ArticlePDF AvailableLiterature Review

The Mental Illness-Health Matrix and the Mental State Space Matrix: Complementary meta-conceptual frameworks for evaluating psychological states

Authors:

Abstract

Objective: It is increasingly appreciated that mental health may not just involve a relative absence of mental illness, but the active presence of positive psychological desiderata. However, research attention on mental illness and health has tended to remain siloed and disconnected-proceeding along parallel tracks-with their potential relationship underexplored and undertheorized. As such, we sought to develop theoretical models to help us better understand the interaction of these two domains of experience. Methods: Through extensive engagement with relevant literature, we created two complementary meta-conceptual frameworks to represent and evaluate states of mental illness and health. Results: The Mental Illness-Health Matrix allows different forms of mental illness and health to be situated and assessed within a common framework. The Mental State Space Matrix further enables these various forms to be conceptualized and appraised in terms of numerous common parameters (e.g., valence and arousal). Conclusion: It is hoped that these frameworks will stimulate and support further research on the inter-relational dynamics of illness and health. Indeed, the matrices themselves are provisional works-in-progress, with their articulation here intended as a foundation for their further development as understanding of these topics evolves and improves.
Received: 28 September 2022
|
Accepted: 4 March 2023
DOI: 10.1002/jclp.23512
REVIEW ARTICLE
The Mental IllnessHealth Matrix and the
Mental State Space Matrix: Complementary
metaconceptual frameworks for evaluating
psychological states
Tim Lomas
1
|Tyler J. VanderWeele
2
1
Department of Epidemiology, Harvard T. H.
Chan School of Public Health, Human
Flourishing Program, Harvard University,
Cambridge, MA, USA
2
Departments of Epidemiology and
Biostatistics, Harvard T. H. Chan School of
Public Health, Human Flourishing Program,
Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA
Correspondence
Tim Lomas
Email: tlomas@hsph.harvard.edu
Abstract
Objective: It is increasingly appreciated that mental health
may not just involve a relative absence of mental illness, but
the active presence of positive psychological desiderata.
However, research attention on mental illness and health
has tended to remain siloed and disconnectedproceeding
along parallel trackswith their potential relationship under-
explored and undertheorized. As such, we sought to develop
theoretical models to help us better understand the interaction
of these two domains of experience.
Methods: Through extensive engagement with relevant
literature, we created two complementary metaconceptual
frameworks to represent and evaluate states of mental
illness and health.
Results: The Mental IllnessHealth Matrix allows different
forms of mental illness and health to be situated and
assessed within a common framework. The Mental State
Space Matrix further enables these various forms to be
conceptualized and appraised in terms of numerous
common parameters (e.g., valence and arousal).
Conclusion: It is hoped that these frameworks will stimulate
and support further research on the interrelational dynamics
of illness and health. Indeed, the matrices themselves are
provisional worksinprogress, with their articulation here
J Clin Psychol. 2023;119. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/jclp © 2023 Wiley Periodicals LLC.
|
1
intended as a foundation for their further development as
understanding of these topics evolves and improves.
KEYWORDS
flourishing, mental health, mental illness, wellbeing
1|INTRODUCTION
Since the field's inception, psychologists have been concerned with mental illness. Then, in more recent decades,
they have also been increasingly interested in mental healthnot merely as an ostensible absence of illness, but the
active presence of certain psychological desiderata. These trends are exemplified in remarks by two of the seminal
figures historically most closely associated with illness and health. The field's traditional emphasis on illness was
captured by Freud, who saw the goal of psychotherapy as mainly limited to turning hysterical misery into ordinary
unhappiness(Breuer & Freud, 1955, p. 308). However, other voices such as William James were arguing that
relative freedom from illness was not the same as actively flourishing, and by the 1940s a new generation of
humanistic psychologists were urging psychology to focus on more positive phenomena, captured by notions like
selfactualization. This vision was eloquently articulated by one of its main architects, Abraham Maslow (1962), who
said It is as if Freud supplied us the sick half of psychology and we must now fill it out with the healthy half(p. 5).
Likewise, pioneers such as Jahoda (1958) advocated a focus on positive mental health,a call embraced in recent
years by positive psychology especially. Yet despite this twin focus on mental illness and health in psychology, these
have tended to mostly remain separate and parallel tracks. However, there are now efforts to more closely explore
their interaction, as reflected in paradigms like positive clinical psychology(Seligman & Peterson, 2003; Wood &
Tarrier, 2010). In this vein, this paper introduces two new frameworks for understanding and assessing the
relationship between mental illness and health: the Mental IllnessHealth Matrix (MIH Matrix) and the Mental State
Space Matrix (MSS Matrix), which we consider in turn.
2|THE MENTAL ILLNESS HEALTH (MIH) MATRIX
This first section introduces the MIH Matrix. First, we provide some theoretical background to the framework in the
form of the dual continuamodel. Next, we outline the matrix itself. We then offer suggestions for how the matrix
might provide an overarching assessment of mental wellbeing.
2.1 |The dual continua model
The concepts of mental illness and health are complex and contested, with ongoing debates regarding their
etiology, symptomology, and so on. Simply in descriptive terms though, mental illness tends to be identified by a
person experiencing various undesirable and/or unpleasant psychological phenomena for an extended duration,
usually accompanied by some psychosocial impairment. With mental health, the situation is slightly more complex.
Historically, and even in the present for some people, health has often just denoted a relative absence of illness.
However, over recent decades, various scholarsparticularly those aligned with humanistic psychology, like
Maslowhave argued that beyond an absence of illness, mental wellbeing also involves positive forms of health,
including the active presence of various desiderata. This case was made influentially by Herzlich (1973) visàvis
physical health, where in contrast to the prevailing view of health as simply the absence of illness, she argued it can
2
|
LOMAS and VANDERWEELE
and should be seen in a positive light as a presence of which one is fully aware because of one's feelings of
freedom and of bodily and functional wellbeing(p. 53). Her work has since been adapted for psychological
functioning, most prominently by Keyes (2002,2005,2014). Initially, efforts to highlight positive mental health
often invoked a continuum metaphor, spanning a negative and positive polerepresenting illness and health,
respectivelywith a neutral zeroat midpoint. This was reflected in Maslow's comment about the need to fill the
healthy halfof psychology, and was influentially articulated by Keyes' (2002) paper, The Mental Health
Continuum: From Languishing to Flourishing in Life.
However, as generative as this metaphor has been, it is increasingly recognized as flawed. To begin with, it is
unidimensional, implying that a person is either in the negative territory of illness or the positive territory of health. Indeed,
Keyes (2002) himself recognized this limitation, arguing that illness and health are actually somewhat separate
physiologically, functionally, experientiallyand people can experience aspects of both concurrently. To that point, his
paper assessed the prevalence of major depression and positive mental health in 3032 American adults over 12 months.
Of the 511 who had experienced a major depressive episode (14.1% of the entire sample), 143 were deemed languishing
(lack of positive mental health), 259 had moderate mental health, and 27 had positive mental health. While the last
category is numerically small, it is conceptually significant, as it suggests the potential coexistence of mental illness and
health, at least within a given year. To reflect these nuances, Keyes (2005,2014) developed a dual continuamodel, with
separate spectra for illness and health, which visually could be placed orthogonally to create a bivariate state space, with a
horizontal axis of illness, and a vertical axis of health, as shown in Figure 1.
This model allows people to be situated in various quadrants. The best option is being without illness and positively
excelling (top right), which Keyes described as flourishing,but which we designate instead as thriving.This subtle
change is because, in our work, we use flourishingin a broader sense to encompass the person and their situational
context (i.e., to describe situations where both are doing well), whereas one could conceivably be in this top right
quadrant (i.e., thriving) despite one's context (Lomas & VanderWeele, 2022), as we consider further below. So, in the
narrower context of the model, thriving is the best option, preferable to being without illness yet not thriving (bottom
right), which we follow Keyes in calling languishing,or excelling while also experiencing illness (top left), which Keyes did
not label but we refer to as struggling.In turn, the latter are both preferable to the worst permutation of being ill while
also not doing well (bottom left), which we label faltering.Moreover, scholarship on the dual continua model has
increasingly recognized that assessments of mental illness/health would ideally not merely place people in one of these
four broad categories, but would allow more nuanced analyses (Iasiello et al., 2020). One option is to account for people's
relative position in these quadrants: with the top right quadrant, for example, the further along the axes one is, the better
one's thriving. While this approach is not harnessed in many studiesmost of which simply assign people into the four
categories and compare the groups on various outcomesone finds it, for example, in longitudinal work on individual
FIGURE 1 The dual continua model.
LOMAS and VANDERWEELE
|
3
change over time (e.g., Trompetter et al., 2017). Another option is to introduce greater taxonomical granularity by
increasing the number of categories. Indeed, Keyes himself differentiated mental health and illness into three levels (low,
moderate, high), creating a matrix of nine permutations. However, as useful as this bivariate (or triviariate) model is, the
picture is even more nuanced, as our next section considers.
First though, let us return to our point about distinguishing thriving from flourishing. This subtle differentiation
is grounded in the linguistic roots of these terms. The verb to thrivesuggests attainment of wellbeing potentially
regardless or even despite inhospitable conditions, as reflected in its etymology, having entered English around
1200 CE from a Scandinavian source akin to Old Norse þrifask, which implied grasp to oneself.One can still thrive,
of course, in hospitable conditions, but this is not required. By contrast, flourishing implies being supported by one's
environment, deriving as it does from the Latin florere (to bloom, blossom, flower). In this way, flourishing suggests
an adaptive interaction and consonance between the individual and their context, such that the latter helps people
within to prosper, and vice versa. Thus, flourishing makes a stronger claim about how a person is faring: not only are
they doing well (the necessary and sufficient criteria for thriving), but so is their environment. Thriving and
flourishing, therefore, can serve linguistically to subtly differentiate people: (a) doing well potentially without help or
even in the face of obstacles (i.e., thriving), versus (b) being actively supported by their environment (which itself is
also prospering). Significantly, recognition of the dynamics implied in this distinction is often downplayed in
psychology; as Prilleltensky et al. (2001, p. 157) observed, we typically psychologize [people's] problems and ignore
the social and political context in which their problems occur.Thus, distinguishing thriving from flourishing allows
an acknowledgment that people living in challenging circumstances cannot truly be said to be flourishing, no matter
their personal qualities. This is, of course, not to disparage people themselves, but rather the society in which they
live that permits such circumstances. Likewise, this view encourages a recognition that if societies wish their
citizens to truly live well, they cannot just leave this to people's own resources or regard this as solely an individual
affair, but instead must look to make the context as salutogenic(i.e., conducive to wellbeing) as possible (e.g., see
Thompson et al., 2014, on this topic in relation to the community provision of green spaces).
This kind of contextual perspective is arguably often lacking in the field, so is much needed. A pervasive critique of
contemporary approaches to mental illness and health, and psychology generally, is a tendency toward individualism.
