
identiable information would be collected and that their survey
results would remain condential. Participants were further informed
that their participation was voluntary and that they could withdraw
from the survey at any time without penalty. Participants were asked
to click ‘I agree’ button if they consent to participate in the survey.
Participants who completed the survey were paid a small fee for their
participation. e collected data was completely anonymous, and the
research team was the only party has access to the data.
Table1 presents participants’ demographic information for both
Time 1 and Time 2.
2.2. Measures
2.2.1. Trust in ocial media
At the early stage of COVID-19 outbreak, the ocial news
reached the public largely through television news and it was also
available online in China. e TV news report is in the format of
news from central government rst and followed by news from local
government. Hence, at Time 1, trust in ocial media was measured
by asking participants to indicate how trustworthy the information
on the Coronavirus outbreak from central government-owned media
and local government-owned media, respectively, on a 4-point scale
(1 = not trustworthy at all, 4 = very trustworthy; α = 0.75). While
2 years later, community social workers also became important
information sources. ey conveyed ocial information on
COVID-19 to the public and implemented preventive and control
measures at community level. erefore, at Time 2, trust in ocial
media was measured by asking participants to indicate how
trustworthy the information on COVID-19 from central government-
owned media, local government-owned media, and community
social workers, respectively, on a 5-point scale (1 = not trustworthy at
all, 5 = very trustworthy; α = 0.75). To compare the change between
Time 1 and Time 2, the score of trust in ocial media at Time 1 was
transformed to a 5-point scale by using the following formula (63, 64):
XX
143
13
=
()
−
()
∗
//
Here:
X1: Transformed score of trust in ocial media (on a 5-point scale).
X: the original score of trust in ocial media (on a 4-point scale).
2.2.2. Trust in social media
In the beginning of COVID-19 outbreak, Weibo and WeChat were
the most popular social media platforms in China for the spread of
information about COVID-19. Besides, acquaintances were also
important information sources during the pandemic. Hence, at Time
1, trust in social media was measured by asking participants to indicate
how trustworthy the information on the Coronavirus outbreak from
Weibo inuencers, WeChat inuencers, and acquaintances,
respectively, on a 4-point scale (1 = not trustworthy at all, 4 = very
trustworthy; α = 0.77). As time passed by, the general netizens became
more and more important in information transmission. erefore, at
Time 2, trust in social media was measured by asking participants to
indicate how trustworthy the information on COVID-19 from internet
inuencers, general netizens, and acquaintances, respectively, on a
5-point scale (1 = not trustworthy at all, 5 = very trustworthy; α = 0.68).
e Cronbach’s alpha for trust in social media at Time 2 is a bit lower
than the widely considered desirable value of 0.70 (65, 66). However, a
low number of items could lead to a low value of Cronbach’s alpha (65).
Since there were only 3 items in this scale, an alpha value of 0.68 is
acceptable (67, 68). To examine the dierence between Time 1 and
Time 2, the score of trust in social media at Time 1 was also transformed
to a 5-point scale by using the formula described above (63, 64).
2.2.3. Rapid dissemination, transparency, and
perceived safety
Rapid dissemination was measured with: “So far, do youthink the
dissemination of information about Coronavirus is rapid?” (1 = very
delayed, 4 = very rapid). Transparency was measured with: “So far,
how transparent do youthink the information on the Coronavirus
outbreak is?” (1 = very low, 4 = very high). Perceived safety was
measured with: “inking about Coronavirus, how safe do youfeel
from being infected?” (1 = not safe at all, 4 = very safe).
2.2.4. Emotional responses
e measurement of emotional responses toward COVID-19
outbreak was adapted from the Florida Shock Anxiety Scale (69, 70).
TABLE1 The sample characteristics.
Variables Values
Time 1
(N = 22,702)
Time 2
(N = 2,901)
Age (years) 28.41 (SD = 9.90/
Range = 18–70)
31.77 (SD = 8.05/
Range = 18–69)
Gender
Male 10,866 (47.9%) 1,274 (43.9%)
Female 11,836 (52.1%) 1,627 (56.1%)
Education
Junior high school and
below (Year 9 or below) 796 (3.5%) 16 (0.6%)
Senior high school
(Year 12) 3,287 (14.5%) 137 (4.7%)
College certicate 3,514 (15.5%) 416 (14.3%)
Bachelor’s degree 10,952 (48.2%) 2,115 (72.9%)
Postgraduate 4,153 (18.3%) 217 (7.5%)
Zhou et al. 10.3389/fpubh.2023.1142230
Frontiers in Public Health 05 frontiersin.org
e Florida Shock Anxiety Scale (FSAS) was developed to measure
patients’ psychological distress caused by the threat and fear of
potential implantable cardioverter debrillators (ICD) shock. e
COVID-19 pandemic has instilled people with a sense of fear of being
infected with the virus. e potential infection may happen but is not
certain, which makes people feel worried, scared, and angry. is
psychological distress is very similar to that elicited from the
anticipation of experiencing ICD shock. Hence, weadapted this scale
to measure the emotional responses toward COVID-19. Participants
were asked to rate their feelings toward COVID-19 outbreak using a
5-point scale (1 = not at all, 5 = very much) on the adjectives describing
positive response (optimistic) and depressive response (worried,
scared, sad, and angry; α = 0.80 at Time 1, α = 0.81 at Time 2).
2.3. Data analysis
SPSS version 22.0 with AMOS version 24.0 was used for the data
analysis. Descriptive statistical analysis, independent samples t-test, and
Pearson correlations were conducted rst. To examine the hypothesized
model (Figure1), A two-stage structural equation modeling approach
was conducted (71–77). e analyses for the model at both Time 1 and
Time 2 utilized a covariance matrix as input and used maximum
likelihood estimation. e goodness of t of the model was assessed
using the comparative t index (CFI), the Non-Normed Fit Index
(NNFI), Goodness-of-t statistic (GFI), and root mean square error of
approximation (RMSEA). A satisfactory t is suggested by CFI > 0.90,
NNFI > 0.90, GFI > 0.90, and Standardized RMSEA < 0.08 (72).