Stepping back, one might suggest this bias reflects and stems from the relatively individualistic nature of Western
cultures, which have historically influenced and indeed dominated the ways these disciplines have developed. This
history means that issues such as depression have tended to frequently be essentialized as conditions in people's minds/
brains (e.g., a chemical imbalance), rather than the complex product of the interaction between the person and their
environment, as per Prilleltensky et al.'s (2001) remark above. However, in part because of such critiques, there is now
greater awareness that social conditions generate variation and inequities in mental wellbeing due to factors such as
identity. The Minorities' Diminished Returnstheory, for instance, holds that the effects of socioeconomic indicators on
positive outcomes are systemically smaller for racial and ethnic minority groups, a trend attributed to economic and
psychological processes connected to racism and discrimination (Assari, 2018). For example, although education
influences mental health, both directly (e.g., a sense of accomplishment), and indirectly (e.g., via income), its boosting
effectin places like the United States is reduced for black people (and other minority groups), who experience higher
depression (Assari, 2017), suicide (Assari, 2015), and mortality (Assari & Lankarani, 2016), relative to equally educated
White people.
Moreover,thiskindofcriticalanalysisraisesimportantquestionsofwhatwemean by mental illness and health. Not
only are these outcomes influenced by people's sociocultural context, so are the very constructs through which we
understand illness and health. Such critiques take various forms. At a more extreme end are perspectives articulated by
the likes of Szasz (1962), who argued that mental illness is a myth,given that any such diagnoses are based on deviation
from social norms that in themselves may be inherently problematic (Benning, 2016). This view is encapsulated in a
remark attributed to Krishnamurti that It is no measure of health to be well adjusted to a profoundly sick society.In a
milder vein, even many scholars who assert the legitimacy of notions of mental illness and health recognize that the way
people experience and understand such statesand moreover the way therapeutic fields diagnose and treat themis
4
|
LOMAS and VANDERWEELE
informed by the wider sociocultural context, with all these dynamics subject to contestation and change. One sees this,
for example, in relation to sadness, given that prolonged sadness (or low mood) is one symptom of depression according
to the Diagnostic and StatisticalManualofMentalDisorders,FifthEdition(DSM5). However, the psychiatric literature is
riven with debates on the appropriacy of such judgments. In the case of grief, for example, critics suggest that some
extended sadness is normal, and hence not an illness in any conventional sense (Horwitz, 2019). Similarly, even apart from
grieving, Horwitz and Wakefield (2007) have argued against the medicalization of regularsadness, instead advocating
regarding it as an inherent part of the human condition. The broader point here is that how mental illness and health are
experienced by people and understood by fields such as clinical psychology are subject to sociocultural dynamics. As
such, we have constructed our matrices with this critical spirit in mind, imbuing them with an openness and flexibility that
allows for these kinds of considerations.
2.2 |The MIH Matrix
Our understanding of mental wellbeing has been enhanced by the recognition that illness and health are separate
realms, rather than a continuous spectrum. However, it has further been acknowledged that not only are illness and
health not continuous, but are not singular either, and themselves contain multiple continua. This recognition is
reflected in certain work on the dualcontinua model which, rather than the standard four categories discussed
above, has explored more granular profiles of mental illness and health. Khumalo et al. (2022), for example, used
confirmatory factor analysis and latent class analysis to test the model in relation to the Mental Health Continuum
Short Form and Patient Health Questionnaire9, and developed a profile featuring 23 different indicators, 9
pertaining to illness (loss of interest, depressed mood, sleep disturbance, fatigue, appetite change, selfreproach,
concentration problems, psychomotor problems, and thoughts of suicide), and 14 to health (happiness, interest in
life, satisfaction, social contribution, social integration, social acceptance, social coherence, selfacceptance,
environmental mastery, positive relations with others, personal growth, autonomy and purpose in life). While in
certain contexts the greater simplicity of the broad fourcategory classification may be preferable, a more granular
approach inevitably provides a more detailed assessment of a person's mental illness and health.
As valuable as these more differentiated illnesshealth profiles are though, there is scope for even further granularity.
In Khumalo et al.'s analysis, the illness components pertain specifically to depression, while the health aspects center
mainly on two specific forms of positive mental wellbeing (hedonic and eudaimonic), together with some social
indicators. Yet with both mental illness and health, numerous other forms have been conceptualized. This differentiation
is already well established with mental illness. The DSM5 (APA, 2013), for example, includes 157 mental disorders,
aggregated into 20 categories. The picture for mental health is more opaque and contested, having not had comparable
attention to mental illness. Even so, recent years have seen a burgeoning effort to differentiate forms of positive health.
Most scholarship has focused on a widely accepted taxonomy of two main types: hedonic and eudaimonic. The former
is usually construed through the prism of subjective wellbeing(Diener et al., 1999), which comprises two main
dimensions: cognitive (i.e., life satisfaction) and affective (i.e., positive affect). The latter derives from classical Greek
philosophy, particularly Aristotle (1986), who valorized itin contrast to hedonic pleasureas a deeper happiness arising
through selfcultivation, defined as the activity of the soul in accordance with virtue(p. 11). Etymologically it means
having a good daimon (a guiding spirit, or perhaps from a modern perspective, one's conscience), and encompasses the
cultivation of character and commitment to ethical development. This form is sometimes operationalized as
psychological wellbeing,following the pioneering work of Ryff's (1989), who identified six dimensions: self
acceptance; positive relations; autonomy; environmental mastery; purpose in life; and personal growth.
These two forms of health have received the vast majority of attention. Even so, scholars have suggested these
two do not exhaust the potential forms of mental wellbeing. Seligman (2002), for instance, proposed engagement
as one of three pathwaysto mental wellbeingalongside hedonia and eudaimoniaencapsulating experiences
such absorption and flow (Csikszentmihalyi, 2013). Each is described by Schueller and Seligman (2010)asneither
LOMAS and VANDERWEELE
|
5
sufficient nor redundant; therefore necessitating cultivation of each to achieve the full life(p. 253). Moreover, even
these three may not comprehensively cover the relevant terrain. It is argued, for instance, that powerful
transcendent experiencesdescribed using adjectives such as numinous, peak, and spiritualare not easily
accommodated by the constructs above. As such, Wong (2011) suggests adding chaironic happinessbased on the
Greek chairo, which has meanings like gladness, joy, bliss, grace, and blessingdefined as feeling blessed and
fortunate because of a sense of awe, gratitude, and oneness with nature or God(p. 70). Moreover, the picture may
be more complicated still; indeed, we (Lomas & VanderWeele, 2023) have developed a taxonomy featuring 16
forms of mental health identified in the literature. While it is beyond our scope to dwell on its details, the salient
point is that mental health can be differentiated into numerous forms. Indeed, these too can, in turn, be
deconstructed into subforms or internal dimensions, as seen, for instance, in the way Ryff's (1989) analyses of
eudaimonia identified six dimensions of psychological wellbeing.
As such, not only may illness and health constitute separate continua, in themselves they encompass numerous
dimensions, with a given person potentially doing poorly on some and better on others at any time. We have,
therefore, adapted the bivariate statespace of the dual continua model into a multivariate MIH Matrix, shown in
Figure 2. This figure allows the depiction of 11 forms of illness and 11 forms of health, represented visually as 11
horizontal lines (denoted by letters ak) and 11 vertical lines (numerals ixi), respectively. Eleven is of course an
arbitrary number, just selected for the purposes of matching a scale from 0 to 10 (which has 11 integers), which can
be used to assess the forms of illness and health (as discussed next). After all, given that the DSM5 includes 157
different mental disorders, one could create a grid with 157 horizontal lines. Indeed, diagnoses for many disorders
FIGURE 2 The Mental IllnessHealth Matrix.
6
|
LOMAS and VANDERWEELE
involve numerous symptoms; as such, it would be possible to create an incredibly finegrained grid, potentially
involving separate lines for all individual psychiatric symptoms. Similarly, great granularity is possible with mental
healththough not to the same degree as mental illnesssuch as the 16 forms we identified (which in themselves
can be further deconstructed into subcomponents).
As such, in using this matrix, we would encourage clinicians and researchers to make it as granular as possible for
their particular needs. Indeed, in allowing and even encouraging such flexibility and adaptability, the matrix is sensitive to
the critical insights aired abovenamely that the ways illness and health are experienced, conceptualized, and treated are
shaped by one's sociocultural contextwhile also remaining relevant to more conventional approaches. Clinicians and
scholars will have diverse views on this topic, from the more conservative (e.g., generally endorsing the ethos and details
of the DSM) to the more critical (e.g., embracing Szasz' notion of mental illness as myth). Crucially though, our matrix can
accommodate this range of perspectives. From a more conservative stance, one could simply structure the horizontal
illness aspect using the current DSM taxonomy. But from a critical perspective, even if one rejected the DSM taxonomy
entirely and replaced it with another framework, this could still be accommodated. The only exception would be if the
idea of psychological disorders was dismissed completely. However, it seems unlikely anyone would endorse that view;
even Szasz acknowledged people suffered from personal problems, which although not conceptualized as illnesses
could nevertheless be developed as a taxonomy and included in the matrix (in which case one might refer to mental
issuesrather than illnesses). Thus, the matrix can accommodate different perspectives and taxonomies in relation to
mental illness and health. Moreover, it enables a scoring system that allows an overarching assessment of mental illness
and health, as we consider next.
2.3 |Mental IllnessHealth assessment
Besides facilitating a finegrained understanding of mental illness and health, the MIH Matrix also allows
assessment and quantification in that respect. For every form of illness (i.e., rows ak) one could assign a number
from 10 to 0, and for every form of health (columns ixi), a number from 0 to +10. How these numbers are actually
identified is an open question. Many scales and assessments exist for the various forms of mental illness and health.
In principle, in most cases, one could convert the assessment into a score on a 10point range, allowing it to be
situated on the matrix. We would ideally want the assessment to reflect the quadrants in how they denote either
the presence or absence of illness and health. With the horizontal illness axis, the lefthand side of the matrix
(numbers 10 to 5) indicates the presence of illness, while the righthand side (5 to 0) signifies its relative
absence. Conversely, with the vertical health axis, the bottom half of the matrix (05) represents the relative
absence of health, while the top half (510) denotes its presence. The question is, how do we convert a continuum
into a categorical assessment (i.e., presence or absence of illness or health)?
For certain aspects of mental illness which have been codified into specific disorders, anything lower than 5
could signify the point a person crosses the boundary from technically not being diagnosed as having an illness (the
5 to 0 range)even if they experience some symptomsto being diagnosed with it (the 10 to 5 range). In the
case of major depression, for instance, the DSM states a person must experience five or more out of eight
symptoms during the same 2week period to warrant a diagnosis. In terms of the matrix, meeting these diagnostic
criteria could place a person in the lefthand territory (10 to 5) of having that illness; conversely, not meeting the
criteria yet still having some symptoms could situate them in the righthand territory (5 to 0) of not technically
having depression, yet experiencing aspects of it. Similarly, with mental health, although the field has not
progressed to the point where one is diagnosedas having particular forms, the +5 mark could mark the transition
from having only minimal levels of a given type (0 to +5), hence not attaining mental health per se in that regard, to
being positively deemed to be attaining it (+5 to +10).
In this way, the MIH Matrix could create a detailed summary of a person's mental wellbeing. For each form of
illness and health, a person could be assessed on that outcomeas discussed above with depressionthereby
LOMAS and VANDERWEELE
|
7
plotting their location along that line. One could even assign an overall score situating them broadly in one of the
four quadrants of the dual continua model. This would involve creating composite scores for both illness and health,
with their intersection locating the person somewhere in the bivariate state space. Taking first mental illness, say a
hypothetical person receives a score of 2 for depression (line a). As noted above, this would constitute not actually
being diagnosed with this disorder (with such a diagnosis taking the person into 10 to 5 territory), and would
reflect a relatively low presence of symptomology (with a total absence of depression being a 0). This is joined by
assessments for 10 other common disorders (lines bk), with respective scores of 1, 4, 6, 5, 2, 3, 2, 1, and
2. An overall composite score for mental illness would, therefore, be their mean value of 2.5, broadly situating the
person in the right hand of Figure 1: a relative absence of mental illness (i.e., averaged across the forms, even
though the person registered 6 with one specific form, so would technically have that one kind). One would
similarly ascertain their scores for 11 forms of mental health, and again calculate the mean to obtain an overall
score. Suppose this produces a health score of 6.8 (e.g., 8, 7, 5, 9, 4, 7, 6, 8, 7, 5, 9). The intersection of 2.6 for
illness and 6.4 for health would locate the person well within the topright thriving quadrant.
Moreover, the matrix further allows graphical representations of a person's overall mental illness and health, as
showninFigure3. In terms of health, one could plot the points on the vertical axes (i.e., upward lines spanning ixi) using
upwardpointing arrows, and connect these to produce a jagged horizontal line denoting the relative presence or absence
of mental health. One could also create a horizontal rectangle, shaded in light gray, whose upper and lower borders
correspond to the minimum and maximum of the mental health scores across all dimensions, indicating the range of
mental health. Additionally, one could plot a straight thick dashed horizontal line signifying the average mental health
FIGURE 3 An example of the Mental IllnessHealth Matrix assessment.
8
|
LOMAS and VANDERWEELE
score. One could likewise map all points on the horizontal axes (i.e., lefttoright lines spanning ak) using lefttoright
arrows, and connect them to produce a jagged vertical line denoting the relative presence or absence of mental illness.
One could similarly also create a vertical rectangle, shaded darker gray, whose lefthand and righthand borders
correspond to the maximum and minimum scores across all dimensions to show the range of mental illness. Finally, one
could plot a straight thick dashed vertical line corresponding to the average mental illness score. The two straight thick
dashed lines would intersect at the point of the matrix (2.5, 6.8) in the upper right quadrant corresponding to the
average scores in each of the composite healthillness dimensions.
We acknowledge that while this exercise might be helpful in seeing and understanding both the multidimensional
nature, and also possible coexistence, of mental illness and health, it is of course, a crude approximation. Most if not all
forms of mental illness (e.g., depression) and health (e.g., eudaimonia) are multidimensional and may be experienced in
different ways with varying symptoms. As such, any given MIH Matrix configuration is necessarily selectivegreater
granularity and additional dimensions will always be possible. Even so, with that limitation in mind, the matrix provides a
helpful framework to visualize the complex multidimensional nature of mental illness and health. However, while it allows
the representation and assessment of mental illness and health, it still leaves open the question of what it means to
experience one state versus another, and howin finegrained detailwe might view differences between states. The
second matrix is designed to address these issues.
3|THE MENTAL STATE SPACE (MSS) MATRIX
Our second framework for understanding mental states is the MSS Matrix. As per the MIH Matrix, it conceptualizes
mental states along numerous dimensions. However, instead of these constituting different forms of illness and
health, the MSS Matrix allows us to hypothesize the construction of illness and health out of various other
parameters. We begin by providing some context regarding the theoretical background to the MSS Matrix, namely
Fell's (2004)state spaceapproach. Next, we introduce the MSS Matrix itself, followed by a metaphor for how one
might construe its operation.
3.1 |The state space approach
The inspiration for the MSS Matrix is Fell's state space model. This model is situated in the neural correlates of
consciousnessparadigm, which examines associations between mind(e.g., subjective mental states of qualia) and
body(e.g., objective neurophysiological brain states). Their relationship remains poorly understood, and is still, at
some level, considered by most to be mysterious. This paradigm cannot yet establish causality, for instance. Mental
states certainly seem to depend—“supervenein philosophical terminologyon brain states for their existence.
However, the brain is perhaps better seen as the physiological architecture or mechanism by which mind is
instantiated, rather than its cause, as for instance elucidated by Lomas et al. (2022) visàvis happiness. Positive
affect, for example, has been associated with neurotransmitters like dopamine and serotonin. But its cause may be a
smile from a loved one. Indeed, from one perspective, mind and body could be regarded as two sides of the same
coin: mind is the body/brain experienced from the inside,and the body/brain is the mind observed from outside
(Nagel, 2012). In any case, while not yet able to ascertain causality, the neural correlates of consciousness paradigm
is based on the notion that any given subjective state of mind is accompanied by an associated objective brain state.
In Fell's model, both subjective mind and objective brain constitute state spaces of ndimensions (i.e., any
number of dimensions). The ndimensional statespace of the mind is intended to map or approximate every
possible quale: all thoughts, feelings, emotions, sensations, perceptions, and other phenomena a human is capable
of experiencing. A given experiential state will, therefore, occupy a specific locationsomewhere in this n
dimensional phenomenological statespace. This location will then correlate with a comparable location in the n
LOMAS and VANDERWEELE
|
9
dimensional physiological statespace of the brain. Consider, for example, a state of joy. According to the model,
this experience is constituted by the configuration of a person's mental statespace along many dimensions (e.g.,
valence, arousal), which then correlates with an analogous neurophysiological statespace configuration pertaining
to neural processes (such as activation of specific brain regions and release of certain neurotransmitters). Should the
configurations change in any waya lessening of subjective intensity say, accompanied by concomitant reduction in
the analogous brainstate dimensionthe feeling of joy would likewise change, shifting perhaps into a less aroused
state recognized as contentment.
However, Fell's theory does not adumbrate which dimensions might configure the mental or neurophysiological
state spaces, so ascertaining these is an open empirical question and a topic for future research. In that respect, our
MSS Matrix offers an initial foundation for such work by suggesting some likely candidates, though any
enumeration will always at best be an approximation. Thus, drawing on diverse psychological literature pertaining to
the mind, we focus on the possible parameters of the mental state space. Then, per the neural correlates of
consciousness paradigm, future work would ideally explore the equivalent neurophysiological parameters of the
brain state space, as considered in the conclusion.
3.2 |The MSS Matrix
There are many possible parameters for configuring the mental state spaces, but we can start with two of the most well
studied and conceptualized, valence and arousal. These are central to Russell's (1980) influential circumplex model of
affective states, which holds that such states are generated through the interaction of two independent
neurophysiological systems: valence (i.e., pleasant vs. unpleasant) and arousal (i.e., high vs. low, or active vs. passive)
(J. Posner et al., 2005). With valence, Cacioppo and Berntson's (1994) model suggests that rather than a bipolar
continuum, affect involves a bivariate space, with positive and negative valence being independent. Nevertheless,
regardless of the underlying mechanisms, valence is one dimension of the circumplex model, with affective states
evaluated as if on a spectrum (most negative to most positive). The second dimension is arousal, sometimes interpreted as
the activationin the central nervous system (from sleep/sleepiness to intense energy/excitement). Juxtaposing these
dimensions creates a twodimensional state space with four affective quadrants: low arousal and negative valence (e.g.,
depression); high arousal and negative valence (e.g., anxiety); low arousal and positive valence (e.g., calmness); and high
arousal and positive valence (e.g., elation). However, arousal and valence are not the only possible parameters on which
one can differentiate affective experiences, and certainly not forqualiamorebroadly(i.e.,allexperientialstates).Assuch,
here we identify other potential candidates, drawing on ideas across psychology.
We can first bring conceptual order to the kaleidoscope of possibilities by drawing on a theoretical framework of
psychological dynamics proposed by Lomas et al. (2015). The salient point about their framework is it differentiates
mental functioning and behavior into five broad heuristic levels (which overlap and are interconnected): consciousness,
emotions, cognitions, physicality, and behavior. Each, moreover, can be subjected internally to varying levels of
differentiation. For instance, one way of looking at consciousness is through the construct of attention. Thus, attention
could be one parameter of the mental state space which, construed simply, could specify how attentive one is at a given
moment.However,attentioncanalsobeunderstoodingranularways.M.I.PosnerandPetersen's(1990)modularmodel
proposes three functionally distinct neural networks: sustained attention (ongoing readiness for processing stimuli),
selective attention (allocation of resources to specific stimuli), and executive attention (monitoring/selecting from
competing stimuli). One could conceivably have a state space dimension for each component. Moreover, attention
whether conceived unitarily or modularlydoes not exhaust the granularity of consciousness. Other relevant concepts
include: awarenessfrom unconscious to maximally alert/awake (Morin, 2006); selfpresencehow selfconscious versus
selfless (Millière, 2020); mindfulnesshow mindfully present(KabatZinn, 2003); interoceptive awarenesshow aware
of the internal state of one's body (Tsakiris & Critchley, 2016); vividnesshow sharp and detailed the contents of
attention (Todd et al., 2012); estheticshow beautiful the contents (Tylka & Iannantuono, 2016); and directionality
10
|
LOMAS and VANDERWEELE
whetherfocusedoninnerqualiaorouterstimuli(Shaw,1996). Even these possibilities do not exhaust the potential
contours of consciousness, but are enough for now (and more than suffice to make the point).
It is beyond our scope to fully delve into the potential dimensions of the MSS Matrix in all their depth and
complexity. After all, each proposed dimension could itself warrant an entire paper devoted to it, as indeed the citations
provided for each dimension have done. Moreover, we cannot even exhaustively elucidate all possible dimensions:
ascertaining these, and moreover studying each in depthparticularly in terms of identifying potential correlates in the
neurophysiological state space of the brainwill be a key goal of future research. Nevertheless, we can briefly suggest
some likely dimensions to show the scope and potential of the MSS Matrix. After consciousness, the next tieris
emotions, within which we could include valence and arousal, as well as qualities like embodimentthe extent to which
mental states are felt throughout the body, as opposed to being more in the head(Coello & Fischer, 2016). Under
cognitions we could include: discursivityhow relatively full or empty the mind is of thought, and the extent to which this
thought is articulated in words (Bar et al., 2007); valencehow positive or negative the thoughts (Hogendoorn
et al., 2010); selfreferentialitythe extent to which thoughts concern the self (Northoff et al., 2006); abstractnesshow
concrete or abstract the thoughts (Lehmann et al., 2010); significancehow important or consequential their content
(Lyhne & Kørnøv, 2013); meaningfulnesshow personally meaningful (Steger et al., 2008); and memorialitythe extent to
which thoughts invoke/involve memories (Mildner & Tamir, 2019).