3. Results
3.1. Changes in measured variables over
time
Table2 presents the means and standard deviations of measured
variables at both survey times and independent samples t-test results
between the two time points. On average, participants displayed sound
trust in ocial media both at Time 1 (M = 3.94, SD = 0.84) and Time 2
(M = 4.17, SD = 0.67), which were signicantly higher than trust in social
media at both times (Time 1, M = 3.07, SD = 0.86, Time 2, M = 3.04,
SD = 0.67); t (22701) = 131.58, p < 0.001 and t (2900) = 72.87, p < 0.001,
respectively. Moreover, trust in ocial media at Time 2 was signicantly
higher than Time 1 [t (4161.46) = −17.18, p < 0.001], while trust in social
media at Time 2 was signicantly lower than Time 1 [t (4225.96) = 2.50,
p < 0.05]. e results indicated that trust in ocial media largely
increased over time, while trust in social media decreased over time.
e dissemination of information about the Coronavirus was
regarded on average less rapid (M = 2.75, SD = 0.87) and transparent
(M = 2.75, SD = 0.78) at Time 1. However, both measures were
signicantly improved at Time 2 (rapid dissemination: M = 3.19,
SD = 0.64, transparency: M = 3.08, SD = 0.71); Rapid dissemination: t
(4394.35) = −33.40, p < 0.001; Transparency: t (3866.79) = −23.31,
p < 0.001. Perceived safety from being infected with the Coronavirus
also enhanced from Time 1(M = 2.80, SD = 0.68) to Time 2 (M = 2.89,
SD = 0.65), t (3760.10) = −7.22, p < 0.001. At last, positive emotional
response toward COVID-19 increased over time (Time 1, M = 3.07,
SD = 1.27, Time 2, M = 3.33, SD = 0.94); t (4405.12) = −13.41, p < 0.001,
while depressive response decreased over time (Time 1, M = 3.22,
SD = 1.00, Time 2, M = 3.09, SD = 0.87); t (3951.96) = 7.41, p < 0.001.
Table 3 presents Pearson correlations between the measured
variables at both survey times. Positive response was positively related
to trust in ocial media and social media as well as rapid
dissemination, transparency, and perceived safety both at Time 1 and
Time 2, while depressive response was negatively associated with these
variables (except for trust in social media at Time 1, which was not
signicantly correlated to depressive response). In addition, trust in
ocial media and social media, rapid dissemination, transparency, and
perceived safety were positively correlated to each other at both survey
times (except for trust in social media and perceived safety at Time 2,
which was not signicantly correlated). Finally, positive response and
depressive response was negatively related at both survey times.
3.2. The relationship among information
dissemination, trust in media sources,
perceived safety, and emotional responses
over time
A two-stage structural equation modeling approach was
conducted (71–77) to examine the hypothesized model. In this
approach, the measurement model, which species the relationships
TABLE2 Descriptive statistics and independent samples t-test results for measured variables.
M (SD) tdf Cohen’ d
Time 1 (N = 22,702) Time 2 (N = 2,901)
Trust in ocial media 3.94 (0.84) 4.17 (0.67) −17.18*** 4161.46 −0.28
Trust in social media 3.07 (0.86) 3.04 (0.67) 2.50*4225.96 0.04
Rapid dissemination 2.75 (0.87) 3.19 (0.64) −33.40*** 4394.35 −0.52
Transparency 2.75 (0.78) 3.08 (0.71) −23.31*** 3866.79 −0.43
Perceived safety 2.80 (0.68) 2.89 (0.65) −7.22*** 3760.10 −0.13
Positive response 3.07 (1.27) 3.33 (0.94) −13.41*** 4405.12 −0.21
Depressive response 3.22 (1.00) 3.09 (0.87) 7.41*** 3951.96 0.13
***p < 0.001, *p < 0.05. Trust in ocial media and trust in social media were measured on a 5-point scale (1 = not trustworthy at all, 5 = very trustworthy). Rapid dissemination was measured
on a 4-point scale (1 = very delayed, 4 = very rapid). Transparency was measured on a 4-point scale (1 = ver y low, 4 = very high). Perceived safety was measured on a 4-point scale (1 = not safe at
all, 4 = ver y safe). Positive response and depressive response were measured on a 5-point scale (1 = not at all, 5 = very much).
Zhou et al. 10.3389/fpubh.2023.1142230
Frontiers in Public Health 06 frontiersin.org
between the latent constructs and the observed measures, was tested
rst via conrmatory factor analysis (CFA); followed by the
structural model, which species the relationships among
independent, dependent, and mediating variables. In addition, the
bias-corrected bootstrap method was carried out to test the indirect
eects. 5,000 bootstrapped samples were generated to approximate
the condence interval (CI) of the indirect eects both at Time 1 and
Time 2. A 95% CI without zero indicates statistical signicance.
Furthermore, following the practice of previous studies (78, 79), the
structural model was tested for robustness by changing the
sample range.
3.2.1. Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) for the
measurement model
Conrmatory factor analysis (CFA) was conducted to examine the
measurement model both at Time 1 and Time 2. e measurement
model was supported by the model t indexes at both survey times:
Time 1, CFI = 0.97, NNFI = 0.96, GFI = 0.98, and RMSEA = 0.06; Time
2, CFI = 0.93, NNFI = 0.90, GFI = 0.96, and RMSEA = 0.08.
Furthermore, the convergent and discriminant validity of the
measurement model were assessed at both times. e convergent
validity was evaluated by using standardized factor loadings,
composite reliability (CR) and the average variance extracted (AVE)
(see Table 4). All items loaded signicantly on their respective
constructs, with the standardized factor loadings ranging from 0.50 to
0.85, reaching the criterion of 0.50 or above (74). e CR values
ranged from 0.68 to 0.81, meeting an acceptable criterion of 0.60 (74).
e AVE values ranged from 0.42 to 0.68, reaching the criterion of
0.36 or above (77). ese results provided evidence of satisfactory
convergent validity. Discriminant validity was assessed by comparing
AVE with the squared correlation between constructs. e squared
correlations between constructs at both times ranged from 0.00 to
0.20, which were all much lower than AVE values, indicating that the
measurement model has satisfactory discriminant validity (73–75).
ese results suggested that the measurement model is of
sucient quality to examine the structural model.
3.2.2. Pathway analysis for the structural model
Our hypothesized model (Figure1) specied rapid dissemination
and transparency of information as exogenous predictors of trust in
ocial media, both trust in ocial media and social media as
exogenous predictors of perceived safety. Perceived safety, in turn, was
identied as a predictor of positive response and depressive response.
Moreover, trust in ocial media and trust in social media also served
as exogenous predictors of positive response and depressive response.
In this model, trust in ocial media, trust in social media, and
depressive response were latent variables presented using ellipses,
while rapid dissemination, transparency, positive response (optimistic)
and perceived safety were observed variables presented
using rectangles.
e model t indices suggest that the model provided good t for
the data at both times: Time 1, CFI = 0.94, NNFI = 0.92, GFI = 0.96,
and RMSEA = 0.07; Time 2, CFI = 0.92, NNFI = 0.90, GFI = 0.95, and
RMSEA = 0.07.
Figure2 presents the standardized parameter estimates for the
model at both Time 1 (T1) and Time 2 (T2). Table 5 presents the
direct, indirect, and total eects of trust in media sources on public’s
wellbeing at both Time 1 (T1) and Time 2 (T2).
First, trust in social media was negatively related to positive
response at Time 1 (β = −0.16, p < 0.001) and positively associated with
depressive response both at Time 1(β = 0.30, p < 0.001) and Time 2
(β = 0.08, p < 0.001), such that the more people trusted the information
about the Coronavirus received in social media, the less they felt
optimistic and the more they felt depressive toward the pandemic,
especially in the beginning of COVID-19 outbreak. Since trust in social
media was no longer signicantly related to positive response at Time 2
(β = 0.01, p = 0.684), Hypothesis 1 was fully supported at Time 1 and was
partially supported at Time 2. Moreover, Trust in social media was
negatively associated with perceived safety at Time 1 (β = −0.10,
p < 0.001), but not signicantly associated with perceived safety at Time
2 (β = −0.04, p = 0.096). In turn, perceived safety was positively related
to positive response (Time 1, β = 0.11, p < 0.001; Time 2, β = 0.16,
p < 0.001) and negatively linked to depressive response (Time 1,
TABLE3 Pearson correlations between the measured variables at Time 1 and Time 2.
Variables 1 2 3 4 5 6
Time 1 Time 2 Time 1 Time 2 Time 1 Time 2 Time 1 Time 2 Time 1 Time 2 Time 1 Time 2
1. Trust in ocial
media 1.00 1.00
2. Trust in social
media 0.33*** 0.22*** 1.00 1.00
3. Rapid
dissemination 0.46*** 0.48*** 0.26*** 0.12*** 1.00 1.00
4. Transparency 0.49*** 0.52*** 0.28*** 0.16*** 0.69*** 0.68*** 1.00 1.00
5. Perceived
safety 0.31*** 0.14*** 0.22*** 0.03 0.35*** 0.14*** 0.37*** 0.16*** 1.00 1.00
6. Positive
response 0.28*** 0.26*** 0.18*** 0.12*** 0.36*** 0.22*** 0.36*** 0.27*** 0.31*** 0.23*** 1.00 1.00
7. Depressive
response
−0.18*** −0.19*** 0.01 −0.04* −0.24*** −0.21*** −0.23*** −0.24*** −0.28*** −0.25*** −0.20*** −0.38***
***p < 0.001, *p < 0.05. Trust in ocial media and trust in social media were measured on a 5-point scale (1 = not trustworthy at all, 5 = very trustworthy). Rapid dissemination was measured
on a 4-point scale (1 = very delayed, 4 = very rapid). Transparency was measured on a 4-point scale (1 = ver y low, 4 = very high). Perceived safety was measured on a 4-point scale (1 = not safe at
all, 4 = ver y safe). Positive response and depressive response were measured on a 5-point scale (1 = not at all, 5 = very much).
Zhou et al. 10.3389/fpubh.2023.1142230
Frontiers in Public Health 07 frontiersin.org
β = −0.20, p < 0.001; Time 2, β = −0.21, p < 0.001) at both survey times,
suggesting that the safer people felt, the more they were optimistic and
the less they were depressed. ese results indicated that perceived
safety served as a mediator between trust in social media and emotional
responses toward COVID-19 at Time 1 but not at Time 2. us,
Hypothesis 2 was only supported at Time 1.
Second, trust in ocial media was strongly associated with rapid
dissemination (Time 1, β = 0.35, p < 0.001; Time 2, β = 0.29, p < 0.001)
and transparency (Time 1, β = 0.44, p < 0.001; Time 2, β = 0.46,
p < 0.001) over time, such that the more people believed information
dissemination as rapid and transparent, the more they trusted ocial
media both at the early stage of COVID-19 outbreak and 2 years later.
us, Hypothesis 3 was supported at both survey times.
ird, trust in ocial media was positively related to positive
response (Time 1, β = 0.46, p < 0.001; Time 2, β = 0.34, p < 0.001) and
was negatively associated with depressive response (Time 1, β = −0.34,
p < 0.001; Time 2, β = −0.32, p < 0.001) over time, such that the more
people trusted the information about the Coronavirus given by ocial
media, the more they responded optimistically and the less they felt
depressively toward the pandemic. Hence, the results provided
support for a positive relationship between trust in ocial media and
public’s wellbeing of Hypothesis 4 at both survey times. Furthermore,
trust in ocial media was positively associated with perceived safety
at both times (Time 1, β = 0.50, p < 0.001; Time 2, β = 0.20, p < 0.001),
such that the more people trusted the information about the
Coronavirus given by ocial media, the more they felt safe from being
TABLE4 The standardized factor loadings, composite reliability (CR), and average variance extracted (AVE) of each construct in measurement model at
Time 1 and Time 2.
Construct Time 1 Time 2
Item Standardized
factor loading
CR AVE Item Standardized
factor loading
CR AVE
Trust in ocial
media
Central
government-
owned media
0.60
0.80 0.68
Central
government-
owned media
0.74
0.76 0.52Local government-
owned media 1.00 Local government-
owned media 0.84
Community social
workers 0.56
Trust in social
media
WeChat
inuencers 0.82
0.79 0.56
Internet
inuencers 0.73
0.68 0.42
Weibo inuencers 0.85 General netizens 0.70
Acquaintances 0.54 Acquaintances 0.50
Depressive
response
Worried 0.62
0.80 0.51
Worried 0.66
0.81 0.52
Scared 0.77 Scared 0.75
Sad 0.79 Sad 0.79
Angry 0.66 Angry 0.68
CR: Composite reliability, AVE: Average variance extracted.
FIGURE2
The relationship among information dissemination, trust in media sources, perceived safety, and emotional responses over time.
Zhou et al. 10.3389/fpubh.2023.1142230
Frontiers in Public Health 08 frontiersin.org
infected. In turn, the safer people felt, the more they felt optimistic and
the less they felt depressed. at is, perceived safety mediated the
relationship between trust in ocial media and emotional responses
toward COVID-19 both at Time 1 and Time 2. us, Hypothesis 5 was
supported by a positive mediating eect of perceived safety between
trust in ocial media and public’s wellbeing at both survey times.
e robustness of the structural model was tested by changing the
sample range (78, 79). To examine if the structural model only held
due to high trust in media sources, weremoved a portion of the
sample with high trust scores (> 4 out of a possible 5) either in ocial
media or social media. e structural model still held aer changing
the sample range. And all signicant coecients in the structural
model remain signicant in robustness check. ese results suggest
that our ndings are relatively robust.