Next, under physicality, we could put attributes like: actionhow much physical activity is involved in the state
(Schutz et al., 2001); laborhow arduous the action (Trudeau et al., 2015); and skilfulnesslevel of skill involved
(Hildenbrand & Sanchez, 2022). Then, under behavior, we can put appraisals like: aspirationthe extent to which
the state reflects progress toward some goal (Harkin et al., 2016); achievementhow indicative of achieving a goal
(Gollwitzer & Sheeran, 2006); value alignmenthow much it resonates with one's values (Veage et al., 2014); and
moral worth (Nash et al., 2013). Finally, we could have parameters for the dynamics of the state itself, such
as: durationhow longlasting (Klostermann & Moeinirad, 2020); frequencyhow often one has this experience
(Clare et al., 2012); complexityhow complex and multifaceted (Martinon et al., 2019); agencyhow in control of
their experience people feel (Tapal et al., 2017); and numinositythe degree to which the state feels qualitatively
special and different from ordinaryexperiences (Underwood & Teresi, 2002).
3.3 |The MSS Matrix Mixing Desk
We suggest that any particular form of mental illness or healthand any experiential state more generallycould be
defined by a specific position on all the myriad dimensions above (all of which are hypothesized to correlate with an
associated neurophysiological dimension). A good way to appreciate these dynamics is through a musical metaphor,
specifically a mixing desk. This is a piece of hardware or software that allows sound engineers to manipulate the sound of
a given musical set up (e.g., a band), either in a live performance or recording session. The signal for each instrument or
voice feeds into the desk (e.g., via a microphone), with each having its own channel.Thus, with each instrument or voice,
the engineer can adjust its volumeand other acoustical properties, like toneas required to obtain a good overall sound.
So, we might construe the various MSS Matrix dimensions as different channels on a mixing desk, as per Figure 4,with
the specific state of that dimension represented by its volume level. Just as we imagined the dimensions of the MIH
Matrix on a standardized 010 scale, so might we picture any given channel as having a volume from 0 to 10. In some
instances, 010 will represent the minimal and maximal level of a given quality (i.e., 0 its absence/inactivation, and 10 its
maximal possible expression). In other cases, 010 represent poles of a particular spectrum (e.g., valence). In both cases, a
given state could be situated somewhere along that particular dimension, with the state as a whole constituting a unique
configuration of the mixing desk. This is currently only a hypothesis though, and will require substantiating by future
research, as we consider further in our concluding section.
LOMAS and VANDERWEELE
|
11
FIGURE 4 The Mental State Space Matrix Mixing desk.
12
|
LOMAS and VANDERWEELE
4|CONCLUSION, LIMITATIONS, AND FUTURE DIRECTIONS
This paper has proposed two related frameworks for understanding and assessing mental illness and health. First,
the MIH Matrix allows an appreciation that both mental illness and health are in themselves multidimensional, and a
person could experience various forms of both to varying degrees at a given time. Thus, the matrix positions types
of illness and health as horizontal versus vertical dimensions, respectively, enabling an overarching summary of
one's overall wellbeing upon these dimensions collectively. The MIH Matrix is augmented by the MSS Matrix,
which is also multidimensional, but instead of the dimensions constituting different forms of illness and health
allows us to hypothesize the construction of illness and health out of various other parameters. To that end, we
proposed 30 possible parameters, though we acknowledge that our selections are purely conceptual, and any actual
mapping requires exploration in future research. In that respect, we conclude by offering suggestions for such
research (as well as work on the MIH Matrix), doing so through the prism of also considering the potential clinical
pay offfor harnessing the matrices. We submit that the benefits could be manifold, including improving scientific
validity, understanding, and clinical care. Let us briefly consider these in turn.
First, the MIH Matrix, in particular, offers the possibility for enhanced scientific validity regarding the
constructs and measures through which mental illness and health are understood. An especially promising line of
research could involve, (a) construct validation in clinical settings (e.g., factor analytic research involving
psychometric measures of illness and health), (b) personcentered research (e.g., latent profile analyses to develop
individualized profiles of mental wellbeing), and even ideally (c) a combination of (a) and (b). Considerable progress
has already been made using these approaches. With (a), for example, Iasiello et al. (2022) conducted a meta
analysis of 46 studies that used the Mental Health ContinuumShort Form, and found the scale provides a robust
measure of general mental wellbeing, tapping into emotional, social, and psychological aspects of wellbeing in
both general and clinical populations. We are also seeing inroads using a personcentered approach, some of which
also combine construct validation, as per Khumalo et al.'s (2022) analysis above. However, the matrices have the
potential to further enhance this line of research. Recall that Khumalo et al.'s work featured 23 indicators, 14
regarding mental health (mainly focusing on hedonic and eudaimonic wellbeing), and 9 pertaining to illness
(focusing on depression). While such granularity is a good start, there are many other relevant indicators, as
discussed above, which would also be worth including in the MIH Matrix. As such, future research on this matrix
could ideally assemble a detailed and exhaustive taxonomy of all proposed forms of mental illness (e.g., spanning
the gamut of the DSM) and health (e.g., taking into account the wealth of emergent constructs in the relevant
literature)a list of indicators which may well run into the hundredsand conduct the kind of analysis exemplified
by Khumalo et al.
The matrices also have the power to enhance our understanding of mental illness and health. This is particularly
so with the MSS Matrix. While the MIH Matrix has great potential in developing our overall taxonomy of illness and
health, it may not necessarily constitute a radical advance in this area, but rather a complexification and refinement
of existing efforts (such as the dual continua model). By contrast, we suggest the MSS Matrix offers a truly new and
innovative perspective on mental illness and health that could greatly enhance our understanding of these topics.
Fruitful lines of enquiry in that regard might involve, (a) qualitative research, (b) neurophysiological analysis, and
even (c) a combination (as per the neurophenomenology paradigm). With (a), for example, people could be recruited
based on having a given type of mental illness or health. Indepth interviews could be conducted about these states,
in which each MSS Matrix dimension is systematically asked about (e.g., open questions like Tell us about the
quality of your attention, such as your level of distraction or focus). The results could be analyzed using
Interpretative Phenomenological Analysis (Smith, 2011), which facilitates indepth exploration of subjectivity. Such
work could be augmented by other qualitative methods, like Online Photovoice, a participatory action technique
where participants take and narrate photographs to share their experiences and perspectives (e.g., Tanhan &
Strack, 2020). Similarly, with (b), people currently experiencing a given type of illness or health could be assessed
using neurophysiological techniques (e.g., EEG) to assess the biological contours of these states (see e.g., Lomas,
LOMAS and VANDERWEELE
|
13
Ivtzan, et al., 2015, for a review of such work in relation to mindfulness). The neurophenomenology paradigm then
brings (a) and (b) together to provide further insights, where neurophysiological analysis of a mental state can be
matched to detailed subjective descriptions of that state, allowing more finegrained and nuanced analysis of its
mental dynamics. In this way, one could identify the MSS Matrix contours of different states of mental illness and
health.
Finally, the matrices have the potential to improve clinical care in various ways. For a start, they can help
practitioners appreciate different trajectories of client change and tailor clinical treatments accordingly. Recent
years have seen increased acknowledgment of the value of moving away from the variablecentered approach (i.e.,
explaining relationships between specific variables in a population) that has dominated mental illness/health work
over recent decades, and toward approaches that are personcentered (i.e., analyzing subgroups of similar subjects
within a population) and personspecific (i.e., explaining relationships between variables in a subject) (Howard &
Hoffman, 2018). Central to the latter paradigms is an appreciation of the idiosyncratic complexity of experiences
and trajectories of mental illness and health, and that clinical treatment and interaction need to be personalized
accordingly. To that end, research is progressing toward creating more individualized profiles of people's mental
states, as discussed above. In that respect, the matrices offer a stepchange in these kinds of personalized profile:
with its focus on different types of mental illness/health, the MIH Matrix allows mental wellbeing to be assessed
with unprecedented granularity, creating a unique profile for a given person; similarly, in focusing on different
elements of experience, the MSS Matrix enables mental states to be understood and analyzed with nuance and
precision. Of course, as noted, much more work is needed to identify and substantiate the details of the matrices,
including the different forms of illness/health for the MIH Matrix, and the various mental parameters for MSS
Matrix. So, as work on the matrices progresses, they will be increasingly useful in personcentered and person
specific approaches.
Moreover, even in their current nascent state, these matrices could inform current clinical practice. As it stands,
the MIH matrix already has potential in terms of encouraging clinicians to harness techniques to promote positive
mental health. Indeed, it is now 20 years since Seligman and Peterson (2003) mooted the idea of positive clinical
psychology.Since then, this approach has proved relatively effective,characterized broadly by taking concepts and
interventions that initially had success in promoting mental health among nonclinical populations (see e.g., Sin &
Lyubomirsky, 2009; White et al., 2019, Koydemir et al., 2021, and van Dierendonck & Lam, 2022, for meta
analyses), and applying these in a clinical context (see e.g., Carr et al., 2021, Geerling et al., 2020, Pina et al., 2021,
and Pan et al., 2022, for metaanalyses). Such activities range from exercises involving specific qualities such as
gratitude, to more involved courses featuring numerous elements, such as wellbeing therapy(Fava et al., 1998;
Guidi & Fava, 2021). However, despite this emergent literature, it remains a relatively niche concern; for instance,
while a search on Google Scholar in February 2023 shows that since the start of 2022 there have been 35,300
articles published with the phrase clinical psychology,there are only 387 featuring positive clinical psychology,
suggesting only around one in a hundred papers is considering the topic. This is understandable, given that a focus
on mental illness is a prerogative and defining feature of clinical psychology; as such, one can hardly expect the field
to have anywhere near an equal focus on mental illness and health (i.e., on the vertical and horizontal aspects of the
MIH Matrix). Nevertheless, we would encourage all clinicians to include at least some consideration of mental
health dimensions in their assessment and treatment of clients. Admittedly, understanding of the terrain of mental
health lags many decades behind that of mental illness, given that the latter has wellestablished taxonomies (e.g.,
the DSM5) and treatment protocols, and the former is still in its early phases of being conceptualized and
categorized. Even so, certain aspects of mental health have received extensive study and validation, and have been
shown to be very relevant in clinical settings.
Perhaps the prime example is Ryff's (1989) framework of psychological wellbeing (PWB) (see Joseph &
Wood, 2010; for a review). Not only is PWB strongly correlated with depression, it appears to be a risk factor in its
etiology: in a 10year cohort study looking at the onset of clinical depression in over 5500 people in middle age,
Wood and Joseph (2010) found people low in PWB were over seven times more likely to meet clinical cutoffs for
14
|
LOMAS and VANDERWEELE
depression 10 years later, and even with very conservative control variables (including initial depression,
personality, demographic, economic, and physical health variables) were still more than twice as likely to be
depressed 10 years later. Thus, they argue that even if middleaged people with low PWB do not currently meet
diagnostic criteria for depression, low PWB is a time bomblikely to lead to this in older age. At the very least then,
clinicians could assess clients in relation to PWB using the scales developed in Ryff's original work. But beyond that,
practices have been developed to help clients in clinical settings cultivate PWB, most prominently wellbeing
therapy (Fava et al., 1998; Guidi & Fava, 2021). This includes, for instance, techniques to help people develop
greater meaning and purpose in life (a key dimension of PWB). As such, even while we wait for the terrain of
positive mental healththe vertical dimensions of the MIH Matrixto be studied and charted in greater detail,
certain elements such as PWB are already wellestablished. So, even as it stands, we encourage clinicians to engage
at least with the notion of PWB, at a minimum assessing this in clients, and more ambitiously harnessing tools like
wellbeing therapy to promote it.