4. Discussion
e present research applied a longitudinal approach to examine
how trust in media sources aect public’s wellbeing through perceived
safety and how the dissemination of information contributes to
increased public trust in ocial media during the course of
COVID-19 pandemic.
e results of the present study suggest that the public had more
trust in the information about COVID-19 from the ocial media
outlets than from the social media both at the early stage of the
pandemic outbreak and 2 years later. e comparatively higher trust
in ocial media is likely due to that the ocial media represents the
voice of the government and is regarded as highly reliable during a
pandemic (80, 81). In addition, trust in ocial media was signicantly
increased over a two-year period, which is opposite to research
ndings from Europe and the USA showing trust in ocial media
decreased both in short-term (82) and in long-term (83, 84) during
the COVID-19 pandemic. In contrast, trust in social media was
slightly decreased two years aer COVID-19 outbreak.
Public perceptions of rapid dissemination and transparency
regarding information about the Coronavirus also increased over
time, which is likely due to the open and transparent risk
communication implemented by governments. During COVID-19
pandemic, the Chinese government disclosed real-time data in detail
on conrmed, suspected, and cured cases, as well as deaths across the
country. It also issued national action plans and released authoritative
interpretations of the coronavirus to mitigate public panic and doubts
(85). Moreover, public’s wellbeing was signicantly improved over the
2 years period, which is in line with research ndings from UK (86)
and the USA (59).
While the social media were ooded with information and
sensational news about COVID-19, public’s trust in them was low.
However, trust in social media played a dominant role in contributing
to increased depressive symptoms in the early stage of COVID
pandemic. e negative impact of trust in social media was largely
reduced over time. In contrast, trust in the information from ocial
media was higher, and it played an inuential role in contributing to
enhanced positive response and decreased depressive symptoms both
at the beginning of the pandemic and over 2 years later. While existing
literature points to both positive and negative directions regarding
how trust in ocial media would aect mental wellbeing during
COVID-19 pandemic (51–58), the present study provides evidence
for a positive eect of trust in COVID-19-related information from
ocial media on public’s wellbeing in Chinese context over time. e
ndings suggest that enhancing public trust in information from
ocial media will bean eective approach to ght against the so called
COVID-19 infodemic and protect public’s wellbeing. is has
signicant implications for public health measures to combat the
pandemic of social media panic. To eectively minimize the negative
impact of social media on public mental health, health authorities
TABLE5 The direct, indirect, and total eects of trust in media sources on public’s wellbeing at Time 1 and Time 2.
Paths Time 1 Time 2
Standardized
eect
95% CI Standardized
eect
95% CI
Direct eects
Trust in ocial media → positive response 0.46*** (0.436, 0.479) 0.34*** (0.289, 0.396)
Trust in ocial media → depressive response −0.34*** (−0.360, −0.312) −0.32*** (−0.373, −0.255)
Trust in social media → positive response −0.16*** (−0.181, −0.133) 0.01 (−0.046, 0.065)
Trust in social media → depressive response 0.30*** (0.281, 0.328) 0.08** (0.020, 0.150)
Indirect eects
Trust in ocial media → perceived safety → positive response 0.06*** (0.048, 0.063) 0.03*** (0.023, 0.044)
Trust in ocial media → perceived safety → depressive response −0.10*** (−0.109, −0.092) −0.04*** (−0.057, −0.032)
Trust in social media → perceived safety → positive response −0.01*** (−0.015, −0.008) −0.01 (−0.015, 0.002)
Trust in social media → perceived safety → depressive response 0.02*** (0.016, 0.026) 0.01 (−0.003, 0.019)
Total eects
Trust in ocial media → positive response 0.51*** (0.494, 0.532) 0.38*** (0.321, 0.427)
Trust in ocial media → depressive response −0.44*** (−0.458, −0.415) −0.36*** (−0.414, −0.298)
Trust in social media → positive response −0.17*** (−0.194, −0.143) 0.00 (−0.054, 0.061)
Trust in social media → depressive response 0.33*** (0.300, 0.350) 0.09** (0.026, 0.160)
***p < 0.001, **p < 0.01.
Zhou et al. 10.3389/fpubh.2023.1142230
Frontiers in Public Health 09 frontiersin.org
need to rapidly detect and respond to misinformation and rumors in
social media.
e present research demonstrated that trust in ocial media was
positively correlated with rapid dissemination and transparency of the
information about COVID-19 over time. Hence, fostering and
maintaining public’s trust requires rapid dissemination and
transparency of information. e trust-building function of
transparency revealed in the present study is in line with literature on
the general relationship between transparency and public trust (43,
48, 87, 88). Research on infectious disease found that public trust in
government and public health authorities as information source
inuences public perceived risk and their responses to the threat (47,
88–91). e present study further shows that rapid dissemination of
information and transparency works hand in hand. ese ndings
suggest that government and health authorities need to rapidly
disseminate information and update the outbreak through various
platforms including their social media accounts to accommodate all
segments of the population. e information needs to betransparent,
even though communicating uncertainty and a lack of knowledge in
the case of the novel COVID-19 can beunsettling. Otherwise, the
absence of ocial information creates a rich breeding ground for
misinformation and rumors in social media, which can further
exacerbate the fear caused by the objectively life-threatening nature of
the coronavirus. A trusted ocial media based on transparency and
rapid dissemination of COVID-19-related information can keep the
public informed and enable them to develop a sense of agency through
knowing how to manage the risks.
While the present study has shed light on the negative impacts of
trust in social media sources on wellbeing, future research needs to
unpack the complexity of social media. e information in social media
is diverse and sometimes contradicting. In addition, the information
may come from a wide range of sources including people sharing
information acquired from ocial sources (48). us, how trust in
social media aect public’s wellbeing may depend on the contents and
sources. For example, a literature review has shown that viewing
stressful content about COVID-19 outbreak on social media was linked
to poor psychological outcomes, while viewing motivational and heroic
speech, knowledge of COVID-19, and entertaining contents was related
to positive psychological wellbeing (45). To unpack the complexity of
trust in social media, future research needs to tease apart the
information source and contents on social media. e insights will help
policy makers and health authorities develop targeted strategy to
harness the benets of social media and mitigate the negative impacts.
Moreover, to fully utilize the protective role of trust in ocial media, an
in-depth examination of what key aspects of pandemic related
information important for the public is needed. Such insights would
inform a more targeted strategy for rapid dissemination. Noticeably,
though trust in ocial media can protect public’s wellbeing against
COVID-19 infodemic, this does not mean all the information given by
ocial media is the absolute truth. Scientic understanding of
COVID-19 is evolving constantly, such that what qualies as
misinformation might besubjective to new scientic discoveries (6). In
addition, fear-based communication strategies may raise public
adherence to health recommendations for COVID-19, but such
strategies might negatively aect public’s wellbeing (51, 53, 55, 92–94).
Future research can unpack the contents and approaches adopted by
ocial media to identify eective communication strategies in
conveying information eciently while protecting public’s wellbeing. At
last, although the current study took a longitudinal approach (95), it’s
not a follow-up study with the same participants. Future research needs
to follow up the same participant sample to further verify the impact of
trust in media sources on public’s wellbeing over time.
In summary, the present study has empirically and longitudinally
demonstrated that the COVID-19 infodemic can have serious
consequence for public’s wellbeing. Especially, trust in the information
about COVID-19in social media was associated with stronger depressive
response at the beginning of pandemic. However, trust in ocial media
can mitigate this negative impact. More importantly, the rapid
dissemination and transparency of information regarding the virus can
enhance public trust in the information from ocial media outlets. e
ndings highlight that, to protect public’s wellbeing against COVID-19
infodemic, government and health authorities need to rapidly disseminate
information and betransparent even though communicating uncertainty
and unknowns can be unsettling. Otherwise, the absence of ocial
information creates a rich breeding ground for misinformation and
rumors in social media, which has huge consequence for public’s
wellbeing, especially at the early stage of the pandemic.
Data availability statement
e raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will
bemade available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics statement
e studies involving human participants were reviewed and
approved by e Academic Committee of Institute of Sociology,
Chinese Academy of Social Sciences (CASS). Written informed
consent for participation was not required for this study in accordance
with the national legislation and the institutional requirements.
Author contributions
JW, YiZ, and AZ conceived and designed the study. JW, XL, and
XT contributed to data collection. YiZ analyzed the data. YiZ, AZ,
RM, XT, and XL wrote the rst dra of the manuscript. YiZ, AZ, and
YaZ revised the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and
approved the submitted version.
Funding
e study was funded by Key Projects of Philosophy and Social
Sciences Research, Ministry of Education of the People’s Republic of
China (Award number: 21JZD038) and China Scholarship Council
(CSC Award Number: 202004920045).
Conflict of interest
e authors declare that the research was conducted in the
absence of any commercial or nancial relationships that could
beconstrued as a potential conict of interest.
Zhou et al. 10.3389/fpubh.2023.1142230
Frontiers in Public Health 10 frontiersin.org
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the
authors and do not necessarily represent those of their aliated
organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the
reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or
claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or
endorsed by the publisher.
References
1. Arnaud Y, Drouin O, Borges Da Silva R. Covid-19 related information seeking: the
impact of media on parental concerns. Front. Public Health. (2022) 10:977634. doi:
10.3389/fpubh.2022.977634
2. Banerjee D, Meena KS. Covid-19 as an "Infodemic" in public health: critical role of
the social media. Front Public Health. (2021) 9:610623. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2021.610623
3. Lu J. emes and evolution of misinformation during the early phases of the
Covid-19 outbreak in China—an application of the crisis and emergency risk
communication model. Front Commun. (2020) 5:57. doi: 10.3389/fcomm.2020.00057
4. Zeng J, Chan C-H. A cross-national diagnosis of infodemics: comparing the topical
and temporal features of misinformation around Covid-19in China, India, the US,
Germany and France. Online Inf Rev. (2021) 45:709–28. doi: 10.1108/oir-09-2020-0417
5. Jennings W, Stoker G, Bunting H, Valgarethsson VO, Gaskell J, Devine D, et al. Lack
of trust, conspiracy beliefs, and social media use predict COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy.
Vaccines (Basel). (2021) 9:593. doi: 10.3390/vaccines9060593
6. Limaye RJ, Sauer M, Ali J, Bernstein J, Wahl B, Barnhill A, et al. Building trust while
inuencing online Covid-19 content in the social media world. Lancet Digit Health.
(2020) 2:e277–8. doi: 10.1016/S2589-7500(20)30084-4
7. Chong SK, Ali SH, Ethoan LN, Yi SS, Trinh-Shevrin C, Kwon SC. Social media use
and misinformation among Asian Americans during COVID-19. Front Public Health.
(2021) 9:764681. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2021.764681
8. Skae I, Nordahl-Hansen A, Quintana DS, Wynn R, Gabarron E. Misinformation
about Covid-19 vaccines on social media: rapid review. J Med Internet Res. (2022)
24:e37367. doi: 10.2196/37367
9. Zhang KC, Fang Y, Cao H, Chen H, Hu T, Chen YQ, et al. Parental acceptability of
Covid-19 vaccination for children under the age of 18 years: cross-sectional online
survey. JMIR Pediatr Parent. (2020) 3:e24827. doi: 10.2196/24827
10. Bhagianadh D, Arora K. Covid-19 vaccine hesitancy among community-dwelling
older adults: the role of information sources. J Appl Gerontol. (2022) 41:4–11. doi:
10.1177/07334648211037507
11. Allington D, McAndrew S, Moxham-Hall V, Duy B. Coronavirus conspiracy
suspicions, general vaccine attitudes, trust and coronavirus information source as
predictors of vaccine hesitancy among UK residents during the Covid-19 pandemic.
Psychol Med. (2021) 53:236–247. doi: 10.1017/S0033291721001434
12. Sallam M, Dababseh D, Eid H, Al-Mahzoum K, Al-Haidar A, Taim D, et al. High
rates of COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy and its association with conspiracy beliefs: a study
in Jordan and Kuwait among other Arab countries. Vaccines (Basel). (2021) 9:42. doi:
10.3390/vaccines9010042
13. Costantino C, Gori D, Alba D, Cimino L, Conforto A, Tomasello F, et al.
Willingness to receive Covid-19 vaccination in costumers accessing community
pharmacies in the province of Palermo, Italy. EuroMediterr Biomed J. (2021) 16:80–4.
doi: 10.3269/1970-5492.2021.16.19
14. Depoux A, Martin S, Karallakis E, Preet R, Wilder-Smith A, Larson H. e
pandemic of social media panic travels faster than the Covid-19 outbreak. J Travel Med.
(2020) 27:1–2. doi: 10.1093/jtm/taaa031
15. Ghebreyesus TA. (2020). World health Organizaion director-general speech at
Munich security conference. Available at: https://www.who.int/dg/speeches/detail/
munich-security-conference (Accessed December 3, 2020).