With the MSS Matrix, its current clinical potentials are less welldefined or selfevident. The MIH Matrix builds
on decades of scholarship emphasizing the importance of augmenting a clinical focus on mental illness with
attention to positive mental health. By contrast, the MSS Matrix is far newer and less wellgrounded in existing
clinical literature. Nevertheless, even without yet having been fully formalized or substantiated, it could still be
useful in clinical settings. For instance, its dimensions could serve as helpful prompts or discussion points in clinical
sessions, augmenting the standard protocol of enquiring about particular symptoms, and empowering clinicians to
explore people's experiences in terms of the MSS Matrix dimensions. The current iteration of the matrix includes 30
parameters, each of which could function as a generative question about a person's recent experience (either
specific events or a general state of affairs over a given period). There are seven dimensions pertaining to
consciousness, for example, which collectively would give an insight into a person's state of attention and
awareness. In the case of vividness, say, one could ask, how vivid was this experience, from very blurry to very
sharp?This kind of questioning would then augment the standard clinical questioning focused around conventional
symptomology.
This would have various benefits, including, for instance, allowing more culturally contextualized interactions
and practices. As discussed above, from a critical perspective, the diagnostic categories in established clinical
taxonomies like the DSM can be regarded as cultural constructions that not all people or cultures may share (e.g.,
what is regarded as an illness in one cultural context may not be in another). In that respect, discussing such clients'
experiences through the prism of these taxonomies can potentially be problematic. Indeed, clinical psychology and
comparable fields are increasingly attuned to the need to develop more culturally sensitive practices (Benuto
et al., 2021). With that in mind, the MSS Matrix offers a framework that could be helpful in discussing clients'
experiences, since it is more normatively neutral and less valuedloaded than the DSM. Rather than being
constructed in terms of symptomswhich already presupposes and imposes a frame of illness around a stateit
asks in more generic terms about the elements of a given experience. This would still allow clinicians to appraise and
understand clients' experiences in depth, yet in ways that can be more accommodating to cultural nuances and
other such contextual dynamics. As such, even while these matrices are just an early workinprogress, we hope
they may already serve a purpose in helping the field better understand and treat experiences of mental illness, and
moreover make progress in appreciating and facilitating states of positive mental health.
DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT
Data sharing not applicable to this article as no datasets were generated or analyzed during the current study.
PEER REVIEW
The peer review history for this article is available at https://www.webofscience.com/api/gateway/wos/peer-
review/10.1002/jclp.23512.
LOMAS and VANDERWEELE
|
15
REFERENCES
American Psychiatric Association (APA). (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.).
Aristotle. (1986). Nicomachean ethics (M. Ostwald, Trans.). Macmillan.
Assari, S. (2015). Ethnic and gender differences in additive effects of socioeconomics, psychiatric disorders, and subjective
religiosity on suicidal ideation among blacks. International Journal of Preventive Medicine,6, 53. https://doi.org/10.
4103/2008-7802.158913
Assari, S. (2017). Combined racial and gender differences in the longterm predictive role of education on depressive
symptoms and chronic medical conditions. Journal of Racial and Ethnic Health Disparities,4(3), 385396. https://doi.
org/10.1007/s40615-016-0239-7
Assari, S. (2018). Unequal gain of equal resources across racial groups. International Journal of Health Policy and
Management,7(1), 19. https://doi.org/10.15171/ijhpm.2017.90
Assari, S., & Lankarani, M. M. (2016). Education and alcohol consumption among older Americans; blackwhite differences.
Frontiers in Public Health,4, 67. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2016.00067
Bar, M., Aminoff, E., Mason, M., & Fenske, M. (2007). The units of thought. Hippocampus,17(6), 420428. https://doi.org/
10.1002/hipo.20287
Benning, T. B. (2016). No such thing as mental illness? Critical reflections on the major ideas and legacy of Thomas Szasz.
BJPsych Bulletin,40(6), 292295. https://doi.org/10.1192/pb.bp.115.053249
Benuto, L. T., Newlands, R., Singer, J., Casas, J., & Cummings, C. (2021). Culturally sensitive clinical practices: A mixed
methods study. Psychological Services,18(4), 632642. https://doi.org/10.1037/ser0000493
Breuer, J., & Freud, S. (1955). Studies on hysteria. In J. Strachey (Ed.), The standard edition of the complete psychological
works of sigmund freud (Vol. 2, pp. 1307). Hogarth Press.
Cacioppo, J. T., & Berntson, G. G. (1994). Relationship between attitudes and evaluative space: A critical review, with
emphasis on the separability of positive and negative substrates. Psychological Bulletin,115(3), 401423. https://doi.
org/10.1037/0033-2909.115.3.401
Carr, A., Cullen, K., Keeney, C., Canning, C., Mooney, O., Chinseallaigh, E., & O'Dowd, A. (2021). Effectiveness of positive
psychology interventions: A systematic review and metaanalysis. The Journal of Positive Psychology,16(6), 749769.
https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2020.1818807
Clare, L., Hindle, J. V., Jones, I. R., Thom, J. M., Nelis, S. M., Hounsome, B., & Whitaker, C. J. (2012). The AgeWell study of
behavior change to promote health and wellbeing in later life: Study protocol for a randomized controlled trial. Trials,
13(1), 115. https://doi.org/10.1186/1745-6215-13-115
Coello, Y. E., & Fischer, M. H. (2016). Foundations of embodied cognition: Perceptual and emotional embodiment. Routledge/
Taylor & Francis Group.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2013). Flow: The psychology of happiness. Random House.
Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., & Smith, H. L. (1999). Subjective wellbeing: Three decades of progress. Psychological
Bulletin,125(2), 276302. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.125.2.276
van Dierendonck, D., & Lam, H. (2022). Interventions to enhance eudaemonic psychological wellbeing: A metaanalytic
review with Ryff's Scales of psychological wellbeing. Applied Psychology. Health and WellBeing. Advance online
publication. https://doi.org/10.1111/aphw.12398
Fava, G. A., Rafanelli, C., Cazzaro, M., Conti, S., & Grandi, S. (1998). Wellbeing therapy. A novel psychotherapeutic
approach for residual symptoms of affective disorders. Psychological Medicine,28(2), 475480.
Fell, J. (2004). Identifying neural correlates of consciousness: The state space approach. Consciousness and Cognition,13(4),
709729. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2004.07.001
Geerling, B., Kraiss, J. T., Kelders, S. M., Stevens, A. W. M. M., Kupka, R. W., & Bohlmeijer, E. T. (2020). The effect of positive
psychology interventions on wellbeing and psychopathology in patients with severe mental illness: A systematic review and
metaanalysis. The Journal of Positive Psychology,15(5), 572587. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2020.1789695
Gollwitzer, P. M., & Sheeran, P. B. (2006). Implementation intentions and goal achievement: A metaanalysis of effects and
processes. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology,38,69119. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(06)38002-1
Guidi, J., & Fava, G. A. (2021). Conceptual and clinical innovations of wellbeing therapy. International Journal of Cognitive
Therapy,14, 196208. https://doi.org/10.1007/s41811-021-00101-1
Harkin, B., Webb, T. L., Chang, B. P. I., Prestwich, A., Conner, M., Kellar, I., Benn, Y., & Sheeran, P. (2016). Does monitoring
goal progress promote goal attainment? A metaanalysis of the experimental evidence. Psychological Bulletin,142(2),
198229. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000025
Herzlich, C. (1973). Health and illnessA social psychological analysis. Academic Press.
Hildenbrand, L., & Sanchez, C. A. (2022). Metacognitive accuracy across cognitive and physical task domains. Psychonomic
Bulletin & Review,29, 15241530. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-022-02066-4
16
|
LOMAS and VANDERWEELE
Hogendoorn, S. M., Wolters, L. H., Vervoort, L., Prins, P. J. M., Boer, F., Kooij, E., & de Haan, E. (2010). Measuring negative
and positive thoughts in children: An adaptation of the Children's Automatic Thoughts Scale (CATS). Cognitive Therapy
and Research,34(5), 467478. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-010-9306-2
Horwitz, A. V. (2019). The medicalization of grief. In M. H. Jacobsen, & A. Petersen (Eds.), Exploring grief (pp. 171187). Routledge.
Horwitz, A. V., & Wakefield, J. C. (2007). The loss of sadness. Oxford University Press.
Howard, M. C., & Hoffman, M. E. (2018). Variablecentered, personcentered, and personspecific approaches: Where
theory meets the method. Organizational Research Methods,21(4), 846876. https://doi.org/10.1177/
1094428117744021
Iasiello, M., Van Agteren, J., & Cochrane, E. M. (2020). Mental health and/or mental illness: A scoping review of the
evidence and implications of the dualcontinua model of mental health. Evidence Base,2020(1), 145. https://doi.org/
10.3316/informit.261420605378998
Iasiello, M., van Agteren, J., SchotanusDijkstra, M., Lo, L., Fassnacht, D. B., & Westerhof, G. J. (2022). Assessing mental
wellbeing using the Mental Health ContinuumShort Form: A systematic review and metaanalytic structural
equation modelling. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice,29(4), 442456. https://doi.org/10.1037/cps0000074
Jahoda, M. (1958). Current concepts of positive mental health. Basic Books.
Joseph, S., & Wood, A. (2010). Assessment of positive functioning in clinical psychology: Theoretical and practical issues.
Clinical Psychology Review,30(7), 830838. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2010.01.002
KabatZinn, J. (2003). Mindfulnessbased interventions in context: Past, present, and future. Clinical Psychology: Science and
Practice,10(2), 144156. https://doi.org/10.1093/clipsy.bpg016
Keyes, C. L. M. (2002). The mental health continuum: From languishing to flourishing in life. Journal of Health and Social
Behavior,43(2), 207222. https://doi.org/10.2307/3090197
Keyes, C. L. M. (2005). Mental illness and/or mental health? Investigating axioms of the complete state model of health.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,73(3), 539548. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.73.3.539
Keyes, C. L. M. (2014). Mental health as a complete state: How the salutogenic perspective completes the picture. In G. F. Bauer
& O. Hämmig (Eds.), Bridging occupational, organizational and public health: A transdisciplinary approach.Springer.
Khumalo, I. P., Appiah, R., & Wilson Fadiji, A. (2022). Measuring positive mental health and depression in Africa: A variable
based and personcentred analysis of the dualcontinua model. Frontiers in Psychology,13, 885278. https://doi.org/
10.3389/fpsyg.2022.885278
Klostermann, A., & Moeinirad, S. (2020). Fewer fixations of longer duration? Expert gaze behavior revisited. German Journal
of Exercise and Sport Research,50(1), 146161. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12662-019-00616-y
Koydemir, S., Sökmez, A. B., & Schütz, A. (2021). A metaanalysis of the effectiveness of randomized controlled positive
psychological interventions on subjective and psychological wellbeing. Applied Research in Quality of Life,16,
11451185.