16. Fridman I, Lucas N, Henke D, Zigler CK. Association between public knowledge
about Covid-19, trust in information sources, and adherence to social distancing: cross-
sectional survey. JMIR Public Health Surveill. (2020) 6:e22060. doi: 10.2196/22060
17. Melki J, Tamim H, Hadid D, Makki M, El Amine J, Hitti E. Mitigating infodemics: the
relationship between news exposure and trust and belief in Covid-19 fake news and social
media spreading. PLoS One. (2021) 16:e0252830. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0252830
18. Karabela SN, Coskun F, Hosgor H. Investigation of the relationships between
perceived causes of Covid-19, attitudes towards vaccine and level of trust in information
sources from the perspective of infodemic: the case of Turkey. BMC Public Health.
(2021) 21:1195. doi: 10.1186/s12889-021-11262-1
19. Petravić L, Arh R, Gabrovec T, Jazbec L, Rupčić N, Starešinič N, et al. Factors
aecting attitudes towards Covid-19 vaccination: an online survey in Slovenia. Vaccines
(Basel). (2021) 9:247. doi: 10.3390/vaccines9030247
20. Chen J, Zhang SX, Yin A, Yanez JA. Mental health symptoms during the Covid-19
pandemic in developing countries: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Glob Health.
(2022) 12:05011. doi: 10.7189/jogh.12.05011
21. Huang Y, Zhao N. Generalized anxiety disorder, depressive symptoms and sleep
quality during COVID-19 outbreak in China: a web-based cross-sectional survey.
Psychiatry Res. (2020) 288:112954. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.112954
22. Li L, Zhang Q, Wang X, Zhang J, Wang T, Gao T-L, et al. Characterizing the
propagation of situational information in social media during Covid-19 epidemic: a case
study on Weibo. IEEE Transac Comput Soc Syst. (2020) 7:556–62. doi: 10.1109/
tcss.2020.2980007
23. Wang C, Pan R, Wan X, Tan Y, Xu L, McIntyre RS, et al. A longitudinal study on
the mental health of general population during the Covid-19 epidemic in China. Brain
Behav Immun. (2020) 87:40–8. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2020.04.028
24. Wong BY, Ho SY, Sit SMM, Gong WJ, Lai AYK, Wang MP, et al. Association of
family wellbeing with forwarding and verifying Covid-19-related information, and
mediation of family communication quality. Front Public Health. (2022) 10:948955. doi:
10.3389/fpubh.2022.948955
25. Xiao X, Yang X, Zheng W, Wang B, Fu L, Luo D, et al. Depression, anxiety and
post-traumatic growth among Covid-19 survivors six-month aer discharge. Eur J
Psychotraumatol. (2022) 13:2055294. doi: 10.1080/20008198.2022.2055294
26. She R, Wong K, Lin J, Zhang Y, Leung K, Yang X. Proles of stress and coping
associated with mental, behavioral, and internet use problems among adolescents during
the Covid-19 pandemic: a stratied random sampling and cluster analysis. Front Public
Health. (2022) 10:826911. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2022.826911
27. Atehortua NA, Patino S. Covid-19, a tale of two pandemics: novel coronavirus and
fake news messaging. Health Promot Int. (2021) 36:524–34. doi: 10.1093/heapro/daaa140
28. Al-Amad SH, Hussein A. Anxiety among dental professionals and its association
with their dependency on social media for health information: insights from the
Covid-19 pandemic. BMC Psychol. (2021) 9:9. doi: 10.1186/s40359-020-00509-y
29. Su Z, McDonnell D, Wen J, Kozak M, Abbas J, Segalo S, et al. Mental health
consequences of Covid-19 media coverage: the need for eective crisis communication
practices. Glob He alth. (2021) 17:4. doi: 10.1186/s12992-020-00654-4
30. Yang X, Yip BHK, Mak ADP, Zhang D, Lee EKP, Wong SYS. e dierential eects
of social media on depressive symptoms and suicidal ideation among the younger and
older adult population in Hong Kong during the Covid-19 pandemic: population-based
cross-sectional survey study. JMIR Public Health Surveill. (2021) 7:e24623. doi:
10.2196/24623
31. Geirdal AO, Ruolo M, Leung J, ygesen H, Price D, Bonsaksen T, et al. Mental
health, quality of life, wellbeing, loneliness and use of social media in a time of social
distancing during the Covid-19 outbreak. A cross-country comparative study. J Ment
Health. (2021) 30:148–55. doi: 10.1080/09638237.2021.1875413
32. Ko NY, Lu WH, Chen YL, Li DJ, Wang PW, Hsu ST, et al. Covid-19-related
information sources and psychological well-being: an online survey study in Taiwan.
Brain Behav Immun. (2020) 87:153–4. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2020.05.019
33. Lin D, Friedman DB, Qiao S, Tam CC, Li X, Li X. Information uncertainty: a
correlate for acute stress disorder during the Covid-19 outbreak in China. BMC Public
Health. (2020) 20:1867. doi: 10.1186/s12889-020-09952-3
34. Olagoke AA, Olagoke OO, Hughes AM. Exposure to coronavirus news on
mainstream media: the role of risk perceptions and depression. Br J Health Psychol.
(2020) 25:865–74. doi: 10.1111/bjhp.12427
35. Xu T. Media, trust in government, and risk perception of COVID-19in the early
stage of epidemic: an analysis based on moderating eect. Healthcare (Basel). (2021)
9:1597. doi: 10.3390/healthcare9111597
36. Kaparounaki CK, Patsali ME, Mousa DV, Papadopoulou EVK, Papadopoulou KKK,
Fountoulakis KN. University students' mental health amidst the Covid-19 quarantine in
Greece. Psychiatry Res. (2020) 290:113111. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113111
37. Liu M, Zhang H, Huang H. Media exposure to Covid-19 information, risk
perception, social and geographical proximity, and self-rated anxiety in China. BMC
Public Health. (2020) 20:1649. doi: 10.1186/s12889-020-09761-8
38. Nissan D, Weiss G, Siman-Tov M, Spitz A, Bodas M, Shenhar G, et al. Dierences
in levels of psychological distress, perceived safety, trust, and ecacy amongst hospital
personnel during the Covid-19 pandemic. Res Nurs Health. (2021) 44:776–86. doi:
10.1002/nur.22165
39. Schneider MB, Greif TR, Galsky AP, Gomez D, Anderson C, Edwards DS, et al.
Giving psychology trainees a voice during the Covid-19 pandemic: trainee mental
health, perceived safety, and support. Training Educ Prof Psychol. (2021) 15:76–85. doi:
10.1037/tep0000343
Zhou et al. 10.3389/fpubh.2023.1142230
Frontiers in Public Health 11 frontiersin.org
40. Moon MJ. Fighting Covid-19 with agility, transparency, and participation: wicked
policy problems and new governance challenges. Public Adm Rev. (2020) 80:651–6. doi:
10.1111/puar.13214
41. Rahimi F, Talebi Bezmin Abadi A. Transparency and information sharing could
help abate the Covid-19 pandemic. Infect Control Hosp Epidemiol. (2020) 41:1366–7. doi:
10.1017/ice.2020.174
42. Spalluto LB, Planz VB, Stokes LS, Pierce R, Arono DM, McPheeters ML, et al.
Transparency and trust during the coronavirus disease 2019 (Covid-19) pandemic. J
AmColl Radiol. (2020) 17:909–12. doi: 10.1016/j.jacr.2020.04.026
43. Menon KU, Goh KT. Transparency and trust: risk communications and the
Singapore experience in managing SARS. J Commun Manag. (2005) 9:375–83. doi:
10.1108/13632540510621614
44. Hsu YC, Chen YL, Wei HN, Yang YW, Chen YH. Risk and outbreak
communication: lessons from Taiwan's experiences in the post-SARS era. Health Secur.
(2017) 15:165–9. doi: 10.1089/hs.2016.0111
45. Anjum N. Covid-19 pandemic: how does social media affect psychological
wellbeing?–a synthesis of literature review. Int J Indian Psychol. (2020)
8:1698–707. doi: 10.25215/0803.173
46. Hua J, Shaw R. Corona virus (Covid-19) "Infodemic" and emerging issues through
a data lens: the case of China. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2020) 17:2309. doi:
10.3390/ijerph17072309
47. Ali SH, Foreman J, Tozan Y, Capasso A, Jones AM, DiClemente RJ. Trends and
predictors of Covid-19 information sources and their relationship with knowledge and
beliefs related to the pandemic: nationwide cross-sectional study. JMIR Public Health
Surveill. (2020) 6:e21071. doi: 10.2196/21071
48. Liu Z, Shan J, Delaloye M, Piguet J-G, Glassey BN. e role of public trust and
media in managing the dissemination of Covid-19-related news in Switzerland. J Media .
(2020) 1:145–58. doi: 10.3390/journalmedia1010010
49. Cáceres CF. Unresolved Covid controversies: ‘normal science’ and potential non-
scientic inuences. Glob Public Health. (2022) 17:622–40. doi:
10.1080/17441692.2022.2036219
50. Elisha E, Guetzkow J, Shir-Raz Y, Ronel N eds. Suppressing scientic discourse on
vaccines? Self-perceptions of researchers and practitioners. HEC Forum. (2022):1–19.
doi: 10.1007/s10730-022-09479-7
51. Giroux M, Park J, Kim J-E, Choi YK, Lee JC, Kim S, et al. e impact of
communication information on the perceived threat of Covid-19 and stockpiling
intention. Australas Mark J. (2021) 31:60–70. doi: 10.1177/18393349211028670
52. Shir-Raz Y, Elisha E, Martin B, Ronel N, Guetzkow J. Censorship and suppression
of Covid-19 heterodoxy: tactics and counter-tactics. Minerva. (2022) 1:1–27. doi:
10.1007/s11024-022-09479-4
53. Bagus P, Pena-Ramos JA, Sanchez-Bayon A. Covid-19 and the political economy
of mass hysteria. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2021) 18:1376. doi: 10.3390/
ijerph18041376
54. Garn DR, Silver RC, Holman EA. e novel coronavirus (Covid-2019) outbreak:
amplication of public health consequences by media exposure. Health Psychol. (2020)
39:355–7. doi: 10.1037/hea0000875
55. Ogbodo JN, Onwe EC, Chukwu J, Nwasum CJ, Nwakpu ES, Nwankwo SU, et al.
Communicating health crisis: a content analysis of global media framing of Covid-19.
Health Promot Perspect. (2020) 10:257–69. doi: 10.34172/hpp.2020.40
56. Bittmann F. How trust makes a dierence: the impact of the rst wave of the
Covid-19 pandemic on life satisfaction in Germany. Appl Res Qual Life. (2022)
17:1389–405. doi: 10.1007/s11482-021-09956-0
57. Figueiras MJ, Ghorayeb J, Coutinho MVC, Maroco J, omas J. Levels of trust in
information sources as a predictor of protective health behaviors during Covid-19
pandemic: a UAE cross-sectional study. Front Psychol. (2021) 12:633550. doi: 10.3389/
fpsyg.2021.633550
58. Olagoke AA, Olagoke OO, Hughes AM. Psychological pathways linking public
trust during the coronavirus pandemic to mental and physical well-being. Front Psychol .
(2020) 11:570216. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.570216
59. Blanchower DG, Bryson A. Covid and mental health in America. PLoS One.
(2022) 17:e0269855. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0269855
60. Li H, He J, Chen J, Pan S, Feng J, Liu S. e governance of imported 2019-Ncov
infections: what can belearned from China's experience? Glob Health Res Policy. (2022)
7:8. doi: 10.1186/s41256-022-00243-5
61. American Psychological Association. (2017). Ethical principles of psychologists and
code of conduct. Available at: https://www.apa.org/ethics/code/index (Accessed
December 3, 2020).
62. WMA Declaration of Helsinki. (2018). Ethical principles for medical research
involving human subjects. Available at: https://www.wma.net/policies-post/wma-
declaration-of-helsinki-ethical-principles-for-medical-research-involving-human-
subjects/ (Accessed December 3, 2020).
63. IBM Support. (2020). Transforming dierent Likert scales to a common scale.
Available at: https://www.ibm.com/support/pages/transforming-dierent-likert-scales-
common-scale (Accessed November 28, 2022).
64. Murat N. A hybrid transformation approach for common scaling on various type
Likert scales in Bayesian structural equation modeling. Commun Stat eory Methods.
(2021) 51:1217–31. doi: 10.1080/03610926.2020.1853774
65. Taber KS. e use of Cronbach’s alpha when developing and reporting research
instruments in science education. Res Sci Educ. (2017) 48:1273–96. doi: 10.1007/
s11165-016-9602-2
66. Tavakol M, Dennick R. Making sense of Cronbach's alpha. Int J Med Educ. (2011)
2:53–5. doi: 10.5116/ijme.4d.8dfd
67. Bland JM, Altman G. Statistics notes: Cronbach's alpha. BMJ. (1997) 314:572. doi:
10.1136/bmj.314.7080.572
68. van Griethuijsen RALF, van Eijck MW, Haste H, den Brok PJ, Skinner NC,
Mansour N, et al. Global patterns in students’ views of science and interest in science.