Lehmann, D., PascualMarqui, R. D., Strik, W. K., & Koenig, T. (2010). Core networks for visualconcrete and abstract
thought content: A brain electric microstate analysis. NeuroImage,49(1), 10731079. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
neuroimage.2009.07.054
Lomas, T., Bartels, M., Van De Weijer, M., Pluess, M., Hanson, J., & VanderWeele, T. J. (2022). The architecture of
happiness. Emotion Review,14(4), 288309. https://doi.org/10.1177/17540739221114109
Lomas, T., Hefferon, K., & Ivtzan, I. (2015). The LIFE model: A metatheoretical conceptual map for applied positive
psychology. Journal of Happiness Studies,16, 13471364. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-014-9563-y
Lomas, T., Ivtzan, I., & Fu, C. H. (2015). A systematic review of the neurophysiology of mindfulness on EEG oscillations.
Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews,57, 401410. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.09.018
Lomas, T., & VanderWeele, T. J. (2023). Toward an expanded taxonomy of happiness: A conceptual analysis of 16 distinct
forms of mental wellbeing. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1177/00221678231155512
Lomas, T., & VanderWeele, T. J. (2022). The garden and the orchestra: Generative metaphors for conceptualizing the
complexities of wellbeing. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health,19(21), 14544. https://
doi.org/10.3390/ijerph192114544
Lyhne, I., & Kørnøv, L. (2013). How do we make sense of significance? Indications and reflections on an experiment. Impact
Assessment and Project Appraisal,31(3), 180189. https://doi.org/10.1080/14615517.2013.795694
Martinon, L. M., Smallwood, J., McGann, D., Hamilton, C., & Riby, L. M. (2019). The disentanglement of the neural and
experiential complexity of selfgenerated thoughts: A users guide to combining experience sampling with
neuroimaging data. NeuroImage,192,1525. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.02.034
Maslow, A. H. (1962). Toward a psychology of being. John Wiley.
Mildner, J. N., & Tamir, D. I. (2019). Spontaneous thought as an unconstrained memory process. Trends in Neurosciences,
42(11), 763777. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2019.09.001
Milliere, R. (2020). varieties of selflessness. Philosophy and the Mind Sciences,1(I), 141. https://doi.org/10.33735/phimisci.
2020.I.48
LOMAS and VANDERWEELE
|
17
Morin, A. (2006). Levels of consciousness and selfawareness: A comparison and integration of various neurocognitive
views. Consciousness and Cognition,15(2), 358371. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2005.09.006
Nagel, T. (2012). Conceiving the impossible and the mindbody problem. Revista Românăde Filosofie Analitică,6(1), 521.
Nash, W. P., Marino Carper, T. L., Mills, M. A., Au, T., Goldsmith, A., & Litz, B. T. (2013). Psychometric evaluation of the
Moral Injury Events Scale. Military Medicine,178(6), 646652. https://doi.org/10.7205/MILMED-D-13-00017
Northoff, G., Heinzel, A., de Greck, M., Bermpohl, F., Dobrowolny, H., & Panksepp, J. (2006). Selfreferential processing in
our brainA metaanalysis of imaging studies on the self. NeuroImage,31(1), 440457. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
neuroimage.2005.12.002
Pan, S., Ali, K., Kahathuduwa, C., Baronia, R., & Ibrahim, Y. (2022). Metaanalysis of positive psychology interventions on the
treatment of depression. Cureus,14(2), 21933. https://doi.org/10.7759/cureus.21933
Pina, I., Braga, C. M., de Oliveira, T. F. R., de Santana, C. N., Marques, R. C., & Machado, L. (2021). Positive psychology
interventions to improve wellbeing and symptoms in people on the schizophrenia spectrum: A systematic review and
metaanalysis. Brazilian Journal of Psychiatry,43, 430437. https://doi.org/10.1590/1516-4446-2021-0030
Posner, J., Russell, J. A., & Peterson, B. S. (2005). The circumplex model of affect: An integrative approach to affective
neuroscience, cognitive development. Development and Psychopathology,17(5), 715734. https://doi.org/10.1017/
S0954579405050340
Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience,13(1),
2542. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ne.13.030190.000325
Prilleltensky, I., Nelson, G., & Peirson, L. (2001). The role of power and control in children's lives: An ecological analysis of
pathways toward wellness, resilience and problems. Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology,11(2), 143158.
https://doi.org/10.1002/casp.616
Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,39(6), 11611178. https://
doi.org/10.1037/h0077714
Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorationsexplorations on the meaning of psychological wellbeing.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,57(6), 10691081. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.57.6.1069
Schueller, S. M., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2010). Pursuit of pleasure, engagement, and meaning: Relationships to subjective and
objective measures of wellbeing. The Journal of Positive Psychology,5(4), 253263. https://doi.org/10.1080/
17439761003794130
Schutz, Y., Weinsier, R. L., & Hunter, G. R. (2001). Assessment of freeliving physical activity in humans: An overview of
currently available and proposed new measures. Obesity Research,9(6), 368379. https://doi.org/10.1038/oby.
2001.48
Seligman, M. E. P. (2002). Authentic happiness: Using the New Positive Psychology to Realize your Potential for Lasting
Fulfillment. Free Press.
Seligman, M. E. P., & Peterson, C. (2003). Positive clinical psychology. In L. G. Aspinwall & U. M. Staudinger (Eds.),
A psychology of human strengths: Fundamental questions and future directions for a positive psychology (pp. 305317).
American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/10566-021
Shaw, J. C. (1996). Intention as a component of the alpharhythm response to mental activity. International Journal of
Psychophysiology,24(12), 723. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-8760(96)00052-9
Sin, N. L., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2009). Enhancing wellbeing and alleviating depressive symptoms with positive psychology
interventions: A practicefriendly metaanalysis. Journal of Clinical Psychology,65(5), 467487. https://doi.org/10.
1002/jclp.20593
Smith, J. A. (2011). Evaluating the contribution of interpretative phenomenological analysis. Health Psychology Review,5(1),
927. https://doi.org/10.1080/17437199.2010.510659
Steger, M. F., Kashdan, T. B., Sullivan, B. A., & Lorentz, D. (2008). Understanding the search for meaning in life: Personality,
cognitive style, and the dynamic between seeking and experiencing meaning. Journal of Personality,76(2), 199228.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2007.00484.x
Szasz, T. S. (1962). The myth of mental illness: Foundations of a theory of personal conduct. Secker & Warburg.
Tanhan, A., & Strack, R. W. (2020). Online photovoice to explore and advocate for muslim biopsychosocial spiritual
wellbeing and issues: Ecological systems theory and ally development. Current Psychology,39, 20102025. https://
doi.org/10.1007/s12144-020-00692-6
Tapal, A., Oren, E., Dar, R., & Eitam, B. (2017). The Sense of Agency Scale: A measure of consciously perceived control over
one's mind, body, and the immediate environment. Frontiers in Psychology,8.www.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/
fpsyg.2017.01552
Thompson, C. W., Aspinall, P., & Roe, J. (2014). Access to green space in disadvantaged urban communities: Evidence of
salutogenic effects based on biomarker and selfreport measures of wellbeing. ProcediaSocial and Behavioral Sciences,
153,1022. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2014.10.036
18
|
LOMAS and VANDERWEELE
Todd, R. M., Talmi, D., Schmitz, T. W., Susskind, J., & Anderson, A. K. (2012). Psychophysical and neural evidence for
emotionenhanced perceptual vividness. The Journal of Neuroscience,32(33), 1120111212. https://doi.org/10.1523/
JNEUROSCI.0155-12.2012
Trompetter, H. R., Lamers, S. M. A., Westerhof, G. J., Fledderus, M., & Bohlmeijer, E. T. (2017). Both positive mental health
and psychopathology should be monitored in psychotherapy: Confirmation for the dualfactor model in acceptance
and commitment therapy. Behaviour Research and Therapy,91,5863. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2017.01.008
Trudeau, F., Laurencelle, L., & Lajoie, C. (2015). Energy expenditure at work in physical education teachers. Applied
Ergonomics,46, 218223. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apergo.2014.08.010
Tsakiris, M., & Critchley, H. (2016). Interoception beyond homeostasis: Affect, cognition and mental health. Philosophical
Transactions of the Royal Society, B: Biological Sciences,371(1708), 20160002. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.
2016.0002
Tylka, T. L., & Iannantuono, A. C. (2016). Perceiving beauty in all women: Psychometric evaluation of the broad
conceptualization of beauty scale. Body image,17,6781. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2016.02.005
Underwood, L. G., & Teresi, J. A. (2002). The daily spiritual experience scale: Development, theoretical description,
reliability, exploratory factor analysis, and preliminary construct validity using healthrelated data. Annals of Behavioral
Medicine,24(1), 2233. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15324796ABM2401_04
Veage, S., Ciarrochi, J., Deane, F. P., Andresen, R., Oades, L. G., & Crowe, T. P. (2014). Value congruence, importance and
success and in the workplace: Links with wellbeing and burnout amongst mental health practitioners. Journal of
Contextual Behavioral Science,3(4), 258264. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcbs.2014.06.004
White, C. A., Uttl, B., & Holder, M. D. (2019). Metaanalyses of positive psychology interventions: The effects are much
smaller than previously reported. PLoS One,14(5), e0216588. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0216588
Wong, P. T. P. (2011). Positive psychology 2.0: Towards a balanced interactive model of the good life. Canadian Psychology/
Psychologie Canadienne,52(2), 6981. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022511
Wood, A. M., & Joseph, S. (2010). The absence of positive psychological (eudemonic) wellbeing as a risk factor for
depression: A ten year cohort study. Journal of Affective Disorders,122(3), 213217. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.
2009.06.032
Wood, A. M., & Tarrier, N. (2010). Positive clinical psychology: A new vision and strategy for integrated research and
practice. Clinical Psychology Review,30(7), 819829. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2010.06.003
How to cite this article: Lomas, T., & VanderWeele, T. J. (2023). The Mental IllnessHealth Matrix and the
Mental State Space Matrix: Complementary metaconceptual frameworks for evaluating psychological
states. Journal of Clinical Psychology,119. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.23512
LOMAS and VANDERWEELE
|
19
... Our conceptual approach is informed by the growing attention to dual-factor understandings of health (Lomas & VanderWeele, 2023), which distinguish distress and vulnerability from positive psychosocial and vocational functioning. That is, it cannot be presumed that religious leaders scoring low on measures of burnout, traumatization, and spiritual distress are necessarily flourishing in their work. ...
... Given their research questions and analyses, some studies were categorized under multiple domains, but articulations of contributors to negative outcomes dominated. Some may expect certain articles to be categorized as well-being enhancers based on the titles; however, when well-being was operationalized simply as a lack of distress, we categorized the study as describing protective factors (Lomas & VanderWeele, 2023). Only studies examining factors associated with positive psychosocial and vocational health were considered well-being enhancers. ...
... In religious leaders' vocational formation and the systems within which they work, inadequate attention has been paid to developing individual capacities, relational contexts, and organizational norms that promote these professionals' sense of flourishing and longevity. Not being burnt out or traumatized through one's vocation is a low bar, and lack of distress is not necessarily synonymous with thriving in one's work (Lomas & VanderWeele, 2023). Absence or reduction of distress may reflect the influence of protective factors or a person's baseline presentation, but a key point is that positive mental health includes the presence of well-being rather than just limited distress. ...
Article
Full-text available
Religious leaders (i.e., clergy and chaplains) face unique, ongoing stressors that can increase risks for psychosocial and vocational vulnerabilities. Emerging evidence indicates concerning prevalence rates of distress and attrition among these professionals, particularly since the COVID-19 pandemic. To date, most empirical work has focused on compromised functioning among religious leaders. Utilizing a more holistic approach, this systematic review explores individual, relational, and organizational factors associated with diverse outcomes. Following the PRISMA methodology, we identified 82 empirical articles investigating (a) risk and protective factors related to burnout, trauma impacts, spiritual distress, and other occupational hazards and/or (b) factors associated with well-being and flourishing, over and above distress reduction. We summarize the state of the available evidence, distinguishing between risk increasers, protective factors, and well-being enhancers. Attention is given to three domains: individual (e.g., demographics, personality factors, virtue development, coping and formation practices), relational (e.g., peer, family, and collegial supports; navigation of conflicts and polarized issues in one’s community of care), and institutional (e.g., role ambiguity or clarity, resource availability, systemic expectations and demands). We identify notable gaps to be addressed in future research; for example, most studies are cross-sectional, lack diversity in religion, gender, and geography, and operationalize well-being as the absence of symptoms rather than the presence of positive states and functioning. Considering the available evidence, we present best practices to guide psychological practitioners, denominational bodies, and others involved in religious leaders’ formation.
... Consistent with dual-factor models of mental health (Lomas & VanderWeele, 2023), more research is needed on psychotherapy's contributions to well-being and the development of capacities that enhance clients' flourishing, including virtues (Proctor, 2019). For this study, we used person-centred quantitative analyses and grounded theory qualitative methods to investigate the following question: ...
Article
Introduction Positive psychology and virtue ethics traditions suggest that virtue development is a pathway to well‐being, but few studies have examined how psychotherapy in naturalistic settings contributes to clients' virtue engagement. Method This study was conducted at a community mental health clinic specialising in contemporary relational psychotherapy (CRP) in the northeast United States. The embedded, explanatory mixed method design included (a) longitudinal mixture‐modelling to identify clients showing gains in relational virtue engagement over time, and (b) interviewing a subsample ( N = 15) of these clients. Results Our findings suggest that virtue engagement in CRP emerges (a) within the dynamic conditions of the alliance and (b) through holistic attunement to clients' identities, experiences, mental health and growth in capacities that promote wisdom and flourishing. Clinical and research implications and future research directions are discussed.
... This hybridization leads us to the concept of 'human ourishing'-as a state of complete human wellbeing and growth(VanderWeele, 2017). Human ourishing stands for the relative attainment of a state in which all aspects of a person's life are good, including the contexts in which that person lives (VanderWeele andLomas, 2023). ...
Technical Report
Report I,‘ An evolutionary Framework for Flourishing Learning Ecosystems’, becomes a cross-analysis of data documented from fieldwork gathering the intentional collaborative work of the NetEdu team to answer: How learning ecosystems change, evolve and flourish over time in diverse contexts? What are the systemic enablers that need to be unleashed and seeded by decision makers and leaders in the ecosystem for learning and flourishing? This report presents a framework as a rubrick that documents the evolutionary nature of learnng ecosystems across 7 dimensions and 4 stages of development. This framework claims to serve as a diagnosis and discussion tool helping national leaders to understand and support holistic learning, equity and wellbeing across the territory.
... Relatedly, PCP reconceptualizes the pathologies of mental disorders by including the absence of adaptive functioning as a complement to the tradition of heavily attending to clients' deficits in psychopathology. The idea is that not only what is going wrong (e.g., depressive or anxious emotions) but what is impeding the right (e.g., a low sense of optimism or self-efficacy) serves the causes of mental disorders, and each part works on ones' adaptation via its distinctive route (Maddux, 2008;Lomas and VanderWeele, 2023;Zhao and Tay, 2023). Following its adaptation view, PCP challenges a traditional positive-negative dichotomy imposed to any clinical conditions. ...
Article
Full-text available
In serving college students with mental disorders, on-campus mental health professionals have been lacking integrative theoretical frameworks to guide their missions of prevention, remedy, and development facilitation. In the current paper, we propose the positive clinical psychology as a theoretically and practically valuable framework for these missions by narratively reviewing the preventive, remedial, and developmental mechanisms derived from the theory and summarizing the most recent empirical evidence that supports each mechanism. We further discuss why and how these mechanisms and findings can be applied to on-campus mental health services to facilitate the resilience and optimal development of college students with mental disorders. Particularly, the use of resilience-focused and strength-based intervention strategies are promoted for services.
... However, as Keyes himself recognised, illhealth/ill-being and health/well-being are somewhat separate -physiologically, functionally, and experientially -and people can have aspects of both concurrently. Moreover, the spectrum implies that both ill-health/ill-being and health/well-being are singular, whereas there are many different forms of both; in the mental domain, for example, there are various types of both psychiatric disorders and forms of well-being, as we have sought to capture in a new Mental Illness-Health Matrix (Lomas & VanderWeele, 2023a). To reflect these nuances, Keyes (2005Keyes ( , 2014) developed a "dual continua" model, with separate spectra for mental ill-health/ill-being and health/well-being, which can be placed orthogonally to create a bivariate state space, with a horizontal ill-health/ill-being axis and a vertical health/well-being axis, as shown in Figure 4 (a few pages below). ...
Article
Full-text available
Concepts like flourishing, well-being, health, and happiness are of increasing interest across many fields, from psychology and medicine to politics and economics. However, these terms are used in diverse and contested ways, which makes it hard to find common ground and understanding. To attempt to help remedy the confusion, this paper offers an overarching conceptual "map" within which these concepts can be situated, thereby providing a common language and framework for their consideration. Moreover, while the overall configuration of this map is conceptually and logically stable, its specific elements are more flexible, particularly in terms of scalable granularity (allowing fine-grained differentiation of internal regions) and epistemological openness (allowing revisions in light of gains in knowledge). As such, the map can be adapted to suit different fields, and updated to accommodate advances in understanding. To that end, we clarify topics of investigation that are still in need of development, providing a roadmap for future research.
... Психоемоційний стан є складною інтерактивною сферою, що охоплює фізіологічні, когнітивні, емоційні, та соціальні аспекти психічного стану особи і відображає загальний стан її психологічного благополуччя [3]. Відомо, що температурні зміни мають значний вплив на фізичний та психологічний стан організму [4]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Зміни клімату та глобальне потепління стали однією з головних проблем сучасного світу, згубно впливаючи на наше навколишнє середовище і суспільство в цілому. Дана стаття присвячена дослідженню впливу теплочутливості на психічний стан індивідів в умовах сучасного глобального потепління. Актуальність проблеми полягає в тому, що зміни клімату стають не тільки екологічними викликами, але і впливають на психологічну стійкість та благополуччя людей. Метою даного дослідження було оцінити особливості психоемоційного стану у осіб з різною теплочутливістю. Для цього було залучено залучено 150 студентів у віці 17-20 років. Учасники дослідження були поділені на дві групи – із вищою та нижчою теплочутливістю. Оцінка індивідуальної теплової чутливості була визначений за допомогою опитувальника "Рівні теплочутливості". Була проведена оцінка особистісних характеристик, оцінка особистісної та реактивної тривожності за допомогою відповідних тесті. Також було проведено визначення рівня адаптованості до різних життєвих викликів. В результаті дослідження було встановлено, що особи з нижчою теплочутливістю є емоційно більш стійкими і можуть адекватніше реагувати на стресові фактори навколишнього середовища. Було відзначено наявність різниці в рівні адаптації між двома групами, а саме - група з нижчою теплочутливістю виявила більш успішно налагоджений механізм адаптації до навколишнього середовища порівняно із групою з вищою теплочутливістю. Загалом, ці результати вказують на індивідуальні особливості у реакції на стресові ситуації та адаптаційні можливості різних груп. Це дослідження не підтверджує важливість врахування теплочутливості при аналізі психоемоційного стану людей, але також надає основи для подальших досліджень у цьому напрямі. Розуміння, як реакція на зміни клімату впливає на людське психічне благополуччя, є одним із ключових факторів для розробки стратегій збереження психологічного здоров'я в умовах зміни клімату.
... Starting from quadrant I in the left towards quadrant IV in a clockwise direction, we see struggling, thriving, languishing, and faltering as dynamic aspects of the mental illness-health continuum. In this, Lomas and VanderWeele (2023) adds the Mental State Space Matrix as a temporal dimension, capturing the changes and fluctuations in mental states over time, allowing for the mapping of transitions, trajectories, and patterns, aiding targeted interventions and personalised treatment plans. Thus, equipping the professionals with visionary policy-making and diagnosis competence beyond the medical model of 'no illness' approach. ...
Article
This paper introduces the innovative integration of the Cartesian Coordinate System (CCS) into psychotherapy to enhance client self-reflection and insight. The historical evolution of psychological theories and therapies is explored, highlighting the continuous quest for effective treatment. Inspired by René Descartes' mathematical system, the CCS is applied in psychotherapy as a tool in collaboration with the therapist-client bond, to graphically represent clients' presenting problems, facilitating structured breakdown and understanding through quadrants. The paper elaborates on the theoretical underpinnings, effectiveness of data visualisation, and the relevance of matrix systems in psychotherapy. The proposed CCS model is introduced, detailing its practical application in therapy through case illustrations. The model not only supports therapeutic progress but also offers potential for coping beyond therapy, enhancing personal awareness, cognitive restructuring, emotion regulation, and problem-solving. The model's inclusive design principles cater to diverse clients, including neurodivergent individuals. By promoting metacognition and empowerment, the CCS model contributes to a holistic approach to mental health and personal growth.
Article
Full-text available
Dual careers (DCs) are challenging trajectories followed by athletes willing to develop their academic/professional career with their athletic careers. These trajectories usually entail additional stressors, which can decrease athletes' mental health or even increase their risk of mental ill‐health. While existing research has recognized the importance of psychological and social factors in both of these areas separately, we lack systematic knowledge on which factors are associated with European DC athlete mental health outcomes, making evidence‐based practice more challenging. In this regard, to advance the European DC tradition and to provide a strong base for researchers and practitioners working within this field, this systematic review aims to appraise this evidence identifying and categorizing the psychological and social factors associated with the European DC athletes' mental health. We conducted this review according to Preferred Reporting for Systematic Reviews and Meta‐Analysis guidelines and performed the systematic search in six databases, finding 56 eligible articles. Our analysis identified 35 different psychological and social factors, most showing significant associations with athletes' mental health. Notably, affect, stress, motivational climate, mindfulness, resilience, perfectionism, goal orientation, motivation, and basic psychological need satisfaction showed the strongest evidence associated with mental health. Overall, this review provides a comprehensive synthesis of psychological and social factors, advancing the holistic ecological approach in dual careers and athlete mental health. Yet, our results highlight the need to strengthen the evidence regarding these factors and provide specific research avenues, such as focus on DC‐specific factors and consideration of DC athlete definition and career trajectories.