Res Sci Educ. (2014) 45:581–603. doi: 10.1007/s11165-014-9438-6
69. Kuhl EA, Dixit NK, Walker RL, Conti JB, Sears SF. Measurement of patient fears
about implantable cardioverter debrillator shock: an initial evaluation of the Florida
shock anxiety scale. Pacing Clin Electrophysiol. (2006) 29:614–8. doi:
10.1111/j.1540-8159.2006.00408.x
70. Tripp C, Huber NL, Kuhl EA, Sears SF. Measuring ICD shock anxiety: status
update on the Florida shock anxiety scale aer over a decade of use. Pacing Clin
Electrophysiol. (2019) 42:1294–301. doi: 10.1111/pace.13793
71. Mueller RO, Hancock GR. Structural equation modeling In: GR Hancock, LM
Stapleton and RO Mueller, editors. e Reviewer’s guide to quantitative methods in the
social sciences. 2nd ed. London, UK: Routledge (2018). 445–56.
72. Dash G, Paul J. Cb-Sem vs Pls-Sem methods for research in social sciences and
technology forecasting. Technol Forecast Soc Chang. (2021) 173:121092. doi: 10.1016/j.
techfore.2021.121092
73. Fornell C, Larcker DF. Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable
variables and measurement error. J Mark Res. (1981) 18:39–50. doi:
10.1177/002224378101800104
74. Hair JFBWC, Babin B, Anderson RE, Tatham RL. Multivariate data analysi. 7th ed.
New York: Prentice Hall (2010).
75. Koueros XA. Testing a model of pull production: a paradigm for manufacturing
research using structural equation modeling. J Oper Manag. (1999) 17:467–88. doi:
10.1016/S0272-6963(99)00002-9
76. Morton NA, Koueros X. Intention to commit online music piracy and its
antecedents: an empirical investigation. Struct Equ Model Multidiscip J. (2008)
15:491–512. doi: 10.1080/10705510802154331
77. Wei H, Xu H, Chen W, Lu L. Zhongyong thinking (doctrine of the mean) and
internet addiction: the mediation of maladaptive cognition and the moderation of
subject. Front Public Health. (2022) 10:1045830. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2022.1045830
78. Guo X, Zhong S, Wu Y, Zhang Y, Wang Z. e impact of lockdown in Wuhan on
residents condence in controlling Covid-19 outbreak at the destination cities. Front
Public Health. (2022) 10:902455. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2022.902455
79. Nie W, Hu M, Ye X. Internet use and rural-urban mental health inequalities:
evidence from China. Front Public Health. (2023) 11:1107146. doi: 10.3389/
fpubh.2023.1107146
80. Siegrist M, Zingg A. e role of public trust during pandemics. Eur Psychol. (2014)
19:23–32. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040/a000169
81. Van der Weerd W, Timmermans DRM, Beaujean DJMA, Oudho J, Van
Steenbergen JE. Monitoring the level of government trust, risk perception and intention
of the general public to adopt protective measures during the inuenza A (H1N1)
pandemic in the Netherlands. BMC Public Health. (2011) 11:575. doi:
10.1186/1471-2458-11-575
82 . Adam S, Urman A, Arlt D, Gil-Lopez T, Makhortykh M, Maier M. Media trust and
the Covid-19 pandemic: an analysis of short-term trust changes, their ideological drivers
and consequences in Switzerland. Commun Res. (2022) 50:205–29. doi:
10.1177/00936502221127484
83. Aassve A, Capezzone T, Cavalli N, Conzo P, Peng C. (2022). Trust in the time of
coronavirus: longitudinal evidence from the United States. SocArXiv. Center for
Open Science.
84. Weinberg J. Trust, governance, and the Covid-19 pandemic: an explainer using
longitudinal data from the United Kingdom. Polit Q. (2022) 93:316–25. doi:
10.1111/1467-923X.13131
85. Cheng ZJ, Zhan Z, Xue M, Zheng P, Lyu J, Ma J, et al. Public health measures and
the control of Covid-19in China. Clin Rev Allergy Immunol. (2021) 64:1–16. doi:
10.1007/s12016-021-08900-2
86. Zhou M, Kan MY. e varying impacts of Covid-19 and its related measures in the
UK: A year in review. PLoS One. (2021) 16:e0257286. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0257286
87. Alessandro M, Cardinale Lagomarsino B, Scartascini C, Streb J, Torrealday J.
Transparency and trust in government. Evidence from a survey experiment. Wor ld D ev .
(2021) 138:105223. doi: 10.1016/j.worlddev.2020.105223
88. Porumbescu G. Linking transparency to trust in government and voice. Am Rev
Public Adm. (2015) 47:520–37. doi: 10.1177/0275074015607301
Zhou et al. 10.3389/fpubh.2023.1142230
Frontiers in Public Health 12 frontiersin.org
89. d'Alessandro E, Hubert D, Launay O, Bassinet L, Lortholary O, Jare Y, et al.
Determinants of refusal of A/H1N1 pandemic vaccination in a high risk population:
a qualitative approach. PLoS One. (2012) 7:e34054. doi: 10.1371/journal.
pone.0034054
90. Mayor E, Eicher V, Bangerter A, Gilles I, Clemence A, Green EG. Dynamic
social representations of the 2009 H1N1 pandemic: shifting patterns of sense-
making and blame. Public Underst Sci. (2013) 22:1011–24. doi:
10.1177/0963662512443326
91. O'Brien BC. Do yousee what Isee? Reflections on the relationship between
transparency and trust. Acad Med. (2019) 94:757–9. doi: 10.1097/
ACM.0000000000002710
92. Stolow JA, Moses LM, Lederer AM, Carter R. How fear appeal approaches in
Covid-19 health communication may beharming the global community. Health Educ
Behav. (2020) 47:531–5. doi: 10.1177/1090198120935073
93. Tannenbaum MB, Hepler J, Zimmerman RS, Saul L, Jacobs S, Wilson K, et al.
Appealing to fear: a meta-analysis of fear appeal eectiveness and theories. Psychol Bull.
(2015) 141:1178–204. doi: 10.1037/a0039729
94. Volpp KG, Loewenstein G, Buttenheim AM. Behaviorally informed strategies for
a national Covid-19 vaccine promotion program. JAMA. (2021) 325:125–6. doi:
10.1001/jama.2020.24036
95. Caruana EJ, Roman M, Hernandez-Sanchez J, Solli P. Longitudinal Studies. J
orac Dis. (2015) 7:E537–40. doi: 10.3978/j.issn.2072-1439.2015.10.63
Content uploaded by Airong Zhang
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Airong Zhang on Apr 26, 2023
Content may be subject to copyright.