Article
Full-text available
For over ten years the World Happiness Report has influentially ranked nations on self-reported life evaluation as measured by the Gallup World Poll. Inspired by this endeavour, this paper aims to broaden our understanding of global flourishing by assessing an expansive battery of 38 items relating to wellbeing in the World Poll, encompassing 386,654 people in 145 countries over three years (2020-2022). The variation in the respective placing of countries across different items reveals a complex picture of flourishing, with many nations ranking highly on certain metrics but faring poorly on others. Moreover, a factor analysis of the items produced a conceptualization of flourishing featuring nine different dimensions. Together, these findings paint a nuanced picture of both the multifaceted nature of flourishing and its complex manifestations around the world.
Article
Full-text available
Academic interest in well-being has blossomed in recent years, to the point that numerous forms of well-being have now been proposed, covering myriad aspects of the person (e.g., mental, physical, social, spiritual) and of life more broadly (e.g., communal, economic, environmental). This proliferation of forms raises the question of how they might ideally interrelate, as well as whether there is some kind of overall well-being that draws them together. To that end, this paper argues that a zenith of ultimate or complete well-being would involve managing to sustain well-being across numerous systems (i.e., configurations of different processes and entities), such that they are in a state of balance and harmony. These systems include: (a) the various dimensions of the person (e.g., physical, mental, social, spiritual); (b) self-and-other (i.e., where both the person and those around are doing well); (c) people-and-environment (i.e., where human well-being is aligned with the well-being of nature); and (d) time (i.e., well-being obtains both in the present and into the future). We suggest that attaining all of these various forms of sustainable well-being constitutes an ideal of flourishing to which people and societies can and should aspire.
Article
Full-text available
Recent decades have seen a surge of scientific interest in happiness. However, its theoretical conceptualization is a work in progress. Much of the literature focuses on two main forms: hedonic (encompassing life satisfaction and positive affect) and eudaimonic (encompassing phenomena such as character development and meaning in life). However, this binary has been critiqued as being incomplete, in part because it reflects a Western-centric perspective that overlooks forms emphasized in non-Western cultures. As a result, scholars have begun to highlight other forms besides hedonia and eudaimonia. This article surveys the literature to identify 16 potential forms in total, classified according to whether they primarily pertain to feelings (hedonic, contented, mature, chaironic, and vital), thought (evaluative, meaningful, intellective, aesthetic, and absorbed) or action (eudaimonic, masterful, accomplished, harmonic, nirvanic, and relational). This article thus offers a more expansive, albeit still just provisional, taxonomy of this vital and still-evolving topic.
Article
Full-text available
Mental wellbeing is an increasingly relevant outcome in clinical psychology, and rigorous measurement tools are required to ensure high quality data. This study aimed to systematically review and meta-analyze the factor structure of a popular measurement tool of mental wellbeing, the Mental Health Continuum—Short Form (MHC-SF). The systematic review identified 46 studies which investigated the performance of the MHC-SF, which consistently supported the psychometric properties of the scale. Meta-analytic structural equation modelling (MASEM) was used with data extracted from 26 studies (n = 108,603). MASEM indicated support for the original tripartite structure of the MHC-SF, as well as a hierarchical model and a bifactor model. The hierarchical model (and the nested tripartite model) was supported theoretically and performed similarly across clinical and general populations. The current study demonstrates that the MHC-SF is a valid measure of general mental wellbeing, which taps into concepts of emotional, social, and psychological wellbeing in general and clinical populations. Caution may be required when comparing scores across clinical and non-clinical cohorts.
Article
Full-text available
Our understanding of well-being, and related concepts such as health and flourishing, is shaped by the metaphors through which we think about such ideas. Current dominant metaphors—including a pyramid, ladder, and continuum—all have various issues. As such, this paper offers two other metaphors which can better do justice to the nuanced complexities of these notions, namely, a garden and an orchestra. Through these metaphors, this paper articulates a comprehensive framework for conceptualizing and appreciating the nature of well-being (and associated concepts), which it is hoped will generate further insights and research into these valued and sought-after phenomena.
Article
Full-text available
Happiness is an increasingly prominent topic of interest across academia. However, relatively little attention has been paid to how it is created, especially not in a multidimensional sense. By ‘created’ we do not mean its influencing factors, for which there is extensive research, but how it actually forms in the person. The work that has been done in this arena tends to focus on physiological dynamics, which are certainly part of the puzzle. But they are not the whole picture, with psychological, phenomenological, and socio cultural processes also playing their part. As a result, this paper offers a multidimensional overview of scholarship on the ‘architecture’ of happiness, providing a stimulus for further work into this important topic.
Article
Full-text available
The dual-continua model of mental health provides a contemporary framework for conceptualising and operationalising mental health. According to this model, mental health is distinct from but related to mental illness, and not the opposite or merely the absence of psychopathology symptoms. To examine the validity of the dual-continua model, previous studies have either applied variable-based analysis such as confirmatory factor analysis (CFA), or used predetermined cut-off points for subgroup division. The present study extends this contribution by subjecting data from an African sample to both CFA and latent class analysis (LCA) to test the dual-continua model in Africa. We applied CFA separately for the Mental Health Continuum—Short Form (MHC-SF) and Patient Health Questionnaire—9 (PHQ-9); and LCA on combined item responses. College students (N = 892; average age = 22.74, SD = 4.92; female = 58%) from Ghana (n = 309), Kenya (n = 262), Mozambique (n = 232), and South Africa (n = 89) completed the MHC-SF and PHQ-9. With minor modifications to the measurement models, the CFA results of this study confirm the three-factor structure of the MHC-SF, and a unidimensional solution for the PHQ-9. LCA results show the presence of three distinct latent classes: languishing with moderate endorsement of depressive symptoms (25.9%), flourishing with low endorsement of depressive symptoms (63.7%), and moderate mental health with high endorsement of depressive symptoms (10.4%). These findings further contribute to affirming the evidence for the dual-continua model of mental health, with implications for the assessment of mental health, to inform policy, practise, and future research in community and clinical settings in Africa.
Article
Full-text available
This meta-analysis examined the efficacy of positive psychology interventions (PPIs) in treating depression in 11 articles. PubMed, Web of Science, and Clinical Key were used to identify papers published from 2010 to 2020 that utilized PPIs. Key terms were "positive psychology" and "treatment of depression." Studies on adults with (a) depressive symptoms or (b) diagnosed clinical depression were included. A random-effects model was used to compare PPIs and control groups on post- vs. pre-intervention differences in depression scores. Data analysis examined Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II), Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D), and Quick Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology-Self-Report (QIDS-SR16) scores. Findings show PPIs are effective in treating depressive symptoms, with significant improvements in depression scores when compared to control groups in all but one study. This was true for both post- vs. pre-intervention (pooled Cohen's d = -0.44 (-0.77, -0.11)) and follow-up- vs. pre-intervention analyses (pooled Cohen's d = -0.46 (-1.02, 0.09)). PPIs can improve the accessibility and affordability of depression treatments.
Book
The Loss of Sadness argues that the increased prevalence of major depressive disorder is due not to a genuine rise in mental disease, but to the way that normal human sadness has been 'pathologised' since 1980. That year saw the publication of the landmark third edition of The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-III), which has since become a dominant force behind our current understanding of mental illness overall. As concerns at least major depression, the authors argue that the DSM's definition of the condition is too broad and that as a result virtually all research and clinical approaches to the condition have been based on a flawed understanding about it. The social, political, and scientific implications of this are far-reaching - from the overselling of antidepressants to treat ordinary sadness, as Big Pharma exploits the DSM for its own purposes; to intrusive and expensive depression screening programs at all levels of society, as well-meaning but misguided initiatives translate the DSM into simple terms to catch any whiff of depressive pathology in our midst; and funded research into the 'epidemic' of depression, which advances the field very litttle and the public even less. Ultimately, the definition of depression that is in operation today has formed the basis for an entire system of social control (e.g. community-wide screening initiatives, intrusive public health policy) that benefits psychiatry, primary care providers, and the pharmaceutical and insurance industries by turning everyone else into a potential consumer of services, needed or not. The authors do recognise that depression is a devastating illness that affects some people. Their chief concern is with the use of this diagnosis as a catch-all for anyone who has experienced sadness for more than a few weeks at a time. The result is a pointed yet nuanced critique of modern psychiatry that will stir controversy of the sort that will reacquaint us with sadness as a primary human emotion and that could productively influence the way that depression the actual illness is characterised in the future.
Article
This meta‐analysis enhances our insight into the effectiveness of interventions aimed at improving eudaemonic well‐being. The focal outcome of these interventions is Ryff's Scales of Psychological Well‐being. We summarized experimental studies and concluded whether a specific intervention approach improves individual positive functioning by assessing the six dimensions of psychological well‐being and the composite score of well‐being. Our study confirmed that eudaemonic well‐being can be improved. The strongest influence is seen in integral programs that link directly to Ryff's conceptual model. Breaking down to dimensional scores, existing interventions had the strongest influence on Environmental Mastery, Personal Growth, and Self‐Acceptance. The weakest influence was on Autonomy and Positive Relations with Others. Overall, our result is an important contribution to the well‐being literature in that it shows, more convincing than previous meta‐analyses due to its exclusive and comprehensive focus on Ryff's model, that psychological eudaemonic well‐being can be enhanced by targeted intervention programs.
Article
Metacognition is often considered a critical component of learning and higher-order cognition; however, does metacognitive accuracy remain constant across all tasks, specifically in tasks that involve physical or procedural components? To investigate the consistency of metacognitive judgments across various task types, participants completed word and number recall tasks, and also completed three simple physical skill tasks (e.g., catching a ball in a cup). Participants made metacognitive judgments about their performance in all tasks. Results indicated that while participants demonstrated traditional levels of relative metacognitive accuracy in more cognitive tasks, participants were significantly more accurate in their judgments for physical skill tasks. In other words, relative accuracy for metacognitive judgments in physical tasks appears to be significantly higher than for cognitive tasks. This represents the first such demonstration of this effect and suggests that characteristics of physical tasks somehow improve participants' judgments of how well they have learned.
Article
The evidence supporting the use of well-being therapy (WBT), a specific psychotherapeutic strategy for modulating psychological well-being, and its specific contribution when it is combined with other psychotherapeutic techniques is still limited. However, the insights gained by the use of WBT may unravel innovative approaches to assessment and treatment of mood and anxiety disorders. An important characteristic of WBT is self-observation of psychological well-being associated with specific homework. Another important feature of WBT is the assumption that imbalances in well-being and distress may vary from one illness to another, and from patient to patient. A final characteristic is its reference to the concept of euthymia, where lack of mood disturbances is associated with positive affect and psychological well-being (flexibility, consistency, and resilience). Progress can be achieved by modifying our clinical approach to mental disorders, and by promoting self-observation of psychological well-being and the pursuit of a state of euthymia.